Explaining Calibration in Classroom Contexts: The Effects of Incentives, Reflection, and Attributional Style. Douglas J. Hacker. University of Utah

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Explaining Calibration in Classroom Contexts: The Effects of Incentives, Reflection, and Attributional Style. Douglas J. Hacker. University of Utah"

Transcription

1 Calibration in Classrooms 1 Hacker, D. J., Bol, L., Bahbahani, K. (in press). Explaining calibration in classroom contexts: The effects of incentives, reflection, and attributional style. Metacognition and Learning. Running Head: CALIBRATION IN CLASSROOMS Explaining Calibration in Classroom Contexts: The Effects of Incentives, Reflection, and Attributional Style Douglas J. Hacker University of Utah Linda Bol Old Dominion University Kamilla Bahbahani Old Dominion University

2 Calibration in Classrooms 2 Abstract A 2 x 2 quasi-experimental design was used to investigate the impact of extrinsic incentives and reflection on students calibration of exam performance. We further examined the relationships among attributional style, performance, and calibration judgments. Participants were 137 college students enrolled in an educational psychology course. Results differed as a function of exam performance. Higher-performing students were very accurate in their calibration and did not show significant improvements across a semester-length course. Attributional style did not significantly contribute to their calibration judgments. Lower-performing students, however, were less accurate in their calibration, and students in the incentives condition showed significant increases in calibration. Beyond exam scores, attributional style constructs were significant predictors of calibration judgments for these students. The constructs targeting study and social variables accounted for most of the additional explained variance. The qualitative data also revealed differences by performance level in open-ended explanations for calibration judgments. Key Words: calibration, metacognitive monitoring, absolute accuracy, prediction, postdiction, attributional style

3 Calibration in Classrooms 3 Explaining Calibration in Classroom Contexts: The Effects of Incentives, Reflection, and Attributional Style Calibration is a measure of the degree to which a person s judged ratings of performance correspond to his or her actual performance (Keren, 1991; Lichtenstein, Fischhoff, & Phillips, 1982; Nietfeld, Cao, & Osborne, 2006; Yates, 1990). Ostensibly, the psychological processes underlying calibration entail a person monitoring his or her knowledge about a specific topic or skill and then self-assessing the extent of that knowledge in comparison to his or her performance on a criterion task. As such, calibration is a metacognitive process that people can use to regulate their behavior. For instance, students who can accurately assess the extent of their knowledge should be in a better position to intensify or redirect their studying for a test, provide self-guidance during reading for better comprehension, or generate self-feedback indicating that a new skill is being properly acquired. Calibration is calculated by taking the difference between a person s self-assessment of performance on a task and his or her actual performance on the task (Keren, 1991; Lichtenstein et al., 1982; Lin & Zabrucky, 1998; Schraw, Potenza, & Nebelsick-Gullet, 1993; Winne, 2004). Self-assessments made prior to performance are called predictions, and those made subsequent to performance are called postdictions. The more closely a person s predicted or postdicted performance matches his or her actual performance (i.e., the difference approaches zero), the better calibrated he or she is. For example, two people may judge that they will get 85% of the items on a test correct, but one gets 80% correct and the other gets 60% correct; the person who earned the 80% is the better calibrated of the two. The majority of calibration studies have been conducted in laboratory settings. These studies typically have strong internal validity and have provided essential information about

4 Calibration in Classrooms 4 calibration, but there is reason to believe that some of the findings generated from these studies may not generalize to naturalistic contexts, such as classrooms (Lundeberg & Fox, 1991; McCormick, 2003; Winne, 2004). In laboratory settings, the materials and procedures used to investigate calibration often involve word-pair associations that are presented over short periods of time, usually have little meaning for the participants, and are tested shortly after learning. These materials and procedures differ markedly from those used in classrooms in which learning is complex, is presented in multiple forms (e.g., lecture, reading, group participation) over long periods of time, is more motivating for students, and is tested at time intervals considerably longer than those used in laboratory studies. If the goal of psychological research is to understand cognition in the context of natural purposeful activity (Neisser, 1976, p. 7), then we need to acknowledge the differences between laboratory and classroom contexts. Although there is a continued need for laboratory work, there is also a need for researchers to move from the laboratory into more naturalistic environments. This move will compromise internal validity for external validity, but the compromise may be necessary to make psychology more practical (Parducci & Sarris, 1984, p. 10). Because there are still very few calibration studies that have been conducted in classrooms, there is much to be learned about calibration in this context. Therefore, our purpose was to conduct a quasi-experiment in four college classrooms to investigate (a) how well calibrated students can be, (b) whether incentives or reflective activities can improve calibration, and (c) whether several social-cognitive factors that are known to contribute to classroom learning also contribute to calibration.

5 Calibration in Classrooms 5 Classroom Studies to Improve Calibration Classroom studies to improve calibration have produced mixed results. In some instances, modest gains have been achieved (Hacker, Bol, Horgan, & Rakow, 2000; Nietfeld, Cao, & Osborne, 2006), but in other instances, gains have been negligible (Bol & Hacker, 2001; Bol, Hacker, O Shea, & Allen, 2005; Nietfeld, Cao, & Osborne, 2005). Moreover, strategies that have been shown to improve calibration in the laboratory do not seem to have the same effect in the classroom. For example, in the laboratory, the use of feedback has improved calibration (Arkes, Christensen, Lai, & Blumer, 1987; Glenberg, Wilkinson, & Epstein, 1987). However, in a classroom study by Nietfeld et al. (2005), feedback on exam scores did not lead to changes in students calibration; and in Bol and Hacker (2001), students who received teacher-generated feedback on practice tests were less accurate in comparison to students who engaged in a traditional review of course material without feedback. Failures to improve calibration have sometimes been attributed to lack of motivation on the part of participants. In an attempt to increase motivation, and consequently accuracy, Schraw, Potenza, and Nebelsick-Gullet (1993) provided participants in a laboratory study with extrinsic incentives, such as rewards. Schraw and colleagues found that providing extrinsic incentives improved both performance and calibration, suggesting that calibration is a controllable process that is subject to the self-efficacy beliefs and motivation of the learner. However, we could find no other study that has replicated this finding in a classroom context. In the present experiment, we attempted to replicate these findings by providing extrinsic incentives that entailed allotting an increasing number of extra credit points on tests to students who exhibited improved calibration.

6 Calibration in Classrooms 6 Interventions designed specifically to improve calibration have led to widely discrepant results. In a laboratory study conducted by Nietfeld and Schraw (2002), participants increased their monitoring accuracy after receiving short-term strategy training that did not require participants to explicitly monitor their training. In contrast, in the classroom study conducted by Nietfeld et al. (2005), in which explicit monitoring was implemented, no increases in accuracy were found. However, in a follow-up classroom study by Nietfeld et al. (2006), an intervention was used in which explicit monitoring focused on calibration, self-efficacy, and performance, and improvements in both calibration and performance were shown. Finally, in the Hacker et al. (2000) classroom study, a complex intervention was used that consisted of feedback, practice tests, and instruction on the benefits of accurate self-assessment. Increases in both prediction and postdiction accuracy were found but only for higher-performing students. To help resolve some of these discrepant findings, our primary goal for the present classroom study was to investigate whether we could improve students calibration through personal reflections on performance and extrinsic incentives. We used three interventions, each being administered three times across a 15-week course, once after each of three exams. Our interventions were developed by fully crossing two independent variables: reflection/no reflection and extrinsic incentives/no extrinsic incentives. Using a between-subjects design, we compared calibration of students across four conditions: (a) students who were asked to reflect on explanations for their calibration judgments but were not provided with extrinsic incentives to improve accuracy; (b) students who were not asked to reflect on their explanations for their calibration judgments but were provided with extrinsic incentives to improve accuracy; (c) students who were asked to reflect on their explanations and provided with extrinsic incentives to improve accuracy; and (d) students who were not asked to reflect on their explanations nor

7 Calibration in Classrooms 7 provided with extrinsic incentives. Also, because of the longitudinal nature of the study, we investigated the within subjects question of whether all students who received an intervention showed improved calibration across the semester. However, because previous studies have demonstrated that calibration varies with student performance (Barnett & Hixon, 1997; Bol & Hacker, 2001; Bol et al., 2005; Flannelly, 2001; Hacker et al., 2000; Kruger & Dunning, 1999, Winne & Jamison-Noel, 2002), our interventions may differentially affect calibration for higher-performing versus lower-performing students. Therefore, we addressed the following research questions; (1) Was calibration improved by our intervention? (2) Does calibration vary with student performance? (3) Do the effects of our interventions to improve calibration vary with student performance? Based on the literature previously discussed, for the between-subjects design we predicted that students who reflected on their explanations or received extrinsic incentives would show improved calibration compared to students who received neither. Students who both reflected on their explanations and received extrinsic incentives would show the greatest improvements in calibration. Moreover, for the within-subjects design component we predicted that students who participated in an intervention would show improvements in calibration across the semester, with students who both reflected and received incentives showing the greatest improvements. We further hypothesized that the effectiveness of our interventions would vary with student performance.

