Although considerable work has been conducted on the speech

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Although considerable work has been conducted on the speech"

Transcription

1 Influence of Hearing Loss on the Perceptual Strategies of Children and Adults Andrea L. Pittman Patricia G. Stelmachowicz Dawna E. Lewis Brenda M. Hoover Boys Town National Research Hospital Omaha, NE To accommodate growing vocabularies, young children are thought to modify their perceptual weights as they gain experience with speech and language. The purpose of the present study was to determine whether the perceptual weights of children and adults with hearing loss differ from those of their normal-hearing counterparts. Adults and children with normal hearing and with hearing loss served as participants. Fricative and vowel segments within consonant-vowelconsonant stimuli were presented at randomly selected levels under two conditions: unaltered and with the formant transition removed. Overall performance for each group was calculated as a function of segment level. Perceptual weights were also calculated for each group using point-biserial correlation coefficients that relate the level of each segment to performance. Results revealed child-adult differences in overall performance and also revealed an effect of hearing loss. Despite these performance differences, the pattern of perceptual weights was similar across all four groups for most conditions. KEY WORDS: children, adults, speech perception, perceptual strategies Although considerable work has been conducted on the speech perception strategies of children with normal hearing, little is known about the perceptual strategies of children who must learn speech and language skills in the presence of hearing loss. We know from studies of children with normal hearing that perceptual strategies are refined as children gain experience with speech and language. For example, Nittrouer and Studdert-Kennedy (1987) found that younger children (3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds) pay particular attention to the acoustic properties of speech that specify vocal tract movement (e.g., formant transitions), whereas older children and adults attend more to the details of the acoustic signal (e.g., fricative noise). These findings, along with additional data, led to the Developmental Weighting Shift model of speech perception, which suggests that the early perceptual strategies of young children are effective as long as their vocabularies are not too large (Nittrouer, 1992, 1996; Nittrouer & Crowther, 1998; Nittrouer & Studdert-Kennedy, 1987). As more words are learned, children must shift their attention to details of the acoustic signal so that items can be represented more precisely. Several questions regarding perceptual strategies become apparent when one considers listeners with hearing loss. For example, what happens to the well-established perceptual strategies of adults when hearing loss is acquired later in life? Do these adults shift their attention 1276 Journal Journal of of Speech, Language, and and Hearing Research Vol. Vol December 2002 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association /02/

2 away from increasingly inaudible acoustic details to more audible but less informative ones (e.g., from a fricative noise to a formant transition), or do they attend to speech as they did when their hearing was normal? More importantly, how does hearing loss influence the development of perceptual strategies in children, and are those strategies optimal for perception? Finally, can perceptual strategies be altered as needed to accommodate changes in amplification systems, progressive hearing loss, and/or difficult listening situations (e.g., noise)? Although little direct evidence is available, two studies by Doherty and Lutfi (1996, 1999) suggest that the perceptual strategies of adults with hearing loss differ from those of adults with normal hearing. In the later study, the perceptual strategies of adults with and without hearing loss were examined for three target tones (0.25, 1, and 4 khz) presented in a six-tone complex. The level of each of the three target tones was randomly varied across trials at intensities sufficiently audible to accommodate the sloping high-frequency hearing losses of the listeners with hearing loss. The listeners were asked to indicate which of two complexes in a two-alternative forced choice task contained the more intense target tone. A correlational analysis was used to determine the relative weight given to each of the three tones. The performance of the adults with normal hearing was found to correlate best with the level of the 0.25-kHz tone, whereas the performance of the listeners with hearing loss correlated best with the level of the 4-kHz tone. In other words, the adults with normal hearing assigned more weight (or attended more) to the 0.25-kHz tone and the adults with hearing loss assigned more weight to the 4-kHz tone. The results of this and an earlier study by the same authors (Doherty & Lutfi, 1996) suggest that the listeners with hearing loss attended more to frequencies associated with cochlear damage even when more tonal information was available in frequency regions of normal or near normal hearing. Zeng and Turner (1990) studied fricative perception in 4 listeners with normal hearing and 3 listeners with hearing loss. Stimuli were four consonant-vowel syllables composed of voiceless fricatives (/s/, /S/, /f/, and /T/) in an /i/ context. To determine the relative contribution of the fricative noise to each syllable, the stimuli were first presented in their entirety and then presented without the vowel segment. All stimuli were presented at a number of levels, taking into consideration the thresholds of each listener. Performance for each group was calculated as a function of audibility. Results revealed that the adults with hearing loss performed almost as well as the listeners with normal hearing when identifying the fricative noise, suggesting that both groups recognized the aperiodic noise segment as meaningful to perception. When the audibility of the lower-frequency formant transition within each syllable was calculated, the results revealed substantially poorer performance for the adults with hearing loss, even though the formant transitions were more audible than the fricative segments. The authors concluded that adults with hearing loss might be unable to discriminate the rapid, dynamic spectral information provided in formant transitions. Unfortunately, little is known about the perceptual strategies of young children with hearing loss. Pittman and Stelmachowicz (2000) studied the strategies of older children (8 12 years old) and adults with normal hearing and with hearing loss. They used four vowel-consonant stimuli composed of the voiceless fricatives /s/, /S/, /f/, and /T/ in an /u/ vowel context. Each syllable was presented in its entirety, but the vowel, transition, and fricative segments were presented at randomly chosen levels. Identification of the fricative was correlated with the level of each segment using a correlational method similar to that of Doherty and Lutfi (1996). Although all four groups attended most to the fricative segments of /s/ and /S/, the strategies of the listeners with hearing loss with regard to both segments of /uf/ and /ut/ were poorly defined. In addition, the children with hearing loss showed poorer overall performance, suggesting that many of these children could not use their strategies as efficiently as their hearing- and age-matched counterparts. Although it would be ideal to address all of the questions noted earlier, they would exceed the scope of what can be answered in a single study. For that reason, the purpose of this study was to compare the perceptual strategies of children and adults with hearing loss with those of children and adults with normal hearing. Methods Listeners A total of 40 listeners participated in this study. Two groups of listeners with normal hearing consisted of 10 adults (20 44 years old, M = 28 years) and 10 children (5 8 years old, M = 6 years, 9 months). These listeners had hearing thresholds of 15 db or less at octave frequencies from 0.25 through 8 khz. Two groups of listeners with hearing loss consisted of 10 adults (49 66 years old, M = 59 years) and 10 children (5 10 years old, M = 8 years, 3 months). These listeners displayed high-frequency sloping sensorineural hearing losses. It was necessary to recruit somewhat older adults with hearing loss to obtain individuals with acquired hearing loss. It was also necessary to include several children with hearing loss older than 8 years to obtain an adequate number of children with homogeneous hearing losses that were congenital in nature. All of the children with hearing loss communicated orally and were performing at grade level in school. Figure 1 shows the Pittman et al.: Perceptual Strategies in Listeners With Hearing Loss 1277

