The Impact of a Dialogic Reading Program on Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Kindergarten and Early Primary School Aged Students in Hong Kong

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Impact of a Dialogic Reading Program on Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Kindergarten and Early Primary School Aged Students in Hong Kong"

Transcription

1 The Impact of a Dialogic Reading Program on Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Kindergarten and Early Primary School Aged Students in Hong Kong Pan-Chung Fung Bonnie Wing-Yin Chow Catherine McBride-Chang The Chinese University of Hong Kong The present study investigated the effects of a special interactive dialogic reading method developed by Whitehurst et al. (1988) on deaf and hard-of-hearing children in Hong Kong. Twenty-eight deaf and hard-of-hearing children in kindergarten, first, or second grade were pretested on a receptive vocabulary test and assigned to one of three conditions, dialogic reading, typical reading, and control, with age and degree of hearing loss matched. After an 8-week intervention, the children were re-tested. The dialogic reading group had a significantly greater improvement in vocabulary scores than did the other two groups. Parentchild interactions of high quality and the use of pictorial materials are likely the key successful factors in the program. The educational value of this intervention is discussed. The present study tested the effects of a special parentchild reading method called dialogic reading (e.g., Zevenbergen & Whitehurst, 2003) to deaf and hardof- hearing children in Hong Kong. By adapting this program, which has been used successfully to promote language skills in English-speaking, typically developing children, to deaf and hard-of-hearing children growing up in a Chinese society, we tested the extent to which this shared parent-child reading technique could promote vocabulary skills in a rarely studied population. Below, we review previous research on deaf and hard-of-hearing children in relation to general language skills, focusing on vocabulary skills, family Correspondence should be sent to Catherine McBride-Chang, Psychology Department, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, N.T., Hong Kong ( cmcbride@psy.cuhk.edu.hk). interactions, and Hong Kong Chinese children s language opportunities and development. Hearing Loss and Language development Language development involves skills and knowledge about the sounds of the language, how words are formed andarerelatedtoeachother,andhowlanguageconveys meaning (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998). Because of their difficulties in hearing, deaf and hard-of-hearing children tend to be relatively slow to develop language, and language deficits are common characteristics of children with hearing loss (Kretchmer & Kretchmer, 1978; Laughton, 1989). Language deficits have been found among the deaf and hard-of-hearing in all of the major language domains: syntax, morphology, semantics, pragmatics, and phonology (Levitt, 1987; Moeller, Osberger, & Eccarius, 1986; Shaw, 1994), and these deficits further lead to reading and academic difficulties, especially in language related subjects (Moeller et al., 1986). Receptive vocabulary delay is one of the most difficult problems children with hearing loss face during language acquisition. A large-scale study by Moeller, Osberger, and Eccarius (1986) examined the receptive vocabulary skills of a group of 150 deaf or hard-of-hearing individuals from the ages of 4 and a half to 20 years old. These individuals demonstrated receptive vocabulary skills that were, on average, equivalent to those achieved by 5- to 7-year-old hearing children. Delays ranged from 2 years developmentally at 6 years of age chronologically to 9 years developmentally at Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education vol. 10 no. 1 Ó Oxford University Press 2005; all rights reserved. doi: /deafed/eni005

2 Dialogic Reading years of age chronologically. Moeller et al. (1986) found that there was a lack of improvement in language skills with age, with a plateau in vocabulary development at 12 to 13 years old. Carney and Moeller (1998) accounted for this finding by postulating that children with hearing loss have problems assessing constant and consistent information from the environment. Such difficulties result in poor early learning experiences that, in turn, create a weak foundation for forming language rules and developing language skills. However, although hearing impairment is related to delay in language development, the degree of hearing loss is not the determining factor in the delay. For example, receptive vocabulary delay has been found in children with only mild hearing losses as well as profound hearing losses (Davis, Elfenbein, Schum & Bentier, 1986). Mayne (1999) also found that degree of hearing loss is not significantly correlated with receptive vocabulary scores. In the present study, we focused on receptive vocabulary skills of young deaf and hard-of-hearing children from Hong Kong. The importance of environmental stimulation for organization of sensory modalities early in development is widely recognized. We therefore adopted a multi-modal perspective on language instruction for children with sensory impairments. We did this by adapting visual stimuli in a shared parent-child reading program to suit the needs of deaf and hard-of-hearing children and their parents. Positive family environment Numerous studies have demonstrated that positive family environment is associated with many aspects of children s language growth among normal hearing children (Zevenbergen & Whitehurst, 2003). Similar results have also been found in children with hearing impairment. Parental involvement in education of students with hearing impairment has yielded positive reports on academic, language, and social-emotional development (Bodner-Johnson, 1986; Calderon & Greenberg, 1993; Calderon, Greenburg, & Kusche, 1991). Also, Moeller (2000) examined the vocabulary skills in 1.5 to 5-year-olds with hearing loss who were enrolled in an intervention program and found that children with greater family involvement in the intervention program had higher language scores, regardless of the degree of hearing loss. Calderon (2000) assessed 28 children with prelingual, moderately severe to profound, sensorineural hearing loss and their parents who were able to sign to their children. Calderon indicated that parental involvement in their children s educational program was the strongest predictor of children s language development, and it shared considerable variance with maternal communication skill. These studies have demonstrated that family involvement, especially maternal communication skills, is critical for language development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Parent-child reading provides one excellent context for parents involvement in children s language learning and communication with their children. Over the past several decades, researchers have demonstrated that parent-child reading is associated with many aspects of language growth of typically developing children. Frequency of parent-child reading in the home can enhance preschool- and primary school-age children s language comprehension and expressive language skills and has contributed to the gains in preschoolers vocabulary, oral language complexity, and narrative skills (Zevenbergen & Whitehurst, 2003). Longitudinal research has demonstrated the relations among early experiences with shared picture book reading and later language skills (Crain-Thoreson & Dale, 1999; Zevenbergen & Whitehurst, 2003). Despite the positive impacts of parent-child reading, few studies have examined the effects of parent-child reading for children with special developmental disorders and even fewer for deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Ezell, Justice, and Parsons (2000) conducted one of the few studies investigating the effectiveness of parent-child reading on preschoolers with communication problems. Four parents and their children completed a 5-week program that included group parent training and individual training in guided reading. Results indicated that this parent-child reading program positively influenced children s concepts of receptive and expressive alphabetic vocabulary. This study is encouraging because it demonstrates effective ways for parents to aide language learning of children with special needs.

3 84 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 10:1 Winter 2005 However, parents of deaf and hard-of-hearing children report difficulties in teaching their children language for at least two reasons. First, because 95% of deaf or deaf and hard-of-hearing children are born to hearing parents (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2004), these children are often unable to exploit their available language learning capabilities because of a sensory mismatch between their own abilities and home environment (Padden & Humphries, 1988; Wilcox & Corwin, 1990). Second, parents of deaf and hardof-hearing children may not feel skilled enough to communicate with the child with hearing loss and feel more comfortable being observers who are not involved in the reading process (Powers & Sackiewicz, 1998). Therefore, methods that can be employed to teach language are particularly needed by parents of deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Parent-child reading is an ideal context for practicing language skills. Merely reading aloud to children during parentchild reading is likely not the best way to improve children s language skills, especially for deaf and hardof-hearing children. Previous research has suggested that parents specific techniques in parent-child reading, such as questioning, praising, and extending information given by children, can more directly improve children s language skills (Ninio & Brunner, 1978; Crain-Thoreson & Dale, 1999; Zevenbergen & Whitehurst, 2003). To maximize the potential of parent-child reading, parents specific communication techniques and children s active participation should be emphasized. Past research has also shown the positive impacts of parents specific assistance and technique when reading with deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Van der Lam and Timmerman (1995) reported that the vocabulary of deaf and hard-of-hearing children was enhanced by parents use of picture books with explanation. The gain in vocabulary acquisition was stronger when SLN (Sign Language of Netherlands) was used jointly. Paul (1998) also pointed out that it is important to enrich (e.g., provide examples, discuss, elaborate upon) students prior knowledge of the topics they might read about. Students should also be encouraged to use or apply this knowledge during reading activities, and it is beneficial to the students if they can be offered assistance to answer different types of questions. Few programs have been developed for teaching parents how to read to their deaf and hard-of hearing children. The Shared Reading Project developed by Laurent Clerc National Deaf Education Center of the Gallaudet University is among the very few programs designed for hearing parents to teach language to their deaf and hard-of-hearing children. This project provides techniques for parents to read to their deaf and hard-of-hearing children using American Sign Language by observing how deaf adults sign a story. A trained deaf tutor visits the family once a week with a specially designed book bag that contains a copy of the storybook, a videotape for practice, a bookmark with tips for reading, and a guide of post-reading activities. The tutor demonstrates how to sign a story to the deaf or hard-of-hearing child and provides feedback to the parent as the parent signs the story to the child. The parent reads to the child with the book bag of the corresponding week between tutor visits, and the tutor brings a new book bag to the family in each of the 20 weeks. According to a post-project survey conducted by the center, participating parents reported that their sign language skills improved, and parent-child communication and children s attention increased with this program (Delk & Weidekamp, 2001). However, no empirical evaluation of this project has been demonstrated. Our program adopted some similar principles. However, the dialogic reading technique, as described below, focuses particularly on the child s interpretations and reactions to the story, rather than on teaching parents particular language (e.g., sign language) skills. Dialogic Reading The dialogic reading technique developed by Whitehurst and colleagues (1988) is a special parent-child reading method that takes parents reading techniques and children s active attention into consideration. Zevenbergen and Whitehurst (2003) defined it as a specific technique of parent-child interactions based on the use of language, feedback, and appropriate scaffolded parent-child interaction in the context of picture book reading. Picture storybooks are used and, with stimulation from the pictures inside, the child gradually becomes the storyteller to the parents.

