Published online: 05 Nov 2013.

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Published online: 05 Nov 2013."

Transcription

1 This article was downloaded by: [University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA)] On: 07 November 2013, At: 06:39 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: Registered office: Mortimer House, Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Applied Neuropsychology: Child Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: A Comparison of IQ and Memory Cluster Solutions in Moderate and Severe Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury Nicholas S. Thaler a, Jennifer Terranova a, Alisa Turner a, Joan Mayfield b & Daniel N. Allen a a Psychology, University of Nevada-Las Vegas, Las Vegas, Nevada b Neuropsychology, Our Children's House at Baylor, Dallas, Texas Published online: 05 Nov To cite this article: Nicholas S. Thaler, Jennifer Terranova, Alisa Turner, Joan Mayfield & Daniel N. Allen, Applied Neuropsychology: Child (2013): A Comparison of IQ and Memory Cluster Solutions in Moderate and Severe Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury, Applied Neuropsychology: Child, DOI: / To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content ) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at

2 APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD, 0: 1 11, 2014 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: print/ online DOI: / A Comparison of IQ and Memory Cluster Solutions in Moderate and Severe Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury Downloaded by [University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA)] at 06:39 07 November 2013 Nicholas S. Thaler, Jennifer Terranova, and Alisa Turner Psychology, University of Nevada-Las Vegas, Las Vegas, Nevada Joan Mayfield Neuropsychology, Our Children s House at Baylor, Dallas, Texas Daniel N. Allen Psychology, University of Nevada-Las Vegas, Las Vegas, Nevada Recent studies have examined heterogeneous neuropsychological outcomes in childhood traumatic brain injury (TBI) using cluster analysis. These studies have identified homogeneous subgroups based on tests of IQ, memory, and other cognitive abilities that show some degree of association with specific cognitive, emotional, and behavioral outcomes, and have demonstrated that the clusters derived for children with TBI are different from those observed in normal populations. However, the extent to which these subgroups are stable across abilities has not been examined, and this has significant implications for the generalizability and clinical utility of TBI clusters. The current study addressed this by comparing IQ and memory profiles of 137 children who sustained moderate-to-severe TBI. Cluster analysis of IQ and memory scores indicated that a fourcluster solution was optimal for the IQ scores and a five-cluster solution was optimal for the memory scores. Three clusters on each battery differed primarily by level of performance, while the others had pattern variations. Cross-plotting the clusters across respective IQ and memory test scores indicated that clusters defined by level were generally stable, while clusters defined by pattern differed. Notably, children with slower processing speed exhibited low-average to below-average performance on memory indexes. These results provide some support for the stability of previously identified memory and IQ clusters and provide information about the relationship between IQ and memory in children with TBI. Key words: cluster analysis, IQ, memory, pediatric, TBI Pediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI) often results in significant impairment in neurocognitive, behavioral, and interpersonal domains. The extent of these deficits is heterogeneous due to variability in the mechanism, severity, and location of injury, secondary factors, and individual differences among patients (Allen, Thaler, Cross, & Address correspondence to Daniel N. Allen, Psychology, University of Nevada-Las Vegas, 4505 S. Maryland Pkwy., Las Vegas, NV Daniel.allen@unlv.edu Mayfield, 2013; Babikian & Asarnow, 2009; Fay et al., 2009; Roman et al., 1998; Schwartz, et al., 2003; Yeates et al., 2005). This variability has proven challenging in classifying cases of TBI into subgroups that clinically differ. General classification methods based on severity of injury, length of coma, and postinjury amnesia may fail to fully capture differential outcomes on important variables, such as behavior, academic achievement, and adaptive functioning. For example, some cases that are initially classified as severe using the Glasgow Coma

3 2 THALER ET AL. Scale (GCS; Teasdale & Jennett, 1974) may ultimately exhibit little long-term disability (Fay et al., 2009), while others may develop significant behavioral disturbances subsequent to their injury (Allen, Leany et al., 2010). Certain characteristics of the injured child, such as premorbid functioning, age and developmental level at time of injury, socioeconomic status, and development of secondary complications, are associated with outcome after TBI (Babikian & Asarnow, 2009; Max et al., 1999). The impact of these variables contributes further to the disparate outcomes after TBI, and general classification approaches may therefore be assisted by additional methods that describe more homogeneous subsets of patients and predict specific outcomes. Multivariate grouping techniques, such as cluster analysis, can provide an empirical and mathematical method to classify groups in a manner that extends beyond observation and description of symptoms and behavior. To that end, previous studies have used cluster analysis to identify discrete subgroups within TBI that demonstrate differential levels and patterns of performance on neuropsychological tests (Crawford, Garthwaite, & Johnson, 1997; Curtiss, Vanderploeg, Spencer, & Salazar, 2001; Donders & Warschausky, 1997; Mottram & Donders, 2006), which have since been found to be associated with different profiles of impairment across a number of lifefunctioning domains (Allen, Leany et al., 2010; Thaler, et al., 2010). Neuropsychological variables may provide a unique approach to classification because they reflect the impact of injury on involved brain regions and are also useful predictors of some important outcomes, such as educational placement, social functioning, and general functional impairment (Miller & Donders, 2003; Rassovsky et al., 2006; Yeates et al., 2004). The extant neuropsychological cluster-analytic literature on pediatric TBI indicates that: (a) neurocognitive clusters identified in children with TBI differ markedly from those identified in normal populations (Allen, Leany et al., 2010; Donders, 1996, 1999; Donders & Warschausky, 1997; Mottram & Donders, 2006); (b) cluster solutions for IQ tests exhibit adequate generalizability across different samples of children with TBI (e.g., an IQ cluster that emerges in pediatric TBI populations that has marked impairments in perceptual organization and processing speed; Donders & Warschausky, 1997; Thaler et al., 2010); and (c) clusters derived from IQ and memory tests show meaningful associations with behavioral disturbances and disruption of other neurocognitive abilities, with children falling in severely impaired clusters consistently exhibiting the worst neurocognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes (Allen, Leany et al., 2010; Thaler et al., 2010). However, one area that has not yet received attention involves the impact of test selection on the clusters identified in pediatric TBI samples. Test selection is, of course, an important consideration in such studies. For example, tests that are insensitive to the sequelae of childhood TBI cannot be expected to distinguish level or pattern of performance differences from the general population. Goldstein, Allen, and Seaton (1998) made this point in relation to cluster-analytic studies of schizophrenia, where selection of tests also had the potential for significantly influencing cluster-analytic results. Even in cases where tests are selected that assess different but relevant neurocognitive domains, different cluster solutions may result if cluster membership was more a function of the psychometric properties of the tests and the abilities they measure, rather than the detection of unique and meaningful subgroups of patients. It follows, then, that analyses that generate comparable cluster solutions using a variety of pertinent measures speak to the stability of the clusters and their subsequent utility in identifying discrete profiles of neurocognitive impairment (Goldstein et al., 1998). Moreover, by demonstrating that these empirically derived subgroups of TBI correspond to current conceptualizations of brain injury presentations and recovery patterns, cluster-analytic studies may provide clinical benefits or additional theoretical understanding of disorders. Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to determine whether neurocognitive clusters derived using different tests generalize across these tests. Another purpose was to characterize a sample of children with TBI referred for a pediatric neuropsychological evaluation on their IQ and memory profiles so that clinicians who work in such settings may be informed about the cognitive impairments expected in this population. Tests were selected from the IQ and memory studies by Allen and colleagues (Allen, Leany et al., 2010; Thaler et al., 2010). Examining clusters through other tests that were not used in deriving the clusters themselves provides a better understanding of the relationships among the tests. If the identified clusters show similar levels and patterns of performance when plotted across the other tests, it would confirm that these clusters are stable and generalizable and were not unduly influenced by psychometric issues. Based on the clinically meaningful solutions obtained in previous cluster-analytic studies (Allen, Leany et al., 2010; Thaler et al., 2010), we predicted that four or five cluster solutions will be the best fit for the IQ and memory tests and that clusters will demonstrate stability when cross-plotted on the other tests. Specifically, IQ clusters similar to those previously identified in children with TBI (Donders & Warschausky, 1997; Thaler et al., 2010) will include a cluster with near-normal IQ functioning, a cluster with impaired functioning, and a cluster with selective weaknesses in processing speed. Regarding memory clusters, in line with Allen, Leany, and colleagues (2010), we anticipated there would be clusters that have differential patterns of performance in verbal and

