Autistic traits affect interpersonal motor coordination by modulating strategic use of role-based behavior

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Autistic traits affect interpersonal motor coordination by modulating strategic use of role-based behavior"

Transcription

1 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 DOI /s RESEARCH Autistic traits affect interpersonal motor coordination by modulating strategic use of role-based behavior Open Access Arianna Curioni 1,2,4, Ilaria Minio-Paluello 1,2*, Lucia Maria Sacheli 2,3, Matteo Candidi 1,2 and Salvatore Maria Aglioti 1,2 Abstract Background: Despite the fact that deficits in social communication and interaction are at the core of Autism Spectrum Conditions (), no study has yet tested individuals on a continuum from neurotypical development to autism in an on-line, cooperative, joint action task. In our study, we aimed to assess whether the degree of autistic traits affects participants' ability to modulate their motor behavior while interacting in a Joint Grasping task and according to their given role. Methods: Sixteen pairs of adult participants played a cooperative social interactive game in which they had to synchronize their reach-to-grasp movements. Pairs were comprised of one and one neurotypical with no cognitive disability. In alternate experimental blocks, one participant knew what action to perform (instructed role) while the other had to infer it from his/her partner s action (adaptive role). When in the adaptive condition, participants were told to respond with an action that was either opposite or similar to their partner. Participants also played a non-social control game in which they had to synchronize with a non-biological stimulus. Results: In the social interactive task, higher degree of autistic traits predicted less ability to modulate joint action according to one s interactive role. In the non-social task, autistic traits did not predict differences in movement preparation and planning, thus ruling out the possibility that social interactive task results were due to basic motor or executive function difficulties. Furthermore, when participants played the non-social game, the higher their autistic traits, the more they were interfered by the non-biological stimulus. Conclusions: Our study shows for the first time that high autistic traits predict a stereotypical interaction style when individuals are required to modulate their movements in order to coordinate with their partner according to their role in a joint action task. Specifically, the infrequent emergence of role-based motor behavior modulation during on-line motor cooperationinparticipantswithhighautistictraitsshedslight on the numerous difficulties have in nonverbal social interactions. Keywords: Autism, Joint actions, Interpersonal coordination, Coordination strategies, Role taking, Cooperation Background Autism Spectrum Conditions () are characterized by such deficits in social communication and interaction as the difficulty in understanding another person s perspective, reduced use of verbal and nonverbal signals to regulate interaction, reduced use and understanding of * Correspondence: ilaria.miniopaluello@uniroma1.it Equal contributors 1 Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Via dei Marsi 78, Rome, Italy 2 IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Via Ardeatina 306, Rome, Italy Full list of author information is available at the end of the article gestures, abnormalities in body language, and difficulties in reciprocal, cooperative social play [1]. cooperative behavior has usually been investigated in one of two ways: (1) via high-order cognitive tasks requiring participants to make a decision after having inferred something about their partner s choices, as in computer-based social hunting games [2] or the prisoner s dilemma [3 5]; or (2) via motor tasks requiring participants to take turns, foresee their partner s actions and possibly modify their behavior, e.g., passing The Author(s) Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

2 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 Page 2 of 13 an object while taking the partner s starting comfort state into account [6, 7]. Very few studies have tested participants in online cooperative motor tasks in which they have to coordinate their actions with those of a partner to achieve a common goal. Distinctive features of realistic interpersonal coordination include the need to predict, imitate, or complement what a partner will do [8, 9], as well as the need to signal one s intentions to the other by modulating movements kinematics [10 15]. These processes are managed according to contextual conditions such as, for example, who is the leader of an interaction and who is the follower required to adapt [16 20]. Coordination strategies to achieve successful interactions are likely based on a variety of motor, social, and cognitive skills. In particular, it has been proposed that on-line prediction of and adaptation to another person s action may rely on a sensory-motor simulation of the observed action [21 24] that is integrated into one s own action plan [25 28]. On-line, reciprocal time- and space-coordination represents a strong test case for social skills, as it requires predictive and sensorimotor monitoring processes to be coupled with higher order social functions (e.g., joint attention, role taking, theory of mind, visual perspective taking) that are impaired early on in the development of individuals [29]. Some studies investigating on-line motor coordination in showed that children are less able than children with developmental delay to coordinate their actions when completing simple motor interactive tasks that require two persons in cooperation to make an object/toy function [30, 31]. Other research has employed a dynamical approach to investigate social synchronization performances in children and adolescents with. Using a paradigm already tested with adult participants [32], Marsh [33] measured the amount of spontaneous interpersonal synchronization that occurred between children with and their parents while sitting on rocking chairs and compared their findings to dyads of parents-children with typical development. Their results indicate that children with showed less entrainment with their parents compared to controls. Similarly, in a study that used a pendulum coordination paradigm, it was shown that children and adolescents with engage in less synchronized behavior than controls, specifically in the anti-phase condition [34]. Taken together, these studies suggest that low-level attentional and motor deficits in individuals with may prevent them from experiencing connectedness with others through spontaneous synchronization. Such synchronization may be a first step toward the ability to coordinate and cooperate with others. Stoit and colleagues [35] measured motor performance while two participants (either two or two control children or adolescents) worked together to lift a virtual bar without dropping the virtual ball placed on top of it. Participants could either use a single joystick to control the movement of one side of the bar, or two joysticks to control both sides. Results showed that participants did not slow their reaction times in movement onset to accommodate their partner. They also favored the single joystick condition for its stronger sense of agency compared to the two joystick condition, and they were less able to synchronize their responses, causing the ball to drop shortly after movement onset. Despite the use of joysticks in their task, which reduces the impact of sensory-motor simulation, Stoit and colleagues [36] interpreted their results as evidence for the reduced ability of participants to predict and coordinate their actions with a partner. These findings are in line with impaired internal modeling in [37]. Importantly, no study has tested whether autistic traits impact sensory-motor simulation used to predict a partner s behavior and adapt to it, or the ability to modulate one s own motor behavior depending on the role played in the interaction. Our study investigates the motor cooperative behavior of and neurotypical individuals during an on-line joint action task in order to address these questions for the first time. We applied our cooperative joint action task (social interactive task [18]) by asking pairs of and neurotypical participants to coordinate in space and time in order to synchronously grasp a bottle. Participants could either have a leader role, in which they were told what specific movement to perform toward the bottle (instructed condition) or a follower role, which required on-line adaption to their partner and the performance of same/opposite movements (adaptive condition). Our aim was to test whether autistic traits modulate (i) participants ability to adjust their behavior depending on their interactional role and (ii) sensorimotor simulation tested by comparing same and opposite movements. We predicted that participants, depending on their role in the interaction and their degree of autistic traits, would differently recruit prediction and adaptation strategies in order to ease motor cooperation and facilitate on-line interpersonal coordination. To distinguish between social interactive performance and baseline level of motor, perceptual, and executive abilities, we used a newly developed non-social task in which participants had to coordinate their reach-to-grasp movements with a nonbiological stimulus. Participants We tested 19 pairs of same sex, adult participants (Table 1). Each pair consisted of one neurotypical () participant and one participant without cognitive

