Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: a quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: a quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs"

Transcription

1 Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 53:1 (2012), pp doi: /j x Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: a quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs Henrik Larsson, 1,2 Henrik Anckarsater, 3 Maria Råstam, 4 Zheng Chang, 1 and Paul Lichtenstein 1 1 Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm; 2 Karolinska Institutet Center for Neurodevelopmental Disorders, Stockholm; 3 Department of Forensic Psychiatry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgren s Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg; 4 Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Clinical Sciences, Lund University, Lund, Sweden Background: Although the clinical utility of categorically defined attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is well established, there is also strong evidence supporting the notion of ADHD as an extreme of a continuous trait. Nevertheless, the question of whether the etiology is the same for different levels of DSM IV ADHD symptoms remains to be investigated. The aim of this study was to assess genetic links between the extreme and the subthreshold range of ADHD symptoms. Method: Parents of all Swedish 9- and 12-year-old twins born between 1992 and 2000 were interviewed for DSM IV ADHD symptoms and associated conditions. Two validated cutoff values were used for screening and assigning research diagnoses. Response rate was 80%. Twin methods were applied to investigate the extent to which ADHD is etiologically distinct from subthreshold variations in ADHD symptoms. Results: Extremes analyses indicated a strong genetic link between the extreme and the subthreshold variation, with almost identical group heritability estimates around.60 for the diagnostic (prevalence 1.78%) and screening (prevalence 9.75%) criteria of ADHD. Conclusion: A strong genetic link between the extreme and the subthreshold variation of DSM IV based assessments of ADHD symptoms was found. The data suggest that ADHD is best viewed as the quantitative extreme of genetic and environmental factors operating dimensionally throughout the distribution of ADHD symptoms, indicating that the same etiologic factors are involved in the full range of symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity. Keywords: ADHD, DSM, etiology, twins. Introduction Although attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most thoroughly researched disorders in medicine (Goldman, Genel, Bezman, & Slanetz, 1998), the question of whether ADHD is best viewed as a categorical disorder or as an extreme of a continuous trait has yet to be finally determined, with consequences for the impact on future etiologic research, treatment and the upcoming DSM V conceptualization of ADHD (Thapar & Lewis, 2009). The two main psychiatric classification systems (ICD 10, DSM IV) used for the diagnosis of ADHD are exclusively categorical in nature. In many contexts, clinical decisions have to be categorical, and data have supported ADHD as a valid diagnostic category associated with serious consequences for the affected individuals (Biederman, 2005). Nevertheless, sophisticated statistical modeling techniques (e.g. factor mixture and taxometric procedures) have been used to show that ADHD exists on a severity continuum (Frazier, Youngstrom, & Naugle, 2007; Haslam et al., 2006; Lubke, Hudziak, Derks, van Bijsterveldt, & Boomsma, 2009). Comparisons of neuropsychological (Faraone et al., Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared. 2006) and personality (Faraone, Kunwar, Adamson, & Biederman, 2009) measures between individuals with subthreshold ADHD and controls provide further support for an ADHD continuum. Thus, although the clinical utility of categorically defined ADHD is well established (Biederman, 2005), there is also strong evidence supporting the notion of ADHD as an extreme of a continuous trait. Because of the difficulty in identifying sufficient numbers of affected twin pairs, only a few twin studies (Goodman & Stevenson, 1989; Sherman, McGue, & Iacono, 1997; Thapar, Harrington, Ross, & McGuffin, 2000), including one that has used the same sample as the present study (Lichtenstein, Carlstrom, Rastam, Gillberg, & Anckarsater, 2010), have explored the genetic impact on categorically defined ADHD. The heritability estimates from these studies range from 75% to 90%, which is consistent with a large number of studies of quantitative measures (i.e. defining ADHD as a continuous trait; Faraone et al., 2005; Thapar, Holmes, Poulton, & Harrington, 1999). Thus, twin studies report strong genetic effect in ADHD regardless of whether it is treated as a categorically defined diagnosis or as continuously distributed dimension. A core issue in the categorical versus dimensional discussion is whether the etiology is the same for Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA 02148, USA

2 74 Henrik Larsson et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012; 53(1): different levels of ADHD symptoms. Epidemiological studies provide evidence of dose response relationships between environmental risks and ADHD symptoms scores across the range of severity (Scahill et al., 1999). Similar treatment response (including improved attention) of low-dose psychostimulant medication has been reported among children diagnosed with ADHD and children with subthreshold problems, which also supports a dimensional model for the neurobiological mechanisms behind ADHD (Rapoport et al., 1980). Twin studies can also be used to address the question whether the genetic and environmental etiology is the same at the extreme end (i.e. at a diagnostic level) and the subthreshold range of a mental health problem, using the so called DeFries Fulker (DF) extremes analysis (DeFries & Fulker, 1988). Thus, in addition to answering the question whether ADHD is best viewed as a categorical disorder or as an extreme of a continuous trait, this method can clarify whether the different levels of ADHD symptoms have the same or different etiologies. One study of 583 same-sexed twin pairs using the DF approach found that the heritability of ADHD III R symptoms was similar at the extreme end of the distribution (92%, confidence interval ± 12) as for the subthreshold range of scores (75%, confidence interval: ± 24; Levy, Hay, McStephen, Wood, & Waldman, 1997). However, this study did not use the ADHD symptoms described in DSM IV and was not based on a large enough sample to find a sufficient number of children meeting diagnostic criteria, applying instead a five-symptom cutoff that identified around 30% of the male twins as cases. This means that the genetic link between the very extreme end of the distribution and the subthreshold range of ADHD symptoms has not yet been investigated. There is a need for studies addressing the categorical versus continuous nature of ADHD in larger twin samples and using the DSM IV definition of ADHD, especially in view of this being one of the many questions that the DSM V Child work group is considering (Rohde, 2008). In the present report, we use quantitative assessments of ADHD symptoms from 16,858 Swedish 9- and 12-year-old twins to investigate the etiology of the ADHD phenotype. The large sample size allows us, for the first time, to assess genetic links between the extreme and the subthreshold range of ADHD symptoms using categorical definitions based on validated (Hansson, Svanstrom Rojvall, Rastam, Gillberg, & Anckarsater, 2005; Larson et al., 2010) cutoff values. DF extremes analyses were applied to investigate the extent to which these definitions of ADHD are etiologically distinct from the subthreshold symptom variation. As this twin cohort also includes sufficient numbers of male, female and opposite-sexed twin pairs, sex differences in genetic or environmental effects could be examined. Methods Subjects Parents of all 21,790 Swedish 9- (born between July 1995 and December 2000) and 12-year-old (born between July 1992 and June 1995) twins were identified through the Swedish Twin Registry and contacted for interviews over the phone as part of the Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (Lichtenstein et al., 2006). The reason for choosing this age group was that most of the major child psychiatric problem constellations have been established by the age of 12, whereas the complex biopsychosocial problems associated with puberty have not yet emerged. Interviewers from a professional company carried out the interviews after a brief introduction in child and adolescent psychiatry and twin research. The study started in July 2004 and is ongoing. Over 80% of the parents of the cohorts born before May 2000 have responded. The mother was interviewed in 88% and the father in 12% of these cases; in 30 cases (0.4%) another member of the family was interviewed. No significant mean differences in child ADHD symptoms were observed across reporters. The total study group consisted of 17,432 consenting and assessed individuals. Of these, 156 had documented brain damage syndromes (most commonly cerebral palsy) and 22 had a known genetic syndrome (most commonly Down s syndrome but also Fragile X). These twins as well as their cotwins were excluded from the analyses. The data collection was approved by the Ethics Committee at Karolinska Institutet and all participants consented to the study. Zygosity determination Zygosity determination for 571 pairs of twins for whom we had DNA from both twins was based on a panel of 48 SNPs derived for zygosity analyses (Hannelius et al., 2007). For the remaining twins, we used an algorithm based on five items concerning twin similarity and confusion (Lichtenstein et al., 2002) derived from the twins with known zygosity. Only twins with more than 95% probability of being correctly classified were assigned a zygosity. Using this algorithm, 734 of 17,100 twins were excluded as zygosity unknown, and the analyses were performed using the resulting 16,366 twins, 2,242 of whom were monozygotic (MZ) male twins, 3,166 dizygotic (DZ) male twins, 2,330 MZ female pairs, 2,790 DZ female twins and 5,838 DZ oppositesexed (DZOS) twins. Measures Child ADHD symptoms were assessed using the Autism Tics, ADHD and other Comorbidities inventory (A-TAC), which is designed for large-scale epidemiological research as an easy-to-administer, dimensional, and comprehensive interview for administration by lay persons over the phone. The instrument includes questions to cover 96 specific child psychiatric symptoms, established based on extensive psychometric analyses of a series of validation studies (Hansson et al., 2005; Larson et al., 2010), and assesses specific

