Citation for published version (APA): Wijnen, J. G. (2009). The role of selective inhibition in adaptive oculomotor control

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Citation for published version (APA): Wijnen, J. G. (2009). The role of selective inhibition in adaptive oculomotor control"

Transcription

1 UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) The role of selective inhibition in adaptive oculomotor control Wijnen, J.G. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Wijnen, J. G. (2009). The role of selective inhibition in adaptive oculomotor control General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam ( Download date: 24 Dec 2018

2 Chapter 2 Response inhibition in motor and oculomotor conflict tasks: Different mechanisms, different dynamics? Jasper G. Wijnen 1 K. Richard Ridderinkhof 1,2 1 Amsterdam center for the study of adaptive control in brain and behaviour, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands 2 Department of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands Chapter 2 - Response inhibition in motor and oculomotor conflict tasks: Different mechanisms, different dynamics? 41

3 Abstract Previous research has shown that the appearance of task irrelevant abrupt onsets influences saccadic eye movements during visual search and may slow down manual reactions to target stimuli. Analysis of reaction time distributions in the present study offers evidence suggesting that top down inhibition processes actively suppress oculomotor or motor responses elicited by a salient distractor, in order to resolve the conflict that arises when reflex like and deliberate responses are in opposition. Twenty six participants carried out a variation of the oculomotor capture task. They were instructed to respond with either a saccade toward or with a button press at the side of the hemifield in which a target color singleton appeared. A distractor stimulus could appear either in the same or in the opposite hemifield. Delta plots revealed competition between reflex like and deliberate response activation, and highlighted selective inhibition of automatic responses: While participants generally responded more slowly in incongruent compared to congruent situations, this effect diminished and even reversed in the slowest speed quantiles. These effects were present in both the oculomotor and motor response mode conditions. Descriptors: Attentional capture; Saccadic eye movements; Motor control; Response inhibition; Distributional analysis 42

4 Introduction In the distinction between automatic and controlled processes, first proposed by William James (1890), and revived by Shiffrin and colleagues (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977), the term automatic (exogenously controlled or stimulus driven) refers to those actions that are initiated indeliberately without awareness as a necessary condition for doing so. An example would be orienting to a loud noise or bright flash in the periphery of the visual field. In contrast, controlled (endogenously controlled or goal directed) refers to processes which require, in the words of James: an additional conscious element in the shape of a fiat, mandate, or expressed consent. These acts are willed and call for some effort to execute. An eye movement away from a salient stimulus, as is required in for example the antisaccade task (Hallet, 1978), necessitates a controlled response as well as inhibition of the automatic response towards the stimulus (for a recent comprehensive review see Reuter & Kathmann, 2004). Ignoring highly salient information in the environment while attending to objects that do not automatically capture attention places similar demands on human information processing. The outcomes of controlled and automatic processes may clash, and the resulting conflict needs to be resolved in favor of the controlled response. This paper aims to examine and compare the dynamics of activation and inhibition during such conflicts in the motor domain on the one hand and the oculomotor domain on the other. Specifically we seek to investigate the role of inhibitory processes in conflict resolution. A task was designed to allow the use of a common method for both domains and to investigate whether processes underlying motor control and oculomotor control exhibit similar dynamics. Oculomotor Capture and Competitive Integration Various saccadic eye movement tasks have been developed to investigate the relationship between goal directed and automatic responses in the oculomotor domain. Theeuwes, Kramer, Hahn, and Irwin (1998) presented a display of six gray circles to their subjects, five of which changed to a red color after a set period of time. Subjects were instructed to make an eye 43

