Deaf Children's Understanding of Beliefs and Desires

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Deaf Children's Understanding of Beliefs and Desires"

Transcription

1 Deaf Children's Understanding of Beliefs and Desires Lucy Steeds University of Oxford Karen Rowe University of Bristol Ann Dowker University of Oxford Twenty-two children (5-12 years old) who were profoundly, prelingually deaf were given two tests designed to tap their "theory of mind," that is, their ability to attribute independent mental states to other people. The tests were versions of Baron-Cohen, Leslie, and Frith's Sally-Anne task and of Baron-Cohen's breakfast task. Seventy percent of the children were successful on all questions requiring belief attribution, a considerably and significantly larger percentage than the 29% obtained by Peterson and Siegal for a similar sample, though it is still lower than would be expected on the basis on chronological age. Children were universally successful on questions requiring the attribution of desire. We discuss implications of the findings. A child credited with having a "theory of mind" is able to attribute independent mental states to himself or herself and others in order to predict and explain behavior (Premack & Woodruff, 1978). Despite the somewhat misleading term, this child need only take account of another person's beliefs, desires, and thoughts; it is not assumed that he or she conceives of these mental states as unobservable entities. In other words, a child with a theory of mind need not have postulated any theoretical constructs (quasi-scientifically). For convenience, the term theory of mind will be used in this article as it has frequently been used in the litera- We think the staff and pupils at Elmfidd School in Bristol and the Frank Barnes School in London; Dr. J. G. Kyle «t the Centre for Deaf Studies, University of Bristol; Dr. R. W. Hiorns for his advice on statistics; and Prof. R. Campbell and Dr. P. L. Harris for helpful discussion. Prof. C Peterson and two anonymous reviewers provided useful feedback on earlier drafts. Correspondence should be sent to Ann Dowker, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, England. Copyright 1997 Oxford Univertity Press. CCC ture: to refer to children's ability to explicitly take account of other people's mental states in a test situation. No assumptions are made either about the presence or absence of theoretical constructs, or about the extent to which children's performance on such tasks may result in underestimates (Chandler, Fritz, & Hala, 1989; Clements & Perner, 1994) or overestimates (Clark, 1990; Murray & Field, 1990; Happe, 1995) of their implicit understanding of mental states in a more naturalistic context. Experiments testing for theory of mind have aimed to tap children's understanding of several different mental states and have taken a variety of forms. The first such experiment (Wimmer & Perner, 1983) and many subsequently (e.g., Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985; Perner, Ruffman, & Leekam, 1994) have focused on whether young children can predict another person's behavior as based on his or her mistaken belief. There is some agreement that success in these so-called false-belief tasks typically emerges in normal children when they are around 4 years old. Growing evidence now shows that many autistic children have difficulties with tasks that assess theory of mind (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 1985), and attempts have been made to account for the full range of autistic deficits in terms of a theory-of-mind impairment (e.g., Leslie, 1987, 1994; Baron-Cohen, 1993). This has led to proposals of a theory-of-mind module that is impaired or absent in autism. A difficulty for such theories is that diagnostic criteria for autism include the development of symptoms before the age of 3 (DSM-IV):

2 186 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 2:3 Summer 1997 that is, autistic children are already showing the disorder at an age when normal children also do not pass false-belief tasks. Attempts to reconcile the conflicting evidence range from suggestions that theory of mind depends on a specific brain mechanism that is present from birth but must mature before theory of mind can be manifested (cf. Fletcher, Happe, Frith, Baker, Dolan, Frackowiack, & Frith, 1995) to proposals that false-belief understanding develops out of other capacities that develop earlier, for example, pretend play (Leslie, 1987) and shared attention (Baron-Cohen, 1991b, 1993, 1994; Baron-Cohen, Cox, Baird, Sweetenham, Nightingale, Morgan, Drew, & Charman, 1996). An alternative or complementary theory is that the rate and extent of development of theory of mind may be influenced by exposure to conversations about mental states (cf. Perner, Ruffman, & Leekam, 1994; Jenkins & Astington, 1996) and that the severe communication disorder of autistic children prevents them from making adequate use of such conversations. Some support for the latter theory comes from findings of associations between verbal ability and theory of mind acquisition in both normal children (Happe, 1995; Jenkins & Astington, 1996) and autistic children (Happe, 1995; Sparrevohn & Howie, 1995), though a much higher verbal mental age was necessary for theory of mind tasks to be passed by autistic than normal children. Also, causal primacy has not been established. If exposure to communication is important to the development of a theory of mind, then one would expect that nonautistic children with communication disorders would also be delayed in theory-of-mind acquisition. There have been several attempts to study this topic Specifically language-impaired (SLI) children are sometimes used as controls for autistic children (e.g., Leslie & Frith, 1988; Perner, Frith, Leslie, & Leekam, 1989) and generally do much better than autistic children in theory-of-mind tasks. It should be noted, however, that SLI is a very heterogeneous condition, and that Shields, Varley, Broks, and Simpson (1996) have found impaired performance on theory-of-mind tasks by children with semantic pragmatic deficits (those that would have the greatest eflfect on comprehension), but not in children with phonological and syntactic deficits. The technique pioneered by Hermelin and O'Connor (1978) of comparing autistic groups with children with specific sensory impairments has also been used. For example, McAlpine and Moore (1995) studied theory of mind in blind children. Although blind children are not impaired in verbal communication, they are impaired in some forms of nonverbal communication, and in particular are not able to detect eye direction: a skill that Baron-Cohen and others (Baron-Cohen, 1993, 1994; Baron-Cohen, Campbell, Karmiloflf- Smith, Grant, & Walker, 1995; Campbell, 1994) regarded as important, though not absolutely essential, for the development of shared attention. McAlpine and Moore (1995) did indeed find that blind children were somewhat delayed in passing a false-belief task, though this may have been in part due to the greater information-processing demands of such a task for children who cannot use visual cues. The focus of this study is performance on theoryof-mind tasks by prelingually deaf children. Being born unable to hear does not automatically give rise to communication difficulties: studies of deaf children born to signing deaf parents have found a timetable of language acquisition roughly parallel to that of hearing children, including an early stage of manual babbling (e.g., Pettito & Marentette, 1991). Nevertheless, only a minority of deaf children have parents who are fluent signers and thus a deaf child's linguistic development is usually delayed, at least until he or she joins a community of native signers in primary school (and probably sometime beyond this, given the Total Communication, TC, ethos of most British schools for the deaf). Several studies have found that relatively few hearing parents achieve sufficient proficiency in manual communication to converse freely with their deaf children about imaginary or unobservable topics (e.g., M. Harris, 1992; Marschark, 1993), and further evidence shows that many deaf children start talking about others' mental states only when they start at a school for the deaf (Wood, Wood, Griffith, & Howarth, 1986). As a result of limited early exposure to language, it seems many deaf children will be delayed in gaining conversational access to information about the intangible thoughts and feelings of others. Furthermore, it is plausible that this will delay theory-of-mind development. Consistent with this theory, Peterson and Siegal (1995) reported that children aged 8 13 who were pro-

