Event frequency and comparative optimism: Another look at the indirect elicitation method of self-others risks q

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Event frequency and comparative optimism: Another look at the indirect elicitation method of self-others risks q"

Transcription

1 Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Event frequency and comparative optimism: Another look at the indirect elicitation method of self-others risks q Yechiel Klar * and Shahar Ayal Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University, Ramat Aviv, 69978, Israel Received 10 April 2003; revised 4 April 2004 Available online 9 June 2004 Abstract Price, Pentecost, and Voth (2002) argue that common rather than rare negative future life events elicit greater indirect self-others comparative optimism. We argue that this finding may reflect a measurement effect: Rare events generate small differences (but large ratios), whereas common events produce larger differences (but smaller ratios). In Study 1, we show that the greater is event s perceived frequency, the greater is the numerical value assigned to express a given degree of risk discrepancy. In Study 2, we found weak negative rather than positive relations between perceived event frequency and the sense of risk discrepancy. Thus, it is crucial to understand how participants relate the two risk probabilities to each other to estimate their indirect comparative optimism. Ó 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Introduction Imagine two judges, A and B, who assess their own risk and the risk of the average peer to experience a particular negative event. Judge A is assigned to a common negative life event, whose rate of incidence in the general population is 80%. This judge concludes that the risk probability of the average peer to experience this negative event is 80% and that her own corresponding risk is 70%. Judge B is assigned to a rare negative life event, whose rate of incidence in the general population is 2%. This judge assesses the risk of the average peer as 2% and her own risk as 1%. Which judge, A or B, exhibits greater comparative optimism? Self-others comparative optimism, in which people systematically judge their own vulnerability to negative future life events as lower than that of their peers, is one of the most robust findings in social psychology (for reviews, Armor & Taylor, 1998; Helweg-Larsen & Shepperd, 2001; Klein & Weinstein, 1997). Two different methodologies are generally employed to study selfothers comparative optimism. In the direct elicitation q Research on this paper was supported by Grant 883/01 from the Israel Science Foundation to the first author. We are grateful to Paul Price, Heather Pentecost, and an anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments on a previous version of this paper. We also thank Chen Magal and Maher Mashour for their help in collecting the data. * Corresponding author. address: yklar@freud.tau.ac.il (Y. Klar). method, participants rate their own risk relative to that of the average peer on a single item (e.g., compared to the average classmate of your age and gender what are your chances of experiencing divorce in your lifetime ). In the indirect elicitation method (as is our two judge example), participants are asked to make two separate absolute risk estimates: one regarding the self and one regarding the average peer (e.g., my/the average member of my group s/probability of experiencing divorce during my/his/her lifetime is %) and the researcher later computes a score which is considered to reflect the participant s perceived discrepancy between personal risk and the risk of the average peer. Although these two methods have traditionally been regarded as basically interchangeable (e.g., Armor & Taylor, 1998; Klein & Weinstein, 1997), several authors have recently argued that they may mirror different judgmental processes (Giladi & Klar, 2002; Helweg-Larsen & Shepperd, 2001; Klar, 2002; Klar & Giladi, 1997, 2001; Otten & Van der Plicht, 1996; Price, Pentecost & Voth, 2002). In the most recent reassessment, Price et al. (2002) proposed a comparative optimism reversal, which states that people, when approached with the comparative method, exhibit a greater sense of self-others comparative optimism regarding rare events (e.g., committing suicide) as compared to common events (e.g., making a bad career choice), but when approached with the indirect /$ - see front matter Ó 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi: /j.jesp

2 806 Y. Klar, S. Ayal / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) comparative method, they demonstrate a greater sense of comparative optimism regarding common (e.g., making a bad career choice) than rare (e.g., committing suicide) events. The fact that the direct method elicits greater comparative self-others optimism for rare negative life events than more common negative life events is consistent with the bulk of the comparative optimism literature. This finding was first reported by Weinstein (1980) and has since been repeatedly replicated (e.g., Chambers, Windschitl, & Suls, 2003; Klar, Medding, & Sarel, 1996; Weinstein, 1987). Several authors (e.g., Diener & Fujita, 1997; Eiser, Pahl, & Prins, 2001; Epley & Dunning, 2000; Klar & Giladi, 1999; Kruger, 1999) have argued that in making comparative risk judgments with the direct method, participants rely primarily on their own perceived risk rather than on the corresponding others risk. As a result, rare negative life events, in which the perceived risk to both self and others is small, generate a greater sense of comparative optimism than common life events, in which both the risk to the self and to others is high. Kruger and Burrus (2004) have even showed most recently (as predicted by Klar et al., 1996) that when the negative life events are highly common, a comparative pessimism bias to the disfavor of the self emerges in the direct comparative method. However, the Price et al. paper s second contention that common rather than rare negative life events provoke greater self-others comparative optimism with the indirect method is a novel proposal, and one which raises a number of problems. First, it is inconsistent with the existing (admittedly, scant) literature on this issue. For example, Klar et al. (1996) found indirect self-others optimism for relatively rare life events (e.g., breaking a bone; injury in a car accident) but no indirect (or direct) self-others optimism whatsoever, for highly common life events (e.g., buying a faulty product; receiving poor service in restaurant). Chambers et al. (2003), using a large set of common (e.g., the store closed just when you arrived ) and rare (e.g., receiving a dog bite ) undesirable life events failed to find any positive event frequency/indirect comparative optimism correlation. Second, and most important in the context of the current paper, this proposal raises several methodological and substantial issues that relate to the very nature of the indirect method. The difference in epistemic status of the direct and indirect comparative methods Despite the fact that both the direct and indirect methods are intended to capture participants sense of self-others comparative risk, their epistemic status is strikingly different. The direct method is a straightforward self-report measure in which participants themselves directly relay to the researchers their sense of the self-others risk discrepancy. 1 In the indirect method, on the other hand, the participants do not present the researchers with their perceptions of the self-others risk discrepancy but rather provide them with the raw data to assess this discrepancy. In other words, in the indirect method, participants assess separately: (1) their own absolute risk and (2) the absolute risk of the average peer. In fact, it is not even necessary for the same participants to make the own and others risk judgments: although many indirect method studies have used a within-participants design, in which the same participants provide the two risk probabilities (e.g., Perloff & Fetzer, 1986), other studies (e.g., Harris and Middleton, 1994; Heine & Lehman, 1995; Voth and Price, 1999 cited in Price et al., 2002) have employed a betweenparticipants design, in which one group of participants provides the own risk judgment and the other group provides the others risk judgment. In both cases the researcher rather than the participants later calculates a discrepancy score from these two separate risk judgments. This may be done in more than one way. Difference or ratio scores? To estimate the magnitude of the discrepancy between two risk probabilities one may use a difference or a ratio framework. This choice determines the direction (e.g., sign) of the correlation between event frequency and comparative optimism. To demonstrate this point, the reader is invited to revisit the Judge A and Judge B example at the beginning of this paper. Clearly, which of the two judges exhibits greater comparative optimism depends on how the comparative score is analyzed. In a difference framework, Judge A, who perceives herself as 10% safer than the others in the group, is the more optimistic of the two (compared to Judge B, who perceives herself to be only 1% less at risk than the others in the group). However, in a ratio framework, Judge B, who perceives the others to be twice as much at risk than herself, is the more optimistic of the two (compared to Judge A, who perceives the others to be only 1.14 times more at risk than herself). Obviously, common (high probability) events are most likely to create greater difference scores and smaller ratios, whereas rare (low probability) events are most likely to create smaller difference scores and larger ratios. Consequently, a difference framework is likely to lead to a positive correlation between event frequency and the score for indirect comparative optimism, whereas a ratio framework for the same behavioral responses is likely to 1 This is not to claim that the direct method truly reflects the difference between own and others perceived risk. As just stated the direct measure relies mostly on own rather than others perceived risk. However, this imbalance can be regarded as a bona fide statement regarding participants sense of direct self-others risk discrepancy.