4 Dialogic Reading 85 Indeed, role alternation is the main feature of dialogic reading. According to Whitehurst, the fundamental reading technique in dialogic reading is the PEER sequence, which is Prompt, Evaluation, Expansion, and Repetition, respectively. The parent prompts the child to say something with the help of the book, evaluates the child s response, expands the child s response by adding some new information, and finally guides the child to repeat the information to make sure that s/he has learned it. There are five different prompts to be used: completion prompts, recall prompts, open-ended prompts, Wh- prompts, and distancing prompts. Several studies have established the positive impact of dialogic reading on language development, especially receptive vocabulary skills, among children without sensory impairments (Hargrave & Sénéchal, 2000; Huebner, 2000; Lim, 1999; Valdez-Menchaca & Whitehurst, 1992; Whitehurst et al., 1988; Whitehurst et al., 1994). For instance, Valdez-Menchaca & Whitehurst (1992) found that children in the dialogic reading condition were several months ahead in language skills relative to their chronological age in a study of children from Mexico. Similarly, significant effects of dialogic reading were found in preschool children from lowincome backgrounds (Lonigan & Whitehurst, 1998) and 2-year-old children from upper and middle SES families (Whitehurst et al., 1988). Zevenbergen and Whitehurst (2003) reported that preschoolers who received the dialogic reading intervention showed advantages in their later reading comprehension, and these effects lasted to elementary school. Dialogic reading was also effective in enhancing children s oral productive and receptive Korean skills in Korea (Lim, 1999, cited in De Temple & Snow, 2003). Chow and McBride-Chang (2003) conducted research on the effectiveness of dialogic reading on Chinese language and literacy skills of typically developing kindergarten children in Hong Kong. Though the gain of receptive language skills in the dialogic reading group compared to the comparison groups was found to be less strong than the gains of literacy skills, the improvement of receptive vocabulary skills in the dialogic reading group was greater than that of the control group. Dialogic reading emphasizes active interaction and parents specific assistance during picture storybook reading, and this matches the needs of deaf and hardof-hearing children. To investigate whether the dialogic reading intervention could produce the same positive effects on language development for deaf and hard-of-hearing children in Hong Kong, the present study extended the dialogic technique to both kindergarteners and early primary students with hearing impairment who use Chinese as their primary communicative language. To enhance the effectiveness of dialogic reading for deaf and hard-of-hearing children, several adaptations to the dialogic reading intervention were made. Most of these adaptations made use of principles of multimodal stimulation as an effective learning tool for young children. First, picture cards were used to assist the dialogic reading processes (see Appendix). Paul and Quigley (1990) attributed the difficulty that children who are deaf or hard of hearing have to the inappropriate inference strategies they use while reading. Such readers frequently use individual word recall as a basic comprehension strategy. However, because of their hearing loss, their development of a complex network of semantic links tends to be limited. These children are often unaware of strategies that assist comprehension, such as visualization. One way of activating comprehension processes that facilitate inference in readers is through pictures. Pictures activate relevant prior knowledge for readers and facilitate learning by inducing readers to form mental images of the information, an essential part of cognitive processing involving interaction with text (Bus, Van Ijzendoorn & Pellegrini, 1995; Chan, Cole, & Morris, 1990). Second, heavy reliance on oral interaction or prompts at the beginning of the program might be relatively difficult for the deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Therefore, reliance on pictures was encouraged. When both parents and children practiced more, pictures could be gradually used less. Third, among the five different prompts included in dialogic reading training, open-ended prompts and Wh- prompts were used more at the beginning in the present study since both of them are easier when compared to the other three. The remaining three were used later and the adults were asked to alter the ratio of the prompts according to children s progress. In

5 86 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 10:1 Winter 2005 addition, repetition, the last step in the PEER sequence, was emphasized. This is the most important step for children to gain novel vocabulary or information, so adults were encouraged to better use lip-motion, gesture, and even signing to clarify the meaning and the pronunciation. Slow but clear examples were given until children could provide correct responses. Fourth, word cards were given to the parents and parents were encouraged to use them for asking questions or introducing new ideas to their children. Children could also use the cards by pointing to the answer in the book or finding the corresponding word cards, after they listened to the question or prompt. Using pictorial materials has proven to be beneficial for receptive vocabulary learning among prelingually deaf children (Walker, Munro, & Rickards, 1998). Deaf and hard-of-hearing children in Hong Kong In Hong Kong, most of the severe to profound deaf and hard-of-hearing children study in four local deaf schools. Those who have milder hearing loss are mainstreamed to normal schools or kindergartens with assistance from the Education Department in Hong Kong (K. M. Yiu, personal communication, 2002). Similar to normal hearing peers, every deaf and hardof-hearing child ages 3 to 6 years enters either a normal kindergarten or the preschool classes offered by the four local deaf schools. In the four local deaf schools, oral communication is encouraged and used in lectures (Yiu, 2002). Even though some children have profound hearing loss, they are encouraged to speak, rather than sign, with the assistance of hearing aids. Objectives of the Present Study In summary, this study was designed to test experimentally the hypothesis that a dialogic reading intervention could produce larger receptive language gains in deaf and hard-of-hearing children as compared to children in typical parent-child reading and control conditions in Hong Kong. We hypothesized that children in the dialogic reading group would have greater improvement in receptive vocabulary skills relative to those in the typical reading group and control group after an 8-week intervention. Method Participants Twenty-eight children with moderate to severe hearing loss were included. All were of normal intelligence with no additional disabilities. These children ranged in age from 5 years 2 months to 9 years 1 month, and were attending kindergarten, first, or second grade in local primary schools. Seventeen children were recruited from one local school for the deaf (The Hong Kong School for the Deaf) while the remaining eleven children were recruited from five different mainstreamed schools through the Special Education Resource Center of the Education Department. Their mothers (with normal hearing) who were able to speak Cantonese and had at least a primary educational level were included in this study. Design All the participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: dialogic reading group, typical reading group or control group. Dialogic reading methods proposed by Whitehurst et al. (1988) were used in the dialogic reading group. In order to investigate the effect of the skills, a typical reading group was used. Also, a control group was included in order to distinguish improvement due to normal growth or development from that caused by the experimental manipulation. Each participant was tested twice before and after the 8-week intervention. Therefore, a 3 (groups) 3 2 (time) factorial design was used. Materials Twenty-eight sets of Chinese picture storybooks (8 different stories per set) were used. The titles of these books are shown in Table 1. Dialogic reading group. In this group, prompt questions were attached to each page of the Chinese storybooks used. Those prompts included the five different prompts that Whitehurst and his colleagues (1988) used. Recall prompts were given at the end of each book. These were intended to act as a guide for the parents to practice dialogic reading with their children. In order to give clear guidelines on the

6 Dialogic Reading 87 Table 1 Titles of the Chinese picture storybooks Title of the story books Author Publisher (2000) (2000) (2000) (2000) (1999) (2001) (2001) (2001) dialogic reading techniques to the parents, a dialogic reading guide accompanied the books. In these guidelines, each step of the technique was demonstrated clearly so that the parents could follow and lead the child with standard procedures. The guideline also explained the aims of dialogic reading. In addition, picture cards and a calendar checklist were also given to the parents. The picture cards were the choices in some of the prompt questions. They could also be used as materials to stimulate children s storytelling. For example, different animal cards were given to the parents so that they could ask questions about the characteristics of those animals. Parents could also ask questions about the content of the storybooks; the answer was one of the animals in those picture cards. Children, thus, could point or take the card out as a response. The calendar checklist was used to remind the parents about the schedule of the program. One example picture card is presented in Appendix A. Typical reading group. The same set of storybooks and the calendar checklists were given to the parent of this group. However, none of those storybooks was accompanied with the prompts or picture cards that the dialogic reading group received. Control group. The same sets of storybooks were given to these children, but they were given to them only after the participants in the other two groups had finished the program, 8 weeks later. Measures Demographic data were collected through a constructed questionnaire that included children s age, sex, class, date of birth, level of hearing loss, signing ability, as well as parental educational level and parental occupation. Information on the degree of hearing loss was given by either the school or the parents. Home reading habits, such as the duration and frequency of parent-child reading before the program, were also collected. Raven s Coloured Progressive Matrices (RCPM; Raven, Court, & Raven, 1995). RCPM is an assessment of cognitive development in children between 5 to 11 years old. It consists of 36 in-color, multiple-choice items in which a missing part has to be chosen from among six alternatives to complete a matrix-like pattern. The test is composed of three subsets A, Ab, and B, each consisting of 12 items. Sets A, Ab, and B measure the apprehension of identity and change in continuous patterns, discrimination of discrete figures as spatially related wholes, and attention to analogous changes in spatially and logically related figures, respectively. Vocabulary. Receptive vocabulary was measured using a translation of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Third Edition (PPVT III; Dunn & Dunn, 1997), which is an achievement and screening test of vocabulary acquisition for English-speaking people older than 2.5 years of age. The PPVT III consists of 204 items. There were four line drawings of nouns per page. Participants were asked to respond by pointing to one of the four picture options after hearing the examiner pronounce a given vocabulary word. In our study, the translated Cantonese version was used; the pilot test was done with 2 deaf and hard-of-hearing children without any problems and was proven to be appropriate to use for deaf and hard-of-hearing children in Hong Kong. The inter-item reliability for