4 nonverbal memory subtests, as well as clusters that are universally impaired or performing in the average range. It was also hypothesized that IQ and memory clusters would correspond in a clinically meaningful way (e.g., a high verbal memory cluster will have high verbal IQ), therefore further cross-validating the cluster solutions. METHOD Participants The sample consisted of 137 children who sustained a TBI and were selected from a consecutive series of cases seen for neuropsychological evaluation during a 5-year period. All children were referred to a pediatric specialty hospital specifically because they were either inpatients at the hospital or because they had a medical disorder, including TBI. Participants were included in the current study if appropriate neuroimaging, laboratory, and examinational findings determined a primary diagnosis of TBI and if they had been administered the Test of Memory and Learning (TOMAL) and Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition (WISC-III) as part of their neuropsychological evaluation. Individuals with preinjury neurological or neurodevelopmental disorders were excluded, as were individuals who underwent a previous evaluation with the WISC-III. Participants in the current study were selected from those used in previous memory and IQ studies (Allen, Leany et al., 2010; Thaler et al., 2010). Of the 137 participants who qualified, 61% were boys with an average age of 11.7 years (SD = 3.0, range = years). The causes of injuries included motor vehicle accident (48.9%), being struck by a motor vehicle (25.5%), gunshot (5.1%), fall (2.2%), four-wheeler accident (5.1%), bike accident (1.5%), skiing accident (5.8%), and other causes (5.1%). The majority of participants had closedhead injuries (93.4%). The children were assessed at an average of 12.1 months (SD = 16.5, range = months) following injury and were clinically stable and capable of cooperating with testing procedures. At the time of assessment, participants had a mean WISC-III Full-Scale IQ of 83.4 (SD = 14.4) and a mean TOMAL Composite Memory score of 81.3 (SD = 14.8). GCS scores were available for 89 of the participants. The median GCS score was 7 (range = 3 15), which indicates the injuries acquired were generally severe in nature. Measures Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition (Wechsler, 1991). The WISC-III was used to assess intellectual ability. The four index factors, composed of 10 subtests, include Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), NEUROCOGNITIVE HETEROGENEITY IN TBI 3 Perceptual Organization Index (POI), Freedom from Distractibility Index (FDI), and Processing Speed Index (PSI) scores. The index scores have a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. Test of Memory and Learning (Reynolds & Bigler, 1994). The TOMAL, composed of 14 subtests (10 that are core and 4 that are supplementary), is used to assess memory ability in children ages 5 to 19 years old. The 10 core subtests include Memory for Stories, Facial Memory, Word Selective Reminding, Visual Selective Reminding, Object Recall, Abstract Visual Memory, Digits Forward, Visual Sequential Memory, Paired Recall, and Memory for Location. The 4 supplementary subtests include Letters Forward, Digits Backward, Letters Backward, and Manual Imitation. Core index scores, calculated by subtest scores, include the Verbal Memory Index (VMI), Nonverbal Memory Index (NMI), Composite Memory Index, Delayed Recall Index (DRI), and Attention/ Concentration Index (ACI). The index scores have a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15. Glasgow Coma Scale score (Teasdale & Jennett, 1974). The GCS was used to evaluate the severity of injury and was administered shortly after injury either by first responders at the scene or in the emergency room of the hospital. The GCS uses three methods to determine severity: Best Eye Response (score range = 1 4), Best Verbal Response (score range = 1 5), and Best Motor Response (score range = 1 6). The sum of the three scores is then used to classify TBI severity as either mild (range = 13 15), moderate (range = 9 12), or severe (range = 3 8). Procedures Tests were administered following standardized procedures by a pediatric neuropsychologist or doctoral-level technicians who were supervised by the pediatric neuropsychologist. The TOMAL and WISC-III were administered to all children, unless there was a reason not to administer the test (age of child, incapable of cooperating for the tests, etc.). Data Analysis Two separate hierarchical cluster analyses were conducted, one for IQ index scores selected from the WISC- III and a second for memory factor scores identified in the TOMAL from a previous study (Allen, Leany et al., 2010). Although TOMAL factor scores were analyzed, results were plotted across TOMAL index scores to allow a more direct clinical interpretation. Four-, five-, and sixcluster solutions were specified in each case. Both analyses used Ward s method as a clustering method, as it is

5 4 THALER ET AL. commonly used in neuropsychological studies and is useful with quantitative variables in accounting for outliers and for generating reliable clustering solutions (Donders, 2008; Goldstein, Allen, & Caponigro, 2010; Milligan & Hirtle, 2003). Squared Euclidian distance served as the similarity measure, as is also consistently done in other neuropsychology studies of this sort to provide a direct measure of Euclidean space (Everitt, Landau, & Leese, 2001). The number of clusters per battery was determined using the selected approach detailed by Aldenderfer and Blashfield (1984). Specifically, visual inspection of the dendograms and subsequent plotting of clusters in discriminant function space served as a preliminary method to determine clusters that have minimal or no overlap. Attributes for cluster solutions also served as predictors of cluster membership to examine classification rates using discriminant function analysis. A second partitional clustering method (K-means) was also examined for each cluster solution with starting centroids specified as the mean index and factor scores of the clusters derived by the hierarchical method. Agreement between K-means and Ward s clusters were then calculated using Cohen s kappa. Cluster solutions with excellent agreement rates (e.g., >.75; Fleiss, 1981) were deemed stable. Final cluster solutions were determined based on such stability, separation among cluster centroids, and theoretical interest. External validity of the solutions was evaluated across demographic and clinical variables not included in the cluster analysis, including age, gender, months since injury, age at injury, and GCS scores. IQ clusters were then plotted across TOMAL index scores rather than factor scores to aid in clinical interpretability. Memory clusters were plotted across WISC- III index scores. Solutions were additionally cross-tabulated and analyzed with chi-square and Cohen s kappa to examine consistency of cluster membership between the two procedures. Cluster Analysis RESULTS IQ clusters. Ward s method was compared with the K-means method for the four-, five-, and six-cluster solutions. In all cases, agreement between the methods was excellent (kappas =.80,.80,.85, respectively). When plotted in discriminant function space, the four- and fivecluster solutions had clear separation among clusters, with classification rates at 86.9% and 91.2%, respectively. However, the separation for the six-cluster solution was not as clear and there was a drop in classification rate from the five-cluster solution at 89.1%. Cluster solutions were next plotted across IQ index scores. The four-cluster solution yielded: a cluster with low-average (i.e., ) standard scores across the WISC-III index scores (C1); a second cluster with average scores slightly above (C2); a third cluster with average scores that were slightly below (C3); and a fourth cluster with low-average VCI and FDI scores and impaired ( ) POI and PSI standard scores (C4). The five-cluster solution split one of the average clusters (C3) into a cluster with average VCI, POI, and FDI scores and low-average PSI scores (C3) and a cluster with lowaverage VCI and POI scores and average FDI and PSI scores (C5). The six-cluster solution again split the same cluster (C3) into a similar cluster with average VCI, POI, and FDI scores and low-average PSI scores, and a new average cluster. The four-cluster was selected for further analysis, as it had wide separation in discriminant function space and comparable kappa agreement with the five-cluster solution and was consistent with previous findings (Donders & Warschausky, 1997; Thaler et al., 2010). The five-cluster solution s new clusters appeared to represent children who performed in the intermediate range between average and low-average clusters and was not particularly of interest. Although the six-cluster solution had a higher kappa agreement across the two clustering methods, it was of less interest because of the overlap in multidimensional space, as well as the fact that the new sixth cluster was essentially an average-functioning cluster that was similar to an existing cluster. See Figure 1 for a plot of the four-cluster solution. Clusters were named for their level and pattern of performances across index scores. As such, the C1 was identified as below average, as all four index scores were between a standard score of 74 and 80. C2 was identified as near normal because all index scores were at or above the mean. C3 had an average PSI index score that was below 90 while other index scores were between 90 and 100, and so C3 was identified as low normal. The fourth cluster (C4) had below-average VCI and FDI scores and impaired POI and PSI scores, and so was coined impaired POI/PSI. As seen in the figure, one of the clusters differed mainly by level of performance, while three had differences in pattern of index score performance. See Table 1 for a complete list of demographic and IQ data for the clusters. Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) revealed no differences among the clusters for age, months since injury, or GCS scores (p >.39 in all cases). Chi-square analysis indicated no significant differences for gender, ethnicity, open versus closed injury, or mechanism of injury (p >.21 in all cases). The clusters therefore appear to be independent of other clinical variables that might influence outcome. Memory clusters. Again, four-, five-, and six-cluster solutions were examined via Ward s method and were compared to the K-means iterative partitional method.