3 mean pure-tone air-condition thresholds (±1 SD) for the children and adults with hearing loss. Stimuli Four consonant-vowel-consonant words were used as stimuli (sack, sock, shack, and shock). These words contained formant transitions unique to the combination of the fricatives and vowels. For example, the peak energy for a fricative /s/ produced by a male talker falls roughly at 5 khz, whereas the peak energy of /S/ falls around 3.5 khz. When these fricatives are followed by the vowels /A/ and /œ/, with second formant frequencies occurring typically at 1.7 and 0.85 khz, respectively, formant transitions unique to each fricative are produced. The intentional combination of these fricative and vowel segments allowed us to determine the weight given to the fricative and vowel segments through a series of decisions made by each listener. It was expected that the listeners would first dichotomize their responses according to one of the segments perceived (fricative or vowel) and then use their perceptual strategies to choose their final response. For example, if the vowel /œ/ was perceived, the listener would ignore the words sock and shock and choose between the words sack and shack based on their ability to use the fricative and/or the formant transition information. A listener having difficulty using formant transition information may dichotomize correctly but fail to choose the correct word. In general, this paradigm allowed us to present a number of response alternatives for meaningful words that differed slightly in acoustic properties. These stimuli were also useful for children in that the fricative-vowel segments became meaningful words to children by simply adding the final plosive /k/ to each syllable. Because the levels Figure 1. Mean pure-tone thresholds (±1 SD) in db HL for the hearing-impaired (HI) adults (open circles) and children (filled circles). of the segments were varied on each trial (described below), this paradigm also allowed us to determine the weight given to each segment along a parameter that is important to listeners with hearing loss, namely audibility (also described below). Each word was recorded by an adult male in a soundtreated room using a microphone with a flat frequency response to 10 khz (AKG Acoustics, C535 EB). The speech samples were amplified (Shure, M267), filtered at 10 khz (TDT, PF1), and digitized at a sampling rate of 22.5 khz. The initial fricative (/s/ or /S/), medial vowel (/A/ or /œ/), and final voiceless plosive (/k/) in each word were identified using a digital editing program (Cool Edit 96). The plosive /k/ and its preceding silent gap were replaced in each word with a single version of the phoneme produced by the same talker. These words were then used to create stimuli for two conditions: with formant transitions and without formant transitions. In the first condition, no changes were made to the fricative and vowel segments. In the second condition, the formant transitions were removed by replacing the /A/ and /œ/ vowel segments with the same vowels spoken in isolation by the same talker. Spectrograms of the stimuli in both conditions are shown in Figure 2. For the original stimuli (left panels), varying degrees of formant transitions are apparent. For example, a larger formant transition occurred for words beginning with /S/ than for /s/. For the stimuli without transitions (right panels), nearly flat formant frequencies are apparent for each stimulus. By removing the formant transition in the second condition, the listeners were forced to use the small spectral differences in the fricative segments for perception. This allowed us to determine the ability of each group to use different aspects of the stimuli when important acoustic information was removed. Procedure Listeners were seated in a sound-treated room and asked to identify each word presented monaurally through the same earphone used to obtain hearing thresholds (Sennheiser, HD-25). Listeners indicated the word they heard by touching the appropriate response box on a touch-screen monitor or by selecting it with a mouse. The response boxes were labeled with pictures of each word. Before testing, the children were asked to identify each picture to determine if the words were familiar to them. For children unfamiliar with a word, additional familiarization tasks were performed until each child was able to identify the words with ease. A computer program (MATLAB, Mathworks) was used to present the stimuli, provide reinforcement for correct responses, and store trial-by-trial data. For each trial, the program randomly selected the stimulus, as well as 1278 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol December 2002

4 separate amplitude levels for the fricative and vowel segments. That is, each word was presented in its entirety, but the separate amplitudes of the fricative and vowel segments within each word were varied from trial to trial. The amplitude of the final plosive /k/ did not vary. For the listeners with normal hearing, five levels were presented in 12-dB steps for a total range of 48 db. For the listeners with hearing loss, the stimuli were frequency shaped to compensate for each listener s hearing thresholds. Due to the reduced dynamic range of these listeners, presentation level, step size, and amplitude range were calculated individually to ensure signal audibility. These stimuli were presented in 5- to 12-dB step sizes, corresponding to a total amplitude range of 20 to 48 db. Fifteen repetitions of the stimuli were presented at each level for a total of 600 trials (4 words 5 segment levels 15 repetitions 2 listening conditions). Figure 3 illustrates the range of amplitude levels for the fricative and vowel segments (light and dark shaded areas, respectively) relative to the hearing thresholds of a listener with hearing loss (filled circles). In this example, the spectra represent the 1% peaks of speech analyzed in 1/3- octave bands. On any given trial, one level of each segment (one dotted and one dashed line) was selected from Figure 2. Spectrograms of the consonant-vowel-consonant stimuli in their original form (left panels) and without formant transitions (right panels). Pittman et al.: Perceptual Strategies in Listeners With Hearing Loss 1279

5 Figure 3. Example of amplitude variation for the fricative (dotted lines) and vowel (dashed lines) segments calculated for a hypothetical hearing loss (solid line). Figure 4. Graphical representation of a point-biserial correlation coefficient calculation. The short-term audibility (STA) of trial-bytrial data are plotted as a function of the response. Data are jittered to illustrate the density of responses at each STA. each shaded area and presented. To reduce spectral splatter at the segment boundaries due to potential discontinuities in amplitude, a moving-average filter was applied to the temporal amplification profile to obtain smooth changes in level between segments. The filter had low-pass characteristics with the 3 db point of its transfer function at 100 Hz. Calculating Perceptual Weight Because attention to specific segments of speech was of interest in the present study, perceptual weight was defined as a change in performance as a function of changing presentation level. Perceptual weight was quantified using a method similar to that of Pittman and Stelmachowicz (2000). Specifically, point-biserial correlation coefficients were calculated using the correct and incorrect responses of each listener relative to the level of each stimulus segment (fricative or vowel). This method is illustrated in Figure 4, where the presentation level of the trial-by-trial data is plotted as a function of the response (incorrect or correct). The slope of a line intersecting the mean presentation level in each column is the correlation coefficient. A positive correlation indicates that performance improved as the level of the segment increased, and a correlation near 0 indicates that performance remained unchanged regardless of the level of the segment. Although negative correlations did not occur, they would indicate a decrease in performance with increasing stimulus level. Short-Term Audibility To accurately compute the level of the stimulus segment relative to the hearing thresholds of each listener, a measure of short-term audibility (STA) was used. Measures of sensation level are typical for these applications; however, the calculation of STA includes values of importance for each of 17 frequency bands between 0.25 and 8 khz. In this way, both the listeners with hearing loss and with normal hearing could be compared using a metric intended for use with speech perception. Because the audibility of speech is typically calculated for continuous discourse, it was necessary to modify the procedure to calculate audibility for the short duration fricative and vowel segments. Each segment was first recorded in a 2-cm 3 coupler at 100 db SPL. The 1% peak amplitudes of each recorded segment were then measured in seventeen 1/3-octave bands (TOB) using a 20-ms Hanning window (50% overlap). Finally, STA was calculated using a modification of the Articulation Index (American National Standards Institute, 1969) for each of the 8 segments (4 words 2 segments) and five amplitude levels. The modified formula was: 17 STA = 1 Σ [TOB θ ] W i i i 20 i=1 where i is the number of the 1/3-octave band, TOB i is the 1% peak level for the ith 1/3-octave band in the stimulus segment (re: 2-cm 3 coupler), and θ i is the hearing threshold in db SPL referenced to the same coupler. 1 1 Hearing thresholds were measured at octave frequencies and interpolated across the seventeen 1/3-octave bands Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol December 2002