7 88 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 10:1 Winter 2005 the translated PPVT III was.70. Raw scores were used for all analyses of this task. Follow-up questionnaire. At the posttest period, a follow-up questionnaire was given to each parent in the dialogic and typical reading group. The purpose of this questionnaire was to evaluate children s reading habits and the parents practices with the dialogic reading intervention. For the dialogic reading group, questions included the duration and frequency of the dialogic reading intervention. Parents use of extra skills and reading methods were also obtained. An example of these questions was,? (How often did you read with your child per week using the dialogic reading technique within these 8 weeks?). In addition, there were questions concerning children s response toward the intervention, which were on a Likert-type scale, from 1 to 10, indicating (maximal response) to (minimal response). An example of these questions was, (How frequently did the child respond during dialogic reading process?). For the typical reading group, follow-up questions included the duration and frequency of reading at home with parents. There were also questions concerning the reading methods the parents used as a comparison with the dialogic group. Procedures After collecting informed consent, the parents were asked to complete the demographics information questionnaire. The children were tested on the RCPM (Raven et al., 1995), and pretested on the PPVT III (Dunn & Dunn, 1997). The 17 children from the local deaf school were tested in the classrooms in their own school. The 11 mainstreamed children were tested in the special center of the Education Department or the Psychology Department of the Chinese University of Hong Kong. All children were one-on-one tested by 10 trained experimenters who were all undergraduates in the Department of Psychology of the Chinese University of Hong Kong. The pretest was conducted in two sections. Each session was about 30 min long. Children were given a 10-minute break between Table 2 Descriptive statistics on the demographic measures Group N Mean SD Chronological Age (in months) Dialogic Reading Typical reading Control Total 28 Level of Hearing Impairment Dialogic Reading Typical reading Control Total 28 Gender Male Female Dialogic Reading 7 2 Typical reading 5 4 Control 5 5 Total sections. All 28 children were then assigned into one of three different conditions: dialogic reading group, typical reading group, and control group, with age and degree of hearing loss controlled. The condition assignment was double-blind to both parents and experimenters. Children from the three groups did not statistically differ in chronological age or level of hearing loss. Those descriptive statistics are presented in Table 2 in the results section. Materials were given to the participants of the dialogic reading and the typical reading groups once the pretesting was complete. Children in the dialogic reading group were given a set of total eight storybooks accompanied with prompt questions on each page of the books. Parents were also given the calendar checklist, the picture cards, and the guidelines. Parents were taught about the dialogic reading method by a trained experimenter through an individual 20-minute training session before the start of the intervention. They were asked to use specific strategies (e.g., the PEER sequence) and the materials given to encourage their children to respond while reading at home. Parents were asked to read with their children each book twice a week for 15 to 30 min each time. As outlined in the PEER sequence of the book, parents were trained to prompt their children to say something about the content, then evaluate children s responses. Parents were then encouraged to expand on the children s

8 Dialogic Reading 89 responses, possibly by adding new information. The parents were taught to repeat the prompt finally to ensure their children actually grasped the idea. Five different prompts were also taught to the parents: Completion, Recall, Open-ended, Wh-, and Distancing, or CROWD. The first are the Completion prompts, fill-in-the-blanks questions that provide the children with information about the structure of language (e.g., Little monkey lives in.). The second ones are the Recall prompts. They are questions that ask for the summary of the content or what happened before in order to help children to understand the story plot (e.g.,? Who is the champion of the competition? ). Third, Open-ended prompts are statements that encourage children to respond in their own words to increase their expressing and explanation ability (e.g.,? Please tell me what little bear and his father do.). Next, Wh-prompts (what, where, why, when etc.) can also be used to help children to learn new meanings (e.g.,? Where can you find sand?). Lastly, parents were asked to use Distancing prompts. These are prompts that require children to link what is in the book to their real life settings or their own experiences. This can improve children s memory for newly learned materials and also conversational abilities (e.g.,? Have you ever seen a cloud?). Parents were contacted over the phone once a week for the first 2 weeks in order to remind them to read the books with their children and also to solve problems they came across during dialogic reading. For the typical reading group, the same set of books and calendar checklist were given without the prompt hints. Parents were also instructed to read the storybook on schedule, one book a week, twice each for 15 to 30 min each time. Nevertheless, they were taught no specific skills about promoting any responses or participation from children. They were asked to read the books to their children as they normally would. These parents were debriefed after the 8-week period and were taught the dialogic reading skills following the posttest section. No books were given to the control group immediately after the pretest section. The parents were asked to continue their original reading habits with their children throughout this 8-week period. The same set of books was given to children in this group after the posttest section. Debriefing was also given to these parents. They were also taught the same dialogic reading skills following children s posttesting. After 8 weeks (the intervention period), children from all three groups were immediately posttested on the PPVT III while the parents were asked to fill in the follow-up questionnaire at the same time. Results Pretest measures The descriptive statistics on chronological age, level of hearing loss, and gender distribution are presented in Table 2. Information on level of hearing loss was obtained from ratings on a Likert-type scale, from 1 to 7, indicating mild to profound, from the children s teachers at school. Statistics indicated that the children s average level of hearing loss was around moderate. One-way ANOVAs were carried out to compare the chronological age (in months) and the level of hearing loss among the three groups. There were no statistically significant differences in either chronological age, F(2, 27) , p..05, or the level of hearing loss, F(2, 27)50.063, p >.05, among the three groups. The gender ratios of the children were similar in both the typical reading and control group but uneven in the dialogic reading group. This was because the age and the degree of hearing loss had to be statistically controlled. A one-way ANOVA was conducted to compare the differences in the levels of nonverbal intelligence, as measured by the RCPM, among the three groups. There was no statistically significant difference in this measure either, F(2, 27) , p..05. The descriptive statistics for the RCPM score are presented in Table 3. Thus, none of these control variables, chronological age, level of hearing loss, or non-verbal intelligence, were included in the subsequent analyses as covariates. A repeated measures analysis was also performed to compare vocabulary scores in the pretest and posttest periods. Although the effect across time was not significant, F(1, 25) , p..05, which means that

9 90 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 10:1 Winter 2005 Table 3 Descriptive statistics for the RCPM and the PPVT III pretest and posttest scores PPVT III RCPM Pretest Posttest Group N Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Dialogic Reading Typical reading Control Total 28 Mean Scores Pretest Posttest Control Normal Dialogical Figure 1 Pretest and posttest scores on the PPVT III of the three groups posttest scores did not significantly differ from the pretest scores across all three groups, a significant difference was found in the changes in scores among the three groups, with F(2, 25) , p,.05, indicating the effect of group was reliable. Different interventions did result in different degrees of changes in scores. Descriptive statistics for the pretest and posttest scores of the PPVT III are presented in Table 3. As indicated in Table 3, the degree of variability on scores was high and that might have been due to the small sample sizes included. Owing to the greater utility of the dialogic reading program predicted, t tests were conducted to compare the differences of PPVT III posttest scores between the dialogic reading and the typical reading group, as well as between the dialogic reading and the control group. Separate t tests were also conducted to compare PPVT III pretest scores between the groups to ensure that any group differences were not due to biased sampling. No significant differences were found on the pretest scores between the dialogic reading and typical reading groups, t(16) , p..05, or between the dialogic reading and control groups, t(17) , p..05. However, differences on the posttest scores between dialogic reading and typical reading groups were significant, t (16) , p,.05, and the difference between the dialogic reading and the control groups was marginally significant, t(17)52.05, p This result indicates the effectiveness of the dialogic reading program in improving children s vocabulary skills. T-tests were also conducted to compare the differences on both PPVT III pretest and posttest scores between typical reading and control groups in order to distinguish improvement due to normal growth or experimental manipulation. No significant differences were found between the typical reading and control groups on either the pretest scores, t(17) , p..05., or the posttest scores, t(17) , p..05. To further investigate the effectiveness of the dialogic reading program, effect size was calculated. In the present study, the partial Eta squared was used as an indicator of effect size. The Eta squared, the proportion of variance accounted for by population membership, is another useful indicator of effect size. Partial Eta squared was more appropriate in this study than Eta squared since more than one independent variable was present (Cohen, 1988). The partial Eta squared is the proportion of the effect plus the error variance that is attributable to the effect. Its formula differs from the Eta squared formula in that the denominator includes the effect sum of squares plus the error sum of squares rather than the total sum of squares, hp25ss effect /(SS effect þ SS error ). The partial Eta squared for this study was.276. Given the commonly used guidelines proposed by Cohen (1988;.01 5 small effect,.06 5 moderate effect,.14 5 large effect), the partial Eta squared in the present study suggested a very large effect of the dialogic reading treatment on PPVT III scores. The comparisons between the pretest and posttest mean scores on the PPVT III are shown in Figure 1. The figure indicates that the dialogic reading group showed the greatest improvement among the three groups, while the other two groups failed to show any improvement. Follow-up questionnaire Follow-up questionnaires were received from parents in the dialogic group with children in mainstreamed