3 Y. Klar, S. Ayal / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) lead to a negative correlation between event frequency and the score for indirect comparative optimism. To illustrate this point further, we analyzed the same dataset (reported in Price et al. Study 2) as either difference (other s risk minus self s risk) or ratio (other s risks divided by self s risk) scores. As can be seen in Figs. 1A and 1B, the same self-others discrepancy led to a positive correlation (r ¼ :39) between event commonness and comparative optimism when computed as difference scores, and to a negative correlation (r ¼ :35) when computed as a ratio scores. The comparative optimism literature is generally silent on whether self-others risk discrepancies are better represented as a difference or a ratio. In fact, the classical Weinstein (1980) study using the direct method adopted a ratio approach in which participants were asked to estimate their comparative risk vis-a-vis the average peer with response categories such as 80% less, 20% less, average, 100% more, 3 times average, and 5 times average (see also Price et al. Study 2 for a similar ratio scale in the direct method condition). However, most direct method studies have used ambiguous terms such as much below average to much above average, which can be construed in either ratio or difference terms. In the indirect method literature, the two separate risk probabilities are commonly analyzed as difference scores. Comparative optimism with rare and common events Clearly, the key psychological issue is how participants themselves perceive the discrepancy between the risk to self and the average peer regarding rare and common life events. As just mentioned, this issue has rarely been touched upon in the comparative optimism literature, but several insights from the perception and decision-making literatures are worth consideration. These involve possible size and unit effects. The size effect A classic psychophysical finding reflected in Weber and Fechner s laws is that the perceived magnitudes of differences change as a function of the absolute size of the stimuli creating this difference. To cite a famous illustration, the transition from the perceived light of one candle to the perceived light of two candles (i.e., small size stimuli) is much greater than the transition from 100 to 101 candles (i.e., stimuli of greater size). This perceptual truth has been noted in the decisionmaking literature, which has acknowledged the nonlinearity of probability (see Lopes, 1983, 1996 for a review). A well known example in this area is the decision weights concept in Kahneman and Tversky (1979) prospect theory which states that decision weights tend to overweight small probabilities and underweight moderate and high probabilities (the latter effect is predicted to be greater than the former). Thus, it is highly probable that the same numerical difference between own and others risk would be experienced differently in the context of rare and common life events. For example, 1.5% less at risk than the others might be perceived as a highly significant discrepancy in the context of a rare (e.g., 2%) life event, but rather insignificant in the context of a highly common (e.g., 62%) life event. Similarly, participants may assign a greater difference score to common rather than rare events to express the same felt magnitude of comparative optimism. 2 Fig. 1. (A) Relationship between mean judged frequency and mean difference scores for 20 negative life events as computed from the Price et al. (Study 2, 2002) means. (B) Relationship between mean judged frequency and mean ratio scores for 20 negative life events as computed from the Price et al. (Study 2, 2002) means. 2 The size effect is not the only problem that complicates the interpretation of the difference scores as a measure of comparative optimism. A related problem is the scale latitude effect: With a 2% life event the most optimistic participants can judge their risk as only 2% less than the others but with a 62% event the same participants can judge their risk up to 62% less at than the others. This has to do with the fact that the probabilistic scale is bounded by the absolute zero at its lower end. This allows for only small differences in the context of low probability events but much larger differences in the context of high probability events.

4 808 Y. Klar, S. Ayal / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) To estimate the possible size effect, participants can be asked to provide a numerical analogue to the verbal statement X is at lower risk than the others in X s group in the context of various life events which differ in their objective frequency (i.e., size). Such a method may help to determine whether the psychological meaning of a given numerical discrepancy (e.g., being 1% less at risk) is variable or invariable across life events of different frequencies. The unit effect The numerical analogues assigned to a particular verbal statement ( X is at lower risk than the others in the group ) may further depend on the level of precision of the numerical units employed. To appreciate this problem, imagine one judge being told that others probability for a particular negative life event is 82% and a second judge being told that the same probability is 80%. Both judges are then asked to provide a numerical estimate for the statement X is at lower risk than the others in X s group. As judgmental graininess research (e.g., Yaniv & Foster, 1997) suggests, probabilistic estimates presented in more refined measurement units (e.g., 82 % rather than 80%) elicit smaller discrepancy estimates. That is, in the 82% case, participants may use single digit numbers to express the downward deviation (e.g., X risk s is 74%), whereas in the 80% case, they may use a two digit number to express the same deviation (e.g., X risk s is 70%). Several indirect method studies (e.g., Price et al., 2002, Study 2; Rothman, Klein, & Weinstein, 1996) have used response scales with varying intervals for different frequency events. Such scales may encourage participants to use more refined units for rare events and less refined units for more common events. 3 It is possible that the unit effect may emerge in conjunction with the size effect. Study 1: What is lower risk in rare and common life events: A psychophysical approach 3 For example, the indirect method participants in the Price et al. Study 2 were presented with the following visual analogue: This was a horizontal line with probabilities from 0 to 1% labeled at intervals of 0.1%, probabilities of 1 10% labeled at intervals of 1%, and probabilities from 10 to 100% labeled at 10%. This was done to encourage participants to use small probabilities including probabilities less than 1% when they thought it appropriate (p. 248). To estimate size and unit effects, we conducted a study in which participants were asked to estimate the numerical equivalent of lower risk than the others in the group for different hypothetical diseases which vary only in their objective frequency which ranges in the general population from 2 to 92% in the size only condition, and from 2 to 90% in the size with rounded units condition. Method Participants The participants were 34 students enrolled in the Israeli Open University (17 female, 17 male) who volunteered to participate in this study (mean age ¼ 29, SD ¼ 7:7). They were randomly assigned to the two experimental conditions. Design and procedure A mixed design was used. In the size only condition, participants were presented with a series of eight hypothetical scenarios of the following type: According to a survey conducted by the Israeli Health Ministry the probability that an adult will contract disease CC in the next 2 years is 72%. Dan is aware of this statistic but he believes his own probability of having the CC disease is lower than the reported probability (i.e., lower than 72%). In your opinion, what odds would Dan assign to himself? (0 100%). All participants in this condition responded to eight versions of the task, which differed only in the reported objective risk (2, 12, 22, 32, 52, 72, 82, and 92%). Order of presentation was counterbalanced and different protagonists first names were used in different versions. The disease was only identified by two letters (e.g., RR, CC, JJ, YY, EE, and MM) to avoid the possible influence of world knowledge on the true frequency of diseases. In the size with rounded units condition, the tasks and procedure were identical, with the exception that all probabilities higher than 10% were rounded off to the nearest 10. The probabilities were 2, 10, 20, 30, 50, 70, 80, and 90%. Results and discussion As can be seen in Fig. 2A, the correlations between the mean difference scores and objective event frequencies were nearly perfect in both the size only (r ¼ :97) and the size with rounded units (r ¼ :98) conditions; the joint correlation for all 16 event frequencies in Fig. 2A is.97. That is, the numerical equivalences of lower risk than the others were almost perfectly predictable by the frequency of the judged event, with a rare event (i.e., 2%), being 1.3% less at risk than the others (in the size only condition) and 1.5% less at risk (in the size with rounded units condition). However the numerical values expressing lower risk increased steadily with event frequency. In the size only condition they were 5.2, 8.4, 12.2, 17.8, 18.7, 24.4, and 22.3%, respectively, and in the size with rounded units condition, they were 4.4, 8.9, 12.3, 16.3, 19.2, 26.8, and 24.9%. We also computed individual correlations between difference scores and