3 doi: /j x ADHD as an extreme of a continuous trait 75 problems corresponding to major clinical diagnostic criteria in child- and adolescent psychiatry, focusing on neuropsychiatric problems such as autism spectrum disorders and ADHD. Questions are worded to assess DSM IV criteria and other well-described clinical features, and should be answered in a life-time perspective and in relation to age peers. Three response categories are used: no (0), yes, to some extent (0.5) and yes (1.0). The two modules used to assess ADHD include 19 questions that were identified in order to achieve the optimal combined sensitivity, specificity and predictive value for clinical diagnoses of ADHD in the validation studies of the instrument with a high internal consistency (alpha reliability for the ADHD scale was.92). The A-TAC ADHD scale can be used to identify categories, as cutoff values yielding optimal sensitivity (for screening) and specificity (for research diagnostics) have been identified based on two validation studies (Hansson et al., 2005; Larson et al., 2010). A score of 6.0 (screening) on the 19 ADHD items identified 9.75% of the children with a screening diagnosis and yielded a sensitivity >.95, whereas a score 12.5 identified 1.78% of the children with a research diagnosis and yielded a specificity > The predictive effect of the score for clinical diagnoses of ADHD was excellent (area under a receiver operating characteristics curve of 0.94 with a 95% confidence interval between 0.92 and 0.96; Larson et al., 2010). Twin method The twin method is a natural experiment that relies on the different levels of genetic relatedness between MZ and DZ twin pairs to estimate the relative contribution of genetic and environmental factors for a phenotype. MZ twins are genetically identical, whereas DZ twins share on average 50% of their segregating alleles. Specifically, we used the twin method to decompose the variance of a phenotype into additive genetic factors (A) reflecting additive effects of different alleles, nonadditive genetic factors (dominance, D) reflecting interaction effects between alleles at the same gene locus, shared environmental factors (C) reflecting nongenetic influences that contribute to similarity within pairs of twins and nonshared environmental factors (E) reflecting experiences that make sibling pairs dissimilar (Plomin, DeFries, McClearn, & McGuffin, 2008). Twin correlations (i.e. within-twin pair maximumlikelihood correlations) were used for an initial examination of the relative contribution of A, D, C and E. Specifically, MZ correlations higher than DZ correlations indicate A, whereas E is indicated by the extent to which MZ correlations are less than unity. DZ correlations higher than half the MZ correlations indicate C, whereas DZ correlations lower than half the MZ correlations suggest D or sibling interaction effects (usually labeled s ). genetic and environmental factors operate differently at varying levels of ADHD cutoffs. DeFries Fulker extremes analysis explores genetic links between the extreme and the subthreshold by bringing together the dichotomized classification of ADHD and the quantitative dimension of ADHD symptoms. Specifically, rather than assessing twin similarity in terms of individual differences on a quantitative trait of ADHD or in terms of concordance for a certain cutoff, DF extremes analysis assesses twin similarity as the extent to which the mean standardized quantitative trait score of the cotwins is as high as the mean standardized score of the selected proband. This measure of twin similarity, called extreme group correlation, is calculated by dividing the quantitative traits scores of the cotwins by the proband mean, specific for each sex and zygosity group. Genetic influences are implied if extreme group correlations are greater for MZ than for DZ twin pairs. The extent to which genetic factors account for the mean differences between probands and the population is called group heritability. Although DF extremes group heritability can be estimated by doubling the differences in MZ and DZ extreme group correlations, DF extremes analysis is more properly conducted using a regression model (DeFries & Fulker, 1988). Briefly, the basic DF model is represented as the regression, C = B 1 P+B 2 R +A, in which the cotwin s score (C) is predicted from probands score (P) and the coefficient of relatedness (R), which is 1.0 for MZ and.5 for DZ twin pairs. Proband means for both MZ and DZ probands are transformed to 1.0 and the population mean is transformed to 0. The regression weight B 2 estimates the group heritability; it compares the MZ and DZ cotwin means taking into account genetic relatedness (R). Group heritability should not be higher than the MZ extreme group correlation, but this can sometimes occur when data show a nonadditive genetic pattern of effects (i.e., when the DZ extreme group correlation is less than half the MZ extreme group correlation). Under these circumstances B 2 (group heritability) were constrained to take the value of the MZ twin group correlation. Finding significant group heritability implies that both extreme and the subthreshold range of ADHD symptoms are heritable and that there are genetic links between ADHD and the subthreshold variation. That is, group heritability itself, not the comparison between the separate group heritability estimates obtained from using different definitions for the identification of ADHD probands (i.e. diagnostic and screening criteria of ADHD), indicates genetic links between extreme and the subthreshold range of ADHD symptoms (for a more detailed description; Plomin & Kovas, 2005). Thus, applying DF extremes analyses on two different definitions of ADHD should be viewed as an attempt to explore whether our findings are robust across different proband definitions. Extremes analysis Twin pairs where at least one member of the pair was exhibiting an extreme ADHD score were selected for the extremes analysis. All analyses were conducted using the two validated ADHD cutoffs (i.e. screening and research diagnosis) to evaluate the extent to which Analysis of individual differences of the full range of the distribution n the model-fitting analyses of the quantitative measure of ADHD symptoms for the whole sample, we used a structural-equation modeling program, called Mx (Neale, Boker, Xie, & Maes, 2003). We used the method

4 76 Henrik Larsson et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012; 53(1): of raw maximum-likelihood estimation. This method allows the inclusion of singletons, where information from only one twin in a pair is available, which increases power in the analyses. The following combinations of variance components were considered in the univariate model-fitting analyses: ACE, ADE, AE, ADEs and AEs (s, sibling interaction term). When modeling parent report data, a negative sibling interaction term often occurs because there is a contrast effect in the parental ratings of their behavior (i.e. parents inadvertently exaggerate behavioral differences between the children). The effect of C and D in the classical twin design is confounded because the effects of C decreases the difference in MZ and DZ twin similarity, whereas the effect of D increases differences in twin similarity. Thus, C and D cannot be estimated simultaneously. We also fitted a series of sex-limitation models to test for qualitative sex differences, quantitative sex differences and phenotypic variance differences between the sexes. Specifically, using data from same-sex twin pairs, the quantitative sex-differences model examines whether the magnitudes of genetic and environmental influences on a phenotype are different in males and females. As our sample also consists of DZOS twins, additional sex-specific parameters could be included to examine whether different genetic components influence the phenotype in one sex but not the other (qualitative sex-differences model; Neale et al. 2006). Potential phenotypic variance differences between the sexes can be explored using a scalar model. Such a model allows the phenotypic variances to differ between males and females, whereas the genetic and environmental parameter estimates are equated across sexes. Finally, in the constraint model, all variance components were set to be equal between the sexes. Goodness of fit for the different twin models was assessed by the Akaike s information criterion. Specifically, AIC was computed as v 2 2 df where v 2 is the difference in )2 log likelihood ()2LL) between the saturated and restricted model and df denotes the difference in degrees of freedom between the two models. A lower AIC value indicates better fit of the model to the observed data. It should be noted that the likelihood-ratio chi-square test revealed identical results (data not shown). Results Mean ADHD scores in the total sample were 1.76 (SD = 2.81; Table 1). Applying the two cutoff values, 9.75% and 1.78% of the total sample were identified as screen-positive for ADHD and meeting a research diagnosis of ADHD, respectively. The 1,590 twins who scored above the screening cutoff had an average ADHD score of 8.72 (SD = 3.03). The corresponding mean score for the 290 twins scoring above the diagnostic threshold was (SD = 1.87). The sex distribution was balanced in the total sample (51% males). As expected, more males than females had a score above the screening (68%) and diagnostic (72%) cutoffs for ADHD. Extremes analysis Extreme group correlations suggest genetic influences because MZ similarity exceeds DZ similarity (Table 2). All MZ extreme group correlations were < 1, suggesting nonshared environmental influences. Extreme group correlations were similar for males and females, and also similar for same-sexed DZ and DZOS twins, which suggest no quantitative or qualitative genetic and environmental sex differences. Finally, extreme group correlations were similar for the screening and the diagnostic cutoffs. As expected from extreme group correlations, DF extremes analysis provided no evidence for sex differences. That is, the 95% confidence intervals around the estimates of the group heritability and group nonshared environment overlapped (see Table S1). The results suggest a strong genetic link between the extreme and the subthreshold variation, with almost identical group heritability estimates across the screening (.60) and the diagnostic (.62) criteria of ADHD (Table 2). In a follow-up analysis, we applied the DF model to the two symptom dimensions of DSM IV ADHD (i.e. inattention and hyperactivity impulsivity). The result indicate that the group heritability for inattention (twins with 6 DSM IV inattention symptoms were selected as probands) and hyperactivity impulsivity (twins with 6 DSM IV hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms were selected as probands) was similar in magnitude; estimated as.53 (.44.62) and.62 (.53.71), respectively. Analysis of individual differences in the full range of the ADHD distribution In the total sample, the within-twin pair correlations of the ADHD scores were similar to the observed extreme group correlations and suggest high heritability, and modest nonshared environmental for both males and females (Table 2). Table 1 Descriptive statistics for total sample (16,298 individuals) and ADHD probands Group No. probands (%) % Males ADHD mean score (SD) Total sample (2.81) A-TAC diagnosis of ADHD Broad screening criteria (raw ADHD score 6.0) 1,590 (9.75) (3.03) Strict diagnostic criteria (raw ADHD score 12.5) 290 (1.78) (1.87) A-TAC, Autism Tics, ADHD and other Comorbidities; ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.