8 Calibration in Classrooms 8 The Role of Attributional Style As previously discussed, laboratory and classroom studies have shown that calibration tends to be relatively stable across time and tasks. Some studies have shown that the stability of students predictions and postdictions of performance is significantly greater than the stability of their performance (e.g., Hacker et al., 2000; Schraw et al., 1993). Such stability suggests that these metacognitive judgments may not be controllable processes as Schraw et al. (1993) have suggested, but rather, may be subject to people s stable and persistent personality traits or beliefs about their performance (Bol et al., 2005; Hacker & Bol, 2004; Hacker et al., 2000). Dunning, Johnson, Ehrlinger, and Kruger (2003) proposed that the stability in people s estimates of their performance arises from a top-down approach. That is, people begin with their preconceived beliefs about their skills and use those beliefs to estimate how well they will do on a task. In other areas of metacognitive research, researchers have found that people make some types of metacognitive judgments not by directly accessing their activated memories, but rather by constructing inferences that may bear a relation to the activated memories but are not directly connected to the memories (e.g., Koriat, 1993). For example, if asked about who the author is to a science fiction novel, a person may base a judgment-of-knowing not on actually knowing the author but on what he or she knows about science fiction novels in general. If the person knows a great deal, he or she will likely infer that the author is known, even though he or she may not know. Thus, the judgment-of-knowing is based on inferences that are generated about one s knowledge and not on actual retrieval of that knowledge. Similarly, we hypothesized that stability in prediction and postdiction judgments may be the result of inferential processes, in this case, inferences based on stable and persistent

9 Calibration in Classrooms 9 personality traits or beliefs about one s performance rather than actual or anticipated performance. Such traits or beliefs have been studied extensively in the social-cognitive literature as attributional style. Attributional style is the general way that individuals explain the causes for their successes or failures in daily events (Graham & Weiner, 1996). Students are said to exhibit a mastery orientation when they attribute their successes to factors internal to themselves, such as ability or effort, and their failures to temporary circumstances that are often under their control, for example, lack of studying (Dweck, 1986; Eccles, Midgley, & Adler, 1984). Students exhibit learned helplessness if they attribute their successes to external and uncontrollable factors, such as luck or an easy test. Conversely, they attribute their failures to internal causes, such as lack of ability, or to external negative factors that are beyond their control. Some support for our hypothesis is provided by Bol et al. (2005), who found significant relationships between calibration and students attributions: Overconfident predictions were related to external attributions and underconfident predictions to internal attributions. Social attributions also may be associated with metacognitive judgments. Jost, Kruglanski, and Nelson (1998) acknowledge that Social and cognitive biases appear to intermingle in the metacognitive realm (p. 140). In support of this thesis, Karabenick (1996) reported that the presence of co-learners questions in a social group setting elicited confusion in the participants, with greater feelings of confusion in knowing as the number of questions increased. Caravalho, Moisses, and Yuzawa (2001) presented participants with social comparative data from a fictitious group and found that the social comparisons had more impact on students with low versus high metacognitive ability and influenced confidence judgments for only low self-regulators. Puncochar and Fox (2004) examined students accuracy and confidence while completing quizzes in groups or individually. They showed that people who

10 Calibration in Classrooms 10 worked in groups to be more accurate and confident in their right answers than people who worked alone. Unfortunately, group confidence for wrong answers continued to increase across quizzes, a finding the authors coined the two heads are worse than one effect. The relationships between attributions and predictions or postdictions, however, may not be direct. Studies have shown that attributions vary with performance: Internal attributions are associated with higher performance and external attributions with lower performance (Bar-Tal & BarZohar, 1977; Borkowski, Carr, Rellinger, & Pressley, 1990; Boss & Taylor, 1989; Seligman, 1991). Other studies have shown that higher performance is associated with underconfidence in predictions and postdictions and lower performance with overconfidence, with the lowestperforming students showing gross overconfidence (e.g., Bol et al. 2005; Hacker et al., 2000). Thus, internal attributions may be associated with underconfident metacognitive judgments and external attributions with overconfident judgments, but these associations may depend on performance level. We addressed this possibility in our fourth research question: (4) Do the relations between attributional style and predictions or postdictions depend on performance level? In answering this question, we made the following predictions. Because higherperforming students tend to be underconfident in their judgments of performance, they must explain why they performed better than what they had judged. To explain the discrepancies between judged and actual performance, we predicted that they should be less inclined to attribute blame for their better performance to factors such as poor instruction, bad study efforts, or to social influences. In contrast, because lower-performing students tend to be overconfident in their judgments of performance, they must explain why they performed worse than what they

11 Calibration in Classrooms 11 had judged. Therefore, we predicted that they should be more inclined to attribute blame for their worse performance to poor instruction, study efforts, or to social influences. Method Participants Participants were 109 female and 28 male freshman, sophomores, or juniors enrolled in a 15-week college introductory educational psychology course. The course is required for most teacher education programs, and students enrolled in the course were either currently in a teacher education program or were about to apply for one. The lowest acceptable grade for the course is a C; therefore, student motivation to perform well was likely high. Students were predominantly white and middle-class. Participation in the study satisfied the research participation requirement for the course. Alternative ways to satisfy the requirement were provided. Design We used a quasi-experimental design in which our interventions were formed by fully crossing extrinsic incentives/no extrinsic incentives with reflection/no reflection: Students received only extrinsic incentives (n = 37), only reflection (n = 27), both extrinsic incentives and reflection (n = 36), or no incentives or reflection (n = 37). The four classroom sections of the course were randomly assigned to one of the four conditions. The textbook, curriculum, and instruction across the classrooms were standardized as were all exams, quizzes, and assignments. There were different instructors in each classroom; however, experimental materials relating to calibration and attributional style were administered by the first author, and instructors did not discuss the materials or the nature of the study with their students. Enrollment in the four sections of the course was open to all students so that there was no bias in academic ability among the classrooms. The curriculum of the course contained nothing about attributional style,

12 Calibration in Classrooms 12 and the instructors neither lectured about it nor encouraged their students to examine attributional style in their testing. An analysis of the four attributional style constructs examined in this study showed that there were no differences among the four groups on any of the constructs except for the task-centered construct, and only one group (i.e., the reflection group) differed from the other three. Measures Performance and calibration. Performance was measured using three exams that were administered across the semester course. Each exam consisted of 35 multiple-choice questions and assessed content covered in discussion groups, lectures, and the textbook. The first author developed all exam items based on a table of specifications aligned with course objectives. The mean scores and standard deviations for the three exams were, (3.97), (3.82), and (3.70), respectively. Calibration data were obtained by asking students prior to testing to predict how many of the 35 items they would get correct and immediately after the exam by asking how many of the items they had gotten correct. Prediction accuracy was calculated using the formula: Prediction - Performance (1 - ) Total Items X 100 Postdiction accuracy was calculated by substituting postdiction for prediction. The values produced by this formula correlate perfectly with the simple absolute value of the difference between prediction/postdiction and performance; however, because the values are expressed as a percentage, calibration is more intuitively clear. For example, a student who predicted a 33 on a 40-item test and earned a 35 would have a calibration score of 95%.

