Effects of Chronic Dietary and Repeated Acute Exposure to Chlorpyrifos on Learning and Sustained Attention in Rats

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Effects of Chronic Dietary and Repeated Acute Exposure to Chlorpyrifos on Learning and Sustained Attention in Rats"

Transcription

1 TOXICOLOGICAL SCIENCES 87(2), (2005) doi: /toxsci/kfi264 Advance Access publication July 20, 2005 Effects of Chronic Dietary and Repeated Acute Exposure to Chlorpyrifos on Learning and Sustained Attention in Rats Tracey E. Samsam, Deborah L. Hunter, and Philip J. Bushnell 1 Neurotoxicology Division, National Health and Environmental Effects Research Laboratory, Office of Research and Development, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina Received June 2, 2005; accepted July 14, 2005 Cognitive and motor impairment often follow acute poisoning with an organophosphorous (OP) pesticide. However, the persistence of these effects and the conditions necessary for their appearance are not clear: two specific concerns are whether symptomatic poisoning is necessary for persistent effects, and whether inhibition of cholinesterase (ChE) activity is a protective metric of OP exposure. This study examined the effects of chronic dietary and repeated high-level acute exposure to the pesticide chlorpyrifos (diethyl 3,5,6-trichloro-2-pyridyl phosphorothionate, CPF) on learning and attention. Beginning at 3 months of age, male Long- Evans rats received dietary CPF at a daily dose of 0, 1, or 5 mg/kg for 1 year. Half of each dietary group also received an acute oral dose of CPF (initial dose at 60 mg/kg, 5 doses at 45 mg/kg) every 2 months. Beginning 2 weeks before the fourth acute dose, behavioral assessments were conducted on the eight rats in each of the six exposure groups (0-Oil, 0-CPF, 1-Oil, 1-CPF, 5-Oil, and 5-CPF). Using an auto-shaping procedure, the groups learned to press a lever for food in the following order: 5-Oil, 5-CPF, 1-Oil, and 0-Oil. The 0-CPF and 1-CPF groups did not learn the response in three 50-trial sessions. Chronic CPF did not affect acquisition of other behaviors required by a signal detection task (SDT) designed to assess sustained attention. The sixth acute CPF dose significantly disrupted the SDT in all dosed groups. Two months after the end of dosing, performance of the SDT was impaired in the 5-CPF group. These data suggest that learning the contingency between an action and reward may be accelerated by chronic exposure to CPF and inhibited by previous symptomatic exposure to CPF, and that persistent cognitive impairment may follow if CPF exposure inhibits brain ChE activity and is accompanied by acute doses sufficient to induce signs of toxicity. Key Words: attention; learning; dietary exposure; acute effect; organophosphate; persistent effect; signal detection. Portions of this research were presented at the Society of Toxicology meeting, March This manuscript has been reviewed by the National Health and Environmental Effects Research Laboratory, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency and approved for publication. Approval does not signify that the contents reflect the views of the Agency nor does mention of trade names or commercial products constitute endorsement or recommendation for use. 1 To whom correspondence should be addressed at Neurotoxicology Division, MD B105 04, National Health and Environmental Effects Research Laboratory, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Research Triangle Park, NC Fax: (919) bushnell.philip@epa.gov. Epidemiological studies of exposure to cholinesteraseinhibiting pesticides suggest that persistent cognitive deficits can follow from overtly symptomatic poisoning with these agents (e.g., Rosenstock et al., 1991; Savage et al., 1988; Stallones and Beseler, 2002; Steenland et al., 1994; see also reviews by Jamal et al., 2002a,b). It is far less certain whether permanent impairment follows prolonged sub-clinical exposure i.e., exposure that does not induce signs or symptoms of poisoning (see Bushnell and Moser, in press; Kamel and Hoppin, 2004). For example, some studies have reported persistent cognitive deficits in farmers and termiticide applicators repeatedly exposed to organophosphates (OPs) but symptom-free (Dick et al., 2001; Steenland et al., 2000; Stephens et al., 1995, 1996), whereas other studies observed no effects in orchard workers whose exposure to pesticides was low and well controlled (Daniell et al., 1992; Karr et al., 1992). Thus, epidemiological studies alone provide inconclusive evidence to determine whether chronic, low-level exposure to OPs, without acute poisoning episodes may result in persistent deficits in occupationally exposed humans (Kamel and Hoppin, 2004). Similarly, evidence for such effects in the general population is entirely lacking. The limitations of epidemiological studies suggest the need for animal models to address the potential long-term effects of subsymptomatic exposure to these compounds. Animal models of OP intoxication have revealed deficits in cognitive and motor functions in adult rats, both after acute administration and during repeated exposures. For example, working memory was impaired for several weeks after a single subcutaneous injection of 60 or 125 mg/kg of chlorpyrifos (CPF) in male rats performing a delayed matching-to-position (DMTP) task (Bushnell et al., 1993). The pattern of effects suggested a primary effect of CPF on attention, which was subsequently confirmed (Bushnell et al., 2001). Learning deficits in a repeated-acquisition task have also been observed during repeated daily oral doses of 12.5 mg/kg of CPF (Cohn and MacPhail, 1997). However, it is rare to find studies of CPF that report cognitive and motor deficits persisting beyond the period during which cholinesterase (ChE) activity is inhibited. For example, acute subcutaneous CPF decreased trial completion, choice accuracy Published by Oxford University Press 2005.

2 EFFECTS OF CHRONIC AND ACUTE CHLORPYRIFOS 461 and response speed in an operant delayed matching-to-position (DMTP) task for about a month after dosing, and functional recovery paralleled recovery of ChE activity in blood and muscarinic receptor density in brain (Bushnell et al., 1993). Similar patterns of behavior change followed by recovery after a single dose have been reported for motor activity and behavior assessed with a functional observational battery (Mattsson et al., 1996; Moser et al., 1997; Moser and Padilla, 1998) and with a visual signal detection task (Bushnell et al., 2001). In addition, repeated dosing studies with CPF in adult animals generally have not yielded evidence for persistent effects (Bushnell et al., 1994; Cohn and MacPhail, 1997; Mattsson et al., 1996; Maurissen et al., 2000; Terry et al. 2003). Nevertheless, one study reported impaired spatial navigation in the water maze and a lack of sensitivity to amphetamineinduced place preference a year after the second of two acute treatments with CPF (Sánchez-Santed et al., 2004), as well as a number of negative results. Despite the facts (1) that the predominant route of public exposure to OPs is dietary and (2) that until recently CPF was the most commonly-used OP insecticide in the U.S., no previous studies have looked for cognitive effects of chronic ingestion of CPF, and few have sought to determine whether effects of oral CPF persist after termination of exposure. In addition, nondietary exposure to higher doses of CPF may occur when, for example, it is used locally to treat lawns and gardens for insects. Therefore, in reality, exposure typically involves combinations of long-term, chronic ingestion accompanied by periodic acute exposures to higher concentrations. Thus, the goals of this study were (1) to evaluate the effects of chronic dietary exposure to CPF; (2) to evaluate the effects of intermittent, acute oral doses of CPF given by gavage; (3) to determine potential interactions between the two dosing scenarios during exposure to the compound; (4) to determine whether these treatments caused effects that persisted beyond the termination of exposure; and (5) to identify the factors of dosing that contributed to these effects. Doses for this study were selected to cause specific degrees of inhibition of ChE (Padilla et al., 2005), rather than to reflect estimated public or occupational exposures to CPF. This approach was taken as a way to determine whether setting regulatory standards on the basis of ChE inhibition provides adequate protection against adverse functional effects of exposure to OP pesticides. That is, if functional deficits occur at doses that do not inhibit ChE activity, then a more protective marker of effect is needed. In contrast, if functional deficits occur only in the presence of significant inhibition of ChE, then ChE inhibition provides a protective marker of exposure to these compounds. By selecting doses of CPF that inhibit ChE activity only in the blood, or in both blood and brain, it is possible to judge the protectiveness of both of these markers of exposure. Based on pilot studies, a low chronic dose of CPF (nominally 1 mg/kg/day) was chosen to inhibit ChE activity in the blood, but not in the brain of rats, and a high chronic dose (nominally 5 mg/kg day) was chosen to inhibit ChE activity in both blood and in brain. Rats received these chronic doses for one year in their diet. In addition, to model episodic exposure to nondietary sources of CPF, half the rats in each chronic dose group were given CPF in corn oil by gavage at 60 mg/kg after two months of chronic exposure, and at 45 mg/kg every two months thereafter during the year of chronic dosing, for a total of six acute doses, while the other rats received corn oil vehicle. We report here the effects of these exposure scenarios in adult male rats on the acquisition and performance of a visual signal detection task designed to assess sustained attention. This task was selected because of its demonstrated sensitivity to acute CPF (Bushnell et al., 2001) and to cholinergic drugs (Bushnell et al., 1997; Rezvani and Levin, 2003). The rats began training to perform the task after 6 months of dietary exposure to CPF. This strategy enabled us to assess the effects of both chronic and acute high-level CPF exposure on the processes engaged by learning and performance of the task, starting after a steady state of ChE inhibition had been established. Testing continued until the animals reached asymptotic performance on the task (2 months after termination of dosing), to determine the persistence of any effects of CPF. Details of the CPF exposure and its neurochemical effects are reported elsewhere (Padilla et al., 2005). MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects. Male Long-Evans rats (440 total for the entire experiment) were received at 75 days of age in two cohorts of 220 animal each (Padilla et al., 2005). Forty-eight rats were selected randomly from the first cohort for the present assessments. All rats were housed individually in suspended polycarbonate cages with heat-treated pine bedding in a vivarium with a 12L:12D light schedule (lights on at 6:00 A.M.). Ambient temperature was controlled at 21 ± 2 C, and relative humidity at 50% ± 10%. The facility followed the NIH guidelines for animal care and was fully accredited by the Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care International. Water was supplied ad libitum in the home cage throughout the study. All rats were weighed once per week and maintained at 350 g ± 10 g by scheduled daily feeding throughout the study. Body weights obtained on Mondays were used to determine the amount of food to be fed each rat for the remaining weekdays; rats were fed a maintenance amount of 15 g of food per day on the weekends and holidays. Chronic CPF dosing began 1 month after arrival (Fig. 1; Padilla et al., 2005). Beginning 4 weeks prior to training (at 8.5 months of age and after 6 months of treatment), the 48 rats assigned to this study were moved to a different holding room and weighed daily (Monday through Friday). Their weights continued to be maintained at 350 g ± 10 g by scheduled daily feedings calculated according to an algorithm developed to maintain constant body weights (Ali et al., 1992). As with the larger study, 15 g of food was provided per rat per day on weekends and holidays. All rats were fed the control diet during the first 30 days, experimental CPF diets for 1 year (see below), and regular rodent chow (PMI Nutrition International, Richmond, IN) thereafter. Behavioral training and testing occurred during the light phase of the diurnal cycle, and feeding occurred in the home cage after behavioral testing. Two rats died prior to training: one rat died during acute dosing, while the cause of death of the second rat is unknown (dose groups were 1-CPF and 5-CPF, respectively).