5 Y. Klar, S. Ayal / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) the joint correlation for all 16 events was ).68. Averaging these correlations for each participant yielded somewhat lower negative correlations in the size only (r ¼ :50; SD ¼ :27) and size with rounded units condition (r ¼ :49; SD ¼ :26). A paired samples t test revealed that the ratio scores were less impacted by the size effect than the difference scores (t ¼ 3:1, df ¼ 33, p <:005). To test whether the rounded units manipulation had an effect over and above the size effect, we conducted a series of independent sample t tests for each of the pairing probabilities (e.g., 70 vs. 72%) in the two conditions but failed to find any added unit effect. However, given the robustness of the size effect, the main withinparticipants variable in this study, it is possible that there was little room for the unit influence to have an effect over and above the size effect. Further research on this issue is needed to conclude whether unit graininess does or does not have an effect on judgments of selfothers risk discrepancies. To conclude, the robust size effect found in Study 1 strongly suggests that people are aware of the fact that the same magnitude of risk discrepancy should be expressed with greater numerical values, the more common the life event is. These results do not support the Price et al. (2002) claim that with the indirect method people experience a greater sense of comparative selfothers optimism regarding common as compared to rare negative life events. Thus, the raw difference score may indeed increase with event frequency but not the sense of risk discrepancy. Study 2: A meta-cognitive measure for further assessing the sense of self-others risk discrepancy Fig. 2. (A) Relationship between objective event frequencies and mean difference scores for 16 fictional diseases in the two experimental conditions in Study 1. (B) Relationship between objective event frequencies and mean ratio scores for sixteen fictional diseases in the two experimental conditions in Study 1. event frequency for each participant (because correlations involving averaged responses are likely to be inflated). The average of these individual correlations was.67 (SD ¼ :35) in the size only condition and.78 (SD ¼ :17) in the size with rounded unit condition. 4 In addition to the difference scores, we also computed ratio scores. The correlations between the mean ratio and event frequency across all participants (presented in Fig. 2B) were negative both in the size only (r ¼ :67) and in the size with rounded units condition (r ¼ :72); 4 We replicated the size effect with a sample of 33 high school teachers. The correlation between the mean difference scores and objective event frequencies was again nearly perfect (r ¼ :996). There are, however, several notable differences between Study 1 and the typical studies involving the indirect method. In addition, there are several limitations to the psychophysical measure employed in this study as discussed below. First, in typical indirect method studies, participants generate both risk probabilities (i.e., own and others) whereas in Study 1, the others probability was provided by the researchers, and participants were only asked to generate a corresponding numerical value for the protagonist s perceived risk. Second, in typical indirect method studies participants can choose any degree of self-others comparative optimism or pessimism, whereas in Study 1, they were explicitly instructed to generate a risk probability that represented lower risk than the others. Third, in typical indirect method studies, participants are required to provide risk estimates regarding concrete life events (e.g., heart attack) for which they most likely have rich real-life information and attitudes, whereas in Study 1 they were asked to generate risk probabilities

6 810 Y. Klar, S. Ayal / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) for hypothetical diseases, with no information about the nature of these diseases, their severity, outbreak conditions, vulnerability, and consequences. Fourth, as was already alluded to, the results of Study 1 could be affected by a scale latitude effect in addition to the size effect. In other words, when others risk is small (e.g., 2%) the maximum possible downward deviation could not be larger than 2% lower than the others. However, when others risk is large (e.g., 60%), the maximum optimism could range to 60% lower than the others. Thus, expression of optimism for small probability events was constrained by the absolute zero at the lower end of the probability scale, and consequently, the scale latitude effect could have contributed to the Study 1 results. Fifth, although the Study 1 results seriously challenge the notion of a true positive correlation between event frequency and the sense of self-others indirect optimism, these results fall considerably short of portraying the relations between perceived frequency of events and the sense of indirect comparative optimism. Sixth, although Study 1 demonstrated that the ratio score is somewhat less confounded with size effect relative to the difference score, it still unclear how these two measures fare with each other in accounting for selfothers risk discrepancies for concrete, real life negative events. Furthermore, the relative efficiency of the ratio and difference scores in capturing the sense of self-others risk frequency might vary across life events which differ in their absolute frequency. It is possible that the ratio score better captures people s sense of self-others discrepancy than the difference score in small frequency events, but the superiority of the ratio may diminish or even reverse in high frequency events. For example, when thinking about a rare event our Judge B could possibly conceive her own risk (1 vs. 2%) as half the risk of the others, whereas our Judge A, thinking about a common event, might be more inclined to judge her risk (70 vs. 80%) as 10% less than the risk of the others. The meta-cognitive measure To address all these issues and further explore the Study 1 results with a converging operation, Study 2 uses a novel meta-cognitive measure for assessing the sense of self-others risk discrepancy (or pessimism), regarding own and others risk probabilities, constructed by the participants themselves. The first two steps required by the meta-cognitive measure are, in fact, identical to the standard, indirect method procedure (in within-participants studies). That is, for each life event, participants are first asked to provide two risk probabilities, own and others. In the third step, however, participants are immediately asked to review their two separate risk probabilities, and estimate how far apart from each other these risk probabilities are. For example, if they estimated others risk as 2% and own risk as 1%, they were asked to state how much lower the 1% probability is from the 2% probability. Similarly, if they estimated others risk as 60% and own risk as 50%, they were asked to estimate how much lower the 50% probability is from the 60%. The meta-cognitive approach thus addresses all the six aforementioned limitations of Study 1. In the meta-cognitive approach, participants are required (1) to generate two separate risk probabilities ( own and others ) rather than one, (2) in which they can report any degree of comparative optimism/pessimism, and (3) this is done in the context of concrete negative life events. In fact, Study 2 tests the same 20 life events used by Price et al. (2002). In addition, (4) the meta-cognitive measure is unbiased by the scale latitude constraints, because it is unbounded by a fixed 0% probability at its lower end, and does not have a fixed 100% probability at its higher end. Thus, it enables the participants to report how large or small the discrepancy between the two risk probabilities is felt to be regardless of the absolute size of the event. Furthermore, (5) the meta-cognitive measure can be used to estimate the relations between the perceived frequency of events (as indicated by others perceived risk) and the sense of selfothers discrepancies (as indicated by the meta-cognitive measure). Finally, (6) estimates of the correlations between both ratio and difference scores with the sense of self-others risk discrepancy and these correlations can be calculated for each one of the 20 events, (which differ from each other in perceived frequency). Assessing the relative efficiency of the difference and ratio frameworks within each of the events can test the notion that the advantage of the ratio over the difference is greater in low rather than high frequency life events. Method Participants Sixty nine (41 males; 28 females) paid participants, all students of the Israeli Open University (mean age ¼ 28.4; SD ¼ 7:5) took part in the study. Procedure Participants were presented with the 20 negative life events from the Price et al. (2002) study and they were asked to assess others risk, self risk, and immediately afterwards they were asked to evaluate the magnitude of risk discrepancy between these two risk judgments. Others risk. What, in your view, is the probability that the average person in the Israeli population (above 18) would experience the event (e.g., limb amputated) in the next 2 years on a scale from 0 to 100%? Participants were asked to write a percentage from 0 to 100%. Owns risk. What, in your view, is the probability that you would experience the event (e.g., limb amputated) in the next 2 years on a scale from 0 to 100%?

