The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Supplementary Norms for Individuals with Autism

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Supplementary Norms for Individuals with Autism"

Transcription

1 Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 28, No. 4, 1998 The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Supplementary Norms for Individuals with Autism Alice S. Carter,1,9 Fred R. Volkmar,2 Sara S. Sparrow,2 Jing-Jen Wang,3 Catherine Lord,4 Geraldine Dawson,5 Eric Fombonne,6 Katherine Loveland,7 Gary Mesibov,8 and Eric Schopler8 Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales Special Population norms are presented for four groups of individuals with autism: (a) mute children under 10 years of age; (b) children with at least some verbal skills under 10 years of age; (c) mute individuals who are 10 years of age or older; and (d) individuals with at least some verbal skills who are 10 years of age or older. The sample included 684 autistic individuals ascertained from cases referred for the DSM-IV autism/pdd field trial collaborative study and five university sites with expertise in autism. Young children had higher standard scores than older individuals across all Vineland domains. In the Communication domain, younger verbal children were least impaired, older mute individuals most impaired, and younger mute and older verbal individuals in the midrange. Verbal individuals achieved higher scores in Daily Living Skills than mute individuals. The expected profile of a relative weakness in Socialization and relative strength in Daily Living Skills was obtained with age-equivalent but not standard scores. Results highlight the importance of employing Vineland special population norms as well as national norms when evaluating individuals with autism. KEY WORDS: Vineland Behavior Scales; special population norms; autism. INTRODUCTION This paper presents special population norms on the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales for individu- 1Departmenl of Psychology and Yale School of Medicine Child Study Center, New Haven, Connecticut. 2Yale School of Medicine Child Study Center and Yale University Department of Psychology, New Haven, Connecticut. 3American Guidance Service, Minneapolis, Minnesota. 4Department of Psychiatry, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois. 5Department of Psychology, University of Seattle, Seattle, Washington. 6Medical Research Council, University of London, London, United Kingdom. Formerly at the 1NSERM, Centre dc Alfred Binet, Paris, France. 7Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, University of Texas, Houston, Texas. 8Division TEACCH, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina. 9Address all correspondence to Alice S. Carter, Yale University, Department of Psychology, 2 Hillhouse Avenue, New Haven, Connecticut als with autism. Additional analyses within the special population normative sample address some of the limitations of employing standard and age equivalent scores from the Vineland national standardization sample with individuals with autism. Autistic individuals show a unique pattern or profile of scores across dimensions of adaptive behavior relative to age- and mental age-matched peers (Ando, Yoshimura, & Wakabayashi, 1980; Freeman, Ritvo, Yokota, Childs, & Pollard, 1988; Jacobson & Ackerman, 1990; Loveland & Kelley, 1988,1991; Rodrigue, Morgan, & Geffken, 1991; Sloan & Marcus, 1981; Volkmar et al., 1987). Studies of adaptive behavior consistently demonstrate that individuals with autism evidence significant deficits in socialization, relative strengths in daily living skills, and intermediate scores in communication (e.g., Loveland & Kelley, 1991; Volkmar et al., 1987). In addition, recent evidence suggests that children with autism show greater intradomain scatter when compared to typi /98/ / Plenum Publishing Corporation

2 288 Carter et al. cally functioning and retarded children (VanMeter, Fein, Morris, Waterhouse, & Allen, 1997). In light of the unique pattern of adaptive functioning observed in individuals with autism (cf. Carter, Gilham, Sparrow, & Volkmar, 1997), comparing individuals with autism to a similarly affected normative group rather than relying on national norms may provide a more appropriate context for understanding a particular autistic individual's current adaptive functioning. The assessment of adaptive behavior in individuals with autism has traditionally been employed along with standardized measures of intellectual functioning to determine whether or not to assign a diagnosis of mental retardation. Approximately three fourths of individuals with autism obtain verbal IQ scores in the mentally retarded range of cognitive functioning (Ritvo et al., 1989) with estimates of mental retardation among individuals with autism as high as 85% (Volkmar & Cohen, 1986, 1988). In addition to the central role of adaptive behavior in diagnosing mental retardation, the assessment of adaptive behavior is clinically informative for educational and vocational planning for individuals with autism throughout the spectrum of intellectual functioning. The traditional inclusion of adaptive behavior assessments to diagnose or rule out mental retardation contributed to the current recognition that it is important to assess adaptive skills across the spectrum of cognitive functioning within autism. To maximize the clinical utility of the assessment of adaptive behavior for individuals with autism, this paper presents new supplemental norms for individuals with autism. These norms enable clinicians and researchers to compare an individual with autism's adaptive functioning to a peer group of similarly affected individuals as well as to national normative data. Diagnosing or ruling out the presence of mental retardation is an essential component of a comprehensive evaluation for an individual with autism (Klin, Carter, & Sparrow, 1997). The most widely accepted definition of mental retardation involves the presence of subaverage cognitive functioning and deficits in adaptive behavior with onset prior to age 18 (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994; Grossman, 1983). Significant deficits in adaptive behavior are demonstrated by significant impairments in the performance of daily activities required for personal and social sufficiency (Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) as measured by standardized instruments designed to assess adaptive behavior. Although there are state and local differences in the adoption of specific criteria for deficits in adaptive behavior, the development of instruments that provide national norms such as the Comprehensive Test of Adaptive Behavior (Adams, 1984) and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Sparrow et al., 1984) has facilitated the use of specific quantifiable guidelines. For example, to assign a diagnosis of mental retardation, some states require performance that falls approximately below the fourth percentile (e.g., Standard Score below 70-75) in two of the following adaptive behavior domains: (a) Communication, (b) Daily Living Skills/Self Help Skills, (c) Socialization/Social Functioning/Interpersonal Skills, and (d) Motor Skills. Although the assessment of mental retardation is central to a comprehensive assessment of an individual with autism (Klin et al., 1997), it is critical to recognize that individuals who are affected with both autism and mental retardation differ significantly from individuals who only meet criterion for mental retardation in the pattern of acquisition of adaptive behavior and other competencies. Indeed, significant progress has been made in the field of mental retardation by recognizing that it is not a homogeneous entity (Bailey, Phillips, & Rutter, 1996). Thus, when evaluating an individual with autism and mental retardation, the use of special population norms for mentally retarded individuals does not provide an adequate comparison group. Independent of their level of cognitive functioning, individuals with autism have a profound inability to relate to people as well as language abnormalities and stereotyped repetitive behaviors (Cohen, Paul, & Volkmar, 1986; Fein, Pennington, Markowitz, Braverman, & Waterhouse, 1986; Frith, 1989; Happe, 1994; Lord, 1993; Rutter, 1978; Sigman, 1995; Wing & Gould, 1979). Indeed, Kanner's (1943) original description of the syndrome of autism emphasized the social impairment of individuals with autism. This focus on the centrality of social deficits has continued in more recent definitions and descriptions of the clinical features of autism (Bailey et al., 1996; Cohen et al., 1986; Fein et al., 1986; Rutter, 1978; Wing & Gould, 1979). In addition, the most recent edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; APA, 1994) is consistent with Kanner's early conceptualization of autism in highlighting social deficits. Thus, the deficits observed in social functioning are far greater than what one would expect, even given the typically observed cognitive limitations. As the social and linguistic deficits and

3 Vineland Supplementary Norms for Autism 289 deviance present in autism impact on the day-to-day performance of adaptive behaviors, it is not surprising that even high-functioning individuals with autism typically show significant deficits in adaptive behaviors (cf. Carter et al., 1996). Recently, Volkmar and colleagues argued that social deficits in individuals with autism can be quantified by determining the magnitude of the discrepancy between a standard score in the adaptive domain of socialization and mental age (Volkmar, Carter, Sparrow, & Cicchetti, 1993; Gilham, Carter, Volkmar, & Sparrow, 1998). This conceptual model emphasizes developmental delay in the acquisition of adaptive social behaviors in contrast to previous categorical models that highlighted developmental social, linguistic and behavioral deviance. They demonstrated that the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Sparrow et al., 1984) were sensitive to the overall impairments observed in autism and could be used to quantify the social deficits observed among autistic individuals. VanMeter et al. (1997) examined intratest scatter among preschoolers who were carefully matched to either typically functioning or mentally deficient children on Vineland raw scores to highlight the presence of deviance as well as delay in the acquisition of adaptive behavior in autism. While these studies have implications for research in the area of autism, they also highlight the clinical utility of the assessment of adaptive behavior in autism and more specifically the use of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales in the assessment of adaptive behavior in autism. Autistic individuals show a unique pattern or profile of scores across dimensions of adaptive behavior relative to age- and mental age-matched peers (Ando et al., 1980; Freeman et al., 1988; Jacobson & Ackerman, 1990: Loveland & Kelley, 1988, 1991; Rodrigue et al., 1991; Sloan & Marcus, 1981; Volkmar et al., 1987). Several studies have employed the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales to document this unique pattern of adaptive behavior across domains (i.e., Communication, Daily Living Skills, and Socialization) in individuals with autism as compared with individuals with mental retardation. These studies consistently indicate that while individuals with mental retardation have relatively flat profiles across adaptive behavior domains, autistic children and adults evidence significant deficits in the Socialization Domain, when compared to relative strengths in Daily Living Skills, and intermediate scores on Communication (Loveland & Kelley, 1991; Volkmar et al., 1987). A similar relative deficit in socialization is apparent when individuals with autism are matched to normally developing children on the basis of overall adaptive behavior scores (Rodrigue et al., 1991). While the pattern of a relative deficit in socialization and a relative strength in daily living skills is apparent early in life (Loveland & Kelley, 1991) and in older individuals with autism (Volkmar et al., 1987), it is important to note that adaptive behavior profiles for individuals with autism may be affected by age (Jacobson & Ackerman, 1990) and level of cognitive functioning (Burack & Volkmar, 1992). Individuals with autism with IQ scores in the average range and higher typically exhibit deficits in adaptive behavior, especially in the socialization domain, coupled with a high frequency of maladaptive behaviors (Burack & Volkmar, 1992; Freeman et al., 1988; Volkmar et al., 1987). While both high- and low-functioning individuals with autism display more scatter in their adaptive behavior profiles than chronologically- and mental aged-matched peers (Volkmar et al., 1987), low-functioning individuals with autism display greater scatter than high-functioning individuals with autism (Burack & Volkmar, 1992). Thus, the chronological and mental age of an individual with autism must be considered when interpreting the level and profile of observed adaptive behavior scores. A number of psychometric issues are relevant to the interpretation of Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale scores for individuals with autism. Specifically, very low functioning individuals with autism may show relatively little scatter in standard scores across domains due to basal effects. Moreover, the use of age-equivalent scores to examine domain differences may be misleading due to lack of comparability in range across domains and subdomains. For example, several Vineland domains have relatively low ceilings (e.g., the highest possible age-equivalent scores in receptive communication is 7 years 10 months). For this reason, standard scores are recommended for use in clinical applications while raw scores are recommended in research applications. When employing raw scores, statistical analyses can be employed to control for chronological and/or mental age differences. The clinical management of children and adults with autism typically requires clinicians to make recommendations regarding appropriate educational services, vocational training and community-based living facilities (Freeman et al., 1991). Adaptive skills may be more predictive of vocational success and level of independence achieved than either academic