13 Calibration in Classrooms 13 Attributional style. We used an instrument (see Table 1) to measure students attributions regarding calibration that was developed by Bol et al. (2005). Eight items pertained to a taskcentered construct that concerned whether students focused on external factors to explain their calibration. Specific items asked students if they found test items tricky, if the test covered the content of the course, or how adequate the instruction was. Nine items pertained to a studentcentered construct and measured the degree to which students focused on internal factors for their calibration. Five of these items focused on students test-taking abilities, for example, whether students evaluated themselves as good test takers, and four items focused on students studying for the test, for instance, had they directed their studying to the right material. Because previous studies have suggested that students tend to be inaccurate in their predictions and postdictions, most items were worded negatively to elicit explanations of discrepancies between their judgments and their actual performance. Even when students make fairly accurate judgments, it is rare to achieve perfect accuracy. Therefore, the wording of the items would apply to all but the very rare student who may have perfect calibration. Our choice to use negative wording is further supported by Reivich and Gillham s (2003) revised, extended versions of the Attributional Style Questionnaire in which higher reliabilities were obtained when individuals were asked to report their explanations for negative events. Using a sample of 356 participants, Bol and colleagues (2005) examined the factor-analytic structure of the instrument and found that the items loaded on three distinct factors, each one corresponding to the proposed constructs. For the present study, we added a social-centered construct, which was intended to measure students attributions concerning social influences on calibration. Four items were used to measure such things as making performance comparisons with other students or whether the teacher s feedback had influenced their performance judgments.

14 Calibration in Classrooms 14 Students responded to the prompt, How would you explain any discrepancy between how well you thought you would do on the first [second, third] exam and how well you actually did? using a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree (1-to-5, respectively). Therefore, the emphasis of students responses was placed on explaining why their actual performance was better or worse than their self-assessed performance. High scores on the task-centered construct indicated a strong emphasis on external causes for discrepancies; high scores on the studying construct indicated a greater emphasis on internal studying behaviors as causes; high scores on the testing construct indicated a greater emphasis on internal testing behaviors as causes; and high scores on the social-centered construct indicated greater emphasis on external social sources as causes. Low scores on all the constructs indicated that the internal or external causes played little or no role in the discrepancies. We conducted a principal components analysis followed by a principal axis factoring with a varimax rotation and eliminated items with loadings less than.30. The analysis produced a four-factor solution that accounted for 44% of the variability (See Table 2). The Kaiser- Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was.79, exceeding the.60 value considered the lower value for good factor analysis. We also examined the variables for multicollinearity and singularity and found that neither was present. All eight of the task-centered items loaded on the first factor, four of the five student-centered testing items loaded on the second factor (one item did not have a clear factor loading and was eliminated from further analyses), all four of the student-centered studying items loaded on factor three, and all four of the social items loaded on factor four. The reliabilities (Cronbach s alpha) for the task-centered, student-centered testing, student-centered studying, and social-centered factors were.88,.64,.65, and.60, respectively, with an overall reliability of.82. With the exception of one item, our solution replicated Bol and

15 Calibration in Classrooms 15 colleagues (2005) three-factor solution, and the addition of the four social items added a clearly distinct factor. Because the present analysis generated the same number of factors, with the same items loading on the different factors for the two sample groups, we did not proceed with further statistical comparisons to confirm the stability and validity of our instrument (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989, p. 642). Table 2 illustrates the similarities between the two samples. In addition to the attributional style instrument, we asked students in all conditions to respond to open-ended items. The items asked students to identify (a) factors that may have influenced their accuracy and (b) strategies they might employ for enhancing their accuracy. Procedure All students shared the following procedure. Immediately before taking an exam, students predicted their scores and recorded their predictions on sheets of paper. After the exam but before grading, they postdicted their scores and recorded their postdictions. Finally, after recording their postdictions, the first author scored their exams, calculated their calibration, and privately provided each student with his or her calibration scores for prediction and postdiction. For the extrinsic incentive condition, students were told prior to testing that they would receive extra points for greater accuracy: Four points if prediction and postdiction were both one or two points from the exam score, three points if both were three or four points from the exam score, two points if both were five or six points from the exam score, and one point if seven or more points from the exam score. When these students were provided with their calibration scores, they also were told how many extra points they had earned. For the reflection condition, students were asked to reflect on the accuracy of their judgments. Immediately after receiving their calibration scores, these students were given the attributional-style questionnaire. In addition to measuring students attributions, the

16 Calibration in Classrooms 16 questionnaire required that students reflect on explanations for the possible discrepancies between their judged and actual performance. By reflecting on the various internal and external causes for the discrepancies, students were provided with substantive information concerning their mastery of the course material and with guidance to increase their calibration. Students in the extrinsic incentives-plus-reflection condition received a combination of the two interventions just described. Finally, students in the fourth condition received neither incentives nor were asked to reflect. These students simply predicted and postdicted their scores and responded to the attributional-style questionnaire only after the third exam. At the end of the study all students received the same number of extra credit points as compensation for their participation in the study, which satisfied the research requirement for the course. Results Was Calibration Improved? We examined whether calibration was improved using both between- and within-subjects analyses. For the between-subjects analysis we compared the calibration scores for prediction and postdiction on the third exam across the four conditions. Because prior research has shown that calibration is moderated by performance, we equalized performance across the four conditions by using the third exam score as a covariate. We conducted an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for both prediction and postdiction accuracy. The means and standard deviations for each condition are shown in Table 3. Levene s test of equality of error variances indicated that the assumption of homogeneity for either prediction or postdiction was not violated. Our analyses showed no significant group differences for either prediction (p =.14) or postdiction accuracy (p =.20). No intervention led to improvements in calibration. Surprisingly, the

17 Calibration in Classrooms 17 students who did not engage in any intervention predicted and postdicted their performance with about the same degree of accuracy as the students who engaged in an intervention. We analyzed within-subjects calibration accuracy by collapsing across the three interventions and conducting a repeated measures analysis of covariance (RM-ANCOVA) for students prediction and postdiction accuracy (see Table 3). We equalized potential differences among the three different classrooms assigned to the three interventions by using either prediction or postdiction accuracy on the first exam as a covariate and then used prediction and postdiction accuracy on the second and third exams as repeated measures. Also, to eliminate the possibility of calibration differences due to moderating effects of performance, we conducted a paired samples t-test using the two exam scores and found no difference between exam 2 and exam 3 (M = 26.41, SD = 3.59; M = 26.65, SD = 3.52; p =.55). The RM-ANCOVA showed no significant within-subject effects for either prediction (p =.48) or postdiction accuracy (p =.57). Students did not improve their calibration accuracy across the second and third exams. Does Calibration Vary with Student Performance? Studies of calibration consistently have shown that accuracy varies with performance. Therefore, we conducted a similar RM-ANCOVA but with performance as a between-subjects variable. Our performance variable was a dichotomous variable created using a median split on the total of the three exam scores. The median score was 84. To eliminate the potential effects of outliers (i.e., abnormally low scores) for the lower performing group, we restricted the range of scores for both groups to 16. This resulted in the elimination of 5 students. Descriptive statistics on the two groups were: higher performing, n = 48, M = 89.46, SD = 4.05, range 84 to 100; lower performing, n = 46, M = 74.87, SD = 4.96, range 66 to 83.