7 Y. Klar, S. Ayal / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) Once again, the participants were asked to write a percentage from 0 to 100%. The sense of self-others risk discrepancy (SORD; the meta-cognitive measure). Immediately after completing the two separate risk judgments, participants were instructed as follows Please look at the two risk probabilities you have just made (1) the risk probability regarding the average person in the population (above 18) % and (2) the risk probability regarding yourself %. Relative to the risk probability you assigned to the average person in the entire population how did you judge your own risk probability? Participants made the SORD judgment on 11-point scale ranging from )5 (I judged my risk probability as much lower than that of the average person in the population) through 0 (I judged my risk probability as equal to that of the average person) to +5 (I judged my risk probability as much higher than that of the average person). Results and discussion The data collected in Study 2 were analyzed in a number of complementary ways. First, to obtain a preliminary picture, we examined how the event means were related to the other variables. Second, because each participant completed all experimental conditions (in a full within-participant design), we created a matrix of correlations for each participant between the main variables (e.g., others risk, own risk, and the sense of discrepancy) across life events. With these 69 (our N) independent replications, we could test for the average relations between the variables. Finally, we looked at the relations between the variables within each of the 20 life events. The events as unit of analysis Table 1 presents the means of self and others perceived risk probabilities for each of the 20 life events. In addition, this table presents the means of the difference and ratio scores, computed for the 20 life events, and the means of the sense of self-others risk discrepancies (SORD) measure for the same life events, as generated by the participants themselves. As can be seen from the table, significant indirect biases were found with the difference, ratio, and SORD measure on 19 out of the 20 life events. The exception was experiencing gum disease that generated no comparative optimism. Of major interest in the current study was whether the higher the frequency of events (as indicated by perceived others risk) was accompanied by a greater sense of selfothers risk discrepancy. The present analysis provides a preliminary assessment of these relations. We correlated the mean others risk judgments with the SORD means Table 1 Means of perceived risk for self and others, difference and ratio scores and the sense of self-others risk discrepancy (SORD) for each of the 20 life events examined in Study 2 1. Perceived others risk 2. Perceived self risk 3. Diff. (others-self) 4. Ratio (others/self) 5. SORD (sense of risk discrepancy) 6. Others- SORD correlation 7. Self-SORD correlation Limb amputated ).03 ).39 Suicide ).02 ).54 House fire ).35 AIDS ).21 ).41 Gum disease ).07 ).50 Hearing loss ).01 ).50 Death by car crash ).18 Sued ).13 ).57 Cancer ).36 Alcoholism ).72 Heart attack ).09 ).65 Car stolen ).49 Death before ).43 years of age Drops out of school ).20 ).718 college Depression ).17 ).60 Burglarized ).01 ).36 Fired from job ).23 ).70 Bad career choice ).01 ).69 Broken bone ).05 ).42 Divorce ).11 ).63 * Correlation is significant at the.05 level (two tailed). ** Correlation is significant at the.01 level (two tailed). *** Correlation is significant at the.001 level (two tailed).

8 812 Y. Klar, S. Ayal / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) for the various life events. As can be seen in Fig. 3A, inconsistent with the Price et al. prediction, the direction of the correlation was negative rather than positive (r ¼ :33; p ¼ :16). In addition, the corresponding correlation with self risk judgments (portrayed in Fig. 3B) was highly negative (r ¼ :78; p <:001). A related question is whether a difference or ratio framework better captures the sense of self-others risk discrepancy. According to the rudimentary calculation that is feasible at this level of analysis, the better fit was with the ratio (r ¼ :90; p <:001) rather than the difference (r ¼ :72; p <:001) score. The participants as the unit of analysis To obtain a more refined level of analysis, we constructed a correlations matrix between the experimental events for each of the participants. The findings replicate the results obtained for the cross-event measure. We found a weak but statistically significant negative correlation (average r ¼ :12; p <:005) between the sense of self-others risk discrepancy and others risk. That is, Fig. 3. (A) Relationship between the means of perceived others risk and the SORD (sense of self-others risk discrepancy) for the 20 negative life events in Study 2. (B) Relationship between the means of perceived self risk and SORD (sense of self-others risk discrepancy) for the 20 negative events in Study 2. the SORD was somewhat lower (rather than higher) when others risk was perceived as high. The correlation of the SORD with own risk was high and negative (average r ¼ :64; p <:001). That is, the lower the self perceived risk, the higher the SORD. We also tested the cross-event correlations between the SORD and the ratio and difference scores. The average correlation was (r ¼ :88; SD ¼ :14) with the ratio and (r ¼ :68; SD ¼ :21) with the difference score. Here too, the ratio rather than the difference score showed better fit with the SORD (t ¼ 7:5; df ¼ 68; p <:001). The participants within events as the unit of analysis Finally, we calculated within-event correlations where the participants served as the unit of analysis (see also Price et al., 2002). These correlations are presented in Table 1, column 6 presents the SORD/others risk correlations for each of the events. As can be seen, 14 of the 20 correlations were negatively rather than positively signed; the average correlation with the others risk (Average r ¼ :05, SD ¼ :1) was significant (t ¼ 2:19, df ¼ 19, p <:05). These within-events results are consistent with the between-events and between-participants results. In addition, and also consistent with previous results, all 20 SORD/self risk correlations were negative and 19 out of the 20 were significantly so. The average within-event correlation with the self risk (average r ¼ :51, SD ¼ :15) was significant (t ¼ 15:33, df ¼ 19, p <:001). Difference or ratio? We analyzed the correlations between the ratio and difference measures and the SORD for each of the 20 life events. As in the previous analyses, the average correlation with the ratio (average r ¼ :81) was significantly higher than the corresponding correlation with the difference (average r ¼ :61) (t ¼ 5:3, df ¼ 19, p <:001). To explore whether the relative advantage of the ratio over the difference framework diminishes in the move from rare to more common life events, we created a ratio over difference relative efficiency measure, in which we subtracted the absolute SORD/difference correlation coefficient from the corresponding absolute SORD/ratio coefficient for each of the life events. The higher the resultant value, the greater the relative advantage of the ratio. As can be seen in Fig. 4, there is a negative slope (b ¼ :44, p <:05), where the advantage of the ratio over the difference decreases with event frequency. It should be noted that this tendency was found despite the fact that our sample of events (taken from the Price et al. study) did not include either very rare life events (the frequency of hearing loss, our lowest frequency event, was about 14%) or very common events (the frequency of bad career choice, our highest frequency event, was about 52%). Yet, despite this restricted range, a tendency for diminishing ratio advantage emerged.

9 Y. Klar, S. Ayal / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) Fig. 4. The relative efficiency of the ratio over the difference score in predicting the SORD score in the 20 negative life events in Study 2 (the SORD/RATIO correlation the SORD/DIFFERENCE correlation). Note. The higher the number, the greater is the relative efficiency of the ratio relative to the difference score. All in all, the Study 2 results showed that the sense of self-others risk discrepancy (SORD) did not increase with the perceived frequency of events. If anything, it decreased mildly. In addition, these results show that the ratio framework is generally advantageous over the difference framework for capturing the SORD. However, there are also preliminary indications that the relative advantage of the ratio over the difference framework is greater for low rather than high frequency events. Conclusions The nature of the indirect measure of self-others optimism regarding future personal risks is that participants provide the researchers with two separate, own and others, risk probabilities. To derive participants sense of self-others risk discrepancy from these two risk probabilities, it is crucial to understand how they actually relate the two risk probabilities to each other. We approached this issue with psychophysical and metacognitive approaches. One question was whether the sense of self-others risk discrepancy is greater for common rather than for rare life events, as suggested by Price et al. (2002). Our results did not support this contention: the results of Study 1, employing the psychophysical approach, showed that participants expressed the same degree of risk discrepancy with greater numerical values, with greater absolute probability. Thus, a greater difference score in the context of common rather than rare events does not necessarily imply greater self-others optimism. The results of Study 2, employing a meta-cognitive approach, suggested that there are weak negative rather than positive relations between the perceived frequency of an event and the sense of perceived risk discrepancy. Thus, we failed to find any evidence for a positive frequency/optimism correlation in the indirect measure, or any evidence for comparative optimism reversal, in the move from the direct to the indirect method. Although it is likely that the two elicitation methods are not the same, there is no reason to assume that they lead to a risk reversal. We also tested whether a ratio or difference framework better fits the sense of self-others risk discrepancy. Overall, we found evidence in both studies for the relative superiority of the ratio over the difference framework. However, we also found some suggestive evidence for the possibility that this relative superiority of the ratio is moderated by event frequency. In other words, the ratio format may better capture self-others optimism in the context of rare events (e.g., others risk is double that of mine rather than others risk is 1% greater than mine ) but this advantage may wane for more common events. It is possible that with highly common life events, difference rather than ratio scores may better capture self-others optimism (e.g., others risk is 10% greater than mine rather than others risk is 1.14 greater than mine ). This, however, is a matter for further research. References Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1998). Situated optimism: Specific outcome expectations and self-regulation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 30, pp ). New York: Academic Press. Chambers, J. R., Windschitl, P. D., & Suls, J. (2003). Egocentrism, event frequency, and comparative optimism: When what happens frequently is more likely to happen to me. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1997). Social comparisons and subjective wellbeing. In P. B. Buunk & F. X. Gibbons (Eds.), Health, coping and well-being: Perspectives from social comparison theory (pp ). Mahwah, NJ: Earlbaum. Eiser, R. J., Pahl, S., & Prins, Y. (2001). Optimism, pessimism, and the direction of self-other comparisons. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling holier than thou : Are selfserving assessments produced by errors in self- or social prediction? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, Giladi, E. E., & Klar, Y. (2002). When standards are wide of the mark: Nonselective superiority and inferiority biases in comparative judgments of objects and concepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, Heine, S., & Lehman, D. (1995). Cultural variation in unrealistic optimism: Does the west feel more invulnerable than the east? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, Helweg-Larsen, M., & Shepperd, J. A. (2001). Do moderators of the optimistic bias affect personal or target risk estimates? A review of the literature. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5,