4 290 Carter et al. achievement or intellectual abilities. For example, Rumsey, Rapoport, and Sceery (1985) reported that 14 high-functioning men with relative deficits in socialization as assessed by the Vineland Socialization domain had considerable difficulty with vocational adjustment and independent living despite their relatively high IQs and academic achievement. Thus, independent of cognitive level, the assessment of adaptive behavior is a critical component of any comprehensive clinical assessment of individuals with autism (Klin et al., 1997). Specifically, the assessment of adaptive behaviors with standardized instruments: (a) is necessary to document a diagnosis of mental retardation; (b) can inform the diagnosis of autism; (c) can help to identify an individual's strengths and weaknesses for planning educational, vocational, and treatment goals; (d) can serve to monitor an individual's progress over time and across settings (Perry & Factor, 1989); and (e) may be used to document the efficacy of intervention programs. The unique profile and greater scatter evident among individuals with autism suggested the need to develop norms based on autistic samples (Volkmar et al., 1987). When making educational and vocational recommendations or when evaluating an individual's progress over time it is often useful to evaluate an individual with special needs relative to their own peer group rather than in contrast to a typically developing normative group (Sparrow et al., 1984). Successful individual intervention efforts and outcome evaluations require the specification of attainable goals. Employing the national standardization sample norms or even special population norms for mentally retarded individuals may lead to unrealistically high goals in socialization and an underestimate of potential within Daily Living Skills. This paper presents Special Supplemental norms for individuals with autism in four categories: (a) below age 10-mute; (b) ages 10 to 59-mute; (c) below age 10-at least some verbal skills; (d) ages 10 to 59-at least some verbal skills. METHOD Participants The sample included 684 autistic individuals from records of evaluations performed either as part of the DSM-IV autism/pdd field trial collaborative study (137 cases) (Volkmar et al., 1994) or from five different sites: the Yale Child Study Center Developmental Disabilities Clinic (225 cases), Division TEACCH (195 cases), University of Texas (42 cases), University of Washington (43 cases), and the Center Alfred Binet (42 cases). These 684 individuals (497 male, 157 female, 30 cases missing gender) had participated in a comprehensive evaluation that included the administration of the Vineland (survey or expanded form), psychological testing, and diagnostic assessment by highly experienced examiners. As the focus of the majority of the assessments was to document a primary diagnosis of Autism, it is possible that some individuals may have met criteria for disorders that have recently been included in the DSM- IV, including Childhood Disintegrative Disorder (Volkmar & Rutter, 1995) or Asperger syndrome. In 42% of the cases the individual was noted to be largely or entirely mute. The definition of mute, derived verbatim from the DSM-IV field trial, was as follows: "little or no language, uses language only occasionally; only single words or echolalic language with limited or no spontaneous speech." The mean age of cases was years (SD = 7.83); cases ranged from preschool to middle age (ages 2 to 59 years). Assessments of full-scale IQ based on various assessment instruments were available in 606 cases; in the remaining 78 cases the individual was noted to be "untestable." The total sample was divided into four norming groups based on age (younger than 10 years of age or 10 to 59 years of age) and language status (mute and verbal). The distinction between mute and verbal is critical for intervention and can be determined without formal psychological assessment. Although ideally, special population norms would be available for smaller chronological and mental age bands (i.e., preschool, school-aged, adolescents, adults), sample size limitations precluded the creation of more refined groups. The decision to divide the sample, at 10 years of age was based on a desire to acknowledge the different developmental concerns of younger versus older children and adults with autism as well as by pragmatic power constraints within the available sample. Table I presents sample characteristics for each of the of the four norming groups (i.e., gender, IQ, and age). The preponderance of males in each of the autistic norm groups is expected given the increased frequency of the condition in males (Lord & Schopler, 1987; Volkmar, Szatmari, & Sparrow, 1993). In addition, consistent with the increased severity in affected

5 Vineland Supplementary Norms for Autism 291 Gender Male Female Missing Age below 10 (n = 141) n % Table I. Sample Characteristics Verbal Age 10 and above (n = 186) n % Age below 10 (n = 252) n % Mute Age 10 and above (n = 104) n % Age M ±SD Range 6.20 ± ± ± ± IQ < >85 Missing females as compared to males (Volkmar et al., 1993), females were more likely to be mute, x2(1) = 4.08, p <.05, and severely delayed cognitively than males, X2(5) = 11.08,p <.05. All cases had a primary diagnosis of autism as assigned by the clinician on the basis of a comprehensive assessment. In most cases this diagnosis was also supported by the results of assessment instruments specifically developed for autism (e.g., the Children's Autism Rating Scale; Schopler, Reichler, & Renner, 1988; the Autism Behavior Checklist; Krug, Arick, & Almond, 1980; Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised; Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994). As both DSM-IV (APA, 1994) and ICD-10 (World Health Organization [WHO], 1992) diagnoses were under preparation at the time of the data collection, it was not always possible to utilize these most recent categorical diagnostic systems. However, for those cases provided as part of the DSM-IV autism/pdd field trial and for all of the Yale cases, the most recent ICD-10 (WHO, 1992) criteria were applied and only subjects who met the ICD-10 criteria were included in the sample. The majority of the cases were receiving special educational services but were not in residential placements. Cases did vary significantly in the distribution of age, sex, IQ, and adaptive functioning across sites. However, due to the widely discrepant numbers of cases from each site (e.g., 42 vs. 252), site was not included as a control variable in analyses. Measures Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-Survey Form (Sparrow et al., 1984). The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales evaluate children's personal and social sufficiency in a semistructured interview with a primary caregiver. This instrument assesses four areas of adaptive behavior: Communication, Daily Living Skills, Socialization, and Motor Skills. Communication refers to those skills required for receptive, expressive, and written language; Daily Living Skills includes the practical skills needed to take care of oneself and contribute to a household and community; Socialization pertains to those skills needed to get along with others, regulate emotions and behavior, as well as skills involved in leisure activities such as play; Motor Skills, comprising both fine and gross motor items, are typically assessed in individuals below the age of 6 years or when significant difficulty in motor development is suspected. The Vineland also contains a Maladaptive Behavior Domain, which assesses the presence of problematic behaviors that

6 292 Carter et al. interfere with an individual's functioning. The Maladaptive Behavior Domain is administered to children aged 5 and older and includes both behaviors that are common in early development but are less common as children get older and more serious behaviors that are of concern throughout development. Based on a nationally representative sample, reliabilities for each of four domains (Communication, Daily Living Skills, Socialization, Motor Skills) range from In some cases the Expanded Vineland was administered rather than the briefer Survey form for which the supplemental norms are derived. As all of the items on the survey form are included in the Expanded form, the Survey form was derived based on the responses to the shared items. Concurrent validity between Vineland (Sparrow et al., 1984) and the AAMD Adaptive Behavior Scale School Edition (Lambert, Windmiller, Thanringer, & Cole, 1981) is good (Perry & Factor, 1989). Statistical Analysis Percentile ranks corresponding to domain raw scores were developed separately for each of the four norming groups. Due to the lack of variability in standard score distributions for these four samples, raw score frequency distributions were used to develop the percentile rank norms. Percentile ranks for the four autism samples were developed by first determining the cumulative percentages for raw scores. Frequency distributions and the first four moments of the distributions (mean, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis) were obtained. An algorithm developed by Hill (1976; Hill, Hill, & Holder, 1976) and Roid (1989) that is based on systems of frequency curves described by Johnson (1949) was used to calculate the percentile ranks. This algorithm provides fitted curves that preserve the first four moments of the observed raw score distributions and yields percentile estimates that are smoothed across the distributions. This method is comparable to the method that was employed with the Vineland standardization sample for the development of national percentiles (Sparrow et al., 1984, p. 23). RESULTS Prior to presenting the special norm tables, several analyses were conducted to describe similarities Prior to presenting the special norm tables, several analyses were conducted to describe similarities and differences observed in the pattern of adaptive functioning across the four groups from which the special norms were derived: Mute and verbal individuals with autism who were below 10 years of age or 10 years of age or older. This large sample allowed the opportunity to replicate previous findings that suggested the need to develop special population norms. Moreover, as this sample was culled from several different clinics, it was important to determine whether there were age differences in the younger and older samples across mute and verbal subjects. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed an age group by mute status interaction, F(l, 679) = 24.37, p <.0001, such that within the older age group, verbal individuals were significantly older than mute individuals (19.3 vs. 14.9, respectively). Given the lack of comparability across these two groups, age was included as a covariate in subsequent analyses designed to examine group differences in Vineland domain standard scores and age equivalents.10 Group Comparisons of Adaptive Behavior Scores The first set of analyses employed analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) to examine differences in Vineland domain standard scores and age equivalents across the two group factors (mute versus verbal and younger than age 10 vs. 10 years old and older) with continuous age as a covariate. Means and standard deviations for raw scores, standard scores, and age equivalents for Communication, Daily Living Skills, Socialization, and the Adaptive Behavior Composite are presented in Table II. Table III presents age-adjusted standard and age-equivalent scores for the three Vineland domains that were assessed across the two age groups for mute and verbal individuals. Standard Scores. The full model for the Communication domain standard score, which included continuous age as a covariate and main effects and the interaction of age group and mute status was statistically significant, F(4, 678) = 65.84, p < Age, F(l, 678) = 18.93, p <.0001; age group, F(1, 678) = 51.44, p <.0001; and mute status, F(1, 678) = 58.09, p <.0001, were statistically significant but the interaction term was not. Children below 10 years of age As the younger children exhibited much higher standard scores than the older groups, a decision was made to covary age for both sets of analyses. A separate set of analyses that did not include age as a covariate yielded comparable findings, than the older groups, a decision was made to covary age for both sets of analyses. A separate set of analyses that did not include age as a covariate yielded comparable findings.