3 Understanding of Beliefs and Desires 187 foundly, prelingually deaf struggled with a false-belief test: only 35% passed a version of the classic "Sally- Anne" task (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985), which tests the understanding that someone who has not seen an object being moved will believe it to be in its original rather than actual location, and normally presents little problem for hearing children past their fourth birthday. The level of performance shown by deaf children did not differ significantly from that reported for autistic children of a comparable (nonverbal) mental age (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). These results were replicated in a later comparison of deaf and autistic children across a wider age range (Peterson & Siegal, 1996). This study was essentially a reexamination of the claims made by Peterson and Siegal (1995), using an English sample and a wider range of tests. A highly similar, if slightly younger, sample of children participated in two different false-belief tests: the modified Sally-Anne task used by Peterson and Siegal and Baron-Cohen's (1991a) adaptation of a task devised by Harris, Johnson, Hutton, Andrews, and Cooke (1989). Inclusion of this second task, which demands emotion prediction rather than behavior prediction, gave a broader base on which to establish the children's understanding of false-belief, as recommended by Gray and Hosie (1996). It also offered a comparison of children's understanding of desires with their understanding of beliefs. There were two grounds for predicting that understanding of desires might be easier for the deaf children than understanding of beliefs. The first was that some previous studies suggest that both normal children (Wellman & Bartsch, 1988; Wellman & Woolley, 1990) and autistic children (Baron-Cohen, 1991a; Phillips, Baron-Cohen, & Rutter, 1995) understand desires earlier and more easily than beliefs. The second is that it may be possible to understand desires on the basis of nonverbal communication (e.g., pointing), whereas the understanding of beliefs is more likely to require verbal communication. Method Participants. The 22 children (13 girls and 9 boys) were drawn from two government-funded schools for deaf children, one in London and one in Bristol. The two schools shared a common teaching ethos and both served children with a range of socioeconomic backgrounds. The schools used TC in their teaching, which entailed the use of both British Sign Language (BSL) and Sign-Supported English, with the aim that the children should become bilingual in English and BSL. All children were audiologically diagnosed as being profoundly deaf: they had an average hearing loss of at least 96 decibels in the better ear and had become deaf prior to the onset of language acquisition. They all were described by their teachers as using BSL as their principal communication medium. Most came from hearing families, though detailed information about home background is not available. The mean age of the children was 9 years 8 months (range: 5;8 to 12;4). No subject had any known associated disabilities such as autism, mental retardation, uncorrected visual impairment, or cerebral palsy. Each was judged by their teachers to be within the normal range of intelligence. (Most of these judgments were based on the teachers' extensive observation of the children in a school setting. Some of the children had also been given standardized intelligence tests, but records of these were not available to the experimenters. Unfortunately, practical constraints also did not permit IQ_tests to be given to the children in this study.) Task design. All children carried out two tasks. Both tasks lent themselves to use with deaf children for the following reasons: they revolve around (a) simple vocabulary and (b) a storyline that is readily comprehensible and also, by virtue of the involvement of props, visually salient. Marble task. The materials were a female doll (40 cm high), a furry cloth dog (30 cm high), a basket with fabric cover, a square box with fitted lid, and a marble. The basic task was the same as that used by Peterson and Siegal (1995): Baron-Cohen et al.'s (1985) adaptation of Wimmer and Perner's (1983) Sally-Anne story-test of false-belief. The task begins like this: a doll hides a marble in a basket and then goes off for a walk. While she is gone, a second character appears, moves the marble from the basket to a box and then leaves. The first doll then returns to the scene and the child being tested is asked where this doll will look for

4 188 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 2:3 Summer 1997 her marble. Two control questions are finally asked, which tap whether the child (a) has noted the marble's new location and (b) can recall its original hiding place. It was inappropriate to produce a sign-for-word translation of the experimental script devised by Baron-Cohen et al. (1985), but the content of this script was faithfully rendered in the BSL version used in the current experiment, as shown in the Appendix. Following the reasoning of Peterson and Siegal, a toy dog was substituted for the second doll of the classic Sally-Anne marble paradigm. Every time the dolls' names are used in a classic version, they must be fingerspelled, and Peterson and Siegal found this posed considerable difficulty for signing deaf children: it increased demands on both attention and memory and presumably put poor spellers at a disadvantage. Breakfast cereal task. The materials were a male doll (40 cm high), a mini-box of Rice Krispies (10 X 7 X 4 cm) with either Weetabix or nothing inside, and a mini-box of Weetabix (9 X 17 X 5 cm) with either Rice Krispies or nothing inside. The basic task was Baron-Cohen's (1991a) adaptation of an experiment developed by Harris et al. (1989). This is a story-test designed to tap children's understanding of false-belief and also their appreciation of beliefs and desires as causes of emotion. The story begins with two types of breakfast cereal being presented to a hungry doll, who likes one of the cereals but not the other. The child being tested is asked what the doll would feel happy or sad if presented with (a) one box, and (b) the other (desire test 1). The doll then leaves the scene and the subject discovers that one of the cereal boxes contains the normal contents of the other (preferred) box, while this other box is empty. The boxes are then resealed. When the doll returns, the subject is once again asked what the doll would feel if presented with either box (belief test). Finally, the subject watches as the surprising contents of each cereal box are revealed to the doll; the subject is then asked what the doll's emotions would now be if given either box (desire test 2). A faithful BSL translation of Baron-Cohen's (1991a) experimental script (for first desire test, belief test, and second desire test) was used. A retranslation of the tasks into English is given in the Appendix. Following Baron-Cohen (1991a), questions within each of the three tests were randomized to avoid any order effect of children preferring to respond "happy" before "sad" or vice versa. Memory questions were asked at the beginning and end of the experiment, in order to check whether each subject had noted and remembered which of the cereals the doll preferred. A partiality check was also made in order to establish that a subject's own preferences between the two cereals did not confound task performance: in the last question posed, each child was asked to sign which of the two they personally liked more. The preferences of the doll were varied across trials: half the children were told the doll liked Weetabix but disliked Rice Krispies; the other half were told the reverse. In either case the preferred cereal was located in the wrong box. Overall procedure. Each child was tested individually in an empty classroom in the school. They were seated across a low table from two women: the experimenter (LS) and a storyteller (KR). KR is profoundly deaf and fluent in BSL, the children's principal means of communicating. She has signing abilities beyond the Stage 3 level. Most teachers in British signing schools for the deaf would be expected to have reached Stage 2 (examining authority: Council for the Advancement of Communication with Deaf People). KR told the stories involved in each task to each child using BSL. Each step in the narrative was followed by illustrative enactment: LS manipulated the animate characters and KR moved all other props in accompaniment. In order to counter fatigue or practice effects, half the subjects were presented with the marble task before the breakfast cereal task, and for the other half the order was reversed. LS and KR independently recorded each child's responses and were in complete agreement. Children from the two schools did not differ in their performance: no significant differences were found on any task (Fisher's Exact in each case, p >.05, 2-tail test). This justified the pooling of data from the two schools in all analyses.

5 Understanding of Beliefs and Desires 189 Table 1 Success on belief and desire tasks (out of 20 children who took all tests) Passed control questions Failed at least one control question Total Succeeded on belief tasks Failed on belief tasks Succeeded on desire tasks Failed on desire tasks Results Of the 22 children given the marble task, 15 (67%) passed the control questions for the marble task, and 7 (33%) failed at least one of them. Omitting over a third of the children from the analysis could have seriously biased the results, so these children were included in the rest of the study. However, the "control failers" and "control passers" are separated in the table and in some of the analyses. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was carried out on the whole initial sample of 22 children with control question success (passed all controls versus failed some controls) as the grouping factor, and age as the dependent variable. This showed a significant effect, with children who passed all controls tending to be older than those who failed some controls The children in this study performed significantly better on the marble task than those deaf children studied by Peterson and Siegal (19/22 versus 9/26; x 2 = 13.13; df= \;p<.01). They also performed considerably better than Baron-Cohen et al.'s (1985) autistic children (19/22 versus 4/20; x 2 = 18.62; df = \;p<.01), while not differing significantly from their normal children (19/22 versus 23/27; X 2 = 0.014; df= \;p = not significant [NS]). Twenty out of 22 children passed the control questions on the breakfast task. The two who did not (aged 5;8 and 10;3) were not included in the rest of the study. Sixteen of the 20 remaining children passed both belief questions. All children who passed one belief question passed the other as well. There was no indication that a child's performance was influenced by his or her own preference between the two cereals: belief test pass-rates of subjectss whose preference coincided with that of the doll did not differ significantly from those of children with the opposite preference (or indeed from the combined performance of subjects who had the opposite preference to the doll and subjects who were indifferent) (Fisher's Exact, p >.05, 2-tail test). The breakfast task had not been previously administered to deaf children. The children in this study performed significantly better than the autistic children in Baron-Cohen's (1991a) study (16/20 versus 9/17; x 2 = 14.3; df= 1; p <.01) and similarly to the normal children in that study (16/20 versus 14/19; x 2 = 0.22; df=,!;/> = NS). However, the high success rate on the marble task may be misleading, as only one belief question was asked, whereas in many other studies, including that of Peterson and Siegal (1995), two questions were asked. This means that if the marble task were considered in isolation, children in this study might appear to have an artificially high success rate through chance alone. Therefore, in subsequent analyses, children were counted as successful on belief only if they passed the marble task belief question and both breakfast task belief questions. As shown in Table 1, 14 children passed all belief questions, and 6 failed some. No child failed all the belief questions in both tasks. A one-way ANOVA was carried out on the 20 children given the belief tasks with belief success (passed versus failed) as the grouping factor and age as the dependent variable. The effect failed to reach significance The overall rate of success on the belief task is 70%