6 These values are multiplied by the importance value assigned to each band (W i ) and summed. The signal-tonoise ratio in each band (TOB i θ i ) as restricted to a range of 0 to 20 db so that when multiplied by 1/20, the resulting index ranged from 0.0 to 1.0. Importance functions for nonsense syllables were used in this calculation (American National Standards Institute, 1969). Figure 5. Mean performance for stimuli with formant transitions (filled bars) and without formant transitions (open bars) for the normal-hearing adults (NHA), normal-hearing children (NHC), hearing-impaired adults (HIA), and hearing-impaired children (HIC). Error bars represent ±1 standard deviation. Results Recall that the purpose of the present study was to compare the perceptual strategies of children and adults with hearing loss with those of children and adults with normal hearing. Initial data analyses revealed similar results for both /s/ stimuli (sack and sock) and for both /S/ stimuli (shack and shock); thus, the data were grouped by fricative. Figure 5 shows overall performance (±1 SD) as a function of group for the /s/ and /S/ stimuli (upper and lower panels, respectively). The solid bars represent the stimuli with formant transitions, and the open bars represent the stimuli without formant transitions. In general, better performance was achieved by all groups for the stimuli with formant transitions than for the stimuli without formant transitions except for the adults with hearing loss listening to the /s/ stimuli. Also, child-adult differences were apparent for the listeners with normal hearing for both stimuli and listening conditions. Similar results were apparent for the children and adults with hearing loss. An Analysis of Variance was conducted on percentcorrect performance to examine differences between groups, stimuli, and listening conditions. The betweensubjects factor was group (adults with normal hearing, children with normal hearing, adults with hearing loss, and children with hearing loss), and the within-subjects factors were listening condition (with and without formant transition) and fricative (/s/ and /S/). Results revealed significant main effects for listening condition [F(1, 36) = , p <.001] and fricative [F(1, 36) = , p =.001] as well as a listening condition fricative interaction [F(1, 36) = , p <.001]. Significant group effects also were found [F(3, 36) = , p =.001]. Post hoc testing using Scheffé s multiple comparisons revealed significantly poorer performance for the /s/ and /S/ stimuli by the children with normal hearing relative to that of the adults with normal hearing in both listening conditions. For the /S/ stimuli, the performance of the children with hearing loss was poorer than that of the adults with hearing loss for the stimuli with formant transitions; the adults with hearing loss performed more poorly than the adults with normal hearing for the stimuli without formant transitions. In general, these results revealed significant effects of fricative and listening condition as well as the effects of hearing loss and development. The perceptual weights that correspond to the performance shown in Figure 5 are illustrated in Figure 6. In this figure, the average correlation coefficients are shown (±1 SD) as a function of stimulus for each group. The results for the stimuli with and without formant transitions are given in the left and right panels, respectively. The filled and open bars represent the correlation coefficients for the fricative and vowel segments, respectively. Each solid and open bar in Figure 5 corresponds to the combined weight of the fricative and vowel segments displayed in Figure 6. Recall that a perceptual weight is calculated by correlating the Pittman et al.: Perceptual Strategies in Listeners With Hearing Loss 1281

7 Figure 6. Point-biserial correlation coefficients for the /s/ and /S/ stimuli plotted as a function of segment for the children and adults with normal hearing and with hearing loss. trial-by-trial STA levels with the response (correct or incorrect). Using this method, these weights indicate the relation between performance for the /s/ and /S/ words and the audibility of each segment. Specifically, the more performance improved as audibility of a segment increased, the higher the weight. For example, a correlation coefficient of.20 or less corresponded to less than a 30% change in performance across all levels of audibility. Correlation coefficients greater than.20 occurred for those segments where performance improved substantially more than 30% across the range of audibility levels. In terms of perceptual weights for the unaltered stimuli (left panels), each group appeared to rely heavily on the audibility of the vowel segment for perception, except for the children with hearing loss when the fricative /S/ was used. Surprisingly, children with hearing loss relied as heavily on the audibility of the fricative segment of the /S/ stimuli as on the vowel segment, suggesting that they were unable to use the dynamic formant transition of /S/ as efficiently as their age- and hearing-matched counterparts. When the formant transitions were removed (right panels), all groups appeared to rely heavily on the audibility of the vowel for the /s/ stimuli. In contrast, all groups shifted their attention to the fricative for the /S/ stimuli when the primary source of information was no longer available. Interestingly, the listeners with hearing loss appeared to rely more heavily on the audibility of the fricative segment than did the listeners with normal hearing. The markedly different perceptual weights for the /s/ and /S/ stimuli may be related to the magnitude of the formant transitions associated with the fricatives /s/ and /S/. Because the formant transitions for the /S/ stimuli were more extensive than for the /s/ stimuli, the absence of the formant information was more detrimental to perception. In general, these results suggest that the formant transitions in each word were important to perception and that the magnitude of the formant transitions influenced performance. It is tempting to try to relate the perceptual weights shown in Figure 6 to the overall performance shown in Figure 5. However, it is important to remember that point-biserial correlation coefficients do not represent overall performance but rather the relation between performance and audibility. This complex relation is illustrated in the performance-sta functions shown in Figure 7. In this figure, each function represents a best fit to the individual data for each group with respect to listening condition and segment. The squares and triangles represent the fricative and vowel segments, respectively. The open symbols indicate conditions where the transition was removed. Point-biserial correlation coefficients are related to the slopes of each function in that performance changed as a function of the audibility of each segment. However, overall performance cannot be predicted from the correlation coefficients. See, for example, the solid boxes and the open triangles representing the adults with hearing loss s data for the /S/ stimuli. In this example, the weights associated with these functions (0.20 and 0.08, respectively) are both relatively low, yet substantial differences in performance were observed. Although the performance-sta functions in Figure 7 provide an estimate of perceptual weights, the goodnessof-fit of each function must be considered. For these functions, goodness-of-fit varied widely, making it difficult to compare them across groups or conditions. Point-biserial correlations, however, can be considered more reliable estimates of perceptual weight because goodness-of-fit measures are not applicable to this calculation Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol December 2002

8 Figure 7. Performance-STA functions for the /s/ (left panels) and /S/ (right panels) stimuli plotted for each segment and listening condition. The filled and open symbols represent the stimuli with and without formant transitions, respectively. The squares and triangles represent the fricative and vowel segments, respectively. Discussion The purpose of the present study was to compare the perceptual strategies of children and adults with hearing loss with those of children and adults with normal hearing. It is important to remember that the term weight used in the present study does not imply a conscious decision on the part of the listener. Although many models of speech perception describe a series of decisions or stages, the use of this term is unrelated to those theories. It is also important to note that the perceptual weights reported in this study cannot be generalized to everyday communication. Instead, the strategies observed were sufficient for differentiating between the four stimuli presented in this study. More or fewer choices may have resulted in very different strategies. Even so, the perceptual weights demonstrated by each of the four groups may indicate which groups are able to accommodate less than perfect acoustic information. In general, the overall performance of the children with normal hearing was lower than that of the adults. This is consistent with a number of studies showing adultchild differences in performance (Hnath-Chisolm, Laipply, & Boothroyd, 1998; Neuman & Hockberg, 1983; Nozza, Rossman, & Bond, 1991; Stelmachowicz, Hoover, Lewis, Kortekaas, & Pittman, 2000). Although similar adult-child differences were observed for the listeners with hearing loss, these findings may have been influenced by the fact that the adults acquired their losses later in adulthood and the children had prelingual and early onset of hearing loss. The effect of hearing status on overall performance also was apparent in that the performance of the listeners with hearing loss was often poorer than that of their counterparts with normal hearing. Despite the differences in overall performance, similar perceptual weights were observed across all four groups for most stimuli. When the formant transitions were available, all four groups relied heavily on the audibility of the vowel. When the formant transitions were removed, each group continued to rely on the audibility of the vowel in the /s/ stimuli but shifted their attention to the fricative for the /S/ stimuli. The differences in overall performance across groups suggest additional effects of hearing loss independent of perceptual weights. The varying magnitude of the perceptual weights suggests that the audibility of certain segments relates more closely to performance than that of other segments. This was particularly true for the listeners with hearing loss for /S/, where removal of the formant transitions resulted in an increase in weight for the fricative. Although the results of previous studies suggest that adults with hearing loss may not be able to discriminate the rapid, dynamic spectral information in a formant transition (Zeng & Turner, 1990), the adults with hearing loss in the present study appeared to rely heavily on that information. In fact, when the formant information was removed, the performance of the adults with hearing loss decreased more than any other group, suggesting an even greater reliance on that information than previously reported. This discrepancy may be due to differences between the magnitude of the formant transitions for the two vowels used in the present study and that of the /i/ vowel used by Zeng and Turner. Although the magnitudes of the transitions were not reported in the Zeng and Turner study, the high-frequency formants of /i/ may have produced relatively weak formant transitions when preceded by the predominantly high-frequency fricatives. Pittman et al.: Perceptual Strategies in Listeners With Hearing Loss 1283