10 Dialogic Reading 91 settings. Those parents generally felt satisfied with the program; all of them liked this intervention. They also rated their children s interest in reading during the intervention, and the mean score was 8.2 out of 10 (10 indicates the most interested while 1 indicates the least interested), which demonstrates that most of the children enjoyed the program, at least as rated by their parents. Discussion The primary purpose of the present study was to investigate whether the dialogic intervention program was effective in improving the receptive vocabulary skills of deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Our results extend the findings of Whitehurst et al. (1988, 1994) by demonstrating that the dialogic reading technique can work for Hong Kong deaf and hard-of-hearing children. In addition, children in the dialogic reading group produced the largest improvements in receptive vocabulary learning among the three groups, controlling for age and the degree of hearing loss. The experimental design of the present study was based on Whitehurst s previous studies (1988, 1994) on dialogic reading research. However, the present study used extra picture cards that had not been used in Whitehurst s previous studies as an adaptation for the deaf and hard-of-hearing children. The effectiveness and importance of these cards is discussed later in this section. Compared to the past research on dialogic reading, the findings of the present study showed similarity and further supported the usefulness of the dialogic reading intervention. Whitehurst et al. (1988) and Huebner (2000) both indicated that the group differences of the PPVT III favored the dialogic reading group although no significant statistics were obtained. Valdez-Menchaca and Whitehurst (1992) showed a significant gain in receptive vocabulary skills as indicated by PPVT III for the children in the dialogic reading group. The results in the present study, therefore, were very similar to those of western studies; deaf and hard-of-hearing children in the dialogic reading group showed a relatively large improvement on the receptive vocabulary test. Greater gain in receptive vocabulary skills in the dialogic group was found compared to the other two groups, and the typical reading group did not show greater improvement compared to the control group. These results highlight the importance of the interactive nature of parent-child reading and indicate that typical reading does not fully exploit the potential of parent-child reading. The main component in facilitating children s language improvement is not only reading per se, but also the parent-child interaction. The comparative success of the dialogic reading intervention on deaf and hard-of-hearing children in the present study can be attributed to several factors. First, high-quality parent-child interaction is one important factor in the success of dialogic reading program for deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Past research has found that deaf and hard-of-hearing children (3 to 5 years old) with hearing parents, when compared to hearing peers, tended to communicate less, use fewer spontaneous communications (i.e., communications that are not a direct response to maternal communication), and be less responsive to maternal directives (Cross, Nienhuys, & Kirkman, 1985; Henggeler & Cooper, 1983). Some researchers have proposed that these deficits might result from poor and inappropriate responding by hearing mothers towards their children s communicative attempts (Schlesinger, 1988). Mothers who are constantly correcting their children and controlling their activities are unlikely to have enjoyable conversations with their children. As a result, these children may withdraw from interactions and communication (Lederberg & Everhart, 2000). In contrast, in the dialogic reading program, parents asked different questions and engaged in interactive activities while reading. Children not only enjoyed the reading process, but also learned through the interaction with parents. Because most of the parents had received training of the dialogic reading skills before the actual intervention, they knew about the importance of the relaxing atmosphere and the interactive nature. This is particularly important for deaf and hard-of-hearing and is believed to be one of the key factors in the success of the dialogic reading intervention. Another possible reason for the success of the dialogic reading intervention for deaf and hardof-hearing children is that picture cards were used in

11 92 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 10:1 Winter 2005 the present study. Although deaf and hard-of-hearing children sometimes have poor knowledge of the semantics and syntax of the spoken language, they, like children without hearing difficulties, are able to use their visual modality to help them read and learn (Musselman, 2000). Multi-modal teaching may improve learning for all children, and our experiences in this study indicate that deaf and hard-of-hearing children appeared to benefit from our focus on both auditory and visual stimulation. In our dialogic reading intervention, parents used picture cards to guide the children to link novel vocabulary words and pictures. Learning was enhanced by having readers use their visual ability as a complement to their hearing ability. Previous studies have also demonstrated the usefulness of the use of pictorial materials in language teaching to deaf and hard-of-hearing children. For instance, in Walker, Munro, and Rickards (1998) study, the use of pictorial materials combined with written text was proven to be effective in making significant reading gains in prelingually deaf children (9 to 18 years old). Results of the present study provided additional support of the usefulness of pictorial materials (e.g., picture cards) in promoting language abilities among the deaf and hard-of-hearing. The present study demonstrated several important aspects of the experimental program of relevance to parents and educators. The success of the dialogic reading is only one of those. In addition, improvements were shown within a relatively short period of time. In the present study, parents and their children participated in the intervention program for only 8 weeks, but significant gains were produced in children s receptive vocabulary skills, and improvement in vocabulary skills should have long-term effects on children s language development. It is believed that if the intervention can be practiced for a longer period of time, the effect will be greater in quantity and quality. In addition, the dialogic reading method is valuable because it is easy to learn and practice (Whitehurst et al., 1988). Parents are not required to have a strong educational background in order to use it effectively. In our experience, even parents with an early primary educational level could learn the dialogic reading technique within a short period of time, without problems in actual practice during the 8-week intervention period. This is particularly important because parental involvement has been proven to be a key factor for children s language improvement (Moeller, 2000), and if parents can contribute to children s learning process, the gain by children will become greater. The training offered to the parents is also easy and requires only a short period of time. This can encourage more parents to join and enjoy the positive effect of dialogic reading. Dialogic reading can provide a positive learning environment at home. According to the feedback from the parents, children in the dialogic reading group were enthusiastic about this reading activity, and the parents believed that the dialogic reading intervention could raise children s interest in reading. Lyytinen, Laakso, and Poikkeus (1998) found that children s interest in reading is related to language development, suggesting that enthusiasm for shared book reading may promote language learning just as cognitive stimulation does. In other words, the dialogic reading intervention might indirectly improve children s language ability by raising children s interest in reading. Recommendations to Parents and Educators To maximize the effect of dialogic reading, it is important to consider the following. First, although the method is not age-limited, it is better to practice the method early, preferably when the deaf and hardof-hearing child is still in kindergarten. In Moeller s (2000) study, for example, results showed that hardof-hearing children who were enrolled early in an intervention program did approximately as well on language tests (e.g. PPVT III) as did their hearing counterparts. There is a strong continuity between the language skills with which children enter school and their later academic performances (Lonigan & Whitehurst, 1998). As a consequence, the earlier the intervention is, the earlier the effect may occur. Apart from the early intervention, the frequent use of pictorial materials is also important. As stated earlier, learning through pictures is particularly important for deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Therefore, parents are encouraged to use different picture cards, which can be any pictures parents can find. Combinations of the use of picture cards with the PEER sequence and the CROWD prompts are

12 Dialogic Reading 93 encouraged. Parents can even ask the children to draw their own pictures instead of using the given cards. Another point to be noted is that a relaxing atmosphere should be provided. Good parent-child interaction is likely the key factor of the success of the intervention. If parents continuously correct children s mistakes or blame them for their inactive responses during the process, positive communication is minimized. This will reduce the effectiveness of dialogic reading. As Whitehurst et al. (1988) suggested, dialogic reading is conversational reading between parents and children. Children will enjoy dialogic reading when parents follow children s interest and vary what they do from reading to reading. The most important theme should be Keep the reading fun. control of the children s language abilities at the beginning is recommended. In addition, there is a special characteristic in Cantonese dialogic reading intervention. Some spoken forms of vocabulary in Cantonese are different from the written form of Standard Chinese. Using the word little duck as an example, the Cantonese form is whereas the Standard Chinese form is. The prompts attached in the books in the present study were written in the Cantonese form, but the vocabularies that appeared in the books were written in the Standard Chinese form. It is not clear whether different forms affect children s language acquisition. Further study may be done to compare the use of these different forms in the dialogic reading skills. Limitations and Further Studies In the present study, only self-reported duration and frequency of reading was obtained and we had no objective information on the degree of implementation of the intervention. As the degree of implementation is an important factor for the successfulness of the program, it is recommended that tape recording or videotaping of the sessions can be done in future studies for some qualitative analysis. Also, owing to the exploratory nature of the present study, the sample size was small, and we included the PPVT III as the only measure of receptive vocabulary skills. Future studies can involve a larger sample size and other measures of vocabulary skills. The effects of dialogic reading on other language skills, such as expressive language and syntactic skills, on deaf and hardof-hearing children can also be explored. In addition, the pretest score of PPVT III of the dialogic reading group was somewhat higher than the other two groups, though this difference was not statistically significant in the present study. The development of individual differences in language ability might be accounted by the Matthew Effect, the idea that children who start out better will improve more quickly over time, and those who start out poorer will improve at a relatively slow rate (e.g., Stanovich, 1986). We cannot rule out the possibility that greater improvement of the dialogic reading condition is affected by the Matthew effect in the present study. Therefore, further study with better Conclusion In conclusion, the present study has successfully demonstrated the effectiveness of dialogic reading intervention on deaf and hard-of-hearing children s receptive vocabulary skills. More importantly, the relationship is a causal one. Although we cannot distinguish whether the effect was caused by dialogic reading per se or the use of pictorial materials, the combination of the two improved receptive vocabulary skills among deaf and hard-of-hearing children over an 8-week intervention. Such improvements in vocabulary skills may also facilitate language development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children in a long run. Given the ease with which the dialogic reading technique can be learned, educators and parents might consider using this technique to further facilitate positive learning in children. Acknowledgment This project was supported by Direct Grant No from the Chinese University of Hong Kong. Sincere thanks are given to Mr. Yiu from the Special Education Resources Center of the Education Department, Ms. Lai, the principal of the Hong Kong School for the Deaf, and Ms. Choi from the same school.