6 190 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 2:3 Summer 1997 as compared with 35% (9 out of 26) of Peterson and Siegal's group of deaf children of hearing parents. The difference between these percentages is significant ( X 2 = 5.65;#-=l;/><.05). In the breakfast task, the children were required to justify all their judgments (both for belief and for desire). Ninety-eight percent of all their justifications were correct: they referred (a) to the apparent (rather than actual) contents, (b) to the doll's desires (e.g., "he likes Weetabix"), or (c) in the belief test, to the doll's restricted knowledge ("he didn't see inside"). It could be argued that children should not be counted as having a true understanding of false belief unless they pass the control questions as well as the belief questions. In fact, the necessity for such a criterion is highly questionable, in view of the children's almost universal success in justifying their judgments. Nonetheless, it seemed worth investigating how the use of the most stringent possible criteria would affect results. Due to the substantial number of control failers, such criteria would reduce the number of successful children to 11. The success rate would thus be reduced to 55% (if we consider only the 20 children who received all tests) or even to 50% (if we consider the entire initial sample of 22 children). These figures are still considerably greater than the 35% for Peterson and^ Segal's children, but due to the relatively small sample sizes, the differences between the samples fail to reach significance if these criteria are employed. If we take the success rate in this sample as 11 out of 20 (55%), then x 2 (df= 1) = If we take it as 11 out of 22, then x 2 (df= 1) = Neither figure reaches significance. However, even at the lowest possible estimate of success rate on the belief task (50%), the children in this sample are still performing significantly better than Baron-Cohen's samples of autistic children This is true whether their performance is compared with Baron-Cohen, Leslie, and Frith's (1985) children in the two-question marble task (x 2 = 4.11; df= 1; p <.05), or with Baron-Cohen's (1991) children in the breakfast belief task (x 2 = 4.36; df= 1; p<.05). By contrast, the children in Peterson and Siegal's (1995) study did not differ significantly from the autistic children in Baron-Cohen's studies. All of the 20 children who were given the breakfast test passed both desire questions. This universal success made more detailed analyses (e.g., by age) unnecessary. This task had not previously been given to deaf children. The performance was considerably better than that of the autistic children in Baron-Cohen's (1991a) study (20/20 versus 9/18; x 2 = \2;df= l;p<.01). Comparisons with Baron-Cohen's (1991) normal children were inappropriate due to the near-universal success in both groups (20/20 versus 17/19). A Wilcoxon sign test was used to compare children's performance on the belief and desire tasks. They performed significantly better on the desire tasks [Wilcoxon statistic (6/20) = 0; p <.05]. Discussion The extent of the difference in false-belief understanding between deaf children in this study and those in Peterson and Siegal's (1995, 1996) studies depends somewhat on the stringency of the criteria used to determine false-belief understanding. However, even when stringent criteria are used, the children in this study did not appear to be as severely delayed as those in Peterson and Segal's (1995, 1996) studies. There are at least two ways of explaining the discrepancies between these results and those reported by Peterson and Siegal. First, it could be argued that the children included in this study have developed some theory of mind that children in the earlier study still lacked, possibly as the result of greater language exposure. Such an argument is consistent with the fact that the few deaf children of deaf parents (i.e., children who had been brought up in a rich conversational environment using sign language) in Peterson and Siegal's (1995, 1996) studies also showed unimpaired understanding of false belief. Moreover, when Clark, Schwanenflugel, and Everhart (1996) assessed the theory of mind of deaf adults (who had presumably had far greater language exposure than deaf children) by the rather different technique of asking them to classify cognitive verbs, they found no difference between deaf and hearing adults. At first sight, this argument may seem improbable, given that the conspicuous difference in pass-rates for the two samples is set against a background of highly comparable life circumstances. Specifically, language

7 Understanding of Beliefs and Desires 191 exposure is likely to have been very similar for the two groups, since (a) the majority of children had nonsigning parents, and (b) all four of the schools that children were drawn from schools that were described as using TC in teaching. In fact, the current sample might be expected to have had marginally less exposure to language, given their somewhat younger age (although age only offers a crude index of language exposure). However, it could still be that the English children had somewhat greater or earlier exposure to sign language than the Australian children. Moreover, some of the children on Peterson and Segal's study had been taught Sign-Supported English as a first medium of communication, whereas all the children in this study had been taught both BSL and Sign-Supported English, with BSL considered to be their primary communication medium. Despite the possibly greater initial difficulty of learning more than one communication system, BSL is a more "naturalistic" language, and as such may be more conducive to rapid early language acquisition, and to the ability to make use of any opportunities for conversation with members of the deaf community. An alternative explanation of the failure to replicate Peterson and Siegal's findings invokes the methodological differences between the two studies: perhaps the poor performance reported by the previous researchers was the result of their style of task delivery. Peterson and Siegal employed the following procedure: one hearing adult spoke the story and manipulated the props in accompaniment; after each stage in the narrative, an interpreter repeated the spoken statement just given and simultaneously offered a translation in signed English. This may be problematic on two counts. The first is that Peterson and Siegal state that their children were "selected on the basis that each child used sign language (signed English and or Auslan) as his or her principal communication medium" (p. 465). Children with Auslan as a first language would have been at a notable disadvantage when presented with a task in Sign-Supported English, and their failure to grasp that desires and beliefs had to be taken into account may reflect poor comprehension of lip-reading and signed English rather than a lack of theory of mind. In this study, the task was given in BSL, which was the children's principal communication medium (although they were also familiar with Sign-Supported English). The second is that the children's attention was divided in more than one way: children were confronted with one adult who spoke while performing with toys and a parroting second who simultaneously signed. Thus, the children not only varied their gaze between the face and hands of the adults in front of them, but also had to shift attention from one adult to the other throughout the experiment. Moreover, such a procedure would result in a relative lack of synchrony between story and actions. These problems may well have led to the children becoming distracted at crucial points: the effect of such distraction would likely be particularly strong for children who already had some difficulties with the language medium used in the experiment. In the current study, the telling of the story running through each task was a fully coherent process: each section of the narrative was conveyed just once and in the children's first language, namely BSL; the contribution of the two presenters was integrated and the performance of both was essential to the presentation. These factors may have led to better performance. Why was performance on the control questions apparently worse in this study than in Peterson and Siegal's (1995) study (and in many studies of normal and autistic children), despite relatively good performance on belief questions (and excellent performance on desire tasks)? There are at least two possible explanations. The first possible explanation is that the children in this study were a little younger on average than those in Peterson and Siegal's (1995) study. Relative to their normal peers, deaf children, and especially young deaf children, have particular problems fully comprehending the task perhaps because the task is more attention draining for deaf children, since for them it imposes continuous demand on a single sensory modality (i.e., vision). Consistent with the age effect on control-test success, children's attention spans tend to increase with age (e.g., Sigman, Cohen, Beckwith, & Robert, 1991). The second possible explanation is that the control questions may have posed particular problems for reasons unrelated to the understanding of mental states.