3 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 Page 3 of 13 Table 1 Participants' demographic information and symptoms' severity N =16 N =16 t test Sex ratio female:male 3:13 3:13 Mean age (age range) 26.1 (18 42) 29.6 (17 49) t(30) = 1.11, p = 0.27 Mean 31.9 (6.7) 10.3 (5.3) t(30) = 10.2, p < 01 ADOS Social Interaction and Communication 12 (5 17) disability. All participants took part in both experiments (i.e., social interactive task and non-social task). Participants cognitive ability was assessed by either the Raven s Standard Progressive Matrices test (RSPM) [38] or the Italian version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised (WAIS-R) [39] (see Additional file 1: Table S2 for individual scores). All participants had an IQ within the normal range (RSPM : N =9, mean = 97 range = ; : N = 9, mean = 94.5 range = ; WAIS : N = 5, average full-scale IQ = 88.8, range = 75 98). IQ scores were not available for one and two participants. As these participants had all received a master s degree, we considered them to be within the normal range. participants received an Asperger syndrome diagnosis according to DSM IV criteria [1] and completed the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised [34] and/or the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic [40] (see Additional file 1: Table S1). It was not possible to run the ADI-R with the parents of 5 participants. Before the experiment, all participants completed the questionnaire, which measures the number of autistic traits in adults [41]. Two pairs were excluded from analyses as they showed outlier kinematic or behavioral values with respect to the group mean; one pair was excluded due to data loss. Our final sample consisted of 32 participants (Table 1). Both females (N =6) and males (N = 26) were included for recruitment convenience. In nine of the pairs, participants already knew each other, while participants in the seven other pairs were informed about their diagnostic status and familiarized with each other for 1 h before the experiment began. The study was approved by the local ethics committee at IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants gave informed consent prior to participation in the study and were paid for their time. Methods Experiment 1: social interactive task In experiment 1, we aimed to test individual motor behavior and pair coordination in a joint cooperative task [18] in which participants were asked to grasp a bottleshaped object placed in front of them (one for each participant) as synchronously as possible with their partner. The experiment lasted approximately 1.5 h. Participants were either told where to grasp the bottle (instructed condition) or required to on-line adapt to their partner s movement (adaptive condition). Fifty percent of the time, participants were asked to perform movements opposite to those of their partner (opposite vs. same). This task allowed us to measure whether autistic traits modulate (i) participants ability to strategically change behavior in the adaptive vs. instructed condition or (ii) recruitment of on-line sensory-motor simulation when performing same vs. opposite movements. Exp. 1 Methods Exp. 1 setup Participants sat at either side of a table equipped with start buttons, bottle-shaped objects, go/feedback LEDs, and a monitor to provide task instructions and performance feedback after each trial (Fig. 1). The bottom part of the bottle had to be grasped with a power grip while the top part had to be grasped with a precision grip. Grasp time was recorded by two copper plates placed at the bottom and top of the bottle. Fig. 1 Setup of the social interactive task. Here, participants perform an opposite trial. The participant on the right is going to grasp the upper part of her bottle-shaped object via a precision grip while the participant on the left is going to grasp the lower part of her bottle-shaped object via a power grip

4 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 Page 4 of 13 Exp. 1 procedure In each block participants took one of the following two roles in counterbalanced order: a) Instructed: participants were told where to grasp their object before starting their movement. Through headphones, they heard either the word down (i.e., grasp the bottom part of the bottle via a power grip) or up (i.e., grasp the top part of the bottle via a precision grip). Participants had to online adapt in time to their partner (i.e., no need to on-line change the target of their movement) in order to correctly perform the task and maximize interpersonal synchrony in touching the object. b) Adaptive: participants did not know where to grasp their object in advance, having to on-line select their movements based on the movement of their partner. Through headphones, they heard either the word opposite (i.e., grasp the bottle in the opposite location your partner does) or same (i.e., grasp the bottle in the same location your partner does). Participants had to take their partner s movement into account and on-line adapt in both space and time to it in order to correctly perform the task and maximize interpersonal synchrony in touching the object. In each experimental block, participants knew which role they were to play by way of color-coded instructions displayed on the monitor at the beginning of each block, as well as a color-coded flag placed next to their start button throughout the entire block (Fig. 1). Trial timeline Participants heard their instructions via headphones. They could start their reach-to-grasp movement when a pair of LEDs placed near the right hand of their partner turned from red to green. Asynchrony between participants grasps was calculated as the absolute time interval between the two participants grasping times on their bottle-shaped objects (grasping asynchrony, see the Exp 1 data processing section). Performance feedback was provided via LEDs (green = win, red = loss) and displayed on the monitor (i.e., 1 point or 0 point and the amount of asynchrony in milliseconds). To encourage participants commitment during the task, monetary reward depended on points collected and average level of synchrony. Pairs earned a point when their asynchrony in grasping the objects was smaller than a variable time window tailored to pair performance via a staircase procedure. This caused the task to become harder as participants improved in their coordination performance, allowing us to control for task difficulty and learning effects. The experimental procedure was controlled by an E- Prime script that randomized conditions, and allowed us to on-line record start time, grasp time, grasp location, as well as to provide auditory instructions and performance feedback. Task structure Pairs performed 4 adaptive/instructed blocks of 32 trials each (128 trials in total, 16 per condition). Role (instructed/adaptive) was fixed within blocks and counterbalanced between participants, while congruency (opposite/same) and position of the target (down/up) were pseudo-randomized within block in order to avoid more than 4 consecutive opposite/same and 2 down/up trials. Experimental design was therefore a 2 role (instructed/adaptive) 2 congruency (opposite/ same) 2 position (down/up) within-subject design. Exp. 1 data processing Only correct trials were entered in the analysis (i.e., incorrect trials, in which at least one of the participants performed a wrong movement, were used to estimate pair accuracy; see below). Trials were discarded when at least one participant in the pair started his/her movement before the instructions, or if one of the grasps was not effective in closing the circuit, thus preventing touch time collection (mean % per pair = 17, SD = 9, range = 6 34%). Pair performance For each trial, we considered the following behavioral measures of pair performance: Pair accuracy (ACC): percentage of trials in which both participants grasped their object in the correct location. Grasping asynchrony (ms) (GraspAsynch): trial-bytrial absolute time interval between each participant s grasp (hand contact) of the object [abs(participant1_grasp time participant2_grasp time)]. Difference in reaction times (ms) (ΔRT): trial-bytrial time interval between the release of the start button by the participant in the adaptive role and that of the participant in the instructed role (ΔRT = adaptive RT instructed RT). Individual performance For each trial, we considered the following behavioral measures of individual performance: Reaction time (ms) (RT): interval between auditory instruction delivery time and start button release time. Movement duration (ms) (MD): interval between start button release time and grasp time. For each dependent variable, participant, and condition, we excluded values that fell 2.5 standard deviations