10 814 Y. Klar, S. Ayal / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, Klar, Y. (2002). Way beyond compare: The nonselective superiority and inferiority biases in judging randomly assigned group members relative to their peers. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, Klar, Y., & Giladi, E. E. (1997). No one in my group can be below the group s average: A robust positivity bias in favor of anonymous peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, Klar, Y., & Giladi, E. E. (1999). Are most people happier than their peers or are they just happy? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, Klar, Y., & Giladi, E.E. (2001) Controllability, probability and comparative unrealistic optimism: Bias1 and Bias2. In W.M. Klein (Chair), Understanding risk perception: Biases, antecedents and consequences. The VII European Congress on Psychology, London, England. Klar, Y., Medding, A., & Sarel, D. (1996). Nonunique invulnerability: Singular versus distributional probabilities and unrealistic optimism in comparative risk judgments. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67, Klein, W. M., & Weinstein, N. D. (1997). Social comparison and unrealistic optimism about personal risk. In B. P. Buunk & F. X. Gibbons (Eds.), Health, coping, and well-being: Perspectives from social comparison theory (pp ). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Kruger, J. (1999). Lake Wobegon be gone! The below-average effect and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, Kruger, J., & Burrus, J. (2004). Egocentrism and focalism in unrealistic optimism (and pessimism). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, Lopes, L. (1983). Some thoughts on the psychological concept of risk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, Lopes, L. (1996). When time is of the essence: Averaging, aspiration, and the short run. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, Otten, W., & Van der Plicht, J. (1996). Context effects in the measurement of comparative optimism in probability judgments. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15, Perloff, L. S., & Fetzer, B. K. (1986). Self-other judgments and perceived vulnerability to victimization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, Price, P. C., Pentecost, H. C., & Voth, R. D. (2002). Perceived event frequency and the optimistic bias: Evidence for a two-process model of personal risk judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, Rothman, A. J., Klein, W. M., & Weinstein, N. D. (1996). Absolute and relative biases in estimations of personal risks. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, Weinstein, N. D. (1987). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10, Yaniv, I., & Foster, D. P. (1997). Precision and accuracy of judgmental estimation. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making., 10,

Egocentrism, Event Frequency, and Comparative Optimism: When What Happens Frequently Is More Likely to Happen to Me

Egocentrism, Event Frequency, and Comparative Optimism: When What Happens Frequently Is More Likely to Happen to Me 10.1177/0146167203256870 PERSONALITY Chambers et al. AND / EGOCENTRISM SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY AND EVENT BULLETIN FREQUENCY ARTICLE Egocentrism, Event Frequency, and Comparative Optimism: When What Happens Frequently

More information

Egocentrism and Focalism in Unrealistic Optimism (and Pessimism) Justin Kruger & Jeremy Burrus. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign

Egocentrism and Focalism in Unrealistic Optimism (and Pessimism) Justin Kruger & Jeremy Burrus. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Egocentrism and Focalism in Unrealistic Optimism (and Pessimism) Justin Kruger & Jeremy Burrus University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Running head: Optimism and Pessimism Word count: 4,971 (including

More information

Comparative optimism in the spontaneous generation of future life-events

Comparative optimism in the spontaneous generation of future life-events 441 British Journal of Social Psychology (2008), 47, 441 451 q 2008 The British Psychological Society The British Psychological Society www.bpsjournals.co.uk Comparative optimism in the spontaneous generation

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

The Effect of Target Group Size on Risk Judgments and Comparative Optimism: The More, the Riskier

The Effect of Target Group Size on Risk Judgments and Comparative Optimism: The More, the Riskier Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 90, No. 3, 382 398 0022-3514/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.382 The Effect of Target

More information

Is the better than average effect better explained by cognitive or motivational accounts?

Is the better than average effect better explained by cognitive or motivational accounts? University of Oslo Fall 2012 PSY4301: Judgment and Decision-Making and Social Cognition Is the better than average effect better explained by cognitive or motivational accounts? Kand. Nr 1515 1 ABSTRACT

More information

SHORT NOTE Which studies test whether self-enhancement is pancultural? Reply to Sedikides, Gaertner, and Vevea, 2007

SHORT NOTE Which studies test whether self-enhancement is pancultural? Reply to Sedikides, Gaertner, and Vevea, 2007 Asian Journal of Social Psychology (2007), 10, 198 200 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2007.00226.x SHORT NOTE Which studies test whether self-enhancement is pancultural? Reply to Sedikides, Gaertner, and Vevea,

More information

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 108 (2009) 39 2 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/obhdp

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

Direct-Comparison Judgments: When and Why Above- and Below- Average Effects Reverse

Direct-Comparison Judgments: When and Why Above- and Below- Average Effects Reverse Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 2008, Vol. 137, No. 1, 182 200 0096-3445/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.1.182 Direct-Comparison

More information

Effect of Choice Set on Valuation of Risky Prospects

Effect of Choice Set on Valuation of Risky Prospects Effect of Choice Set on Valuation of Risky Prospects Neil Stewart (neil.stewart@warwick.ac.uk) Nick Chater (nick.chater@warwick.ac.uk) Henry P. Stott (hstott@owc.com) Department of Psychology, University

More information

Explaining Bargaining Impasse: The Role of Self-Serving Biases

Explaining Bargaining Impasse: The Role of Self-Serving Biases Explaining Bargaining Impasse: The Role of Self-Serving Biases Linda Babcock and George Loewenstein Journal of Economic Perspectives, 1997 報告人 : 高培儒 20091028 1 1. Introduction Economists, and more specifically

More information

Psychological. Influences on Personal Probability. Chapter 17. Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc.

Psychological. Influences on Personal Probability. Chapter 17. Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc. Psychological Chapter 17 Influences on Personal Probability Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc. 17.2 Equivalent Probabilities, Different Decisions Certainty Effect: people

More information

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task Neath, I., & Brown, G. D. A. (2005). Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task. Memory Lab Technical Report 2005-01, Purdue University. Scale Invariance and Primacy

More information

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies Emma ter Mors 1,2, Mieneke Weenig 1, Naomi Ellemers 1, Dancker Daamen 1 1 Leiden University,

More information

Minimizing Uncertainty in Property Casualty Loss Reserve Estimates Chris G. Gross, ACAS, MAAA

Minimizing Uncertainty in Property Casualty Loss Reserve Estimates Chris G. Gross, ACAS, MAAA Minimizing Uncertainty in Property Casualty Loss Reserve Estimates Chris G. Gross, ACAS, MAAA The uncertain nature of property casualty loss reserves Property Casualty loss reserves are inherently uncertain.