8 192 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 2:3 Summer 1997 In particular, they referred to past situations and so required the children to override their likely response tendency to answer in terms of present states and instead to rely on memory. By contrast, the belief and desire questions dealt with present states. A related point is that it is possible that the children tested in the current study gave full consideration to the first (crucial) question, but when this was immediately followed by presentation of two further (control) questions, their attention and application rapidly fell off. This suggestion is perhaps more compelling because no new information, no more story, separates the three test questions: the first question is more likely to be accepted within the context of the interactive storytelling that has gone before; whereas the second and third questions are further removed from the storytelling phase, and in this sense offer less incentive for the subject to attend. This is not incompatible with the results cited by Peterson and Siegal (1995), on the basis of the assumption that their children were less engaged in understanding the story as a whole and more focused on understanding each verbal statement as it came. The latter hypothesis could be tested by varying the order of presentation of crucial and control questions and investigating whether children perform better on the control questions if these are presented first. This had not been done in this study for fear of biasing responses to the crucial questions. The importance of methodological factors does not necessarily reduce the deaf children's performance deficits to an artefact of the specialized testing situation. In many real-life situations as well, deaf children are likely to be exposed to suboptimal communicative contexts, which result in greater demands on their attentional and information-processing capacities than would a more "user-friendly" context. This may have considerable implications for deaf children's theory-of mind abilities in naturalistic situations, if insufficient attention is given to their communication needs. So to what extent does delayed exposure to conversation delay acquisition of theory of mind? The fact that these children performed better than Peterson and Siegal's children should not obscure the fact that they still performed less well than would be expected on the basis of their chronological age. Indeed, if extremely (probably excessively) stringent criteria for success are used, the differences between these children and those of Peterson and Segal (1995) ceased to be significant, though they continued to perform significantly better than Baron-Cohen's autistic children (Baron-Cohen et al, 1985; Baron-Cohen, 1991a). Even if one uses far less stringent criteria and treats the different belief tasks separately, the 80% success rate on the breakfast task is presumably lower than would be expected for this age range (though this assumption should be tested by giving the task to hearing children 7 years old and over). Thus, deaf children of hearing parents do appear to be somewhat, though not grossly, delayed in their ability to pass false-belief tasks. Their problems may be in comprehending the task as a whole, more than with false belief as such, given that they were as likely to fail control questions as belief questions. It must, however, be remembered that two children (both over 7, and one as old as 10; 10) failed the belief questions despite passing all control questions. By contrast, the children in this study appeared to have no problems in understanding desire. They performed perfectly on desire tests, whatever their age, and whether or not they passed all control questions (though it must be remembered that the two children who failed the breakfast test control questions were excluded from the study). The significant difference between performance on the belief and desire questions is intriguing. It may simply reflect differences in ease of task comprehension. It is, however, plausible that children with verbal communication difficulties do understand desire better than belief, because the former is more communicable by nonverbal means (e.g., pointing, other gestures, and facial expression). If this were the case, it would support the view that not only does communication experience play some role in the development of theory of mind, but different forms of communication experience have differential effects on different aspects of theory of mind. Further studies are needed if we are to gain a fuller understanding of the extent to which deaf children are delayed in theory-of-mind acquisition: severely delayed (as suggested by Peterson & Siegal, 1995), slightly delayed (as suggested by this study), or not delayed at all. It would clearly be desirable to carry out a

9 Understanding of Beliefs and Desires 193 study of deaf children aged 4 5 years. If these young deaf children fail to match the high pass-rates commonly achieved by hearing children of the same age, then language and conversation will be implicated in theory-of-mind development. Research attempting to clarify this point empirically will have to address several issues. The number of younger children who failed control questions in the current study highlights potential difficulties in testing 4- to 5-year-olds, and some modified experimental format, which reduces attentional demand, may be called for. For very young and/ or language-delayed children, even this may not be sufficient. The dilemma arises: if linguistic demands are greatly simplified (e.g., by use of predominantly nonverbal techniques and/or by removing demands for justifications), then the task may become less demanding in other ways, and no longer be comparable with standard theory-of-mind tasks. If linguistic demands are not simplified, then children with limited language may fail for reasons unconnected with theory of mind. Nonetheless, some attempt should be made to investigate younger deaf children. Future studies should also include language tests, to elucidate the relationship between language and theory-of-mind performance more precisely. Longitudinal studies would be particularly useful, both for investigating the possiblity of language ability "thresholds" for performance in theory-of-mind tasks and for investigating the extent to which the understanding of desire typically precedes the understanding of belief. Appendix Experimental Script for Marble Task Basket set up near left edge of table; box positioned far right. Female doll (Ann) held in middle. What's this?... wait for reply... basket. What's this?... matt for reply... box. What's this?... matt for reply... girl. Name Ann Ann likes playing marbles. Marble producedfromkr's pocket and that placed in Ann's hand. She's had enough... hides marble. Ann made to bend over basket as marble placed under the cover there. Ann leaves. Ann walkedfromtable. Time passes... dog. Dog brought to table. He's nosey... sniffs. Dog moved towards basket, cover removed. What's this?... wait for reply... marble. Dog and marble moved together over to box; marble hidden there. Dog leaves. Dog removedfromtable. Ssh! Ann doesn't know; don't tell Ann. Ann comes back. Ann brought to table. Belief She, marble looks where? Reality Marble now where? Memory First, marble put where? Experimental Script for Breakfast Cereal Task Male doll (Ben) sat on table with mini Weetabix box and mini Rice Krispies box. Preferences of the doll varied across trials: half the subjects told Ben liked Weetabix but disliked Rice Krispies (as scripted below); other half told the reverse. Contents of the boxes adjusted accordingly. Desire Test 1 What's this?... wait for reply... boy. Name Ben He wakes up, goes downstairs, is hungry. What's this?... maitfor reply, e.g., subjectfingerspells W.. E.., etc., or shrugs to say sign not known... Weetabix. He likes Weetabix. What's this?... wait for reply... Rice Krispies. He doesn't like Rice Krispies. Memory 1 He likes which? Pointing to boxes. Give THIS box. Weetabix box moved towards Ben. Desire 1 Ben feels what? Prompt usually needed. Happy or sad? Why? Give THIS box. Rice Krispies box moved towards Ben. Desire 2 Ben feels what? Why? Belief Test Ben leaves. Ben walkedfrom table Look inside this (Weetabix box). Yes open i t... noth-

10 194 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 2:3 Summer 1997 ing!... Ssh! close it up. Look inside this (Rice Krispies box). What?... Weetabix!... Ssh! close it up. Boxes replaced on table as before, only their positions swapped. Ben doesn't know; don't tell Ben. Ben comes back. Ben brought back to table. He has not opened this or this. Belief 1 Ben sees this (Weetabix box) feels what? Why? Belief 2 Ben sees this (Rice Krispies box) feels what? Why? Desire Test 2 Give Ben THIS box (Weetabix). Open it. Box opened and given to doll. Desire 3 Ben feels what? Why? Give Ben THIS box (Rice Krispies.) Open it. Box opened and given to doll. Desire 4 Ben feels what? Why? Final Checks Preference You like which? Subject signs "Weetabix" or "Rice Krispies." Memory 2 Ben likes which? References Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, Baron-Cohen, S. (1991a). Do people with autism understand what causes emotion? Child Development, 62, Baron-Cohen, S. (1991b). Precursors to a theory of mind: understanding attention in others, In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of minds: evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading (pp Oxford: Blackwell. Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology: The development of a theory of mind and its dysfunction. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager- Flusberg, and D. J. Cohen (Eds.). Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism (pp ). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreiding. Cahiers de Psychologic Cognitive/ Current Psychology of Cognition, 13, Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a 'theory of mind'? Cognition, 21, Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., & Walker, J. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., Sweetenham, J., Nightingale, N., Morgan, K., Drew, A., & Charman, T. (19%). Psychological markers in the detection of autism in infancy in a large population. British Journal ofpsychiatry, 168, Campbell, R. (1994). How to build a model that works. Cahiers de Psychologic Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 13, Chandler, M, Fritz, A. S., & Hala, S. (1989). Small-scale deceit: Deception as a marker of 2-year-olds', 3-year-olds' and 4- year-olds' early theories of mind. Child Development, 60, Clark, J. (1990). Investigating the use of theory of mind tasks in naturalistic settings by children of 4, 5, and 6 years old. University of Oxford: Undergraduate research project (supervised by Stephen Milan). dark, M. D, Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Everhart, V. S. (1996). Theory of mind in deaf adults and the organisation of verbs of knowing. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1, dements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9, Fletcher, P. C, Happe, F, Frith, U, Baker, S. C, Dolan, R. J, Frackowiack, R. S. J., & Frith, C D. (1995). Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of theory of mind in story comprehension. Cognition, 57, Gray, C. D, & Hosie, J. (19%). Deafness, story understanding and theory of mind. Journal ofdeaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1, Happe, F. G. E. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, Harris, M. (1992). Language experience and early language development. Hove: Erlbaum Associates. Harris, P. L., Johnson, C N., Hutton, D, Andrews, G, & Cooke T. (1989). Young children's theory of mind and emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 3, Hermelin, B., & O'Connor, N. (1978). Seeing and hearing and space and time. London: Academic Press. Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (1996).Cognitive factors and family structure associated with theory of mind development in young children. Developmental Psychology, 32, Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of'theory of mind'. Psychological Review, 94, Leslie, A. M. (1994). Pretending and believing: Issues in the ToMM. Cognition, SO, Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children's understanding of seeing, knowing and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, Marschark, M. (1993). Psychological development of deaf children. New York: Oxford University Press. McAlpine, L. M., & Moore, C L. (1995). The development of social understanding in children with visual impairments. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 89,