9 These results are also interesting in light of the weighting strategies reported by Doherty and Lutfi (1996). In their study, the adults with hearing loss assigned more weight to stimuli with respect to frequency regions of greatest hearing loss. That would suggest that the adults with hearing loss in the present study would favor the high-frequency fricative information over the lower-frequency vowel information in both listening conditions. However, the results suggest that the adults with hearing loss were equally capable of using information in all frequency regions as long as the information was sufficiently audible. Again, differences in stimuli might account for these discrepancies. Doherty and Lutfi (1996) presented nonmeaningful tonal complexes, whereas meaningful consonant-vowel-consonant stimuli were used in the present study. Although the stimulus manipulations in this study were artificial, theoretically the perceptual weights used by each group may indicate whether or not they can accommodate less than perfect communication environments. For example, a listener who can easily change his or her attention to alternate acoustic information may be better able to communicate in adverse listening environments where noise, reverberation, or distance may make important acoustic information difficult to perceive. The methodology in the current study did not allow us to explore the relation between perceptual weights and performance directly. Future studies are necessary to determine how perceptual weights relate to performance, if perceptual weights change with experience, and if perceptual weights can be systematically altered through training to improve performance. Acknowledgments This project was funded by a grant from the National Institutes of Health. The authors would like to thank Susan Nittrouer, Kathy Beauchaine, and Patricia Dorn for their comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript. References American National Standards Institute. (1969). Methods for calculation of the articulation index. New York: ANSI. Doherty, K. A., & Lutfi, R. A. (1996). Spectral weights for overall level discrimination in listeners with sensorineural hearing loss. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99, Doherty, K. A., & Lutfi, R. A. (1999). Level discrimination of single tones in a multitone complex by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105, Hnath-Chisolm, T. E., Laipply, E., & Boothroyd, A. (1998). Age-related changes on a children s test of sensorylevel speech perception capacity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, Neuman, A., & Hochberg, I. (1983). Children s perception of speech in reverberation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73, Nittrouer, S. (1992). Age-related differences in perceptual effects of formant transitions with syllables and across syllable boundaries. Journal of Phonetics, 20, Nittrouer, S. (1996). Discriminability and perceptual weighting of some acoustic cues to speech perception by 3-year-olds. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, Nittrouer, S., & Crowther, C. S. (1998). Examining the role of auditory sensitivity in the developmental weighting shift. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, Nittrouer, S., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1987). The role of coarticulatory effects in the perception of fricatives by children and adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, Nozza, R. J., Rossman, R. N., & Bond, L. C. (1991). Infant-adult differences in unmasked thresholds for the discrimination of consonant-vowel syllable pairs. Audiology, 30, Parnell, M. M., & Amerman, J. D. (1978). Maturational influences on perception of coarticulatory effects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 21, Pavlovic, C. V. (1984). Use of the articulation index for assessing residual auditory function in listeners with sensorineural hearing impairment. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 75, Pittman, A. L., & Stelmachowicz, P. G. (2000). Perception of voiceless fricatives by normal-hearing and hearingimpaired children and adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 43, Stelmachowicz, P. G., Hoover, B. M., Lewis, D. E., Kortekaas, R. W., & Pittman, A. L. (2000). The relation between stimulus context, speech audibility, and perception for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, Zeng, F. G., & Turner, C. W. (1990). Recognition of voiceless fricatives by normal and hearing-impaired subjects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, Received September 14, 2001 Accepted March 21, 2002 DOI: / (2002/102) Contact author: Andrea Pittman, PhD, Boys Town National Research Hospital, 555 N. 30th Street, Omaha, NE pittmana@boystown.org 1284 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol December 2002

Anumber of studies have shown that the perception of speech develops. by Normal-Hearing and Hearing- Impaired Children and Adults

Anumber of studies have shown that the perception of speech develops. by Normal-Hearing and Hearing- Impaired Children and Adults Perception of Voiceless Fricatives by Normal-Hearing and Hearing- Impaired Children and Adults Andrea L. Pittman Patricia G. Stelmachowicz Boys Town National Research Hospital Omaha, NE This study examined

More information

Speech Cue Weighting in Fricative Consonant Perception in Hearing Impaired Children

Speech Cue Weighting in Fricative Consonant Perception in Hearing Impaired Children University of Tennessee, Knoxville Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange University of Tennessee Honors Thesis Projects University of Tennessee Honors Program 5-2014 Speech Cue Weighting in Fricative

More information

I. INTRODUCTION. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110 (4), October /2001/110(4)/2183/8/$ Acoustical Society of America

I. INTRODUCTION. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110 (4), October /2001/110(4)/2183/8/$ Acoustical Society of America Effect of stimulus bandwidth on the perception of ÕsÕ in normal- and hearing-impaired children and adults Patricia G. Stelmachowicz, Andrea L. Pittman, Brenda M. Hoover, and Dawna E. Lewis Boys Town National

More information

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths Jennifer J. Lentz a Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington,

More information

Changes in the Role of Intensity as a Cue for Fricative Categorisation

Changes in the Role of Intensity as a Cue for Fricative Categorisation INTERSPEECH 2013 Changes in the Role of Intensity as a Cue for Fricative Categorisation Odette Scharenborg 1, Esther Janse 1,2 1 Centre for Language Studies and Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and

More information

Best Practice Protocols

Best Practice Protocols Best Practice Protocols SoundRecover for children What is SoundRecover? SoundRecover (non-linear frequency compression) seeks to give greater audibility of high-frequency everyday sounds by compressing

More information

For most hearing losses, the primary goal of amplification is to provide

For most hearing losses, the primary goal of amplification is to provide Spectral Characteristics of Speech at the Ear: Implications for Amplification in Children Andrea L. Pittman Patricia G. Stelmachowicz Dawna E. Lewis Brenda M. Hoover Boys Town National Research Hospital

More information

C HAPTER FOUR. Audiometric Configurations in Children. Andrea L. Pittman. Introduction. Methods

C HAPTER FOUR. Audiometric Configurations in Children. Andrea L. Pittman. Introduction. Methods C HAPTER FOUR Audiometric Configurations in Children Andrea L. Pittman Introduction Recent studies suggest that the amplification needs of children and adults differ due to differences in perceptual ability.

More information

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

HCS 7367 Speech Perception Long-term spectrum of speech HCS 7367 Speech Perception Connected speech Absolute threshold Males Dr. Peter Assmann Fall 212 Females Long-term spectrum of speech Vowels Males Females 2) Absolute threshold

More information

Juan Carlos Tejero-Calado 1, Janet C. Rutledge 2, and Peggy B. Nelson 3

Juan Carlos Tejero-Calado 1, Janet C. Rutledge 2, and Peggy B. Nelson 3 PRESERVING SPECTRAL CONTRAST IN AMPLITUDE COMPRESSION FOR HEARING AIDS Juan Carlos Tejero-Calado 1, Janet C. Rutledge 2, and Peggy B. Nelson 3 1 University of Malaga, Campus de Teatinos-Complejo Tecnol

More information

Issues faced by people with a Sensorineural Hearing Loss

Issues faced by people with a Sensorineural Hearing Loss Issues faced by people with a Sensorineural Hearing Loss Issues faced by people with a Sensorineural Hearing Loss 1. Decreased Audibility 2. Decreased Dynamic Range 3. Decreased Frequency Resolution 4.