13 94 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 10:1 Winter 2005 Appendix A Example of picture cards for the dialogic reading group References Bodner-Johnson, B. (1986). The family environment and achievement of deaf students: A discriminant analysis. Exceptional Children, 52, Bus, A. G., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission literacy. Review of Education Research, 65, Calderon, R. (2000). Parental involvement in deaf children s education programs as a predictor of child s language, early reading, and social-emotional development. Journal of Deaf studies and Deaf Education, 5(2), Calderon, R., & Greenberg, M. (1993). Considerations in the adaptation of families with school-aged deaf children. In M. Marschark & D. Clarks (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on deafness. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Calderon, R., Greenberg, M., & Kusche, C. (1991). The influence of family coping on the cognitive and social skills of deaf children. In D. Martin (Ed.), Advances in cognition, education, and deafness (pp ). Washington, DC: Gallaudet Press. Carney, A. E., & Moeller, M. P. (1998). Treatment efficacy: Hearing loss in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(1), Chan, L., Cole, P., & Morris, J. (1990). Effects of instruction on the use of visual imagery strategy on the reading comprehension competence of disabled and average readers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 13(1), Chow, B. W.-Y., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Promoting language and literacy through parent-child reading in Hong King preschoolers. Early Education and Development, 14(2), Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2 nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Crain-Thoreson, C., & Dale, P. S. (1999). Enhancing linguistic performance: Parents and teachers as book reading partners for children with language delays. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 19, Cross, T. G., Nienhuys, T. G., & Kirkman, M. (1985). Parentchild interaction with receptively disabled children: Some determinants of maternal speech style. In Z. Reger & K. E. Nelson (Eds.), Children s language, Vol. 5, (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Davis, J., Elfenbein, J., Schum, R., & Bentier, R. (1986). Effects of mild and moderate hearing impairments on language, educational, and psychosocial behavior of children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, Delk, L., & Weidekamp, L. (2001, April). Shared reading project: Evaluating implementation processes and family outcomes. Laurent Cleric National Deaf Education Center. Retrieved August 22, 2004, from gallaudet.edu/products/sharing-results/sharedreading Project/index.html De Temple, J., & Snow, C. E. (2003). Learning words from books. In A. van Kleeck, S. A. Stahl, and E. B. Bauer (Eds.), On reading books to children: Parents and Teachers (pp ). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Third Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Ezell, H. K., Justice, L. M., & Parsons, D. (2000). Enhancing the emergent literacy skills of pre-schoolers with communication disorders: A pilot investigation. Chinese Language Teaching and Therapy, 16(2), Hargrave, A. C., & Sénéchal, M. (2000). A book reading intervention with preschool children who have limited vocabularies: The benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15(1), Henggeler, S. W., & Cooper, P. F. (1983). Deaf child-hearing mother interaction: Extensiveness and reciprocity. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 8(1), Huebner, C. E. (2000). Promoting toddlers language development through community-based intervention. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21(5), Kretchmer, R., & Kretchmer, L. (1978). Language development and intervention with the hearing-impaired. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press. Laurent Clerc National Deaf Education Center. (2004). Literacy: Shared reading project. Retrieved October 22, 2004, from Laughton, J. (1989). The learning disabled, deaf and hard-ofhearing student: Reality, myth, or overextension? Topics in Language Disorders, 9,

Implementation of dialogic reading techniques by teachers of the deaf

Implementation of dialogic reading techniques by teachers of the deaf Washington University School of Medicine Digital Commons@Becker Independent Studies and Capstones Program in Audiology and Communication Sciences 2013 Implementation of dialogic reading techniques by teachers

More information

Interactive Storybook Reading: Strategies That Work Across Settings

Interactive Storybook Reading: Strategies That Work Across Settings Interactive Storybook Reading: Strategies That Work Across Settings Janna Hasko-Dungan M.S. University of Arizona Doctoral Student NLCSD Scholar dunagan@email.arizona.edu Joy Kane Idaho Educational Services

More information

Sign Language Fun in the Early Childhood Classroom

Sign Language Fun in the Early Childhood Classroom Sign Language Fun in the Early Childhood Classroom Enrich Language and Literacy Skills of Young Hearing Children, Children with Special Needs, and English Language Learners by Sherrill B. Flora My name

More information

FOUNDATIONS FOR LITERACY: AN EARLY LITERACY INTERVENTION FOR DHH CHILDREN

FOUNDATIONS FOR LITERACY: AN EARLY LITERACY INTERVENTION FOR DHH CHILDREN FOUNDATIONS FOR LITERACY: AN EARLY LITERACY INTERVENTION FOR DHH CHILDREN Amy Lederberg, Susan Easterbrooks, Stacey Tucci, Victoria Burke, Elizabeth Miller Georgia State University Carol Connor, Arizona

More information

SIGN LANGUAGE IN CO-ENROLLMENT EDUCATION FOR DEAF CHILDREN

SIGN LANGUAGE IN CO-ENROLLMENT EDUCATION FOR DEAF CHILDREN SIGN LANGUAGE IN CO-ENROLLMENT EDUCATION FOR DEAF CHILDREN Gladys Tang and Chris, Kun-Man Yiu, Centre for Sign Linguistics and Deaf Studies, Chinese University of Hong Kong ABSTRACT When inclusive education

More information

CLASSROOM PARTICIPATION OF DEAF AND HARD OF HEARING STUDENTS IN A SIGN BILINGUALISM AND CO-ENROLLMENT (SLCO) EDUCATION SETTING

CLASSROOM PARTICIPATION OF DEAF AND HARD OF HEARING STUDENTS IN A SIGN BILINGUALISM AND CO-ENROLLMENT (SLCO) EDUCATION SETTING CLASSROOM PARTICIPATION OF DEAF AND HARD OF HEARING STUDENTS IN A SIGN BILINGUALISM AND CO-ENROLLMENT (SLCO) EDUCATION SETTING Chris Kun-man Yiu, Centre for Sign Linguistics and Deaf Studies, Department

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

Purpose and Objectives of Study Objective 1 Objective 2 Objective 3 Participants and Settings Intervention Description Social peer networks

Purpose and Objectives of Study Objective 1 Objective 2 Objective 3 Participants and Settings Intervention Description Social peer networks 1 Title: Autism Peer Networks Project: Improving Social-Communication and Literacy for Young Children with ASD Funding: Institute of Education Sciences R324A090091 Session Presenters: Debra Kamps and Rose

More information

Shared Reading Project: Evaluating Implementation Processes and Family Outcomes

Shared Reading Project: Evaluating Implementation Processes and Family Outcomes Shared Reading Project: Evaluating Implementation Processes and Family Outcomes by Linda Delk, Ph.D., and Lisa Weidekamp, B.S.W. Laurent Clerc National Deaf Education Center Gallaudet University, Washington,

More information

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby A Fully Integrated Platform for Observation, Daily Reporting, Communications and Assessment For Early Childhood Professionals and the Families that they Serve Alignment

More information

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Marci Laurel, MA, CCC-SLP mlaurel@salud.unm.edu and Services (UCEDD) 1 Training Objectives Participants will: 1. Name at least three communication

More information

Family-centered early intervention for families and children who are deaf or hard of hearing

Family-centered early intervention for families and children who are deaf or hard of hearing Family-centered early intervention for families and children who are deaf or hard of hearing Christine Yoshinaga-Itano, Ph.D. University of Colorado, Boulder Best Practice Matters Family-centered early

More information

Keeping Autism in Mind

Keeping Autism in Mind Page 1 of 7 With a clear understanding of the nature of Autism there is much that can be done to help people with Autism through their everyday life. In highlighting some of the features of Autism this

More information

Early Educational Placement and Later Language Outcomes for Children With Cochlear Implants

Early Educational Placement and Later Language Outcomes for Children With Cochlear Implants Otology & Neurotology 31:1315Y1319 Ó 2010, Otology & Neurotology, Inc. Early Educational Placement and Later Language Outcomes for Children With Cochlear Implants *Jean Sachar Moog and Ann E. Geers *Moog

More information

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Deaf-blindness

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Deaf-blindness [Slide 1] Welcome Welcome to the New Mexico TEAM technical assistance module on making eligibility determinations under the category of deaf-blindness. This module will review the guidance of the NM TEAM

More information

Hearing, Deaf, and Hard-of-Hearing Students Satisfaction with On-Line Learning

Hearing, Deaf, and Hard-of-Hearing Students Satisfaction with On-Line Learning Hearing, Deaf, and Hard-of-Hearing Students Satisfaction with On-Line Learning By James R. Mallory, M.S. Professor Applied Computer Technology Department jrmnet@rit.edu Gary L. Long, PhD, Associate Professor

More information

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism

New Mexico TEAM Professional Development Module: Autism [Slide 1]: Welcome Welcome to the New Mexico TEAM technical assistance module on making eligibility determinations under the category of autism. This module will review the guidance of the NM TEAM section

More information

Attitudes, Accommodations and Advocacy, Oh My!