11 Understanding of Beliefs and Desires 195 Murray, D., & Field, E. (1990). A naturalistic study of the theory of mind paradigm and understanding of false belief. University of Oxford: Undergraduate research project (supervised by Stephen Milan). Pemer, J., Frith, U, Leslie, A. M., & Leekam, S. R. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: Knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Development, 60, Perner, J., Ruffman, T, & Leekam, S. R. Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65, Peterson, C. C, & Siegal M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, Peterson, C C., & Siegal, M. (1996). Domain-specificity and everyday knowledge in normal, autistic and deaf children: Differentiation between biological, physical and psychological thinking in the context of early conversational deprivation. Paper presented at the Conference of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Quebec, Canada. Pettito, L. A., & Marentette, P. F. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251, Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1995). To what extent can children with autism understand desire? Development and Psychopathology, 7, Premack, DC, & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 1, Shields, J., Varley, R., Broks, P., & Simpson, A. (1996). Social cognition in developmental language disorders and highlevel autism. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 38, Sigman, M., Cohen, S. E., Beckwith, L., & Robert, A. (1991). Continuity in cognitive abilities from infancy to 12 years of age. Cognitive Development, 6, Sparrevohn, R., & Howie, P. M. (1995). Theory of mind in children with autistic disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, Wellman, H. M., & Bartsch, K. (1988). Young children's reasoning and beliefs. Cognition, 30, Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, y Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, Wood, D, Wood, H., Griffith, A., & Howarth, I. (1986). Teaching and talking with deaf children. London: John Wiley.

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document.

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document. Title Understanding how children think: A theory of mind perspective Author(s) Carolyn Tan-Niam S. L. Source REACT, 1998(1), 27-31 Published by National Institute of Education (Singapore) This document

More information

Processing demands in belief-desire reasoning: inhibition or general difficulty?

Processing demands in belief-desire reasoning: inhibition or general difficulty? Developmental Science 8:3 (2005), pp 218 225 REPORT Blackwell Publishing, Ltd. Processing demands in belief-desire reasoning: inhibition or general difficulty? Ori Friedman 1 and Alan M. Leslie 1, 2 1.

More information

Mechanisms of Belief-Desire Reasoning Inhibition and Bias

Mechanisms of Belief-Desire Reasoning Inhibition and Bias PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Mechanisms of Belief-Desire Reasoning Inhibition and Bias Ori Friedman and Alan M. Leslie Department of Psychology and Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University

More information

Mechanisms of belief-desire reasoning: Inhibition and bias

Mechanisms of belief-desire reasoning: Inhibition and bias Psychological Science, in press Mechanisms of belief-desire reasoning: Inhibition and bias Ori Friedman and Alan M. Leslie Department of Psychology and Center for Cognitive Science Rutgers University 3607

More information

The Development of Theory of Mind in Early Childhood*

The Development of Theory of Mind in Early Childhood* SOCIAL COGNITION The Development of Theory of Mind in Early Childhood* 1 Janet Wilde Astington, PhD, 2 Margaret J. Edward, MA 1 Institute of Child Study, University of Toronto, Canada 2 School District

More information

Theory of Mind. By Margaret Alic. Updated on Dec 23, 2009 from

Theory of Mind. By Margaret Alic. Updated on Dec 23, 2009 from Theory of Mind By Margaret Alic Updated on Dec 23, 2009 from http://www.education.com/reference/article/theory-of-mind/ Theory of mind (ToM) is the intuitive understanding of one's own and other people's

More information

Acknowledgements. Theory of Mind. Theory of Mind Evaluation and Clinical Applications. Theory of Mind and Language. Evaluating Theory of Mind

Acknowledgements. Theory of Mind. Theory of Mind Evaluation and Clinical Applications. Theory of Mind and Language. Evaluating Theory of Mind Theory of Mind Evaluation and Clinical Applications Allison Bean, M.S., CCC-SLP Beth Walker, M.A., CCC-SLP/A Karla McGregor, Ph.D., CCC-SLP University of Iowa Acknowledgements University of Iowa Word Learning

More information

Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups

Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups Megan Gresholdt Allison M. Haskill, Ph.D Augustana College Megan-Gresholdt@augustana.edu AllisonHaskill@augustana.edu What is Theory of Mind (ToM)?

More information

An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions

An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions Leigh-Ann Heale Classic autism was first described by Kanner in 1943 (cited in Ellis,

More information

A behaviour analysis of Theory of Mind: from interpretation to application

A behaviour analysis of Theory of Mind: from interpretation to application A behaviour analysis of Theory of Mind: from interpretation to application Francesca degli Espinosa, Ph.D., BCBA-D, CPsychol. TXABA, Houston, March 2018 Theory of Mind The ability to attribute independent

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/1176170/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Mindblind Eyes: An Absence of Spontaneous Theory of Mind in Asperger Syndrome Atsushi Senju,* Victoria Southgate, Sarah White,

More information

USING VIDEO MODELING AND REINFORCEMENT TO TEACH PERSPECTIVE-TAKING SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM LINDA A. LEBLANC AND ANDREA M.

USING VIDEO MODELING AND REINFORCEMENT TO TEACH PERSPECTIVE-TAKING SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM LINDA A. LEBLANC AND ANDREA M. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2003, 36, 253 257 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 2003) USING VIDEO MODELING AND REINFORCEMENT TO TEACH PERSPECTIVE-TAKING SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM LINDA A. LEBLANC AND ANDREA

More information

Language Facility and Theory of Mind Development in Deaf Children

Language Facility and Theory of Mind Development in Deaf Children Empirical Articles Language Facility and Theory of Mind Development in Deaf Children A. Lyn Jackson University of Cardiff, Wales Deaf children with signing parents, nonnative signing deaf children, children

More information

Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children

Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children Confronting the Language Barrier: Theory of Mind in Deaf Children 1 Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children Anna C. Jones a, Roberto Gutierrez b, Amanda K. Ludlow b c a Deafness,CognitionandLanguageResearchCentre,UniversityCollegeLondon,LondonWC1H0PD,UnitedKingdom

More information

Insights into Theory of Mind from Deafness and Autism

Insights into Theory of Mind from Deafness and Autism Insights into Theory of Mind from Deafness and Autism CANDIDA C. PETERSON AND MICHAEL SIEGAL Abstract: This paper summarizes the results of 11 separate studies of deaf children s performance on standard

More information

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION INTRODUCTION Research on the Deaf Brain is beginning to provide a new evidence base for policy and practice in relation to intervention with deaf children. This talk outlines the multi-channel nature of

More information

Brief Report: Prompted Pretend Play in Autism1

Brief Report: Prompted Pretend Play in Autism1 Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 27, No., 997 Brief Reports Brief Report: Prompted Pretend Play in Autism Tony Charman2 University College London Simon Baron-Cohen University of Cambridge

More information

[ 2011] Lu Wang ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

[ 2011] Lu Wang ALL RIGHTS RESERVED [ 2011] Lu Wang ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ! PRESCHOOLERS AND ADULTS BELIEF REASONING AND TASK DEMAND By LU WANG A thesis submitted to the Graduate School New Brunswick Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey

More information

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: In school-aged children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), what oral narrative elements differ from their typically developing peers? Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University

More information

Guessing versus choosing and seeing versus believing in false belief tasks

Guessing versus choosing and seeing versus believing in false belief tasks 451 British Journal of Developmental Psychology (2005), 23, 451 469 q 2005 The British Psychological Society The British Psychological Society www.bpsjournals.co.uk Guessing versus choosing and seeing

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

Simplifying Second-order Belief Attribution: What Facilitates Children s Performance on Measures of Conceptual Understanding?