7 Vineland Supplementary Norms for Autism 293 Vineland domain Table II. Standard and Age-Equivalent Scores for Vineland Domainsa Below 10 years of age (n = 141) M SD Verbal Above 10 years of age (n = 186) M SD Below 10 years of age (n = 252) M SD Mute Above 10 years of age (n = 104) M SD Communication Raw scores Standard scores Age equivalent a a a a a Daily Living Raw scores Standard scores Age equivalent b 3.20, a b b Socialization Raw scores Standard scores Age equivalent C b b 1.70, b Composite Standard scores adifferent letter subscripts indicate significant differences between domains within groups for standard scores. Different numeric subscripts indicate significant differences between domains within groups for age equivalents (p <.05). Table HI. Mean Standard and Age-Equivalent Scores for Vineland Domains Adjusted for Agea Vineland domain Below 10 years of age (n = 141) M Verbal Above 10 years of age (n = 186) M Below 10 years of age (n = 252) M Mute Above 10 years of age (n = 104) M Communication Standard scores Age equivalent 59.74a 3.70a 47.26b 2.54b 43.31b 4.76c 32.82c 2.46b Daily Living Standard scores Age equivalent 58.54a 4.59a 54.16b 4.17b 33.25c 3.83b 29.94c 3.23c Socialization Standard scores Age equivalent 59.14a 3.04a 56.73a 2.93a 35.08b 2.01b 33.57b adifferent letter subscripts indicate significant differences between cells within a domain across groups for standard scores and age equivalents (p <.05). 1.81b had higher Communication standard scores than individuals 10 years of age and older (adjusted means, combining mute and verbal subjects were and 38.06, respectively, p <.0001). Consistent with expectations, verbal individuals were more likely to have higher scores than mute individuals (adjusted means were and 40.04, respectively, p <.0001). The full model for the Daily Living Skills standard

8 294 Carter et al. score was statistically significant, F(4, 678) = 30.09, p <.0001; with age, F(1, 678) = 17.48, p <.0001; age group, F(l, 678) = , p <.0001; and mute status, F(l, 678) = 4.98, p <.05, contributing significantly to the model. The interaction term was not statistically significant. Younger individuals (i.e., younger than 10 years of age) had higher standard scores than older individuals (i.e., those 10 years of age or older) with autism (adjusted means combining mute and verbal individuals were and 31.59, respectively, p <.0001) and verbal individuals achieved higher scores than mute individuals (adjusted means, combining younger and older individuals, were and 42.05, respectively, p <.05). The full model for Socialization standard scores was also statistically significant, F(4, 678) = , p < Only age group, F(l, 678) = , p <.0001, contributed significant variance to the model. There was no effect of continuous age as a covariate, main effect of mute status, or interaction of age group and mute status. Thus, consistent with findings for Daily Living Skills, younger children (i.e., younger than 10 years of age) were more likely to have higher standard scores on Socialization than older individuals (i.e., 10 years of age or older; adjusted means, combining mute and verbal individuals, were and 34.33, respectively, p <.0001). However, in contrast to Communication and Daily Living Skills, mute and verbal individuals did not differ in Socialization. Age Equivalent Scores. The full model for the Communication age-equivalent scores was significant, F(4, 678) = 67.41, p <.0001; with age group F(l, 678) = 30.84, p <.0001; mute status, F(l, 678) = 69.42, p <.0001; and the interaction between age group and mute status, F(l, 678) = 7.51, p <.01, contributing significantly to the model. There was no main effect of continuous age as a covariate. A comparison of adjusted means across the four groups indicated that younger verbal children were least impaired, older mute individuals were most impaired, and younger mute and older verbal individuals were functioning in the midrange (see Table III for adjusted means). The full model for the Daily Living Skills age equivalent was statistically significant, F(4, 678) = , p <.0001; with age as a covariate, F(l, 678) = , p <.0001; age group, F(l, 678) = 12.57, p <.001; and mute status, F(l, 678) = 9.64, p <.01, contributing significant variance. The interaction term was not significant. Younger children appeared to be less impaired than older individuals (adjusted means were 4.37 and 3.53, respectively, p <.001) and verbal individuals were less impaired than mute individuals (adjusted means were 4.21 and 3.70, respectively, p <.01). For the Socialization age equivalent score, the full model was significant, F(4, 678) = 53.74, p <.0001; with age as a covariate, F(l, 678) = , p <.0001; and age group, F(l, 678) = 22.72, p <.0001, contributing significantly. Neither mute status or the interaction term contributed significant variance. Consistent with the Communication and Daily Living Skills domains, younger children appeared less impaired than older individuals (adjusted means, combining mute and verbal individuals were 2.99 and 1.91, respectively, p <.001). Profile Analyses To examine similarities and differences in the profiles of domain scores across the four groups, two repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted to compare standard scores and age equivalents scores across the three Vineland domains assessed at all ages (i.e., Communication, Socialization, and Daily Living Skills). As the critical comparison was within group, age was not covaried for this analysis. Parallel analyses that included age as a covariate provide the same pattern of results. Specifically, a model was tested that included main effects of mute status (mute vs. verbal) and age group (below age 10 years vs. age 10 or older) and Vineland domain standard scores and age equivalents as a repeated factor. The expectation was that there would be a main effect of the repeated Vineland profile factor such that all individuals with autism would show highest scores in Daily Living Skills and poorest performance in Socialization. There was also an expectation that due to basal effects for the older mute group, there would be an interaction between mute status, age group and Vineland profile. Standard Score Profiles. Results for the three domain standard scores revealed a main effect for Vineland profile (Wilks's X =.9877), F(2, 678) = 4.22, p <.05; an interaction between Vineland profile and age group (Wilks's X =.9371), F(2, 678) = 22.74, p <.0001; an interaction between Vineland profile and mute status (Wilks's K =.9187), F(2, 678) = 30.02, p <.0001); and a trend for a three-way interaction between Vineland profile, age group, and mute status (Wilks's X =.9929), F(2, 678) = 2.42, p <.10. Both age grouping and mute status contrib-

9 Vineland Supplementary Norms for Autism 295 uted significant variance to differences between each possible pairing of domains (i.e., Communication vs. Socialization, Communication vs. Daily Living Skills, and Socialization vs. Daily Living Skills; all ps <.001). The interaction term was only statistically significant in predicting to differences between Communication and Daily Living Skills (F = 4.82, p <.05). Age-Equivalent Profiles. For the age-equivalent scores, there was a main effect for Vineland profile (Wilks's A. =.4336), F(2, 678) = , p <.0001; an interaction between Vineland profile and age group (Wilks's X =.7481), F(2, 678) = , p <.0001; an interaction between Vineland profile and mute status (Wilks's X =.8711), F(2, 678) = 50.12, p <.0001; and a significant three-way interaction between Vineland profile, age group, and mute status (Wilks's A =.98427), F(2, 678) = 5.42, p <.01. Both age grouping and mute status contributed significant variance to differences between each possible pairing of domains (i.e., Communication vs. Socialization, Communication vs. Daily Living Skills, and Socialization vs. Daily Living Skills; all ps <.001). In addition, the interaction term was statistically significant in predicting differences between Communication and Socialization (F = 5.31, p <.05) and Daily Living Skills and Socialization (F = 6.65, p <.05) but not for differences in Communication and Daily Living Skills. Although each of the four groups (i.e., younger and older mute and verbal individuals) evidenced a statistically significant profile effect for both standard scores and age-equivalent scores, patterns of differences across standard patterns of differences across standard scores and age equivalents were not comparable. Most groups evidenced the expected pattern of a relative strength in Daily Living Skills and a relative weakness in Socialization when profiles of ageequivalent scores were examined. In contrast, standard scores revealed inconsistent and unexpected patterns across the four groups (see Table II that details significant differences in means across domains within each group). For example, the younger mute group exhibited a relative strength on Daily Living Skills on the age-equivalent scores but evidenced a relative strength in Socialization when standard scores were examined. With respect to the supplementary norms, percentile ranks are reported in Tables IV through X. Table IV. Supplemental Norm Group Percentile Ranks Corresponding Raw Scores: Ages Less Than 10 Years Old Autism Special Population: Mute Supplementary norm group percentile rank Communication Daily Living Skills Raw scores Socialization Motor Skills SP 99 SP 98 SP 95 SP 90 SP 85 SP 80 SP 75 SP 70 SP 65 SP 60 SP 55 SP 50 SP 45 SP 40 SP 35 SP 30 SP 25 SP 20 SP 15 SP 10 SP 5 SP 2 SP