5 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 Page 5 of 13 (s.d.) above or below the mean as outliers. Z- transformation within participant/couple and across conditions was performed on all dependent variables but ΔRT, as it would not have allowed us to investigate trialby-trial time course of pair interaction. Movement s spatial kinematics (max index-thumb aperture, max wrist height) was also recorded with a SMART-D motion capture system (Bioengineering Technology & Systems (B T S)). See Additional file 1 for methods and results. Exp. 1 data analysis In our social interactive task, we wanted to investigate behavioral changes outside the classic and often limiting case-control framework. When possible, we thus analyzed the data from all participants collapsed via an ANOVA with the number of autistic traits (AQ) as continuous predictor. We report parametric repeated measures ANOVA when variables are all normally distributed (Shapiro- Wilk s test) or when their skewness and kurtosis can be considered normal [42]. This is the case for grasping asynchrony, delta reaction time, and movement duration. For the latter variable, being it a measure of individual performance, the analysis also includes the number of autistic traits (AQ) as a moderator. For reaction time, we report in the main text parametric ANOVA with AQ as a continuous predictor because (i) deviation from normality is minor, (ii) parametric analyses suggest a role for the AQ in mediating the effects, (iii) nonparametric results generally confirm nonparametric ones, and (iv) there is not a simple nonparametric counterpart of mixed model ANOVA nor of ANOVA with a continuous predictor (in our case, the AQ). We include nonparametric analyses in the Additional file 1. Lastly, we report nonparametric analysis when the majority of variables are not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk s test) and their skewness and kurtosis are not normal [42]. Pair performance ACC entered nonparametric, repeated measure, Friedman ANOVA, and, if significant, we ran nonparametric Wilcoxon matched pair tests. GraspAsynch and ΔRT entered separate repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with 2 role (instructed/adaptive) 2 congruency (opposite/same) as within-subjects factors. In pair performance analyses only, (i) position (down/up) was collapsed because 50% of trials (i.e., in the opposite condition) involve one participant going down while the other going up; and (ii) role was coded according to the role of the participant, i.e., the trial was coded as adaptive when the participant of the pair had the adaptive role. Individual performance RT and MD entered separate repeated measures ANOVAs with 2 role (instructed/ adaptive) 2 congruency (opposite/same) 2 position (down/up) as within-subjects factors and the autism quotient [41] scores as moderator. The use of the AQ as a moderator in the analyses on individual performance allowed us to test all participants on a continuum and investigate the influence of autistic traits across the entire spectrum from neurotypical to neurodiverse behavior. The AQ moderator was centered across all subjects, i.e., individual AQ score group mean AQ (26) [43]. As a control analysis, we also divided participants depending on their diagnosis and entered the same dependent variables in a mixed model ANOVA with 2 role (instructed/adaptive) 2 congruency (opposite/ same) 2 position (down/up) as within-subjects factors and group (/) as between subjects factor (see Additional file 1). All tests of significance were based upon an α level of 5. Significant interactions and main effects were analyzed by Newman-Keuls post hoc tests. In order to make the role of the AQ continuous predictor easier to interpret, significant interactions including the AQ were further tested using a correlational approach. From each correlation, participants with absolute standard residuals greater than 2 (range ) were removed as outliers. Results Exp. 1 Pair performance Friedman ANOVA on ACC did not show significant main effects of role and congruency, nor a significant role congruency significant interaction (chi square (16,3) = 3.22, p = 0.36), indicating that pairs reached an equal level of accuracy in all experimental conditions. Analysis on GraspAsynch showed a significant main effect of congruency (F(1,15) = 6.84, p = 2, η 2 = 0.31), with greater asynchrony when participants performed opposite vs. same movements. All other ps > Analysis on ΔRT showed a significant main effect of congruency (F(1,15) = 75.07, p < 0, η 2 = 0.83), indicating longer ΔRT for opposite trials and a significant role congruency interaction (F(1,15) = 5.27, p = 4, η 2 = 0.26) (Fig. 2). Note that positive ΔRT values indicate that the participant in the adaptive role (either or ) started to move after his/her partner, while negative values indicate that he/she started to move before his/ her partner. Post hoc tests showed that ΔRT values are longer in opposite vs. same trials (ps < 01) and that only in same trials there is a difference in ΔRT, depending on which pair member takes the adaptive role (p = 2). This difference is not significant in opposite trials (p = 0.60).

6 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 Page 6 of 13 RT Adaptive- Instructed Adaptive- Instructed OPPOSITE SAME Fig. 2 Difference in RT (ΔRT) between participants playing in the same pair according to their interactional role and diagnostic status. Lower and upper bars correspond to the first and third quartiles respectively. Horizontal dark lines correspond to the median. ***p <.001, **p <.01 Mean ΔRT was always positive in opposite trials (significant one sample t tests against zero adaptive t(16) = 4.75; adaptive t(16) = 5.16, ps < 01), meaning that the adaptive player always started to move after his/her partner, regardless of being or. This was not the case for same trials, when ΔRT was positive only when had the adaptive role (one sample t tests against zero t(16) = 0.92, p = 0.37; t(16) = 2.09, p = 5). That is, in same trials, participants in the adaptive role started their movement at the same time as their partner, rather than waiting to see which grip their partner would make. Individual performance Analysis on RT showed a significant main effect of role (F(1,30) = 17.88, p < 01, η 2 = 0.37), indicating that participants in the adaptive role exhibited longer reaction times than those in the instructed role. This effect was modulated by participants autistic traits (role AQ interaction = F(1,30) = 4.635, p = 39, η 2 = 0.13). To interpret this interaction, we subtracted individuals RTs in the instructed condition from their RTs in the adaptive condition (higher positive values index participants tendency to be slower when they need to adapt) and correlated this index with their AQ score. We found a negative correlation (N = 31, r = 0.40, p =2), indicating that the lower the AQ, the longer the RT in the adaptive as compared to the instructed condition (i.e., indexing more strategic modulation of one s RTs) (Fig. 3a). Additionally, we investigated the correlation between individual mean RTs and the AQ moderator separately for the adaptive and instructed conditions. We found a negative linear correlation in the adaptive condition (N = 30, r = 0.38, p = 4), indicating that the higher the autistic traits, the shorter the time taken to initiate the movement (i.e., less strategic adaptation) (Fig. 3b). We also found a positive correlation in the instructed condition (N = 30, r = 0.43, p = 2), indicating that the higher the autistic traits, the longer the time taken to initiate the movement (Fig. 3c). Furthermore, results also included significant main effects of position (F(1,30) = 42.78, p < 01, η 2 = 0.58) and congruency (F(1,30) = 90.84, p < 01, η 2 =0.75). The former indicates that RTs were longer for up than they were for down grasps; the latter indicates that opposite movements caused longer RTs than same ones. This latter effect was modulated by the autistic traits of participants (congruency AQ interaction F(1,30) = 5.8, p = 2, η 2 = 0.16). To interpret this interaction, we subtracted individuals RTs in the same condition from their RTs in the opposite condition (higher positive values index longer time to initiate the movement in opposite compared to same trials) and correlated this index with their AQ score. We found a positive correlation (N = 30, r =0.53, p = 02), which indicates that the higher the AQ, the slower the RT in the opposite compared to the same condition (Fig. 4a). Additionally, we investigated the correlation between individual mean RTs and the AQ moderator separately for opposite and same trials. Correlational analysis showed that participants with higher autistic traits tended to show longer RT in opposite trials (N = 31, r =0.38, p =3) (Fig. 4b) and shorter RT in same trials (N =31, r = 0.38, p = 3) (Fig. 4c). Significant interactions include role congruency (F(1,30) = 23.3, p < 01, η 2 = 0.43), congruency position (F(1,30) = 31.7, p < 01, η 2 = 0.51), and role congruency position (F(1,30) = 22.0, p < 01, η 2 = 0.42). The latter indicates longer RT in opposite-up trials for participants in the adaptive condition (p = 01). All other ps > 0.16.

7 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 Page 7 of 13 RT Adaptive - RT Instructed a) b) c) r = p = 2 RT Adaptive r = p = 4 RT Instructed r = 0.43 p = Fig. 3 Correlations between individual RT and number of autistic traits (AQ) according to role in the social interaction. a The higher participant s AQ, the smaller his/her RT difference for adaptive vs. instructed movements; b the higher participant s AQ, the faster his/her RT for adaptive movements; c the higher participant s AQ, the slower his/her RT for instructed movements Analysis on MD showed significant main effect of congruency (F(1,30) = 5.15, p = 3, η 2 = 0.14) and position (F(1,30) = 5.7, p = 2, η 2 = 0.16). The former indicates longer MD in opposite vs. same trials; the latter indicates longer MD in up vs. down trials (as expected given the setup physical constraints), as well as a significant role AQ interaction (F(1,30) = 6.15, p = 2, η 2 = 0.17). To interpret the latter interaction, we subtracted individual MD in the instructed from individual MD in the adaptive condition (higher positive values index longer movement duration in participants playing adaptive compared to instructed) and correlated this index with the AQ. We found a positive correlation (N = 31, r = 0.50, p < 1). This indicates that the lower the AQ, the longer the MD in the instructed vs. the adaptive condition (Fig. 5a). The AQ moderator correlated negatively with MD in the instructed condition (N = 30, r = 0.58, p < 1) and positively in the adaptive condition (N = 31, r = 0.50, p < 1). This suggests that the lower the autistic traits in the instructed role, the longer the MD (Fig. 5b). The opposite pattern was true for the adaptive role (Fig. 5c). Analysis on MD also showed a significant role congruency interaction (F(1,30) = 36.3, p < 01, η 2 = 0.54) and a trend to significance of the role congruency AQ interaction (F(1,30) = 3.9, p = 6, η 2 = 0.11). All other ps > To interpret the latter interaction, we subtracted individual MD in the instructed condition from individual MD in the adaptive condition. We did this separately for opposite and same trials, correlating this index with the AQ. We found a positive correlation for both opposite and same trials (N =31, r = 0.43, p < 2 and N = 31, r = 0.55, p < 01), indicating that the higher the AQ, the longer the MD in the adaptive vs. instructed condition of both trials. These results indicate that participants with high autistic traits performed slower movements in the adaptive condition, regardless of their congruency with their partner s movement. Experiment 2: non-social task In experiment 2, we tested whether autistic traits modulate motor behavior and coordination abilities of and participants in an individual coordination task. The task lasted approximately 1 h. As in RT Opposite - RT Same a) b) c) r = 0.53 p < 1 RT Opposite r = 0.38 p = 3 RT Same r = p = Fig. 4 Correlations between individual RT and number of autistic traits (AQ) according to the congruency between observed and executed actions. a The higher participant s AQ, the greater his/her RT difference for opposite vs. same movements; b the higher participant s AQ, the slower his/her RT for opposite movements; c the higher participant s AQ, the faster his/her RT for same movements