Simplifying Second-order Belief Attribution: What Facilitates Children s Performance on Measures of Conceptual Understanding? Reprinted Article Simplifying Second-order Belief Attribution: What Facilitates Children s Performance on Measures of Conceptual Understanding? Greig J. Coull and Susan R. Leekam, University of Durham,

More information

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism [Slide 1]: Welcome Welcome to the New Mexico TEAM technical assistance module on making eligibility determinations under the category of autism. This module will review the guidance of the NM TEAM section

More information

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING AND THE FALSE BELIEF TASK: A DEVELOPMENTAL STUDY

COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING AND THE FALSE BELIEF TASK: A DEVELOPMENTAL STUDY University of Plymouth PEARL https://pearl.plymouth.ac.uk 04 University of Plymouth Research Theses 01 Research Theses Main Collection 2007 COUNTERFACTUAL THINKING AND THE FALSE BELIEF TASK: A DEVELOPMENTAL

More information

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Deaf-blindness

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Deaf-blindness [Slide 1] Welcome Welcome to the New Mexico TEAM technical assistance module on making eligibility determinations under the category of deaf-blindness. This module will review the guidance of the NM TEAM

More information

Research on theory of mind in deaf education

Research on theory of mind in deaf education Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Independent Studies and Capstones Program in Audiology and Communication Sciences 2011 Research on theory of mind in deaf education Ashley

More information

A Computational Developmental Model of the Implicit False Belief Task

A Computational Developmental Model of the Implicit False Belief Task A Computational Developmental Model of the Implicit False Belief Task Vincent G. Berthiaume (Vincent.Berthiaume@McGill.ca) Department of Psychology, 105 Dr. Penfield Avenue Montreal, Qc, H3A 1B1 Canada

More information

Comprehension and Story Retelling. Abilities among Children with ASD. Compared to Children with Typical. Development

Comprehension and Story Retelling. Abilities among Children with ASD. Compared to Children with Typical. Development BAR-ILAN UNIVERSITY Comprehension and Story Retelling Abilities among Children with ASD Compared to Children with Typical Development Liat Kadosh Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for

More information

",! " ", " " " " "!!! " $.

,!  ,     !!!  $. 27 35 5,6, Theory of Mind,,!, #!! Theory of Mind, ToM Theory of Mind (ToM)! ToM!!! $ Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 985; Astington &)! (Jenkins, 995; Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2,, $ #,# (joint attention)

More information

In this chapter we discuss validity issues for quantitative research and for qualitative research.

In this chapter we discuss validity issues for quantitative research and for qualitative research. Chapter 8 Validity of Research Results (Reminder: Don t forget to utilize the concept maps and study questions as you study this and the other chapters.) In this chapter we discuss validity issues for

More information

Understanding Perspective-taking, False Belief, and Deception from a Behavioural Perspective

Understanding Perspective-taking, False Belief, and Deception from a Behavioural Perspective Understanding Perspective-taking, False Belief, and Deception from a Behavioural Perspective Louise McHugh, Yvonne Barnes-Holmes, Dermot Barnes-Holmes, National University of Ireland, Maynooth Authors'

More information

Question 2. The Deaf community has its own culture.

Question 2. The Deaf community has its own culture. Question 1 The only communication mode the Deaf community utilizes is Sign Language. False The Deaf Community includes hard of hearing people who do quite a bit of voicing. Plus there is writing and typing

More information

The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis

The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis Developmental Science 1:2 pp 255-259 REPORT The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis Gergely Csibra 1 and György Gergely 2 1. MRC Cognitive Development Unit,

More information

The Deaf Brain. Bencie Woll Deafness Cognition and Language Research Centre

The Deaf Brain. Bencie Woll Deafness Cognition and Language Research Centre The Deaf Brain Bencie Woll Deafness Cognition and Language Research Centre 1 Outline Introduction: the multi-channel nature of language Audio-visual language BSL Speech processing Silent speech Auditory

More information

Meeting someone with disabilities etiquette

Meeting someone with disabilities etiquette Meeting someone with disabilities etiquette Many people unsure how to go about meeting someone with a disability because they don t want to say or do the wrong thing. Here are a few tips to keep in mind

More information

Eye-tracking brings focus to 'theory of mind'

Eye-tracking brings focus to 'theory of mind' NEWS Eye-tracking brings focus to 'theory of mind' BY VIRGINIA GEWIN 29 JULY 2009 People with Asperger syndrome exhibit a mix of social abilities that has long puzzled researchers. For example, when read

More information

Siblings and Theory of Mind in Deaf Native Signing Children

Siblings and Theory of Mind in Deaf Native Signing Children Siblings and Theory of Mind in Deaf Native Signing Children Tyron Woolfe Stephen C. Want Michael Siegal University of Sheffield We report a study designed to examine the basis of theory of mind (ToM) reasoning

More information

OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION

OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS IN EMERGING ADULTS Douglas J. Scambler, Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist Presented at the University of Wyoming November 6, 2012 OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION What are the autism spectrum

More information

Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children Jones, Anna C.; Gutierrez, Roberto; Ludlow, Amanda

Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children Jones, Anna C.; Gutierrez, Roberto; Ludlow, Amanda Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children Jones, Anna C.; Gutierrez, Roberto; Ludlow, Amanda DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2015.06.005 License: Creative Commons: Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs

More information

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not.

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not. Autism Summary Autism What is Autism? The Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a developmental disability that can have significant implications on a child's ability to function and interface with the world

More information

PATIENCE YOU MUST HAVE MY YOUNG PADAWAN"

PATIENCE YOU MUST HAVE MY YOUNG PADAWAN PATIENCE YOU MUST HAVE MY YOUNG PADAWAN" HOW YODA CAN HELP STUDENTS WITH ASPERGER DEAL WITH FRUSTRATION. Timothy P Kowalski, M.A.,C.C.C. Professional Communication Services, Inc. 1401-A Edgewater Dr. Orlando,

More information

Multi-modality in Language. Bencie Woll Deafness Cognition and Language Research Centre, UCL

Multi-modality in Language. Bencie Woll Deafness Cognition and Language Research Centre, UCL Multi-modality in Language Bencie Woll Deafness Cognition and Language Research Centre, UCL 1 Language evolved, develops, and is processed in rich multimodal settings 2 Language and Communication Human

More information

Durham Research Online

Durham Research Online Durham Research Online Deposited in DRO: 15 October 2010 Version of attached le: Accepted Version Peer-review status of attached le: Peer-reviewed Citation for published item: Lind, S.E. and Bowler, D.M.

More information

Good Communication Starts at Home

Good Communication Starts at Home Good Communication Starts at Home It is important to remember the primary and most valuable thing you can do for your deaf or hard of hearing baby at home is to communicate at every available opportunity,

More information

5. Theory of Mind. Toyoaki Nishida Kyoto University

5. Theory of Mind. Toyoaki Nishida Kyoto University Conversational Informatics, November 2, 2016 5. Theory of Mind Toyoaki Nishida Kyoto University Copyright 2016, Toyoaki Nishida, Atsushi Nakazawa, Yoshimasa Ohmoto, Yasser Mohammad, At,Inc. All Rights

More information

Representational Difficulties in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders

Representational Difficulties in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders Representational Difficulties in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders Sharon Weiss-Kapp Med CCC SL/P Adjunct Clinical Assistant Professor MGH- Institute of Health Professions Boston MA Senior Clinical

More information

AP Psychology -- Chapter 02 Review Research Methods in Psychology

AP Psychology -- Chapter 02 Review Research Methods in Psychology AP Psychology -- Chapter 02 Review Research Methods in Psychology 1. In the opening vignette, to what was Alicia's condition linked? The death of her parents and only brother 2. What did Pennebaker s study

