Checking Behavior, Fear of Recurrence, and Daily Triggers in Breast Cancer Survivors

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Checking Behavior, Fear of Recurrence, and Daily Triggers in Breast Cancer Survivors"

Transcription

1 ann. behav. med. (2018) XX:1 11 DOI: /abm/kay033 REGULAR ARTICLE Checking Behavior, Fear of Recurrence, and Daily Triggers in Breast Cancer Survivors Emily C. Soriano, MA 1 Rosmeiry Valera, BA 2,7 Elizabeth C. Pasipanodya, PhD 3 Amy K. Otto, MA 4 Scott D. Siegel, PhD 5 Jean-Philippe Laurenceau, PhD 6 Society of Behavioral Medicine All rights reserved. For permissions, please journals.permissions@oup.com. Abstract Background Fear of cancer recurrence (FCR) is a top ongoing concern of breast cancer (BC) survivors and thus the focus of recent intervention development. The Self-Regulation Model of FCR (Lee-Jones C, Humphris G, Dixon R, Hatcher MB. Fear of cancer recurrence a literature review and proposed cognitive formulation to explain exacerbation of recurrence fears. Psychooncology. 1997;6: ) states that everyday cancer-related events trigger FCR, which, in turn, leads to specific behavioral responses, including checking the body for signs or symptoms of cancer. Links between triggering events, FCR, and checking behavior have not yet been studied in the context of daily life or at the within-person level. Purpose The goal of this study was to examine whether FCR has a within-person link with daily checking Emily C. Soriano esoriano@psych.udel.edu Jean-Philippe Laurenceau jlaurenceau@psych.udel.edu 1 Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, 108 Wolf Hall, Newark, DE 19716, USA 2 Department of Psychology, Bloomfield College, Bloomfield, NJ, USA 3 Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA 4 Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA 5 Helen F. Graham Cancer Center & Research Institute, Newark, DE, USA 6 Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA 7 Present address: Department of Psychology, William Paterson University, Wayne, NJ, USA behavior and whether FCR mediates the link between triggering events and checking behavior. Methods Seventy-two early-stage BC survivors completed daily diaries over a 21-day period approximately 5 months after BC surgery. FCR, checking behavior, and triggering events were assessed each evening. Results Multilevel modeling results indicated that FCR predicted greater odds of same-day, but not next-day, checking behavior. We found that daily FCR significantly mediated the same-day effect of triggering events on checking behavior. These average within-person effects varied substantially between patients and were not explained by momentary negative affect. Conclusions Findings support the within-person relationship between triggering events, FCR, and checking behavior posited by guiding theory, and can inform FCR intervention development. Keywords Breast cancer Fear of cancer recurrence Survivorship Reassurance Checking behavior Breast self-examination Fear of cancer recurrence (FCR) is defined as fear, worry, or concern relating to the possibility that cancer will come back or progress [1]. Particularly for cancers with high survival rates, such as breast cancer (BC), FCR can become a permanent fixture in the lives of patients whose cancer has been treated successfully. For the 3.6 million BC survivors in the USA [2], the possibility of recurrence is often the top ongoing concern [3 5]. FCR can be best understood as a chronic rather than acute experience, as it has been shown to persist years following BC treatment [6, 7]. A growing literature points to numerous negative outcomes of FCR, including psychological distress [6, 8], worse quality of life [4, 9], and anxiety [6].

2 2 ann. behav. med. (2018) XX:1 11 The Self-regulation Model of FCR The most prominent theoretical framework for conceptualizing FCR is an adaptation of Leventhal s Self-Regulation Model of Illness (SRM [10]) by Lee-Jones and colleagues [11]. The SRM posits a network of processes, beginning with antecedents or triggers of FCR and ending with its consequences. Posited triggers of FCR include internal cues, such as experiencing pain and other physical symptoms, and external cues, such as annual cancer screenings and commercials for cancer treatment. The SRM points to both behavioral and psychological consequences of FCR, including checking for signs or symptoms of cancer and anxiety. It is important to note that some view these triggers and consequences as focal aspects of the clinical manifestation of FCR, such that clinical FCR is defined and measured, in part, by their presence [1, 12 14]. Although there is currently a lack of consensus as to the best way to define and measure both clinical and non-clinical FCR, we conceptualize triggers and consequences as important and related constructs, but distinct from FCR [12, 15, 16]. Rather, the central feature of FCR is viewed as thoughts and fears related to the possibility of recurrence. Psychological consequences (or correlates) of FCR are one well-studied aspect of the SRM framework [4]. However, there have been few empirical tests of the proximal triggers and behavioral consequences of FCR. In terms of the theorized triggers, studies have found crosssectional associations between FCR and internal triggers including pain, physical symptoms, fatigue, and changes in appearance (for a review, see [4, 17]). These studies indicate that patients who experience more physical symptoms, treatment side effects, or changes to their appearance tend to be the same patients who report greater FCR. Research on external triggers of FCR is less developed. Qualitative work suggests that common external triggers include doctor appointments, hearing about someone else having cancer, and annual mammograms [8, 18]. Behavioral consequences of FCR have also received little empirical attention. One theorized behavioral consequence is checking the body for signs or symptoms of cancer [11]. It is thought that when a patient experiences FCR, one way she may attempt to regulate her distress is by seeking confirmation that no new signs or symptoms have surfaced. When she gains this confirmation by checking her body, she may be reassured in the moment that her cancer has not progressed or recurred. Thus, theoretically, checking behavior temporarily attenuates the distress associated with FCR. For patients who experience FCR frequently, checking behavior may become excessive, problematic, and even compulsive. Over the past several decades, scholars have frequently described and drawn upon this theorized process [19 23]. In fact, excessive checking behavior is an explicit target of an intervention designed to reduce FCR [24]. Although checking behavior may attenuate FCR, excessive use of this strategy may lead to other negative outcomes. Guidelines from the American Society of Clinical Oncology recommend monthly breast self-examination for BC survivors [25] based on evidence that a greater frequency increases rates of invasive procedures resulting in benign findings, but does not improve rates of mortality [26]. Self-examination is not necessarily a certain or straightforward method for assessing signs or symptoms of cancer progression or recurrence, especially when performed in an emotionally aroused state [19, 27, 28]. Thus, checking in response to FCR may actually evoke more uncertainty about disease status or lead to unnecessary doctor appointments and procedures. Fear of Recurrence and Checking Behavior as Within-Person Phenomena The strong theoretical and intuitive interest in checking behavior does not currently correspond to its empirical support as a consequence of FCR. Only two quantitative studies, to our knowledge, have assessed FCR and checking behavior in BC patients. In a cross-sectional study, Thewes and colleagues [29] found that greater FCR was associated with more frequent self-reported breast self-examination. In another cross-sectional study, survivors who had more severe FCR reported more frequent use of checking as a strategy to cope with FCR [16]. In related research on healthy women with elevated BC risk, several studies have found positive links between BC worry and self-exam frequency [30, 31], although others have not [32]. Although preliminary support for the SRM is encouraging, it is exclusively based on cross-sectional data. Cross-sectional designs lend themselves well to the study of between-person associations, which describe the comparison of individuals to one another based on their global or average levels of a particular variable. For example, the cross-sectional study reviewed above suggests that patients with greater global FCR tend to report a higher frequency of breast self-examination than patients with lower global FCR [29]. However, typically also of interest are within-person associations, which describe the comparison of individuals to themselves (rather than to other individuals) based on their within-person variability captured in repeated measures. For example, is a patient more likely to report checking behavior on a day in which she has higher FCR compared with a day in which she has lower FCR? Longitudinal studies are required to assess both between- and within-person effects. Echoing Ryu, West, and Sousa [33], we argue that the within-person association between FCR and checking behavior is of substantive theoretical and clinical interest, and it is important to examine whether the between-person effects found previously extend to