Attitudes, Accommodations and Advocacy, Oh My! Attitudes, Accommodations and Advocacy, Oh My! Must Reads Classroom Acoustics Impact on Listening and Learning https://successforkidswithhearingloss.com/for-professionals/classroom-acoustics-impact-on-listening-and-learning/

More information

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER SERIES. Strategies for Social Skills for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER SERIES. Strategies for Social Skills for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER SERIES Strategies for Social Skills for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder Introduction One of the most important areas for intervention for children with autism will be in

More information

Making ASL/English Bilingualism Work in Your Home -A Guide for Parents and their EI Providers-

Making ASL/English Bilingualism Work in Your Home -A Guide for Parents and their EI Providers- Making ASL/English Bilingualism Work in Your Home -A Guide for Parents and their EI Providers- Created by Cassandra Lucas-Szumigala August 2017 Table of Contents Section 1: ASL, English, & Bilingualism:

More information

Recommendations for Jeevan Gnanodaya School for the Deaf Sarika D. Mehta (sarikadmehta AT gmail DOT com)

Recommendations for Jeevan Gnanodaya School for the Deaf Sarika D. Mehta (sarikadmehta AT gmail DOT com) Recommendations for Jeevan Gnanodaya School for the Deaf Sarika D. Mehta (sarikadmehta AT gmail DOT com) I spent two days (Feb 7 and 8, 2010) observing classes at JG. I observed all classes 1-10 th standards,

More information

Overview of research findings on word decoding in deaf children

Overview of research findings on word decoding in deaf children Academic excellence for business and the professions Overview of research findings on word decoding in deaf children Putting Research into Practice Workshop May 2016 Fiona Kyle Fiona.kyle.1@city.ac.uk

More information

Coordinated Family Services Plan

Coordinated Family Services Plan Page 1 of 8 Dear family, Creating a (CFSP) is a family-centred process. The goal of the CFSP is to: 1. Document shared goals for your child/family in a written/visual format; 2. Support communication between

More information

1. Placement of students in the appropriate program is not based on

1. Placement of students in the appropriate program is not based on 179 CON C L U S ION S An analysis of the research on communication methods used in educating the deaf showed that: 1. Placement of students in the appropriate program is not based on professional diagnosis

More information

DATA Model Skills Checklist: Curriculum Crosswalk

DATA Model Skills Checklist: Curriculum Crosswalk APPENDIX B DATA Model Skills Checklist: Curriculum Crosswalk This document is a crosswalk of the DATA Model Skills Checklist with the Assessment, Evaluation, Programming System (AEPS). The crosswalk with

More information

Optimizing Conceptual Understanding and Literacy Development: The Role of Sign Language

Optimizing Conceptual Understanding and Literacy Development: The Role of Sign Language Optimizing Conceptual Understanding and Literacy Development: The Role of Sign Language Christopher Kurz, Ph.D. & Susan Lane-Outlaw, Ph.D. Public Talk Centre for Sign Linguistics and Deaf Studies 14 AUG

More information

Identifying Critical Delays in Primary Language Development:

Identifying Critical Delays in Primary Language Development: Identifying Critical Delays in Primary Language Development: Data from Infants and School Age Deaf Children ACE-DHH Conference February 18, 2010 Pamela Luft Kent State University Research Objectives To

More information

Critical Review: Late Talkers : What Can We Expect?

Critical Review: Late Talkers : What Can We Expect? Critical Review: Late Talkers : What Can We Expect? Ian Gallant M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate Western University: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders This critical review examines two specific questions

More information

A Randomized- Controlled Trial of Foundations for Literacy

A Randomized- Controlled Trial of Foundations for Literacy A Randomized- Controlled Trial of Foundations for Literacy Dr. Stacey Tucci Georgia Pathway July 2018 Texas Statewide Conference on Education of the Deaf (SWCED) Funded by US Dept of Education Institute

More information

TExES Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing (181) Test at a Glance

TExES Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing (181) Test at a Glance TExES Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing (181) Test at a Glance See the test preparation manual for complete information about the test along with sample questions, study tips and preparation resources. Test Name

More information

IMPACT OF SLCO PROGRAMME VIEWS FROM DEAF TEACHERS. Mr Ricky Sung, Miss Lucia Chow, Mr Chris Yiu, Miss Anna Pun

IMPACT OF SLCO PROGRAMME VIEWS FROM DEAF TEACHERS. Mr Ricky Sung, Miss Lucia Chow, Mr Chris Yiu, Miss Anna Pun IMPACT OF SLCO PROGRAMME VIEWS FROM DEAF TEACHERS Mr Ricky Sung, Miss Lucia Chow, Mr Chris Yiu, Miss Anna Pun Acknowledgement Introduction Jockey Club Sign Bilingualism and Co-enrolment Deaf Education

More information

Inclusive Education. De-mystifying Intellectual Disabilities and investigating best practice.

Inclusive Education. De-mystifying Intellectual Disabilities and investigating best practice. Inclusive Education De-mystifying Intellectual Disabilities and investigating best practice. Aims for this session: To understand what the term Intellectual Defiency means To understand the broad spectrum

More information

THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK THE COLLEGE OF STATEN ISLAND Department of World Languages and Literatures American Sign Language Program

THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK THE COLLEGE OF STATEN ISLAND Department of World Languages and Literatures American Sign Language Program THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK THE COLLEGE OF STATEN ISLAND Department of World Languages and Literatures American Sign Language Program ASL 114--AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE II Instructor: Email: Office: 2S--208

More information

Communication Options and Opportunities. A Factsheet for Parents of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children

Communication Options and Opportunities. A Factsheet for Parents of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children Communication Options and Opportunities A Factsheet for Parents of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children This factsheet provides information on the Communication Options and Opportunities available to Deaf

More information

Professor Brenda Schick

Professor Brenda Schick Fingerspelling and Phonological Awareness: An Intervention to Improve Phonological Awareness in Fingerspelling as an Alternative Pathway to Literacy in Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Children Professor Brenda

More information

Good Communication Starts at Home

Good Communication Starts at Home Good Communication Starts at Home It is important to remember the primary and most valuable thing you can do for your deaf or hard of hearing baby at home is to communicate at every available opportunity,

More information

Areas to Address with All Families

Areas to Address with All Families Areas to Address with All Families Whose Infants/Toddlers are Identified as Deaf or Hard of Hearing A Checklist and Guide for Family Resources Coordinators and Early Intervention Services Providers This

More information

Running Head: Overcoming Social and Communication Barriers 1

Running Head: Overcoming Social and Communication Barriers 1 Running Head: Overcoming Social and Communication Barriers 1 Overcoming Social and Communication Barriers for Students with Deaf-Blindness Lauren Bethune University of North Florida Running Head: Overcoming

More information

Working with students with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Harvey County Autism Team November 25, 2013

Working with students with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Harvey County Autism Team November 25, 2013 Working with students with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Harvey County Autism Team November 25, 2013 Life with Autism: In Their Own Words http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zfzqb CC30as Why Autism is a Gift

More information

SIGN BILINGUALISM IN MAINSTREAM DEAF EDUCATION IN HK (2006-NOW)

SIGN BILINGUALISM IN MAINSTREAM DEAF EDUCATION IN HK (2006-NOW) SIGN BILINGUALISM IN MAINSTREAM DEAF EDUCATION IN HK (2006-NOW) Centre for Sign Linguistics and Deaf Studies, The Chinese University of Hong Kong 14 August 2015 Current situation of deaf education in Hong

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs DSM-IV to DSM-5: Primary Changes Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs Laurie Swineford, PhD CCC-SLP Washington State University DSM-IV Previously we used the

More information

Director of Testing and Disability Services Phone: (706) Fax: (706) E Mail:

Director of Testing and Disability Services Phone: (706) Fax: (706) E Mail: Angie S. Baker Testing and Disability Services Director of Testing and Disability Services Phone: (706)737 1469 Fax: (706)729 2298 E Mail: tds@gru.edu Deafness is an invisible disability. It is easy for

More information

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: In school-aged children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), what oral narrative elements differ from their typically developing peers? Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University

More information

Executive Functioning

Executive Functioning Executive Functioning What is executive functioning? Executive functioning is a process of higher brain functioning that is involved in goal directed activities. It is the part of the brain that enables

More information

Brinton & Fujiki Brigham Young University Social Communication Intervention Script for The Easter Bunny s Assistant

Brinton & Fujiki Brigham Young University Social Communication Intervention Script for The Easter Bunny s Assistant Brinton & Fujiki Brigham Young University Social Communication Intervention Script for The Easter Bunny s Assistant The Easter Bunny s Assistant by Jan Thomas, 2012, Harper, New York. Sharing a Story to

More information

SLP s Personal Shift in Language Pedagogy as a Parent of a Deaf Child Benefits of Speech and Sign

SLP s Personal Shift in Language Pedagogy as a Parent of a Deaf Child Benefits of Speech and Sign SLP s Personal Shift in Language Pedagogy as a Parent of a Deaf Child Benefits of Speech and Sign Lynn Gold, M.A. Speech Language Pathologist Anaheim School District, CA Erika Thompson, M.A., Community

More information

Disclosure statement

Disclosure statement Early verbal working memory as a predictor of later language outcomes in late talkers Jayne Newbury, Thomas Klee, Stephanie Stokes and Catherine Moran University of Canterbury, New Zealand Disclosure statement

More information

Web-Based Radio Show. Structure and Behavioral Goals of the DIR /Floortime Program