More information

Self-Enhancement for Driving Ability 1. I Am a Better Driver Than You Think: Examining Self-Enhancement for Driving Ability

Self-Enhancement for Driving Ability 1. I Am a Better Driver Than You Think: Examining Self-Enhancement for Driving Ability Self-Enhancement for Driving Ability 1 I Am a Better Driver Than You Think: Examining Self-Enhancement for Driving Ability Michael M. Roy 1 & Michael J. Liersch 2 1 Elizabethtown College 2 Leonard N. Stern

More information

Evaluating framing e ects

Evaluating framing e ects Journal of Economic Psychology 22 2001) 91±101 www.elsevier.com/locate/joep Evaluating framing e ects James N. Druckman * Department of Political Science, University of Minnesota, 1414 Social Sciences

More information

Risk attitude in decision making: A clash of three approaches

Risk attitude in decision making: A clash of three approaches Risk attitude in decision making: A clash of three approaches Eldad Yechiam (yeldad@tx.technion.ac.il) Faculty of Industrial Engineering and Management, Technion Israel Institute of Technology Haifa, 32000

More information

Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary. NLPcourses.com

Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary. NLPcourses.com Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary NLPcourses.com Table of Contents Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE by Richard Bandler Summary... 6 Chapter 1 Who s Driving the Bus?... 6 Chapter 2 Running Your Own

More information

SAMENESS AND REDUNDANCY IN TARGET DETECTION AND TARGET COMPARISON. Boaz M. Ben-David and Daniel Algom Tel-Aviv University

SAMENESS AND REDUNDANCY IN TARGET DETECTION AND TARGET COMPARISON. Boaz M. Ben-David and Daniel Algom Tel-Aviv University SAMENESS AND REDUNDANCY IN TARGET DETECTION AND TARGET COMPARISON Boaz M. Ben-David and Daniel Algom Tel-Aviv University boazb@post.tau.ac.il Abstract Searching for targets, people perform better and reap

More information

Out of Sight, Out of Mind: On the Irrational Side of Egocentrism in Social Comparisons. Steven Chan. New York University. John R.

Out of Sight, Out of Mind: On the Irrational Side of Egocentrism in Social Comparisons. Steven Chan. New York University. John R. Egocentric Social Comparisons 1 Running head: THE IRRATIONAL SIDE OF EGOCENTRISM Out of Sight, Out of Mind: On the Irrational Side of Egocentrism in Social Comparisons Steven Chan New York University John

More information

Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1. Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him. Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2

Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1. Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him. Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2 Running Head: BETTER THAN THEM, BUT NOT HIM 1 Why I Think I m Better Than Them, but Not Him Clayton R. Critcher 1 and David Dunning 2 1 University of California, Berkeley 2 Cornell University Author Note

More information

Evaluating the Causal Role of Unobserved Variables

Evaluating the Causal Role of Unobserved Variables Evaluating the Causal Role of Unobserved Variables Christian C. Luhmann (christian.luhmann@vanderbilt.edu) Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University 301 Wilson Hall, Nashville, TN 37203 USA Woo-kyoung

More information

FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER

FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER 1 ST SEMESTER 2017 ASSIGNMENT 2 ORGANISATIONAL BEHAVIOUR OSB611S 1 Page1 OSB611S - FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER FOR ASSIGNMENT 2-2016 Dear student The purpose of this tutorial letter

More information

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Uri Hasson (uhasson@princeton.edu) Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 USA Geoffrey P. Goodwin (ggoodwin@princeton.edu)

More information

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1 Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen gert.haubensak@t-online.de Abstract Two experiments are reported in which the participants judged

More information

Sawtooth Software. The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? RESEARCH PAPER SERIES

Sawtooth Software. The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? RESEARCH PAPER SERIES Sawtooth Software RESEARCH PAPER SERIES The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? Dick Wittink, Yale University Joel Huber, Duke University Peter Zandan,

More information

Exploring the reference point in prospect theory

Exploring the reference point in prospect theory 3 Exploring the reference point in prospect theory Gambles for length of life Exploring the reference point in prospect theory: Gambles for length of life. S.M.C. van Osch, W.B. van den Hout, A.M. Stiggelbout

More information

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 BLL R. BROWN AND THOMAS J. REBBN2 UNlVERSTY OF LOUSVLLE This experiment required Ss to make Markov-generated histoforms that were

More information

Measuring and Assessing Study Quality

Measuring and Assessing Study Quality Measuring and Assessing Study Quality Jeff Valentine, PhD Co-Chair, Campbell Collaboration Training Group & Associate Professor, College of Education and Human Development, University of Louisville Why

More information

MCAS Equating Research Report: An Investigation of FCIP-1, FCIP-2, and Stocking and. Lord Equating Methods 1,2

MCAS Equating Research Report: An Investigation of FCIP-1, FCIP-2, and Stocking and. Lord Equating Methods 1,2 MCAS Equating Research Report: An Investigation of FCIP-1, FCIP-2, and Stocking and Lord Equating Methods 1,2 Lisa A. Keller, Ronald K. Hambleton, Pauline Parker, Jenna Copella University of Massachusetts

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2014, 42(7), 1105-1116 Society for Personality Research http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2014.42.7.1105 ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN

More information

Shiken: JALT Testing & Evaluation SIG Newsletter. 12 (2). April 2008 (p )

Shiken: JALT Testing & Evaluation SIG Newsletter. 12 (2). April 2008 (p ) Rasch Measurementt iin Language Educattiion Partt 2:: Measurementt Scalles and Invariiance by James Sick, Ed.D. (J. F. Oberlin University, Tokyo) Part 1 of this series presented an overview of Rasch measurement

More information

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Ethan D. Montag Rochester Institute of Technology Munsell Color Science Laboratory Chester F. Carlson Center for Imaging Science

More information

Psychological Factors Influencing People s Reactions to Risk Information. Katherine A. McComas, Ph.D. University of Maryland

Psychological Factors Influencing People s Reactions to Risk Information. Katherine A. McComas, Ph.D. University of Maryland Psychological Factors Influencing People s Reactions to Risk Information Katherine A. McComas, Ph.D. University of Maryland What This Tutorial Covers Reasons for understanding people s risk perceptions

More information

Ambiguous Data Result in Ambiguous Conclusions: A Reply to Charles T. Tart

Ambiguous Data Result in Ambiguous Conclusions: A Reply to Charles T. Tart Other Methodology Articles Ambiguous Data Result in Ambiguous Conclusions: A Reply to Charles T. Tart J. E. KENNEDY 1 (Original publication and copyright: Journal of the American Society for Psychical

More information

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 118 128 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/obhdp

More information

Administering and Scoring the CSQ Scales

Administering and Scoring the CSQ Scales CSQ Scales 2012 All Rights Reserved Administering and Scoring the CSQ Scales Inquiries: Web: info@csqscales.com www.csqscales.com Copyright of the CSQ Scales Copyright:, Tamalpais Matrix Systems, 35 Miller

More information

On the diversity principle and local falsifiability

On the diversity principle and local falsifiability On the diversity principle and local falsifiability Uriel Feige October 22, 2012 1 Introduction This manuscript concerns the methodology of evaluating one particular aspect of TCS (theoretical computer

More information

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH

ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH ORIGINS AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGENETICS RESEARCH The following document provides background information on the research and development of the Emergenetics Profile instrument. Emergenetics Defined 1. Emergenetics

More information

Iowa Gambling Treatment Outcomes System: Year 4

Iowa Gambling Treatment Outcomes System: Year 4 Iowa Gambling Treatment Outcomes System: Year 4 Prepared by Gene M. Lutz, Melvin E. Gonnerman Jr., Rod Muilenburg, Anne Bonsall Hoekstra, Karen Dietzenbach, & Ki H. Park Center for Social and Behavioral

More information

Judging the Relatedness of Variables: The Psychophysics of Covariation Detection

Judging the Relatedness of Variables: The Psychophysics of Covariation Detection Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1985, Vol. II, No. 5. 640-649 Copyright 1985 by Ihe Am =an Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/85/$00.75 Judging the Relatedness