5 doi: /j x ADHD as an extreme of a continuous trait 77 Table 2 Results of extremes analyses of ADHD symptoms and of analysis of individual differences of the full range of the distribution Extremes analyses of ADHD symptoms Extreme group correlations (No. probands) DeFries Fulker group estimates (95% CI) MZM DZM MZF DZF DZOS Heritability Nonshared environment Broad screening criteria.62 (248).15 (421).55 (136).14 (220).12 (555).60 (.54.66).40 (.34.46) Strict diagnostic criteria.65 (57).14 (80).53 (15).12 (36).09 (100).62 (.52.72).38 (.28.48) Analysis of individual differences of the full range of the distribution Intraclass correlations (95% CI) Model-fitting estimates (95% CI) MZM DZM MZF DZF DZOS Heritability Nonshared environment A-TAC ADHD symptom score.69 (.66.72).24 (.19.29).65 (.61.68).28 (.27.33).30 (.26.33).74 (.71.76).26 (.24.29) A-TAC, Autism Tics, ADHD and other Comorbidities; ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; MZM, monozygotic male twins; DZM, dizygotic male twins; MZF, monozygotic female twins; DZF, dizygotic female twins; DZOS, dizygotic opposite-sexed twins. Model-fitting analyses shows that the scalar AEs model had the lowest AIC value (Table 3). That is, a model that constrains the genetic and environmental parameter estimates to be equal across sex (but allows for variance differences), excludes variance due to the dominant genetic factor (D), and allows for contrast effects (s, sibling interaction term) provided the most parsimonious fit of the data. This best-fitting model (Table 2) estimated the heritability and nonshared environment contribution as.74 and.26, respectively. Discussion We found a strong genetic link between the extreme and the subthreshold variation of DSM IV based assessments of ADHD symptoms, corroborating a previous smaller DSM III R-based study (Levy et al., 1997) and extending the previous results to include also very narrow definitions of ADHD with a high probability of correspondence to real-life clinical problem presentations. Our data suggest that ADHD is best viewed as the quantitative extreme of genetic and environmental factors operating dimensionally Table 3 Model-fitting results of univariate analysis of ADHD symptoms Fit of model compared to saturated model Models )2LL df v 2 Ddf AIC Saturated model 8, ,273 ADEs Qualitative sex differences 8, , Quantitative sex differences 8, , Scalar model 8, , Constraint model 8, , ADE Qualitative sex differences 8, , Quantitative sex differences 8, , Scalar model 8, , Constraint model 8, , AEs Qualitative sex differences 8, , Quantitative sex differences 8, , Scalar model 8, , Constraint model 8, , AE Qualitative sex differences 8, , Quantitative sex differences 8, , Scalar model 8, , Constraint model 8, , Best-fitting model indicated in bold. )2LL, likelihood fit statistic; df, degrees of freedom; v 2, the difference in )2LL between the saturated and restricted model; Ddf, difference in df between the saturated and restricted model; AIC, Akaike s Information Criterion; s, sibling interaction term; AE, model excluding variance explained by genetic dominance effects

6 78 Henrik Larsson et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012; 53(1): throughout the distribution of ADHD symptoms, indicating that the same etiologic factors are involved in the full range of symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity. The results from our DF extremes analysis are important because it is the first to be based on DSM IV assessments of ADHD symptoms. The use of a large twin data set also allowed us to explore, in contrast to previous studies (Levy et al., 1997), genetic links between the extreme and the subthreshold range of ADHD using clinically relevant threshold definitions. Finding similar estimates for the group heritability of both the diagnostic and screening criteria of ADHD and also the two DSM IV symptom dimensions of ADHD highlight the robust nature of our results. Our individual differences results suggest high heritability, no shared environment and modest nonshared environmental effects; a finding that is largely consistent with prior twin study results using both categorical and continuous definitions of ADHD (Burt, 2009; Faraone et al., 2005; Thapar et al., 1999) although it should be noted that a small but significant shared environmental contribution has emerged in other studies (Wood, Buitelaar, Rijsdijk, Asherson, & Kuntsi, 2010). In line with previously reported twin study results (Levy et al., 1997), our data support the view that ADHD is the quantitative extreme of the genetic and environmental influences that operate across the distribution of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms. As several different research methods with different weaknesses in validity have reached the same conclusion (Faraone et al., 2006, 2009; Frazier et al., 2007; Haslam et al., 2006; Lubke et al., 2009; Rapoport et al., 1980; Scahill et al., 1999), there is now strong evidence supporting the notion of ADHD as an extreme end of a continuous trait rather than as a disorder category. This continuous, higher order trait may consist of normally distributed variations in cognitive abilities (e.g. executive functions; Nigg, Willcutt, Doyle, & Sonuga- Barke, 2005) and extroverted personality traits (e.g. novelty seeking; Anckarsater et al., 2006), giving rise to deficient adaptive strategies and ensuing mental health problems in extreme ends of the distribution. One implication of these findings is that it may not be fruitful for etiologic models of ADHD to only propose causes that are specific to rare diagnosed cases. Rather, it is probably sufficient to also focus on the factors involved in the transition from milder to more severe levels of ADHD symptoms. In addition, if ADHD is the quantitative extreme of genetic and environmental factors operating dimensionally throughout the distribution, gene identification efforts may benefit from examining the dimensional distribution of symptoms that underlie ADHD within the normal population. The high heritability suggests that quantitative ratings of the ADHD symptoms described in DSM IV could be used as dimensional measures in genetic association studies. Finding genetic links between the extreme and the subthreshold range of ADHD symptoms is also in line with the quantitative trait locus model of molecular genetics (Plomin, Haworth, & Davis, 2009), which predicts that many genes, each with a small effect size, influence the whole phenotypic variations, that is, the genetic effects affecting ADHD may also give rise to subthreshold ADHD and vice versa. Such effects could include a not only large number functional polymorphisms, but also larger structural changes, including so called copy number variants (Franke, Neale, & Faraone, 2009; Poelmans, Pauls, Buitelaar, & Franke, 2011; Williams et al., 2010). Sex effects are difficult to study because their reliable detection requires large samples and a narrow participant age range, which might explain why prior studies have produced mixed results, with evidence both for (Rhee, Waldman, Hay, & Levy, 1999) and against (Hudziak, Derks, Althoff, Rettew, & Boomsma, 2005) sex differences underlying the etiology of ADHD. We found no evidence for sex differences in heritability across the continuum; a result that is consistent with prior twin studies exploring the extreme end of the ADHD distribution (Levy et al., 1997; Thapar et al., 2000). Our results are also consistent with findings from a prior clinical-based study reporting strong associations between psychosocial adversity and ADHD independently of gender (Biederman, Faraone, & Monuteaux, 2002). Thus, genetic and environmental risk factors appear to act, independently of gender, on ADHD symptoms scores across the range of severity, which indicate that the dimensional model of ADHD seems appropriate for both males and females. In this very large twin study of childhood ADHD symptoms, we screened 10,895 twin pairs, that is all twins born in Sweden between 1992 and 2000, with a very high response rate (> 80%). An advantage of our design is that we were able to study the etiology at the very extreme end of the distribution and that age/ period effects were controlled for. This is a noteworthy strength as prior twin studies have studied the etiology of less extreme levels of ADHD using samples with wide age-ranges. Nevertheless, the results should be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, as this epidemiological study included over 17,000 children, complete clinical evaluations were beyond the feasible, and, therefore we had to rely on parental report. Although the items used here closely mirrored the DSM IV criteria, parental interviews are not equivalent to a psychiatrist s diagnosis. However, we used an instrument that has shown good psychometric properties and thorough validation data are available (Hansson et al., 2005; Larson et al., 2010). Second, twins may not be representative of the general population in terms of mental health problems. Some studies have reported that twins might have an increased risk for ADHD compared to singletons