5 movement towards the one circle that did not change color. On some trials a new red circle was introduced between two of the existing circles. The novel object was found to disrupt the programming and execution of the saccade towards the target (gray) circle, even though the new circle was irrelevant to the task. In many instances the eye first moved toward the distractor and paused there briefly before moving on toward the target. The automatic attraction of the eyes towards a salient stimulus in the environment was termed oculomotor capture. Since the brief fixation durations at the location of the distractor stimulus were shorter than the time typically needed to program a saccade, Theeuwes et al. concluded that the saccades toward the target and toward the distractor must have been programmed, at least partly, in parallel. The question remained whether the resulting saccades were simply those programmed by the first pathway to complete its processing or whether the pathways interact and compete even before a saccade is executed. Godijn and Theeuwes (2002), building in part on Trappenberg, Dorris, Munoz, and Klein (2001) and others, proposed a competitive integration model. In this model, the activation and inhibition signals for exogenous and endogenous saccades converge on a common saccade map which is presumably located in the superior colliculus (SC). On the saccade map, where information is represented retinotopically, the saccade goal is determined by a competitive process. In general, activation in a certain location will inhibit all distant locations, while having an excitatory effect on adjacent locations. In the model, saccades are executed once the activation in a certain location on the saccade map reaches a threshold level. A modified version of the oculomotor capture paradigm was used by Godijn and Theeuwes (2002) to test predictions made by the competitive integration model with regard to the timing of eye movements (saccade latencies). As described above, the oculomotor capture paradigm requires participants to overtly search for a specific target stimulus, which is presented at the same time as a highly salient distractor stimulus. In their rendering of this experimental paradigm Godijn and Theeuwes varied the distances between the locations at which the distractor and stimulus appeared. Since activation at distant locations on the saccade map will result in the mutual inhibition of both locations, it was expected that in this situation 44

6 activation levels would need longer to reach the threshold level. On the other hand when the distractor and target are close, activation in both locations will be strengthened and a threshold will be reached sooner. In accordance with these predictions, saccade latencies were shorter in the latter condition. Motor Capture and Mutliple Routes to Response Activation To study the dynamics of attentional and response conflicts in the motor domain, researchers have developed a variety of experimental speeded response time tasks which induce conflicting processing. Well known examples of such conflict tasks are the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935), the Simon task (Simon, 1969), and the Eriksen flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). All of these tasks include a relevant stimulus or relevant stimulus aspect on which the participant has to base his or her response. On the other hand there are irrelevant stimuli or stimulus aspects which are often highly salient and which may or may not suggest a response opposite to the response induced by the relevant stimulus. The condition in which irrelevant stimulus features manage to more or less automatically activate the corresponding motor response can tentatively be referred to as motor (or ideomotor) capture (analogous to oculomotor capture), the tendency of the motor system to engage in automatic, stimulus driven acts despite task instructions that specify otherwise. In conflict tasks those trials where relevant and irrelevant dimensions of the stimuli suggest responses that are opposed are usually called incongruent whereas trials in which the dimensions suggest the same response are called congruent. The mean difference in reaction time (RT) or accuracy between incongruent and congruent trials consistently found in conflict tasks is taken as a measure of the time needed to overcome automatic stimulus driven activation between different processing streams and is called the congruence effect. In recent years, dual process conceptions of how sensory information leads to activation of the correct response have become increasingly popular. In such conceptions, perceptionaction coupling can be established via two parallel routes, one controlled and deliberate, the other fast, direct, and more or less automatic. Kornblum, Hasbroucq, and Osman (1990) set the 45

7 stage with their dual route model for congruence effects. Although dual route models had been formulated previously (e.g., Sanders, 1967; Frith & Done, 1986), the Kornblum et al. model has served as a significant impetus for subsequent research into congruence effects. Conceived on the basis of theoretical considerations, the model contains a number of discrete stages of processing, arranged partly in parallel. Upon identification, a stimulus is thought to deliberately activate an instruction based response via the controlled route, and to activate an automatic, instruction independent response via the direct route. If the two response codes match, the motor program already activated via the direct route can be carried out quickly; if they mismatch, this motor program must be aborted in favor of the alternative motor program, whose retrieval and execution costs extra time. The rudimentary dual route architecture of the Kornblum et al. model has been embraced by many authors in the field (e.g., de Jong, Liang, & Lauber, 1994; Eimer, Hommel, & Prinz, 1995; Munoz & Everling, 2004; Proctor, Lu, Wang, & Dutta, 1995; Ridderinkhof, van der Molen, & Bashore, 1995), and has been implemented in formal connectionist models capable of simulating many of the key phenomena reported in the conflict task literature (e.g., Cohen et al., 1991). Interestingly, this type of multiple process model for motor capture is apparently compatible with the competitive integration model for oculomotor capture (in which the saccade map is also affected by two separate processing streams). Bottom Up Activation and Top Down Control Strong habitual connections between sensory stimuli and corresponding responses may afford rapid execution of natural responses without demanding much attention. However, these responses need to be overruled when prepotent stimulus response mappings are inappropriate and more controlled goal directed action selection is required. This top down bias is especially important when the pathways leading to the desired action compete for expression in behavior with concurrent, more habitual pathways (Miller & Cohen, 2001). Crucial in this architecture is that the information processing system knows when top down control is required without recourse to a homunculus. According to the conflict monitoring hypothesis (Botvinick, Braver, 46