18 Calibration in Classrooms 18 For prediction and postdiction accuracy, the RM-ANCOVAs showed only significant main effects for performance group, F(1, 91) = 28.46, p <.01, η 2 =.24, F(1, 91) = 12.34, p <.01, η 2 =.12, respectively. Interactions between the repeated measure and performance group were not significant nor were the interactions between the repeated measure and the covariate. Adjusted and unadjusted means and standard errors are shown in Table 4. Higher-performing students showed greater prediction and postdiction accuracy than lower-performing students, but neither higher- nor lower-performing students showed increases in accuracy from the second to third exams. For the higher-performing students, the high rates of accuracy and the constrained standard errors on the unadjusted means for both prediction and postdiction in comparison to the lower-performing students indicate that higher-performing students were nearing a ceiling on both measures. Therefore, whether increased accuracy was possible is uncertain. Do the Effects of Our Interventions to Improve Calibration Vary with Student Performance? Although our interventions did not significantly improve calibration, because we did find strong main effects for calibration by level of performance, we examined whether our interventions impacted calibration differently for lower- than higher-achievers. To test this we conducted a RM-ANCOVA with prediction or postdiction accuracy on exams two and three as the repeated measure, prediction or postdiction accuracy on exam one as a covariate, and both intervention and performance group as between-subjects variables. For prediction and postdiction we found the main effects for performance group, but for postdiction accuracy we also found a significant intervention x performance group x postdiction accuracy interaction, F(2, 87) = 3.89, p <.03, η 2 =.08. Table 5 shows that higher-performing students in all three conditions were consistent in their postdiction accuracy across the two exams, but lower

19 Calibration in Classrooms 19 performing students in the incentives and reflection-plus-incentives groups improved accuracy while the reflection group lost accuracy. To What Extent Does Attributional Style and Performance Contribute to Calibration Judgments? Apart from the question of whether calibration can be improved, we also were interested in identifying what contributes to people s prediction and postdiction judgments. Why do these judgments appear to be so stable across tasks and time? We hypothesized that stability in these judgments may be the result of inferential processes, in this case, inferences based on stable and persistent personality traits or beliefs about one s performance rather than actual or anticipated performance. We identified four attributional-style constructs that have been associated with classroom performance and that could explain such stability, and then examined whether these constructs made significant contributions to calibration beyond performance. First, however, if the attributional-style instrument that we used validly measured the four constructs, the following two predictions should be confirmed: Higher-performing students will be less inclined than lower-performing students to attribute blame for their performance to factors such as poor instruction, bad study efforts, or to social influences. Thus, higher-performing students should score lower on the four attributional-style constructs than lower-performing students. Because students who were assigned to the reflection conditions responded to the attributional-style questionnaire three times during the semester, we avoided biasing of the attributional constructs by those students by using students responses to the questionnaire from its first administration. Therefore, in the following analyses, we used the questionnaire responses, predictions, postdictions, and exam scores from the first exam for those students in the reflection and reflection-plus-incentives condition, and the questionnaire responses, predictions, postdictions, and exam scores from the third exam for those students in the

20 Calibration in Classrooms 20 incentives condition. This may have meant that the predictions and postdictions were biased toward those in the incentives condition, who made repeated calibration judgments by the third exam; however, we conducted two ANOVAs and found that there were no significant differences among the three groups for prediction (p =.69) or postdiction (p =.26). As indicated by the high Cronbach s alpha values (.85 for prediction,.78 for postdiction), students were remarkably consistent in their predictions and postdictions across the three exams. In fact, their consistency in making these judgments equaled or exceeded their consistency in performance (Cronbach s alpha =.77). We conducted a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) using the dichotomous median split on performance as the independent variable and the four attributional-style constructs as the dependent variables. Levene s test of equality of error variances indicated that we did not violate the assumption of homogeneity for any of the variables. The analysis confirmed our prediction that all four measures were significantly lower for higher-performing students than for lower-performing students (see Table 6). We then investigated the unique contributions that the four attributional-style constructs made to students predictions and postdictions. Given that higher-performing students as compared to lower-performing students scored lower on the four constructs, we expected that any contributions these constructs made to students predictions and postdictions would be less for the former group than for the latter. Moreover, because of the strong relationships between exam score and prediction and postdiction that were found in this study (see Table 7), and is commonly found in other studies, we examined the unique contributions from exam score. We used standard multiple regression, with exam score entered first and the attributional-style constructs entered as a separate block.

21 Calibration in Classrooms 21 For higher-performing students, we found that exam score was a significant contributor to students predictions and postdictions; however, the attributional-style constructs did not significantly add to the R 2 value for either judgment. For lower-performing students, the attributional-style constructs were significant contributors beyond exam scores for both prediction and postdiction (see Tables 8 & 9). For prediction, the attributional variables increased the adjusted R 2 value from.04 to.24, and for postdiction, increased the adjusted R 2 value from.31 to.57. An examination of the four constructs shows that the social and studying variables were responsible for the increases in R 2 for both prediction and postdiction. In sum, for higher-performing students, how much they knew about the to-be-tested course material was the most significant contributor to their predictions and postdictions and attributional style did not add to this. For lower-performing students, beyond how much students knew about the to-betested material, the attributional constructs concerning their internal studying behaviors and external social influences strongly contributed to their predictions and postdictions. Qualitative Responses by Attributional Style and Performance In addition to quantitative analyses, we explored differences in attributional style by performance using students qualitative responses to the open-ended questions on the attributional-style questionnaire. The first question asked them to identify factors that influenced the accuracy of their predictions and postdictions. We generated a coding scheme of the students responses using the four attributional constructs and added an other category to include themes that did not fit within the four constructs or themes. Within each construct and the other category, we developed categories through multiple readings of the data. The development of categories was conducted by two researchers to strengthen the credibility of findings through triangulation. An inductive approach was employed in which the researchers

22 Calibration in Classrooms 22 identified topics among responses and clustered related topics. Category labels that best captured the meaning for a cluster of topics were generated. Once a set of categories was developed, we conducted trial codings to ensure that the categories were mutually exclusive and best represented the data. Because attributional styles differed by performance group, we divided students into higher- and lower-performing groups using the same median split that we used earlier and then examined responses for higher- and lower-performing students for each construct and other category. Responses were calculated as percentages of the number of responses coded by performance group, not on the number of students in the study. Only those categories that contained at least two percent of the responses were included. Idiosyncratic responses not represented by the categories were excluded. Table 10 presents each attributional construct, the categories within those constructs, and the percentages. We found that higher- and lower-performing students differed little in identifying taskcentered constructs as influencing their accuracy (19% for higher performers and 17% for lower). Most responses pertained to the students expectations about and reactions to tests. For example, one higher-performing student wrote, This test seemed a lot different than the previous two. A lower-performing student asserted, the exam is not at all like the quizzes. Also I learn better through discussion and this class was lacking in discussion. In both cases, the discrepancy between scores and predictions and postdictions was attributed to an external source, characteristics of the task, unexpected items, or test content. Differences between groups emerged on the student-centered constructs. Lowerperforming students were more likely to attribute discrepancies between scores and calibration judgments to student-centered studying sources (48% for lower performers vs. 31% for higher). The themes within this construct reflected how much or how well they studied, the study content,

23 Calibration in Classrooms 23 or how well they felt they knew the material. One lower-performing student said that her calibration was negatively influenced by, How well I was able to focus on the material while I studied I wasn t able to focus I didn t retain much and I didn t study enough. In reference to not studying the right or enough content, another lower-performing student said, I didn t know what the content was that was most important, so I guessed on what to study as well as how well I should do. These comments demonstrate students awareness that a lack of familiarity with course material influenced their abilities to predict and postdict their performance. A third theme pertained to how well students felt they knew the material. Many explained, I just based my predictions on how well I knew the material. Others used phrases such as how comfortable, confident, or prepared they were to make their predictions or postdictions. Again, students identified a relationship between their level of knowledge of the material and their calibration abilities. In contrast, higher-performing students were more likely than lower-performing students to attribute discrepancies between scores and calibration judgments to test-taking ability or performance sources (24% for higher performers vs. 19% for lower). However, the two groups differed little across the three themes within the student-centered testing construct. Two of the themes related to using prior quiz or test performance. One higher-performing student noted, I just figured I would do the same as I did on other exams in this class because I prepared in the same way. A third theme to emerge was anxiety and other stressors related to the test. For instance, one student was nervous before grading so I lowered my postdictions. A lowerperforming student wrote, I missed class last week. I don t take tests well. I have terrible test anxiety. Anxiety was at least as common in the higher performers as lower (7% vs. 5%).