3 462 SAMSAM, HUNTER, AND BUSHNELL FIG. 1. Exposure schedule. Rats were fed CPF at nominal concentrations of 0, 1, or 5 mg/kg body weight per day. Half of the rats in each feeding group received an acute oral dose of CPF every 2 months (Dose 1: 60 mg/kg; Doses 2 6: 45 mg/kg) while the remaining rats received the corn oil vehicle. Behavioral testing began at 10 months of age (Exposure month 7) and continued for 2 months after all CPF exposure had stopped. AS ¼ Autoshaping, VSD ¼ Visual Discrimination, SDT ¼ Signal Detection Task. Chlorpyrifos dosing. Beginning 30 days after arrival, rats were fed CPF incorporated into nutritionally-complete, grain-based, 3-g wafers (BioServ, Frenchtown, NJ) at concentrations intended to deliver doses of 0, 1, or 5 mg/kg body weight per day. The 1 mg/kg/day dose was designed to inhibit cholinesterase in the blood only, whereas the 5 mg/kg/day dose was designed to inhibit cholinesterase in both blood and brain (Padilla et al., 2005). Each lot of 3-g wafers was assayed using HPLC to determine its actual CPF concentration, and lots were accepted if the CPF concentration was within 10% of the specified level. The control diet was also analyzed to verify the absence of CPF. Thus the 3-g wafers contained CPF at nominal concentrations of 0, 23.33, and mg CPF per kg food, in the 0, 1, and 5 mg/kg dietary groups, respectively. Methods and results of the assays of 3-g wafers are detailed elsewhere (Padilla et al., 2005). In addition, half of the rats in each feeding group received an acute oral dose of CPF every 2 months, while the remaining half received corn oil vehicle at the same times (Fig. 1). For these doses, CPF was dissolved in corn oil at a concentration of 60 mg/ml for Dose 1, and at 45 mg/ml for the remaining doses. All doses were administered by gavage (1 ml/kg volume) between 9:00 and 11:00 A.M. on two consecutive days, with half of the rats in each chronic treatment group dosed each day. A low incidence of mortality following the 60 mg/kg dose was eliminated by reducing the remaining five acute doses to 45 mg/kg. The resulting exposure groups were designated by their chronic and acute conditions as 0-Oil, 0-CPF, 1-Oil, 1-CPF, 5-Oil and 5-CPF. Analysis of 45-mg pellets for chlorpyrifos. Behavioral testing required the use of 45-mg nutritionally complete pellets, which were manufactured from the same premix as the 3-g wafers (BioServ, Frenchtown, NJ). These pellets were assayed identically for CPF concentration using HPLC methods (Padilla et al., 2005). Five groups of 20 pellets each were randomly selected from each lot and weighed; the average weight per pellet was calculated to verify that the pellet weights were as specified (45 mg ± 10%). Each group of pellets was pulverized in a Micro-Mill grinder (Bel-Art Products, Pequannock, NJ) and assayed as in Padilla et al. (2005). Each lot of control diet was also analyzed to verify the absence of chlorpyrifos. Behavioral apparatus. Twelve standard operant conditioning chambers (Model E1010, Coulbourn Instruments, Lehigh Valley, PA) were each assembled as previously described (Bushnell, 1999) with a house light, an incandescent signal light, a 5-cm cone loudspeaker, and two retractable levers, one on each side of a lighted food cup with a hinged clear plastic door. Background white noise (60 db(a)) was generated in each chamber. In half of the boxes, the left lever was designated the signal lever, and the right lever as the blank lever; the converse designations were made in the other boxes. The signal light was located directly above the signal lever for initial training and was moved to above the food cup after all rats reached a criterion accuracy of 80% correct. A signal consisted of an increase in the brightness of the signal light, from a background intensity (1.22 lux) to signal intensities of 1.52, 1.91, 2.37, 2.96, 3.61, 4.54 or 5.63 lux, above the ambient illumination of the houselight (0.097 lux). The sound intensities were measured using a Bruel & Kjaer (Nærum, Denmark) sound level meter (Model #2235), and the light intensities were measured using a photometer (Model 450 with cosine probe, EG & G, Salem, MA). All aspects of stimulus presentation and data collection were accomplished using SKED-11 software (State Systems, Kalamazoo, MI) running under RSX-11M-plus on a PDP11/73 computer (Digital Equipment, Maynard, MA). Behavioral procedures. Operant training began at 10 months of age, after 34 weeks of chronic treatment, and 6 weeks after the third acute dose had been administered (Fig. 1). The rats earned most of their daily food allotment as 45-mg pellets in the operant chambers and received supplemental feedings with 3-g wafers in their home cages to maintain their body weights during all phases of training. The training schedule required that a series of behavioral tests be administered in a specific sequence. The timing of the training and dosing schedules resulted in the following conditions. All but the final two months of training were conducted during chronic dosing; autoshaping tests preceded acute dose 4; acute dose 5 was given during visual discrimination training; and acute dose 6 was given after three weeks of signal detection task (SDT) training. These last two acute doses provided an opportunity to assess the effect of acute inhibition of ChE activity on learning and performance of the SDT. Behavioral methods for each training phase are detailed below. Autoshaping. All rats began training with an autoshaping-operant method, as previously described (Bushnell, 1999; Davenport, 1974), in which the retraction of a lever beneath a visual signal is paired with the delivery of food. The lever was inserted and the signal light illuminated for 15 s on each of 50 trials in a test session. If the rat pressed the lever, the lever retracted immediately, the signal light was extinguished, and food was delivered. If the rat did not press the lever within 15 s, the lever was retracted, the signal light was extinguished, and food was delivered. Lever presses were counted in each of five 10-trial blocks in each of three test sessions. Hand-shaping was used to train rats that did not consistently press the lever under these contingencies; all rats eventually learned the task. Visual discrimination training. After each rat pressed the lever reliably, training continued with 100-trial sessions in which rats were required to report during each trial the presence or absence of the signal (a single, brief illumination of the signal light) by a single press on one of the two levers.