3 ann. behav. med. (2018) XX: the within-person level. The presence of a between-person effect does not imply the presence of a within-person effect and vice versa; in fact, these two effects can differ in magnitude and even direction [34]. Although prior cross-sectional findings suggest that individuals with higher FCR tend to report checking more frequently, global and retrospective assessment may only distally capture this link. Global cross-sectional methods are vulnerable to retrospective biases [35] and do not rule out unmeasured between-person confounds or address the directionality of an association [36 38]. Behavioral consequences of FCR, including checking behavior, have not yet been examined prospectively and at the within-person level, despite the fact that checking behavior is targeted in FCR interventions. Moreover, FCR triggers hypothesized to initiate the SRM sequence, while well-studied cross-sectionally, have also not yet been examined at this level. Intensive longitudinal methods, which involve repeated measurements of individuals over time (e.g., daily diaries [37]), can provide valuable information that maps onto our theoretical and intuitive understanding of FCR. This approach can limit the retrospective biases associated with global self-report of behaviors or feelings over a longer span of time and provide a clearer picture of the processes flowing into and from FCR [35 38]. Furthermore, intensive longitudinal data offer the potential of identifying the directionality and sequence of effects [36, 37]. For example, one can examine whether a particular effect carries over from one assessment to the next, which could provide insight into the potency and duration of the effect. This level of insight can be easily translated and applied to the development and refinement of FCR interventions. The Present Study We examined the associations between triggering events, FCR, and checking behavior on a daily, within-person level, which has not been previously examined. Daily diary data from early-stage BC patients were used to accomplish several aims. Our first aim was to determine whether FCR was associated with checking behavior on the same day (concurrently). Based on the SRM, we hypothesized that patients who reported greater FCR on one day would be more likely to check for signs or symptoms of cancer on that same day. Note that a concurrent within-person association is not synonymous with a cross-sectional between-person association the former models within-person day-to-day variability while the latter does not. We also sought to explore the directionality of the association between FCR and checking behavior by examining carryover of the effect in both directions from one day to the next. No prior research has attempted to delineate the timeline for these effects. Furthermore, it has been suggested that checking behavior may reinforce and strengthen FCR, but this has not been examined empirically. Thus, an exploratory aim was to investigate whether FCR was associated with a greater likelihood of checking the next day, and vice versa. Finally, we examined the posited sequence of triggers predicting FCR, in turn, leading to checking behavior. Our second aim was to determine the extent to which the effect of daily triggers on checking behavior is mediated by FCR. Based on the SRM, we hypothesized that FCR would mediate the within-person association between triggers and checking behavior. To rule out the possibility that feelings of negativity could explain the hypothesized links among these variables, we included momentary negative affect as a within-person covariate in all tests. Method Participants Participants were female early-stage BC patients recruited from a community cancer center in the mid-atlantic region of the USA for a larger longitudinal study with Institutional Review Board approval. Patients were asked to participate if they met the following criteria: (a) diagnosed with Stage 0 (lobular/ductal carcinoma in situ), I, II, or IIIA BC; (b) had recent BC surgery; (c) in a committed romantic relationship with a partner who also agreed to participate; (d) English-speaking; (e) lived within an hour of the recruitment site; and (f) did not have prior cancer diagnoses. Patients who had a recent positive breast biopsy and appeared eligible per electronic medical records were sent a letter about the study and contacted by phone. Note that although the larger study obtained data from patients and their partners, only patient data are examined here. Of the 1,161 patients who had a recent positive breast biopsy, 698 were identified via medical records as ineligible. The 463 potentially eligible patients were mailed a letter and contacted by phone. Of those who were contacted, 117 verbally agreed to participate and were mailed a consent form, 110 were ineligible based on patient-provided information (78% ineligible due to being unpartnered), 82 could not be reached, and 154 declined to participate (most frequent reasons were not enough time and spouse does not wish to participate ). Seventy-nine patients provided informed consent and participated, 4 provided consent but did not participate, and 34 did not return the consent form. This article examined one assessment period of the larger longitudinal study (discussed in more detail below), to which 72 patients contributed data, resulting in a final sample of 72 patients described here.

4 4 ann. behav. med. (2018) XX:1 11 Approximately 89% of patients identified as Caucasian, 10% as African American, 1% as Asian, and all as non-hispanic. The average age was years (SD = 9.48) and 94% were married. In terms of occupational status, about 43% reported not working, 39% reported working full time, and 18% reported working part time. Most (73%) reported an annual household income over $60,000. Fifteen percent of patients were diagnosed with Stage 0 BC, 49% Stage IA, 24% Stage IIA, 11% Stage IIB, and 1% Stage IIIA. The majority of patients did not receive adjuvant chemotherapy (67%) but did receive adjuvant radiation (71%) and hormonal therapy (81%). Procedure Patients were recruited around the start of adjuvant treatment. After providing informed consent, patients completed a series of baseline questionnaires. The baseline assessment took place an average of 3 months after surgery (SD = 1.56, range = 1 8). Data for the current study came from a daily diary period that took place after the baseline assessment, as soon as possible after adjuvant treatment. This time period was targeted for study based on evidence suggesting that FCR emerges soon after the end of adjuvant treatment, when patients see their health care providers less frequently and often feel as though they are no longer actively fighting the cancer [39, 40]. Patients who did not receive adjuvant chemotherapy or radiotherapy completed the diary period as soon as possible after the baseline assessment. Patients started the diary period an average of 5 months after surgery (SD = 2.09, range = 2 12). The daily diary period took place over 21 consecutive days. During this period, patients were asked to complete a short survey every evening within about an hour of going to sleep. Before the diary period began, patients were provided with a web link to access this survey from their home each evening. The evening survey was designed to be brief and took patients 13 min on average to complete. Because several items asked patients about the events of their day, compliance with the requested completion time was monitored via the survey software. Data were only included in analyses if the survey was completed between 6:00 p.m. and 3:00 a.m. On average, patients completed the surveys on 17 out of the 21 days (81% compliance rate). Measures Daily FCR Six items from the Fear of Cancer Recurrence Inventory (FCRI [8]), which were among the highest-loading items in past analytic work [8], were adapted for daily use. One item (from the FCRI severity subscale) assessed how much time patients spent thinking about the possibility of cancer recurrence that day, with responses ranging from 0 (didn t think about it at all) to 4 (several hours). Four items (from the distress subscale) assessed negative emotional reactions (worried, afraid, or anxious; sad, discouraged, or disappointed; frustrated, angry, or outraged; helpless or resigned) related to the possibility of recurrence. The fifth item, drawn from the insight subscale, assessed the extent to which one felt that he/she worried excessively about the possibility of recurrence. The latter five items were rated from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). The six items were summed to create a daily FCR composite, which had strong within-person reliability (ω =.91). The average daily FCR score was 2.09 (within-person SD = 2.53, between-person SD = 2.20). FCR was positively skewed with a large proportion (57.8%) of zero scores. As a result, FCR was modeled as a multilevel count outcome with a Poisson distribution in all analyses. Daily triggers Eight daily triggers were assessed. As described earlier, although some include triggers in their measurement of overall FCR, we conceptualize these as distinct constructs. In the absence of a previously validated daily measure, these items were developed through consultation with a clinical psycho-oncologist and a nurse navigator specializing in cancer survivorship needs. Several items overlapped with items from the FCRI triggers subscale [8]. Patients indicated whether each trigger occurred that day. The following five internal triggers were assessed: noticed skin irritation, tingling or loss of sensation in hands or feet, had swelling or trouble moving the arm, cognitive difficulties (e.g., issues with memory, everyday decision making, focusing, finding words), and noticed changes in physical appearance (e.g., scars, hair loss, change in complexion). Three external triggers were assessed: had a long wait in doctor s office, received negative news from physician, and frustrating interaction with medical professional. The eight items were summed to represent the number of triggers experienced daily. The average number of daily triggers was 0.86 (within-person SD = 0.76, between-person SD = 0.84). Although it could be argued that internal and external triggers should be examined separately, the current sample size, limited number of items, and relatively low frequency of triggers led to the decision to utilize a single trigger composite. Daily checking behavior One item assessed whether patients engaged in daily checking behavior: Today, did you examine yourself physically for signs or symptoms of cancer? Patients responded yes or no. On average, patients reported checking on 19% of the diary days (within-person SD = 0.32, between-person SD = 0.23).

5 ann. behav. med. (2018) XX: Momentary negative affect Momentary negative affect was measured using seven standard items from the negative affect subscale of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Expanded Form [41], which were averaged to create a composite. Only seven negative affect items (sad, angry, afraid, lonely, blue, scared, and frightened) were included to keep the daily diaries as brief as possible. Patients indicated the extent they experienced each item at this moment from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). Coefficient omega estimated acceptable within-person reliability of the momentary negative affect scale (ω =.79). The average momentary negative affect score was 0.17 (within-person SD = 0.26, between-person SD = 0.24). Results Data Analytic Approach See Supplementary Material 1 for additional descriptive statistics and correlations. Multilevel modeling was used to accommodate the nested structure of the daily diary data (i.e., days nested within patients). Multilevel analyses were conducted in R (version 3.3.2; R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) using the lme4 package (version [42]). The linear effect of time and momentary negative affect were both included as within-person covariates in all models (note that the exclusion of negative affect as a covariate resulted in the same pattern of findings for all analyses). Time is included as a covariate to control for autocorrelation in Level 1 residuals as well as any systematic increase or decrease in the outcome over the diary period (e.g., as a result of reactivity to study procedures [37]). As mentioned earlier, since, by definition, person-level (i.e., between-person) factors, such as global or baseline levels of FCR severity, cannot confound within-person relationships, such factors need not be included as covariates in these models. A random intercept was estimated in each model, allowing for person-to-person variability in levels of the outcome. Random slope effects for focal predictors were estimated when possible. Timevarying predictors were person-mean centered and person-level predictors were grand-mean centered [37]. The multilevel model provides valid inferences assuming data are missing at random [43]. To address our first aim, we examined the concurrent daily association between FCR and checking behavior (a binary outcome variable) by estimating a multilevel logistic regression model. Daily FCR was specified as a predictor of daily checking behavior (the outcome variable), for which a random slope effect was estimated. In addition to the within-person effect of interest, we estimated the between-person effect by including average FCR over the diary period as a person-level predictor. To explore the directionality of the association between FCR and checking behavior, we examined whether FCR predicted not only concurrent checking behavior, but also future checking behavior, and vice versa. An additional set of models were estimated in which the outcome was set one day after the predictor. First, same-day FCR was specified as a predictor of next-day checking behavior (the outcome variable), while controlling for same-day checking behavior. We then specified same-day checking behavior as a predictor of next-day FCR (the outcome variable), while controlling for same-day FCR. A random slope effect for the focal predictor of each model (i.e., FCR or checking behavior) was also estimated. For our second aim, we conducted a multilevel mediation analysis [44 46], in which the predictor (triggering events), mediator (FCR), and outcome (checking behavior) were all time-varying variables (i.e., measured daily). The hypothesized mediation model is depicted in Fig. 1. Because FCR was modeled as a count outcome and checking behavior as a binary outcome, the traditional approach to mediation analysis could not be used the standard test of the mediated effect (i.e., the product of the indirect paths [47]) would produce a biased estimate [48]. To obtain a valid estimate of the indirect effect for our model, we used a more general approach to mediation analysis [48] based on the counterfactual, or potential outcomes, framework [49, 50]. This more general approach to mediation analysis accommodates conditions that traditional approaches do not, including noncontinuous outcome and mediator distributions (e.g., here, checking behavior is a binary outcome modeled using multilevel logistic regression [51]). The mediation package in R was used to test the hypothesized mediated effect and obtain valid estimates of the average direct, mediated, and total effects in the multilevel context (v4.4.5 [52]). For readers interested in a detailed description of this approach and how it differs from traditional mediational analysis, see Supplementary Material 2 and [48, 49, 51, 52]. Fig. 1. Hypothesized within-person multilevel mediation model. The a path was estimated by regressing fear of cancer recurrence (FCR) on daily triggers, controlling for momentary negative affect, average triggers, and time, with a random slope estimated for daily triggers. The b and c paths were estimated by regressing checking behavior on daily FCR and daily triggers. Although not depicted, we controlled for momentary negative affect, average FCR, average triggers, and time.