6 NEUROCOGNITIVE HETEROGENEITY IN TBI 5 Downloaded by [University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA)] at 06:39 07 November 2013 FIGURE 1 IQ and memory performance on respective solutions. VCI = Verbal Comprehension Index; POI = Perceptual Organization Index; FDI = Freedom from Distractibility Index; PSI = Processing Speed Index; VMI = Verbal Memory Index; NMI = Nonverbal Memory Index; ACI = Attention/ Concentration Index; DRI = Delayed Recall Index. Variables TABLE 1 Descriptive and Clinical Variables of the IQ Four-Cluster Solution Near Normal (n = 21) Below Average (n = 45) Clusters Low Normal (n = 55) Impaired POI/ PSI (n = 16) Total (n = 137) Gender (% Male) TBI Type (Closed) M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD Age (years) Time Elapsed (months) GCS WISC-III VCI POI FDI PSI FSIQ TOMAL VMI NMI ACI DRI CMI GCS = Glasgow Coma Scale; WISC-III = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition; TOMAL = Test of Memory and Learning; VCI = Verbal Comprehension Index; POI = Perceptual Organization Index; FDI = Freedom from Distractibility Index; PSI = Processing Speed Index; FSIQ = Full-Scale IQ; VMI = Verbal Memory Index; NMI = Nonverbal Memory Index; ACI = Attention/Concentration Index; DRI = Delayed Recall Index; CMI = Composite Memory Index.

7 6 THALER ET AL. Downloaded by [University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA)] at 06:39 07 November 2013 Agreement between the two methods for the clusters was excellent (.79,.88,.86, respectively) in all cases. When plotted in multidimensional space, again the four- and five-cluster solutions demonstrated a separation of cluster centroids while the six-cluster solution had significant overlap with two clusters. Discriminant function classification accuracy was comparable across solutions with 87.6% for the four-cluster solution, 89.8% for the five-cluster solution, and 91.2% for the six-cluster solution. Although the memory clusters were derived from TOMAL factors identified in the study by Allen, Leany, and colleagues (2010), they were plotted across TOMAL VMI, NMI, ACI, and DRI scores to ease interpretability for theoretical and clinical application. The four-cluster solution produced a cluster with below-average performance on all index scores (C1); a cluster with near-normal performance on all index scores except the ACI, which had a mean low-average standard score of 89.5 (C2); a cluster with impaired functioning across all indexes (C3); and a cluster with average VMI and DRI scores and below-average NMI and ACI scores (C4). The five-cluster solution split the below-average cluster (C1) into a below-average cluster and a cluster with average NMI scores and below-average scores on the other indexes. The six-cluster solution again split the below-average cluster into a below-average cluster and a second average Variables Near Normal (n = 45) TABLE 2 Descriptive and Clinical Variables of the Memory Five-Cluster Solution Below Average (n = 49) cluster. The memory five-cluster solution was deemed the most useful for interpretation, as it added a new cluster of clinical and theoretical interest (nonverbal cluster) while the six-cluster solution was redundant, with its second average cluster. Refer to Figure 1 for a plot of the five-cluster memory solution. TOMAL clusters were named based on their levels and patterns of performance on index scores. C1 had belowaverage scores on all memory indexes and was so named below average. C2 was identified as near normal, as average performance on all index scores approached the population mean. C3 was identified as impaired, given that all index scores were below a standard score of 70. C4 was notable for having average performance on the VMI and DRI but below-average performance on the NMI and ACI and was therefore identified as verbal. Finally, C5 showed reverse findings an average NMI score but below-average scores on other indexes and was identified as nonverbal. See Table 2 for demographic and memory data of the clusters. ANOVAs among the memory clusters revealed no significant differences for age, months since injury, and GCS scores (p >.09 in all cases). Chi-square analyses showed no significant differences for gender, open versus closed head injury, and mechanism of injury (p >.33 in all cases). However, there were significant differences for ethnicity (p <.01). Clusters Impaired (n = 20) Verbal (n = 13) Nonverbal (n = 10) Total (n = 137) Gender (% Male) TBI Type (Closed) M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD Age (years) Time Elapsed (months) GCS TOMAL VMI NMI ACI DRI CMI WISC-III VCI POI FDI PSI FSIQ GCS = Glasgow Coma Scale; WISC-III = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition; TOMAL = Test of Memory and Learning; VCI = Verbal Comprehension Index; POI = Perceptual Organization Index; FDI = Freedom from Distractibility Index; PSI = Processing Speed Index; FSIQ = Full-Scale IQ; VMI = Verbal Memory Index; NMI = Nonverbal Memory Index; ACI = Attention/Concentration Index; DRI = Delayed Recall Index; CMI = Composite Memory Index.

8 NEUROCOGNITIVE HETEROGENEITY IN TBI 7 Downloaded by [University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA)] at 06:39 07 November 2013 FIGURE 2 IQ and memory clusters plotted on counterpart batteries. VCI = Verbal Comprehension Index; POI = Perceptual Organization Index; FDI = Freedom from Distractibility Index; PSI = Processing Speed Index; VMI = Verbal Memory Index; NMI = Nonverbal Memory Index; ACI = Attention/Concentration Index; DRI = Delayed Recall Index. Comparisons between the IQ and memory clusters. Cluster memberships of the IQ and memory clusters were next plotted against their counterpart batteries, as seen in Figure 2. Three clusters from each solution appeared to match, as they represent three discrete levels of general intellectual and memory functioning. Specifically, the near-normal clusters, the below-average clusters, and the impaired POI/PSI IQ cluster and impaired memory cluster correspond visually with similar level and pattern of performance across batteries. Along with these distinctions, additional unique relationships were identified. The low-normal IQ cluster had generally low-average memory performance with TABLE 3 Relations Between IQ and Memory-Based Cluster Analyses scores across TOMAL indexes that ranged from 84.9 to 87.3 (see Table 1). The verbal memory cluster had nearaverage scores on the VCI, FDI, and PSI but low-average scores on the NMI, while the nonverbal cluster had below-average VCI and PSI scores and average POI and FDI scores, consistent with their performance on the memory indexes. To determine the extent to which the IQ and memory clusters classified the cases into comparable groups, cluster memberships were cross-tabulated. The clusters that were chiefly defined by level of performance were matched together with percentages of agreement rates provided. Results are presented in Table 3. IQ-Based Clusters Near Normal Low Normal Below Average Impaired POI/PSI Total Memory-Based Clusters Below Average Near Normal Nonverbal Verbal Impaired Total Note. Percentages reflect number of cases correctly classified into corresponding level of performance on the other battery. Percentages were not calculated for clusters with unique pattern of performance because no corresponding cluster was available on the other battery.