More information

FREQUENCY COMPRESSION AND FREQUENCY SHIFTING FOR THE HEARING IMPAIRED

FREQUENCY COMPRESSION AND FREQUENCY SHIFTING FOR THE HEARING IMPAIRED FREQUENCY COMPRESSION AND FREQUENCY SHIFTING FOR THE HEARING IMPAIRED Francisco J. Fraga, Alan M. Marotta National Institute of Telecommunications, Santa Rita do Sapucaí - MG, Brazil Abstract A considerable

More information

1706 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113 (3), March /2003/113(3)/1706/12/$ Acoustical Society of America

1706 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113 (3), March /2003/113(3)/1706/12/$ Acoustical Society of America The effects of hearing loss on the contribution of high- and lowfrequency speech information to speech understanding a) Benjamin W. Y. Hornsby b) and Todd A. Ricketts Dan Maddox Hearing Aid Research Laboratory,

More information

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

HCS 7367 Speech Perception Babies 'cry in mother's tongue' HCS 7367 Speech Perception Dr. Peter Assmann Fall 212 Babies' cries imitate their mother tongue as early as three days old German researchers say babies begin to pick up

More information

AJA. Research Article. Effects of Low-Pass Filtering on the Perception of Word-Final Plurality Markers in Children and Adults With Normal Hearing

AJA. Research Article. Effects of Low-Pass Filtering on the Perception of Word-Final Plurality Markers in Children and Adults With Normal Hearing AJA Research Article Effects of Low-Pass Filtering on the Perception of Word-Final Plurality Markers in Children and Adults With Normal Hearing Lori J. Leibold, a Hannah Hodson, a Ryan W. McCreery, b Lauren

More information

Perceptual Effects of Nasal Cue Modification

Perceptual Effects of Nasal Cue Modification Send Orders for Reprints to reprints@benthamscience.ae The Open Electrical & Electronic Engineering Journal, 2015, 9, 399-407 399 Perceptual Effects of Nasal Cue Modification Open Access Fan Bai 1,2,*

More information

ACOUSTIC AND PERCEPTUAL PROPERTIES OF ENGLISH FRICATIVES

ACOUSTIC AND PERCEPTUAL PROPERTIES OF ENGLISH FRICATIVES ISCA Archive ACOUSTIC AND PERCEPTUAL PROPERTIES OF ENGLISH FRICATIVES Allard Jongman 1, Yue Wang 2, and Joan Sereno 1 1 Linguistics Department, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 66045 U.S.A. 2 Department

More information

2/25/2013. Context Effect on Suprasegmental Cues. Supresegmental Cues. Pitch Contour Identification (PCI) Context Effect with Cochlear Implants

2/25/2013. Context Effect on Suprasegmental Cues. Supresegmental Cues. Pitch Contour Identification (PCI) Context Effect with Cochlear Implants Context Effect on Segmental and Supresegmental Cues Preceding context has been found to affect phoneme recognition Stop consonant recognition (Mann, 1980) A continuum from /da/ to /ga/ was preceded by

More information

Consonant Perception test

Consonant Perception test Consonant Perception test Introduction The Vowel-Consonant-Vowel (VCV) test is used in clinics to evaluate how well a listener can recognize consonants under different conditions (e.g. with and without

More information

Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults

Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults Daniel Fogerty Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of South Carolina, Columbia, South

More information

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen. Lecture 2 Properties of Waves Frequency and period are distinctly different, yet related, quantities. Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

More information

Audibility, discrimination and hearing comfort at a new level: SoundRecover2

Audibility, discrimination and hearing comfort at a new level: SoundRecover2 Audibility, discrimination and hearing comfort at a new level: SoundRecover2 Julia Rehmann, Michael Boretzki, Sonova AG 5th European Pediatric Conference Current Developments and New Directions in Pediatric

More information

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL?

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL? 1 Psychoacoustics What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL? The decibel (db ) is a logarithmic unit of measurement used to express the magnitude of a sound relative to some reference level. Decibels

More information

Speech (Sound) Processing

Speech (Sound) Processing 7 Speech (Sound) Processing Acoustic Human communication is achieved when thought is transformed through language into speech. The sounds of speech are initiated by activity in the central nervous system,

More information

Spectral weighting strategies for hearing-impaired listeners measured using a correlational method

Spectral weighting strategies for hearing-impaired listeners measured using a correlational method Spectral weighting strategies for hearing-impaired listeners measured using a correlational method Lauren Calandruccio a and Karen A. Doherty Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Institute

More information

SoundRecover2 the first adaptive frequency compression algorithm More audibility of high frequency sounds

SoundRecover2 the first adaptive frequency compression algorithm More audibility of high frequency sounds Phonak Insight April 2016 SoundRecover2 the first adaptive frequency compression algorithm More audibility of high frequency sounds Phonak led the way in modern frequency lowering technology with the introduction

More information

Effects of noise and filtering on the intelligibility of speech produced during simultaneous communication

Effects of noise and filtering on the intelligibility of speech produced during simultaneous communication Journal of Communication Disorders 37 (2004) 505 515 Effects of noise and filtering on the intelligibility of speech produced during simultaneous communication Douglas J. MacKenzie a,*, Nicholas Schiavetti

More information

High-Frequency Amplification: Sharpening the Pencil

High-Frequency Amplification: Sharpening the Pencil CHAPTER ELEVEN High-Frequency Amplification: Sharpening the Pencil Andrea Pittman Introduction High-frequency amplification is similar to a sharpened pencil. When amplification is extended to frequencies

More information

A comparison of manufacturer-specific prescriptive procedures for infants

A comparison of manufacturer-specific prescriptive procedures for infants A comparison of manufacturer-specific prescriptive procedures for infants By Richard Seewald, Jillian Mills, Marlene Bagatto, Susan Scollie, and Sheila Moodie Early hearing detection and communication

More information

EEL 6586, Project - Hearing Aids algorithms

EEL 6586, Project - Hearing Aids algorithms EEL 6586, Project - Hearing Aids algorithms 1 Yan Yang, Jiang Lu, and Ming Xue I. PROBLEM STATEMENT We studied hearing loss algorithms in this project. As the conductive hearing loss is due to sound conducting

More information

WIDEXPRESS. no.30. Background

WIDEXPRESS. no.30. Background WIDEXPRESS no. january 12 By Marie Sonne Kristensen Petri Korhonen Using the WidexLink technology to improve speech perception Background For most hearing aid users, the primary motivation for using hearing

More information

FREQUENCY. Prof Dr. Mona Mourad Dr.Manal Elbanna Doaa Elmoazen ALEXANDRIA UNIVERSITY. Background

FREQUENCY. Prof Dr. Mona Mourad Dr.Manal Elbanna Doaa Elmoazen ALEXANDRIA UNIVERSITY. Background FREQUENCY TRANSPOSITION IN HIGH FREQUENCY SNHL Prof Dr. Mona Mourad Dr.Manal Elbanna Doaa Elmoazen Randa Awad ALEXANDRIA UNIVERSITY Background Concept Of Frequency Transposition Frequency transposition

More information

Research Article The Acoustic and Peceptual Effects of Series and Parallel Processing

Research Article The Acoustic and Peceptual Effects of Series and Parallel Processing Hindawi Publishing Corporation EURASIP Journal on Advances in Signal Processing Volume 9, Article ID 6195, pages doi:1.1155/9/6195 Research Article The Acoustic and Peceptual Effects of Series and Parallel

More information

Limited available evidence suggests that the perceptual salience of

Limited available evidence suggests that the perceptual salience of Spectral Tilt Change in Stop Consonant Perception by Listeners With Hearing Impairment Joshua M. Alexander Keith R. Kluender University of Wisconsin, Madison Purpose: To evaluate how perceptual importance