4 EFFECTS OF CHRONIC AND ACUTE CHLORPYRIFOS 463 Two trial types, signal and blank, were presented in equal number in each test session in a pseudorandom sequence. Signal and blank trials differed only in that no signal was presented during a blank trial. Each correct response (a press on the signal lever during a signal trial, hit, or a press on the blank lever during a blank trial, correct rejection ) was followed by the delivery of a food pellet into the illuminated food cup. After each incorrect response (a press on the signal lever during a blank trial, false alarm, or a press on the blank lever during a signal trial, miss ) or response failure, the houselight was turned off for 3 s (timeout), and no food was delivered. The criterion for advancement at each stage of training was 80% correct responding. Signal detection task. At the end of training, rats performed daily 300-trial sessions that lasted approximately 60 min. The signal duration was 300 ms and its intensity was varied randomly within each session across seven intensities (see above). A variable signal period (range, 7 to 13 s) was used to make the occurrence of the signal temporally unpredictable. Correct responses were followed by illumination of the food cup (2 s), and a food pellet was delivered after 80% of the correct responses to prevent satiation during the test session and weight gain during the study. Each incorrect response and response failure was followed by a timeout (3 s). A trial was not repeated after a response failure. These temporal parameters yielded an event rate of approximately five trials per minute. Data analysis. To quantify sustained attention in the signal detection task, the number of hits, misses, correct rejections, and false alarms were recorded for each session. The proportion of hits [P(hit) ¼ (number of hits) / (number of hits þ number of misses)] was calculated for each signal intensity, while the proportion of false alarms [P(fa) ¼ (number of false alarms) / (number of false alarms þ number of correct rejections)] was calculated for each entire session, because no signal was present on these trials. These measures served to quantify performance accuracy. All statistical analyses were performed using SAS Ó 6.12 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) and included two between-groups factors, Chronic (3 levels: 0, 1, and 5 mg/kg/day) and Acute (2 levels: Oil and CPF). These variables were crossed with within-subject variables appropriate for each assessment. For autoshaping, the number of lever presses emitted by each rat during each of the five 10-trial blocks in each of the three test sessions of autoshaping training was counted and analyzed by categorical modeling (CATMOD: SAS, 1990). For the visual discrimination training, the proportion of correct responses for each rat in each test session was analyzed by a mixed ANOVA (GLM: SAS, 1990) with test session as a repeated measure. For the assessment of asymptotic SDT performance, P(hit) values for each rat at each signal intensity were averaged across four sessions, and these averages were analyzed similarly with signal intensity as a repeated measure (GLM: SAS, 1990). The P(fa) values for each rat were averaged across the four sessions, and these averages were analyzed (GLM: SAS, 1990) with no repeated measure. Step-down ANOVAs were performed to characterize significant interactions among the independent variables in each assessment. After significant Chronic by Acute interactions, one-way step-down ANOVAs for dose were performed for each acute condition. In all analyses, Huynh-Feldt degree of freedom (df) corrections were used to reduce the effects of asymmetrical variance-covariance matrices for repeated measures. The criterion for significance was set at 0.05 for all tests. RESULTS Analysis of 45-mg CPF Pellets Although made from the same premix as the 3-g wafers, the pellets did not contain the same CPF concentrations. Assay of the pellets yielded concentrations of 0, 16.37, and mg CPF per kg of food in the 0, 1, and 5 mg/kg dietary groups respectively (concentrations in the wafers were within 10% of their nominal values of 0, 23, and 117 mg/kg, respectively). When multiplied by daily food consumption (apportioned between 45-mg pellets and 3-g wafers each day) and divided by the body weights of the rats, these concentrations yielded the daily doses of CPF shown in Table 1. Autoshaping The groups differed in acquisition of the lever press response. Whereas the main effects of Chronic and Acute were not significant, the main effect of Block [v 2 (11) ¼ , p ¼ ] and the interactions of Chronic by Block [v 2 (22) ¼ 42.60, p ¼ ], Acute by Block [v 2 (11) ¼ 21.49, p ¼ ], and Chronic by Acute by Block [v 2 (22) ¼ 84.15, p ¼ ] were significant. In the groups not acutely dosed with CPF, acquisition of the response was facilitated by chronic CPF in a dose-related fashion (Fig. 2). In those groups that were acutely dosed with CPF, the 5-CPF group learned the lever press response, while the 0-CPF and 1-CPF groups did not learn the response during the three sessions of autoshaping (Fig. 2). Signal Detection Task The effects of dietary CPF during autoshaping did not transfer to learning the subsequent visual discrimination needed for the SDT, as there were no significant differences among the groups during this period between acute doses (Fig. 3). The last acute dose of CPF was given at the end of the year of chronic dosing. Prior to this dose, baseline accuracy did not differ between the groups. Acute CPF reduced P(hit) in all dosed groups (Fig. 4) [Acute main effect F(1, 38) ¼ 12.92, p ¼ ; Acute by Day interaction F(3, 114) ¼ 19.76, e ¼ , p ¼ ]; the Chronic effect and the Acute by Chronic interactions were not significant. P(fa) was significantly increased in all acute groups [Acute main effect F(1, 38) ¼ 11.13, p ¼ ; Acute by Day interaction F(3, 114) ¼ 9.34, e ¼ , p ¼ ] (Fig. 4). The Chronic effect and the Acute by Chronic interactions were not significant for P(fa). Asymptotic performance of the SDT was achieved 2 months after all dosing had stopped. Examination of functions relating P(hit) to intensity at this stage showed no significant differences related to either chronic CPF or to the acute doses (Figs. 5A, 5B). In contrast, P(fa) was differentially affected by acute and chronic treatments [Chronic by Acute interaction F(2,38) ¼ 4.61, p ¼ ]. The step-down analysis revealed that P(fa) was significantly higher in the 5-CPF group compared to the 0-CPF and 1-CPF groups, whereas there was no difference in P(fa) among the 0-Oil, 1-Oil, and 5-Oil groups. DISCUSSION Four major conclusions can be drawn from these results: First, chronic exposure to CPF at doses that inhibited ChE activity in blood (to ~10% of control) and brain (to ~50% of

5 464 SAMSAM, HUNTER, AND BUSHNELL TABLE 1 Food and Chlorpyrifos Consumed Acute condition Oil CPF Chronic dose condition (mg/kg/day) Average weight of rat (g) Day 1 Day 30 a Day 31 Day Day 224 Day Day 409 End of Exp Food consumed per rat (g/day) Day 1 Day 30 a Day 31 Day 223 b Day 224 Day 408 c Day 409 End of Exp CPF concentration in 3-g wafer (mg/kg) b CPF concentration in 45-mg pellets (mg/kg) c CPF consumed in food per rat (mg/kg/day) Day 1 Day Day 31 Day Day 224 Day Day 409 End of Exp Note. Summary of daily consumption of food and chlorpyrifos per rat by exposure condition. Behavioral testing began on Day 224. a Assumed daily body weight of 350 g, so that amount fed per day was equal to 15 g. b Rats received 3 g wafers during Days c During Days , rats earned most of their daily food allotment as 45 mg pellets during testing and received supplemental feedings with 3 g wafers in their home cages. FIG. 2. Acquisition of the lever-press response by autoshaping. Five 10-trial blocks were administered during each of three 50-trial test sessions. Values are median frequencies of lever presses per 10-trial block. Training by autoshaping was terminated when the rats were given their fourth high-level acute dose of CPF. Training resumed by hand-shaping 2 days after administration of that dose. The number of animals in each group that learned the task during the three autoshaping sessions was: 0 mg/kg-oil: 6 of 8; 1 mg/kg-oil: 5 of 8; 5 mg/kg-oil: 6 of 8; 0 mg/kg-cpf: 3 of 8; 1 mg/kg-cpf: 3 of 7; 5 mg/kg-cpf: 6 of 7. control) (McCollister et al., 1974; Padilla et al., 2005), but caused no visible signs of intoxication (Moser et al., 2005), did not affect learning or sustained attention in rats, either during or after a year of treatment. Second, acute doses of CPF that were sufficient to induce signs of cholinergic toxicity (Moser et al., 2005) impaired learning and sustained attention. Third, a subtle deficit in the accuracy of signal detection was seen two months after termination of dosing in the group of rats most highly exposed to CPF (5 mg/kg/day plus six acute doses). Fourth, the facilitative effects of ongoing chronic exposure to CPF on learning were reversed by acute exposure to CPF. The first conclusion does not support epidemiological evidence (Dick et al., 2001; Steenland et al., 2000; Stephens et al., 1995) that chronic, subclinical exposure to OP pesticides leads to later cognitive deficits. Rather, this observation is more consistent with reports that such exposure does not lead to measurable cognitive sequelae (e.g., Daniell et al., 1992). This conclusion also speaks to the issue of using ChE inhibition as a marker of exposure to ChE-inhibiting pesticides. Both the acute doses and the 5 mg/kg chronic dose inhibited ChE activity in the brains of other rats in this study, whereas the 1 mg/kg chronic dose inhibited ChE activity in blood but not in brain (Padilla et al., 2005). The absence of behavioral effects