More information

MA 1 Notes. Deaf vs deaf p. 3 MA1 F 13

MA 1 Notes. Deaf vs deaf p. 3 MA1 F 13 Name Period MA 1 Notes Fingerspelling Consider frngerspelling to be like your handwriting. Being clear is vital to being understood, be confident enough not to worry, whether each letter is exactly right,

More information

Language and Theory of Mind in Children with Hearing Impairment - A Preliminary Study

Language and Theory of Mind in Children with Hearing Impairment - A Preliminary Study ToM in HI JISHA 26 (1), 36-42 Language and Theory of Mind in Children with Hearing Impairment - A Preliminary Study Meera S. S. 1, Thulasi Prasad 2 & Rashmi. J. Bhat 3 Abstract Theory of Mind (ToM) is

More information

"Few are my friends Tried and true But one by one I lose my few"

Few are my friends Tried and true But one by one I lose my few SOCIALISATION Social Interaction "Few are my friends Tried and true But one by one I lose my few" Impaired social awareness ar~d skills are perhaps the most distinguishing features of students with Autism

More information

Views of autistic adults on assessment in the early years

Views of autistic adults on assessment in the early years Views of autistic adults on what should be assessed and how assessment should be conducted on children with autism in the early years Summary of autistic adults views on assessment 1. Avoid drawing negative

More information

Solving belief problems: toward a task analysis

Solving belief problems: toward a task analysis COGNITION Cognition 66 (1998) 1 31 Solving belief problems: toward a task analysis Daniel Roth a, Alan M. Leslie b, *,1 a Department of Psychology, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel b Department of

More information

Tips When Meeting A Person Who Has A Disability

Tips When Meeting A Person Who Has A Disability Tips When Meeting A Person Who Has A Disability Many people find meeting someone with a disability to be an awkward experience because they are afraid they will say or do the wrong thing; perhaps you are

More information

Deaf Children and Young People

Deaf Children and Young People Deaf Children and Young People Professor Barry Wright Clinical Lead - National Deaf Children Young People and Family Service, York National Deaf Child and Adolescent Mental Health Service (NDCAMHS) Definitions

More information

Emotion recognition in children with Down syndrome: ability, influential factors and supporting development

Emotion recognition in children with Down syndrome: ability, influential factors and supporting development Emotion recognition in children with Down syndrome: ability, influential factors and supporting development Katie Cebula 1, Jennifer Wishart 1, Diane Willis 2, Tom Pitcairn 1 & Amanda Gillooly 1 1 University

More information

Other Papers. Predictions of Actions and Their Justifications in False-Belief Tasks: The Role of Executive Function

Other Papers. Predictions of Actions and Their Justifications in False-Belief Tasks: The Role of Executive Function Other Papers Polish Psychological Bulletin 2014, vol 45(4), 500-510 DOI - 10.2478/ppb-2014-0060 Adam Putko * Agata Złotogórska ** Predictions of Actions and Their Justifications in False-Belief Tasks:

More information

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments Consciousness and Cognition 9, 215 219 (2000) doi:10.1006/ccog.2000.0438, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

More information

Fukuoka University of Education

Fukuoka University of Education Tomoko Sugimura sugitomo@fukuoka-edu.ac.jp Fukuoka University of Education 18 5 6 facial perception, gender-discrimination, young children Developmental studies have demonstrated that young children inaccurately

More information

Communication Options and Opportunities. A Factsheet for Parents of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children

Communication Options and Opportunities. A Factsheet for Parents of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children Communication Options and Opportunities A Factsheet for Parents of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children This factsheet provides information on the Communication Options and Opportunities available to Deaf

More information

Gold and Hohwy, Rationality and Schizophrenic Delusion

Gold and Hohwy, Rationality and Schizophrenic Delusion PHIL 5983: Irrational Belief Seminar Prof. Funkhouser 2/6/13 Gold and Hohwy, Rationality and Schizophrenic Delusion There are two plausible departments of rationality: procedural and content. Procedural

More information

Allen Independent School District Bundled LOTE Curriculum Beginning 2017 School Year ASL III

Allen Independent School District Bundled LOTE Curriculum Beginning 2017 School Year ASL III Allen Independent School District Bundled LOTE Curriculum Beginning 2017 School Year ASL III Page 1 of 19 Revised: 8/1/2017 114.36. American Sign Language, Level III (One Credit), Adopted 2014. (a) General

More information

Is Asperger Syndrome The Same As Autism?

Is Asperger Syndrome The Same As Autism? Asperger Syndrome BY: Rosalynd Lord Asperger Syndrome is a developmental disorder falling within the autistic spectrum affecting two- way social interaction, verbal and nonverbal communication and a reluctance

More information

REPORT Why specific developmental disorders are not specific: On-line and developmental effects in autism and dyslexia

REPORT Why specific developmental disorders are not specific: On-line and developmental effects in autism and dyslexia Developmental Science 1:2 pp 267 272 REPORT Why specific developmental disorders are not specific: On-line and developmental effects in autism and dyslexia Uta Frith 1 and Francesca Happé 2 1. MRC Cognitive

More information

READY. Book. CURRICULUM ASSOCIATES, Inc. A Quick-Study Program TEST

READY. Book. CURRICULUM ASSOCIATES, Inc. A Quick-Study Program TEST A Quick-Study Program TEST Book 6 READY LONGER READING PASSAGES READY Reviews Key Concepts in Reading Comprehension Provides Practice Answering a Variety of Comprehension Questions Develops Test-Taking

More information

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi We ll cover Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) ASD in young adults Social Communication (definition, components, importance,

More information

EL.LE ISSN Vol. 4 Num. 1 Marzo Ada Bier (Università Ca Foscari Venezia, Italia)

EL.LE ISSN Vol. 4 Num. 1 Marzo Ada Bier (Università Ca Foscari Venezia, Italia) EL.LE ISSN 2280-6792 Vol. 4 Num. 1 Marzo 2015 Dörnyei, Z.; Kubanyiova, M. (2014). Motivating Learners, Motivating Teachers: Building vision in the language classroom. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University

More information

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Alex Rice M.Cl.Sc SLP Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication

More information

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Marci Laurel, MA, CCC-SLP mlaurel@salud.unm.edu and Services (UCEDD) 1 Training Objectives Participants will: 1. Name at least three communication

More information

Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary. NLPcourses.com

Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary. NLPcourses.com Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary NLPcourses.com Table of Contents Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE by Richard Bandler Summary... 6 Chapter 1 Who s Driving the Bus?... 6 Chapter 2 Running Your Own

More information

Keeping Autism in Mind

Keeping Autism in Mind Page 1 of 7 With a clear understanding of the nature of Autism there is much that can be done to help people with Autism through their everyday life. In highlighting some of the features of Autism this

More information

Municipal Employee Guide to Autism Awareness

Municipal Employee Guide to Autism Awareness RISK Winter 2014 Municipal Employee Guide to Autism Awareness A D M M R M A V I S O A D M R Y C I N O I S T R A T M M I T T I V E E Michigan Municipal Risk Management Authority Administrative Advisory

More information

Autism & intellectual disabilities. How to deal with confusing concepts

Autism & intellectual disabilities. How to deal with confusing concepts Autism & intellectual disabilities How to deal with confusing concepts dr. Gerard J. Nijhof Orthopedagogue / GZ-psychologist PhD, Free University Amsterdam Private practice contact@gerardnijhof.nl +31

More information

How Can Memory-Augmented Neural Networks Pass a False-Belief Task?