7 doi: /j x ADHD as an extreme of a continuous trait 79 (Levy, Hay, McLaughlin, Wood, & Waldman, 1996), whereas other studies have found little evidence for such differences (Gjone & Novik, 1995; Robbers et al., 2010; Simonoff et al., 1997). Regardless, this unlikely affects the interpretation of the genetic results. Also, most empirical studies that have examined general psychiatric outcomes have found no or very small differences between twins and singletons (Evans & Martin, 2000). Third, recall bias might have affected the results. However, previous studies on the same sample have shown very few significant differences between the 9- and 12-year-old children, supporting the view that the A-TAC generally succeeds in assessing symptoms and problems in a lifetime perspective (Lichtenstein et al., 2010). Conclusions We found a strong genetic link between the extreme and the subthreshold range variation of DSM IVbased assessments of ADHD symptoms, suggesting that ADHD is best viewed as the quantitative extreme of genetic and environmental factors operating dimensionally throughout the full range of variation in inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity. Thus, the data provide evidence for a need to identify the factors involved in the transition from milder to more severe levels of ADHD symptoms. Supporting information Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article: Table S1 Estimates of heritability and nonshared environment along with 95% confidence intervals from DF extremes analysis, by sex. Please note: Wiley-Blackwell are not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting materials supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing material) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article. Acknowledgements This study was supported in part by the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research and the Swedish Research Council. Henrik Larsson was supported by grants from the Swedish Research Council ( ), Swedish brain foundation and Karolinska Institutet center of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. H.L. had full access to all the data and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Correspondence to Henrik Larsson, Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, P.O. Box 281, SE Stockholm, Sweden; Tel: ; henrik.larsson@ki.se Key points The question of whether the etiology is the same for different levels of DSM IV ADHD symptoms remains to be investigated Extremes analyses indicated a strong genetic link between the extreme and the subthreshold variation, with almost identical group heritability estimates around.60 for the diagnostic and screening criteria of ADHD. Our data suggest that ADHD is best viewed as the quantitative extreme of genetic and environmental factors operating dimensionally throughout the distribution of ADHD symptoms The data provide evidence for a need to identify the factors involved in the transition from milder to more severe levels of ADHD symptoms. References Anckarsater, H., Stahlberg, O., Larson, T., Hakansson, C., Jutblad, S.B., Niklasson, L., & Rastam, M. (2006). The impact of ADHD and autism spectrum disorders on temperament, character, and personality development. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, Biederman, J. (2005). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A selective overview. Biological Psychiatry, 57, Biederman, J., Faraone, S.V., & Monuteaux, M.C. (2002). Differential effect of environmental adversity by gender: Rutter s index of adversity in a group of boys and girls with and without ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, Burt, S.A. (2009). Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: A meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, DeFries, J.C., & Fulker, D.W. (1988). Multiple regression analysis of twin data: Etiology of deviant scores versus individual differences. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae (Roma), 37, Evans, D.M., & Martin, N.G. (2000). The validity of twin studies. Gene Screen, 1, Faraone, S.V., Biederman, J., Doyle, A., Murray, K., Petty, C., Adamson, J.J., & Seidman, L. (2006). Neuropsychological studies of late onset and subthreshold diagnoses of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 60, Faraone, S.V., Kunwar, A., Adamson, J., & Biederman, J. (2009). Personality traits among ADHD adults: Implications of late-onset and subthreshold diagnoses. Psychological Medicine, 39, Faraone, S.V., Perlis, R.H., Doyle, A.E., Smoller, J.W., Goralnick, J.J., Holmgren, M.A., & Aklar, P. (2005). Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57, Franke, B., Neale, B.M., & Faraone, S.V. (2009). Genomewide association studies in ADHD. Human Genetics, 126,