10 296 Carter et al. Table V. Supplemental Norm Group Percentile Ranks Corresponding Raw Scores: Ages Less Than 10 Years Old Autism Special Population: Verbal Supplementary norm group percentile rank Communication Daily Living Skills Raw scores Socialization Motor Skills SP 99 SP 98 SP 95 SP 90 SP 85 SP 80 SP 75 SP 70 SP 65 SP 60 SP 55 SP 50 SP 45 SP 40 SP 35 SP 30 SP 25 SP 20 SP 15 SP 10 SP 5 SP 2 SP Table VI. Supplemental Norm Group Percentile Ranks Corresponding Raw Scores: Ages 10 Years and Older Autism Special Population: Mute Supplementary norm group percentile rank Communication Raw scores Daily Living Skills Socialization SP 99 SP 98 SP 95 SP 90 SP 85 SP 80 SP 75 SP 70 SP 65 SP 60 SP 55 SP 50 SP 45 SP 40 SP 35 SP 30 SP 25 SP 20 SP 15 SP 10 SP 5 SP 2 SP

11 Vineland Supplementary Norms for Autism 297 Table VII. Supplemental Norm Group Percentile Ranks Corresponding Raw Scores: Ages 10 Years and Older Autism Special Population: Verbal Supplementary norm group percentile rank SP 99 SP 98 SP 95 SP 90 SP 85 SP 80 SP 75 SP 70 SP 65 SP 60 SP 55 SP 50 SP 45 SP 40 SP 35 SP 30 SP 25 SP 20 SP 15 SP 10 SP 5 SP 2 SP 1 Communication Raw scores Daily Living Skills Socialization Table VIII. Supplemental Norm Group Percentile Ranks Corresponding to Sum of Raw Scores: Ages Less Than 10 Years Old Autism Special Population: Mute Supplementary norm group percentile rank Sum of 3 domain raw scores (Communication + Daily Living Skills + Socialization) Sum of 4 domain raw scores (Communication + Daily Living Skills + Socialization + Motor) Up ~Up

12 298 Carter et at. Table IX. Supplemental Norm Group Percentile Ranks Corresponding to Sum of Raw Scores: Ages Less Than 10 Years Old Autism Special Population: Not Mute Supplementary norm group percentile rank Sum of 3 domain raw scores (Communication + Daily Living Skills + Socialization) Sum of 4 domain raw scores (Communication + Daily Living Skills + Socialization + Motor) Up ~Up Table X. Supplemental Norm Group Percentile Ranks Corresponding to Sum of Raw Scores Ages Greater Than or Equal to 10 Years Old: Autism Special Population Sample Supplementary norm group percentile rank Sum of 3 domain raw scores (Communication + Daily Living Skills + Socialization) Not mute (n = 171) Mute (n = 98) ~Up Up

13 Vineland Supplementary Norms for Autism 299 Tables IV and V present the supplementary norm group percentile ranks corresponding to raw scores in mute and verbal children with autism less than 10 years old across the four domains assessed by the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Tables VI and VII present the supplementary norm group percentile ranks corresponding to raw scores in mute and verbal individuals with autism who are 10 years of age or older across three domains assessed by the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. The motor domain was not included in the older samples. Finally, Tables VIII, IX, and X present supplementary norm group percentile ranks corresponding to the sums of raw scores across three or four domains for mute and verbal younger and older samples. DISCUSSION This paper presents special population norms for four groups of individuals with autism: (a) mute children under age 10 years; (b) children with at least some verbal skills under age 10 (verbal); (c) mute individuals who are age 10 or older; and (d) verbal individuals who are age 10 or older. The sample was ascertained from cases referred for the DSM-IV autism/pdd field trial collaborative study and five other university-based sites with expertise in the diagnosis of autism. Thus, although the sample is one of "convenience," the cases that were utilized to generate these special population norms most likely represent valid cases of autism that are representative of the general population of individuals with autism. Representativeness has been increased by aggregating individuals across sites that tend to serve slightly different populations of individuals with autism. That the total sample included the expected higher ratio of males to females and of mute females relative to mute males (Volkmar et al., 1993) lends further support to the likelihood that the sample is representative. In addition, it should be noted that the age range of the older samples is from years for mute and years for verbal individuals, with 95% of the verbal individuals younger than age 35 years at the time of assessment. Thus, the use of these norms is most appropriate for individuals with autism below age 35 years. Differences in cognitive functioning across the four groups (e.g., the younger verbal group achieved the highest IQ scores), although statistically significant should not interfere with the clinical use of the special population norms across functional levels in each of the four groupings. The observed pattern of verbal individuals achieving higher cognitive and adaptive scores than mute individuals is not surprising. Previous research would suggest that individuals with autism at all levels of cognitive functioning would exhibit a distinctive adaptive profile (Ando et al., 1980; Freeman et al., 1988; Jacobson & Ackerman, 1990: Loveland & Kelley, 1988, 1991; Rodrigue et al., 1991; Sloan & Marcus, 1981; Volkmar et al., 1987). Of course, when developing special population norms an epidemiological sample of individuals with autism would ultimately be preferable to aggregating across clinical sites. Consistent with previous research, individuals with autism demonstrated a unique profile of adaptive behavior scores across the domains of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Composite. Specifically, when age equivalent scores were employed in profile analyses, there was a relative strength in Daily Living Skills, a relative weakness in Socialization, and intermediate scores on Communication. In contrast, profile analyses employing standard scores were not consistent with expectations based on previous research. The inconsistency in these findings is a reflection, in part, of range restriction due to basal effects in this population. The fact that typically employed standard scores revealed inconsistent patterns highlights the need for the special population norms presented in this paper. When examining adaptive behavior profiles, the use of age-equivalent scores appear more appropriate for lower functioning and/or mute individuals, as basal effects are most likely to obscure actual differences between adaptive domains. Scores for the younger and older mute individuals were significantly lower than for the individuals with some verbal skills, suggesting the need for developing independent norms for these two groups. Younger children's higher standard scores may in part reflect a developmental feature of the disorder, such that the failure to acquire appropriate adaptive skills contributes to a greater relative deficit with age. This could be part of a more global developmental decline in functioning, as younger verbal children achieved the highest IQ scores. However, it is likely that floor effects are responsible for the observed patterns. The basal standard scores for younger children are often higher than for older individuals; the lowest standard score possible at a very young age may be 40, whereas for the older individuals the lowest possible standard score is 20. It is important to note that this problem is not unique to the Vineland

14 300 Carter et al. Adaptive Behavior Scales. Indeed, the Vineland was the first standardized instrument of adaptive behavior to develop a normative range that included standard scores as low as 20. Moreover, the availability of special supplementary norms for individuals with autism addresses this psychometric problem in the Vineland, especially for younger children. Dividing the sample on the basis of mute status (i.e., mute vs. at least some verbal skills) rather than relying on mental age or IQ scores, greatly simplifies the employment of these special population norms and extends the norms to individuals who may be labeled "untestable." Given the importance of including an assessment of adaptive behavior skills for educational and vocational planning for individuals with autism and the difficulty inherent in obtaining an adequate assessment of IQ (cf. Klin et al., 1997), classification that is independent of a determination of IQ is advantageous. The assessment of adaptive behavior in individuals with autism is central to a comprehensive, developmentally sensitive assessment. As noted earlier, including standardized assessments of adaptive behavior: (a) is necessary for documenting mental retardation, a condition that is extremely common among individuals with autism; (b) can help identity strengths and weaknesses when planning educational, vocational, residential, or home-based treatment goals; and (c) can serve to monitor progress over time and across settings. In addition, the inclusion of adaptive behavior can provide important information for diagnosing autism. Specifically, individuals with autism achieve Socialization scores that are much lower than would be expected on the basis of their mental age (Gilham et al., 1998; Volkmar et al., 1993). Finally, the assessment of adaptive behavior can be used as an outcome measure to document the efficacy of intervention programs. The availability of special population norms for individuals with autism provides an additional point of comparison that will often be more appropriate than either national norms for typically developing individuals or special population norms for individuals with mental retardation. In contrast to individuals with mental retardation, who typically achieve a relatively flat profile of scores across adaptive behavior domains, individuals with autism show significant scatter within and across domains. Thus, an evaluation will be able to determine an individual with autism's percentile relative to national norms, which may be essential for documenting mental retardation, but will also be able to provide more realistic expectations for adaptive behavior skill acquisition based on the distribution of performance within a more comparable sample of individuals with autism. The special population norms provided in this paper can assist clinicians in assigning appropriate goals for intervention. Specifically, expectations for adaptive functioning that are based on the special populations norms will factor not only cognitive or linguistic functioning, but also the social delays and deficits that are a core feature of autism. A highfunctioning 12-year 6-month-old child with autism achieves a Verbal IQ of 95, a Performance IQ of 112, a Vineland Communication raw score of 112, which corresponds to a Standard Score of 68 (2nd percentile), a Daily Living Skills raw score of 138, which corresponds to a Standard Score of 76 (5th percentile), and a Socialization raw score of 52, which corresponds to a Standard Score of 40 (below 0.1 percentile). As this child is not mentally retarded, no previously available special population norms would be appropriate. The raw scores that correspond to these standard scores were obtained in Appendix C Table B.1 of the Vineland manual (Sparrow et al., 1984, p. 213). Using the special population autism norms presented in Table VII for individuals 10 years and older who are not mute, the child's raw scores correspond to the following percentile scores: SP 75 in Communication, SP 80 in Daily Living Skills, and SP 45 in Socialization. Thus, Socialization appears to be a relative weakness and a potential focus for intervention even when employing the special population norms. A second 12-year 6-month-old boy with minimal verbal expression is declared untestable, but achieves the following Vineland standard scores based on parent report: a Vineland Communication raw score of 73, which corresponds to a Standard Score of 35 (below 0.1 percentile), Daily Living Skills raw score of 101, which corresponds to a Standard Score of 37 (0.1 percentile), and a Socialization raw score of 47, which corresponds to a Standard Score of 36 (below 0.1 percentile). The new special population norms yield the following profile: SP 75 in Communication, SP 75 in Daily Living Skills, and SP 65 in Socialization. In this case, the profile of scores does not shift, but expectations for performance can be modified appropriately. In contrast to intellectual functioning, adaptive behavior is modifiable. For all individuals, however, cognitive functioning will set some constraints on the level of adaptive functioning that can be achieved.