How Can Memory-Augmented Neural Networks Pass a False-Belief Task? How Can Memory-Augmented Neural Networks Pass a False-Belief Task? Erin Grant, Aida Nematzadeh, and Thomas L. Griffiths University of California, Berkeley {eringrant, nematzadeh, tom griffiths}@berkeley.edu

More information

SERVING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM IN SCHOOLS 1

SERVING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM IN SCHOOLS 1 SERVING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM IN SCHOOLS 1 Serving Students with Autism in Schools Beth Kost Abnormal Psychology (PSY 406) SERVING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM IN SCHOOLS 2 Serving Students with Autism in Schools

More information

SIGN LANGUAGE IN CO-ENROLLMENT EDUCATION FOR DEAF CHILDREN

SIGN LANGUAGE IN CO-ENROLLMENT EDUCATION FOR DEAF CHILDREN SIGN LANGUAGE IN CO-ENROLLMENT EDUCATION FOR DEAF CHILDREN Gladys Tang and Chris, Kun-Man Yiu, Centre for Sign Linguistics and Deaf Studies, Chinese University of Hong Kong ABSTRACT When inclusive education

More information

Communication Tips for Serving Individuals With Dementia. Begin

Communication Tips for Serving Individuals With Dementia. Begin Communication Tips for Serving Individuals With Dementia Begin Index 1 Communication Vital to Function and Well-Being...2 Adapting Your Communication Style...3 Tip 1: Gain Attention and Trust...4 Tip 2:

More information

Module 2/3 Research Strategies: How Psychologists Ask and Answer Questions

Module 2/3 Research Strategies: How Psychologists Ask and Answer Questions Myers PSYCHOLOGY 8 th Edition in Modules Module 2/3 Research Strategies: How Psychologists Ask and Answer Questions 1 The Need for Psychological Science Psychologists, like all scientists, use the scientific

More information

Sign Language and Early Childhood Development

Sign Language and Early Childhood Development University of Arkansas, Fayetteville ScholarWorks@UARK Rehabilitation, Human Resources and Communication Disorders Undergraduate Honors Theses Rehabilitation, Human Resources and Communication Disorders

More information

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Uri Hasson (uhasson@princeton.edu) Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 USA Geoffrey P. Goodwin (ggoodwin@princeton.edu)

More information

Pointing, reference, explicitness

Pointing, reference, explicitness UNIVERSITY OF BRISTOL CENTRE FOR DEAF STUDIES Introduction and maintenance of reference in narratives: Evidence from native and non-native signers of British Sign Language Kearsy Cormier Deafness Cognition

More information

Running Head: TRUST INACCURATE INFORMANTS 1. In the Absence of Conflicting Testimony Young Children Trust Inaccurate Informants

Running Head: TRUST INACCURATE INFORMANTS 1. In the Absence of Conflicting Testimony Young Children Trust Inaccurate Informants Running Head: TRUST INACCURATE INFORMANTS 1 In the Absence of Conflicting Testimony Young Children Trust Inaccurate Informants Kimberly E. Vanderbilt, Gail D. Heyman, and David Liu University of California,

More information

Disparity of Non-verbal Language Learning

Disparity of Non-verbal Language Learning Bethel Sileshi Disparity of Non-verbal Language Learning Identical Rates of Acquisition, Neurological Adaptations, Cognitive Outcomes Science 100 Capstone, University of Alberta April 7, 2014 Introduction

More information

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: Can joint attention, imitation, and/or play skills predict future language abilities of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)? Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology)

More information

Minds, Modules, and Meta-Analysis

Minds, Modules, and Meta-Analysis Child Development, May/June 2001, Volume 72, Number 3, Pages 696 701 Minds, Modules, and Meta-Analysis Brian J. Scholl and Alan M. Leslie Wellman and colleagues meta-analysis of performance on the false-belief

More information

Learning to use a sign language

Learning to use a sign language 85 Chapter 8 Learning to use a sign language It is easy for a young child to learn a complete sign language. A child will first begin to understand the signs that others use, especially for people and

More information

Involving people with autism: a guide for public authorities

Involving people with autism: a guide for public authorities People with autism frequently don t receive the services and support that they need and they are usually excluded from the planning and development of services and policies. This needs to change. This

More information

Deaf Children Attending Different School Environments: Sign Language Abilities and Theory of Mind

Deaf Children Attending Different School Environments: Sign Language Abilities and Theory of Mind Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education Deaf Children Attending Different School Environments: Sign Language Abilities and Theory of Mind Elena Tomasuolo*,1, Giovanni Valeri 2, Alessio Di Renzo 1, Patrizio

More information

Alternative Augmentative Communication. American Sign Language

Alternative Augmentative Communication. American Sign Language Alternative Augmentative Communication American Sign Language Autism Spectrum Disorder A variable neuro-developmental disorder with impairments in communication, social interaction, and restrictive and

More information

CLASSROOM & PLAYGROUND

CLASSROOM & PLAYGROUND The information contained in this booklet was most generously provided to Autism South Africa by the UK National Autistic Society The production and printing of this and 12 other brochures was made possible

More information

Don t wait-and-see, research suggests

Don t wait-and-see, research suggests Don t wait-and-see, research suggests By Lauren Lowry Hanen S-LP and Clinical Staff Writer Historically, intervening with the group of children known as late talkers has been the source of some debate

More information

Conversations inviting change: a pragmatic approach to Narrative Medicine. Fondazione ISTUD, 30 October Dr John Launer

Conversations inviting change: a pragmatic approach to Narrative Medicine. Fondazione ISTUD, 30 October Dr John Launer Conversations inviting change: a pragmatic approach to Narrative Medicine Fondazione ISTUD, 30 October 2015 Dr John Launer Introduction: where these ideas come from. By profession I m both a family physician,

More information

Don t Mention the Marble! The Role of Attentional Processes in False-Belief Tasks

Don t Mention the Marble! The Role of Attentional Processes in False-Belief Tasks Rev.Phil.Psych. DOI 10.1007/s13164-015-0290-z Don t Mention the Marble! The Role of Attentional Processes in False-Belief Tasks Paula Rubio-Fernández 1 & Bart Geurts 2 # Springer Science+Business Media

More information

Technical Report #2 Testing Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing

Technical Report #2 Testing Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing Technical Report #2 Testing Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing September 4, 2015 Lori A. Day, PhD 1, Elizabeth B. Adams Costa, PhD 2, and Susan Engi Raiford, PhD 3 1 Gallaudet University 2 The River

More information

Developing a policy for sexual health education for children and young people with Autism Spectrum Disorders and learning disabilities

Developing a policy for sexual health education for children and young people with Autism Spectrum Disorders and learning disabilities Scottish Journal of Residential Child Care Oct/Nov 2011 Vol.11, No.1 Developing a policy for sexual health education for children and young people with Autism Diane Ballantyne Abstract Linn Moor School

More information

Understanding Autism. Julie Smith, MA, BCBA. November 12, 2015

Understanding Autism. Julie Smith, MA, BCBA. November 12, 2015 Understanding Autism Julie Smith, MA, BCBA November 12, 2015 2 Overview What is Autism New DSM-5; changes to diagnosis Potential causes Communication strategies Managing difficult behaviors Effective programming

More information

CHINO VALLEY UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT INSTRUCTIONAL GUIDE AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE 1

CHINO VALLEY UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT INSTRUCTIONAL GUIDE AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE 1 CHINO VALLEY UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT INSTRUCTIONAL GUIDE AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE 1 Course Number 5716 Department Elective Length of Course One (1) year/two (2) semesters Grade Level 9-12 Prerequisite None

More information

MA 1 Notes. moving the hand may be needed.

MA 1 Notes. moving the hand may be needed. Name Period MA 1 Notes Fingerspelling Consider frngerspelling to be like your. Being clear is vital to being understood, be enough not to worry, whether each letter is exactly right, and be able to spell

More information

Director of Testing and Disability Services Phone: (706) Fax: (706) E Mail:

Director of Testing and Disability Services Phone: (706) Fax: (706) E Mail: Angie S. Baker Testing and Disability Services Director of Testing and Disability Services Phone: (706)737 1469 Fax: (706)729 2298 E Mail: tds@gru.edu Deafness is an invisible disability. It is easy for

More information

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing This document contains 4 examples of writing and feedback comments from Level 2A lab reports, and 4 steps to help you apply

More information

Autism. Laura Schreibman HDP1 11/29/07 MAIN DIAGNOSTIC FEATURES OF AUTISTIC DISORDER. Deficits in social attachment and behavior

Autism. Laura Schreibman HDP1 11/29/07 MAIN DIAGNOSTIC FEATURES OF AUTISTIC DISORDER. Deficits in social attachment and behavior Autism Laura Schreibman HDP1 11/29/07 MAIN DIAGNOSTIC FEATURES OF AUTISTIC DISORDER Deficits in social attachment and behavior Deficits in verbal and nonverbal communication Presence of perseverative,

More information