8 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 Page 8 of 13 MD Adaptive - MD Instructed a) b) c) r = 0.50 p < 1 MD Adaptive r = 0.50 p < 1 MD Instructed r = p < Fig. 5 Correlations between individual movement duration (MD) and number of autistic traits (AQ) according to the role in the social interaction. a The higher participant s AQ, the greater his/her MD difference for adaptive vs. instructed movements; b the higher participant s AQ, the longer his/her MD for adaptive movements; c the higher participant s AQ, the shorter his/her MD for instructed movements the social task, participants had to grasp a bottleshaped object in front of them, but this time they had to do so in synchrony with a dot (i.e., non-social stimulus) that moved on a screen toward two possible targets placed next to either the upper or lower part of the bottle (Fig. 6). Participants grasping location was either spatially congruent or incongruent with the dot s final position on the screen. As in exp. 1, participants were told one of two things before each trial: (1) where to grasp the bottle (instructed condition), or (2) to grasp the bottle in the same/opposite position with respect to the dot, therefore having to on-line adapt to its trajectory (adaptive condition). We introduced the non-social task to control whether differences in motor performance and executive control per se (independently from the interactive context) were accountable for the results found in the social interactive task. Exp. 2 Methods Exp. 2 setup Participants sat in front of a cm table with a LCD monitor used for stimuli presentation. Stimuli consisted of 128 videos of a dot moving along a diagonal straight line toward one of two green rectangles placed next to the grasping points on the bottle. The dot started its movement from a location spatially similar to the starting position of the partner's hand in exp. 1. In order to prevent participants from predicting the dot s stopping time, its velocity profiles were taken from the kinematics of individuals wrists while reaching and grasping the bottles. Videos were created using MatLab software. The dot s stopping times during the task were recorded by a photodiode attached to the screen and connected to a PC running E-Prime2 software via a TTL cable (Psychology Software Tools Inc., Pittsburgh, PA). Participants received auditory instructions as in exp. 1. Exp. 2 procedure As described in exp. 1, participants performed the task while following one of two different roles (instructed or adaptive, in counterbalanced order) and were asked to perform opposite or same movements with respect to the dot s trajectory and target location. Trial-time line and Task structure is same as exp. 1. Fig. 6 Setup of the non-social task. Example of a congruent trial in which the participant is about to grasp the upper part of the bottleshaped object with a precision grip and the dot is about to stop next to the upper target Exp. 2 data processing Only correct trials were entered in the analysis. Trials in which participants started their movement before hearing the auditory instructions were discarded, as were those in which the wrong part of the bottle was grasped (mean % per participant = 9, SD = 5, range = 1 21) (or used to compute ACC, see below). For each trial, we considered the following behavioral measures:

9 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 Page 9 of 13 Accuracy (ACC): percentage of trials in which participant grasped the object in the correct location. Grasping asynchrony (ms) (GraspAsynch): trial-bytrial absolute time interval between participant s grasp time and dot s stop time [abs(participant_grasp time dot_stop time)]. Reaction time (ms) (RT): interval between delivery time of auditory instructions and start button release time. Movement duration (ms) (MD): interval between start button release time and grasp time. Exp. 2 data analysis The data analysis of experiment 2 is as Exp. 1. Results Exp. 2 Individual performance Friedman ANOVA on ACC was significant (chi square (32,7) = 16.55, p = 2). We then ran a Wilcoxon matched pair test in order to investigate differences in ACC between roles and between congruency. None of the contrasts survived Bonferroni s correction for multiple comparisons (ps > 2, corrected α = 5/4 = 1), indicating that participants reached an equal level of accuracy in all experimental conditions. Analysis on GraspAsynch showed significant main effects of role (F(1,30) = 55.19, p <01, η 2 = 0.64) and position (F(1,30) = 16.14, p <01, η 2 = 0.34). The former indicates that participants in the instructed role were more synchronous with the dot than in the adaptive role; the latter indicates that participants were more synchronous with the dot when grasping the lower part of the bottle. Results also included significant congruency AQ (F(1,30) = 4.24, p =48,η 2 = 0.12) and role congruency (F(1,30) = 6.45, p =16,η 2 = 0.17) interactions, explained by the significant third level Role x Congruency x AQ interaction (F(1,30) = 4.95, p =3,η 2 =0.14). To interpret this last interaction, we calculated an index of the significant effect (the GraspAsynch difference between opposite and same trials, collapsed for the factor position as it did not interact with the others) for each role and correlated it with the AQ moderator (higher positive values indexing participants tendency to show worse GraspAsynch when performing opposite movements compared to same ones). Correlational analysis revealed a positive linear correlation between AQ and adaptive (opposite-same) index (N = 31, r = 0.49, p = 05) where the higher the autistic traits, the worse the performance (i.e., greater asynchrony) in the opposite vs. the same condition (Fig. 7a). This suggests that participants with higher autistic traits were less able to ignore the dot s spatially incongruent trajectory and target location in the adaptive role. Instead, the correlation between AQ and instructed (opposite-same) index was not significant (N = 31, r = 3, p = 0.85) (Fig. 7b). All other ps > Analysis on RT showed significant main effects of role (F(1,30) = 260, p < 01, η 2 = 0.90), and congruency (F(1,30) = 55.74, p <01, η 2 = 0.65) that are explained by the significant second-order role congruency interaction (F(1,30) = 36.61, p < 01, η 2 = 0.55). This indicates that participants showed longer RTs in opposite vs. same trials when taking the adaptive (p <01) but not the instructed (p = 0.33) role. Results also show a significant role position interaction (F(1,30) = 14.70, p < 01, η 2 = 0.32), indicating that RTs are longer for up trials when taking the instructed (p = 01) but not the adaptive (p = 7) role. The absence of significant interactions with the AQ moderator (all ps > 0.13) indicates that autistic traits did not modulate participants RTs in the individual coordination task. All other ps > Analysis on MD showed significant main effects of congruency (F(1,30) = 94.41, p < 01, η 2 = 0.75) and position (F(1,30) = 94.41, p < 01, η 2 = 0.75), as well as a significant congruency position interaction (F(1,30) = 4.25, p =48, η 2 = 0.12) that was explained by the significant higher order role congruency position AQ interaction (F(1,30) = 6.07, p =1, η 2 =0.17). To interpret this last interaction, we calculated the difference between individuals mean MD in opposite minus same trials for each role and position, and correlated it with the AQ moderator. Correlational analysis revealed a positive linear correlation between AQ and the adaptive (oppositesame) down index (N =32, r =0.36, p =4). This indicates that in the adaptive role, movement duration in the opposite down trials is longer the higher the autistic traits. This suggests that when performing a down movement in the adaptive role, participants with higher autistic traits were less able to ignore the dot s upward spatially incongruent trajectory and target location. All other ps > Discussion The goal of the present study was to investigate cooperative motor behavior of participants on a continuum from neurotypical development to autism. Given the high variability within the autism spectrum and the often blurry distinction between neurotypical and neurodiverse behavior when considering individuals without cognitive disability, we favored an investigation that would go beyond dichotomous diagnostic boundaries. Our study addressed if and how the number of autistic traits impacts participants ability to aid motor cooperation and facilitate on-line interpersonal coordination by predicting and adapting to the movements of a partner, as well as by adjusting one s behavior depending on the role taken during the interaction.