6 EFFECTS OF CHRONIC AND ACUTE CHLORPYRIFOS 465 FIG. 3. Acquisition of the visual discrimination required for the signal detection task for the six groups of animals. Accuracy of the groups did not differ significantly during acquisition of the task. Values are mean (±SEM) proportion of correct responses in each 100-trial test session. of the 1 mg/kg chronic CPF dose (present data and Moser et al., 2005) suggests that this level of exposure is insufficient to affect cognitive function in rats, and that using blood ChE activity as a marker of exposure may provide a conservative estimate of exposure for these effects. In contrast, inhibition of autoshaping was observed 6 weeks after the third of three acute FIG. 4. Effect of the sixth acute dose of CPF on the accuracy of visual signal detection. The four chronically-dosed groups continued to receive dietary CPF during this time. Animals received 45 mg/kg of CPF or corn oil vehicle by gavage, but were not tested, on Day 0. Baseline accuracy did not differ between treatment groups prior to the dose (Test Days ÿ4 toÿ1). P(hit) was significantly reduced and P(fa) significantly increased on Test Days þ1 and þ2 in all three groups dosed acutely with CPF. Values are mean (±SEM) proportions of responses, either of hits [correct detections of the signal, P(hit)] or false alarms [incorrect reports that a signal had occurred, P(fa)] doses of mg/kg (Fig. 2), and impaired sustained attention was seen 2 months after exposure to six acute doses and the 5 mg/kg chronic dose (Fig. 5). These observations suggest that persistent, detrimental sequelae may follow a CPF exposure scenario that includes repeated acute doses that inhibit ChE activity in blood to less than 10% of control (Padilla et al., 2005) and cause overt signs of intoxication (Moser et al., 2005), with or without chronic inhibition of ChE activity. However, because ChE activity was not inhibited when the last behavioral effects were noted (Fig. 5; Moser and Padilla, 1998; Padilla et al., 2005), explanation of observed effects cannot rely solely on current measurements of ChE activity, but require knowledge of the exposure history as well. Second, the effects of acute high-level doses are entirely consistent with previous studies of the acute effects of CPF (Bushnell et al., 1993, 2001; Mattsson et al., 1996; Moser and Padilla, 1998), which demonstrate behavioral disruption in parallel with inhibition of ChE activity. These observations confirm that the tests used in this study are sensitive to the acute effects of high doses of CPF and show that they are not sensitive to chronic dietary exposure at the doses administered here. It should also be noted that the parameters of the test at this stage of training (one signal intensity) did not permit us to evaluate potential shifts in visual threshold associated with CPF-induced miosis, as we have previously observed (Bushnell et al., 2001). Indeed, the pattern of deficit in Figure 4 (increased false alarms along with decreased hits) is more consistent with a deficit in attention than with a change in threshold (Bushnell, 1998). Third, the subtle deficit observed in the 5-CPF group 2 months after termination of CPF exposure involved a slight decrease in P(hit) and a significant increase in P(fa). In terms of the theory of signal detection (Green and Swets, 1974), these changes reflect a reduction in sensitivity and a relaxation of the response criterion; that is, more events both signal and noise events were reported as signals by the rats in the 5-CPF group, thus increasing their incorrect reports that a signal had occurred. To the extent that discriminating signals from noise requires attention to the test conditions, this pattern can be interpreted as an impairment of sustained attention (Bushnell, 1998). This change very likely represents a persistent effect of treatment, because previous work has shown that effects of acute CPF on signal detection recover within 8 days after 50 mg/kg of CPF orally, and 4 weeks after 250 mg/kg subcutaneously (Bushnell et al., 2001). Further, effects of acute oral CPF on unconditioned behaviors and neurochemical indices of intoxication returned to normal within 2 weeks after doses of 20 or 80 mg/kg (Moser et al., 1998; Moser and Padilla, 1998). Fourth, the effects of CPF on acquisition of the lever-press response in the autoshaping test suggested that chronic and acute exposures of CPF exerted opposite effects on learning. That is, chronic exposure to dietary CPF facilitated the acquisition of the lever-press response in a dose-related manner. In contrast, three previous acute high-level doses of CPF, 2 months apart and 1 to 5 months prior to the autoshaping test

7 466 SAMSAM, HUNTER, AND BUSHNELL FIG. 5. Accuracy of signal detection in rats during asymptotic performance of the signal detection task. All exposure to CPF had ceased 2 months prior to this assessment. (A) Chronic dietary CPF with acute oral doses of corn oil vehicle. (B) Chronic dietary CPF with acute doses of CPF. P(hit) was not significantly affected by either acute or chronic treatment. P(fa) was significantly increased in the 5-CPF group compared to the 0-CPF and 1-CPF groups, whereas there was no difference in P(fa) among the Oil groups. Values are mean (±SEM) proportions of responses, either of hits [correct detections of the signal, P(hit)] or false alarms [incorrect reports that a signal had occurred, P(fa)], plotted as a function of the intensity of the signal. Because false alarms occur on trials lacking a signal, P(fa) values are not related to signal intensity and were calculated for all nonsignal trials combined. sessions, suppressed acquisition of the response in control rats and in those rats receiving chronic dietary CPF at 1 mg/kg/day. Each of these three acute doses of CPF had inhibited ChE activity to about 10% of control in blood and 25% of control in brain. These prior treatments did not, however, prevent acquisition of the lever press response in rats receiving dietary CPF at 5 mg/kg/day. This pattern suggests that a history of intermittent acute doses of oral CPF blocked an enhancing effect of chronic dietary CPF on the behavioral processes engaged by autoshaping. The possibility that the facilitation of autoshaping by chronic CPF was mediated by inhibition of ChE activity in the CNS cannot be discounted, given the cognitive enhancement afforded by other ChE inhibitors in animals (e.g., van der Staay and Bouger, 2005) and the therapeutic benefit provided by ChE inhibitors in the treatment of human dementia (e.g., Doody, 2003; Giacobini, 2003). On the other hand, facilitation of autoshaping has previously been associated with exposure to other toxicants, including p-xylene (Bushnell, 1988), colchicine (Nanry et al., 1989), ethanol (Tomie et al., 1998), and Aroclor 1254 (Geller et al., 2001). In the case of p-xylene, the facilitation was eliminated by increasing the force required to press the response lever, providing evidence that poor motor control increased the likelihood that the intoxicated rats operated the lever while exploring it early in training. In this scenario, the intoxicated rats experienced the contingency between movement of the lever and food reward after fewer trials than did unintoxicated rats and, once this contingency was experienced, acquired the press response at a normal rate (i.e., the slopes of the acquisition curves were not affected by treatment) (Bushnell, 1988). A similar pattern was observed in this study, in that the effects of CPF appeared as a change in the number of trials given before pressing began, whereas the rate of acquisition was not altered (Fig. 2). This interpretation is also consistent with other reports. For example, Nanry et al. (1989) suggested that an increase in locomotor activity or reactivity to the experimental stimuli contributed to a facilitated acquisition in colchicine-treated rats. Similarly, Tomie et al. (1998) attributed facilitation of autoshaping by ethanol to a drug-induced enhancement of impulsiveness, causing the treated rats to press the lever more freely and thus acquire the response earlier than controls. Alterations in motor function, including reduced locomotor activity (Mattsson et al., 1996; Moser, 1996; Moser et al., 1997, Pope et al., 1992), slowing of responding (Bushnell et al., 1993, 1994; Maurissen et al., 2000), and reduced response rates (Cohn and MacPhail, 1997), have been associated with CPF treatment in rats. These effects of CPF, together with the effects of other neurotoxicants on autoshaping (Bushnell, 1988; Geller et al., 2001; Nanry et al., 1989; Tomie et al., 1998), suggest the possibility that the facilitation of lever-press acquisition observed here may also have been mediated by CPF-induced