More information

Enrique A. Lopez-Poveda Alan R. Palmer Ray Meddis Editors. The Neurophysiological Bases of Auditory Perception

Enrique A. Lopez-Poveda Alan R. Palmer Ray Meddis Editors. The Neurophysiological Bases of Auditory Perception Enrique A. Lopez-Poveda Alan R. Palmer Ray Meddis Editors The Neurophysiological Bases of Auditory Perception 123 The Neurophysiological Bases of Auditory Perception Enrique A. Lopez-Poveda Alan R. Palmer

More information

Spectral-peak selection in spectral-shape discrimination by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

Spectral-peak selection in spectral-shape discrimination by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners Spectral-peak selection in spectral-shape discrimination by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners Jennifer J. Lentz a Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington,

More information

AJA. Research Article

AJA. Research Article AJA Research Article Development and Evaluation of an English Language Measure of Detection of Word-Final Plurality Markers: The University of Western Ontario Plurals Test Danielle Glista a and Susan Scollie

More information

Improving Audibility with Nonlinear Amplification for Listeners with High-Frequency Loss

Improving Audibility with Nonlinear Amplification for Listeners with High-Frequency Loss J Am Acad Audiol 11 : 214-223 (2000) Improving Audibility with Nonlinear Amplification for Listeners with High-Frequency Loss Pamela E. Souza* Robbi D. Bishop* Abstract In contrast to fitting strategies

More information

Hello Old Friend the use of frequency specific speech phonemes in cortical and behavioural testing of infants

Hello Old Friend the use of frequency specific speech phonemes in cortical and behavioural testing of infants Hello Old Friend the use of frequency specific speech phonemes in cortical and behavioural testing of infants Andrea Kelly 1,3 Denice Bos 2 Suzanne Purdy 3 Michael Sanders 3 Daniel Kim 1 1. Auckland District

More information

Infant Hearing Development: Translating Research Findings into Clinical Practice. Auditory Development. Overview

Infant Hearing Development: Translating Research Findings into Clinical Practice. Auditory Development. Overview Infant Hearing Development: Translating Research Findings into Clinical Practice Lori J. Leibold Department of Allied Health Sciences The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Auditory Development

More information

Speech Intelligibility Measurements in Auditorium

Speech Intelligibility Measurements in Auditorium Vol. 118 (2010) ACTA PHYSICA POLONICA A No. 1 Acoustic and Biomedical Engineering Speech Intelligibility Measurements in Auditorium K. Leo Faculty of Physics and Applied Mathematics, Technical University

More information

Speech perception in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer s type (DAT) Mitchell S. Sommers Department of Psychology Washington University

Speech perception in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer s type (DAT) Mitchell S. Sommers Department of Psychology Washington University Speech perception in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer s type (DAT) Mitchell S. Sommers Department of Psychology Washington University Overview Goals of studying speech perception in individuals

More information

Validation Studies. How well does this work??? Speech perception (e.g., Erber & Witt 1977) Early Development... History of the DSL Method

Validation Studies. How well does this work??? Speech perception (e.g., Erber & Witt 1977) Early Development... History of the DSL Method DSL v5.: A Presentation for the Ontario Infant Hearing Program Associates The Desired Sensation Level (DSL) Method Early development.... 198 Goal: To develop a computer-assisted electroacoustic-based procedure

More information

Categorical Perception

Categorical Perception Categorical Perception Discrimination for some speech contrasts is poor within phonetic categories and good between categories. Unusual, not found for most perceptual contrasts. Influenced by task, expectations,

More information

THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING

THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING Vanessa Surowiecki 1, vid Grayden 1, Richard Dowell

More information

Evaluating the role of spectral and envelope characteristics in the intelligibility advantage of clear speech

Evaluating the role of spectral and envelope characteristics in the intelligibility advantage of clear speech Evaluating the role of spectral and envelope characteristics in the intelligibility advantage of clear speech Jean C. Krause a and Louis D. Braida Research Laboratory of Electronics, Massachusetts Institute

More information

Role of F0 differences in source segregation

Role of F0 differences in source segregation Role of F0 differences in source segregation Andrew J. Oxenham Research Laboratory of Electronics, MIT and Harvard-MIT Speech and Hearing Bioscience and Technology Program Rationale Many aspects of segregation

More information

Jitter, Shimmer, and Noise in Pathological Voice Quality Perception

Jitter, Shimmer, and Noise in Pathological Voice Quality Perception ISCA Archive VOQUAL'03, Geneva, August 27-29, 2003 Jitter, Shimmer, and Noise in Pathological Voice Quality Perception Jody Kreiman and Bruce R. Gerratt Division of Head and Neck Surgery, School of Medicine

More information

Intelligibility of clear speech at normal rates for older adults with hearing loss

Intelligibility of clear speech at normal rates for older adults with hearing loss University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2006 Intelligibility of clear speech at normal rates for older adults with hearing loss Billie Jo Shaw University

More information

Even though a large body of work exists on the detrimental effects. The Effect of Hearing Loss on Identification of Asynchronous Double Vowels

Even though a large body of work exists on the detrimental effects. The Effect of Hearing Loss on Identification of Asynchronous Double Vowels The Effect of Hearing Loss on Identification of Asynchronous Double Vowels Jennifer J. Lentz Indiana University, Bloomington Shavon L. Marsh St. John s University, Jamaica, NY This study determined whether

More information

Optimal Filter Perception of Speech Sounds: Implications to Hearing Aid Fitting through Verbotonal Rehabilitation

Optimal Filter Perception of Speech Sounds: Implications to Hearing Aid Fitting through Verbotonal Rehabilitation Optimal Filter Perception of Speech Sounds: Implications to Hearing Aid Fitting through Verbotonal Rehabilitation Kazunari J. Koike, Ph.D., CCC-A Professor & Director of Audiology Department of Otolaryngology

More information

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 104 (6), December /98/104(6)/3597/11/$ Acoustical Society of America 3597

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 104 (6), December /98/104(6)/3597/11/$ Acoustical Society of America 3597 The relation between identification and discrimination of vowels in young and elderly listeners a) Maureen Coughlin, b) Diane Kewley-Port, c) and Larry E. Humes d) Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences,

More information

Digital noise reduction in hearing aids and its acoustic effect on consonants /s/ and /z/

Digital noise reduction in hearing aids and its acoustic effect on consonants /s/ and /z/ ORIGINAL ARTICLE Digital noise reduction in hearing aids and its acoustic effect on consonants /s/ and /z/ Foong Yen Chong, PhD 1, 2, Lorienne M. Jenstad, PhD 2 1 Audiology Program, Centre for Rehabilitation

More information

The development of a modified spectral ripple test

The development of a modified spectral ripple test The development of a modified spectral ripple test Justin M. Aronoff a) and David M. Landsberger Communication and Neuroscience Division, House Research Institute, 2100 West 3rd Street, Los Angeles, California

More information

The Salience and Perceptual Weight of Secondary Acoustic Cues for Fricative Identification in Normal Hearing Adults

The Salience and Perceptual Weight of Secondary Acoustic Cues for Fricative Identification in Normal Hearing Adults City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Graduate Center 6-2014 The Salience and Perceptual Weight of Secondary Acoustic Cues for Fricative Identification

More information

Effects of speaker's and listener's environments on speech intelligibili annoyance. Author(s)Kubo, Rieko; Morikawa, Daisuke; Akag