10 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 Page 10 of 13 GrasAsy Instructed OP-SA Ave(Down,Up) a) b) r = 3 p = 0.85 GrasAsy Adaptive OP-SA Ave(Down,Up) Fig. 7 Correlations between individual grasping asynchrony (GraspAsynch) and number of autistic traits (AQ) according to movements congruency and participant s diagnostic status. a No significant relationship between participants AQ and their GraspAsynch difference between opposite vs. same instructed movements, b the higher participant s AQ, the larger his/her GraspAsynch difference for opposite vs. same adaptive movements r = 0.49 p < 1 Social interactive task: the higher your autistic traits, the less you wait for your partner s movement Pairs of and participants were less coordinated (i.e., synchronous) when they had to perform opposite movements, regardless of which pair member took the adaptive or instructed role. Previous results on pairs of participants [18, 44] instead showed no difference in coordination between same and opposite trials. Such an unexpected difference may be related to the fact that interaction is facilitated by withingroup similarities in communication and interaction styles [45, 46], while interactions between out-group individuals may be more difficult [40]. Results on the difference in reaction times showed that when played adaptive, they always waited for their instructed partner to start his/her movement. This was not the case for participants, who did not wait for their partner when performing same movements. A possible interpretation is that participants may not feel the need to gather additional information from their partner s movement in same trials and therefore start to move independently from him/her as soon as they hear the instructions. This is supported by individual reaction time results from the adaptive condition showing that the higher the autistic traits, the faster participants started to move. Social interactive task: the higher your autistic traits, the less you modulate your movement duration according to your role Modulation of action planning and execution during joint action depends on the role individuals take to achieve the shared goal [19, 47]. In our task, successful coordination required participants not only to represent their individual task (e.g., grasp the bottom part of the bottle) but also to integrate it with their partner s one (i.e., guide the interaction when playing instructed or adapt to their partner when playing adaptive). Our results show that in the instructed condition, participants took more time to perform movements than in the adaptive condition and that this was more true for participants with low autistic traits. A number of studies have found that participants have longer reaction times and movement durations than controls in simple goal-directed reach-to-grasp movements. These results have been interpreted either as atypical movement planning [48 52] or atypical action execution [53]. Our findings seem unlikely to be related to either movement planning or execution deficits per se, as the differences in motor behavior are selectively modulated by the role taken in the interaction. We interpret them in the light of previous studies on sensorimotor communication strategies during joint action, which found that neurotypical participants engage in systematic modulations of their movements to foster coordination and synchrony [10, 13, 15, 18, 19, 26]. When taking the instructed role, our participants with low autistic traits may have therefore slowed their movement in an attempt to reach better pair synchronization by making adaptation easier for their partner. In the adaptive condition, on the other hand, participants with low autistic traits showed shorter movement duration, possibly because they had to keep up with their high autistic traits partner who did not slow his/her movement to foster coordination. reduced use of communicative and adaptive strategies to facilitate coordination during joint action suggests a difficulty in online updating individual actions according to the constraints of the interactive task, which might relate to the tendency to not benefit from an interactive context, e.g., when they have to detect an interacting agent presented via point light display [54]. On the one hand, this might be linked to the difficulty in picking up

11 Curioni et al. Molecular Autism (2017) 8:23 Page 11 of 13 subtle, interactive-related cues from another person s kinematics, and to reduced use of such cues during an interaction. On the other hand, previous studies have shown that children are able to cognitively represent another person s task, as well as his/her relative needs [30], and that adults are even interfered by it when playing next to another individual [55]. This may suggest that the absence of role-based adjustment of motor behavior in participants with high autistic traits might not be due to a lack of understanding that one s partner has a different role, but rather to a reduced ability in recruiting strategies aimed to facilitate the other s task [13]. Non-social task: the higher your autistic traits, the more you are interfered by a non-social stimulus The results of our non-social reach-to-grasp task are informative about individuals motor performance in a way that goes beyond the social nature of the interactive task and support the idea that the social task s results do not depend on AQ-related differences in motor planning or execution. Coordination performance in the adaptiveopposite condition was predicted by participants autistic traits, i.e., the higher the autistic traits, the worse participants were able to synchronize with the dot when they had to move toward an opposite location. Worse performance in opposite vs. same trials can be considered as evidence of interference effect exerted by the incongruent trajectory of the observed dot over participants movement execution. Previous studies found no difference in the interference effect when and participants observe real human motion or a dot moving with a biological velocity profile [56, 57]. Further, no group difference was found in classical response compatibility tasks where the observed movement is irrelevant [58 60]. Accordingly, autistic traits in our task did not predict performance in the instructed condition, where no on-line coordination in space was required. On the contrary, autistic traits modulated the interference effect exerted by the dot in the adaptive condition, when participants needed to take the dot s trajectory into account in order to on-line adapt to it and select the target location. Given the significant interaction between role (instructed/adaptive) and congruency (same/opposite), we interpret our results as evidence of difficulty in both ignoring the dot s opposite trajectory and target location and inhibiting the automatic tendency to follow a similar trajectory, which might relate to -reduced executive control [61 63]. Consistent with our results on synchrony performance, participants with higher autistic traits showed longer movement duration in the adaptive-opposite condition, which could be regarded as another index of interference effect. Due to the setup s physical properties, this interference effect might only become apparent when participants follow the dot and go up, while all that is required is to go down and grasp the bottom part of the object. Motor difficulties, often present in individuals [64, 52], could contribute to the AQ-related differences found in the current experiments. The reach-to-grasp movement has been studied in autism [65] and a recent review of reach-to-grasp [66] reports that individuals show longer movement duration ([67, 68] but see [36]), greater change in acceleration ([68], but see [65]), and slower reaction times ([48] but see [67]) than neurotypicals. In our non-social task, the fact that the number of autistic traits effect on movement duration and synchrony is always based on the role played by participants seems to suggest that these effects are not related to action execution per se. In addition, the absence of an interaction with participants autistic traits in the analyses on reaction times clarifies that, in a non-social context in which participants had to coordinate their reach-to-grasp movements with a non-biological stimulus, autistic traits do not predict differences in movement preparation and planning. Therefore, basic differences in motor behavior appear not to be responsible for the results of the social interactive task. Conclusions In this study, we investigated for the first time the influence of autistic traits on the ability to engage in a cooperative joint action task requiring predictive and adaptive abilities. We tested pairs of and neurotypical participants in a joint-grasping task in which they had to synchronously grasp objects in front of them. By assigning participants to the instructed or adaptive role, we were able to investigate their ability to modify their behavior consistently with their role in order to ease coordination. We also tested participants in a non-social coordination task that allowed us to control for differences in motor execution per se. Results showed that higher autistic traits lessened participants ability to modulate their joint action according to their interactive role in the social task. In particular, participants with high autistic traits did not wait for their partner and showed longer movement duration when performing both same and opposite actions during trials in which they needed to adapt to their partner s movement. No such effect was found in the conditions that did not require adaptation to the partner s movements nor was it found in the non-social task. Indeed, autistic traits did not predict differences in movement preparation and planning in the non-social task, thus ruling out the possibility that the results of the social interactive task were due to basic motor or executive function difficulties. Overall, reduced use of communicative and adaptive strategies to facilitate coordination during joint action seems to suggest that participants with high autistic