More information

Exploring the Causes of Comparative Optimism

Exploring the Causes of Comparative Optimism Psychologica Belgica, 42, pp. 65-98, 2002 Exploring the Causes of Comparative Optimism James A. Shepperd, Patrick Carroll, Jodi Grace and Meredith Terry University of Florida We review explanations offered

More information

How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations

How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations Motoyuki Saito (m-saito@kwansei.ac.jp) Department of Psychological Science, Kwansei Gakuin University Hyogo 662-8501, JAPAN Abstract

More information

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing This document contains 4 examples of writing and feedback comments from Level 2A lab reports, and 4 steps to help you apply

More information

Handout 16: Opinion Polls, Sampling, and Margin of Error

Handout 16: Opinion Polls, Sampling, and Margin of Error Opinion polls involve conducting a survey to gauge public opinion on a particular issue (or issues). In this handout, we will discuss some ideas that should be considered both when conducting a poll and

More information

Confidence Intervals On Subsets May Be Misleading

Confidence Intervals On Subsets May Be Misleading Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods Volume 3 Issue 2 Article 2 11-1-2004 Confidence Intervals On Subsets May Be Misleading Juliet Popper Shaffer University of California, Berkeley, shaffer@stat.berkeley.edu

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

Self-Serving Assessments of Fairness and Pretrial Bargaining

Self-Serving Assessments of Fairness and Pretrial Bargaining Self-Serving Assessments of Fairness and Pretrial Bargaining George Loewenstein Samuel Issacharoff Colin Camerer and Linda Babcock Journal of Legal Studies 1993 報告人 : 高培儒 20091028 1 1. Introduction Why

More information

Is a picture worth a thousand words? The interaction of visual display and attribute representation in attenuating framing bias

Is a picture worth a thousand words? The interaction of visual display and attribute representation in attenuating framing bias Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 8, No. 4, July 2013, pp. 482 491 Is a picture worth a thousand words? The interaction of visual display and attribute representation in attenuating framing bias Eyal

More information

Framing the frame: How task goals determine the likelihood and direction of framing effects

Framing the frame: How task goals determine the likelihood and direction of framing effects McElroy, T., Seta, J. J. (2007). Framing the frame: How task goals determine the likelihood and direction of framing effects. Judgment and Decision Making, 2(4): 251-256. (Aug 2007) Published by the Society

More information

The Influence of Framing Effects and Regret on Health Decision-Making

The Influence of Framing Effects and Regret on Health Decision-Making Colby College Digital Commons @ Colby Honors Theses Student Research 2012 The Influence of Framing Effects and Regret on Health Decision-Making Sarah Falkof Colby College Follow this and additional works

More information

MEA DISCUSSION PAPERS

MEA DISCUSSION PAPERS Inference Problems under a Special Form of Heteroskedasticity Helmut Farbmacher, Heinrich Kögel 03-2015 MEA DISCUSSION PAPERS mea Amalienstr. 33_D-80799 Munich_Phone+49 89 38602-355_Fax +49 89 38602-390_www.mea.mpisoc.mpg.de

More information

In this chapter we discuss validity issues for quantitative research and for qualitative research.

In this chapter we discuss validity issues for quantitative research and for qualitative research. Chapter 8 Validity of Research Results (Reminder: Don t forget to utilize the concept maps and study questions as you study this and the other chapters.) In this chapter we discuss validity issues for

More information

Understanding Uncertainty in School League Tables*

Understanding Uncertainty in School League Tables* FISCAL STUDIES, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 207 224 (2011) 0143-5671 Understanding Uncertainty in School League Tables* GEORGE LECKIE and HARVEY GOLDSTEIN Centre for Multilevel Modelling, University of Bristol

More information

Highlighting Effect: The Function of Rebuttals in Written Argument

Highlighting Effect: The Function of Rebuttals in Written Argument Highlighting Effect: The Function of Rebuttals in Written Argument Ryosuke Onoda (ndrysk62@p.u-tokyo.ac.jp) Department of Educational Psychology, Graduate School of Education, The University of Tokyo,

More information

Self enhancement and voting intentions: A proposal for questions to be included in the

Self enhancement and voting intentions: A proposal for questions to be included in the Self enhancement and voting intentions: A proposal for questions to be included in the ANES pilot study and in the following surveys Abstract: Self enhancement entails perceiving oneself more positively

More information

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective Academic Leadership Journal in Student Research Volume 4 Spring 2016 Article 4 2016 Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective Kevin L. Kenney Fort Hays State University Follow this

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

GENERALIZABILITY AND RELIABILITY: APPROACHES FOR THROUGH-COURSE ASSESSMENTS

GENERALIZABILITY AND RELIABILITY: APPROACHES FOR THROUGH-COURSE ASSESSMENTS GENERALIZABILITY AND RELIABILITY: APPROACHES FOR THROUGH-COURSE ASSESSMENTS Michael J. Kolen The University of Iowa March 2011 Commissioned by the Center for K 12 Assessment & Performance Management at

More information

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework Modified from Organizing Scientific Thinking Using the QuALMRI Framework Written by Kevin Ochsner and modified by others. Based on a scheme devised by

More information

Chapter 5: Field experimental designs in agriculture

Chapter 5: Field experimental designs in agriculture Chapter 5: Field experimental designs in agriculture Jose Crossa Biometrics and Statistics Unit Crop Research Informatics Lab (CRIL) CIMMYT. Int. Apdo. Postal 6-641, 06600 Mexico, DF, Mexico Introduction

More information

Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting

Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2012, 65 (11), 2231 2257 Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting Michael J. Serra and Benjamin D. England Department

More information

Hindsight Bias: A Cross-Cultural Analysis. STEVEN J. HEINE and DARRIN R. LEHMAN University of British Columbia, Canada

Hindsight Bias: A Cross-Cultural Analysis. STEVEN J. HEINE and DARRIN R. LEHMAN University of British Columbia, Canada The Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1996, Vol. 35, No.3, 317-323 \ General Article \ Hindsight Bias: A Cross-Cultural Analysis STEVEN J. HEINE and DARRIN R. LEHMAN University of British

More information

introduction to the CFS PROCESS

introduction to the CFS PROCESS F A C U LT Y C O N V E R S A T I O N S E R I E S B O O K S E V E N introduction to the CFS PROCESS The purpose of the CFS process is to orient and acclimatize new faculty at BYU-Idaho to give them access

More information

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 16 (2001) 183±191 Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples Grant L. Iverson* Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, 2255 Wesbrook

More information

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Hidehito Honda (hito@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Kimihiko Yamagishi (kimihiko@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Graduate School of Decision Science

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

Center for Advanced Studies in Measurement and Assessment. CASMA Research Report

Center for Advanced Studies in Measurement and Assessment. CASMA Research Report Center for Advanced Studies in Measurement and Assessment CASMA Research Report Number 39 Evaluation of Comparability of Scores and Passing Decisions for Different Item Pools of Computerized Adaptive Examinations

More information

Thank you very much for your guidance on this revision. We look forward to hearing from you.

Thank you very much for your guidance on this revision. We look forward to hearing from you. 1 PAINTED BUNTING MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) We would like to submit a revision of our manuscript. As you requested, we have run new studies that employ additional

More information

Comparing Direct and Indirect Measures of Just Rewards: What Have We Learned?

Comparing Direct and Indirect Measures of Just Rewards: What Have We Learned? Comparing Direct and Indirect Measures of Just Rewards: What Have We Learned? BARRY MARKOVSKY University of South Carolina KIMMO ERIKSSON Mälardalen University We appreciate the opportunity to comment

More information

o^ &&cvi AL Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 20, Southern Universities Press 1965

o^ &&cvi AL Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 20, Southern Universities Press 1965 Ml 3 Hi o^ &&cvi AL 44755 Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 20, 311-316. Southern Universities Press 1965 m CONFIDENCE RATINGS AND LEVEL OF PERFORMANCE ON A JUDGMENTAL TASK 1 RAYMOND S. NICKERSON AND

More information

Desirability Bias: Do Desires Influence Expectations? It Depends on How You Ask.