8 EFFECTS OF CHRONIC AND ACUTE CHLORPYRIFOS 467 motor dysfunction, though direct evidence for this supposition was not obtained in this study. The fact that dietary CPF did not affect the subsequent training of visual discrimination is consistent with the hypothesis of chronic CPF-induced motor dysfunction. Acquisition of the discrimination necessary for the SDT does not involve learning new motor skills, but rather involves associating the presence or absence of the signal light with the location of the reinforced response. This lack of effect on associative learning is also consistent with observations of rats exposed to p-xylene, who were not impaired in automaintained reversal learning, despite facilitated autoshaping (Bushnell, 1988). Nevertheless, acute high-level oral exposure to CPF in this study did disrupt signal detection in a manner similar to that previously observed (Bushnell et al. 2001). That is, P(hit) was reduced, P(fa) was increased, and response times were prolonged for several days after the last acute dose. This behavioral pattern reflects a deficit in sustained attention, as we have proposed elsewhere (Bushnell, 1998; Bushnell et al., 1997). The lower concentration of CPF in the 45-mg pellets fed to these rats during the behavioral tests reduced the daily dose of CPF from the nominal values of 1 and 5 mg/kg/day to 0.7 and about 3.6 mg/kg/day, depending on the group (Days in Table 1). This exposure very probably reduced the degree of inhibition of ChE activity in these rats relative to the inhibition measured after the first 6 months of exposure in other rats, and relative to inhibition in the rats maintained on 3-g wafers throughout the study (Padilla et al., 2005). The degree of inhibition of brain ChE activity expected from the reduced dose can be estimated from a study by McCollister et al. (1974), who dosed rats daily with CPF for 2 years and reported that CPF at 3 mg/kg/day inhibited brain ChE to 44% of control after 12 months of dosing. This value falls between those observed in the 1 and 5 mg/kg/day groups, at the end of this study (Padilla et al., 2005), and suggests that the brain ChE activity in the 5-Oil and 5-CPF groups in this report may be estimated at about 50% during the 6 months of treatment and behavioral testing. Because Padilla et al. saw no significant inhibition of brain ChE activity in rats dosed daily at 1 mg/kg of CPF, it is very unlikely that brain ChE was inhibited in the 1-Oil and 1- CPF groups either. In conclusion, these data suggest that mild cognitive deficits might be expected to follow chronic exposure to CPF, if the dose level is high enough to inhibit ChE activity in the brain (to about 50% of control), and if this chronic exposure is also accompanied by intermittent exposure to doses that cause signs of intoxication. The fact that these observations were made 2 months after termination of exposure indicates that they persisted beyond the inhibition of ChE activity, because biochemical indices of exposure to oral CPF in rats recover within 2 weeks after dosing (Moser and Padilla, 1998; Moser et al., 1998). The fact that P(fa) was not increased in either the 0-CPF or the 5-Oil group indicates that neither repeated acute exposure nor chronic exposure alone was sufficient to cause this effect, and that the effects of the two treatments were somehow additive. Conversely, the interaction of chronic and acute dosing on autoshaping suggests antagonistic effects of the two dose scenarios, with acute high-level exposure impairing acquisition of the lever-press response, and chronic exposure facilitating it. Summary Three acute, high-level oral doses of CPF (one of 60 mg/kg, two of 45 mg/kg) administered during the first 6 months of chronic exposure impaired acquisition of a lever-press response for food that began 1 month after the third acute dose. In the same test, chronic dietary CPF (1 and 5 mg/kg/day) facilitated acquisition of the lever press response; the 5mg/kg chronic dose also ameliorated the effect of the three acute doses on lever-press acquisition. Later in training, the sixth acute oral dose of CPF (45 mg/kg) transiently reduced the accuracy of visual signal detection, and this effect was not ameliorated by chronic CPF. The combination of the 5 mg/kg/day chronic dose and the six acute doses caused a subtle deficit in asymptotic performance of the signal detection task during tests conducted two months after all CPF exposure had stopped. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Renée S. Marshall for her diligence in managing this project; Wendy M. Oshiro, Katherine L. McDaniel, and Pamela M. Phillips for technical assistance; Kay Rigsbee for animal care; Charles Hamm for engineering support; and Drs. Stephanie Padilla, Suzanne McMaster, and William Boyes for thoughtful reviews of the manuscript. REFERENCES Ali, J. S., Olszyk, V. B., Dunn, D. D., Lee, K. A., Kendall, S. M., Rhoderick, R. R., and Bushnell, P. J. (1992). A Lotus based system for recording and maintaining body weight of laboratory animals. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 24, Bushnell, P. J. (1988). Behavioral effects of acute p-xylene inhalation in rats: Autoshaping, motor activity, and reversal learning. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 10, Bushnell, P. J. (1998). Behavioral approaches to the assessment of attention in animals. Psychopharmacology 138, Bushnell, P. J. (1999). Advanced behavioral testing in rodents. In Current Protocols in Toxicology, Unit 12.4, Wiley, New York. Bushnell, P. J., Kelly, K. L., and Ward T. R. (1994). Repeated inhibition of cholinesterase by chlorpyrifos in rats: Neurochemical, pharmacological, and behavioral indices of tolerance. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap. 270, Bushnell, P. J., and Moser V. C. Behavioral toxicology of cholinesterase inhibitors. In Toxicology of Organophosphate and Carbamate Pesticides (R. Gupta, Ed.), Elsevier, New York (in press). Bushnell, P. J., Moser, V. C., and Samsam, T. E. (2001). Effects of acute chlorpyrifos on visual signal detection and a neurobehavioral test battery in rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 23, Bushnell, P. J., Oshiro, W. M., and Padnos, B. K. (1997). Effects of chlordiazepoxide and cholinergic and adrenergic drugs on sustained attention in rats. Psychopharmacology 134,