9 8 THALER ET AL. Degree of agreement was fair (kappa =.29, p <.01), which might be expected given the imbalance in number of clusters between measures. Inspection of the table reveals that there was some correspondence in which cases fell into the below-average clusters, with 55.6% of cases in the IQ cluster and 51.0% of cases in the memory cluster agreeing on membership. Of interest, 34.7% of the cases classified as below average in memory were classified as low normal in IQ. Although 71.4% of the cases in the near-normal IQ cluster were classified as near normal in memory, only 33.3% of near-normal memory cases were classified as near normal in IQ, while 55.6% of these cases were instead classified as low normal. Regarding the impaired POI/PSI cluster, 68.8% were classified as impaired in memory, while the remaining 31.2% were classified as below average. For the impaired memory cluster, 55.0% were identified as impaired POI/PSI, while the remaining 45.0% were classified as low PSI. For the clusters that had no apparent corresponding cluster, classifications were more diffuse. The low-normal cluster had 30.9% of its cases classified as below average in memory, 45.5% of its cases classified as near normal, 10.9% as verbal, and 12.7% as nonverbal. For the nonverbal cluster, 60% were classified as low normal and the remaining 40% were classified as either below average or near normal. For the verbal cluster, 54.8% were classified as low normal with the others being classified as below average or near normal. DISCUSSION Findings from this study demonstrate that cluster analysis of IQ and memory batteries identifies somewhat distinct subgroups of children with TBI, though the patterns of performance on the IQ and memory batteries made theoretical sense and were consistent enough to confirm that clusters represent actual levels of ability as measured by the tests. Our hypothesis that the cluster solutions identified in previous studies would be replicated was met. Demographic and clinical variables, including age, gender, and mechanism of injury, did not influence cluster membership, confirming that these clusters were based primarily on neurocognitive outcome. There were TOMAL cluster differences due to ethnicity, which may be related to family socioeconomic status, access to rehabilitation services, or other environmental factors that we were not able to directly address. Differences on the GCS were nonsignificant, though there was a trend for the TOMAL clusters toward differentiating (p =.09) with a medium effect size (partial η² =.08), suggesting that a larger sample might identify differences in which the most impaired cluster initially had the severest injury, as might be expected. The identified profiles appear to reflect specific cognitive subtypes of functioning across IQ and memory. However, cluster analysis uses mathematical models to classify cases and will do so even with random data (Goldstein et al., 1998). It is therefore necessary to determine the external validity of the clusters by comparing them on variables that were not included in the cluster analysis. In this case, we cross-examined clusters on corresponding batteries in which IQ clusters were plotted on memory indexes and memory clusters on IQ indexes. IQ and memory share approximately 40% to 50% of the same variance (Reynolds & Voress, 2007), so it was expected that interpretable clusters would share similar profiles across respective batteries, albeit with some unique insights. Both the IQ and memory tests classified the sample into three corresponding levels of general performance specifically, a group that performed near the average range, a group that performed below average, and a group that exhibited moderate-to-severe impairment. The near-normal cluster made up nearly a third of the total memory sample (32.8%) but considerably less of the IQ sample (15.3%). A relatively large proportion of the participants were in the below-average clusters for both the IQ and memory batteries (32.8% and 34.3%, respectively), while fewer participants were in the severe clusters (11.7% and 14.5%, respectively). The near-normal, below-average, and impaired clusters appeared stable when cross-plotted. For example, the near-normal IQ cluster also performed in the average range on the memory indexes, while the near-normal memory cluster was in the average range on the IQ indexes. The memory subgroup with impaired performance exhibited severe impairments in the POI and PSI indexes when plotted across the WISC-III indexes, resembling the corresponding IQ subgroup with severe impairment on the POI and PSI and suggesting that severe impairments in processing speed and perceptual organization may be associated with a similarly severe impairment in memory functioning. Not all clusters had corresponding solutions. One IQ cluster (low normal) and two memory clusters (verbal, nonverbal) had unique profile patterns that were not replicated by the other battery. As these clusters represented selective cognitive deficits, they provide additional insights on the relationship between IQ and memory when cross-plotted. The low-normal cluster had overall low-average performance across all memory indexes, approaching one standard deviation below the mean, though it had bet ter scores on the VCI and FDI. This suggests that mild weaknesses in processing speed and perceptual organization may contribute to a drop in general memory functioning. The verbal cluster exhibited a relative weakness on the POI index and average to low-average performance on the other IQ indexes, while the nonverbal cluster had a reverse relationship,

10 indicating that these clusters patterns of performance were consistent across batteries and providing evidence of modality-specific deficits in verbal/nonverbal processing, respectively. When the two solutions were compared for similarities between group membership, the agreement rate was fair. This is inevitable as the number of clusters between the two measures differed, but it also reflects that IQ and memory capture separate cognitive processes that differentially vary. Most of the children who were classified as below average on one cluster were also classified as below average on the corresponding cluster, though more than a third (34.6%) of the cases in the below-average memory cluster were classified instead as low normal in IQ. Most of the children who performed near the average range with IQ were classified in the average range with memory. However, a substantial proportion of children who were classified in the near-normal cluster in memory were classified as being in the low-normal cluster in IQ (55.6%), as might be expected given that both clusters reflect similar levels of functioning. The two impaired clusters were either classified into the corresponding cluster or otherwise put into the below-average cluster, confirming that these children are underperforming on both IQ and memory measures. Finally, the verbal and nonverbal memory clusters were spread out on IQ clusters, though a majority of each was classified as low normal. All in all, clusters defined by level of performance generally concurred, while those defined by pattern of performance were diffused across the other battery s cluster groups. These findings are particularly relevant for clinicians working with children referred for TBI, as they provide expected profile variations with corresponding profiles for each cluster, as well as an indication of differences that might be expected on other measures among the clusters. As these data reflect a representative sample of children referred for evaluation during a 5-year period, they capture a range of neurocognitive functioning that might be observed in a pediatric hospital setting. Profiles observed here in turn can inform clinicians about expected neurocognitive functioning in their patients, and by extension, expected outcomes, as have been identified in other studies (Allen, Leany et al., 2010; Thaler et al., 2010). In general, children with near-normal cognition following TBI perform equally well on intellectual and memory tests. However, children with IQ scores that are roughly two thirds of a standard deviation below average performed close to a full standard deviation below the mean on memory measures. Below-average and impaired IQ scores also reflect below-average and impaired memory scores, with the POI and PSI indexes particularly corresponding with overall impaired memory performance. Memory clusters representing overall level of performance reflect similar levels on IQ clusters. The NEUROCOGNITIVE HETEROGENEITY IN TBI 9 verbal memory cluster had an 8-point difference between the VCI and POI favoring the VCI, while the nonverbal cluster had a 7-point difference favoring the POI. The current results are limited in a number of ways. This study relied on archival data using outdated measures such as the WISC-III and TOMAL. Future studies should examine the generalizability of these findings to the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2003) and the Test of Memory and Learning-2 (Reynolds & Voress, 2007), especially in light of findings identifying differences in WISC- III and WISC-IV performance in children with TBI (Allen, Thaler, Donohue, & Mayfield, 2010). However, the purpose of this article was not to examine the use of the measures themselves, but rather the constructs they measure. Symptom validity data were unavailable, although patients were not assessed for compensatory or disability claims and trained pediatric neuropsychologists took care to obtain accurate data whenever possible during assessments. It was also impossible to ascertain how premorbid functioning might have contributed to cluster membership, as premorbid data were unavailable. Although all children underwent standard neuroimaging protocols, data were not collected for research purposes and therefore were also not available for analysis. The sample primarily consisted of children with severe injuries and consequently lower IQ scores than those observed in other cluster-analytic studies of TBI (Donders & Warschausky, 1997; Goldstein et al., 2010). As such, the results should primarily generalize to other children who were classified with severe injuries in a pediatric care setting. However, some (4%) children included in this study had sustained milder injuries with GCS scores ranged from 3 to 15, and data were missing from others. The inclusion of a smaller portion of children with less severe injuries may have had some impact on the obtained cluster solution, though the purpose of this study was to characterize a population of pediatric patients referred for TBI and results should be generalizable to clinicians working in similar settings. Similarly, length of time since injury ranged from 3 to 127 months, indicating that some children included in the study may still have been experiencing spontaneous recovery while others recovery had likely plateaued; however, given that clusters did not differ among this variable, this does not appear to have significantly influenced membership. Some of the clusters consisted of only 7% to 12% of the entire sample, limiting their power to detect cluster differences. However, as clusters replicate those from previous studies (Allen, Leany et al., 2010; Thaler et al., 2010), they appear reliable. Finally, no control sample was available to compare clusters, though we have evidence that IQ and memory clusters presented in children with severe TBI substantially differ from those in healthy controls (Allen, Leany et al., 2010; Donders & Warschausky, 1997).