8 80 Henrik Larsson et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012; 53(1): Frazier, T.W., Youngstrom, E.A., & Naugle, R.I. (2007). The latent structure of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinic-referred sample. Neuropsychology, 21, Gjone, H., & Novik, T.S. (1995). Parental ratings of behaviour problems: A twin and general population comparison. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, Goldman, L.S., Genel, M., Bezman, R.J., & Slanetz, P.J. (1998). Diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Council on Scientific Affairs, American Medical Association. JAMA, 279, Goodman, R., & Stevenson, J. (1989). A twin study of hyperactivity II. The aetiological role of genes, family relationships and perinatal adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, Hannelius, U., Gherman, L., Makela, V.V., Lindstedt, A., Zucchelli, M., Lagerberg, C., & Lindgren, C.M. (2007). Largescale zygosity testing using single nucleotide polymorphisms. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, Hansson, S.L., Svanstrom Rojvall, A., Rastam, M., Gillberg, C., & Anckarsater, H. (2005). Psychiatric telephone interview with parents for screening of childhood autism Tics, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and other comorbidities (A-TAC): Preliminary reliability and validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 187, Haslam, N., Williams, B., Prior, M., Haslam, R., Graetz, B., & Sawyer, M. (2006). The latent structure of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: A taxometric analysis. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40, Hudziak, J.J., Derks, E.M., Althoff, R.R., Rettew, D.C., & Boomsma, D.I. (2005). The genetic and environmental contributions to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder as measured by the Conners Rating Scales Revised. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, Larson, T., Anckarsater, H., Gillberg, C., Stahlberg, O., Carlstrom, E., Kadesjo, B., & Gillberg, C. (2010). The autism Tics, AD/HD and other comorbidities inventory (A-TAC): Further validation of a telephone interview for epidemiological research. BMC Psychiatry, 10, 1. Levy, F., Hay, D., McLaughlin, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1996). Twin sibling differences in parental reports of ADHD, speech, reading and behaviour problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, Levy, F., Hay, D.A., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, Lichtenstein, P., Carlstrom, E., Rastam, M., Gillberg, C., & Anckarsater, H. (2010). The genetics of autism spectrum disorders and related neuropsychiatric disorders in childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167 (11), Lichtenstein, P., De Faire, U., Floderus, B., Svartengren, M., Svedberg, P., & Pedersen, N.L. (2002). The Swedish Twin Registry: A unique resource for clinical, epidemiological and genetic studies. Journal of Internal Medicine, 252, Lichtenstein, P., Sullivan, P.F., Cnattingius, S., Gatz, M., Johansson, S., Carlstrom, E., & Pedersen, N.L. (2006). The Swedish Twin Registry in the third millennium: An update. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, Lubke, G.H., Hudziak, J.J., Derks, E.M., van Bijsterveldt, T.C., & Boomsma, D.I. (2009). Maternal ratings of attention problems in ADHD: Evidence for the existence of a continuum. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, Neale, M.C., Boker, S.M., Xie, G., & Maes, H.H. (2003). Mx statistical modeling (6th edn). Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry. Neale, M.C., Roysamb, E., & Jacobson, K. (2006). Multivariate genetic analysis of sex limitation and G x E interaction. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, Nigg, J.T., Willcutt, E.G., Doyle, A.E., & Sonuga-Barke, E.J. (2005). Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry, 57, Plomin, R., DeFries, J.C., McClearn, G.E., & McGuffin, P. (2008). Behavioral genetics (4th edn). New York: Worth. Plomin, R., Haworth, C.M., & Davis, O.S. (2009). Common disorders are quantitative traits. Nature Reviews Genetics, 10, Plomin, R., & Kovas, Y. (2005). Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psychological Bulletin, 131, Poelmans, G., Pauls, D.L., Buitelaar, J.K., & Franke, B. (2011). Integrated genome-wide association study findings: Identification of a neurodevelopmental network for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, Rapoport, J.L., Buchsbaum, M.S., Weingartner, H., Zahn, T.P., Ludlow, C., & Mikkelsen, E.J. (1980). Dextroamphetamine. Its cognitive and behavioral effects in normal and hyperactive boys and normal men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, Rhee, S.H., Waldman, I.D., Hay, D.A., & Levy, F. (1999). Sex differences in genetic and environmental influences on DSM-III-R attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, Robbers, S.C., Bartels, M., van Oort, F.V., van Beijsterveldt, C.E., van der Ende, J., Verhulst, F.C., & Huizink, A.C. (2010). A twin-singleton comparison of developmental trajectories of externalizing and internalizing problems in 6- to 12-year-old children. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 13, Rohde, L.A. (2008). Is there a need to reformulate attention deficit hyperactivity disorder criteria in future nosologic classifications? Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clincs of North America, 17, Scahill, L., Schwab-Stone, M., Merikangas, K.R., Leckman, J.F., Zhang, H., & Kasl, S. (1999). Psychosocial and clinical correlates of ADHD in a community sample of school-age children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, Sherman, D.K., McGue, M.K., & Iacono, W.G. (1997). Twin concordance for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A comparison of teachers and mothers reports. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Meyer, J.M., Silberg, J.L., Maes, H.H., Loeber, R., & Eaves, L.J. (1997). The Virginia twin study of adolescent behavioral development. Influences of age, sex, and impairment on rates of disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, Thapar, A., Harrington, R., Ross, K., & McGuffin, P. (2000). Does the definition of ADHD affect heritability? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, Thapar, A., Holmes, J., Poulton, K., & Harrington, R. (1999). Genetic basis of attention deficit and hyperactivity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, Thapar, A., & Lewis, G. (2009). Should we be rethinking how we assess and manage ADHD? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, Williams, N.M., Zaharieva, I., Martin, A., Langley, K., Mantripragada, K., & Thapar, A. (2010). Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A genome-wide analysis. Lancet, 376(9750), Wood, A.C., Buitelaar, J., Rijsdijk, F., Asherson, P., & Kuntsi, J. (2010). Rethinking shared environment as a source of variance underlying attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms: Comment on Burt (2009). Psychological Bulletin, 376 (9750), Accepted for publication: 29 July 2011 Published online: 16 September 2011

Evidence for shared genetic influences on selfreported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian twins

Evidence for shared genetic influences on selfreported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian twins Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Open Access Publications 2008 Evidence for shared genetic influences on selfreported ADHD and autistic symptoms in young adult Australian

More information

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder DOI 10.1007/s10519-014-9670-x ORIGINAL RESEARCH Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder J. L. Ebejer S. E. Medland J.

More information

Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study

Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study Genetic Influences on Childhood Competencies: A Twin Study JAMES J. HUDZIAK, M.D., WILLIAM COPELAND, B.A., LAWRENCE P. RUDIGER, PH.D., THOMAS M. ACHENBACH, PH.D., ANDREW C. HEATH, D.PHIL., AND RICHARD

More information

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating Cynthia M. Bulik,* Patrick F. Sullivan, and Kenneth S. Kendler Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics of Virginia

More information

The Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Oppositional Defiant Behavior: A Multi-informant Twin Study

The Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Oppositional Defiant Behavior: A Multi-informant Twin Study The Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Oppositional Defiant Behavior: A Multi-informant Twin Study JAMES J. HUDZIAK, M.D., ESKE M. DERKS, M.A., ROBERT R. ALTHOFF, M.D., PH.D., WILLIAM COPELAND,

More information

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study Diabetologia (2013) 56:1031 1035 DOI 10.1007/s00125-013-2859-3 SHORT COMMUNICATION Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study S. Carlsson & A. Ahlbom & P. Lichtenstein

More information

Continuity and Change in Preschool ADHD Symptoms: Longitudinal Genetic Analysis with Contrast Effects

Continuity and Change in Preschool ADHD Symptoms: Longitudinal Genetic Analysis with Contrast Effects Behavior Genetics, Vol. 35, No. 2, March 2005 (Ó 2005) Continuity and Change in Preschool ADHD Symptoms: Longitudinal Genetic Analysis with Contrast Effects Thomas S. Price, 1,4 Emily Simonoff, 2 Philip

More information

Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale

Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48:11 (27), pp 18 187 doi:1.1111/j.1469-761.27.1783.x Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale Tinca J.C. Polderman,

More information

Association between childhood asthma and ADHD symptoms in adolescence--a prospective populationbased twin study. Allergy. 2011;66(9)

Association between childhood asthma and ADHD symptoms in adolescence--a prospective populationbased twin study. Allergy. 2011;66(9) This is an author produced version of a paper accepted by Allergy. This paper has been peer-reviewed but does not include the final publisher proof-corrections or journal pagination. Association between

More information

Genetics of ADHD, Hyperactivity, and Attention Problems

Genetics of ADHD, Hyperactivity, and Attention Problems Chapter 25 Genetics of ADHD, Hyperactivity, and Attention Problems Eske M. Derks, James J. Hudziak, and Dorret I. Boomsma Introduction Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is characterized by

More information

Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease

Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease (2007) 15, 872 877 & 2007 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 1018-4813/07 $30.00 ARTICLE www.nature.com/ejhg Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease Slobodan

More information

Seventy years since Kanner s account, what do we think causes autism in 2013?

Seventy years since Kanner s account, what do we think causes autism in 2013? www.gel.bbk.ac.uk @gelironald Seventy years since Kanner s account, what do we think causes autism in 2013? 24 th September 2013 Karolinska Institutet Dr Angelica Ronald Genes Environment Lifespan laboratory,

More information

A twin study of autism symptoms in Sweden

A twin study of autism symptoms in Sweden A twin study of autism symptoms in Sweden Angelica Ronald, Henrik Larsson, Henrik Anckarsäter, Paul Lichtenstein To cite this version: Angelica Ronald, Henrik Larsson, Henrik Anckarsäter, Paul Lichtenstein.

More information

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins Behavior Genetics, Vol. 33, No. 5, September 2003 ( 2003) Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt,

More information

Studies have shown that familial aggregation is of

Studies have shown that familial aggregation is of No Evidence of Sex Differences in Heritability of Irritable Bowel Syndrome in Swedish Twins Pia Svedberg, 1 Saga Johansson, 3,4 Mari-Ann Wallander, 3,6 and Nancy L. Pedersen 2,5 1 Section of Personal Injury

More information

What can genetic studies tell us about ADHD? Dr Joanna Martin, Cardiff University

What can genetic studies tell us about ADHD? Dr Joanna Martin, Cardiff University What can genetic studies tell us about ADHD? Dr Joanna Martin, Cardiff University Outline of talk What do we know about causes of ADHD? Traditional family studies Modern molecular genetic studies How can

More information

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H PSYC H O PAT H O LO G Y : PSYC H I AT R I C P R O B LEMS A N D T H E A S SO C I

More information

Etiology of Inattention and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity in a Community Sample of Twins with Learning Difficulties

Etiology of Inattention and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity in a Community Sample of Twins with Learning Difficulties Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Vol. 28, No. 2, 2000, pp. 149 159 Etiology of Inattention and Hyperactivity/Impulsivity in a Community Sample of Twins with Learning Difficulties Erik G. Willcutt,