6 6 ann. behav. med. (2018) XX:1 11 Daily Link Between FCR and Checking Behavior The results of our first aim (determine whether FCR was associated with same-day checking behavior) are displayed in Table 1. FCR emerged as a significant within-person predictor of checking behavior, indicating that when a patient experiences a one-unit increase in FCR (compared with what is typical for her), the odds are 32% greater that she will check herself for signs or symptoms of cancer the same day. The random effect variance of this slope was 0.03 (SD = 0.18), indicating that the slopes of about 95% of patients fell between 0.07 (odds ratio [OR] = 0.93) and 0.64 (OR = 1.89). Thus, for some patients, the effect of FCR on checking behavior approached zero, while for others, the effect was greater than the average effect. In addition, average FCR over the diary period was a significant predictor of average checking behavior (γ = 0.37, OR = 1.45, z = 3.18, p =.002). This between-person effect indicates that patients who tend toward higher average daily FCR also tend toward more frequent checking. Prospective Link Between FCR and Checking Behavior Results of our exploratory aim (determine whether FCR was associated with greater odds of checking the next day, and vice versa) are shown in Table 1. First, we did not find evidence that FCR predicted next-day checking behavior on average (OR = 1.04, p =.473). Thus, our results suggest that FCR is a strong predictor of sameday checking behavior, but does not have a detectable effect on next-day checking behavior (above and beyond its same-day effect). The FCR random slope effect estimated a variance of 0.03 (SD = 0.17), indicating that 95% of individual patient slopes in the population fall between 0.29 (OR = 0.75) and 0.38 (OR = 1.46). Second, results suggested a larger but nonsignificant average effect of checking behavior on next-day FCR (rate ratio [RR] = 1.37, p =.112). When a patient checked, regardless of her FCR score that day, she was predicted to have a 1.37 times greater FCR score the next day. The random slope of checking behavior also pointed to substantial variability (variance = 0.73, SD = 0.85), indicating that 95% of patient slopes fall between 1.40 (RR = 0.25) and 2.02 (RR = 7.53). Within-Person Mediation Model The results of the tests of the hypothesized mediation model (depicted in Fig. 1) are displayed in Table 2. The a path was estimated by regressing FCR on daily triggers, momentary negative affect, average triggers, and time, with a random slope effect of daily triggers also estimated. As expected, triggering events were a significant and positive predictor of same-day FCR. Specifically, on a day a patient experienced one additional triggering event (compared with her average number of daily triggers), she was predicted to have a 1.51 times greater FCR score that same-day. The random effect variance of this fixed effect was 0.14 (SD = 0.37), indicating that about 95% of patient slopes in the population fall between 0.32 (RR = 0.72) and 1.15 (RR = 3.16). Average number of triggers over the diary period also significantly predicted higher average FCR, γ = 0.71, RR = 2.04, z = 3.76, p <.001. Table 1 Multilevel regression results of concurrent and prospective link between FCR and checking behavior 95% CI Effect Estimate SE Odds/rate ratio p Lower Upper Concurrent link: FCR Same-day checking behavior a FCR < Negative affect Time Prospective link: FCR Next-day checking behavior a FCR Negative affect Time Prospective link: Checking behavior Next-day FCR b Checking behavior Negative affect < Time While not shown, same-day level of the outcome was also included as a covariate in prospective models. FCR = daily fear of cancer recurrence. a Poisson regression estimates are exponentiated to obtain rate ratios. b Logistic regression estimates are exponentiated to obtain odds ratios.

7 ann. behav. med. (2018) XX: Table 2 Results of within-person multilevel mediation: FCR triggering events FCR checking behavior 95% CI Effect Estimate SE Odds/rate ratio p Lower Upper Mediator model: FCR triggers FCR a FCR triggers (a path) < Negative affect < Time < Outcome model: Triggers and FCR Checking behavior b FCR (b path) < FCR triggers (c path) Negative affect Time Mediation effects c Average mediated effect 0.02 < Average direct effect Total effect Proportion mediated effect FCR = daily fear of cancer recurrence. a Estimates from Poisson regression are exponentiated to obtain rate ratios. b Estimates from logistic regression are exponentiated to obtain odds ratios. c Mediation effect estimates are displayed in probability units. Next, we tested the b and c paths by regressing checking behavior on daily FCR, daily triggers, momentary negative affect, average FCR, average triggers, and time. Results are shown in Table 2. No random slope effects were able to be estimated in this model due to nonconvergence. Because we were unable to estimate a random effect for this slope using lme4, we also used Bayesian and generalized estimating equation (GEE) methods to confirm that the focal fixed effect results remained after estimating random slope effects (Bayesian) or correcting standard errors (GEE). These fixed effect results remained statistically significant. Consistent with the results of the first aim, results revealed a significant positive effect of FCR on checking behavior (b path). However, the effect of triggers on checking behavior was not statistically significant (c path). The R mediation package uses the results of these models to estimate the mediated effects (provided in probability units; shown in Table 2). The average mediated causal effect was 0.02, indicating that when a patient experiences one more trigger relative to what is typical for her, there is a 2% increase in the probability of her checking on the same day that is due solely to increases in FCR. The average direct effect was 0.01, suggesting that a one-unit increase in daily triggers results in a 1% increase in the probability that the patient will exhibit checking behavior that is not explained by increases in FCR. The average proportion mediated effect is 0.66, indicating that 66% of the total effect of triggers on checking behavior is explained by FCR. Discussion Although a growing literature points to FCR as a critical component of BC adjustment, we are aware of no published work examining antecedents and consequences of FCR within the context of daily life. The present study examined daily checking behavior, a consequence of FCR according to the prominent SRM [11] and a target of FCR interventions [24]. Checking behavior has long been theorized to function as a distress-reducing strategy and resemble a compulsion in patients with very high FCR [11, 19 23]. Surprisingly, this link has only been directly examined in one cross-sectional study [29]. Despite the paucity of empirical support, emerging FCR interventions draw heavily on the SRM and explicitly target excessive checking behavior [24]. Using daily diary data from BC survivors, the current study addresses this gap by examining the proximal (FCR) and distal (FCRtriggering events) predictors of daily checking behavior proposed by the SRM. The current study provides the first test of the within-person processes outlined in the SRM and targeted in FCR interventions but not captured in the existing literature, which is based almost exclusively on global and cross-sectional assessments [16]. Results provided support for our hypothesis that patients who experience greater FCR on one day would be more likely to report checking their bodies for signs or symptoms of cancer. We found that on a day that a patient reports a one-unit increase in FCR, the odds are 32% greater that she will check for signs or symptoms of cancer