11 10 THALER ET AL. In summary, the cognitive heterogeneity evidenced in children with TBI manifests in consistent levels and patterns across memory and IQ batteries. The comparison between the WISC-III and TOMAL batteries identified four or five clusters in the solutions, with clusters replicating profiles represented in other studies (Allen, Leany et al., 2010; Donders & Warschausky, 1997; Thaler et al., 2010). Both solutions identified a subgroup of patients with average performance on IQ and memory measures, consistent with previous clinical studies of adult patients with TBI and schizophrenia (Goldstein et al., 1998, 2010). Both solutions also identified children with overall below-average and impaired cognitive performance across measures. Allen, Leany, and colleagues (2010) and Thaler and colleagues (2010) both found significant behavioral and emotional dysfunction in their impaired clusters, suggesting that subtyping cognitive performance may assist in providing insight into the functional outcome of patients. Demographic variable analyses indicate that most of the differences among clusters are independent of age, onset and mechanism of injury, and GCS scores, and so these clusters appear to capture neurocognitive differences that are not related to these characteristics. Results confirm that subtyping cognitive heterogeneity in TBI produces consistent profiles across a continuum of impairment severity. Even with this consistency, the inherent differences between intelligence and memory translate to subtle variations that may further define the nature of cognitive impairment resulting from injury. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Some of the data presented in this article were used in related cluster-analytic studies on pediatric TBI including those by Allen, Leany et al. (2010) and Thaler et al. (2010). REFERENCES Aldenderfer, M. S., & Blashfield, R. K. (1984). Cluster analysis. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Allen, D. N., Leany, B. D., Thaler, N. S., Cross, C., Sutton, G. P., & Mayfield, J. (2010). Memory and attention profiles in pediatric traumatic brain injury. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25, Allen, D. N., Thaler, N. S., Cross, C., & Mayfield, J. (2013). Classification of traumatic brain injury severity: A neuropsychological approach. In D. N. Allen & G. G. Goldstein (Eds.), Cluster analysis in neuropsychological research: Recent applications (pp ). New York, NY: Springer. Allen, D. N., Thaler, N. S., Donohue, B., & Mayfield, J. (2010). WISC- IV profiles in children with traumatic brain injury: Similarities to and differences from the WISC-III. Psychological Assessment, 22, Babikian, T., & Asarnow, R. (2009). Neurocognitive outcomes and recovery after pediatric TBI: Meta-analytic review of the literature. Neuropsychology, 23, Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., & Johnson, D. A. (1997). WAIS-R subtest pattern clusters in closed-head injured and healthy samples. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 11, Curtiss, G., Vanderploeg, R. D., Spencer, J., & Salazar, A. M. (2001). Patterns of verbal learning and memory in traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, Donders, J. (1996). Cluster subtypes in the WISC-III standardization sample: Analysis of factor index scores. Psychological Assessment, 8, Donders, J. (1999). Cluster subtypes in the standardization sample of the California Verbal Learning Test-Children s Version. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, Donders, J. (2008). Subtypes of learning and memory on the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) in the standardization sample. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, Donders, J., & Warschausky, S. (1997). WISC-III factor index score patterns after traumatic head injury in children. Child Neuropsychology, 3, Everitt, B. S., Landau, S., & Leese, M. (2001). Cluster analysis (4th ed.). London, England: Arnold. Fay, T. B., Yeates, K. O., Wade, S. L., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., & Taylor, H. G. (2009). Predicting longitudinal patterns of functional deficits in children with traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 23, Fleiss, J. L. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions (2nd ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Goldstein, G., Allen, D. N., & Caponigro, J. M. (2010). A retrospective study of heterogeneity in neurocognitive profiles associated with traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 24, doi: / Goldstein, G., Allen, D. N., & Seaton, B. E. (1998). A comparison of clustering solutions for cognitive heterogeneity in schizophrenia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, Max, J. E., Roberts, M., Koele, S. L., Lindgren, S. D., Robin, D. A., Arndt, S., & Sato, Y. (1999). Cognitive outcome in children and adolescents following severe traumatic brain injury: Influence of psychosocial, psychiatric, and injury-related variables. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, Miller, L. J., & Donders, J. (2003). Prediction of educational outcome after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, 48, doi: / Milligan, G. W., & Hirtle, S. C. (2003). Clustering and classification methods. In J. Schinka & W. Velicer (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Research methods in psychology (Vol. 2, pp ). New York, NY: Wiley. Mottram, L., & Donders, J. (2006). Cluster subtypes on the California Verbal Learning Test Children s Version after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30, Rassovsky, Y., Satz, P., Alfano, M. S., Light, R. K., Zaucha, K., McArthur, D. L., & Hovda, D. (2006). Functional outcome in TBI: I. Neuropsychological, emotional, and behavioral mediators. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, doi: / Reynolds, C. R., & Bigler, E. D. (1994). Test of Memory and Learning: Examiner s manual. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Reynolds, C. R., & Voress, J. K. (2007). Test of Memory and Learning: Second edition. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Roman, M. J., Delis, D. C., Willerman, L., Magulac, M., Demadura, T. L., de la Peña, J. L., Kracun, M. (1998). Impact of pediatric traumatic brain injury on components of verbal memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20(2), Schwartz, L., Taylor, H. G., Drotar, D., Yeates, K. O., Wade, S. L., & Stantin, T. (2003). Long-term behavior problems following pediatric traumatic brain injury: Prevalence, predictors, and correlates. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 28,

12 NEUROCOGNITIVE HETEROGENEITY IN TBI 11 Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: A practical scale. Lancet, 2, Thaler, N. S., Bello, D. T., Randall, C., Goldstein, G., Mayfield, J., & Allen, D. N. (2010). IQ profiles are associated with differences in behavioral functioning following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25, Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children- III. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Yeates, K., Armstrong, K., Janusz, J., Taylor, H., Wade, S., Stancin, T., & Drotar, D. (2005). Long-term attention problems in children with traumatic brain injury. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, doi: /01.chi Yeates, K., Swift, E., Taylor, H., Wade, S. L., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., & Minich, N. (2004). Short- and long-term social outcomes following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, doi: /s

To link to this article:

To link to this article: This article was downloaded by: [University of Notre Dame] On: 12 February 2015, At: 14:40 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

Chapter 5 Classification of Traumatic Brain Injury Severity: A Neuropsychological Approach

Chapter 5 Classification of Traumatic Brain Injury Severity: A Neuropsychological Approach Chapter 5 Classification of Traumatic Brain Injury Severity: A Neuropsychological Approach Daniel N. Allen, Nicholas S. Thaler, Chad L. Cross, and Joan Mayfield Introduction Traumatic brain injury (TBI)

More information

To link to this article:

To link to this article: This article was downloaded by: [University of Kiel] On: 24 October 2014, At: 17:27 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Memory and Attention Profiles in Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury

Memory and Attention Profiles in Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 618 633 Memory and Attention Profiles in Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury Daniel N. Allen 1, *, Brian D. Leany 1, Nicholas S. Thaler 1, Chad Cross 2, Griffin

More information

Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods

Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods Animal Ecology Publications Animal Ecology 1996 Back-Calculation of Fish Length from Scales: Empirical Comparison of Proportional Methods Clay L. Pierce National Biological Service, cpierce@iastate.edu

More information

Anne A. Lawrence M.D. PhD a a Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada Published online: 11 Jan 2010.

Anne A. Lawrence M.D. PhD a a Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada Published online: 11 Jan 2010. This article was downloaded by: [University of California, San Francisco] On: 05 May 2015, At: 22:37 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[university of Virginia] On: 26 November 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 785020474] Publisher: Informa Healthcare Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo

Costanza Scaffidi Abbate a b, Stefano Ruggieri b & Stefano Boca a a University of Palermo This article was downloaded by: [Costanza Scaffidi Abbate] On: 29 July 2013, At: 06:31 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece. Published online: 13 Nov 2014.