15 Vineland Supplementary Norms for Autism 301 Specifically, it is not reasonable to expect an individual to achieve a level of adaptive functioning that is significantly more advanced than their intellectual level or mental age. For individuals with autism, social deviance sets further constraints, especially within the Socialization domain. As the acquisition of specific adaptive behavior skills is often a key component of intervention for individuals with autism, it is critical that individuals designing such intervention programs set attainable goals across domains of adaptive functioning. When such goals are met, clients, staff, and family members experience increased self-efficacy. The availability of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales Special population norms presented in this paper should assist in the evaluation of individuals with autism, by providing a more proximal point of reference (i.e., other individuals with autism) that will facilitate setting realistic recommendations for intervention. REFERENCES Adams, G. (1984). Comprehensive test of adaptive behavior. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington DC: Author. Ando, H., Yoshimura, I., & Wakabayashi, S. (1980). Effects of age on adaptive behavior levels in autistic an mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10, Bailey, A., Phillips, W., & Rutter, M. (1996). Autism: Towards an integration of clinical, genetic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, Burack, J. A., & Volkmar, F. R. (1992). Development of low- and high-functioning autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, Carter, A. S., Gillham, J. E., Sparrow, S. S., & Volkmar, F. R. (1996). Adaptive behavior in autism. In F. Volkmar (Ed.), Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinic of North America, 5, Cohen, D. J., Paul, R., & Volkmar, F. R. (1986). Issues in the classification of pervasive and other developmental disorders: Toward DSM-1V Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 25, Fein, D., Pennington, B., Markowitz, P., Bravcrman, M., & Waterhouse (1986). Toward a neuropsychological model of infantile autism: Are the social deficits primary? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 25, Firth, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Freeman, B. J., Ritvo, E. R., Yokota, A., Childs, J., & Pollard, J. (1988). WISC-R and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale Scores in Autistic Children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, Freeman, B. J., Rahbar, B., Ritvo, E. R., Bice, T. L., Yokota, A., & Ritvo, R. (1991). The stability of cognitive and behavioral parameters in Autism: A twelve-year prospective study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescence Psychiatry, 30, Gilham, J. G., Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (1998). Toward a developmental operational definition of autism: Replication and extension. Manuscript submitted for publication. Grossman, H. J. (Ed.). (1983). Classification in mental retardation. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Deficiency. Happe, F. (1994). Autism: An introduction to psychological theory. London: UCL Press. Hill, I. D. (1976). Normal-Johnson and Johnson-normal transformations. Applied Statistics, 45, Hill, I. D., Hill, R., & Holder, R. L. (1976). Algorithm AS 99: Fitting Johnson curves by moments. Applied Statistics, 45, Jacobson, J. W., & Ackerman, L. J. (1990). Differences in adaptive functioning among people with autism or mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, Johnson, N. L. (1949). Systems of frequency curves generated by methods of translation. Biometrika, 36, Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, Klin, A., Carter, A. S., & Sparrow, S. S. (1997). Psychological assessment of children with autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders (2nd ed., pp ). New York: Wiley. Krug, D. A., Arick, J., & Almond, P. (1980). Behavior checklist for identifying severely handicapped individuals with high levels of autistic behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 21, Lambert, N., Windmiller, M., Tharinger, D., & Cole, L (1981). AAMD Adaptive Behavior Scale School edition. Monterey. CA: Publishers' Test Service. Lord, C. (1993). Complexity of social behavior in autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp ). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Lord, C, Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1987). Neurobiological implications of sex differences in autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Neurobiological issues in autism (pp ). New York: Plenum Press. Loveland, K. A., & Kelley, M. L. (1988). Development of adaptive behavior in adolescents and young adults with autism and Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 93, Loveland, K. A., & Kelley, M. L, (1991). Development of adaptive behavior in preschoolers with Autism or Down Syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 96, Perry, A., & Factor, D. C. (1989). Psychometric validity and clinical usefulness of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales and the AAMD Adaptive Behavior Scale for an Autistic Sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, Ritvo, E., Freeman, B. J., Pingree, C., Mason-Brothers, A., Jorde, L., Jenson, W.R., McMahon, W. M., Peterson, P. B., Mo, A., & Ritvo, A. (1989). The UCLA-University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: Prevalence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, Rodrigue, J. R., Morgan, S. B., & Geffken, G. R. (1991). A comparative evaluation of adaptive behavior in children and adolescents with autism, Down syndrome, and normal development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21,

16 302 Carter et al. Roid, G. H. (1989). Programs to fa skewed distributions and generate percenlile norms for skewed or kurtotic distributions: Continuous norming with the first four moments. (Technical Report No ). Salem, OR: Assessment Research. Rumsey, J. M., Rapoport, J. L., & Sceery, W. R. (1985). Autistic children as adults: Psychiatric, social, and behavioral. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 1-25). New York: Plenum Press. Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1988). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Sigman, M. (1995). Behavioral research in childhood autism. In M. Lenzenweger & J. Haugaard (Eds.), Frontiers of developmental psychopathology (pp ). New York: Springer/Verlag. Sloan, J. L., & Marcus, L. (1981). Some findings on the use of the Adaptive Behavior Scale with autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. VanMeter, L., Fein, D., Morris, R., Waterhouse, L., & Allen, D. (1997). Delay versus deviance in autistic social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, Volkmar, F. R., Carter, A. S., Sparrow, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Assessment of autism: Developmental approach. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1986). Current concepts: Infantile autism and the pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Developments! and Behavioral Pediatrics, 7, Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1988). Issues in the diagnosis and classification of infantile autism. In B. Lahey & A. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology. New York: Plenum Press. Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Siegel, B., Szatmari, P., Lord, C, Campbell, M., Freeman, B. J., Cicchetti, D. V, Rutter, M., Kline, W., Buitelaar, J., Hattab, Y., Fombonne, E., Fuentes, J., Werry, J., Stone, W. Kerbeshian, J., Hoshino, Y., Bregman, J., Loveland, K., Syzmanski, L., & Towbin, K. (1994). Field trial for autistic disorder in DSM-IV American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Goudreau, D., Cicchetti, D. V, Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1987). Social deficits in autism: An operational approach using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, Volkmar, F. R., Szatmari, E, & Sparrow, S. S. (1993). Sex differences in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, Volkmar, F. R., & Rutter, M. (1995). Childhood Disintegrative Disorder: Results of the DSM-IV Autism Field Trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, World Health Organization. (1990). International Classification of Diseases (draft version: Diagnostic criteria for research) (10th ed.). Geneva, Switzerland: Author.

1/30/2018. Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Disclosures. Learning Objectives

1/30/2018. Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Disclosures. Learning Objectives Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Associate Professor Emory University School of Medicine Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition 1 Disclosures As

More information

Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Associate Professor Emory University School of Medicine Director of Research Operations Marcus Autism Center Vineland Adaptive

More information

Brief Report: Interrater Reliability of Clinical Diagnosis and DSM-IV Criteria for Autistic Disorder: Results of the DSM-IV Autism Field Trial

Brief Report: Interrater Reliability of Clinical Diagnosis and DSM-IV Criteria for Autistic Disorder: Results of the DSM-IV Autism Field Trial Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 30, No. 2, 2000 Brief Report: Interrater Reliability of Clinical Diagnosis and DSM-IV Criteria for Autistic Disorder: Results of the DSM-IV Autism Field

More information

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS MEDICAL POLICY PAGE: 1 OF: 7 If the member's subscriber contract excludes coverage for a specific service it is not covered under that contract. In such cases, medical policy criteria are not applied.