8 8 ann. behav. med. (2018) XX:1 11 the same-day. Moreover, this effect was not explained by momentary negative affect during the evening survey. Although these results are based on concurrent data and do not permit inferences about the causal nature of this relationship, they complement prior cross-sectional findings by indicating the presence of a significant within-person association between FCR and checking behavior, which has not been demonstrated previously. Furthermore, our approach minimizes biases associated with typical crosssectional studies that require patients to retrospectively report on far longer periods of time (e.g., past month or year). In addition, person-level (i.e., between-person) factors, such as global or baseline FCR severity, cannot confound within-person associations between FCR and checking behavior; therefore, any stable factors that vary only from person to person are not plausible alternative explanations for the current within-person findings. We also examined the extent to which the association between FCR and checking behavior holds across different patients, which has not been described previously. The significant fixed effect was found to vary between patients, such that for some, the estimated effect was near zero, while for others, it was over twice that of the average fixed effect. These results point to potential moderating variables that attenuate or intensify the effect of FCR on checking behavior. Although the current study was not designed or sufficiently powered to examine moderators of this effect, future studies should investigate variables that may explain why some patients engage in checking behavior when they experience FCR and others do not. Given the lack of empirical or theoretical work on the timescale of the SRM, we explored carryover effects of FCR on checking behavior the next day. We did not find evidence of an effect of FCR on next-day checking behavior (controlling for same-day checking behavior). This finding does not necessarily support or conflict with the hypothesis that FCR plays a causal role, but does suggest that future studies must implement more frequent assessments to determine the directionality and temporal sequence of these momentary experiences. Intuitively, it seems likely that the effect of FCR on checking behavior occurs within a shorter time frame than one day, making it impossible for a daily diary design to capture. Importantly, there was evidence of variability in this effect, such that greater FCR on one day predicted increased odds of checking the next day for some patients and decreased odds for others. It will be important to investigate moderators that explain these individual differences in future research. Scholars have suggested that checking behavior may maintain or exacerbate FCR [11, 19 23]. We explored this by examining whether checking behavior predicted nextday FCR, controlling for FCR and momentary negative affect the previous day. Although this effect was not statistically significant, there was substantial variability in this effect, such that for some it was strongly negative and others strongly positive. Although checking behavior has been conceptualized as an FCR-reinforcing compulsion, more research is required to determine for whom and under what conditions this rings true. Again, more frequent assessments are likely needed to clarify the sequence and cycle of FCR and checking behavior over time. Finally, we tested a multilevel within-person mediation model representing the full sequence delineated in the SRM triggering events predict FCR, which, in turn, predicts checking behavior. The results supported this mediation model, indicating that the effect of daily triggers on checking behavior was significantly mediated by daily FCR. The direct effect of daily triggers on checking behavior, not explained by FCR, was not statistically significant. These results support the antecedents, FCR, and consequences posited by the SRM at the same momentary, within-person level that inherently defines the model. Furthermore, the methods employed minimize retrospective bias and eliminate the possibility of person-level confounding variables. Taken together, the results and methodological strengths of the current study provide strong support for the SRM that has not been previously documented. Limitations and Future Directions We attempted to address directionality and temporal precedence in the association between FCR and checking behavior by examining the effects from one day to the next. Because we did not find strong support for these next-day carryover effects, it is possible that checking behavior temporally precedes FCR. Similarly, we cannot rule out the possibility that checking behavior leads to increased sensitivity or awareness of daily triggering events. As described above, it seems plausible that the lack of strong evidence of temporal precedence resulted from an incongruence between the timeframe of assessments and unfolding of effects. It is possible to engage in several acts of checking behavior within a day, which, furthermore, may occur with or without conscious awareness. Future studies should be designed to capture these processes that may unfold over minutes or hours. Our use of intensive longitudinal methodology allowed us to quantify heterogeneity in the within-person effects among BC patients. Although this study was not designed to explain this variability, our results suggest that this is a promising avenue for future research. It will be important to understand the individual characteristics and contextual factors that attenuate or intensify the effect of FCR on checking behavior. For example, patients who perceive themselves at higher risk of recurrence may be more likely to actively check for signs of recurrence [53]. Trait anxiety may also differentiate the patient who has this behavioral response to FCR from the patient who does not [22, 54, 55].

9 ann. behav. med. (2018) XX: Patients who use adaptive coping strategies, such as disclosing concerns about recurrence to a supportive and responsive loved one, may be less likely to engage in checking behavior. Furthermore, if checking behavior resembles a compulsion, then one may expect for those who check more frequently to exhibit a stronger daily link between FCR and checking behavior. Finally, variability in the association between triggering events and FCR suggests individual differences in reactivity to certain events. For example, patients with higher trait anxiety may be more reactive to events. Relatedly, although our daily triggers measure was based on the widely used and validated FCRI [8], given the paucity of prior studies of FCR in daily life, it is unlikely to capture all possible daily triggers. Future studies with large samples are required to investigate whether between-person factors, such as trait anxiety, moderate the within-person associations between these variables. It is possible that insufficient power contributed to the null finding for checking behavior on next-day FCR. A larger and more diverse sample may be necessary to determine whether checking behavior functions as a FCR-reinforcing compulsion. Patients in the current sample were largely high-functioning and typically reported low levels of daily FCR. The nature of the associations under study may differ across a range of FCR severity, and our findings may not generalize to those suffering from very high levels of FCR. At the same time, our findings highlight the importance of examining the normative experience and dimensional nature of FCR, as they revealed meaningful and theory-driven associations on a day-to-day basis between FCR, triggering events, and checking behavior in our sample. Nonetheless, future work should address these limitations because understanding the potential harm of checking behavior has important implications for mental and physical health care. Ideally, future research will define the bounds between psychologically adaptive and maladaptive checking behavior. In the absence of such work, an empirically-based definition of excessive checking behavior from a psychological standpoint is unavailable. Thus, our current conceptualization of excessive checking behavior relies on the medical literature, which shows that checking more than monthly is maladaptive for physical health [26]. However, FCR interventions that target checking behavior will be more effective if guided by empirical support for the distinction between psychologically adaptive and maladaptive checking behavior, and how and for whom this behavior perpetuates a cyclical process. Such work will also inform the guidance and education about self-examination provided to patients. Little is known about other potential outcomes of checking behavior. The excessive checker may be more vulnerable to false positives that result in unnecessary health care utilization [26]. Theory also points to health care utilization as another consequence of FCR itself, although extant research is limited. The few studies that have examined FCR and health care utilization suggest inconsistent associations across different forms of health care [29, 56, 57]. For example, Thewes and colleagues [29] found that FCR was associated with more frequent self-checking behavior but less frequent formal screenings (e.g., mammograms). Another study found that FCR predicted more outpatient and emergency room visits, but not specialist visits or other health care appointments [56]. More research is needed to gauge the extent and domains of the consequences of FCR, as well as the associations among the consequences themselves for example, does FCR lead to health care utilization in the absence of excessive checking behavior? Finally, the current sample was fairly homogeneous in terms of racial/ethnic makeup (mostly Caucasian) and income (relatively high on average). These socio-cultural characteristics relate to the low levels of distress observed in this sample, which limit the generalizability of our findings to patients with greater distress and impairment, as noted above. In addition, culture plays a role in the nature of adaptation to cancer [58], and therefore, the reported associations between FCR, triggering events, and checking behavior may not generalize to other socio-cultural groups. More cross-cultural research on FCR and cross-culturally validated measures are sorely needed [59]. The associations studied here must be tested in samples characterized by greater distress and impairment, as well as greater socio-cultural diversity. In addition, patients with prior cancer diagnoses were excluded from the study. Therefore, future research is necessary to determine whether the antecedents and consequences of FCR posited by the SRM hold for patients with longer and more complex histories of cancer-related experiences. Conclusion The SRM is a widely used framework for the conceptualization of FCR and development of FCR interventions. This was the first study to move beyond global and cross-sectional tests of the model and examine the daily within-person processes that are inherent in the framework. Our findings support the posited sequence of triggering events, FCR, and checking behavior. These findings have potential implications for health care utilization, self-checking instruction for BC patients, and FCR intervention development. Supplementary Material Supplementary material is available at Annals of Behavioral Medicine online.