Dimitris Pnevmatikos a a University of Western Macedonia, Greece. Published online: 13 Nov 2014. This article was downloaded by: [Dimitrios Pnevmatikos] On: 14 November 2014, At: 22:15 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Cognitive Enhancement Using 19-Electrode Z-Score Neurofeedback

Cognitive Enhancement Using 19-Electrode Z-Score Neurofeedback This article was downloaded by: [Lucas Koberda] On: 22 August 2012, At: 09:31 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

The Flynn effect and memory function Sallie Baxendale ab a

The Flynn effect and memory function Sallie Baxendale ab a This article was downloaded by: [University of Minnesota] On: 16 August 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 917397643] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury. Seth Warschausky, PhD Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation University of Michigan

Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury. Seth Warschausky, PhD Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation University of Michigan Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury Seth Warschausky, PhD Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation University of Michigan Modules Module 1: Overview Module 2: Cognitive and Academic Needs Module

More information

Lora-Jean Collett a & David Lester a a Department of Psychology, Wellesley College and

Lora-Jean Collett a & David Lester a a Department of Psychology, Wellesley College and This article was downloaded by: [122.34.214.87] On: 10 February 2013, At: 16:46 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies

Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 363 374 Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies Reid

More information

Published online: 17 Feb 2011.

Published online: 17 Feb 2011. This article was downloaded by: [Iowa State University] On: 23 April 2015, At: 08:45 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

NANCY FUGATE WOODS a a University of Washington

NANCY FUGATE WOODS a a University of Washington This article was downloaded by: [ ] On: 30 June 2011, At: 09:44 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer

More information

Version of record first published: 25 Apr 2012.

Version of record first published: 25 Apr 2012. This article was downloaded by: [Dr William T. Tsushima] On: 23 October 2012, At: 13:21 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

Neurocognitive Correlates of the Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) in Brain Injured Children

Neurocognitive Correlates of the Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) in Brain Injured Children UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones 5-1-2017 Neurocognitive Correlates of the Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) in Brain Injured Children Abigail Rose Mayfield University

More information

Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury

Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 143 149 Criterion validity of the California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury Monica L. Jacobs, Jacobus Donders

More information

Wild Minds What Animals Really Think : A Museum Exhibit at the New York Hall of Science, December 2011

Wild Minds What Animals Really Think : A Museum Exhibit at the New York Hall of Science, December 2011 This article was downloaded by: [Dr Kenneth Shapiro] On: 09 June 2015, At: 10:40 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan Grant L. Iverson, Ph.D, Professor Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation Harvard Medical School & Red Sox

More information

Richard Lakeman a a School of Health & Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Lismore, Australia. Published online: 02 Sep 2013.

Richard Lakeman a a School of Health & Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Lismore, Australia. Published online: 02 Sep 2013. This article was downloaded by: [UQ Library] On: 09 September 2013, At: 21:23 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use: This article was downloaded by: [Chiara, Andrea Di] On: 30 December 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 931692396] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[university of Virginia] On: 26 November 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 785020474] Publisher: Informa Healthcare Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Clinical Utility of Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised and Predicted IQ Discrepancies in Closed Head Injury

Clinical Utility of Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised and Predicted IQ Discrepancies in Closed Head Injury @ Pergamon Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 12, No. 8, pp. 757 762, 1997 Copyright 1997 Nationaf Academy ofneuropsychology Printed inthe USA, All rights reserved 0887-6177/97$17.00+.00 PIIS0887-6177(97)OO049-8

More information

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD Using Neuropsychological Experts Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD Prepared for Advocate. Arizona Association for Justice/Arizona Trial Lawyers Association. September, 2011 Neurocognitive Associates 9813 North

More information

Performance discrepancies on the California Verbal Learning Test Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury

Performance discrepancies on the California Verbal Learning Test Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 23 (2008) 113 118 Brief report Performance discrepancies on the California Verbal Learning Test Second Edition (CVLT-II) after traumatic brain injury Monica L. Jacobs,

More information

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised*

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised* The Clinical Neuropsychologist -//-$., Vol., No., pp. - Swets & Zeitlinger Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised* Rodney D. Vanderploeg, John A. Schinka, Tatyana Jones, Brent J. Small,

More information

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 917 924 Abstract Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition Alison J. Donnell a, Neil Pliskin a, James Holdnack

More information

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 16 (2001) 183±191 Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples Grant L. Iverson* Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, 2255 Wesbrook

More information

Advanced Projects R&D, New Zealand b Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Online publication date: 30 March 2011

Advanced Projects R&D, New Zealand b Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Online publication date: 30 March 2011 This article was downloaded by: [University of Canterbury Library] On: 4 April 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 917001820] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in

More information

Laura N. Young a & Sara Cordes a a Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut

Laura N. Young a & Sara Cordes a a Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut This article was downloaded by: [Boston College] On: 08 November 2012, At: 09:04 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome 5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome Published in: Neuropsychologia, 2008, 47 (3), 652-656. Chapter 5 Abstract The semantic and phonemic fluency performance of adults with high functioning

More information

WAIS-R Subtest Pattern Clusters in Closed-Head-Injured and Healthy Samples*

WAIS-R Subtest Pattern Clusters in Closed-Head-Injured and Healthy Samples* The Clinical Neuropsychologist 1997, Vol. 11, No. 3, pp. 249-257 1385-4046/97/1103-249$12.00 Swets & Zeitlinger WAIS-R Subtest Pattern Clusters in Closed-Head-Injured and Healthy Samples* J.R. Crawford

More information

Predictors of Neuropsychological Test Performance After Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury

Predictors of Neuropsychological Test Performance After Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury ASSESSMENT 10.1177/1073191104268914 Donders, Nesbit-Greene / DEMOGRAPHIC VARIABLES Predictors of Neuropsychological Test Performance After Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury Jacobus Donders Mary Free Bed

More information

Marie Stievenart a, Marta Casonato b, Ana Muntean c & Rens van de Schoot d e a Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Universite

Marie Stievenart a, Marta Casonato b, Ana Muntean c & Rens van de Schoot d e a Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Universite This article was downloaded by: [UCL Service Central des Bibliothèques], [Marie Stievenart] On: 19 June 2012, At: 06:10 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

TOPF (Test of Pre-Morbid Function)

TOPF (Test of Pre-Morbid Function) TEST OF PREMORBID FUNCTIONING TOPF (Test of Pre-Morbid Function) Case Studies TOPF (Test of Pre-Morbid Function) Case Studies Case Study 1 Client C is a 62-year-old White male with 18 years of education,

More information

(Received 30 March 1990)

(Received 30 March 1990) Person, individ. Diff. Vol. II, No. 11, pp. 1153-1157, 1990 0191-8869/90 $3.00 + 0.00 Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved Copyright 1990 Pergamon Press pic ESTIMATING PREMORBID INTELLIGENCE BY

More information

Chapter Three BRIDGE TO THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES

Chapter Three BRIDGE TO THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES Chapter Three BRIDGE TO THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES Developmental Psychopathology: From Infancy through Adolescence, 5 th edition By Charles Wenar and Patricia Kerig When do behaviors or issues become pathologies?

More information

Cluster analysis of the TOMAL standardization sample

Cluster analysis of the TOMAL standardization sample UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones 5-2010 Cluster analysis of the TOMAL standardization sample Nicholas Shizuo Thaler University of Nevada Las Vegas Follow this and additional

More information

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA

Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA b University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA This article was downloaded by: [Hicks, Joshua A.][Texas A&M University] On: 11 August 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 915031380] Publisher Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered

More information

AC : NORMATIVE TYPOLOGIES OF EPICS STUDENTS ON ABET EC CRITERION 3: A MULTISTAGE CLUSTER ANALYSIS

AC : NORMATIVE TYPOLOGIES OF EPICS STUDENTS ON ABET EC CRITERION 3: A MULTISTAGE CLUSTER ANALYSIS AC 2007-2677: NORMATIVE TYPOLOGIES OF EPICS STUDENTS ON ABET EC CRITERION 3: A MULTISTAGE CLUSTER ANALYSIS Susan Maller, Purdue University Tao Hong, Purdue University William Oakes, Purdue University Carla

More information

Heterogeneity of Symptom Presentation in Sexually Abused Youth: Complex Profiles of a Complex Problem

Heterogeneity of Symptom Presentation in Sexually Abused Youth: Complex Profiles of a Complex Problem Heterogeneity of Symptom Presentation in Sexually Abused Youth: Complex Profiles of a Complex Problem Genelle K. Sawyer, Poonam Tavkar, C. Thresa Yancey, David J. Hansen, and Mary Fran Flood University

More information

A Comparison of Recall and Recognition Memory in Adults with Learning Disabilities and Acquired Brain Injured

A Comparison of Recall and Recognition Memory in Adults with Learning Disabilities and Acquired Brain Injured Neurology, Brain and Psychiatry: Open Access Received: Jan 15, 2016, Accepted: Mar 24, 2016, Published: Mar 28, 2016 Neurol Brain Psychiatry, Volume 1, Issue 1 http://crescopublications.org/pdf/nbpoa/nbpoa-1-004.pdf

More information

The merits of mental age as an additional measure of intellectual ability in the low ability range. Simon Whitaker

The merits of mental age as an additional measure of intellectual ability in the low ability range. Simon Whitaker The merits of mental age as an additional measure of intellectual ability in the low ability range By Simon Whitaker It is argued that mental age may have some merit as a measure of intellectual ability,

More information

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 20 (2005) 1043 1051 Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores Brian L.