9 468 SAMSAM, HUNTER, AND BUSHNELL Bushnell, P. J., Pope, C. N., and Padilla, S. (1993). Behavioral and neurochemical effects of acute chlorpyrifos in rats: Tolerance to prolonged inhibition of cholinesterase. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap. 266, Cohn, J., and MacPhail, R. C. (1997). Chlorpyrifos produces selective learning deficits in rats working under a schedule of repeated acquisition and performance. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap. 283, Daniell, W. E., Barnhart, S., Demers, P., Costa, L. G., Eaton, D. L., Miller, M., and Rosenstock, L. (1992). Neuropsychological performance among agricultural pesticide applicators. Envir. Res. 59, Davenport, J. W. (1974). Combined autoshaping-operant (AO) training: CS- UCS interval effects in rats. Bull. Psychon. Sci. 3, Dick, R. B., Steenland, K., Krieg, E. F., and Hines, C. J. (2001). Evaluation of acute sensory-motor effects and test sensitivity using termiticide workers exposed to chlorpyrifos. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 23, Doody, R. S. (2003). Current treatments for Alzheimer s disease: Cholinesterase inhibitors. J. Clin. Psychiatry 64 (Suppl. 9), Geller, A. M., Oshiro, W. M., Haykal-Coates, N., Kodavanti, P. R. S., and Bushnell, P. J. (2001). Gender-dependent behavioral and sensory effects of a commercial mixture of polychlorinated biphenyls (Aroclor 1254) in rats. Toxocol. Sci. 59, Giacobini, E. (2003). Cholinergic function and Alzheimer s disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18 (Suppl. 1), S1 S5. Green, D. M., and Swets, J. A. (1974). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. Robert E. Krieger Publishing Company, Huntington, New York. Jamal, G. A., Hansen, S., and Julu, P. O. O. (2002a). Low level exposures to organophosphorus esters may cause neurotoxicity. Toxicology , Jamal, G. A., Hansen, S., Pilkington, A., Buchanan, D., Gillham, R. A., Abdel- Azis, M., Julu, P. O. O., Al-Rawas, S. F., Hurley, F., and Ballantyne, J. P. (2002b). A clinical neurological, neurophysiological, and neuropsychological study of sheep farmers and dippers exposed to organophosphate pesticides. Occup. Environ. Med. 59, Kamel, F., and Hoppin, J. A. (2004). Association of pesticide exposure with neurologic dysfunction and disease. Environ. Health Perspect. 112, Karr, C., Demers, P., Costa, L. G., Daniell, W. E., Barnhart, S., Miller, M., Gallagher, G., Horstman, S. W., Eaton, D. L., and Rosenstock, L. (1992). Organophosphate pesticide exposure in a group of Washington State orchard applicators. Environ. Res. 59, Mattsson, J. L., Wilmer, J. W., Shankar, M. R., Berdasco, N. M., Crissman, J. W., Maurissen, J. P., and Bond, D. M. (1996). Single-dose and 13-week repeated-dose neurotoxicity screening studies of chlorpyrifos insecticide. Food Chem. Toxicol. 34, Maurissen, J. P. J., Shankar, M. R., and Mattsson, J. L. (2000). Chlorpyrifos: Lack of cognitive effects in adult Long-Evans rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 22, McCollister, S. B., Kociba, R. J., Humiston, C. G., McCollister, D. D., and Gehring, P. J. (1974). Studies of the acute and long-term oral toxicity of chlorpyrifos (O,O-diethyl-O-( 3,5,6-trichloro-2-pyridyl) phosphorothioate). Food Cosmet. Toxicol. 12, Moser, V. C. (1996). Comparisons of the acute effects of cholinesterase inhibitors using a neurobehavioral screening battery in rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 17, Moser, V. C., Chanda, S. M., Mortensen, S. R., and Padilla, S. (1998). Age- and gender-related differences in sensitivity to chlorpyrifos in the rat reflect developmental profiles of esterase activities. Toxicol. Sci. 46, Moser, V. C., McDaniel, K. L., and Phillips, P. M. (1997). Neurobehavioral effects of low-level repeated exposure to chlorpyrifos in adult rats. Toxicologist 36, 345. Moser, V. C., and Padilla, S. (1998). Age- and gender-related differences in the time course of behavioral and biochemical effects produced by oral chlorpyrifos in rats. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 149, Moser, V. C., Phillips, P. M., McDaniel, K. L., Marshall, R. S., Hunter, D. L., and Padilla, S. (2005). Neurobehavioral effects of chronic dietary and repeated high-level spike exposure to chlorpyrifos in rats. Toxicol. Sci. 86, Nanry, K. P., Mundy, W. R., and Tilson, H. A. (1989). Colchicine-induced alterations of reference memory in rats: Role of spatial versus non-spatial task components. Behav. Brain Res. 35, Padilla, S., Marshall, R. S., Hunter, D. L., Oxendine, S., Moser, V. C., Southerland, S. B. and Mailman, R. B. (2005). Neurochemical effects of chronic dietary and repeated high-level acute exposure to chlorpyrifos in rats. Toxicol. Sci. doi: /toxsci/kfi274. Pope, C. N., Chakraborti, T. K., Chapman, M. L., and Farrar, J. D. (1992). Long-term neurochemical and behavioral effects induced by acute chlorpyrifos treatment. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 42, Rezvani, A. H., and Levin, E. D. (2003). Nicotine-alcohol interactions and attentional performance on an operant visual signal detection task in female rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 76, Rosenstock, L., Kiefer, M., Daniell, W. E., McConnel, R., Claypoole, K., and the Pesticide Health Effects Study Group. (1991). Chronic central nervous system effects of acute organophosphate pesticide intoxication. Lancet 338, Sánchez-Santed, F. Cañadas, F., Flores, P., López-Francha, M., and Cardona, D. (2004). Long-term functional neurotoxicity of paraoxon and chlorpyrifos: Behavioural and pharmacological evidence. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 26, Savage, E. P., Keefe, T. J., Mounce, L. M., Heaton, R. K., Lewis, J. A., and Burcar, P. J. (1988). Chronic neurological sequelae of acute organophosphate pesticide poisoning. Arch. Envir. Health. 43, Stallones, L., and Beseler, C. (2002). Pesticide illness, farm practices, and neurological symptoms among farm residents in Colorado. Envir. Res. 90, Steenland, K., Dick, R. B., Howell, R. J., Chrislip, D. W., Hines, C. J., Reid, T. M., Lehman, E., Laber, P., Krieg, E. F., and Knott, C. (2000). Neurologic function among termiticide applicators exposed to chlorpyrifos. Envir. Health Perspect. 108, Steenland, K., Jenkins, B., Ames, R. G., O Malley, M., Chrislip, D., and Russo, J. (1994). Chronic neurological sequelae to organophosphate pesticide poisoning. Am. J. Public Health. 84, Stephens, R., Spurgeon, A., and Berry, H. (1996). Organophosphates: The relationship between chronic and acute exposure effects. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 18, Stephens, R., Spurgeon, A., Calvert, I. A., Beach, J., Levy, L. S., Berry, H., and Harrington, J. M. (1995). Neuropsychological effects of long-term exposure to organophosphate in sheep dip. Lancet 345, Terry, A. V. Jr., Stone, J. D., Buccafusco, J. J., Sicles, D. W., Sood, A., and Prendergast, M. A. (2003). Repeated exposures to subthreshold doses of chlorpyrifos in rats: Hippocampal damage, impaired axonal transport, and deficits in spatial learning. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 305, Tomie, A., Cunha, C., Mosakowski, E. M., Quartarolo, N. M., Pohorecky, L. A., and Benjamin, D. (1998). Effects of ethanol on Pavlovian autoshaping in rats. Psychopharmacology 139, Van der Staay, F. J., and Bouger, P. C. (2005). Effects of the cholinesterase inhibitors donepezil and metrifonate on scopolamine-induced impairments in the spatial cone field orientation task in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 156, 1 10.

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Effects of Tetrachloroethylene on Visual and Cognitive Function in Rats

Effects of Tetrachloroethylene on Visual and Cognitive Function in Rats Effects of Tetrachloroethylene on Visual and Cognitive Function in Rats Philip J. Bushnell William K. Boyes Vernon A. Benignus Elaina M. Kenyon Wendy M. Oshiro For the National Research Council Committee

More information

Assessment of chlorpyrifos and lead acetate combination on neurobehavioral aspects in Wistar rats after subchronic dietary exposure

Assessment of chlorpyrifos and lead acetate combination on neurobehavioral aspects in Wistar rats after subchronic dietary exposure Assessment of chlorpyrifos and lead acetate combination on neurobehavioral aspects in Wistar rats after subchronic dietary exposure 1 H Krishna*, 2 AV Ramachandran 1 Dhirubhai Ambani Life Sciences Centre,

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys

Effects of a Novel Fentanyl Derivative on Drug Discrimination and Learning in Rhesus Monkeys PII S0091-3057(99)00058-1 Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, Vol. 64, No. 2, pp. 367 371, 1999 1999 Elsevier Science Inc. Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0091-3057/99/$ see front matter Effects

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

AN ACUTE ORAL TOXICITY STUDY IN RATS WITH ADVANTRA Z

AN ACUTE ORAL TOXICITY STUDY IN RATS WITH ADVANTRA Z AN ACUTE ORAL TOXICITY STUDY IN RATS WITH ADVANTRA Z AMENDED FINAL REPORT Author Deborah A Douds, M.S. Original Study Completion Date July 8, 1997 Amended Study Completion Date November 19, 1997 Performing

More information

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction

postreinforcement pause for a minute or two at the beginning of the session. No reduction PUNISHMENT A ND RECO VER Y D URING FIXED-RA TIO PERFORMA NCE' NATHAN H. AZRIN2 ANNA STATE HOSPITAL When a reinforcement is delivered according to a fixed-ratio schedule, it has been found that responding

More information

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1985, 439 235-241 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES LANNY FIELDS THE COLLEGE OF

More information

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience Dopamine and Learning Michael Dorris Associate Professor of Physiology & Neuroscience Studies dorrism@biomed.queensu.ca http://brain.phgy.queensu.ca/dorrislab/ NSCI 324 Systems

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques

Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques C O N F I D E N T I A L Delayed Matching-To-Sample Test in Macaques DATE This study was conducted under the terms of a Materials Transfer and Services Agreement between NeuroDetective International and

More information

I. Classical Conditioning

I. Classical Conditioning Learning Chapter 8 Learning A relatively permanent change in an organism that occur because of prior experience Psychologists must study overt behavior or physical changes to study learning Learning I.