More information

COMPARATIVE STUDY OF EARLY CHILDHOOD HIGH- RECEPTIVE-EXPRESSIVE LANGUAGE DISORDER FUNCTION AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL MIXED

COMPARATIVE STUDY OF EARLY CHILDHOOD HIGH- RECEPTIVE-EXPRESSIVE LANGUAGE DISORDER FUNCTION AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL MIXED COMPARATIVE STUDY OF EARLY CHILDHOOD HIGH- FUNCTION AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL MIXED RECEPTIVE-EXPRESSIVE LANGUAGE DISORDER Pinchen Yang, Yuh-Jyh Jong, 1 Hsiu-Yi Hsu, 1 and Cheng-Sheng Chen Departments of

More information

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Overview Diagnostic Criteria Current: Diagnostic & Statistical

More information

Myths! Myths and Realities of Evaluation, Identification, and Diagnosis of ASD 10/11/10. Facts. The Ziggurat Group

Myths! Myths and Realities of Evaluation, Identification, and Diagnosis of ASD 10/11/10. Facts. The Ziggurat Group Myths and Realities of Evaluation, Identification, and Diagnosis of ASD Ruth Aspy, Ph.D., Barry Grossman, Ph.D. The Ziggurat Group Myths and the Culture of Misunderstanding Belief in Myths Causes Harm

More information

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS MEDICAL POLICY SUBJECT: APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR PAGE: 1 OF: 8 If a product excludes coverage for a service, it is not covered, and medical policy criteria do not apply. If a commercial product, including

More information

The use of Autism Mental Status Exam in an Italian sample. A brief report

The use of Autism Mental Status Exam in an Italian sample. A brief report Life Span and Disability XX, 1 (2017), 93-103 The use of Autism Mental Status Exam in an Italian sample. A brief report Marinella Zingale 1, Simonetta Panerai 2, Serafino Buono 3, Grazia Trubia 4, Maurizio

More information

10/18/2016. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition 1. Meet Dr. Saulnier. Bio. Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Vineland-3 Author

10/18/2016. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition 1. Meet Dr. Saulnier. Bio. Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Vineland-3 Author Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Vineland-3 Author Director of Research Operations at the Marcus Autism Center & Associate Professor in the Department of Pediatrics

More information

BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS Page: 1 of 7 MEDICAL POLICY MEDICAL POLICY DETAILS Medical Policy Title APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS FOR THE TREATMENT OF AUTISM SPECTRUM Policy Number 3.01.11 Category Behavioral Health/ Government Mandate

More information

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) DSM-5 (Criteria and Major Changes for SLP-Related Conditions) Individuals meeting the criteria will be given a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder with three levels of severity based on degree of support

More information

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Assessment and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder For this month s topic, I am excited to share my recent experience using the fifth edition of the Diagnostic

More information

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D.

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. Autism Spectrum Disorder Neurodevelopmental disorder Reflects understanding of the etiology of disorder as related to alterations

More information

DISABILITY IN PERVASIVE DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS: A COMPARATIVE STUDY WITH MENTAL RETARDATION IN INDIA

DISABILITY IN PERVASIVE DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS: A COMPARATIVE STUDY WITH MENTAL RETARDATION IN INDIA ISSN: 0973-5755 DISABILITY IN PERVASIVE DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS: A COMPARATIVE STUDY WITH MENTAL RETARDATION IN INDIA Krishan Kumar*, V. K. Sinha**, B.L. Kotia*** and Sushil Kumar**** ABSTRACT Pervasive

More information

University of Connecticut. University of Connecticut Graduate School

University of Connecticut. University of Connecticut Graduate School University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Master's Theses University of Connecticut Graduate School 7-11-2012 Repetitive and Stereotyped Behaviors from Age 2 to Age 4: A Look at the Development of

More information

Joanna Bailes M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Joanna Bailes M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: Can imitation, joint attention and the level of play in preschool years predict later language outcomes for children with autism spectrum disorder? Joanna Bailes M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate

More information

Research Article Cognitive Skills of Young Children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder Using the BSID-III

Research Article Cognitive Skills of Young Children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder Using the BSID-III Autism Research and Treatment Volume 2011, Article ID 759289, 7 pages doi:10.1155/2011/759289 Research Article Cognitive Skills of Young Children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder Using the BSID-III

More information

Benefits of the Treatment and Education of Autistic and Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH) programme as compared with a non-specific approach

Benefits of the Treatment and Education of Autistic and Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH) programme as compared with a non-specific approach 318 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 46 PART 4 pp 318 327 MAY 2002 Benefits of the Treatment and Education of Autistic and Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH) programme as compared

More information

Diagnosis Advancements. Licensee OAPL (UK) Creative Commons Attribution License (CC-BY) Research study

Diagnosis Advancements. Licensee OAPL (UK) Creative Commons Attribution License (CC-BY) Research study Page 1 of 6 Diagnosis Advancements Relationship between Stereotyped Behaviors and Restricted Interests (SBRIs) measured on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) and diagnostic results. C Schutte

More information

Differential Diagnosis. Not a Cookbook. Diagnostic Myths. Starting Points. Starting Points

Differential Diagnosis. Not a Cookbook. Diagnostic Myths. Starting Points. Starting Points Educational Identification of Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders Ohio Center for Autism and Low Incidence (OCALI) 5220 N. High Street Columbus, OH 43214 Main Line: 866-886-2254 Fax: 614-410-1090

More information

University of Groningen. The Friesland study Bildt, Alida Anna de

University of Groningen. The Friesland study Bildt, Alida Anna de University of Groningen The Friesland study Bildt, Alida Anna de IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: Can joint attention, imitation, and/or play skills predict future language abilities of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)? Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology)

More information

University of Groningen. The Friesland study de Bildt, Annelies

University of Groningen. The Friesland study de Bildt, Annelies University of Groningen The Friesland study de Bildt, Annelies IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

11/22/10. The best tool is a trained and experienced examiner. Which is the Best Tool for Evaluating ASD?

11/22/10. The best tool is a trained and experienced examiner. Which is the Best Tool for Evaluating ASD? Toothpicks and Rubber Bands: Transdisciplinary Assessment of Autism Spectrum Disorders Which is the Best Tool for Evaluating ASD? Ruth Aspy, Ph.D., Barry Grossman, Ph.D., Penny Woods, M.S., Ed., Nicole

More information

University of Groningen. The Friesland study de Bildt, Annelies

University of Groningen. The Friesland study de Bildt, Annelies University of Groningen The Friesland study de Bildt, Annelies IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

Daily living skills in individuals with autism spectrum disorder from 2 to 21 years of age

Daily living skills in individuals with autism spectrum disorder from 2 to 21 years of age 575840AUT0010.1177/1362361315575840AutismBal et al. research-article2015 Special Issue Article Daily living skills in individuals with autism spectrum disorder from 2 to 21 years of age Autism 2015, Vol.

More information

Applied Behavior Analysis for Autism Spectrum Disorders

Applied Behavior Analysis for Autism Spectrum Disorders Applied Behavior Analysis for Autism Spectrum Disorders I. Policy University Health Alliance (UHA) will reimburse for Applied Behavioral Analysis (ABA), as required in relevant State of Hawaii mandates,

More information

Oklahoma Psychological Association DSM-5 Panel November 8-9, 2013 Jennifer L. Morris, Ph.D.

Oklahoma Psychological Association DSM-5 Panel November 8-9, 2013 Jennifer L. Morris, Ph.D. Oklahoma Psychological Association DSM-5 Panel November 8-9, 2013 Jennifer L. Morris, Ph.D. DSM-5 continues developmental progression, starting with disorders that are observed in early life. Disorders

More information

Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders

Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders Synonyms DISCO Abbreviations ADI-R PDD Autism diagnostic interview-revised Pervasive developmental disorders Description The (DISCO) is a semistructured

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

Cover Page. The handle   holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/19149 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Author: Maljaars, Janne Pieternella Wilhelmina Title: Communication problems in children

More information

Factors Influencing How Parents Report. Autism Symptoms on the ADI-R

Factors Influencing How Parents Report. Autism Symptoms on the ADI-R Factors Influencing How Parents Report Autism Symptoms on the ADI-R Diana Wexler Briarcliff High School Diana Wexler Briarcliff High School 1 Abstract Background: The Autism Diagnostic Interview - Revised

More information

Depression in Children with Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders: A Case-Control Family History Study

Depression in Children with Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders: A Case-Control Family History Study Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol 28, No. 2, 1998 Depression in Children with Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders: A Case-Control Family History Study Mohammad Ghaziuddin1,2 and John

More information

UC Merced UC Merced Undergraduate Research Journal

UC Merced UC Merced Undergraduate Research Journal UC Merced UC Merced Undergraduate Research Journal Title Establishing a Standardized Measurement Tool for children with ASD for use in PECS research Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/76t0h49d

More information

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER What is the DSM-5? The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (the DSM) is developed by the

More information

Developmental Assessment of Young Children Second Edition (DAYC-2) Summary Report

Developmental Assessment of Young Children Second Edition (DAYC-2) Summary Report Developmental Assessment of Young Children Second Edition (DAYC-2) Summary Report Section 1. Identifying Information Name: Marcos Sanders Gender: M Date of Testing: 05-10-2011 Date of Birth: 09-15-2009

More information

Supplementary Information. Enhancing studies of the connectome in autism using the Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange II

Supplementary Information. Enhancing studies of the connectome in autism using the Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange II Supplementary Information Enhancing studies of the connectome in autism using the Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange II 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Selection of spatial and temporal quality metrics for the

More information

Analysis of new diagnostic criteria for autism sparks debate

Analysis of new diagnostic criteria for autism sparks debate NEWS Analysis of new diagnostic criteria for autism sparks debate BY EMILY SINGER 2 APRIL 2012 1 / 6 Draft definition: A new study questions the sensitivity of the new DSM-5's diagnostic criteria. A new

More information

Magellan Healthcare ABA Treatment Plan/Concurrent Review

Magellan Healthcare ABA Treatment Plan/Concurrent Review Magellan Healthcare ABA Treatment Plan/Concurrent Review Member s Name: Member s DOB: Insurance ID#: Guardian Name and phone number: Agency Name and address: Agency Tax ID # and NPI #: Agency contact name

More information

USE OF THE MULLEN SCALES OF EARLY LEARNING FOR THE ASSESSMENT OF YOUNG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

USE OF THE MULLEN SCALES OF EARLY LEARNING FOR THE ASSESSMENT OF YOUNG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS Child Neuropsychology, 12: 269 277, 2006 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0929-7049 print / 1744-4136 online DOI: 10.1080/09297040500473714 USE OF THE MULLEN SCALES OF EARLY LEARNING FOR THE

More information

Table 1: Comparison of DSM-5 and DSM-IV-TR Diagnostic Criteria. Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Pervasive Developmental Disorders Key Differences

Table 1: Comparison of DSM-5 and DSM-IV-TR Diagnostic Criteria. Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Pervasive Developmental Disorders Key Differences Comparison of the Diagnostic Criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder Across DSM-5, 1 DSM-IV-TR, 2 and the Individuals with Disabilities Act (IDEA) 3 Definition of Autism Colleen M. Harker, M.S. & Wendy L.