10 10 ann. behav. med. (2018) XX:1 11 Acknowledgments Work presented in this manuscript was generously supported by the National Cancer Institute (grant number R21CA ). Compliance with Ethical Standards Authors Statement of Conflict of Interest and Adherence to Ethical Standards The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. Authors' Contributions E. C. Soriano wrote the manuscript with input from all authors. E. C. Soriano and J. P. Laurenceau conducted the analyses. E. C. Soriano, R. Valera, E. C. Pasipanodya, and J. P. Laurenceau conceptualized the paper. E. C. Soriano, E. C. Pasipanodya, and A. K. Otto collected data. S. D. Siegel and J. P. Laurenceau designed the parent study. Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. References 1. Lebel S, Ozakinci G, Humphris G, et al.; University of Ottawa Fear of Cancer Recurrence Colloquium attendees. From normal response to clinical problem: Definition and clinical features of fear of cancer recurrence. Support Care Cancer. 2016;24(8): Miller KD, Siegel RL, Lin CC, et al. Cancer treatment and survivorship statistics, CA Cancer J Clin. 2016;66(4): Baker F, Denniston M, Smith T, West MM. Adult cancer survivors: How are they faring? Cancer. 2005;104(S11): Simard S, Thewes B, Humphris G, et al. Fear of cancer recurrence in adult cancer survivors: A systematic review of quantitative studies. J Cancer Surviv. 2013;7(3): Vickberg SM. The Concerns About Recurrence Scale (CARS): A systematic measure of women s fears about the possibility of breast cancer recurrence. Ann Behav Med. 2003;25(1): Deimling GT, Bowman KF, Sterns S, Wagner LJ, Kahana B. Cancer-related health worries and psychological distress among older adult, long-term cancer survivors. Psychooncology. 2006;15(4): Mellon S, Kershaw TS, Northouse LL, Freeman-Gibb L. A family-based model to predict fear of recurrence for cancer survivors and their caregivers. Psychooncology. 2007;16(3): Simard S, Savard J. Fear of cancer recurrence inventory: Development and initial validation of a multidimensional measure of fear of cancer recurrence. Support Care Cancer. 2009;17(3): Mehnert A, Berg P, Henrich G, Herschbach P. Fear of cancer progression and cancer-related intrusive cognitions in breast cancer survivors. Psychooncology. 2009;18(12): Leventhal H, Diefenbach M, Leventhal EA. Illness cognition: Using common sense to understand treatment adherence and affect cognition interactions. Cogn Ther Res. 1992;16(2): Lee-Jones C, Humphris G, Dixon R, Hatcher MB. Fear of cancer recurrence a literature review and proposed cognitive formulation to explain exacerbation of recurrence fears. Psychooncology. 1997;6: Mutsaers B, Jones G, Rutkowski N, et al. When fear of cancer recurrence becomes a clinical issue: A qualitative analysis of features associated with clinical fear of cancer recurrence. Support Care Cancer. 2016;24(10): Sharpe L, Thewes B, Butow P. Current directions in research and treatment of fear of cancer recurrence. Curr Opin Support Palliat Care. 2017;11(3): Thewes B, Lebel S, Seguin Leclair C, Butow P. A qualitative exploration of fear of cancer recurrence (FCR) amongst Australian and Canadian breast cancer survivors. Support Care Cancer. 2016;24(5): Costa DS, Smith AB, Fardell JE. The sum of all fears: Conceptual challenges with measuring fear of cancer recurrence. Support Care Cancer. 2016;24(1): Custers JAE, Gielissen MFM, de Wilt JHW, et al. Towards an evidence-based model of fear of cancer recurrence for breast cancer survivors. J Cancer Surviv. 2017;11(1): Crist JV, Grunfeld EA. Factors reported to influence fear of recurrence in cancer patients: A systematic review. Psychooncology. 2013;22(5): Gill KM, Mishel M, Belyea M, et al. Triggers of uncertainty about recurrence and long-term treatment side effects in older African American and Caucasian breast cancer survivors. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2004;31(3): Fardell JE, Thewes B, Turner J, et al. Fear of cancer recurrence: A theoretical review and novel cognitive processing formulation. J Cancer Surviv. 2016;10(4): Ghazali N, Cadwallader E, Lowe D, Humphris G, Ozakinci G, Rogers SN. Fear of recurrence among head and neck cancer survivors: Longitudinal trends. Psychooncology. 2013;22(4): Lasry JC, Margolese RG. Fear of recurrence, breast-conserving surgery, and the trade-off hypothesis. Cancer. 1992;69(8): Stark DP, House A. Anxiety in cancer patients. Br J Cancer. 2000;83(10): Ziner KW, Sledge GW, Bell CJ, Johns S, Miller KD, Champion VL. Predicting fear of breast cancer recurrence and self-efficacy in survivors by age at diagnosis. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2012;39(3): Humphris G, Ozakinci G. The AFTER intervention: A structured psychological approach to reduce fears of recurrence in patients with head and neck cancer. Br J Health Psychol. 2008;13(2): Khatcheressian JL, Hurley P, Bantug E, et al.; American Society of Clinical Oncology. Breast cancer follow-up and management after primary treatment: American Society of Clinical Oncology clinical practice guideline update. J Clin Oncol. 2013;31(7): Thomas DB, Gao DL, Ray RM, et al. Randomized trial of breast self-examination in Shanghai: Final results. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2002;94(19): Miller SM. Monitoring versus blunting styles of coping with cancer influence the information patients want and need about their disease. Implications for cancer screening and management. Cancer. 1995;76(2): Taylor C, Richardson A, Cowley S. Surviving cancer treatment: An investigation of the experience of fear about, and monitoring for, recurrence in patients following treatment for colorectal cancer. Eur J Oncol Nurs. 2011;15(3):

FEAR OF CANCER RECURRENCE, CHECKING BEHAVIOR, AND TRIGGERING EVENTS IN THE DAILY LIVES OF BREAST CANCER SURVIVORS. Emily C.

FEAR OF CANCER RECURRENCE, CHECKING BEHAVIOR, AND TRIGGERING EVENTS IN THE DAILY LIVES OF BREAST CANCER SURVIVORS. Emily C. FEAR OF CANCER RECURRENCE, CHECKING BEHAVIOR, AND TRIGGERING EVENTS IN THE DAILY LIVES OF BREAST CANCER SURVIVORS by Emily C. Soriano A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the University of Delaware in

More information

Available from Deakin Research Online:

Available from Deakin Research Online: This is the published version: Richardson, Ben and Fuller Tyszkiewicz, Matthew 2014, The application of non linear multilevel models to experience sampling data, European health psychologist, vol. 16,

More information

Current Directions in Mediation Analysis David P. MacKinnon 1 and Amanda J. Fairchild 2

Current Directions in Mediation Analysis David P. MacKinnon 1 and Amanda J. Fairchild 2 CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Current Directions in Mediation Analysis David P. MacKinnon 1 and Amanda J. Fairchild 2 1 Arizona State University and 2 University of South Carolina ABSTRACT

More information

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Risk factors for the development and outcome of childhood psychopathology SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Chapter 147 In this chapter I present a summary of the results of the studies described in this thesis followed

More information

Changes Over Time in Occurrence, Severity, and Distress of Common Symptoms During and After Radiation Therapy for Breast Cancer

Changes Over Time in Occurrence, Severity, and Distress of Common Symptoms During and After Radiation Therapy for Breast Cancer 98 Journal of Pain and Symptom Management Vol. 45 No. June Original Article Changes Over Time in Occurrence, Severity, and Distress of Common Symptoms During and After Radiation Therapy for Breast Cancer

More information

Prof Marion Eckert Rosemary Bryant AO Research Centre

Prof Marion Eckert Rosemary Bryant AO Research Centre Willingness of cancer survivors to complete patient reported outcomes (PRO) surveys: a pilot study at Flinders Centre for Innovation in Cancer (FCIC), South Australia Prof Marion Eckert Rosemary Bryant

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

FCR work at St Andrews

FCR work at St Andrews FCR work at St Andrews Gerry Humphris 1, 2 21 st January 2019 1 2 Acknowledgements Participants Breast Cancer Now Chief Scientist Office (Scot Gov) Post docs: Mara van Beusekom Calum McHale Colleagues:

More information

Enhancing Quality of Life for Cancer Survivors in South Dakota. Outcomes from the South Dakota Cancer Survivorship Program

Enhancing Quality of Life for Cancer Survivors in South Dakota. Outcomes from the South Dakota Cancer Survivorship Program Enhancing Quality of Life for Cancer Survivors in South Dakota Outcomes from the South Dakota Cancer Survivorship Program The South Dakota Survivorship Program was funded through cooperative agreement

More information

Dispositional and Situational Avoidance and Approach as Predictors of Physical Symptom Bother Following Breast Cancer Diagnosis

Dispositional and Situational Avoidance and Approach as Predictors of Physical Symptom Bother Following Breast Cancer Diagnosis ann. behav. med. (2016) 50:370 384 DOI 10.1007/s12160-015-9763-7 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Dispositional and Situational Avoidance and Approach as Predictors of Physical Symptom Bother Following Breast Cancer Diagnosis

More information

Objective: To describe a new approach to neighborhood effects studies based on residential mobility and demonstrate this approach in the context of

Objective: To describe a new approach to neighborhood effects studies based on residential mobility and demonstrate this approach in the context of Objective: To describe a new approach to neighborhood effects studies based on residential mobility and demonstrate this approach in the context of neighborhood deprivation and preterm birth. Key Points:

More information

Addressing relationships following a breast cancer diagnosis: The impact on partners, children, and caregivers

Addressing relationships following a breast cancer diagnosis: The impact on partners, children, and caregivers Addressing relationships following a breast cancer diagnosis: The impact on partners, children, and caregivers Shoshana M. Rosenberg, ScD, MPH Dana-Farber Cancer Institute April 5, 2018 Why is it important

More information

CHAMP: CHecklist for the Appraisal of Moderators and Predictors

CHAMP: CHecklist for the Appraisal of Moderators and Predictors CHAMP - Page 1 of 13 CHAMP: CHecklist for the Appraisal of Moderators and Predictors About the checklist In this document, a CHecklist for the Appraisal of Moderators and Predictors (CHAMP) is presented.