More information

Review Article Sex Differences in Genetic and Environmental Influences on Adolescent Depressive Symptoms: A Meta-Analytic Review

Review Article Sex Differences in Genetic and Environmental Influences on Adolescent Depressive Symptoms: A Meta-Analytic Review Depression Research and Treatment Volume 2015, Article ID 476238, 7 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/476238 Review Article Sex Differences in Genetic and Environmental Influences on Adolescent Depressive

More information

Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to12

Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to12 Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45:3 (2004), pp 577 588 Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to12 M.J.H. Rietveld, 1 J.J. Hudziak,

More information

Examining and interpreting the female protective effect against autistic behavior

Examining and interpreting the female protective effect against autistic behavior Examining and interpreting the female protective effect against autistic behavior Elise B. Robinson a,b,1, Paul Lichtenstein c, Henrik Anckarsäter d, Francesca Happé e, and Angelica Ronald e,f a Analytic

More information

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children J. Intell. 2015, 3, 160-167; doi:10.3390/jintelligence3040160 Brief Report OPEN ACCESS Journal of Intelligence ISSN 2079-3200 www.mdpi.com/journal/jintelligence Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations

More information

Genetic Heterogeneity Between the Three Components of the Autism Spectrum: A Twin Study

Genetic Heterogeneity Between the Three Components of the Autism Spectrum: A Twin Study Genetic Heterogeneity Between the Three Components of the Autism Spectrum: A Twin Study ANGELICA RONALD, PH.D., FRANCESCA HAPP1, PH.D., PATRICK BOLTON, F.R.C.PSYCH., LEE M. BUTCHER, M.SC., THOMAS S. PRICE,

More information

The Genetic Basis of Problem Behavior in 5-Year-Old Dutch Twin Pairs

The Genetic Basis of Problem Behavior in 5-Year-Old Dutch Twin Pairs Behavior Genetics, Vol. 34, No. 3, May 2004 ( 2004) The Genetic Basis of Problem Behavior in 5-Year-Old Dutch Twin Pairs C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt, 1,4 F. C. Verhulst, 2 P. C. M. Molenaar, 3 and D. I.

More information

Overview. Definitions for this talk. Comorbidity is the rule, not the exception

Overview. Definitions for this talk. Comorbidity is the rule, not the exception Percent of cases Understanding the whirling ball of comorbidity : Disability, Disability, and Erik Willcutt, Ph.D. Professor of Psychology and Neuroscience University of Colorado, Boulder Overview What

More information

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Risk factors for the development and outcome of childhood psychopathology SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Chapter 147 In this chapter I present a summary of the results of the studies described in this thesis followed

More information

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H

More information

Developmental trajectories of DSM-IV symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: genetic effects, family risk and associated psychopathology

Developmental trajectories of DSM-IV symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: genetic effects, family risk and associated psychopathology Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52:9 (2011), pp 954 963 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02379.x Developmental trajectories of DSM-IV symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: genetic

More information

Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions

Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions Psychological Medicine, 2000, 30, 1315 1325. 2000 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions

More information

Heritability of Anxious-Depressive and Withdrawn Behavior: Age-Related Changes During Adolescence

Heritability of Anxious-Depressive and Withdrawn Behavior: Age-Related Changes During Adolescence NEW RESEARCH Heritability of Anxious-Depressive and Withdrawn Behavior: Age-Related Changes During Adolescence Diane J. Lamb, M.Sc., Christel M. Middeldorp, M.D., Ph.D., Catarina E.M. van Beijsterveldt,

More information

Generalist Genes and Learning Disabilities

Generalist Genes and Learning Disabilities Psychological Bulletin Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 131, No. 4, 592 617 0033-2909/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.592 Generalist Genes and Learning Disabilities

More information

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement Psychological Methods 2007, Vol. 12, No. 4, 451 466 CORRECTED APRIL 10, 2008; SEE LAST PAGE Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 1082-989X/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.4.451

More information

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Self-Report Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms in Dutch Adolescents at Ages 12, 14, and 16

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Self-Report Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms in Dutch Adolescents at Ages 12, 14, and 16 Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Self-Report Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms in Dutch Adolescents at Ages 12, 14, and 16 DANIËL S. VAN GROOTHEEST, M.D., MEIKE BARTELS, PH.D., CATARINA E.M. VAN

More information

Where are the strongest associations between autistic traits and traits of ADHD? evidence from a community based twin study

Where are the strongest associations between autistic traits and traits of ADHD? evidence from a community based twin study DOI 10.1007/s00787-014-0666-0 ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION Where are the strongest associations between autistic traits and traits of ADHD? evidence from a community based twin study Mark J. Taylor Tony Charman

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Cortex. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 November 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Cortex. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 November 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Cortex. 2010 ; 46(10): 1345 1361. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.009. Etiology And Neuropsychology Of Comorbidity Between RD And ADHD:

More information

Kuja-Halkola, Ralf; Lichtenstein, Paul; D'Onofrio, Brian; Larsson, Henrik

Kuja-Halkola, Ralf; Lichtenstein, Paul; D'Onofrio, Brian; Larsson, Henrik This is an author produced version of a paper published by Journal of child psychology and psychiatry. This paper has been peer-reviewed but does not include the final publisher proof-corrections or journal

More information

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance Twin Research (1999) 2, 99 107 1999 Stockton Press All rights reserved 1369 0523/99 $12.00 http://www.stockton-press.co.uk/tr Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of

More information

The Swedish Twin study of CHild and Adolescent Development. the TCHAD-study

The Swedish Twin study of CHild and Adolescent Development. the TCHAD-study 2007-04-27 1 The Swedish Twin study of CHild and Adolescent Development the TCHAD-study Paul Lichtenstein, Catherine Tuvblad, Henrik Larsson, Eva Carlström Author s affiliations: Department of Medical

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. The Heritability of Bipolar Affective Disorder and the Genetic Relationship to Unipolar Depression

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. The Heritability of Bipolar Affective Disorder and the Genetic Relationship to Unipolar Depression ORIGINAL ARTICLE The Heritability of Bipolar Affective Disorder and the Genetic Relationship to Unipolar Depression Peter McGuffin, MB, PhD, FRCP, FRCPsych; Fruhling Rijsdijk, PhD; Martin Andrew, MB, MRCPsych;

More information

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION Chapter 8 GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION A Longitudinal Study in 3- to 12-Year-Old Children Dorret I. Boomsma, Ph.D. C.E.M. van Beijsterveldt, Ph.D. Meike Bartels, Ph.D. James

More information

Obsessive compulsive disorder, tics and anxiety in 6-year-old twins

Obsessive compulsive disorder, tics and anxiety in 6-year-old twins Psychological Medicine, Page 1 of 10. f 2006 Cambridge University Press doi:10.1017/s0033291706008816 Printed in the United Kingdom Obsessive compulsive disorder, tics and anxiety in 6-year-old twins DEREK

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Relation Between Attention Problems and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Relation Between Attention Problems and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Behav Genet (2008) 38:11 23 DOI 10.1007/s10519-007-9178-8 ORIGINAL RESEARCH Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Relation Between Attention Problems and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

More information

Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): A Longitudinal Multiple Informant Study of Problem Behavior

Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): A Longitudinal Multiple Informant Study of Problem Behavior Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): A Longitudinal Multiple Informant Study of Problem Behavior Meike Bartels, 1 C. E. M. (Toos) van Beijsterveldt, 1 Eske M. Derks, 1 Therese M. Stroet, 1 Tinca J.

More information

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability Eric Turkheimer and Erin E. Horn In 3, our lab published a paper demonstrating that the heritability

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Scienc es 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 V Congress of Russian Psychological Society Genetic and Environmental Influences on

More information

King s Research Portal

King s Research Portal King s Research Portal DOI: 10.1007/s10802-013-9771-7 Document Version Peer reviewed version Link to publication record in King's Research Portal Citation for published version (APA): Kuntsi, J., Pinto,

More information

Discussion. were best fit by models of determination that did not include genetic effects.