24 Calibration in Classrooms 24 Given the prominent role that social attributes played in contributing to lower-performing students predictions and postdictions, as indicated in our earlier regression analyses, we were surprised that only higher-performing students (2%) provided open-ended comments categorized as social. For example, Others around had lower postdiction scores or got lower grades than they expected, so I lowered my postdiction 1 point. This illustrates at least some awareness of social influences among this group of students. There were numerous responses that did not fit into the attributional-style constructs, and higher-performing students tended to provide more of these other explanations than lowerperforming students (24% vs. 17%, respectively). When comparing the responses by themes within this category, we found that higher-performing students were more likely to attribute discrepancies between scores and calibration judgments to a lack of confidence or personal expectations. Given that these students scored well on the exams, these types of responses were unexpected. I usually do very well on tests, but I always underestimate my abilities. Another admitted, after taking tests I always worry I didn t do well because I second guess my answers and knowledge. Only responses from the higher-performing students were coded as wishing or hoping for high scores or to avoid disappointment. One student said, I just took a guess in hopes that I would get that many right. Another explained, I went low so I wouldn t be disappointed. Lower-performing students were a bit more likely to attribute the discrepancies between scores and calibration judgments to self-knowledge or intuition. One student wrote, I know myself and how much I studied. Another noted, I am so unpredictable. I can study a lot and bomb a test, or hardly study at all and do ok so I am never sure how well I ll do.

25 Calibration in Classrooms 25 Discussion Our goal for the present study was to understand cognition in the context of natural purposeful activity (Neisser, 1976, p. 7). In particular, we sought to expand on laboratory studies of calibration by examining calibration in classroom contexts, in this case, four classes of an undergraduate introductory educational psychology course. Because of the contextual differences between the laboratory and classroom most notably, more complex and multi-form learning, longer periods of learning, greater motivation, and longer delays between learning and testing we, along with others (e.g., Lundeberg & Fox, 1991; McCormick, 2003; Winne, 2004) believe that laboratory findings may not readily generalize to classroom contexts. Admittedly, the quasi-experiment that we conducted did not possess the degree of control that is possible in laboratory studies; however, we took several measures to hold as many variables constant across the four classes as possible: the classes were randomly assigned to an intervention; the teachers in the classes did differ, but each teacher had similar experiences teaching the course; the curriculum was standardized, as were the exams; student performance on the exams did not differ across the classes; the curriculum did not include material on attributional style; the instructors neither lectured about attributional style nor encouraged their students to examine it in the context of the course; and the teachers did not administer the interventions. We recognize that conducting research in naturalistic contexts will not provide the same degree of rigor as a true experiment, but we also recognize that quasi-experiments may be the best alternative available when investigating behavior and cognition in naturalistic contexts. Because classroom studies of calibration have produced mixed results, we sought to resolve some of these discrepant findings by investigating three aspects of calibration: (a) how well calibrated students can be, (b) whether incentives or reflective activities can improve

26 Calibration in Classrooms 26 calibration, and (c) whether several social-cognitive factors that are known to contribute to classroom learning also contribute to calibration. We used three interventions, each being administered three times across a 15-week course, once after each of three exams. Our interventions were developed by fully crossing two independent variables: reflection/no reflection and extrinsic incentives/no extrinsic incentives. Using a between-subjects design, we compared calibration of students across four conditions: (a) students who were asked to reflect on explanations for their calibration judgments but were not provided with extrinsic incentives to improve accuracy; (b) students who were not asked to reflect on their explanations for their calibration judgments but were provided with extrinsic incentives to improve accuracy; (c) students who were asked to reflect on their explanations and provided with extrinsic incentives to improve accuracy; and (d) students who were not asked to reflect on their explanations nor provided with extrinsic incentives. Also, because of the longitudinal nature of the study, we investigated in a within-subjects design whether students who received an intervention showed improved calibration across the semester. When all students were considered together, we found that our interventions did not impact calibration. Calibration was remarkably consistent across the 15 weeks, even for the control group who engaged in no intervention. However, when we examined calibration in terms of student performance, we found numerous differences between higher-performing and lowerperforming students. Higher-performing students students who scored above the median on the combined three examinations were well calibrated, that is, the discrepancies between predictions or postdictions and actual performance approached zero. These students were about 94% accurate in their predictions and postdictions, which means that their judgments differed from perfect

27 Calibration in Classrooms 27 accuracy by only 1 or 2 items on an exam. Given the constrained standard deviations for this group, their accuracy was approaching a ceiling. This degree of accuracy differs markedly from many laboratory studies that have shown accuracy rates ranging from 50-75%. Thus, in a classroom context, students who possess high levels of knowledge of the course content can be expected to be well calibrated. These findings suggest along with many other studies that highly accurate calibration may be possible only when a person possesses high knowledge of the content being judged (e.g., Dunning, Heath, & Suls, 2004; Hacker et al., 2000; Kruger & Dunning, 1999; Winne & Jamieson-Noel, 2002). In our investigations of attributional style, we found that the relations between attributional style and predictions or postdictions do depend on performance level. Our predictions were confirmed that higher-performing students would be less likely than lowerperforming students to attribute blame for the discrepancies between judged performance and actual performance to class instruction, study efforts, or social influences. These findings seem intuitively clear in that if students believe there is less of a discrepancy between judged and actual performance, there is less reason to find excuses for inaccurate judgments. Therefore, none of the attributional-style constructs contributed beyond exam score to higher-performing students predictions or postdictions. Lower-performing students students who scored below the median on the combined three examinations presented a much different picture. These students were not as well calibrated as higher-performing students, with mean prediction and postdiction accuracy at about 86-88%. Although these values are high, there was room for improvement, and in our analyses of the interactions among the interventions and student performance we did find some evidence of improvement. Lower-performing students in all conditions showed stable prediction accuracy

Calibration research: where do we go from here?

Calibration research: where do we go from here? HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY ARTICLE published: 09 July 2012 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00229 : where do we go from here? Linda Bol 1 * and Douglas J. Hacker 2 1 Department of Educational Foundations and Leadership,

More information

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science Ghali Hassan Faculty of Education, Queensland University of Technology, Australia Abstract The purpose of this exploratory study was

More information

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students Mia Bonner Millersville University Abstract Previous research (Elion, Slaney, Wang and French, 2012) found

More information

Reliability, validity, and all that jazz

Reliability, validity, and all that jazz Reliability, validity, and all that jazz Dylan Wiliam King s College London Introduction No measuring instrument is perfect. The most obvious problems relate to reliability. If we use a thermometer to

More information

Teachers Sense of Efficacy Scale: The Study of Validity and Reliability

Teachers Sense of Efficacy Scale: The Study of Validity and Reliability EUROPEAN ACADEMIC RESEARCH Vol. II, Issue 12/ March 2015 ISSN 2286-4822 www.euacademic.org Impact Factor: 3.1 (UIF) DRJI Value: 5.9 (B+) Teachers Sense of Efficacy Scale: The Study of Validity and Dr.

More information

The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016

The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016 The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016 This course does not cover how to perform statistical tests on SPSS or any other computer program. There are several courses

More information

HPS301 Exam Notes- Contents

HPS301 Exam Notes- Contents HPS301 Exam Notes- Contents Week 1 Research Design: What characterises different approaches 1 Experimental Design 1 Key Features 1 Criteria for establishing causality 2 Validity Internal Validity 2 Threats

More information

VARIABLES AND MEASUREMENT

VARIABLES AND MEASUREMENT ARTHUR SYC 204 (EXERIMENTAL SYCHOLOGY) 16A LECTURE NOTES [01/29/16] VARIABLES AND MEASUREMENT AGE 1 Topic #3 VARIABLES AND MEASUREMENT VARIABLES Some definitions of variables include the following: 1.