More information

The Current Landscape of Nurse Navigators: Oncology and the Impact on Outcomes

The Current Landscape of Nurse Navigators: Oncology and the Impact on Outcomes The Current Landscape of Nurse Navigators: Oncology and the Impact on Outcomes SHERYL RILEY RN, OCN, CMCN DIRECTOR OF CLINICAL SERVICES SAI SYSTEMS SRILEY@SAISYSTEMS.COM 2015 SAI SYSTEMS INTERNATIONAL

More information

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX 2 Web Appendix A: Panel data estimation approach As noted in the main

More information

A RANDOMISED CONTROLLED TRIAL OF CONQUER FEAR DELIVERED IN A GROUP FORMAT

A RANDOMISED CONTROLLED TRIAL OF CONQUER FEAR DELIVERED IN A GROUP FORMAT A RANDOMISED CONTROLLED TRIAL OF CONQUER FEAR DELIVERED IN A GROUP FORMAT Nina Moeller Tauber, MSc. in psychology and Ph.D. fellow 1, Professor Robert Bobby Zachariae 1, Associate Professor Mia Skytte

More information

PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH

PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH DANIEL Z. LEVIN Management and Global Business Dept. Rutgers Business School Newark and New Brunswick Rutgers

More information

Implicit Theories Relate to Youth Psychopathology, But How? A Longitudinal Test of Two Predictive Models

Implicit Theories Relate to Youth Psychopathology, But How? A Longitudinal Test of Two Predictive Models Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (2016) 47:603 617 DOI 10.1007/s10578-015-0595-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Implicit Theories Relate to Youth Psychopathology, But How? A Longitudinal Test of Two Predictive Models Jessica

More information

Research in Real-World Settings: PCORI s Model for Comparative Clinical Effectiveness Research

Research in Real-World Settings: PCORI s Model for Comparative Clinical Effectiveness Research Research in Real-World Settings: PCORI s Model for Comparative Clinical Effectiveness Research David Hickam, MD, MPH Director, Clinical Effectiveness & Decision Science Program April 10, 2017 Welcome!

More information

Cross-Lagged Panel Analysis

Cross-Lagged Panel Analysis Cross-Lagged Panel Analysis Michael W. Kearney Cross-lagged panel analysis is an analytical strategy used to describe reciprocal relationships, or directional influences, between variables over time. Cross-lagged

More information

How many speakers? How many tokens?:

How many speakers? How many tokens?: 1 NWAV 38- Ottawa, Canada 23/10/09 How many speakers? How many tokens?: A methodological contribution to the study of variation. Jorge Aguilar-Sánchez University of Wisconsin-La Crosse 2 Sample size in

More information

Communicating with Patients with Heart Failure and their Families

Communicating with Patients with Heart Failure and their Families Communicating with Patients with Heart Failure and their Families Nathan Goldstein, MD Associate Professor Hertzberg Palliative Care Institute Brookdale Department of Geriatrics and Palliative Medicine

More information

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity Measurement & Variables - Initial step is to conceptualize and clarify the concepts embedded in a hypothesis or research question with

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) Bower, J. E., Crosswell, A. D., Stanton, A. L., Crespi, C. M., Winston, D., Arevalo, J.,... & Ganz, P. A. (2015). Mindfulness meditation for younger breast cancer survivors:

More information

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) 1 VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) Thank you for providing us with the opportunity to revise our paper. We have revised the manuscript according to the editors and

More information

Evaluating Risk Assessment: Finding a Methodology that Supports your Agenda There are few issues facing criminal justice decision makers generating

Evaluating Risk Assessment: Finding a Methodology that Supports your Agenda There are few issues facing criminal justice decision makers generating Evaluating Risk Assessment: Finding a Methodology that Supports your Agenda There are few issues facing criminal justice decision makers generating more interest than fairness and bias with risk assessments.

More information

NeuRA Obsessive-compulsive disorders October 2017

NeuRA Obsessive-compulsive disorders October 2017 Introduction (OCDs) involve persistent and intrusive thoughts (obsessions) and repetitive actions (compulsions). The DSM-5 (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition) defines

More information

Author's response to reviews

Author's response to reviews Author's response to reviews Title:Mental health problems in the 10th grade and non-completion of upper secondary school: the mediating role of grades in a population-based longitudinal study Authors:

More information

CAP Lung Cancer Medical Writers Circle

CAP Lung Cancer Medical Writers Circle Emotional Effects of Lung Cancer on Survivors and Their Spouses Cindy L. Carmack, Ph.D. Associate Professor, The University of Texas M. D. Anderson Cancer Center Receiving a lung cancer diagnosis and undergoing

More information

THE APPLICATION OF ORDINAL LOGISTIC HEIRARCHICAL LINEAR MODELING IN ITEM RESPONSE THEORY FOR THE PURPOSES OF DIFFERENTIAL ITEM FUNCTIONING DETECTION

THE APPLICATION OF ORDINAL LOGISTIC HEIRARCHICAL LINEAR MODELING IN ITEM RESPONSE THEORY FOR THE PURPOSES OF DIFFERENTIAL ITEM FUNCTIONING DETECTION THE APPLICATION OF ORDINAL LOGISTIC HEIRARCHICAL LINEAR MODELING IN ITEM RESPONSE THEORY FOR THE PURPOSES OF DIFFERENTIAL ITEM FUNCTIONING DETECTION Timothy Olsen HLM II Dr. Gagne ABSTRACT Recent advances

More information

multilevel modeling for social and personality psychology

multilevel modeling for social and personality psychology 1 Introduction Once you know that hierarchies exist, you see them everywhere. I have used this quote by Kreft and de Leeuw (1998) frequently when writing about why, when, and how to use multilevel models

More information

Running head: EMOTION REGULATION MODERATES PERFECTIONISM 1. Depression in College Students. Jessica Drews. Faculty Advisor: Scott Pickett

Running head: EMOTION REGULATION MODERATES PERFECTIONISM 1. Depression in College Students. Jessica Drews. Faculty Advisor: Scott Pickett Running head: EMOTION REGULATION MODERATES PERFECTIONISM 1 Emotion Regulation Difficulties as a Moderator of the Relationship between Perfectionism and Depression in College Students Jessica Drews Faculty

More information

GENERAL COMMENTS. The Task Force Process Should be Fully Open, Balanced and Transparent

GENERAL COMMENTS. The Task Force Process Should be Fully Open, Balanced and Transparent December 9, 2013 Submitted Electronically United States Preventive Services Task Force c/o Dr. Robert Cosby Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality 540 Gaither Road Rockville, MD 20850 RE: USPSTF Draft

More information

Dr Sylvie Lambert, RN, PhD

Dr Sylvie Lambert, RN, PhD Is it the most frequent unmet supportive care needs that predict caregivers anxiety and depression? Results from Australia s Partners and Caregivers Longitudinal Well-being Study Dr Sylvie Lambert, RN,

More information

Supportive Care Audit Mercy Hospital for Women - Heidelberg

Supportive Care Audit Mercy Hospital for Women - Heidelberg Supportive Care Audit 2013-2014 Mercy Hospital for Women - Heidelberg Melissa Shand Service Improvement Facilitator NEMICS July 2015 Acknowledgments Mandy Byrne NEMICS Cancer and Data Information Analyst

More information

Coping with Advanced Stage Heart Failure and LVAD/Transplant. Kristin Kuntz, Ph.D. Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Health

Coping with Advanced Stage Heart Failure and LVAD/Transplant. Kristin Kuntz, Ph.D. Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Health Coping with Advanced Stage Heart Failure and LVAD/Transplant Kristin Kuntz, Ph.D. Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Health What is Health Psychology? Health psychology focuses on how biology, psychology,

More information

Data and Statistics 101: Key Concepts in the Collection, Analysis, and Application of Child Welfare Data

Data and Statistics 101: Key Concepts in the Collection, Analysis, and Application of Child Welfare Data TECHNICAL REPORT Data and Statistics 101: Key Concepts in the Collection, Analysis, and Application of Child Welfare Data CONTENTS Executive Summary...1 Introduction...2 Overview of Data Analysis Concepts...2

More information

Adjustment to Retirement: The Moderating Role of Attachment. Dikla Segel, Peter Bamberger

Adjustment to Retirement: The Moderating Role of Attachment. Dikla Segel, Peter Bamberger Adjustment to Retirement: The Moderating Role of Attachment Dikla Segel, Peter Bamberger Introduction: Adjustment to Retirement Retirement has become a long and meaningful phase in the older adult s life

More information

Watching and waiting : what it means for patients. Dr Christian Aldridge Consultant Dermatologist Cwm Taf NHS Trust

Watching and waiting : what it means for patients. Dr Christian Aldridge Consultant Dermatologist Cwm Taf NHS Trust Watching and waiting : what it means for patients Dr Christian Aldridge Consultant Dermatologist Cwm Taf NHS Trust Watching and waiting or...watching and worrying Once you have a cancer diagnosis, you

More information

Title:Continuity of GP care is associated with lower use of complementary and alternative medical providers A population-based cross-sectional survey

Title:Continuity of GP care is associated with lower use of complementary and alternative medical providers A population-based cross-sectional survey Author's response to reviews Title:Continuity of GP care is associated with lower use of complementary and alternative medical providers A population-based cross-sectional survey Authors: Anne Helen Hansen

More information

Propensity Score Methods for Estimating Causality in the Absence of Random Assignment: Applications for Child Care Policy Research