Web-Based Radio Show. Structure and Behavioral Goals of the DIR /Floortime Program Web-Based Radio Show Structure and Behavioral Goals of the DIR /Floortime Program Stanley I. Greenspan, M.D. July 2, 2008 Welcome to our Web-based Radio Show. The title of today s show is, The Structure

More information

TIPS FOR TEACHING A STUDENT WHO IS DEAF/HARD OF HEARING

TIPS FOR TEACHING A STUDENT WHO IS DEAF/HARD OF HEARING http://mdrl.educ.ualberta.ca TIPS FOR TEACHING A STUDENT WHO IS DEAF/HARD OF HEARING 1. Equipment Use: Support proper and consistent equipment use: Hearing aids and cochlear implants should be worn all

More information

Sign Language and Early Childhood Development

Sign Language and Early Childhood Development University of Arkansas, Fayetteville ScholarWorks@UARK Rehabilitation, Human Resources and Communication Disorders Undergraduate Honors Theses Rehabilitation, Human Resources and Communication Disorders

More information

Signing High School Science

Signing High School Science 1 Signing High School Science Judy Vesel TERC, Inc. Judy_Vesel@terc.edu Abstract This paper discusses evaluation research conducted by TERC, Inc. in classrooms with students in grades 9 12 who are deaf

More information

3/16/2016. Learning Objectives. Benefits of Bilingual/Bimodal Preschool Programming. Bilingual/Bimodal Inclusive Early Childhood Program

3/16/2016. Learning Objectives. Benefits of Bilingual/Bimodal Preschool Programming. Bilingual/Bimodal Inclusive Early Childhood Program Bilingual/Bimodal Inclusive Early Childhood Program. Maine Educational Center for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Learning Objectives will be able to explain the benefits of bilingual bimodal programming

More information

This dissertation is available at Iowa Research Online:

This dissertation is available at Iowa Research Online: University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Theses and Dissertations 2008 The effects of a fluent signing narrator in the Iowa E-Book on deaf children's acquisition of vocabulary, book related concepts, and

More information

British Sign Language Level 2 Online Course Evening

British Sign Language Level 2 Online Course Evening British Sign Language Level 2 Online Course Evening Thank you for requesting an information guide from Teach Me Sign. Please enjoy! About Teach Me Sign... At Teach Me Sign we aim to make British Sign Language

More information

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers!

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers! Marisa Filipe Dezembro de 2013 pdpsi10020@fpce.up.pt EXCL/MHC-LIN/0688/2012 Summary Introduction: Prosodic Abilities in Children and Young Adults with Typical & Non-Typical Development Prosodic abilities

More information

Success for All. Curiosity Corner. Scope and Sequence Success for All Foundation. All rights reserved.

Success for All. Curiosity Corner. Scope and Sequence Success for All Foundation. All rights reserved. Success for All Scope and Sequence Curiosity Corner 2014 Success for All Foundation. All rights reserved. Contents Success for All Scope and Sequence Curiosity Corner A Comprehensive reschool rogram...

More information

Characteristics of the Text Genre Nonfi ction Text Structure Three to eight lines of text in the same position on each page

Characteristics of the Text Genre Nonfi ction Text Structure Three to eight lines of text in the same position on each page LESSON 14 TEACHER S GUIDE by Karen J. Rothbardt Fountas-Pinnell Level J Nonfiction Selection Summary Children with special needs use a variety of special tools to help them see and hear. This simply written

More information

Learning to use a sign language

Learning to use a sign language 85 Chapter 8 Learning to use a sign language It is easy for a young child to learn a complete sign language. A child will first begin to understand the signs that others use, especially for people and

More information

Cued Speech and Cochlear Implants: Powerful Partners. Jane Smith Communication Specialist Montgomery County Public Schools

Cued Speech and Cochlear Implants: Powerful Partners. Jane Smith Communication Specialist Montgomery County Public Schools Cued Speech and Cochlear Implants: Powerful Partners Jane Smith Communication Specialist Montgomery County Public Schools Jane_B_Smith@mcpsmd.org Agenda: Welcome and remarks Cochlear implants how they

More information

What is the Role of the Hearing Specialist in Early Steps? Karen Anderson, PhD Coordinator of Early Intervention Services for Hearing and Vision

What is the Role of the Hearing Specialist in Early Steps? Karen Anderson, PhD Coordinator of Early Intervention Services for Hearing and Vision What is the Role of the Hearing Specialist in Early Steps? Karen Anderson, PhD Coordinator of Early Intervention Services for Hearing and Vision What is a Hearing Specialist? A person with expertise in

More information

COMPLEX LEARNING DIFFICULTIES AND DISABILITIES RESEARCH PROJECT (CLDD)

COMPLEX LEARNING DIFFICULTIES AND DISABILITIES RESEARCH PROJECT (CLDD) Page 1 What is sensory impairment? The term sensory impairment encompasses visual loss (including blindness and partial sight), hearing loss (including the whole range) and multisensory impairment (which

More information

A DIALOGIC STORYTELLING PROGRAM FOR MOTHERS OF 0-3 YEARS OLD DEAF CHILDREN THROUGH SOCIAL NETWORKS: AN IRANIAN EXPERIENCE

A DIALOGIC STORYTELLING PROGRAM FOR MOTHERS OF 0-3 YEARS OLD DEAF CHILDREN THROUGH SOCIAL NETWORKS: AN IRANIAN EXPERIENCE A DIALOGIC STORYTELLING PROGRAM FOR MOTHERS OF 0-3 YEARS OLD DEAF CHILDREN THROUGH SOCIAL NETWORKS: AN IRANIAN EXPERIENCE Movallali,Guita University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran,

More information

Inferences: What inferences about the hypotheses and questions can be made based on the results?

Inferences: What inferences about the hypotheses and questions can be made based on the results? QALMRI INSTRUCTIONS QALMRI is an acronym that stands for: Question: (a) What was the broad question being asked by this research project? (b) What was the specific question being asked by this research

More information

CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDENTS WHO ARE: DEAF OR HARD OF HEARING

CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDENTS WHO ARE: DEAF OR HARD OF HEARING CHARACTERISTICS OF STUDENTS WHO ARE: DEAF OR HARD OF HEARING 1. In General: An estimated twenty one million Americans have some degree of hearing loss, mild to severe. Of the 60,000+ students identified

More information

In this chapter, you will learn about the requirements of Title II of the ADA for effective communication. Questions answered include:

In this chapter, you will learn about the requirements of Title II of the ADA for effective communication. Questions answered include: 1 ADA Best Practices Tool Kit for State and Local Governments Chapter 3 In this chapter, you will learn about the requirements of Title II of the ADA for effective communication. Questions answered include:

More information

One or more genetic anomalies that affect brain development, such as Down syndrome

One or more genetic anomalies that affect brain development, such as Down syndrome Intellectual disability Hearing Definition Characteristics Causes Prevalence Potential Impact on Learning Formerly known as mental retardation, it is defined as an individual with an IQ lower than 70-75

More information

PRESCOTT UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT District Instructional Guide

PRESCOTT UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT District Instructional Guide Math Counting and Cardinality number count compare less more number name Begins rote counting (numbers 1 10) Begins counting with 1:1 correspondence Begins to recognize number names and symbols Begins

More information

From: What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015

From:   What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015 Overcoming Intellectual Disability and Autism to Achieve Vocational & Academic Success Pathway to Empowerment Objectives 1 2 4 Learn to distinguish between intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorders.

More information

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi We ll cover Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) ASD in young adults Social Communication (definition, components, importance,

More information

I. Language and Communication Needs

I. Language and Communication Needs Child s Name Date Additional local program information The primary purpose of the Early Intervention Communication Plan is to promote discussion among all members of the Individualized Family Service Plan

More information

Phonological awareness and early reading skills in children with cochlear implants

Phonological awareness and early reading skills in children with cochlear implants See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: http://www.researchgate.net/publication/262694779 Phonological awareness and early reading skills in children with cochlear implants

More information

Interpreter Preparation (IPP) IPP 101 ASL/Non-IPP Majors. 4 Hours. Prerequisites: None. 4 hours weekly (3-1)

Interpreter Preparation (IPP) IPP 101 ASL/Non-IPP Majors. 4 Hours. Prerequisites: None. 4 hours weekly (3-1) Interpreter Preparation (IPP) IPP 101 ASL/Non-IPP Majors 4 hours weekly (3-1) This course is designed for students who have no knowledge of American Sign Language. The focus of this course will be on developing

More information

Critical Review: What Presenting Speech and Language Characteristics of Late Talkers Distinguish Those Who Recover from Those Who Do Not?