8 Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001), the response system is monitored for the occurrence of conflict. When response conflict is detected, this information is used by other mechanisms to implement appropriate top down control, as described in more detail below. Inhibitory control has been postulated as one of the mechanisms by which top down bias may be implemented to optimize behavior (for a review see Munoz & Everling, 2004; Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, Segalowitz, & Carter, 2004a). Response inhibition can be defined descriptively as the mechanism or set of processes that results in the containment of prepotent behavioral responses when such actions are reflex like, premature, inappropriate, or incorrect (Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, Wijnen, & Burle, 2004b). Thus, response inhibition can be used as an instrument to suppress exogenous direct activation of motor programming structures to prevent them from resulting in overt behavior. Several experiments have pointed to a role for inhibitory processes in conflict tasks. For instance, Eimer & Schlaghecken (1998) asked participants to react to the orientation of a target stimulus (an arrow) and found performance costs when target stimuli were preceded by a masked prime in the same orientation. Benefits were found when the prime suggested the opposite response. The effect reversed when the interval between prime and target was shortened. The pattern suggests that participants were actively inhibiting the non predictive prime, but failed to do so in time when the interval between prime and target was relatively short. To accommodate a wider range of empirical phenomena, models for oculomotor and motor capture may be supplemented to incorporate such inhibitory mechanisms. The competitive integration model for instance may be extended so that the distractor is inhibited soon after both target and distractor have been identified. A Method for Studying Inhibitory Control To accommodate a mechanism that might perform the selective response suppression functions described above, at least conceptually, Ridderinkhof (2002) argued for an addition to the dual process model in which an activation suppression mechanism, that would be triggered in the event of conflict between the two routes and its operations, would affect the exogenous 47

9 direct activation route (or the response induced by that route). The effect of such a mechanism would be to suppress exogenous influences (such as motor capture) and to allow endogenous processing to effectively compete and gain control over the response. On the assumption that selective response suppression takes some time to evolve during the course of response activation and thus would not be operational immediately after presentation of the stimulus giving rise to this response activation, the effects of response inhibition would be manifest especially in relatively slow responses, in which response suppression would have more time to build up than in responses that happen to be fast. Hence, responses at the slower tail of the RT distribution are more sensitive to the manifestations of selective suppression than responses at the fast end. Thus, in case of incongruent trials, in which selective suppression of the inappropriate response should benefit the activation of the correct response, slow responses should benefit more than fast trials, and this is indeed what is often observed in distribution analyses of RTs in conflict tasks (e.g. Bub, Masson & Lalonde, 2006; Burle, Possamaï, Vidal, Bonnet & Hasbroucq, 2002; de Jong, Liang & Lauber, 1994, Eimer, 1999, Ridderinkhof, 2002; for an overview see Ridderinkhof et al., 2004b). Analyses of RT distributions typically focus on cumulative density functions (CDFs), which represent the cumulative probability of responding as a function of response speed. In analyzing CDFs from conflict tasks, delta plots can be abstracted from vincentized CDFs (Ratcliff, 1979; Vincent, 1912) for illustrational purposes. Delta plots represent the congruence effect (the difference in RT between incongruent and congruent trials) as a function of response speed. This technique involves dividing the RT distributions of congruent and incongruent trials into a number of equal size RT quantiles, subtracting mean RTs for congruent trials, quantile by quantile, from those of incongruent trials and then plotting these differences, again quantile by quantile, against the average RT of the congruent and incongruent speed quantiles. The typical pattern for the sigmoid shaped CDFs is an increase of RT mean differences with slower response speeds. This is a consequence of the proportional difference between conditions remaining constant as the average RT increases, resulting in an increase of absolute differences between conditions. In delta plots this is reflected in a positive slope. However if selective suppression of the inappropriate response in incongruent trials benefits the activation 48