Effects of speaker's and listener's environments on speech intelligibili annoyance. Author(s)Kubo, Rieko; Morikawa, Daisuke; Akag JAIST Reposi https://dspace.j Title Effects of speaker's and listener's environments on speech intelligibili annoyance Author(s)Kubo, Rieko; Morikawa, Daisuke; Akag Citation Inter-noise 2016: 171-176 Issue

More information

functions grow at a higher rate than in normal{hearing subjects. In this chapter, the correlation

functions grow at a higher rate than in normal{hearing subjects. In this chapter, the correlation Chapter Categorical loudness scaling in hearing{impaired listeners Abstract Most sensorineural hearing{impaired subjects show the recruitment phenomenon, i.e., loudness functions grow at a higher rate

More information

Sonic Spotlight. SmartCompress. Advancing compression technology into the future

Sonic Spotlight. SmartCompress. Advancing compression technology into the future Sonic Spotlight SmartCompress Advancing compression technology into the future Speech Variable Processing (SVP) is the unique digital signal processing strategy that gives Sonic hearing aids their signature

More information

Best practice protocol

Best practice protocol Best practice protocol April 2016 Pediatric verification for SoundRecover2 What is SoundRecover? SoundRecover is a frequency lowering signal processing available in Phonak hearing instruments. The aim

More information

Hearing Loss in Children and Adults: Audiometric Configuration, Asymmetry, and Progression

Hearing Loss in Children and Adults: Audiometric Configuration, Asymmetry, and Progression Hearing Loss in Children and Adults: Audiometric Configuration, Asymmetry, and Progression A. L. Pittman and P. G. Stelmachowicz Objective: The purpose of this study was to characterize the sensorineural

More information

Speech conveys not only linguistic content but. Vocal Emotion Recognition by Normal-Hearing Listeners and Cochlear Implant Users

Speech conveys not only linguistic content but. Vocal Emotion Recognition by Normal-Hearing Listeners and Cochlear Implant Users Cochlear Implants Special Issue Article Vocal Emotion Recognition by Normal-Hearing Listeners and Cochlear Implant Users Trends in Amplification Volume 11 Number 4 December 2007 301-315 2007 Sage Publications

More information

Fitting Frequency Compression Hearing Aids to Kids: The Basics

Fitting Frequency Compression Hearing Aids to Kids: The Basics Fitting Frequency Compression Hearing Aids to Kids: The Basics Presenters: Susan Scollie and Danielle Glista Presented at AudiologyNow! 2011, Chicago Support This work was supported by: Canadian Institutes

More information

Speech intelligibility in simulated acoustic conditions for normal hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

Speech intelligibility in simulated acoustic conditions for normal hearing and hearing-impaired listeners Speech intelligibility in simulated acoustic conditions for normal hearing and hearing-impaired listeners Ir i s Arw e i l e r 1, To r b e n Po u l s e n 2, a n d To r s t e n Da u 1 1 Centre for Applied

More information

SPEECH PERCEPTION IN A 3-D WORLD

SPEECH PERCEPTION IN A 3-D WORLD SPEECH PERCEPTION IN A 3-D WORLD A line on an audiogram is far from answering the question How well can this child hear speech? In this section a variety of ways will be presented to further the teacher/therapist

More information

ACOUSTIC ANALYSIS AND PERCEPTION OF CANTONESE VOWELS PRODUCED BY PROFOUNDLY HEARING IMPAIRED ADOLESCENTS

ACOUSTIC ANALYSIS AND PERCEPTION OF CANTONESE VOWELS PRODUCED BY PROFOUNDLY HEARING IMPAIRED ADOLESCENTS ACOUSTIC ANALYSIS AND PERCEPTION OF CANTONESE VOWELS PRODUCED BY PROFOUNDLY HEARING IMPAIRED ADOLESCENTS Edward Khouw, & Valter Ciocca Dept. of Speech and Hearing Sciences, The University of Hong Kong

More information

Spectral processing of two concurrent harmonic complexes

Spectral processing of two concurrent harmonic complexes Spectral processing of two concurrent harmonic complexes Yi Shen a) and Virginia M. Richards Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, California 92697-5100 (Received 7 April

More information

Effect of Consonant Duration Modifications on Speech Perception in Noise-II

Effect of Consonant Duration Modifications on Speech Perception in Noise-II International Journal of Electronics Engineering, 2(1), 2010, pp. 75-81 Effect of Consonant Duration Modifications on Speech Perception in Noise-II NH Shobha 1, TG Thomas 2 & K Subbarao 3 1 Research Scholar,

More information

Age and hearing loss and the use of acoustic cues in fricative categorization

Age and hearing loss and the use of acoustic cues in fricative categorization Age and hearing loss and the use of acoustic cues in fricative categorization Odette Scharenborg a),b) Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University Nijmegen, Erasmusplein 1, 6525 HT, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

More information

Speech Spectra and Spectrograms

Speech Spectra and Spectrograms ACOUSTICS TOPICS ACOUSTICS SOFTWARE SPH301 SLP801 RESOURCE INDEX HELP PAGES Back to Main "Speech Spectra and Spectrograms" Page Speech Spectra and Spectrograms Robert Mannell 6. Some consonant spectra

More information

S everal studies indicate that the identification/recognition. Identification Performance by Right- and Lefthanded Listeners on Dichotic CV Materials

S everal studies indicate that the identification/recognition. Identification Performance by Right- and Lefthanded Listeners on Dichotic CV Materials J Am Acad Audiol 7 : 1-6 (1996) Identification Performance by Right- and Lefthanded Listeners on Dichotic CV Materials Richard H. Wilson* Elizabeth D. Leigh* Abstract Normative data from 24 right-handed

More information

The University of Western Ontario Plurals Test v1.4

The University of Western Ontario Plurals Test v1.4 The University of Western Ontario Plurals Test v1.4 Susan Scollie & Danielle Glista, June 2012 Copyright 2012, The University of Western Ontario, Not for Distribution Table of Contents Overview... 3 Set-up

More information

BINAURAL DICHOTIC PRESENTATION FOR MODERATE BILATERAL SENSORINEURAL HEARING-IMPAIRED

BINAURAL DICHOTIC PRESENTATION FOR MODERATE BILATERAL SENSORINEURAL HEARING-IMPAIRED International Conference on Systemics, Cybernetics and Informatics, February 12 15, 2004 BINAURAL DICHOTIC PRESENTATION FOR MODERATE BILATERAL SENSORINEURAL HEARING-IMPAIRED Alice N. Cheeran Biomedical

More information

Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had

Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS Subjects Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had previous exposure to a tone language. Subjects were divided into two groups based on musical

More information

ACOUSTIC MOMENTS DATA

ACOUSTIC MOMENTS DATA ACOUSTIC MOMENTS DATA FOR PALATALIZED AND DENTALIZED SIBILANT PRODUCTIONS FROM SPEECH DELAYED CHILDREN WITH AND WITHOUT HISTORIES OF OTITIS MEDIA WITH EFFUSION Phonology Project Technical Report No. 12

More information

The clinical Link. Distortion Product Otoacoustic Emission

The clinical Link. Distortion Product Otoacoustic Emission Distortion product otoacoustic emissions: Introduction Michael P. Gorga, Ph.D. Boys Town National Research Hospital Work supported by the NIH Collaborators on BTNRH OAE Projects Stephen Neely Kathy Beauchaine

More information

The Situational Hearing Aid Response Profile (SHARP), version 7 BOYS TOWN NATIONAL RESEARCH HOSPITAL. 555 N. 30th St. Omaha, Nebraska 68131

The Situational Hearing Aid Response Profile (SHARP), version 7 BOYS TOWN NATIONAL RESEARCH HOSPITAL. 555 N. 30th St. Omaha, Nebraska 68131 The Situational Hearing Aid Response Profile (SHARP), version 7 BOYS TOWN NATIONAL RESEARCH HOSPITAL 555 N. 30th St. Omaha, Nebraska 68131 (402) 498-6520 This work was supported by NIH-NIDCD Grants R01

More information

Feedback and feedforward control in apraxia of speech: Noise masking effects on fricative production.