Propensity Score Methods for Estimating Causality in the Absence of Random Assignment: Applications for Child Care Policy Research 2012 CCPRC Meeting Methodology Presession Workshop October 23, 2012, 2:00-5:00 p.m. Propensity Score Methods for Estimating Causality in the Absence of Random Assignment: Applications for Child Care Policy

More information

Edmonton Symptom Assessment System (ESAS r) Objectives

Edmonton Symptom Assessment System (ESAS r) Objectives Edmonton Symptom Assessment System (ESAS r) Lyn Ceronsky DNP, GNP, FPCN lceronsky@gmail.com December 11, 2018 Objectives Explore the use of Edmonton Symptom Assessment System (ESAS r) tool in clinical

More information

Psychometric Properties of the Japanese Version of the Concerns About Recurrence Scale (CARS-J)

Psychometric Properties of the Japanese Version of the Concerns About Recurrence Scale (CARS-J) Jpn J Clin Oncol 2014;44(5)456 462 doi:10.1093/jjco/hyu032 Advance Access Publication 7 April 2014 Psychometric Properties of the Japanese Version of the Concerns About Recurrence Scale (CARS-J) Kanae

More information

TAMING THAT WILD HORSE: HELPING CANCER SURVIVORS

TAMING THAT WILD HORSE: HELPING CANCER SURVIVORS + TAMING THAT WILD HORSE: HELPING CANCER SURVIVORS MANAGE THEIR FEAR OF CANCER RECURRENCE Dr. Sophie Lebel Associate professor School of Psychology University of Ottawa Canada St Paul University, Ottawa

More information

Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis

Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis EFSA/EBTC Colloquium, 25 October 2017 Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis Julian Higgins University of Bristol 1 Introduction to concepts Standard

More information

Chemo Fog. Ottawa Lymphoma Support Group February 5, Barbara Collins, Ph. D., C. Psych. The Ottawa Hospital

Chemo Fog. Ottawa Lymphoma Support Group February 5, Barbara Collins, Ph. D., C. Psych. The Ottawa Hospital Chemo Fog Ottawa Lymphoma Support Group February 5, 2013 Barbara Collins, Ph. D., C. Psych. The Ottawa Hospital Chemo Fog Devlen et al., 1987 (Br Med J 295:953-957) Surveyed patients with Hodgkin s disease

More information

Lecture Outline. Biost 517 Applied Biostatistics I. Purpose of Descriptive Statistics. Purpose of Descriptive Statistics

Lecture Outline. Biost 517 Applied Biostatistics I. Purpose of Descriptive Statistics. Purpose of Descriptive Statistics Biost 517 Applied Biostatistics I Scott S. Emerson, M.D., Ph.D. Professor of Biostatistics University of Washington Lecture 3: Overview of Descriptive Statistics October 3, 2005 Lecture Outline Purpose

More information

Background. 2 5/30/2017 Company Confidential 2015 Eli Lilly and Company

Background. 2 5/30/2017 Company Confidential 2015 Eli Lilly and Company May 2017 Estimating the effects of patient-reported outcome (PRO) diarrhea and pain measures on PRO fatigue: data analysis from a phase 2 study of abemaciclib monotherapy, a CDK4 and CDK6 inhibitor, in

More information

By Lora A. Connor B.A., 2008, California State University, Long Beach

By Lora A. Connor B.A., 2008, California State University, Long Beach By Lora A. Connor B.A., 2008, California State University, Long Beach A Thesis Proposal December 2014 Committee Members: James Amirkhan, Ph.D. (Chair) Courtney Ahrens, Ph.D. Young-Hee Cho, Ph.D. Agenda

More information

Yoga therapy and breast cancer

Yoga therapy and breast cancer Yoga therapy and breast cancer Yoga is being utilized with positive results in breast cancer patients as an intervention for lymphedema. This treatment has some medical and psychological benefits to offer.

More information

Trait Negative Affect Relates to Prior-Week Symptoms, But Not to Reports of Illness Episodes, Illness Symptoms, and Care Seeking Among Older Persons

Trait Negative Affect Relates to Prior-Week Symptoms, But Not to Reports of Illness Episodes, Illness Symptoms, and Care Seeking Among Older Persons Trait Negative Affect Relates to Prior-Week Symptoms, But Not to Reports of Illness Episodes, Illness Symptoms, and Care Seeking Among Older Persons PABLO A. MORA, MS, CHANTAL ROBITAILLE, PHD, HOWARD LEVENTHAL,

More information

Instrumental Variables Estimation: An Introduction

Instrumental Variables Estimation: An Introduction Instrumental Variables Estimation: An Introduction Susan L. Ettner, Ph.D. Professor Division of General Internal Medicine and Health Services Research, UCLA The Problem The Problem Suppose you wish to

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) Gellis, L. A., Arigo, D., & Elliott, J. C. (2013). Cognitive refocusing treatment for insomnia: A randomized controlled trial in university students. Behavior Therapy,

More information

Donna L. Coffman Joint Prevention Methodology Seminar

Donna L. Coffman Joint Prevention Methodology Seminar Donna L. Coffman Joint Prevention Methodology Seminar The purpose of this talk is to illustrate how to obtain propensity scores in multilevel data and use these to strengthen causal inferences about mediation.

More information

A critical look at the use of SEM in international business research

A critical look at the use of SEM in international business research sdss A critical look at the use of SEM in international business research Nicole F. Richter University of Southern Denmark Rudolf R. Sinkovics The University of Manchester Christian M. Ringle Hamburg University

More information

Multivariate Multilevel Models

Multivariate Multilevel Models Multivariate Multilevel Models Getachew A. Dagne George W. Howe C. Hendricks Brown Funded by NIMH/NIDA 11/20/2014 (ISSG Seminar) 1 Outline What is Behavioral Social Interaction? Importance of studying

More information

TERMINOLOGY INSECURITY? APPLICATION OF ATTACHMENT THEORY TO THE HUMAN-COMPANION ANIMAL BOND, AND REVIEW OF ITS EFFECTS

TERMINOLOGY INSECURITY? APPLICATION OF ATTACHMENT THEORY TO THE HUMAN-COMPANION ANIMAL BOND, AND REVIEW OF ITS EFFECTS TERMINOLOGY INSECURITY? APPLICATION OF ATTACHMENT THEORY TO THE HUMAN-COMPANION ANIMAL BOND, AND REVIEW OF ITS EFFECTS Stephanie Wilmore, M. Ed. Doctoral Candidate, Washington State University Pre-Doctoral

More information

Epidemiologic Methods I & II Epidem 201AB Winter & Spring 2002

Epidemiologic Methods I & II Epidem 201AB Winter & Spring 2002 DETAILED COURSE OUTLINE Epidemiologic Methods I & II Epidem 201AB Winter & Spring 2002 Hal Morgenstern, Ph.D. Department of Epidemiology UCLA School of Public Health Page 1 I. THE NATURE OF EPIDEMIOLOGIC

More information

Multidimensional fatigue and its correlates in hospitalized advanced cancer patients

Multidimensional fatigue and its correlates in hospitalized advanced cancer patients Chapter 5 Multidimensional fatigue and its correlates in hospitalized advanced cancer patients Michael Echtelda,b Saskia Teunissenc Jan Passchierb Susanne Claessena, Ronald de Wita Karin van der Rijta

More information

The moderating impact of temporal separation on the association between intention and physical activity: a meta-analysis

The moderating impact of temporal separation on the association between intention and physical activity: a meta-analysis PSYCHOLOGY, HEALTH & MEDICINE, 2016 VOL. 21, NO. 5, 625 631 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2015.1080371 The moderating impact of temporal separation on the association between intention and physical

More information

Background. Interoceptive Exposure: An Underused Weapon in the Arsenal against OCD. Outline. Exposure Therapy for OCD

Background. Interoceptive Exposure: An Underused Weapon in the Arsenal against OCD. Outline. Exposure Therapy for OCD Outline Interoceptive Exposure: An Underused Weapon in the Arsenal against OCD Shannon Blakey, MS & Jonathan Abramowitz, PhD University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill 30 July 2016 Background Anxiety

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) FOCUSED QUESTION What are the observed effects on pain and fatigue when comparing two occupational therapy activity-pacing interventions in adults with osteoarthritis?