More information

Starting Points. Starting Points. Autism Screening and Resources for the Practitioner. The Importance of Screening

Starting Points. Starting Points. Autism Screening and Resources for the Practitioner. The Importance of Screening Autism Screening and Resources for the Practitioner Ruth Aspy, Ph.D., and Barry G. Grossman, Ph.D. The Ziggurat Group, PA www.texasautism.com Keynote Presented for the Greater Texas Chapter National Association

More information

Evidence presented during the hearing fails to establish an eligible diagnosis for the MR/DD Waiver Program.

Evidence presented during the hearing fails to establish an eligible diagnosis for the MR/DD Waiver Program. Joe Manchin Governor State of West Virginia DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN RESOURCES Office of Inspector General Board of Review 1027 N. Randolph Ave. Elkins, WV 26241 July 11, 2008 Martha Yeager Walker

More information

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers!

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers! Marisa Filipe Dezembro de 2013 pdpsi10020@fpce.up.pt EXCL/MHC-LIN/0688/2012 Summary Introduction: Prosodic Abilities in Children and Young Adults with Typical & Non-Typical Development Prosodic abilities

More information

The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders: algorithms for ICD-10 childhood autism and Wing and Gould autistic spectrum disorder

The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders: algorithms for ICD-10 childhood autism and Wing and Gould autistic spectrum disorder Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 43:3 (2002), pp 327±342 The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders: algorithms for ICD-10 childhood autism and Wing and Gould autistic spectrum

More information

Autism and Related Disorders:

Autism and Related Disorders: Autism and Related Disorders: CHLD 350a/PSYC350 Lecture II: Assessment Katherine D. Tsatsanis, Ph.D. Yale Child Study Center Clinical Director, Developmental Disabilities Clinic Pervasive Developmental

More information

Dr. Pushpal Desarkar & Dr. Anna M. Palucka. Presentation objectives / overview. Why do we use the DSM?

Dr. Pushpal Desarkar & Dr. Anna M. Palucka. Presentation objectives / overview. Why do we use the DSM? Changes to the DSM-5: Implications for Diagnostic Identification and Accessing Services relating to Autism Spectrum Disorder and Intellectual Disability Dr. Pushpal Desarkar & Dr. Anna M. Palucka Presentation

More information

Research Article The Michigan Autism Spectrum Questionnaire: A Rating Scale for High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders

Research Article The Michigan Autism Spectrum Questionnaire: A Rating Scale for High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders Hindawi Publishing Corporation Autism Research and Treatment Volume 2013, Article ID 708273, 5 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2013/708273 Research Article The Michigan Autism Spectrum Questionnaire: A

More information

Brief Report: An Independent Replication and Extension of Psychometric Evidence Supporting the Theory of Mind Inventory

Brief Report: An Independent Replication and Extension of Psychometric Evidence Supporting the Theory of Mind Inventory DOI 10.1007/s10803-016-2784-7 BRIEF REPORT Brief Report: An Independent Replication and Extension of Psychometric Evidence Supporting the Theory of Mind Inventory Kathryn J. Greenslade 1,2 Truman E. Coggins

More information

STATE OF WEST VIRGINIA

STATE OF WEST VIRGINIA STATE OF WEST VIRGINIA DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN RESOURCES OFFICE OF INSPECTOR GENERAL Earl Ray Tomblin BOARD OF REVIEW Karen L. Bowling Governor 1400 Virginia Street Cabinet Secretary Oak Hill, WV

More information

Social Skills and Problem Behaviours in School Aged Children with High-Functioning Autism and Asperger s Disorder

Social Skills and Problem Behaviours in School Aged Children with High-Functioning Autism and Asperger s Disorder DOI 10.1007/s10803-006-0139-5 ORIGINAL PAPER Social Skills and Problem Behaviours in School Aged Children with High-Functioning Autism and Asperger s Disorder Kathleen Macintosh Æ Cheryl Dissanayake Ó

More information

Subtypes of Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Implications for Early and Future Diagnosis

Subtypes of Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Implications for Early and Future Diagnosis Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Psychology Dissertations Department of Psychology 4-16-2009 Subtypes of Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Implications for Early

More information

Comparing social skills in children with Autistic disorder and Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified

Comparing social skills in children with Autistic disorder and Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2009 Comparing social skills in children with Autistic disorder and Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified

More information

A Longitudinal Pilot Study of Behavioral Abnormalities in Children with Autism

A Longitudinal Pilot Study of Behavioral Abnormalities in Children with Autism Volume 1, Issue 4 Research Article A Longitudinal Pilot Study of Behavioral Abnormalities in Children with Autism Robin A. Libove 1, Thomas W. Frazier 2, Ruth O Hara 1, Jennifer M. Phillips 1, Booil Jo

More information

Cognitive styles sex the brain, compete neurally, and quantify deficits in autism

Cognitive styles sex the brain, compete neurally, and quantify deficits in autism Cognitive styles sex the brain, compete neurally, and quantify deficits in autism Nigel Goldenfeld 1, Sally Wheelwright 2 and Simon Baron-Cohen 2 1 Department of Applied Mathematics and Theoretical Physics,

More information

Index. Hyperlexia, 66-67

Index. Hyperlexia, 66-67 AAPEP. See Adolescent and Adult Psycho educational Profile (AAPEP) Activities. See also Work/activity systems visually-structured,45-47 Adolescent and Adult Psychoeducational Profile (AAPEP), 4, 160 development

More information

School of Public Health

School of Public Health School of Public Health Drexel E-Repository and Archive (idea) http://idea.library.drexel.edu/ Drexel University Libraries www.library.drexel.edu The following item is made available as a courtesy to scholars

More information

Applied Behavior Analysis Therapy for Treatment of Autism Spectrum Disorder

Applied Behavior Analysis Therapy for Treatment of Autism Spectrum Disorder Applied Behavior Analysis Therapy for Treatment of Autism Spectrum Disorder Policy Number: Original Effective Date: MM.12.022 01/01/2016 Line(s) of Business: Current Effective Date: HMO; PPO; Fed 87; FEP;

More information

Fact Sheet 8. DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder

Fact Sheet 8. DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder Fact Sheet 8 DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder A diagnosis of autism is made on the basis of observed behaviour. There are no blood tests, no single defining symptom and no physical characteristics that

More information

Practitioners working with children and youths with autism spectrum disorders

Practitioners working with children and youths with autism spectrum disorders Comparing the Functional Performance of Children and Youths With Autism, Developmental Disabilities, and No Disability Using the Revised Pediatric Evaluation of Disability Inventory Item Banks Ying-Chia

More information

Language Comprehension Predicts Later Cognitive Ability and Symptom Severity in Toddlers with ASD

Language Comprehension Predicts Later Cognitive Ability and Symptom Severity in Toddlers with ASD Language Comprehension Predicts Later Cognitive Ability and Symptom Severity in Toddlers with ASD McGarry, E., Fiorello, K., Heldenberg, S., Reifler, A., Klaiman, C., Saulnier, C., & Lewis, M.. Marcus

More information

In 1943, Leo Kanner first described autism. This perplexing and highly

In 1943, Leo Kanner first described autism. This perplexing and highly Intro-Simpson.qxd 4/28/04 11:20 AM Page 1 Introduction In 1943, Leo Kanner first described autism. This perplexing and highly enigmatic disorder is characterized by difficulty in interacting normally with

More information

Mullen Scales of Early Learning: AGS Edition

Mullen Scales of Early Learning: AGS Edition Overview The Mullen Scales of Early Learning: AGS Edition (Mullen, 1995) is an individually administered, standardized measure of cognitive functioning designed to be used with children from birth through

More information

ABAS-II Ratings and Correlates of Adaptive Behavior in Children with HFASDs

ABAS-II Ratings and Correlates of Adaptive Behavior in Children with HFASDs J Dev Phys Disabil (2012) 24:391 402 DOI 10.1007/s10882-012-9277-1 ORIGINAL ARTICLE ABAS-II Ratings and Correlates of Adaptive Behavior in Children with HFASDs Christopher Lopata & Jeffery D. Fox & Marcus

More information

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of Education VALIDITY AND DIAGNOSTIC ACCURACY OF SCORES FROM THE AUTISM

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of Education VALIDITY AND DIAGNOSTIC ACCURACY OF SCORES FROM THE AUTISM The Pennsylvania State University The Graduate School College of Education VALIDITY AND DIAGNOSTIC ACCURACY OF SCORES FROM THE AUTISM DIAGNOSTIC OBSERVATION SCHEDULE-GENERIC A Dissertation in School Psychology

More information

5. Diagnostic Criteria

5. Diagnostic Criteria 5. Diagnostic Criteria The questions that are going to be answered in this chapter are: What are the diagnostic criteria of ASD? Are the diagnostic criteria laid down in the DSM-IV-TR or ICD-10 manuals

More information

Early Childhood Predictors of the Social Competence of Adults with Autism

Early Childhood Predictors of the Social Competence of Adults with Autism J Autism Dev Disord (2012) 42:161 174 DOI 10.1007/s10803-011-1222-0 ORIGINAL PAPER Early Childhood Predictors of the Social Competence of Adults with Autism Kristen Gillespie-Lynch Leigh Sepeta Yueyan