More information

Using Multiple Methods to Distinguish Active Delay and Procrastination in College Students

Using Multiple Methods to Distinguish Active Delay and Procrastination in College Students Using Multiple Methods to Distinguish Active Delay and Procrastination in College Students Suzanne F Lindt Midwestern State University Danya M Corkin Houston ISD Shirley L Yu The Ohio State University

More information

Reliability, validity, and all that jazz

Reliability, validity, and all that jazz Reliability, validity, and all that jazz Dylan Wiliam King s College London Published in Education 3-13, 29 (3) pp. 17-21 (2001) Introduction No measuring instrument is perfect. If we use a thermometer

More information

Retrospective bias in test performance: Providing easy items at the beginning of a test makes students believe they did better on it

Retrospective bias in test performance: Providing easy items at the beginning of a test makes students believe they did better on it Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (3), 366-376 doi:10.3758/mc.38.3.366 Retrospective bias in test performance: Providing easy items at the beginning of a test makes students believe they did better on it YANA

More information

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 6.1 Research Design Research is an organized, systematic, data based, critical, objective, scientific inquiry or investigation into a specific problem, undertaken with the

More information

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity Measurement & Variables - Initial step is to conceptualize and clarify the concepts embedded in a hypothesis or research question with

More information

Running head: How large denominators are leading to large errors 1

Running head: How large denominators are leading to large errors 1 Running head: How large denominators are leading to large errors 1 How large denominators are leading to large errors Nathan Thomas Kent State University How large denominators are leading to large errors

More information

Test-Taking Strategies and Task-based Assessment: The Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Test-Taking Strategies and Task-based Assessment: The Case of Iranian EFL Learners Test-Taking Strategies and Task-based Assessment: The Case of Iranian EFL Learners Hossein Barati Department of English, Faculty of Foreign Languages, University of Isfahan barati@yahoo.com Zohreh Kashkoul*

More information

Quantitative Methods in Computing Education Research (A brief overview tips and techniques)

Quantitative Methods in Computing Education Research (A brief overview tips and techniques) Quantitative Methods in Computing Education Research (A brief overview tips and techniques) Dr Judy Sheard Senior Lecturer Co-Director, Computing Education Research Group Monash University judy.sheard@monash.edu

More information

2013 Supervisor Survey Reliability Analysis

2013 Supervisor Survey Reliability Analysis 2013 Supervisor Survey Reliability Analysis In preparation for the submission of the Reliability Analysis for the 2013 Supervisor Survey, we wanted to revisit the purpose of this analysis. This analysis

More information

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2014, 42(7), 1105-1116 Society for Personality Research http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2014.42.7.1105 ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN

More information

PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES OF CLINICAL PERFORMANCE RATINGS

PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES OF CLINICAL PERFORMANCE RATINGS PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES OF CLINICAL PERFORMANCE RATINGS A total of 7931 ratings of 482 third- and fourth-year medical students were gathered over twelve four-week periods. Ratings were made by multiple

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions Readings: OpenStax Textbook - Chapters 1 5 (online) Appendix D & E (online) Plous - Chapters 1, 5, 6, 13 (online) Introductory comments Describe how familiarity with statistical methods can - be associated

More information

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH The following document provides background information on the research and development of the Emergenetics Profile instrument. Emergenetics Defined 1. Emergenetics

More information

Propensity Score Methods for Estimating Causality in the Absence of Random Assignment: Applications for Child Care Policy Research

Propensity Score Methods for Estimating Causality in the Absence of Random Assignment: Applications for Child Care Policy Research 2012 CCPRC Meeting Methodology Presession Workshop October 23, 2012, 2:00-5:00 p.m. Propensity Score Methods for Estimating Causality in the Absence of Random Assignment: Applications for Child Care Policy

More information

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo Business Statistics The following was provided by Dr. Suzanne Delaney, and is a comprehensive review of Business Statistics. The workshop instructor will provide relevant examples during the Skills Assessment

More information

Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting

Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2012, 65 (11), 2231 2257 Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting Michael J. Serra and Benjamin D. England Department

More information

Chapter 2. Behavioral Variability and Research

Chapter 2. Behavioral Variability and Research Chapter 2 Behavioral Variability and Research Chapter Outline Variability and the Research Process Variance: An Index of Variability Systematic and Error Variance Effect Size: Assessing the Strength of

More information

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE Previous chapter namely Review of the Literature was concerned with the review of the research studies conducted in the field of teacher education, with special reference

More information

3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS

3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS 3 CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATIONS OF STATISTICS In this chapter, we examine the conceptual foundations of statistics. The goal is to give you an appreciation and conceptual understanding of some basic statistical

More information

CHAPTER 10 Educational Psychology: Motivating Students to Learn

CHAPTER 10 Educational Psychology: Motivating Students to Learn BEFORE YOU READ... The material included in Chapter 10 focuses on motivation: the internal processes that activate, guide, and maintain behavior. Some of the theoretical concepts associated with motivation

More information

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Adam Hahn 1 Bertram Gawronski 2 Word count: 20,754 excluding acknowledgements, abstract,

More information

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: SOCIAL AFFILIATION CUES PRIME HELP-SEEKING INTENTIONS 1 This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: Rubin, M. (2011). Social

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo Please note the page numbers listed for the Lind book may vary by a page or two depending on which version of the textbook you have. Readings: Lind 1 11 (with emphasis on chapters 10, 11) Please note chapter

More information

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework Modified from Organizing Scientific Thinking Using the QuALMRI Framework Written by Kevin Ochsner and modified by others. Based on a scheme devised by

More information

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions Readings: OpenStax Textbook - Chapters 1 5 (online) Appendix D & E (online) Plous - Chapters 1, 5, 6, 13 (online) Introductory comments Describe how familiarity with statistical methods can - be associated

More information

The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation

The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation Running head: SELF-EFFICACY, FEEDBACK PERCEPTION AND INTRINSIC MOTIVATION The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation Group 16 Lindy Wijsman

More information

2016 Children and young people s inpatient and day case survey

2016 Children and young people s inpatient and day case survey NHS Patient Survey Programme 2016 Children and young people s inpatient and day case survey Technical details for analysing trust-level results Published November 2017 CQC publication Contents 1. Introduction...

More information

CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHOD. method the major components include: Research Design, Research Site and

CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHOD. method the major components include: Research Design, Research Site and CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHOD This chapter presents the research method and design. In this research method the major components include: Research Design, Research Site and Access, Population and Sample,

More information

AN ANALYSIS ON VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY OF TEST ITEMS IN PRE-NATIONAL EXAMINATION TEST SMPN 14 PONTIANAK

AN ANALYSIS ON VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY OF TEST ITEMS IN PRE-NATIONAL EXAMINATION TEST SMPN 14 PONTIANAK AN ANALYSIS ON VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY OF TEST ITEMS IN PRE-NATIONAL EXAMINATION TEST SMPN 14 PONTIANAK Hanny Pradana, Gatot Sutapa, Luwandi Suhartono Sarjana Degree of English Language Education, Teacher

More information

Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS

Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS INTEREST and ENJOYMENT TASK VALUE GOALS (Purposes for doing) INTRINSIC vs EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION EXPECTANCY BELIEFS SELF-EFFICACY SELF-CONCEPT (Self-Esteem) OUTCOME

More information

Business Statistics Probability

Business Statistics Probability Business Statistics The following was provided by Dr. Suzanne Delaney, and is a comprehensive review of Business Statistics. The workshop instructor will provide relevant examples during the Skills Assessment

More information

Chapter 5: Field experimental designs in agriculture

Chapter 5: Field experimental designs in agriculture Chapter 5: Field experimental designs in agriculture Jose Crossa Biometrics and Statistics Unit Crop Research Informatics Lab (CRIL) CIMMYT. Int. Apdo. Postal 6-641, 06600 Mexico, DF, Mexico Introduction

More information

REPEATED MEASURES DESIGNS

REPEATED MEASURES DESIGNS Repeated Measures Designs The SAGE Encyclopedia of Educational Research, Measurement and Evaluation Markus Brauer (University of Wisconsin-Madison) Target word count: 1000 - Actual word count: 1071 REPEATED

More information

Combining Dual Scaling with Semi-Structured Interviews to Interpret Rating Differences

Combining Dual Scaling with Semi-Structured Interviews to Interpret Rating Differences A peer-reviewed electronic journal. Copyright is retained by the first or sole author, who grants right of first publication to the Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation. Permission is granted to