Discussion. were best fit by models of determination that did not include genetic effects. A Behavior Genetic Investigation of the Relationship Between Leadership and Personality Andrew M. Johnson 1, Philip A. Vernon 2, Julie Aitken Harris 3, and Kerry L. Jang 4 1 Faculty of Health Sciences,The

More information

Genetic influences on disordered eating behaviour are largely independent of body mass index

Genetic influences on disordered eating behaviour are largely independent of body mass index Acta Psychiatr Scand 2008: 117: 348 356 All rights reserved DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01132.x Copyright Ó 2007 The Authors Journal Compilation Ó 2007 Blackwell Munksgaard ACTA PSYCHIATRICA SCANDINAVICA

More information

Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis

Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis Separation Anxiety Disorder and Adult Onset Panic Attacks Share a Common Genetic Diathesis Roxann Roberson-Nay, PhD Virginia Commonwealth University Department of Psychiatry The SAD PD link Don Klein proposed

More information

Learning Abilities and Disabilities

Learning Abilities and Disabilities CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Learning Abilities and Disabilities Generalist Genes, Specialist Environments Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry,

More information

Birth Order and Sibling Gender Ratio of a Clinical Sample of Children and Adolescents Diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Birth Order and Sibling Gender Ratio of a Clinical Sample of Children and Adolescents Diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Birth Order and Sibling Gender Ratio of a Clinical Sample Original Article Birth Order and Sibling Gender Ratio of a Clinical Sample of Children and Adolescents Diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity

More information

Language and mathematical problems as precursors of psychotic-like experiences and juvenile mania symptoms

Language and mathematical problems as precursors of psychotic-like experiences and juvenile mania symptoms Psychological Medicine, Page 1 of 10. Cambridge University Press 2013 doi:10.1017/s0033291713002018 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Language and mathematical problems as precursors of psychotic-like experiences and juvenile

More information

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORders

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORders ORIGINAL ARTICLE Autism Spectrum Disorders and Autisticlike Traits Similar Etiology in the Extreme End and the Normal Variation Sebastian Lundström, PhD; Zheng Chang, MSc; Maria Råstam, MD, PhD; Christopher

More information

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail.

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. Author(s): Salmela-Aro, Katariina; Read, Sanna; Vuoksimaa, Eero; Korhonen,

More information

Longitudinal Stability of Genetic and Environmental Influences on Irritability: From Childhood to Young Adulthood.

Longitudinal Stability of Genetic and Environmental Influences on Irritability: From Childhood to Young Adulthood. This is an author produced version of a paper accepted by American Journal of Psychiatry. This paper has been peerreviewed but does not include the final publisher proofcorrections or journal pagination.

More information

Testing Hypotheses Regarding the Causes of Comorbidity: Examining the Underlying Deficits of Comorbid Disorders

Testing Hypotheses Regarding the Causes of Comorbidity: Examining the Underlying Deficits of Comorbid Disorders Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 114, No. 3, 346 362 0021-843X/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.346 Testing Hypotheses Regarding

More information

Autistic-like traits and their association with mental health problems in two nationwide twin cohorts of children and adults

Autistic-like traits and their association with mental health problems in two nationwide twin cohorts of children and adults Psychological Medicine (2011), 41, 2423 2433. f Cambridge University Press 2011 doi:10.1017/s0033291711000377 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Autistic-like traits and their association with mental health problems in

More information

Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood

Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood CE ACTIVITY Bivariate Analysis of Disordered Eating Characteristics in Adolescence and Young Adulthood Melissa A. Munn, MPE 1,2 * Michael C. Stallings, PhD 1,2 Soo Hyun Rhee, PhD 1,2 Laura E. Sobik, PhD

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Literacy and Numeracy Performance in Australian School Children in Grades 3, 5, 7, and 9

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Literacy and Numeracy Performance in Australian School Children in Grades 3, 5, 7, and 9 Genetic and Environmental Influences on Literacy and Performance in Australian School Children in Grades 3, 5, 7, and 9 Katrina L. Grasby, William L. Coventry, Brian Byrne, Richard K. Olson & Sarah E.

More information

In Australian twins participating in three different

In Australian twins participating in three different Heritability and Stability of Resting Blood Pressure in Australian Twins Jouke-Jan Hottenga, 1 John B. Whitfield, 2 Eco J. C. de Geus, 1 Dorret I. Boomsma, 1 and Nicholas G. Martin 2 1 Department of Biological

More information

Etiology and Measurement of Relational Aggression: A Multi-Informant Behavior Genetic Investigation

Etiology and Measurement of Relational Aggression: A Multi-Informant Behavior Genetic Investigation Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 118, No. 4, 722 733 0021-843X/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0016949 Etiology and Measurement of Relational Aggression: A Multi-Informant

More information

Parenting as Phenotype: A Behavioral Genetic Approach to Understanding Parenting

Parenting as Phenotype: A Behavioral Genetic Approach to Understanding Parenting The University of San Francisco USF Scholarship: a digital repository @ Gleeson Library Geschke Center Psychology College of Arts and Sciences 2012 Parenting as Phenotype: A Behavioral Genetic Approach

More information

The existence, prevalence, and taxonomy of juvenile bipolar

The existence, prevalence, and taxonomy of juvenile bipolar Longitudinal Stability of the CBCL-Juvenile Bipolar Disorder Phenotype: A Study in Dutch Twins Dorret I. Boomsma, Irene Rebollo, Eske M. Derks, Toos C.E.M. van Beijsterveldt, Robert R. Althoff, David C.

More information

Unseen and Stressed? Gender Differences in Parent and Teacher Ratings of ADHD Symptoms and Associations With Perceived Stress in Children With ADHD

Unseen and Stressed? Gender Differences in Parent and Teacher Ratings of ADHD Symptoms and Associations With Perceived Stress in Children With ADHD 658381JADXXX10.1177/1087054716658381Journal of Attention DisordersIsaksson et al. research-article2016 Article Unseen and Stressed? Gender Differences in Parent and Teacher Ratings of ADHD Symptoms and

More information

Associations Between Conduct Disorder, Neurodevelopmental Problems and Psychopathic Personality Traits in a Swedish Twin Youth Population

Associations Between Conduct Disorder, Neurodevelopmental Problems and Psychopathic Personality Traits in a Swedish Twin Youth Population Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2018) 40:586 592 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-018-9689-z Associations Between Conduct Disorder, Neurodevelopmental Problems and Psychopathic Personality

More information

Florida State University Libraries

Florida State University Libraries Florida State University Libraries Faculty Publications Department of Psychology 2010 Exploring how symptoms of Attention# Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder are related to reading and mathematics performance:

More information

Liability Threshold Models

Liability Threshold Models Liability Threshold Models Frühling Rijsdijk & Kate Morley Twin Workshop, Boulder Tuesday March 4 th 2008 Aims Introduce model fitting to categorical data Define liability and describe assumptions of the

More information

Contributions of parental alcoholism, prenatal substance exposure, and genetic transmission to child ADHD risk : A female twin study

Contributions of parental alcoholism, prenatal substance exposure, and genetic transmission to child ADHD risk : A female twin study Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Open Access Publications 2005 Contributions of parental alcoholism, prenatal substance exposure, and genetic transmission to child ADHD risk

More information

More boys than girls with attention deficit hyperactivity

More boys than girls with attention deficit hyperactivity Why More Boys Than Girls With ADHD Receive Treatment: A Study of Dutch Twins Eske M. Derks, 1 James J. Hudziak, 2,3 and Dorret I. Boomsma 1 1 Department of Biological Psychology,Vrije Universiteit,Amsterdam,

More information

The Influence of Religion on Alcohol Use Initiation: Evidence for Genotype X Environment Interaction

The Influence of Religion on Alcohol Use Initiation: Evidence for Genotype X Environment Interaction Behavior Genetics, Vol. 29, No. 6, 1999 The Influence of Religion on Alcohol Use Initiation: Evidence for Genotype X Environment Interaction Judith R. Koopmans, 1,3 Wendy S. Slutske, 2 G. Caroline M. van

More information

RELIABILITY OF AUTISM TICS, AD/HD, AND OTHER COMORBIDITIES (A TAC) INVENTORY IN A TEST-RETEST DESIGN 1

RELIABILITY OF AUTISM TICS, AD/HD, AND OTHER COMORBIDITIES (A TAC) INVENTORY IN A TEST-RETEST DESIGN 1 Psychological Reports: Measures & Statistics 2014, 114, 1, 93-103. Psychological Reports 2014 RELIABILITY OF AUTISM TICS, AD/HD, AND OTHER COMORBIDITIES (A TAC) INVENTORY IN A TEST-RETEST DESIGN 1 TOMAS

More information

Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds

Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 46:6 (2005), pp 592 597 doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00393.x Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds Essi Viding, 1 R. James R.