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson, H Djafari Marbini, AJ Toner, TW Robbins, BJ Everitt Departments of

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer RN Cardinal, JA Parkinson *, TW Robbins, A Dickinson, BJ Everitt Departments of Experimental

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF TOXICOLOGY

GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF TOXICOLOGY GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF TOXICOLOGY Laboratory of toxicology Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology College of Pharmacy, University of Baghdad 2015 TOXICOLOGY - PRACTICAL SYLLABUS 2015 General Principles

More information

Neurotoxicological and Statistical Analyses of a Mixture of Five Organophosphorus Pesticides Using a Ray Design

Neurotoxicological and Statistical Analyses of a Mixture of Five Organophosphorus Pesticides Using a Ray Design TOXICOLOGICAL SCIENCES 86(1), 11 115 () doi:1.193/toxsci/kfi163 Advance Access publication March 3, Neurotoxicological and Statistical Analyses of a Mixture of Five Organophosphorus Pesticides Using a

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

! " #$ %! &! ' ( ) * + ( (, -!! )! ". " " / ),

!  #$ %! &! ' ( ) * + ( (, -!! )! .   / ), !"#$ %!&! '( )*+ ((,-!!)!"." "/),01-2234 DISPOSITION INSTRUCTIONS: Destroy this report when no longer needed. Do not return to the originator. DISCLAIMERS: The views of the authors do not reflect the position

More information

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's JOURNAt, OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1968, 1 1, 1-8 NUMBER I (JANUARY) A UTO-SHAPING OF THE PIGEON'S KEY-PECK' PAUL L. BROWN AND HERBERT M. JENKINS MCMASTER UNIVERSITY Reliable acquistion

More information

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING Pergamon Neuroscience Vol. 83, No. 3, pp. 837 841, 1998 Copyright 1998 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved PII: S0306-4522(97)00457-0 0306 4522/98 $19.00+0.00

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

Acute Oral Toxicity Study in Rats with Argentyn 23 (EPA/OECD Guidelines) Study Title. Monica M. Vegarra, BS. Sponsor

Acute Oral Toxicity Study in Rats with Argentyn 23 (EPA/OECD Guidelines) Study Title. Monica M. Vegarra, BS. Sponsor Note: This study was done using our Professional strength 23 ppm product (Argentyn 23), therefore making Sovereign Silver at 10 ppm, 2.3 times safer. Conclusion on page 6 shows the product to be non-toxic

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning Operant Conditioning The nature of reinforcement: Premack s probability differential theory Response deprivation

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

ESTIMATION OF TOXICITY TO HUMANS

ESTIMATION OF TOXICITY TO HUMANS 240 TABLE 21-5. (continued) Mammalian bone marrow chromosome aberration test Mammalian erythrocyte micronucleus test Rodent dominant lethal assay Rodent heritable translocation assays Bacterial DNA damage

More information

VASILE HEFCO 1*, LUCIAN HRITCU 1, ADRIAN TIRON 1, ANDREEA-IOANA HEFCO 1

VASILE HEFCO 1*, LUCIAN HRITCU 1, ADRIAN TIRON 1, ANDREEA-IOANA HEFCO 1 THE EFFECTS OF NICOTINIC TREATMENT ON MEMORY AND LEARNING IMPAIRMENT INDUCED BY BLOCKADE OF MUSCARINIC ACETYLCHOLINE RECEPTORS ON PERFORMANCE IN RADIAL ARM-MAZE TASK IN RATS VASILE HEFCO, LUCIAN HRITCU,

More information

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias Lewis O. Harvey, Jr. Department of Psychology University of Colorado Boulder, Colorado The Brain (Observable) Stimulus System (Observable) Response System

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

Psychomotor Vigilance Task Performance During and Following Chronic Sleep Restriction in Rats

Psychomotor Vigilance Task Performance During and Following Chronic Sleep Restriction in Rats pii: sp-00282-14 http://dx.doi.org/10.5665/sleep.4562 PVT PERFORMANCE AND CHRONIC SLEEP RESTRICTION IN RATS Psychomotor Vigilance Task Performance During and Following Chronic Sleep Restriction in Rats

More information

Mrs. Vicky Major, 2 Professor Hans Kromhout, 2. Dr. Tim Meijster, 3 Professor Leslie London

Mrs. Vicky Major, 2 Professor Hans Kromhout, 2. Dr. Tim Meijster, 3 Professor Leslie London THE ROLE OF ORGANOPHOSPHATE EXPOSURE IN THE AETIOLOGY OF DEPRESSION & SUICIDALITY AMONGST FARM WORKERS ON GRAPE FARMS IN THE WESTERN CAPE PROVINCE, SOUTH AFRICA 1 Mrs. Vicky Major, 2 Professor Hans Kromhout,

More information

418 Adopted:

418 Adopted: 418 Adopted: 27.07.95 OECD GUIDELINE FOR TESTING OF CHEMICALS Adopted by the Council on 27 th July 1995 Delayed Neurotoxicity of Organophosphorus Substances Following Acute Exposure INTRODUCTION 1. OECD

More information

Effects of the dopamine D2 agonist, quinpirole, on time and number processing in rats

Effects of the dopamine D2 agonist, quinpirole, on time and number processing in rats Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 68 (2001) 147±155 www.elsevier.com/locate/pharmbiochembeh Effects of the dopamine D2 agonist, quinpirole, on time and number processing in rats Angelo Santi*, Romina

More information

Instrumental Conditioning I

Instrumental Conditioning I Instrumental Conditioning I Basic Procedures and Processes Instrumental or Operant Conditioning? These terms both refer to learned changes in behavior that occur as a result of the consequences of the

More information

ASSOCIATED WITH NALORPHINE IN

ASSOCIATED WITH NALORPHINE IN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION BY A STIMULUS ASSOCIATED WITH NALORPHINE IN MORPHINE-DEPENDENT MONKEYS1 STEVEN R. GOLDBERG AND CHARLES R. SCHUSTER UNIVERSITY OF

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives Theoretical Perspectives REBT (1955) Albert Ellis Action & Result Oriented Teaches how to identify self-defeating thoughts Replaces thoughts w/ life enhancing ones 1 A B C s of personality formation: A

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University Undergraduate Honors Theses 5-2014 Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives? Tarra L. Baker Follow

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 322 328 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.322

More information

Scopolamine Effects Under a Titrating-Delayed-

Scopolamine Effects Under a Titrating-Delayed- The Psychological Record, 2008, 58, 37 49 Scopolamine Effects Under a Titrating-Delayed- Nonmatching-to-Position Procedure M. Porritt and A. Poling Western Michigan University In a study of working memory,

More information

Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK),

Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK), Supplemental Material: Subjects. Thirty-five adult male Lister Hooded rats (Charles River, Kent, UK), weighing between 280 300 g at the beginning of the experiment, were housed singly in holding rooms

More information

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING JOURNL OF THE EXPERMENTL NLYSS OF BEHVOR 1979, 32, 121-127 NUMBER (JULY) CQUSTON OF STMULUS CONTROL WHLE NTRODUCNG NEW STMUL N FDNG LNNY FELDS THE COLLEGE OF STTEN SLND fter establishing a discrimination

More information

CONNERS K-CPT 2. Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

CONNERS K-CPT 2. Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. CONNERS K-CPT 2 Conners Kiddie Continuous Performance Test 2 nd Edition C. Keith Conners, Ph.D. Assessment Report Name/ID: Jen Sample / 334 Age: 5 Gender: Female Birth Date: June 30, 2008 Grade: Administration

More information

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli. BACB 4 th Edition Task List s Content Area E: Specific Behavior-Change Procedures E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

More information

Veterinary Science Communications, 3 (1979) Elsevier Scientific Publishing Company, Amsterdam - Printed in The Netherlands

Veterinary Science Communications, 3 (1979) Elsevier Scientific Publishing Company, Amsterdam - Printed in The Netherlands Veterinary Science Communications, 3 (1979) 165-169 Elsevier Scientific Publishing Company, Amsterdam - Printed in The Netherlands 165 TASTE AVERSION LEARNING IN SUCKLING AND WEANLING PIGS K. A. HOUPT,

More information

Chapter 5 Study Guide

Chapter 5 Study Guide Chapter 5 Study Guide Practice Exam Questions: Which of the following is not included in the definition of learning? It is demonstrated immediately Assuming you have eaten sour pickles before, imagine

More information

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2004, Vol. 30, No. 2, 104 117 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.2.104

More information

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Chapter 6/9: Learning Chapter 6/9: Learning Learning A relatively durable change in behavior or knowledge that is due to experience. The acquisition of knowledge, skills, and behavior through reinforcement, modeling and natural

More information

Overview of Presentation. The State of Farming: But it has always been this way! Prevalence of depression in farmers. Mental Health in UK Farmers

Overview of Presentation. The State of Farming: But it has always been this way! Prevalence of depression in farmers. Mental Health in UK Farmers Pesticide Poisoning and Depressive Symptoms as Associated with Safety Behaviors and Injury in Colorado Farm Residents Cheryl L. Beseler, PhD University of Nebraska Medical Center Lorann Stallones, PhD,

More information

MACIEJ GASIOR, 1 MARIA JASZYNA, 2 JAMIE PETERS, 3 and STEVEN R. GOLDBERG

MACIEJ GASIOR, 1 MARIA JASZYNA, 2 JAMIE PETERS, 3 and STEVEN R. GOLDBERG 0022-3565/00/2953-1101 THE JOURNAL OF PHARMACOLOGY AND EXPERIMENTAL THERAPEUTICS Vol. 295, No. 3 U.S. Government work not protected by U.S. copyright 2783/864754 JPET 295:1101 1111, 2000 Printed in U.S.A.