More information

Title:Problematic computer gaming, console-gaming, and internet use among adolescents: new measurement tool and association with time use

Title:Problematic computer gaming, console-gaming, and internet use among adolescents: new measurement tool and association with time use Author's response to reviews Title:Problematic computer gaming, console-gaming, and internet use among adolescents: new measurement tool and association with time use Authors: Mette Rasmussen (mera@niph.dk)

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) Drahota, A., Wood, J. J., Sze, K. M., & Van Dyke, M. (2011). Effects of cognitive behavioral therapy on daily living skills in children with high-functioning autism and

More information

Prevalence of Procrastination in the United States, United Kingdom, and Australia: Arousal and Avoidance Delays among Adults

Prevalence of Procrastination in the United States, United Kingdom, and Australia: Arousal and Avoidance Delays among Adults Prevalence of Procrastination in the United States, United Kingdom, and Australia: Arousal and Avoidance Delays among Adults Joseph R. Ferrari DePaul University Jean O'Callaghan & Ian Newbegin Roehampton

More information

Identity Theory: Reconstruction and Application

Identity Theory: Reconstruction and Application P a g e 1 Identity Theory: Reconstruction and Application Curtis M. Kularski University of North Carolina at Charlotte 12 December 2014 SOCY 6651 P a g e 2 Identity Theory: Reconstruction and Application

More information

The American Cancer Society National Quality of Life Survey for Caregivers

The American Cancer Society National Quality of Life Survey for Caregivers The American Cancer Society National Quality of Life Survey for Caregivers Rachel S. Cannady Strategic Director, Cancer Caregiver Support Atlanta, GA Agenda Definition and prevalence of cancer survivorship

More information

Religious and Non-Religious Pathways to Stress-Related Growth in Younger Adult Cancer Survivors

Religious and Non-Religious Pathways to Stress-Related Growth in Younger Adult Cancer Survivors Religious and Non-Religious Pathways to Stress-Related Growth in Younger Adult Cancer Survivors Crystal L. Park, Donald Edmondson, & Thomas O. Blank University of Connecticut In collaboration with the

More information

Module 4: Case Conceptualization and Treatment Planning

Module 4: Case Conceptualization and Treatment Planning Module 4: Case Conceptualization and Treatment Planning Objectives To better understand the role of case conceptualization in cognitive-behavioral therapy. To develop specific case conceptualization skills,

More information

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen The following full text is a publisher's version. For additional information about this publication click this link. http://hdl.handle.net/2066/170351

More information

UW MEDICINE PATIENT EDUCATION. Baby Blues and More DRAFT. Knowing About This in Advance Can Help

UW MEDICINE PATIENT EDUCATION. Baby Blues and More DRAFT. Knowing About This in Advance Can Help UW MEDICINE PATIENT EDUCATION Baby Blues and More Recognizing and coping with postpartum mood disorders Some women have baby blues or more serious postpartum mood disorders. It helps to know about these

More information

George B. Ploubidis. The role of sensitivity analysis in the estimation of causal pathways from observational data. Improving health worldwide

George B. Ploubidis. The role of sensitivity analysis in the estimation of causal pathways from observational data. Improving health worldwide George B. Ploubidis The role of sensitivity analysis in the estimation of causal pathways from observational data Improving health worldwide www.lshtm.ac.uk Outline Sensitivity analysis Causal Mediation

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Differential Item Functioning

Differential Item Functioning Differential Item Functioning Lecture #11 ICPSR Item Response Theory Workshop Lecture #11: 1of 62 Lecture Overview Detection of Differential Item Functioning (DIF) Distinguish Bias from DIF Test vs. Item

More information

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping Amanda Palmer, Ken Sinclair and Michael Bailey University of Sydney Paper prepared for presentation

More information

CONSTITUENT VOICE FISCAL YEAR 2016 RESULTS REPORT

CONSTITUENT VOICE FISCAL YEAR 2016 RESULTS REPORT CONSTITUENT VOICE FISCAL YEAR 2016 RESULTS REPORT April 2015 to May 2016 INTRODUCTION At LIFT, we strongly believe our members are the experts on their own lives. And, when working with LIFT staff, they

More information

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing? Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC How are we doing? Prepared by Maura McCool, M.S. Office of Research, Evaluation and Assessment Metropolitan Community Colleges Fall 2003 1 General Education Assessment

More information

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Adam Hahn 1 Bertram Gawronski 2 Word count: 20,754 excluding acknowledgements, abstract,

More information

What is analytical sociology? And is it the future of sociology?

What is analytical sociology? And is it the future of sociology? What is analytical sociology? And is it the future of sociology? Twan Huijsmans Sociology Abstract During the last few decades a new approach in sociology has been developed, analytical sociology (AS).

More information

ISA 540, Auditing Accounting Estimates, Including Fair Value Accounting Estimates, and Related Disclosures Issues and Task Force Recommendations

ISA 540, Auditing Accounting Estimates, Including Fair Value Accounting Estimates, and Related Disclosures Issues and Task Force Recommendations Agenda Item 1-A ISA 540, Auditing Accounting Estimates, Including Fair Value Accounting Estimates, and Related Disclosures Issues and Task Force Recommendations Introduction 1. Since the September 2016

More information

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module

Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module 2090 The PedsQL in Pediatric Cancer Reliability and Validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory Generic Core Scales, Multidimensional Fatigue Scale, and Cancer Module James W. Varni, Ph.D. 1,2

More information

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H

More information

Summary and conclusions

Summary and conclusions Summary and conclusions Aggression and violence, posttraumatic stress, and absenteeism among employees in penitentiaries The study about which we have reported here was commissioned by the Sector Directorate

More information

The State of the Art in Indicator Research

The State of the Art in Indicator Research International Society for Quality-of-Life Studies (ISQOLS) The State of the Art in Indicator Research Filomena Maggino filomena.maggino@unifi.it The State of the Art in Indicator Research I 1. Developing

More information

1. Family context. a) Positive Disengaged

1. Family context. a) Positive Disengaged Online Supplementary Materials for Emotion manuscript 015-197 Emotions and Concerns: Situational Evidence for their Systematic Co-Occurrence. by Jozefien De Leersnyder, Peter Koval, Peter Kuppens, & Batja

More information

Hello and welcome to Patient Power sponsored by UCSF Medical Center. I m Andrew Schorr.

Hello and welcome to Patient Power sponsored by UCSF Medical Center. I m Andrew Schorr. The Integrated Approach to Treating Cancer Symptoms Webcast March 1, 2012 Michael Rabow, M.D. Please remember the opinions expressed on Patient Power are not necessarily the views of UCSF Medical Center,

More information

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task Olga Lositsky lositsky@princeton.edu Robert C. Wilson Department of Psychology University

More information

Technical Specifications

Technical Specifications Technical Specifications In order to provide summary information across a set of exercises, all tests must employ some form of scoring models. The most familiar of these scoring models is the one typically

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

peer relations; psychological adjustment; context; methodology; adolescent development

peer relations; psychological adjustment; context; methodology; adolescent development Contextual Variations in Negative Mood and State Self-Esteem What Role Do Peers Play? Bridget M. Reynolds Rena L. Repetti University of California, Los Angeles Journal of Early Adolescence Volume 28 Number

More information

San Diego Center for the Treatment of Mood Disorders 1

San Diego Center for the Treatment of Mood Disorders 1 San Diego Center for the Treatment of Mood Disorders 1 DATE NAME Last Middle First REFERRAL HISTORY: How did you find us (via a referral, web search, recommendation)? Please provide the name and phone

More information

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA COGNITION AND EMOTION 2007, 21 (3), 681688 BRIEF REPORT Vulnerability to depressive symptoms: Clarifying the role of excessive reassurance seeking and perceived social support in an interpersonal model

More information

How do we combine two treatment arm trials with multiple arms trials in IPD metaanalysis? An Illustration with College Drinking Interventions

How do we combine two treatment arm trials with multiple arms trials in IPD metaanalysis? An Illustration with College Drinking Interventions 1/29 How do we combine two treatment arm trials with multiple arms trials in IPD metaanalysis? An Illustration with College Drinking Interventions David Huh, PhD 1, Eun-Young Mun, PhD 2, & David C. Atkins,

More information

What is Multilevel Modelling Vs Fixed Effects. Will Cook Social Statistics

What is Multilevel Modelling Vs Fixed Effects. Will Cook Social Statistics What is Multilevel Modelling Vs Fixed Effects Will Cook Social Statistics Intro Multilevel models are commonly employed in the social sciences with data that is hierarchically structured Estimated effects

More information

Partners in Care: A Model of Social Work in Primary Care

Partners in Care: A Model of Social Work in Primary Care Partners in Care: A Model of Social Work in Primary Care Common problems in the elderly, such as reduced cognitive functioning, depression, medication safety, sleep abnormalities, and falls have been shown

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

16 May/June 2014 Energy Magazine

16 May/June 2014 Energy Magazine 16 Healing Touch An Integrative Therapy in Outpatient Oncology Debra Reis, MSN, RN, CNP, Program Coordinator - Healing Care, ProMedica Cancer Institute Tisha Jones, MSW, ProMedica Hickman Cancer Center

More information

What do We Know about Uncertainly in Illness?

What do We Know about Uncertainly in Illness? 特別講演 What do We Know about Uncertainly in Illness? Merle H. Mishel, RN, PhD. FAAN Kenan Professor School of Nursing University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Chapel Hill, NC,. USA When I began my original

More information

Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) / Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Encephalopathy (ME)

Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) / Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Encephalopathy (ME) Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) / Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Encephalopathy (ME) This intervention (and hence this listing of competences) assumes that practitioners are familiar with, and able to deploy,

More information

Underlying Theory & Basic Issues

Underlying Theory & Basic Issues Underlying Theory & Basic Issues Dewayne E Perry ENS 623 Perry@ece.utexas.edu 1 All Too True 2 Validity In software engineering, we worry about various issues: E-Type systems: Usefulness is it doing what

More information