More information

Student Performance Q&A:

Student Performance Q&A: Student Performance Q&A: 2009 AP Statistics Free-Response Questions The following comments on the 2009 free-response questions for AP Statistics were written by the Chief Reader, Christine Franklin of

More information

Calibration in tennis: The role of feedback and expertise

Calibration in tennis: The role of feedback and expertise Calibration in tennis: The role of feedback and expertise Gerard J. Fogarty (fogarty@usq.edu.au) Department of Psychology University of Southern Queensland, Toowoomba QLD 4350 Australia Anthony Ross (a_ross4@hotmail.com)

More information

The Asian Conference on Education & International Development 2015 Official Conference Proceedings. iafor

The Asian Conference on Education & International Development 2015 Official Conference Proceedings. iafor Constructing and Validating Behavioral Components Scale of Motivational Goals in Mathematics Nancy Castro, University of Santo Tomas, Philippines Michelle Cruz, University of Santo Tomas, Philippines Maria

More information

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing? Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC How are we doing? Prepared by Maura McCool, M.S. Office of Research, Evaluation and Assessment Metropolitan Community Colleges Fall 2003 1 General Education Assessment

More information

Massachusetts Alzheimer s Disease & Other Dementias Online Training Program 2017

Massachusetts Alzheimer s Disease & Other Dementias Online Training Program 2017 Massachusetts Alzheimer s Disease & Other Dementias Online Training Program 2017 Final Evaluation Report from Boston University April 2018 1 I. INTRODUCTION Boston University s Center for Aging and Disability

More information

INTRODUCTION TO STATISTICS SORANA D. BOLBOACĂ

INTRODUCTION TO STATISTICS SORANA D. BOLBOACĂ INTRODUCTION TO STATISTICS SORANA D. BOLBOACĂ OBJECTIVES Definitions Stages of Scientific Knowledge Quantification and Accuracy Types of Medical Data Population and sample Sampling methods DEFINITIONS

More information

Variable Measurement, Norms & Differences

Variable Measurement, Norms & Differences Variable Measurement, Norms & Differences 1 Expectations Begins with hypothesis (general concept) or question Create specific, testable prediction Prediction can specify relation or group differences Different

More information

Unit 1 History and Methods Chapter 1 Thinking Critically with Psychological Science

Unit 1 History and Methods Chapter 1 Thinking Critically with Psychological Science Myers PSYCHOLOGY (7th Ed) Unit 1 History and Methods Chapter 1 Thinking Critically with James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University Worth Publishers Fact vs. Falsehood 1. Human intuition is remarkably accurate

More information

Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed

Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed 1. Method and measures 1.1 Sample Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed In the cross-country study, the age of the participants ranged between 20 and 79 years, with

More information

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation The description and presentation of results involves a number of topics. These include scales of measurement, descriptive statistics used to summarize

More information

Exam 2 PS 306, Spring 2004

Exam 2 PS 306, Spring 2004 Exam 2 PS 306, Spring 2004 1. Briefly define the term confound. Then, using a very explicit example of practice effects (maybe even with numbers?), illustrate why conducting a repeated measures experiment

More information

Psychology Department Assessment

Psychology Department Assessment Psychology Department Assessment 2008 2009 The 2008-2009 Psychology assessment included an evaluation of graduating psychology seniors regarding their experience in the program, an analysis of introductory

More information

Essential Skills for Evidence-based Practice: Statistics for Therapy Questions

Essential Skills for Evidence-based Practice: Statistics for Therapy Questions Essential Skills for Evidence-based Practice: Statistics for Therapy Questions Jeanne Grace Corresponding author: J. Grace E-mail: Jeanne_Grace@urmc.rochester.edu Jeanne Grace RN PhD Emeritus Clinical

More information

TLQ Reliability, Validity and Norms

TLQ Reliability, Validity and Norms MSP Research Note TLQ Reliability, Validity and Norms Introduction This research note describes the reliability and validity of the TLQ. Evidence for the reliability and validity of is presented against

More information

Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz

Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz This study presents the steps Edgenuity uses to evaluate the reliability and validity of its quizzes, topic tests, and cumulative

More information

EVALUATING AND IMPROVING MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS

EVALUATING AND IMPROVING MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS DePaul University INTRODUCTION TO ITEM ANALYSIS: EVALUATING AND IMPROVING MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS Ivan Hernandez, PhD OVERVIEW What is Item Analysis? Overview Benefits of Item Analysis Applications Main

More information

SURVEY TOPIC INVOLVEMENT AND NONRESPONSE BIAS 1

SURVEY TOPIC INVOLVEMENT AND NONRESPONSE BIAS 1 SURVEY TOPIC INVOLVEMENT AND NONRESPONSE BIAS 1 Brian A. Kojetin (BLS), Eugene Borgida and Mark Snyder (University of Minnesota) Brian A. Kojetin, Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2 Massachusetts Ave. N.E.,

More information

Decision Making Consequences of the Paradoxical Flip. Houston F. Lester

Decision Making Consequences of the Paradoxical Flip. Houston F. Lester Decision Making Consequences of the Paradoxical Flip by Houston F. Lester A thesis submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Auburn University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Master

More information

The Relationship between YouTube Interaction, Depression, and Social Anxiety. By Meredith Johnson

The Relationship between YouTube Interaction, Depression, and Social Anxiety. By Meredith Johnson The Relationship between YouTube Interaction, Depression, and Social Anxiety By Meredith Johnson Introduction The media I would like to research is YouTube with the effects of social anxiety and depression.

More information

Measuring mathematics anxiety: Paper 2 - Constructing and validating the measure. Rob Cavanagh Len Sparrow Curtin University

Measuring mathematics anxiety: Paper 2 - Constructing and validating the measure. Rob Cavanagh Len Sparrow Curtin University Measuring mathematics anxiety: Paper 2 - Constructing and validating the measure Rob Cavanagh Len Sparrow Curtin University R.Cavanagh@curtin.edu.au Abstract The study sought to measure mathematics anxiety

More information

Lec 02: Estimation & Hypothesis Testing in Animal Ecology

Lec 02: Estimation & Hypothesis Testing in Animal Ecology Lec 02: Estimation & Hypothesis Testing in Animal Ecology Parameter Estimation from Samples Samples We typically observe systems incompletely, i.e., we sample according to a designed protocol. We then

More information

Still important ideas

Still important ideas Readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 11 + 13 & Appendix D & E (online) Plous - Chapters 2, 3, and 4 Chapter 2: Cognitive Dissonance, Chapter 3: Memory and Hindsight Bias, Chapter 4: Context Dependence Still

More information

Underestimating Correlation from Scatterplots

Underestimating Correlation from Scatterplots Underestimating Correlation from Scatterplots Robert F. Strahan and Chris J. Hansen Iowa State University Eighty subjects estimated the correlation coefficient, r, for each of 13 computer-printed scatterplots.