Desirability Bias: Do Desires Influence Expectations? It Depends on How You Ask. University of Iowa Honors Theses University of Iowa Honors Program Spring 2018 Desirability Bias: Do Desires Influence Expectations? It Depends on How You Ask. Mark Biangmano Follow this and additional

More information

Exploring a Counterintuitive Finding with Methodological Implications

Exploring a Counterintuitive Finding with Methodological Implications Exploring a Counterintuitive Finding with Methodological Implications Why is 9 > 221 in a Between-subjects Design? Stuart J. McKelvie Department of Psychology Bishop s University 2600 College Street, Sherbrooke,

More information

Numeracy, frequency, and Bayesian reasoning

Numeracy, frequency, and Bayesian reasoning Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 4, No. 1, February 2009, pp. 34 40 Numeracy, frequency, and Bayesian reasoning Gretchen B. Chapman Department of Psychology Rutgers University Jingjing Liu Department

More information

Subjective experience of depressed mood among medical students at the University of Pretoria

Subjective experience of depressed mood among medical students at the University of Pretoria Subjective experience of depressed mood among medical at the University of Pretoria L van Niekerk, Final-year medical student A J Viljoen, Final-year medical student P Rischbieter, Final-year medical student

More information

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE Åke Hellström Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, S-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden Email: hellst@psychology.su.se Abstract Participants compared successive

More information

ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR

ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR CHAPTER 6 ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR Several studies were done as part of the UTDFP that were based substantially on subjective data, reflecting travelers beliefs, attitudes, and intentions.

More information

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints Jean-Pierre Thibaut (jean-pierre.thibaut@univ-poitiers.fr) University of Poitiers, CeRCA, CNRS UMR 634, 99 avenue du recteur Pineau

More information

COMPUTING READER AGREEMENT FOR THE GRE

COMPUTING READER AGREEMENT FOR THE GRE RM-00-8 R E S E A R C H M E M O R A N D U M COMPUTING READER AGREEMENT FOR THE GRE WRITING ASSESSMENT Donald E. Powers Princeton, New Jersey 08541 October 2000 Computing Reader Agreement for the GRE Writing

More information

ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition. Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation

ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition. Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th edition Chapter 2: Principles of Adherence and Motivation 1 Learning Objectives Based on Chapter 2 of the ACE Personal Trainer Manual, 4 th ed., this session describes

More information

Running head: ARTICLE CRITIQUE 1

Running head: ARTICLE CRITIQUE 1 Running head: ARTICLE CRITIQUE 1 Article Critique Anonymous Student Anonymous University ARTICLE CRITIQUE 2 Introduction The effect of linguistic tendencies on behaviors is an interesting field for research;

More information

Wendy J. Reece (INEEL) Leroy J. Matthews (ISU) Linda K. Burggraff (ISU)

Wendy J. Reece (INEEL) Leroy J. Matthews (ISU) Linda K. Burggraff (ISU) INEEL/CON-98-01115 PREPRINT Estimating Production Potentials: Expert Bias in Applied Decision Making Wendy J. Reece (INEEL) Leroy J. Matthews (ISU) Linda K. Burggraff (ISU) October 28, 1998 October 30,

More information

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 1. THE HUMANISTIC FRAMEWORK FOR INTERVIEWER SKILLS

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 1. THE HUMANISTIC FRAMEWORK FOR INTERVIEWER SKILLS INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 1. THE HUMANISTIC FRAMEWORK FOR INTERVIEWER SKILLS 1.1. Foundation of the Humanistic Framework Research interviews have been portrayed in a variety of different ways,

More information

Automatic Optimism: The Affective Basis of Judgments About the Likelihood of Future Events

Automatic Optimism: The Affective Basis of Judgments About the Likelihood of Future Events Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 138, No. 2, 187 200 0096-3445/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015380 Automatic Optimism: The Affective Basis of Judgments

More information

Sawtooth Software. MaxDiff Analysis: Simple Counting, Individual-Level Logit, and HB RESEARCH PAPER SERIES. Bryan Orme, Sawtooth Software, Inc.

Sawtooth Software. MaxDiff Analysis: Simple Counting, Individual-Level Logit, and HB RESEARCH PAPER SERIES. Bryan Orme, Sawtooth Software, Inc. Sawtooth Software RESEARCH PAPER SERIES MaxDiff Analysis: Simple Counting, Individual-Level Logit, and HB Bryan Orme, Sawtooth Software, Inc. Copyright 009, Sawtooth Software, Inc. 530 W. Fir St. Sequim,

More information

WESTERN MEADOWLARK MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY MANUSCRIPT (ROUND 2)

WESTERN MEADOWLARK MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY MANUSCRIPT (ROUND 2) 1 WESTERN MEADOWLARK MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY MANUSCRIPT (ROUND 2) Abstract This research examines how consumers utilize base rate (e.g., disease prevalence in a population) and case information (e.g.,

More information

Langer and Rodin (1976) Aims

Langer and Rodin (1976) Aims Langer and Rodin (1976) Aims Langer and Rodin aimed to investigate the effect of personal control on general well-being and engagement in activities in elderly people in a nursing home. In the context

More information

Conducting Refugee Status Determination

Conducting Refugee Status Determination Conducting Refugee Status Determination Hong Kong Academy of Law Training Program on CAT Claims and Refugee Law 14-17 December 2009 David Welin UNHCR Regional Protection Hub for the Asia-Pacific Refugees

More information

When Falsification is the Only Path to Truth

When Falsification is the Only Path to Truth When Falsification is the Only Path to Truth Michelle Cowley (cowleym@tcd.ie) Psychology Department, University of Dublin, Trinity College, Dublin, Ireland Ruth M.J. Byrne (rmbyrne@tcd.ie) Psychology Department,

More information

What do Americans know about inequality? It depends on how you ask them

What do Americans know about inequality? It depends on how you ask them Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 7, No. 6, November 2012, pp. 741 745 What do Americans know about inequality? It depends on how you ask them Kimmo Eriksson Brent Simpson Abstract A recent survey of

More information

On the provenance of judgments of conditional probability

On the provenance of judgments of conditional probability On the provenance of judgments of conditional probability Jiaying Zhao (jiayingz@princeton.edu) Anuj Shah (akshah@princeton.edu) Daniel Osherson (osherson@princeton.edu) Abstract In standard treatments

More information

An Analytical Review of the Pew Report Entitled, It Takes A Team and the Accompanying Productivity and Profit Calculator

An Analytical Review of the Pew Report Entitled, It Takes A Team and the Accompanying Productivity and Profit Calculator An Analytical Review of the Pew Report Entitled, It Takes A Team and the Accompanying Productivity and Profit Calculator The report, It Takes a Team, was released by the Pew Center on the States in December

More information

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use Amanda Hahn (achahn30@gmail.com) Takashi Yamauchi (tya@psyc.tamu.edu) Na-Yung Yu (nayungyu@gmail.com) Department of Psychology, Mail

More information

SDT Clarifications 1. 1Colorado State University 2 University of Toronto 3 EurekaFacts LLC 4 University of California San Diego

SDT Clarifications 1. 1Colorado State University 2 University of Toronto 3 EurekaFacts LLC 4 University of California San Diego SDT Clarifications 1 Further clarifying signal detection theoretic interpretations of the Müller Lyer and sound induced flash illusions Jessica K. Witt 1*, J. Eric T. Taylor 2, Mila Sugovic 3, John T.

More information

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 KAZUO MORI HIDEKO MORI

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 KAZUO MORI HIDEKO MORI Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, 785-789. Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 EXAMINATION OF THE PASSIVE FACIAL FEEDBACK HYPOTHESIS USING AN IMPLICIT MEASURE: WITH A FURROWED BROW, NEUTRAL OBJECTS

More information