7 Grip aperture and target shape

7 Grip aperture and target shape 7 Grip aperture and target shape Based on: Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. The influence of target object shape on maximum grip aperture in human grasping movements. Exp Brain Res, In revision 103 Introduction

More information

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task? Bachelor s Thesis Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a sequential movement task? Nils Hatger Prof. Dr. Ing. Willem Verwey Jonathan Barnhoorn, PhD student BMS Faculty/Cognitive

More information

Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed

Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed Manard et al. BMC Neuroscience 2014, 15:7 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed Marine Manard 1,2, Delphine Carabin

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants Supplementary experiment: neutral faces This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants would automatically shift their attention towards to objects the seen

More information

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Anna SORKIN¹, Avi PELED 2, Daphna WEINSHALL¹ 1 Interdisciplinary Center for Neural Computation, Hebrew University of Jerusalem,

More information

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Overview Diagnostic Criteria Current: Diagnostic & Statistical

More information

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D.

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. Autism Spectrum Disorder Neurodevelopmental disorder Reflects understanding of the etiology of disorder as related to alterations

More information

Grace Iarocci Ph.D., R. Psych., Associate Professor of Psychology Simon Fraser University

Grace Iarocci Ph.D., R. Psych., Associate Professor of Psychology Simon Fraser University Grace Iarocci Ph.D., R. Psych., Associate Professor of Psychology Simon Fraser University Theoretical perspective on ASD Where does attention and perception fit within the diagnostic triad of impairments?

More information

Include Autism Presents: The Volunteer Handbook

Include Autism Presents: The Volunteer Handbook Include Autism Presents: The Volunteer Handbook 1 The Volunteer Handbook: Working With People Who Have An Autism Spectrum Disorder Brought to you by: Include Autism 2 Include Autism, Inc. 2014 Table of

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/1176170/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Mindblind Eyes: An Absence of Spontaneous Theory of Mind in Asperger Syndrome Atsushi Senju,* Victoria Southgate, Sarah White,

More information

Interactions Between Auditory Elevation, Auditory Pitch and Visual Elevation during Multisensory Perception

Interactions Between Auditory Elevation, Auditory Pitch and Visual Elevation during Multisensory Perception Multisensory Research Interactions Between Auditory Elevation, Auditory Pitch and Visual Elevation during Multisensory Perception Yaseen Jamal 1, Simon Lacey 1, Lynne Nygaard 2 and K. Sathian 1,2,3,4 1

More information

The Gaze Cueing Paradigm with Eye Tracking Background Set-up Lab

The Gaze Cueing Paradigm with Eye Tracking Background Set-up Lab iworx Physiology Lab Experiment Experiment HP-17 The Gaze Cueing Paradigm with Eye Tracking Background Set-up Lab Note: The lab presented here is intended for evaluation purposes only. iworx users should

More information

Virginia s Autism Competencies for Direct Support Professionals and Supervisors who support individuals with Developmental Disabilities

Virginia s Autism Competencies for Direct Support Professionals and Supervisors who support individuals with Developmental Disabilities Autism Competencies Checklist (rev. 9.1.17) DMAS#P201 Virginia s Autism Competencies for Direct Support Professionals and Supervisors who support individuals with Developmental Disabilities The Autism

More information

7 Mistakes HR Professionals Make When Accommodating Employees Living on the Autism Spectrum By Sarah Taylor

7 Mistakes HR Professionals Make When Accommodating Employees Living on the Autism Spectrum By Sarah Taylor 7 Mistakes HR Professionals Make When Accommodating Employees Living on the Autism Spectrum By Sarah Taylor Sarah Taylor - Next Level ASD Consulting - www.nextleveasdconsult.com 1 7 Mistakes HR Professionals

More information

AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS. Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician

AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS. Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician Disclosure I do not have any financial relationships to disclose I do not plan to discuss

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism [Slide 1]: Welcome Welcome to the New Mexico TEAM technical assistance module on making eligibility determinations under the category of autism. This module will review the guidance of the NM TEAM section

More information

Down Syndrome and Autism

Down Syndrome and Autism Down Syndrome and Autism Lina Patel, Psy.D. Sie Center for Down Syndrome Children s Hospital Colorado University of Colorado School of Medicine Lina.Patel@childrenscolorado.org Financial Disclosure Objectives

More information

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

CANTAB Test descriptions by function CANTAB Test descriptions by function The 22 tests in the CANTAB battery may be divided into the following main types of task: screening tests visual memory tests executive function, working memory and

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

T. Rene Jamison * and Jessica Oeth Schuttler

T. Rene Jamison * and Jessica Oeth Schuttler Jamison and Schuttler Molecular Autism (2015) 6:53 DOI 10.1186/s13229-015-0044-x RESEARCH Open Access Examining social competence, self-perception, quality of life, and internalizing and externalizing

More information

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction Imitation and anticipation 1 It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction Roland Pfister 1, David Dignath 1, Bernhard Hommel 2, & Wilfried Kunde 1 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

DSM 5 Criteria to Diagnose Autism

DSM 5 Criteria to Diagnose Autism DSM 5 Criteria to Diagnose Autism Patient Name Patient Date of Birth Patient Health Plan Provider Name and Credential Date of Exam Only a doctoral level clinician (MD, PhD, and/or PsyD) can complete this

More information

What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA.

What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA. What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA. What is Autism? ) Autism is a complex developmental disability that has a neurological basis that causes impairments in social interactions, communication,

More information

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 1 The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning Anila D Mello Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 2 Abstract Learning and changing

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 284 (2015) 167 178 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report How coordinate and categorical

More information

Presents: Asperger Syndrome: From Diagnosis to Independence

Presents: Asperger Syndrome: From Diagnosis to Independence Presents: Asperger Syndrome: From Diagnosis to Independence Presents: Asperger Syndrome: From Diagnosis to Independence made possible by our underwriter: Presents: Asperger Syndrome: From Diagnosis to

More information

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Toddler Module

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Toddler Module The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Toddler Module A new module of a standardized diagnostic measure for Autism spectrum disorders in toddlers Romina Moavero About the ADOS The Autism Diagnostic

More information

MOVEMENT PREPARATION LAB. Name: Score: Activity I: Predictability of the correct response choice & Influence of Pre cueing

MOVEMENT PREPARATION LAB. Name: Score: Activity I: Predictability of the correct response choice & Influence of Pre cueing MOVEMENT PREPARATION LAB Name: Score: Activity I: Predictability of the correct response choice & Influence of Pre cueing Introduction: Research evidence has consistently shown that as the predictability

More information

Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders

Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders Background Autistic is a broad term coined in the twentieth century by Bleuler that

More information

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome 5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome Published in: Neuropsychologia, 2008, 47 (3), 652-656. Chapter 5 Abstract The semantic and phonemic fluency performance of adults with high functioning

More information

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses. Which of the following findings does NOT support the existence of task-specific mental resources? 1. a) It is more difficult to combine two verbal tasks than one verbal task and one spatial task. 2. b)

More information

The inner workings of working memory: Preliminary data from unimpaired populations

The inner workings of working memory: Preliminary data from unimpaired populations Hayes 1 The inner workings of working memory: Preliminary data from unimpaired populations Abstract Wright et al. (2007) tested Persons With Aphasia (PWA) using three N-Back tasks featuring different types

More information

OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION

OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS IN EMERGING ADULTS Douglas J. Scambler, Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist Presented at the University of Wyoming November 6, 2012 OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION What are the autism spectrum

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Effects of Flashing Lights and Beeping Tones on Subjective Time Estimation

Effects of Flashing Lights and Beeping Tones on Subjective Time Estimation ME 224 EXPERIMENTAL ENGINEERING Effects of Flashing Lights and Beeping Tones on Subjective Time Estimation ME 224 Final Project Report Aamir Habib, Yoke Peng Leong, Yuchen Yang 12/3/2011 Contents Abstract...