Feedback and feedforward control in apraxia of speech: Noise masking effects on fricative production. Feedback and feedforward control in apraxia of speech: Noise masking effects on fricative production. The present study tested two hypotheses about apraxia of speech (AOS), framed in the DIVA model (Guenther,

More information

Benefits of Advanced Hearing Aid Technologies to Children (and Adults) with Hearing Loss

Benefits of Advanced Hearing Aid Technologies to Children (and Adults) with Hearing Loss Benefits of Advanced Hearing Aid Technologies to Children (and Adults) with Hearing Loss CAA 2012 Andrea Pittman Arizona State University www.pedamp.asu.edu Children with Hearing Loss 1 million 30 million

More information

WIDEXPRESS THE WIDEX FITTING RATIONALE FOR EVOKE MARCH 2018 ISSUE NO. 38

WIDEXPRESS THE WIDEX FITTING RATIONALE FOR EVOKE MARCH 2018 ISSUE NO. 38 THE WIDEX FITTING RATIONALE FOR EVOKE ERIK SCHMIDT, TECHNICAL AUDIOLOGY SPECIALIST WIDEX GLOBAL RESEARCH & DEVELOPMENT Introduction As part of the development of the EVOKE hearing aid, the Widex Fitting

More information

Hearing the Universal Language: Music and Cochlear Implants

Hearing the Universal Language: Music and Cochlear Implants Hearing the Universal Language: Music and Cochlear Implants Professor Hugh McDermott Deputy Director (Research) The Bionics Institute of Australia, Professorial Fellow The University of Melbourne Overview?

More information

Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation

Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation Paper No. 556 Proceedings of 20 th International Congress on Acoustics, ICA 2010 23-27 August 2010, Sydney, Australia Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation Pandurangarao N. Kulkarni

More information

Influence of acoustic complexity on spatial release from masking and lateralization

Influence of acoustic complexity on spatial release from masking and lateralization Influence of acoustic complexity on spatial release from masking and lateralization Gusztáv Lőcsei, Sébastien Santurette, Torsten Dau, Ewen N. MacDonald Hearing Systems Group, Department of Electrical

More information

From /I/ to /fis/ Terminology. How do frequency compressing hearing aids differ from conventional-style digital aids?

From /I/ to /fis/ Terminology. How do frequency compressing hearing aids differ from conventional-style digital aids? From /I/ to /fis/ Using Speech Production as a Guide to Fitting Frequency Compression Hearing Aids in Children. Julie Christensen, M.S., CCC-A Boys Town National Research Hospital Terminology Frequency

More information

Recognition of Multiply Degraded Speech by Young and Elderly Listeners

Recognition of Multiply Degraded Speech by Young and Elderly Listeners Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 38, 1150-1156, October 1995 Recognition of Multiply Degraded Speech by Young and Elderly Listeners Sandra Gordon-Salant University of Maryland College Park

More information

Intelligibility of narrow-band speech and its relation to auditory functions in hearing-impaired listeners

Intelligibility of narrow-band speech and its relation to auditory functions in hearing-impaired listeners Intelligibility of narrow-band speech and its relation to auditory functions in hearing-impaired listeners VRIJE UNIVERSITEIT Intelligibility of narrow-band speech and its relation to auditory functions

More information

The Influence of Audibility on Speech Recognition With Nonlinear Frequency Compression for Children and Adults With Hearing Loss

The Influence of Audibility on Speech Recognition With Nonlinear Frequency Compression for Children and Adults With Hearing Loss The Influence of Audibility on Speech Recognition With Nonlinear Frequency Compression for Children and Adults With Hearing Loss Ryan W. McCreery, 1 Joshua Alexander, Marc A. Brennan, 1 Brenda Hoover,

More information

64 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 61, NO. 1, JANUARY 2014

64 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 61, NO. 1, JANUARY 2014 64 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 61, NO. 1, JANUARY 2014 Signal-Processing Strategy for Restoration of Cross-Channel Suppression in Hearing-Impaired Listeners Daniel M. Rasetshwane,

More information

Consonant recognition loss in hearing impaired listeners

Consonant recognition loss in hearing impaired listeners Consonant recognition loss in hearing impaired listeners Sandeep A. Phatak Army Audiology and Speech Center, Walter Reed Army Medical Center, Washington, DC 237 Yang-soo Yoon House Ear Institute, 21 West

More information

Hearing in the Environment

Hearing in the Environment 10 Hearing in the Environment Click Chapter to edit 10 Master Hearing title in the style Environment Sound Localization Complex Sounds Auditory Scene Analysis Continuity and Restoration Effects Auditory

More information

Coupling of FM Systems to Individuals With Unilateral Hearing Loss

Coupling of FM Systems to Individuals With Unilateral Hearing Loss Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 35, 21-27, February 1992 Coupling of FM Systems to ndividuals With Unilateral Hearing Loss Judy G. Kopun Patricia G. Stelmachowicz Edward Carney Laura Schulte

More information

Pitfalls in behavioral estimates of basilar-membrane compression in humans a)

Pitfalls in behavioral estimates of basilar-membrane compression in humans a) Pitfalls in behavioral estimates of basilar-membrane compression in humans a) Magdalena Wojtczak b and Andrew J. Oxenham Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis,

More information

Temporal Location of Perceptual Cues for Cantonese Tone Identification

Temporal Location of Perceptual Cues for Cantonese Tone Identification Temporal Location of Perceptual Cues for Cantonese Tone Identification Zoe Wai-Man Lam, Kathleen Currie Hall and Douglas Pulleyblank Department of Linguistics University of British Columbia 1 Outline of

More information

Should digital noise reduction be activated in pediatric hearing-aid fittings?

Should digital noise reduction be activated in pediatric hearing-aid fittings? Should digital noise reduction be activated in pediatric hearing-aid fittings? Ryan McCreery, ABD CCC-A Research Audiologist Boys Town National Research Hospital Omaha, Nebraska, USA Supported by NIDCD

More information

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17 Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17 related lectures: Applied and Virtual Acoustics (Winter Term) Advanced Psychoacoustics (Summer Term) Sound Perception 2 Frequency and Level Range of Human Hearing Source:

More information

Individual Variability of Hearing-Impaired Consonant Perception

Individual Variability of Hearing-Impaired Consonant Perception Individual Variability of Hearing-Impaired Consonant Perception Andrea Trevino, MSEE, 1 and Jont B. Allen, Ph.D. 1 ABSTRACT The study of hearing impaired speech recognition at the consonant level allows

More information

Extended frequency amplification, speech recognition and functional performance in children with mild to severe sensorineural hearing loss

Extended frequency amplification, speech recognition and functional performance in children with mild to severe sensorineural hearing loss Extended frequency amplification, speech recognition and functional performance in children with mild to severe sensorineural hearing loss by Claudia Müller A dissertation submitted in fulfillment of the

More information

Paediatric Amplification

Paediatric Amplification Paediatric Amplification The paediatric technical advisory group (PTAG) of the NZAS recommends following the protocols found in UNHSEIP Diagnostic and Amplification Protocols (January, 2016). These guidelines

More information

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080 Perceptual segregation of a harmonic from a vowel by interaural time difference in conjunction with mistuning and onset asynchrony C. J. Darwin and R. W. Hukin Experimental Psychology, University of Sussex,

More information

Spectrograms (revisited)

Spectrograms (revisited) Spectrograms (revisited) We begin the lecture by reviewing the units of spectrograms, which I had only glossed over when I covered spectrograms at the end of lecture 19. We then relate the blocks of a

More information