Critical Review: What Presenting Speech and Language Characteristics of Late Talkers Distinguish Those Who Recover from Those Who Do Not? Critical Review: What Presenting Speech and Language Characteristics of Late Talkers Distinguish Those Who Recover from Those Who Do Not? Melissa Dumoulin M.Cl.Sc. Speech-Language Pathology Candidate University

More information

Interpreter Preparation (IPP) IPP 101 ASL/Non-IPP Majors. 4 Hours. Prerequisites: None. 4 hours weekly (3-1)

Interpreter Preparation (IPP) IPP 101 ASL/Non-IPP Majors. 4 Hours. Prerequisites: None. 4 hours weekly (3-1) Interpreter Preparation (IPP) IPP 101 ASL/Non-IPP Majors Prerequisites: None 4 hours weekly (3-1) This course is designed for students who have no knowledge of American Sign Language. The focus of this

More information

Bringing Your A Game: Strategies to Support Students with Autism Communication Strategies. Ann N. Garfinkle, PhD Benjamin Chu, Doctoral Candidate

Bringing Your A Game: Strategies to Support Students with Autism Communication Strategies. Ann N. Garfinkle, PhD Benjamin Chu, Doctoral Candidate Bringing Your A Game: Strategies to Support Students with Autism Communication Strategies Ann N. Garfinkle, PhD Benjamin Chu, Doctoral Candidate Outcomes for this Session Have a basic understanding of

More information

It is also possible to have a mixed hearing loss, which arises from both the above.

It is also possible to have a mixed hearing loss, which arises from both the above. 1 EARLY INCLUSION THROUGH LEARNING FROM EACH OTHER BRIEFING SHEETS PACK SENSORY IMPAIRMENT This is a general overview of the implications of vision impairment, hearing impairment and multi-sensory impairment.

More information

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc Evaluating Educational Services for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders Erik Mayville, Ph.D., BCBA-D The Institute for Educational Planning, LLC Connecticut Center for Child Development, Inc. & mayville@iepinc.org

More information

D/hh students literacy development in the SLCO Programme

D/hh students literacy development in the SLCO Programme D/hh students literacy development in the SLCO Programme Qun LI, Gladys TANG, Chris YIU, Scholastica LAM Jockey Club Sign Bilingualism and Co-enrolment in Deaf Education Programme Centre for Sign Linguistics

More information

Bilingualism: Part II. ASL/English Bilingual Education. ASHA Convention 2006 Susanne Scott

Bilingualism: Part II. ASL/English Bilingual Education. ASHA Convention 2006 Susanne Scott Bilingualism: Part II ASL/English Bilingual Education ASHA Convention 2006 Susanne Scott Laurent Clerc National Deaf Education Center Gallaudet University Agenda Who We Are Language Planning at the Laurent

More information

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1

Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1 Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1 Please use the following citation when referencing this work: McGill, R. J. (2013). Test review: Children s Psychological Processing Scale (CPPS). Journal of Psychoeducational

More information

Interviewer: Tell us about the workshops you taught on Self-Determination.

Interviewer: Tell us about the workshops you taught on Self-Determination. INTERVIEW WITH JAMIE POPE This is an edited translation of an interview by Jelica Nuccio on August 26, 2011. Jelica began by explaining the project (a curriculum for SSPs, and for Deaf-Blind people regarding

More information

DOES EARLY SIGN LANGUAGE INPUT MAKE A DIFFERENCE ON DEAF CHILDREN WITH AUDITORY BRAINSTEM IMPLANTS?

DOES EARLY SIGN LANGUAGE INPUT MAKE A DIFFERENCE ON DEAF CHILDREN WITH AUDITORY BRAINSTEM IMPLANTS? The 2014 Symposium on Sign Bilingualism and Deaf Education DOES EARLY SIGN LANGUAGE INPUT MAKE A DIFFERENCE ON DEAF CHILDREN WITH AUDITORY BRAINSTEM IMPLANTS? Chris K.-M. YIU, Emily LAM, Tammy LAU Jockey

More information

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) DSM-5 (Criteria and Major Changes for SLP-Related Conditions) Individuals meeting the criteria will be given a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder with three levels of severity based on degree of support

More information

Allen Independent School District Bundled LOTE Curriculum Beginning 2017 School Year ASL III

Allen Independent School District Bundled LOTE Curriculum Beginning 2017 School Year ASL III Allen Independent School District Bundled LOTE Curriculum Beginning 2017 School Year ASL III Page 1 of 19 Revised: 8/1/2017 114.36. American Sign Language, Level III (One Credit), Adopted 2014. (a) General

More information

A New Era for the Identification and Treatment of Children with Auditory Disorders

A New Era for the Identification and Treatment of Children with Auditory Disorders 1 A New Era for the Identification and Treatment of Children with Auditory Disorders ROSS J. ROESER AND MARION P. DOWNS Nothing great was ever achieved without enthusiasm. Ralph Waldo Emerson It is a new

More information

Life History Screen. a. Were you raised by someone other than your biologic/birth parents? Yes No

Life History Screen. a. Were you raised by someone other than your biologic/birth parents? Yes No Childhood History 1. Childhood History Life History Screen a. Were you raised by someone other than your biologic/birth parents? b. How many living situations (different primary caregivers) did you have

More information

Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome

Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome Jessica Klusek, MS CCC-SLP FPG Child Development Institute (FPG) University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (UNC) Molly Losh, PhD Northwestern

More information

Speak Out! Sam Trychin, Ph.D. Copyright 1990, Revised Edition, Another Book in the Living With Hearing Loss series

Speak Out! Sam Trychin, Ph.D. Copyright 1990, Revised Edition, Another Book in the Living With Hearing Loss series Speak Out! By Sam Trychin, Ph.D. Another Book in the Living With Hearing Loss series Copyright 1990, Revised Edition, 2004 Table of Contents Introduction...1 Target audience for this book... 2 Background

More information

Meeting someone with disabilities etiquette

Meeting someone with disabilities etiquette Meeting someone with disabilities etiquette Many people unsure how to go about meeting someone with a disability because they don t want to say or do the wrong thing. Here are a few tips to keep in mind

More information

Potential Outcomes for Children Who Are Deaf-Blind with Cochlear Implants

Potential Outcomes for Children Who Are Deaf-Blind with Cochlear Implants Facilitating Communication & Language for Children with Cochlear Implants and Vision Impairments EARLY HEARING DETECTION & INTERVENTION Addison, Texas March 10 th, 2009 Kathleen Stremel stremelk@wou.edu

More information

Don t wait-and-see, research suggests

Don t wait-and-see, research suggests Don t wait-and-see, research suggests By Lauren Lowry Hanen S-LP and Clinical Staff Writer Historically, intervening with the group of children known as late talkers has been the source of some debate

More information

China Summer Institute 2015 Connie Steinman Connecting Chinese & American Cultures Through Sign Language & Religious Gestures

China Summer Institute 2015 Connie Steinman Connecting Chinese & American Cultures Through Sign Language & Religious Gestures 1 China Summer Institute 2015 Connie Steinman Connecting Chinese & American Cultures Through Sign Language & Religious Gestures Organizing Questions: How is sign language used to communicate in an area

More information

Syllabus. ASL Level 3. Instructor: Deanne Bray-Kotsur (Video Relay Service) Class Meets: In Room I-7

Syllabus. ASL Level 3. Instructor: Deanne Bray-Kotsur (Video Relay Service) Class Meets: In Room I-7 Syllabus ASL Level 3 Instructor: Deanne Bray-Kotsur dbray@opusd.org 818-483-0411 (Video Relay Service) Class Meets: In Room I-7 August 8, 2017 to May 25, 2018 Course Description Signing Naturally, Units

More information

boys with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) while focusing on two main

boys with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) while focusing on two main Kathryn Smith EDUC 201: Educational Psychology Research Review March 2, 2009 Watkins, D. E., & Wentzel, K. R. (2008). Training boys with ADHD to work collaboratively: Social and learning outcomes. Contemporary

More information

Cochlear Implant Education Center

Cochlear Implant Education Center Cochlear Implant Education Center Cochlear Implants and Sign Language: Building Foundations for Effective Educational Practices Designing Programs and Services for Children with Cochlear Implants: The

More information

College of Education and Human Services Exceptional Student & Deaf Education Course Descriptions

College of Education and Human Services Exceptional Student & Deaf Education Course Descriptions CATALOG 2010-2011 Undergraduate Information College of Education and Human Services Exceptional Student & Deaf Education Course Descriptions ASL2140: American Sign Language I 4 This course in American

More information

Title Content Summary Audience

Title Content Summary Audience Title Content Summary Audience Teach Your Child How To Think by Edward de Bono Sound Practice By Lyn Layton & Karen Deeny More than Words By Fern Sussman Clear, easy to read chapters on the different thinking

More information

#8 Assessment: The competent teacher understands various formal and informal assessment

#8 Assessment: The competent teacher understands various formal and informal assessment #8 Assessment: The competent teacher understands various formal and informal assessment strategies and uses them to support the continuous development of all students. As a teacher I need to be able to

More information

2 form. Written in first person, it is a brief account describing how the student s hero, who is associated with his special interest, solves a proble

2 form. Written in first person, it is a brief account describing how the student s hero, who is associated with his special interest, solves a proble 1 Simple Strategies that Work: Tools for Success Brenda Smith Myles, Ph.D. Ziggurat Group Several strategies have been found effective in addressing the behavior problems of students with autism spectrum

More information

OHIO ASSESSMENTS FOR EDUCATORS (OAE) FIELD 044: SPECIAL EDUCATION SPECIALIST: DEAF/HARD OF HEARING

OHIO ASSESSMENTS FOR EDUCATORS (OAE) FIELD 044: SPECIAL EDUCATION SPECIALIST: DEAF/HARD OF HEARING OHIO ASSESSMENTS FOR EDUCATORS (OAE) FIELD 044: SPECIAL EDUCATION SPECIALIST: DEAF/HARD OF HEARING June 2013 Content Domain Range of Competencies Approximate Percentage of Assessment Score I. Understanding

More information