More information

Applied Behavior Analysis Therapy for Treatment of Autism Spectrum Disorder

Applied Behavior Analysis Therapy for Treatment of Autism Spectrum Disorder Applied Behavior Analysis Therapy for Treatment of Autism Spectrum Disorder Policy Number: Original Effective Date: MM.12.022 01/01/2016 Line(s) of Business: Current Effective Date: HMO; PPO; Fed 87; FEP;

More information

Early Start Denver Model

Early Start Denver Model RESEARCH UPDATE Early Start Denver Model What is the Early Start Denver Model (ESDM)? The ESDM is an early intervention model designed to target the key features of autism. A child s early interpersonal

More information

Age of diagnosis for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Reasons for a later diagnosis: Earlier identification = Earlier intervention

Age of diagnosis for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Reasons for a later diagnosis: Earlier identification = Earlier intervention Identifying Autism Spectrum Disorders: Do You See What I See? Age of diagnosis for Autism Spectrum Disorders 1970 s it was around 5-6 years of age 1980 s it was around 4-5 years of age presently the mean

More information

Towards a convergent account of pragmatic language deficits in children with

Towards a convergent account of pragmatic language deficits in children with 1 Towards a convergent account of pragmatic language deficits in children with high-functioning autism: Depicting the phenotype using the Pragmatic Rating Scale 1 Abstract This is the pre-published version.

More information

Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism

Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism By Lauren Lowry Hanen Certified SLP and Clinical Staff Writer Several years ago I worked on a diagnostic assessment team. I remember the first

More information

Creation and Use of the Pervasive Developmental Disorder Behavior Inventory (PDDBI) Parent Form

Creation and Use of the Pervasive Developmental Disorder Behavior Inventory (PDDBI) Parent Form Spanish Translation Creation and Use of the Pervasive Developmental Disorder Behavior Inventory (PDDBI) Parent Form Spanish Translation Amy Kovacs Giella, BA Executive Summary Approximately 13% of the

More information

DSM-5 Autism Criteria Applied to Toddlers with DSM-IV-TR Autism

DSM-5 Autism Criteria Applied to Toddlers with DSM-IV-TR Autism University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Master's Theses University of Connecticut Graduate School 9-23-2014 DSM-5 Autism Criteria Applied to Toddlers with DSM-IV-TR Autism Dasal T. Jashar University

More information

University of Groningen. The Friesland study Bildt, Alida Anna de

University of Groningen. The Friesland study Bildt, Alida Anna de University of Groningen The Friesland study Bildt, Alida Anna de IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome 5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome Published in: Neuropsychologia, 2008, 47 (3), 652-656. Chapter 5 Abstract The semantic and phonemic fluency performance of adults with high functioning

More information

Education Options for Children with Autism

Education Options for Children with Autism Empowering children with Autism and their families through knowledge and support Education Options for Children with Autism Starting school is a major milestone in a child s life, and a big step for all

More information

Eligibility Criteria for Children with ASD

Eligibility Criteria for Children with ASD AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER SERIES Eligibility Criteria for Children with ASD Review the Characteristics of Children with ASD* The following are the most common signs and symptoms of a child with ASD: The

More information

Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update. Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011

Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update. Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011 Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011 Overview Diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorders Screening/referral

More information

Tony Charman: Longitudinal studies for autism research

Tony Charman: Longitudinal studies for autism research VIEWPOINT Tony Charman: Longitudinal studies for autism research BY TONY CHARMAN 24 JANUARY 2012 1 / 8 2 / 8 3 / 8 4 / 8 Facing forward: When Tony Charman first began studying autism in the 1980s, long-term

More information

Long-term outcome of social skills intervention based on interactive LEGO play

Long-term outcome of social skills intervention based on interactive LEGO play 02 LEGOFF 064403 (bc-t) 30/3/06 3:38 pm Page 1 Long-term outcome of social skills intervention based on interactive LEGO play autism 2006 SAGE Publications and The National Autistic Society Vol 10(4) 1

More information

Cognitive Functioning in Children with Motor Impairments

Cognitive Functioning in Children with Motor Impairments Cognitive Functioning in Children with Motor Impairments Jan P. Piek School of Psychology & Speech Pathology Curtin Health Innovation Research Institute (CHIRI) Curtin University Perth Western Australia

More information

High-Functioning Autism and Asperger s Disorder: A Neuropsychological Comparison. A Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty. of Drexel University

High-Functioning Autism and Asperger s Disorder: A Neuropsychological Comparison. A Thesis. Submitted to the Faculty. of Drexel University High-Functioning Autism and Asperger s Disorder: A Neuropsychological Comparison A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Drexel University by Kaira M. Hayes in partial fulfillment of the requirements for

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorder

Autism Spectrum Disorder Myths and Realities of Evaluation, Identification, and Diagnosis of ASD Autism Spectrum Disorder Ruth Aspy, Ph.D., Barry Grossman, Ph.D., Penny Woods, M.S. Ed., & Susan Jamieson, M.Ed. The Ziggurat Group

More information

What Works Clearinghouse

What Works Clearinghouse What Works Clearinghouse U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION July 2012 WWC Review of the Report Randomized, Controlled Trial of the LEAP Model of Early Intervention for Young Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders

More information

From: What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015

From:   What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015 Overcoming Intellectual Disability and Autism to Achieve Vocational & Academic Success Pathway to Empowerment Objectives 1 2 4 Learn to distinguish between intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorders.

More information

Communication What does the research say?

Communication What does the research say? Communication Communication is often perceived as referring merely to the act of verbalising. However, verbalisations (and actions) can and do occur devoid of communicative intent. Communication is comprised

More information

Editorial: DSM-5 and autism spectrum disorders two decades of perspectives from the JCPP

Editorial: DSM-5 and autism spectrum disorders two decades of perspectives from the JCPP 1 Editorial: DSM-5 and autism spectrum disorders two decades of perspectives from the JCPP The newest revision of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), the DSM-5, will be published

More information

Autism and Other Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) or Pervasive Developmental Disorders (PDD)

Autism and Other Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) or Pervasive Developmental Disorders (PDD) June 22, 2011 Autism and Other Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) or Pervasive Developmental Disorders (PDD) Institute for Development of Mind and Behavior Masataka Ohta MD (Email: mohta-dmb@nifty.com) OUTLINE

More information

Autistic Behavior, FMR1 Protein, and Developmental Trajectories in Young Males with Fragile X Syndrome

Autistic Behavior, FMR1 Protein, and Developmental Trajectories in Young Males with Fragile X Syndrome Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 31, No. 2, 2001 Autistic Behavior, FMR1 Protein, and Developmental Trajectories in Young Males with Fragile X Syndrome Donald B. Bailey, Jr., 1 Deborah

More information

Essential behaviours for the diagnosis of DSM-5 Autism Spectrum Disorder

Essential behaviours for the diagnosis of DSM-5 Autism Spectrum Disorder Essential behaviours for the diagnosis of DSM-5 Autism Spectrum Disorder Dr Sarah Carrington Wales Autism Research Centre, Cardiff University Australasian Society for Autism Research 4 th December, 2014

More information

Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Psychoeducational Profile Third Edition Caregiver Report

Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Psychoeducational Profile Third Edition Caregiver Report Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability ISSN: 1366-8250 (Print) 1469-9532 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cjid20 Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of

More information

CLINICAL BOTTOM LINE Early Intervention for Children With Autism Implications for Occupational Therapy

CLINICAL BOTTOM LINE Early Intervention for Children With Autism Implications for Occupational Therapy Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, J.,... Varley, J. (2010). Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: The Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics,

More information

Autism Update: Classification & Treatment

Autism Update: Classification & Treatment Autism Update: Classification & Treatment Dana Battaglia, Ph.D., CCC-SLP NYSUT Professional Issues Forum on Healthcare April 26 th, 2013 10:30-12:30 1 Who is here today? Our Goals for This Morning Introduce

More information

Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors as Predictors of Outcome in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors as Predictors of Outcome in Autism Spectrum Disorders University of Connecticut DigitalCommons@UConn Doctoral Dissertations University of Connecticut Graduate School 8-1-2014 Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors as Predictors of Outcome in Autism Spectrum

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Clinical evidence that Asperger's disorder is a mild form of autism

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Clinical evidence that Asperger's disorder is a mild form of autism Comprehensive Psychiatry xx (2007) xxx xxx www.elsevier.com/locate/comppsych Clinical evidence that Asperger's disorder is a mild form of autism Riva Ariella Ritvo a,, Edward R. Ritvo b, Donald Guthrie

More information

DSM-IV Criteria. (1) qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following:

DSM-IV Criteria. (1) qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following: DSM-IV Criteria Autistic Disorder A. A total of six (or more) items from (1), (2), and (3), with at least two from (1), and one each from (2) and (3): (1) qualitative impairment in social interaction,

More information

An Autism Primer for the PCP: What to Expect, When to Refer

An Autism Primer for the PCP: What to Expect, When to Refer An Autism Primer for the PCP: What to Expect, When to Refer Webinar November 9, 2016 John P. Pelegano MD Chief of Pediatrics Hospital for Special Care Disclosures None I will not be discussing any treatments,

More information

Developmental Disabilities: Diagnosis and Treatment. Sara Sanders, Psy.D. 03/05/15

Developmental Disabilities: Diagnosis and Treatment. Sara Sanders, Psy.D. 03/05/15 Developmental Disabilities: Diagnosis and Treatment Sara Sanders, Psy.D. 03/05/15 1 Developmental Disability A severe and chronic disability that is attributable to a mental or physical impairment that

More information