More information

How to Determine if an Additional 10 Safety Factor Is Needed for Chemicals: A Case Study with Chlorpyrifos

How to Determine if an Additional 10 Safety Factor Is Needed for Chemicals: A Case Study with Chlorpyrifos TOXICOLOGICAL SCIENCES 48, 117 122 (1999) Copyright 1999 by the Society of Toxicology How to Determine if an Additional 10 Safety Factor Is Needed for Chemicals: A Case Study with Chlorpyrifos James E.

More information

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia AM. ZOOI.OCIST, 12:507-512 (1972). Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia PAUL DOWNEY AND BEHRUS JAHAN-PARWAR Biology Department, Clark University, Worcester, Massachusetts 01610 and Worcester Foundation

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning.

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning. In Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/xan/index.aspx 2013, vol. 39 (2), pp 107 116 2013 American Psychological Association DOI: 10.1037/a0031315

More information

Functionality. A Case For Teaching Functional Skills 4/8/17. Teaching skills that make sense

Functionality. A Case For Teaching Functional Skills 4/8/17. Teaching skills that make sense Functionality Teaching skills that make sense Mary Jane Weiss, Ph.D., BCBA-D Eden Princeton Lecture Series April, 2017 A Case For Teaching Functional Skills Preston Lewis, Dec. 1987, TASH Newsletter excerpt

More information

N-Methylneodecanamide (MNDA)

N-Methylneodecanamide (MNDA) United States Prevention, Pesticides EPA-730-F-99-008 Environmental Protection And Toxic Substances July 1999 Agency (7505C) New Pesticide Fact Sheet Description of the Chemical N-Methylneodecanamide (MNDA)

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

A Method for Assessing Attentional Bias in Anxious Rats. A Senior Honors Thesis

A Method for Assessing Attentional Bias in Anxious Rats. A Senior Honors Thesis Attentional Bias 1 A Method for Assessing Attentional Bias in Anxious Rats A Senior Honors Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for Graduation with distinction in Psychology in the

More information

1, 2, 3-Trichloropropane (TCP): Assessment of Risks from Drinking Water

1, 2, 3-Trichloropropane (TCP): Assessment of Risks from Drinking Water 1, 2, 3-Trichloropropane (TCP): Assessment of Risks from Drinking Water 1, 2, 3-Trichloropropane (TCP): Assessment of Risks from Drinking Water Prepared for Alaimo Group (Engineers and Architects) 200

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (3), 230-245 doi:10.3758/lb.37.3.230 Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning JUSTIN A. HARRIS, SABA A GHARA EI, AND CLINTON A. MOORE

More information

Developmental Effects of Prenatal Exposure to Organophosphate Pesticides

Developmental Effects of Prenatal Exposure to Organophosphate Pesticides Developmental Effects of Prenatal Exposure to Organophosphate Pesticides Research findings from the Columbia Center for Children s Environmental Health Rauh 1, Garfinkel 1, Perera 1, Andrews 1, Hoepner

More information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (4),380-384 Copyright 1978 by The Psychonornic Society, nc. Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information BRUCE A. WALD Utah State University, Logan,

More information

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change*

Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2 (4), 262-266 Some effects of short-term immediate prior exposure to light change on responding for light change* ALAN RUSSELLt and PETER H. GLOW University ofadelaide,

More information

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, 152 162 2329-8456/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000053 Memory Systems

More information

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 00, 79, 6 NUMBER (MARCH) RESPONDING FOR SUCROSE AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: EFFECTS OF SUCROSE CONCENTRATION AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCER DURATION

More information

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 42, No. 4, 401 414 2329-8456/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000112 Contextual

More information

Learning and Motivation

Learning and Motivation Learning and Motivation 4 (29) 178 18 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Learning and Motivation journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Context blocking in rat autoshaping: Sign-tracking

More information

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR LICK-SHOCK CONTINGENCIES IN THE RATT1 VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY 1972, 17, 119-125 NUMBER I (JANUARY) Hungry rats were allowed to lick an 8%

More information

A TRANSLATIONAL EVALUATION OF THE INTERPOLATED REINFORCEMENT PROCEDURE WITH YOUNG CHILDREN

A TRANSLATIONAL EVALUATION OF THE INTERPOLATED REINFORCEMENT PROCEDURE WITH YOUNG CHILDREN Behavioral Interventions Behav. Intervent. 27: 5 55 (212) Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 1.12/bin.1337 A TRANSLATIONAL EVALUATION OF THE INTERPOLATED REINFORCEMENT

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events

acquisition associative learning behaviorism A type of learning in which one learns to link two or more stimuli and anticipate events acquisition associative learning In classical conditioning, the initial stage, when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine

The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-009 The Development of Context-specific Operant Sensitization to d-amphetamine Wesley Paul Thomas Utah

More information

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010 Notes Textbook used Week 1-3: Bouton, M.E. (2016). Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis. 2nd Ed. Sinauer Week 4-6: Rieger, E. (Ed.) (2014) Abnormal Psychology:

More information

Interaction of social and individual learning in food preferences of Norway rats

Interaction of social and individual learning in food preferences of Norway rats ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1, 62, 41 46 doi:1.16/anbe..1721, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Interaction of social and individual learning in food preferences of Norway rats BENNETT G. GALEF,

More information

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, & JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR1 1982, 38, 71-85 NUMBER I (JULY) MEMORY FOR RECENT BEHAVIOR IN THE PIGEON STEPHEN P. KRAMER CORRECTIONS DIVISION, DRAPER, UTAH Variations of the symbolic

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 5, NUMBF- 4 OCITOBER, 1 962 THE EFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES OF PRIMARY AND CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT JOAN G. STEVENSON1 AND T. W. REESE MOUNT HOLYOKE

More information

ABSENCE OF SENSORY NEUROPATHY AMONG WORKERS WITH OCCUPATIONAL EXPOSURE TO CHLORPYRIFOS

ABSENCE OF SENSORY NEUROPATHY AMONG WORKERS WITH OCCUPATIONAL EXPOSURE TO CHLORPYRIFOS ABSTRACT: Several studies have reported the occurrence of sensory neuropathy with exposure to chlorpyrifos and other organophosphorus insecticides, at levels not associated with overt toxicity. We evaluated

More information

Complexity Made Simple

Complexity Made Simple Complexity Made Simple Bussey-Saksida Touch Screen Chambers for Rats and Mice Tasks translate directly to well established monkey and human touch models e.g. CANTAB High throughput screening of multiple

More information

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making Matthew A J Apps 1,2, Laura Grima 2, Sanjay Manohar 2, Masud Husain 1,2 1 Nuffield Department of Clinical Neuroscience,

More information

Open Research Online The Open University s repository of research publications and other research outputs

Open Research Online The Open University s repository of research publications and other research outputs Open Research Online The Open University s repository of research publications and other research outputs Anxiety and depression following cumulative low-level exposure to organophosphate pesticides Journal

More information

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 348 370 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Chris Hope and Angelo Santi Wilfrid

More information

fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1

fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1 BEHAVIORAL AND NEURAL BIOLOGY 36, 298-303 (1982) BRIEF REPORT Food-Aversion Conditioning in Japanese Monkeys (Macaca fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1 TETSURO MATSUZAWA Primate Research Institute,

More information