More information

Serial 7s and Alphabet Backwards as Brief Measures of Information Processing Speed

Serial 7s and Alphabet Backwards as Brief Measures of Information Processing Speed Pergamon Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 11, No. 8, pp. 651-659, 1996 Copyright 9 1996 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All fights reserved 0887-6177/96 $15.00 +.00 PH

More information

Kyle Richard Stephenson a & Cindy M. Meston a a The University of Texas at Austin, Psychology, Austin, Texas, USA

Kyle Richard Stephenson a & Cindy M. Meston a a The University of Texas at Austin, Psychology, Austin, Texas, USA This article was downloaded by: [University of Texas at Austin] On: 16 January 2013, At: 11:58 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample

An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21 (2006) 495 501 Abstract An empirical analysis of the BASC Frontal Lobe/Executive Control scale with a clinical sample Jeremy R. Sullivan a,, Cynthia A. Riccio b

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use: This article was downloaded by: [University of Cardiff] On: 3 March 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 906511392] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

Family Assessment Device (FAD)

Family Assessment Device (FAD) Outcome Measure Sensitivity to Change Population Domain Type of Measure ICF-Code/s Description Family Assessment Device (FAD) No Paediatric and adult Family Environment Self-report d7, d9 The Family Assessment

More information

Moneyball: The Art of Winning the American Dental Association Membership Renewal Game

Moneyball: The Art of Winning the American Dental Association Membership Renewal Game This article was downloaded by: [97.73.50.115] On: 19 June 2014, At: 03:22 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Neuropsychology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Ronna Fried, Ed.D. Director of Neuropsychology in the Clinical and Research Programs in Pediatric Psychopharmacology and Adult ADHD, Massachusetts

More information

Optimizing Concussion Recovery: The Role of Education and Expectancy Effects

Optimizing Concussion Recovery: The Role of Education and Expectancy Effects Rehabilitation Institute of Michigan Optimizing Concussion Recovery: The Role of Education and Expectancy Effects Robin Hanks, Ph.D., ABPP Chief of Rehabilitation Psychology and Neuropsychology Professor

More information

10/5/2015. Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation Part I: Importance of Considering Normal Variability. Financial Disclosures

10/5/2015. Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation Part I: Importance of Considering Normal Variability. Financial Disclosures Advances in Pediatric Neuropsychology Test Interpretation: Importance of Considering Normal Variability and Performance Variability Brian L. Brooks, PhD Alberta Children s Hospital University of Calgary

More information

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content Supplementary Online Content Sun LS, Li G, Miller TLK, et al. Association between a single general anesthesia exposure before age 36 months and neurocognitive outcomes in later childhood. JAMA. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.6967

More information

Conceptualization of Functional Outcomes Following TBI. Ryan Stork, MD

Conceptualization of Functional Outcomes Following TBI. Ryan Stork, MD Conceptualization of Functional Outcomes Following TBI Ryan Stork, MD Conceptualization of Functional Outcomes Following Traumatic Brain Injury Ryan Stork, MD Clinical Lecturer Brain Injury Medicine &

More information

Neuropsychology and Metabolic Conditions: The Neurocognitive Profile of FOD/OAA and the benefits of neuropsychological assessment

Neuropsychology and Metabolic Conditions: The Neurocognitive Profile of FOD/OAA and the benefits of neuropsychological assessment Neuropsychology and Metabolic Conditions: The Neurocognitive Profile of FOD/OAA and the benefits of neuropsychological assessment Christopher Boys, PhD, LP Pediatric Neuropsychologist Associate Professor

More information

Process of a neuropsychological assessment

Process of a neuropsychological assessment Test selection Process of a neuropsychological assessment Gather information Review of information provided by referrer and if possible review of medical records Interview with client and his/her relative

More information

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist M------- P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION Name: Date of Birth: Date of Evaluation: 05-28-2015 Tests Administered: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth

More information

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments Annual Review of Cybertherapy and Telemedicine 2017 123 An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments Thomas D. PARSONS a,*, Paul SCHERMERHORN b, Timothy MCMAHAN a,

More information

Les McFarling a, Michael D'Angelo a, Marsha Drain a, Deborah A. Gibbs b & Kristine L. Rae Olmsted b a U.S. Army Center for Substance Abuse Programs,

Les McFarling a, Michael D'Angelo a, Marsha Drain a, Deborah A. Gibbs b & Kristine L. Rae Olmsted b a U.S. Army Center for Substance Abuse Programs, This article was downloaded by: [Florida State University] On: 10 November 2011, At: 13:53 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory Psychon Bull Rev (2014) 21:721 727 DOI 10.3758/s13423-013-0523-z BRIEF REPORT Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory Karen L. Siedlecki & Timothy A. Salthouse Published online:

More information

COGMED CLINICAL EVALUATION SERIES

COGMED CLINICAL EVALUATION SERIES COGMED CLIICAL EVALUATIO SERIES Cogmed Working Memory Training Pearson Clinical Assessment Part II Prepared by: Sissela utley, Ph.D. Stina Söderqvist, Ph.D. Kathryn Ralph, M.A. R&D Project Manager R&D

More information

Neuropsychology in Spina Bifida. Dr Ellen Northcott Clinical Neuropsychologist Kids Rehab, CHW

Neuropsychology in Spina Bifida. Dr Ellen Northcott Clinical Neuropsychologist Kids Rehab, CHW Neuropsychology in Spina Bifida Dr Ellen Northcott Clinical Neuropsychologist Kids Rehab, CHW Who are neuropsychologists? Undergraduate Degree (eg. BPsych, BSc, BA) Honours in Psychology Master or Doctor

More information

Verbal IQ performance IQ differentials in traumatic brain injury samples

Verbal IQ performance IQ differentials in traumatic brain injury samples Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 17 (2002) 49 56 Verbal IQ performance IQ differentials in traumatic brain injury samples Keith A. Hawkins*, Kirsten Plehn, Susan Borgaro Department of Psychiatry, Yale

More information

Common and specific impairments in attention functioning in girls with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion, fragile X or Turner syndromes.

Common and specific impairments in attention functioning in girls with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion, fragile X or Turner syndromes. Thomas Jefferson University Jefferson Digital Commons Department of Pediatrics Faculty Papers Department of Pediatrics 3-14-2014 Common and specific impairments in attention functioning in girls with chromosome

More information

EHPS 2012 abstracts. To cite this article: (2012): EHPS 2012 abstracts, Psychology & Health, 27:sup1, 1-357

EHPS 2012 abstracts. To cite this article: (2012): EHPS 2012 abstracts, Psychology & Health, 27:sup1, 1-357 This article was downloaded by: [158.197.72.142] On: 30 August 2012, At: 04:44 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,

More information

Published online: 14 Dec 2007.