More information

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SESORY COSTRAIS? TOWARD A UIFIED THEORY OF GEERAL TASK DIFFICULTY Hanna Benoni and Yehoshua Tsal Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University hannaben@post.tau.ac.il

More information

Understanding Autism. Julie Smith, MA, BCBA. November 12, 2015

Understanding Autism. Julie Smith, MA, BCBA. November 12, 2015 Understanding Autism Julie Smith, MA, BCBA November 12, 2015 2 Overview What is Autism New DSM-5; changes to diagnosis Potential causes Communication strategies Managing difficult behaviors Effective programming

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching Ayla Barutchu 1 *, Stefanie I. Becker 2, Olivia Carter 3, Robert Hester 3, Neil L. Levy 1,4 1 Florey Institute

More information

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance

Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Mem Cogn (2011) 39:1222 1231 DOI 10.3758/s13421-011-0105-0 Effects of delay of prospective memory cues in an ongoing task on prospective memory task performance Dawn M. McBride & Jaclyn K. Beckner & Drew

More information

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze Psychology, 2014, 5, 451-460 Published Online April 2014 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.55055 Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift

More information

BAR ILAN UNIVERSITY. Peer Collaboration Intervention for Minimally. Verbal Children with Autism Spectrum

BAR ILAN UNIVERSITY. Peer Collaboration Intervention for Minimally. Verbal Children with Autism Spectrum BAR ILAN UNIVERSITY Peer Collaboration Intervention for Minimally Verbal Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder in Relation to their Sensory Profile Karen Samuel Magal Submitted in partial fulfillment

More information

An Interim Report for the Mitsubishi Foundation January, 1989 Directed by : Masataka Ohta

An Interim Report for the Mitsubishi Foundation January, 1989 Directed by : Masataka Ohta Annex1 An Interim Report for the Mitsubishi Foundation January, 1989 Directed by : Masataka Ohta On the operational criteria of Ohta s Staging as an instrument measured cognitive development in autism

More information

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use Amanda Hahn (achahn30@gmail.com) Takashi Yamauchi (tya@psyc.tamu.edu) Na-Yung Yu (nayungyu@gmail.com) Department of Psychology, Mail

More information

An Autism Primer for the PCP: What to Expect, When to Refer

An Autism Primer for the PCP: What to Expect, When to Refer An Autism Primer for the PCP: What to Expect, When to Refer Webinar November 9, 2016 John P. Pelegano MD Chief of Pediatrics Hospital for Special Care Disclosures None I will not be discussing any treatments,

More information

Collaboration in neurodivergent teams

Collaboration in neurodivergent teams Collaboration in neurodivergent teams March 2018 Neurodiversity Life Goal social conformance knowledge action art power 1. Neurotypical Disorder 90% (roughly) 2. Autism Spectrum Disorder(s) 1.5% (1 in

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

The Whole Child Approach of the Social Security Administration:

The Whole Child Approach of the Social Security Administration: THESE ARE THE FORMS I USE THIS IS NOT LEGAL ADVICE AND INTENDED TO SUPPLEMENT YOUR PARTICULAR FACTUAL SITUATION ONLY It is crucial you educate yourself on the Social Security Regulations that define and

More information

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the 1 Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 835-54. doi:10.1037/a0012726

More information

Tracking the compatibility effect of hand grip and stimulus size

Tracking the compatibility effect of hand grip and stimulus size Tracking the compatibility effect of hand grip and stimulus size Andrea Flumini Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Italy Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Granada, Spain

More information

Starting Strong 2015 Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders and An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis

Starting Strong 2015 Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders and An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis Starting Strong 2015 Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders and An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis Robin Talley, M.Ed., BCBA UW Autism Center Presentation Overview Overview of Autism Spectrum

More information

Reflexive Spatial Attention to Goal-Directed Reaching

Reflexive Spatial Attention to Goal-Directed Reaching Reflexive Spatial Attention to Goal-Directed Reaching Alexis A. Barton (aabarton@indiana.edu) Bennett I. Bertenthal (bbertent@indiana.edu) Samuel Harding (hardinsm@indiana.edu) Department of Psychological

More information

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments Perception, 2010, volume 39, pages 514 ^ 532 doi:10.1068/p6501 The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments Alexander Todorov, Valerie Loehr, Nikolaas N Oosterhof Department

More information

Response dynamics in prospective memory

Response dynamics in prospective memory Psychon Bull Rev (2015) 22:1020 1028 DOI 10.3758/s13423-014-0771-6 BRIEF REPORT Response dynamics in prospective memory Drew H. Abney & Dawn M. McBride & Angela M. Conte & David W. Vinson Published online:

More information

What is Autism? ASD 101 & Positive Behavior Supports. Autism Spectrum Disorders. Lucas Scott Education Specialist

What is Autism? ASD 101 & Positive Behavior Supports. Autism Spectrum Disorders. Lucas Scott Education Specialist ASD 101 & Positive Behavior Supports Lucas Scott Education Specialist What is Autism? Autism Spectrum Disorders Developmental, neurological condition that affects social interaction, communication of ideas

More information

Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare. Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos

Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare. Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos Supplemental Experimental Procedures Subjects Seven adult capuchin monkeys were tested.

More information

The Effects of Racial Similarity and Dissimilarity on the Joint Simon Task

The Effects of Racial Similarity and Dissimilarity on the Joint Simon Task The Effects of Racial Similarity and Dissimilarity on the Joint Simon Task Steve Croker (s.croker@ilstu.edu), J. Scott Jordan (jsjorda@ilstu.edu), Daniel S. Schloesser (dschloe@ilstu.edu), Vincent Cialdella

More information

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk

Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Exp Brain Res (1998) 123:346±350 Springer-Verlag 1998 RESEARCH NOTE Jinsung Wang George E. Stelmach Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk Received: 1 April

More information

Experiment HP-13: The Gaze Cue Paradigm

Experiment HP-13: The Gaze Cue Paradigm Experiment HP-13: The Gaze Cue Paradigm Background During almost all social interactions, people s eyes convey information about their direction of attention as well as their emotional and mental status.

More information

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load Psychonomic Bulletin & Review In press Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load JOSHUA D. COSMAN AND SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Attentional capture by abrupt onsets can be

More information

Influence of Distraction on Task. Performance in Children and Adults

Influence of Distraction on Task. Performance in Children and Adults 1 Influence of Distraction on Task Performance in Children and Adults Liza Timmermans S767534 Master thesis Augustus 2010 Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences Department of Developmental Psychology

More information

PDF created with pdffactory trial version Control of memory

PDF created with pdffactory trial version   Control of memory Control of memory Craik, Govoni, Naveh-Benjamin, & Anderson (1996): Retrieval mode Words were presented auditorily and recalled verbally. The secondary visual-manual task required pressing a key where

More information

From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM IV

From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM IV From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM IV (I) A total of six (or more) items from (A), (B), and (C), with at least two from (A), and one each from (B) and (C) (A) qualitative impairment

More information

The social brain. We saw that people are better than chance at predicting a partner s cooperation. But how?