More information

Examining the Origins of Gender Differences in Marital Quality: A Behavior Genetic Analysis

Examining the Origins of Gender Differences in Marital Quality: A Behavior Genetic Analysis Journal of Family Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 20, No. 4, 605 613 0893-3200/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.4.605 Examining the Origins of Gender Differences

More information

DOI:

DOI: This is the peer reviewed version of the following article: Childhood Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms and the Development of Adolescent Alcohol Problems: A Prospective, Population-Based

More information

King s Research Portal

King s Research Portal King s Research Portal DOI: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00370.x Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link to publication record in King's Research Portal Citation for published

More information

Relations Between Multi-Informant Assessments of ADHD Symptoms, DAT1, and DRD4

Relations Between Multi-Informant Assessments of ADHD Symptoms, DAT1, and DRD4 Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 2008, Vol. 117, No. 4, 869 880 0021-843X/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013297 Relations Between Multi-Informant Assessments

More information

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Same-sex Sexual Behavior: A Population Study of Twins in Sweden

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Same-sex Sexual Behavior: A Population Study of Twins in Sweden Arch Sex Behav (2010) 39:75 80 DOI 10.1007/s10508-008-9386-1 ORIGINAL PAPER Genetic and Environmental Effects on Same-sex Sexual Behavior: A Population Study of Twins in Sweden Niklas Långström Æ Qazi

More information

Rebecca Pinto 1 & Fruhling Rijsdijk 1 & Angelica Ronald 2 & Philip Asherson 1 & Jonna Kuntsi 1

Rebecca Pinto 1 & Fruhling Rijsdijk 1 & Angelica Ronald 2 & Philip Asherson 1 & Jonna Kuntsi 1 J Abnorm Child Psychol (2016) 44:335 345 DOI 10.1007/s10802-015-0037-4 The Genetic Overlap of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Autistic-like Traits: an Investigation of Individual Symptom Scales

More information

Childhood problem behavior and parental divorce: evidence for gene environment interaction

Childhood problem behavior and parental divorce: evidence for gene environment interaction Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol (2012) 47:1539 1548 DOI 10.1007/s00127-011-0470-9 ORIGINAL PAPER Childhood problem behavior and parental divorce: evidence for gene environment interaction Sylvana Robbers

More information

Familial confounding of the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD in offspring

Familial confounding of the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD in offspring Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry **:* (2013), pp ** ** doi:10.1111/jcpp.12124 Familial confounding of the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD in offspring Charlotte

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Educ Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 September 14.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Educ Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 September 14. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Educ Psychol. 2007 February 1; 99(1): 128 139. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.128. The Origins of Diverse Domains of Mathematics: Generalist

More information

Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration

Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration J Fam Viol (2007) 22:121 129 DOI 10.1007/s10896-006-9059-6 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration

More information

Exploring the inter-relationship of smoking age-at-onset, cigarette consumption and smoking persistence: genes or environment?

Exploring the inter-relationship of smoking age-at-onset, cigarette consumption and smoking persistence: genes or environment? Psychological Medicine, 2007, 37, 1357 1367. f 2007 Cambridge University Press doi:10.1017/s0033291707000748 First published online 30 April 2007 Printed in the United Kingdom Exploring the inter-relationship

More information

Psychosocial problems in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with oppositional defiant disorder

Psychosocial problems in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with oppositional defiant disorder Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences (2002), 56, 365 369 Regular Article Psychosocial problems in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with oppositional defiant disorder YUZURU HARADA, md, phd, 1

More information

The role of conduct disorder in explaining the comorbidity between alcohol and illicit drug dependence in adolescence

The role of conduct disorder in explaining the comorbidity between alcohol and illicit drug dependence in adolescence Drug and Alcohol Dependence 87 (2007) 46 53 The role of conduct disorder in explaining the comorbidity between alcohol and illicit drug dependence in adolescence Tanya M.M. Button a,, Soo Hyun Rhee a,b,

More information

Genetic Influences on Childhood IQ in 5- and 7-Year-Old Dutch Twins

Genetic Influences on Childhood IQ in 5- and 7-Year-Old Dutch Twins DEVELOPMENTAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 14{\), 115-126 Copyright 1998, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Genetic Influences on Childhood IQ in 5- and 7-Year-Old Dutch Twins Dorret I. Boomsma and G. Caroline M.

More information

The Familial Risk of Autism

The Familial Risk of Autism Research Original Investigation The Familial Risk of Autism Sven Sandin, MSc; Paul Lichtenstein, PhD; Ralf Kuja-Halkola, MSc; Henrik Larsson, PhD; Christina M. Hultman, PhD; Abraham Reichenberg, PhD IMPORTANCE

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Girls and Boys Gender-Typed and Gender-Neutral Values

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Girls and Boys Gender-Typed and Gender-Neutral Values Developmental Psychology 2010 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 47, No. 3, 726 731 0012-1649/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021910 Genetic and Environmental Influences on Girls and Boys Gender-Typed

More information

Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures

Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures Journal of Substance Abuse, 5, 227-246 (7993) Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures Andrew C. Heath Departments of Psychiatry, Psychology

More information

Multiple Regression Analysis of Twin Data: Etiology of Deviant Scores versus Individuai Differences

Multiple Regression Analysis of Twin Data: Etiology of Deviant Scores versus Individuai Differences Acta Genet Med Gemellol 37:205-216 (1988) 1988 by The Mendel Institute, Rome Received 30 January 1988 Final 18 Aprii 1988 Multiple Regression Analysis of Twin Data: Etiology of Deviant Scores versus Individuai

More information

Journal of Research in Personality

Journal of Research in Personality Journal of Research in Personality 43 (2009) 737 746 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Research in Personality journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp Temperament in middle childhood:

More information

An Introduction to Quantitative Genetics I. Heather A Lawson Advanced Genetics Spring2018

An Introduction to Quantitative Genetics I. Heather A Lawson Advanced Genetics Spring2018 An Introduction to Quantitative Genetics I Heather A Lawson Advanced Genetics Spring2018 Outline What is Quantitative Genetics? Genotypic Values and Genetic Effects Heritability Linkage Disequilibrium

More information

Research Article. Keywords genes, environments, ADHD behaviors, reading performance, math performance

Research Article. Keywords genes, environments, ADHD behaviors, reading performance, math performance Research Article Exploring How Symptoms of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Are Related to Reading and Mathematics Performance: General Genes, General Environments Psychological Science 21(11)

More information

Appendix A. Basics of Latent Class Analysis

Appendix A. Basics of Latent Class Analysis 7/3/2000 Appendix A. Basics of Latent Class Analysis Latent class analysis (LCA) is a statistical method for discovering subtypes of related cases from multivariate categorical data. A latent class may

More information

Development and Psychopathology 25 (2013), # Cambridge University Press 2013 doi: /s

Development and Psychopathology 25 (2013), # Cambridge University Press 2013 doi: /s Development and Psychopathology 25 (2013), 487 500 # Cambridge University Press 2013 doi:10.1017/s0954579412001198 Genetic origin of the relationship between parental negativity and behavior problems from

More information

Externalizing problems in childhood and adolescence predict subsequent educational achievement but for different genetic and environmental reasons

Externalizing problems in childhood and adolescence predict subsequent educational achievement but for different genetic and environmental reasons Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 58:3 (2017), pp 292 304 doi:10.1111/jcpp.12655 Externalizing problems in childhood and adolescence predict subsequent educational achievement but for different

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Dev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 January 1.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Dev Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 January 1. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychol. 2009 January ; 45(1): 77 89. doi:10.1037/a0014549. Gene Environment Interactions in Reading Disability and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity

More information

Research Article Power Estimation for Gene-Longevity Association Analysis Using Concordant Twins

Research Article Power Estimation for Gene-Longevity Association Analysis Using Concordant Twins Genetics Research International, Article ID 154204, 8 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2014/154204 Research Article Power Estimation for Gene-Longevity Association Analysis Using Concordant Twins Qihua

More information