Published online: 14 Dec 2007. This article was downloaded by: [University of Cambridge] On: 09 October 2014, At: 08:25 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is a major cause of mortality, cognitive and

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is a major cause of mortality, cognitive and Disorder: Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) Essay Title: Paediatric Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) Title: Associate Professor Name: Cathy Surname: Catroppa Qualifications: BBSc., DipEdPsych., M.Ed.Psych., PhD

More information

Chapter 3. Psychometric Properties

Chapter 3. Psychometric Properties Chapter 3 Psychometric Properties Reliability The reliability of an assessment tool like the DECA-C is defined as, the consistency of scores obtained by the same person when reexamined with the same test

More information

What do we know about improving later outcomes following early brain injury?

What do we know about improving later outcomes following early brain injury? What do we know about improving later outcomes following early brain injury? Liam Dorris Consultant Paediatric Neuropsychologist Royal Hospital for Sick Children Glasgow CBIT Conference Edinburgh 2013

More information

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance in Above Average and Superior School Children: Relationship to Intelligence and Age

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance in Above Average and Superior School Children: Relationship to Intelligence and Age Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vol. 13, No. 8, pp. 713 720, 1998 Copyright 1998 National Academy of Neuropsychology Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0887-6177/98 $19.00.00 PII S0887-6177(98)00007-9

More information

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module 2090 The PedsQL in Pediatric Cancer Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module James W. Varni, Ph.D. 1,2

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER FOCUSED QUESTION For individuals with memory and learning impairments due to traumatic brain injury, does use of the self-generation effect (items self-generated by the subject)

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

Sex Differences in Depression in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis

Sex Differences in Depression in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis 171 Sex Differences in Depression in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis Andrae J. Laws, McNair Scholar, Penn State University Faculty Research Advisor Dr. Peter A. Arnett, Associate Professor of Psychology

More information

Neurocognitive Correlates of the Trail Making Test for Older Children in Patients with Traumatic Brain Injury

Neurocognitive Correlates of the Trail Making Test for Older Children in Patients with Traumatic Brain Injury Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 446 452 Neurocognitive Correlates of the Trail Making Test for Older Children in Patients with Traumatic Brain Injury Nicholas S. Thaler 1, Daniel N. Allen

More information

The Short NART: Cross-validation, relationship to IQ and some practical considerations

The Short NART: Cross-validation, relationship to IQ and some practical considerations British journal of Clinical Psychology (1991), 30, 223-229 Printed in Great Britain 2 2 3 1991 The British Psychological Society The Short NART: Cross-validation, relationship to IQ and some practical

More information

Plenary Session 2 Psychometric Assessment. Ralph H B Benedict, PhD, ABPP-CN Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry SUNY Buffalo

Plenary Session 2 Psychometric Assessment. Ralph H B Benedict, PhD, ABPP-CN Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry SUNY Buffalo Plenary Session 2 Psychometric Assessment Ralph H B Benedict, PhD, ABPP-CN Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry SUNY Buffalo Reliability Validity Group Discrimination, Sensitivity Validity Association

More information

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) DSM-5 (Criteria and Major Changes for SLP-Related Conditions) Individuals meeting the criteria will be given a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder with three levels of severity based on degree of support

More information

WISC-IV Profiles in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Karen Stack, Dr. Raegan Murphy, Paula Prendeville and Dr.

WISC-IV Profiles in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Karen Stack, Dr. Raegan Murphy, Paula Prendeville and Dr. WISC-IV Profiles in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Karen Stack, Dr. Raegan Murphy, Paula Prendeville and Dr. Maria O'Halloran Background to Research Assistant Psychologist working with a specialist

More information

Concise Reference Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia Richard Keefe, Martin Lambert, Dieter Naber

Concise Reference Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia Richard Keefe, Martin Lambert, Dieter Naber Concise Reference Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia Richard Keefe, Martin Lambert, Dieter Naber Concise Reference Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia Extracted from Current Schizophrenia, Third

More information

Life Events, Social Support, and Depression Among Taiwanese Female Homemakers

Life Events, Social Support, and Depression Among Taiwanese Female Homemakers This article was downloaded by: [National Taiwan University] On: 17 November 2014, At: 18:56 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

Neuropsychological Testing (NPT)

Neuropsychological Testing (NPT) Neuropsychological Testing (NPT) POLICY Psychological testing (96101-03) refers to a series of tests used to evaluate and treat an individual with emotional, psychiatric, neuropsychiatric, personality

More information

Error in the estimation of intellectual ability in the low range using the WISC-IV and WAIS- III By. Simon Whitaker

Error in the estimation of intellectual ability in the low range using the WISC-IV and WAIS- III By. Simon Whitaker Error in the estimation of intellectual ability in the low range using the WISC-IV and WAIS- III By Simon Whitaker In press Personality and Individual Differences Abstract The error, both chance and systematic,

More information

Key Knowledge Generation Publication details, including instructions for author and Subscription information:

Key Knowledge Generation Publication details, including instructions for author and Subscription information: This article was downloaded by: Publisher: KKG Publications Registered office: 18, Jalan Kenanga SD 9/7 Bandar Sri Damansara, 52200 Malaysia Key Knowledge Generation Publication details, including instructions

More information

Youth Using Behavioral Health Services. Making the Transition from the Child to Adult System

Youth Using Behavioral Health Services. Making the Transition from the Child to Adult System Youth Using Behavioral Health Services Making the Transition from the Child to Adult System Allegheny HealthChoices Inc. January 2013 Youth Using Behavioral Health Services: Making the Transition from

More information

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 182 190 Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents Brian L. Brooks 1, *, Elisabeth

More information

Myers Psychology for AP, 2e

Myers Psychology for AP, 2e Myers Psychology for AP, 2e David G. Myers PowerPoint Presentation Slides by Kent Korek Germantown High School Worth Publishers, 2014 AP is a trademark registered and/or owned by the College Board, which

More information

Snohomish Middle School 321 West B Street Snohomish, Wa Initial Evaluation

Snohomish Middle School 321 West B Street Snohomish, Wa Initial Evaluation Angela Deering 6 th grade DOB: 1/17/1999 Age 11 Snohomish Middle School 321 West B Street Snohomish, Wa 98297 Initial Evaluation Evaluation Team: Sarah Pemble, School Psychologist Nurse Miles Ms. Truman,

More information

Robert K. Heaton, Charles G. Matthews b, Igor Grant a c & Nanci Avitable d a University of California at San Diego. Available online: 04 Jan 2008

Robert K. Heaton, Charles G. Matthews b, Igor Grant a c & Nanci Avitable d a University of California at San Diego. Available online: 04 Jan 2008 This article was downloaded by: [Kevn McGrew] On: 05 April 2012, At: 09:40 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE 5.1 GENERAL BACKGROUND Neuropsychological assessment plays a crucial role in the assessment of cognitive decline in older age. In India, there

More information

Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients

Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 21 (2006) 161 165 Effects of severe depression on TOMM performance among disability-seeking outpatients Y. Tami Yanez, William Fremouw, Jennifer Tennant, Julia Strunk,

More information

Measurement Issues in Concussion Testing

Measurement Issues in Concussion Testing EVIDENCE-BASED MEDICINE Michael G. Dolan, MA, ATC, CSCS, Column Editor Measurement Issues in Concussion Testing Brian G. Ragan, PhD, ATC University of Northern Iowa Minsoo Kang, PhD Middle Tennessee State

More information

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation University at Albany, State University of New York Scholars Archive Psychology Honors College 5-2010 Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation Michelle

More information

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 190 195 The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale Julia Novitski 1,2, Shelly Steele 2, Stella Karantzoulis 3, Christopher

More information

Gender differences in memory test performance among children and adolescents

Gender differences in memory test performance among children and adolescents Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 865 878 Gender differences in memory test performance among children and adolescents Patricia A. Lowe a, Joan W. Mayfield b, Cecil R. Reynolds c, a University

More information

Online publication date: 08 June 2010

Online publication date: 08 June 2010 This article was downloaded by: [Vrije Universiteit, Library] On: 1 June 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 907218003] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales

More information

Psychological & Neuropsychological Test

Psychological & Neuropsychological Test An Independent Licensee of the Blue Cross and Blue Shield Association Psychological & Neuropsychological Test BEACON HEALTH STRATEGIES, LLC ORIGINAL EFFECTIVE DATE HAWAII LEVEL OF CARE CRITERIA 2013 CURRENT

More information