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 4 Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Online readings: Plous - Chapters 1, 5, 6, 13

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 4 Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Online readings: Plous - Chapters 1, 5, 6, 13 Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 4 Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Online readings: Plous - Chapters 1, 5, 6, 13 Introductory comments Describe how familiarity with statistical methods

More information

A framework for predicting item difficulty in reading tests

A framework for predicting item difficulty in reading tests Australian Council for Educational Research ACEReSearch OECD Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) National and International Surveys 4-2012 A framework for predicting item difficulty in

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 4, Number 1 Submitted: August 20, 2008 Revisions: October 16, 2008 Accepted: October 17, 2008 Publication Date: October 20, 2008 Start Today or the Very

More information

Section 4 - Dealing with Anxious Thinking

Section 4 - Dealing with Anxious Thinking Section 4 - Dealing with Anxious Thinking How do we challenge our unhelpful thoughts? Anxiety may decrease if we closely examine how realistic and true our unhelpful/negative thoughts are. We may find

More information

The Role of Metacognitive and Self-Efficacy Beliefs in Students' Test Anxiety and Academic Achievement

The Role of Metacognitive and Self-Efficacy Beliefs in Students' Test Anxiety and Academic Achievement Australian Journal of Basic and Applied Sciences, 6(12): 418-422, 2012 ISSN 1991-8178 The Role of Metacognitive and Self-Efficacy Beliefs in Students' Test Anxiety and Academic 1 H.M. Abdi, 2 S. Bageri,

More information

Hypothesis Testing. Richard S. Balkin, Ph.D., LPC-S, NCC

Hypothesis Testing. Richard S. Balkin, Ph.D., LPC-S, NCC Hypothesis Testing Richard S. Balkin, Ph.D., LPC-S, NCC Overview When we have questions about the effect of a treatment or intervention or wish to compare groups, we use hypothesis testing Parametric statistics

More information

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance 1 Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Erica Behrens Winona State University Abstract This study examines the effects of self-consciousness

More information

Samantha Wright. September 03, 2003

Samantha Wright. September 03, 2003 BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory By Reuven Bar-On, Ph.D. Development Report Name: ID: Admin. Date: Samantha Wright September 03, 2003 The information given in this report should be used as a means of

More information

Best on the Left or on the Right in a Likert Scale

Best on the Left or on the Right in a Likert Scale Best on the Left or on the Right in a Likert Scale Overview In an informal poll of 150 educated research professionals attending the 2009 Sawtooth Conference, 100% of those who voted raised their hands

More information

The Effect of Being a Student-Athlete on Academic Performance

The Effect of Being a Student-Athlete on Academic Performance The Effect of Being a Student-Athlete on Academic Performance Thomas Kohs Department of Biological Sciences, College of Liberal Arts & Sciences and the Honors College University of Illinois at Chicago

More information

The effects of domain knowledge on metacomprehension accuracy

The effects of domain knowledge on metacomprehension accuracy Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1001-1013 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1001 The effects of domain knowledge on metacomprehension accuracy THOMAS D. GRIFFIN, BENJAMIN D. JEE, AND JENNIFER WILEY University of Illinois,

More information

CHAPTER V. Summary and Recommendations. policies, including uniforms (Behling, 1994). The purpose of this study was to

CHAPTER V. Summary and Recommendations. policies, including uniforms (Behling, 1994). The purpose of this study was to HAPTER V Summary and Recommendations The current belief that fashionable clothing worn to school by students influences their attitude and behavior is the major impetus behind the adoption of stricter

More information

Title: Validation of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire with parents of 10-to-12-year-olds

Title: Validation of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire with parents of 10-to-12-year-olds Author's response to reviews Title: Validation of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire with parents of 10-to-12-year-olds Authors: Elisabeth L Melbye (elisabeth.l.melbye@uis.no) Torvald Øgaard

More information

IDEA Technical Report No. 20. Updated Technical Manual for the IDEA Feedback System for Administrators. Stephen L. Benton Dan Li

IDEA Technical Report No. 20. Updated Technical Manual for the IDEA Feedback System for Administrators. Stephen L. Benton Dan Li IDEA Technical Report No. 20 Updated Technical Manual for the IDEA Feedback System for Administrators Stephen L. Benton Dan Li July 2018 2 Table of Contents Introduction... 5 Sample Description... 6 Response

More information

ADMS Sampling Technique and Survey Studies

ADMS Sampling Technique and Survey Studies Principles of Measurement Measurement As a way of understanding, evaluating, and differentiating characteristics Provides a mechanism to achieve precision in this understanding, the extent or quality As

More information

Evaluation of the Type 1 Diabetes Priority Setting Partnership

Evaluation of the Type 1 Diabetes Priority Setting Partnership Evaluation of the Type 1 Diabetes Priority Setting Partnership Introduction The James Lind Alliance (JLA) Type 1 Diabetes Priority Setting Partnership (PSP) was established in 2010. The PSP began its process

More information

Exploring a Counterintuitive Finding with Methodological Implications

Exploring a Counterintuitive Finding with Methodological Implications Exploring a Counterintuitive Finding with Methodological Implications Why is 9 > 221 in a Between-subjects Design? Stuart J. McKelvie Department of Psychology Bishop s University 2600 College Street, Sherbrooke,

More information

Section 7 Assessment. CAT 1 - Background Knowledge Probe. Carol Donlon EDAE 590. Colorado State University. Dr. Jeff Foley

Section 7 Assessment. CAT 1 - Background Knowledge Probe. Carol Donlon EDAE 590. Colorado State University. Dr. Jeff Foley Section 7 Assessment CAT 1 - Background Knowledge Probe Carol Donlon EDAE 590 Colorado State University Dr. Jeff Foley May 3, 2013 1 Background and Reason for Selecting CAT 1 For my CAT 7 assignment, I

More information

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels A peer-reviewed electronic journal. Copyright is retained by the first or sole author, who grants right of first publication to Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation. Permission is granted to distribute

More information

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Heather Dudley What influences people s willingness to interact with someone of a different cultural background from

More information

Organizing Scientific Thinking Using the QuALMRI Framework

Organizing Scientific Thinking Using the QuALMRI Framework Organizing Scientific Thinking Using the QuALMRI Framework Written by Kevin Ochsner and modified by others. Based on a scheme devised by Steve Kosslyn. This handout outlines a structured process for generating,

More information

Research Questions, Variables, and Hypotheses: Part 2. Review. Hypotheses RCS /7/04. What are research questions? What are variables?

Research Questions, Variables, and Hypotheses: Part 2. Review. Hypotheses RCS /7/04. What are research questions? What are variables? Research Questions, Variables, and Hypotheses: Part 2 RCS 6740 6/7/04 1 Review What are research questions? What are variables? Definition Function Measurement Scale 2 Hypotheses OK, now that we know how

More information

investigate. educate. inform.

investigate. educate. inform. investigate. educate. inform. Research Design What drives your research design? The battle between Qualitative and Quantitative is over Think before you leap What SHOULD drive your research design. Advanced

More information

ISSN X Journal of Educational and Social Research Vol. 2 (8) October 2012

ISSN X Journal of Educational and Social Research Vol. 2 (8) October 2012 The Investigation of Czech Lower Secondary School Pupils Toward Science Subjects Milan Kubiatko Doi:10.5901/jesr.2012.v2n8p11 Institute for Research in School Education, Masaryk University, Brno,Czech

More information

Student attributions and performance: problems with unfounded optimism?

Student attributions and performance: problems with unfounded optimism? Research in Higher Education Journal Abstract Student attributions and performance: problems with unfounded optimism? Shaun A Saunders Consulting Psychologist, Australia John Nolan University of Newcastle,

More information

What Solution-Focused Coaches Do: An Empirical Test of an Operationalization of Solution-Focused Coach Behaviors

What Solution-Focused Coaches Do: An Empirical Test of an Operationalization of Solution-Focused Coach Behaviors www.solutionfocusedchange.com February, 2012 What Solution-Focused Coaches Do: An Empirical Test of an Operationalization of Solution-Focused Coach Behaviors Coert F. Visser In an attempt to operationalize

More information

AP Psychology -- Chapter 02 Review Research Methods in Psychology

AP Psychology -- Chapter 02 Review Research Methods in Psychology AP Psychology -- Chapter 02 Review Research Methods in Psychology 1. In the opening vignette, to what was Alicia's condition linked? The death of her parents and only brother 2. What did Pennebaker s study

More information

Designing a Questionnaire

Designing a Questionnaire Designing a Questionnaire What Makes a Good Questionnaire? As a rule of thumb, never to attempt to design a questionnaire! A questionnaire is very easy to design, but a good questionnaire is virtually

More information

The Effect of Perspective on Misconceptions in Psychology: A Test of Conceptual Change Theory 1. Eric Amsel. Department of Psychology

The Effect of Perspective on Misconceptions in Psychology: A Test of Conceptual Change Theory 1. Eric Amsel. Department of Psychology The Effect of 1 Running head: CONCEPTUAL CHANGE The Effect of Perspective on Misconceptions in Psychology: A Test of Conceptual Change Theory 1 Eric Amsel Department of Psychology Weber State University

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information