The social brain. We saw that people are better than chance at predicting a partner s cooperation. But how? The social brain We saw that people are better than chance at predicting a partner s cooperation. But how? cognitive adaptations: is there a cheater-detection module? reading minds with a theory of mind

More information

Study Registration for the KPU Study Registry Single-Trial Confirmatory Presentiment Experiment

Study Registration for the KPU Study Registry Single-Trial Confirmatory Presentiment Experiment KPU Registry ID Number: 1018 Date submitted: 16th December 2015 Study Registration for the KPU Study Registry Single-Trial Confirmatory Presentiment Experiment The registration information for the study

More information

INDICATORS OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

INDICATORS OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER INDICATORS OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER While many of the behaviors typical of Autism Spectrum Disorder are also typical of earlier stages of normal development, it is the combination or pattern of behaviors

More information

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Parkland College A with Honors Projects Honors Program 2015 Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children Hannah Li Parkland College Recommended Citation Li, Hannah, "Social and Pragmatic Language

More information

4 Detailed Information Processing in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome: The Usefulness of Neuropsychological Tests and Self-reports

4 Detailed Information Processing in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome: The Usefulness of Neuropsychological Tests and Self-reports 4 Detailed Information Processing in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome: The Usefulness of Neuropsychological Tests and Self-reports Submitted Chapter 4 Abstract Detailed information processing in 42

More information

Learning Support for Students with High Functioning Autism in. Post-secondary Learning Communities. Jeanne L. Wiatr, Ed.D.

Learning Support for Students with High Functioning Autism in. Post-secondary Learning Communities. Jeanne L. Wiatr, Ed.D. Learning Support for Students with High Functioning Autism in Post-secondary Learning Communities Jeanne L. Wiatr, Ed.D. Collierville Teaching and Learning Consortium Author Note This is an article reviewing

More information

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Nart Bedin Atalay (natalay@selcuk.edu.tr) Selcuk University, Konya, TURKEY Mine Misirlisoy

More information

Living with Newton's Laws

Living with Newton's Laws Task #1 - Newton s 1 st Law - This is a pain in the neck Let's suppose you are in your car, waiting at a stop light. Like any good driver, you have your seat belt buckled. (It's the law.) Suddenly, a car

More information

Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments

Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments Robert W. Proctor & Motonori Yamaguchi Purdue University, W. Lafayette, Indiana MURI Grant W9112NF-05-1-0153 from

More information

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction Imitation and anticipation 1 It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction ACCEPTED FOR PUBLICATION IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Roland Pfister 1, David Dignath 1, Bernhard Hommel

More information

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45, 431 435 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 2012) COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO MARCUS

More information

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Laboratory Equipment & Test Procedures. Motor learning and control historically has built its discipline through laboratory research. This has led to the

More information

An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions

An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions Leigh-Ann Heale Classic autism was first described by Kanner in 1943 (cited in Ellis,

More information

Autism and other developmental disorders. Dr. Martin Maldonado

Autism and other developmental disorders. Dr. Martin Maldonado Autism and other developmental disorders Dr. Martin Maldonado Autistic Spectrum Autistic Disorder Autistic Psychopathy Childhood psychoses (Europe and Latin America) Multi-system developmental disorder

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making Matthew A J Apps 1,2, Laura Grima 2, Sanjay Manohar 2, Masud Husain 1,2 1 Nuffield Department of Clinical Neuroscience,

More information

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1 The attentional window configures to object boundaries University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 2 ABSTRACT When

More information

How To Optimize Your Training For i3 Mindware v.4 And Why 2G N-Back Brain Training Works

How To Optimize Your Training For i3 Mindware v.4 And Why 2G N-Back Brain Training Works How To Optimize Your Training For i3 Mindware v.4 And Why 2G N-Back Brain Training Works Mark Ashton Smith, Ph.D. CONTENTS I. KEY SCIENTIFIC BACKGROUND 3 II. PRACTICE: WHAT STRATEGIES? 10 III. SCHEDULING

More information

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King

Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Deconstructing the DSM-5 By Jason H. King Assessment and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder For this month s topic, I am excited to share my recent experience using the fifth edition of the Diagnostic

More information

Two Experiments on Co-located Mobile Groupware

Two Experiments on Co-located Mobile Groupware Two Experiments on Co-located Mobile Groupware Miguel A. Nacenta*, Dzimitri Aliakseyeu**, Tadeuz Stach*, Sriram Subramanian* and Carl Gutwin* *Computer Science Department University of Saskatchewan http://hci.usask.ca

More information

KPU Registry ID Number: 1005 Date revised, 5 th December 2014 (This revised version was submitted prior to any data collection)

KPU Registry ID Number: 1005 Date revised, 5 th December 2014 (This revised version was submitted prior to any data collection) KPU Registry ID Number: 1005 Date revised, 5 th December 2014 (This revised version was submitted prior to any data collection) Study Registration For the Koestler Parapsychology Unit Study Registry 1.

More information

Fostering Communication Skills in Preschool Children with Pivotal Response Training

Fostering Communication Skills in Preschool Children with Pivotal Response Training Fostering Communication Skills in Preschool Children with Pivotal Response Training Mary Mandeville-Chase, MS, CCC-SLP 1 Training Objectives 1. Participants will name two pivotal behaviors associated with

More information

Autism & intellectual disabilities. How to deal with confusing concepts

Autism & intellectual disabilities. How to deal with confusing concepts Autism & intellectual disabilities How to deal with confusing concepts dr. Gerard J. Nijhof Orthopedagogue / GZ-psychologist PhD, Free University Amsterdam Private practice contact@gerardnijhof.nl +31

More information

Chapter 7: Cognitive Aspects of Personality. Copyright Allyn & Bacon (2009)

Chapter 7: Cognitive Aspects of Personality. Copyright Allyn & Bacon (2009) Chapter 7: Cognitive Aspects of Personality Roots in Gestalt Psychology Human beings seek meaning in their environments We organize the sensations we receive into meaningful perceptions Complex stimuli

More information

Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism

Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism By Lauren Lowry Hanen Certified SLP and Clinical Staff Writer Several years ago I worked on a diagnostic assessment team. I remember the first

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

Supplementary Online Content 2

Supplementary Online Content 2 Supplementary Online Content 2 Bieleninik Ł, Geretsegger M, Mössler K, et al; TIME-A Study Team. Effects of improvisational music therapy vs enhanced standard care on symptom severity among children with

More information

STATISTICS AND RESEARCH DESIGN

STATISTICS AND RESEARCH DESIGN Statistics 1 STATISTICS AND RESEARCH DESIGN These are subjects that are frequently confused. Both subjects often evoke student anxiety and avoidance. To further complicate matters, both areas appear have

More information

PROGRAMMING FOR STUDENTS WITH ASD IN THE GENERAL EDUCATION SETTING

PROGRAMMING FOR STUDENTS WITH ASD IN THE GENERAL EDUCATION SETTING PROGRAMMING FOR STUDENTS WITH ASD IN THE GENERAL EDUCATION SETTING Middle School Point Person Training District 204 2010-2011 Pam Leonard & Sabrina Beaudry WELCOME! HOW DID WE GET HERE? SMART GOAL By the

More information

Running head: EFFECTS OF COLOR, CONGRUENCY AND INTERFERENCE

Running head: EFFECTS OF COLOR, CONGRUENCY AND INTERFERENCE Color, Congruency and Interference 1 Running head: EFFECTS OF COLOR, CONGRUENCY AND INTERFERENCE The Effects of Color, Context Congruency and Interference on Short Term Memory Brianna Raatz Hanover College

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Automaticity of Number Perception

Automaticity of Number Perception Automaticity of Number Perception Jessica M. Choplin (jessica.choplin@vanderbilt.edu) Gordon D. Logan (gordon.logan@vanderbilt.edu) Vanderbilt University Psychology Department 111 21 st Avenue South Nashville,

More information

All Things Considered. Autism Eligibility Revisited

All Things Considered. Autism Eligibility Revisited All Things Considered Autism Eligibility Revisited 1 What We ll Consider... Overview of Legal Standards and Framework for Eligibility Recognizing and Assessing Autism: Child Find, Need for Assessment and

More information