10 of slow correct responses more than fast responses, then slow responses should benefit more than fast trials, and this will be expressed in the delta plot as a leveling off or even turning negative of the delta plot at slower quantiles. Indeed, in the Simon task, where subjects typically have to press a left or right hand button according to stimulus color, whereas the stimulus itself may be presented to the left or right of a fixation point (creating compatible and incompatible stimulus response relations), delta plots typically run negative, with strongly reduced or even reversed congruence effects at the slowest response quantiles. However, when the exact same Simon task is intermixed with a small number of trials in which location is the target stimulus dimension, subjects can no longer suppress the location driven response a priori, even though location remains irrelevant in the Simon task itself. In such a context, immediate selective suppression of the location driven response is prevented, and consequently the delta plot for the Simon effect is found to run much more positive (Ridderinkhof, 2002). Several studies have shown similar effects using this method. Burle et al. (2002) used electromyographic recordings in a Simon task to separate trials in which participants made partial errors (as indicated by subthreshold activation of muscles involved in the incorrect response) from correct trials. These partial error trials were associated with reductions of the congruence effect with increasing response speed, seen in a more downward going delta plot compared to the delta plots for fully correct responses. The high conflict in these partial error trials elicited stronger selective suppression of exogenously induced activation which made for a more efficient inhibition of the erroneous response. Common Dynamics of Inhibition and Activation for Different Response Modalities Although different brain circuits are involved in the activation and inhibition of reponses in the motor and oculomotor systems we anticipated that the respective mechanisms of selective response suppression behave similarly on a functional level, and thus will be expressed in similar ways in CDFs and delta plots of manual and saccadic RTs. Evidence for some commonalities in the mechanisms involved in inhibition and activation for manual, saccadic and 49

11 vocal response modalities was found in a study by Eimer and Schlaghecken (2001). In this study centrally and peripherally positioned masked cues were presented to subjects in a cue priming procedure (i.e. a conflict task whereby subjects are presented with two consecutive signals, one of which ( the cue ) is task irrelevant and the other ( the target ) requiring a discriminative response. In such a procedure task irrelevant cues are sufficiently similar to targets to capture the attentional or response systems). For all response modalities it was found that under these circumstances (masked cues) only motor activation processes triggered by foveal visual stimuli are subject to inhibition. Given these similarities in oculomotor and motor control we sought to explore whether differences still exist in the dynamics of inhibitory control for both response modalities, using the distribution analyses method described earlier. To this end we developed a variation of the oculomotor capture paradigm suitable for both saccadic and manual responses, which includes congruent and incongruent trials (as opposed to the distractor and no distractor trials typically used in the oculomotor capture paradigm), thus allowing us to chart congruence effects. Participants were asked to respond to one of four circles that did not change color while being simultaneously distracted by a displacement of one of the circles. They were instructed to indicate the side of the screen which contained the target either by making an eye movement towards that side or by pressing one of two buttons corresponding to the relevant side. Congruent trials were defined as those trials in which both target and distractor appeared on the same half of the screen; other trials were labeled incongruent. We expected a leveling off of the delta plot (similar to that found in earlier studies using manual conflict tasks) for both oculomotor and manual response modes. In order to test the commonalities of motor and oculomotor control processes more specifically we varied the interval between the distractor and the target (this interval is often called the Stimulus Onset Asynchrony or SOA). An effect of SOA on delta plot slopes was found by Burle, van den Wildenberg and Ridderinkhof (2005) using a cue priming procedure. Delta plot slopes were found to decline more steeply for larger SOAs. We investigated whether a comparable effect of SOA could be found for the current task.and, crucially, whether such an effect of SOA on delta plot slopes would be similar across response modalities. 50

12 Method Participants Twenty six undergraduate students (17 female) from the University of Amsterdam took part in the experiment and obtained course credits for their participation. Mean age was (SD: 3.87, Min: 18, Max: 36). All participants reported to be healthy and to have normal or corrected to normal vision (however persons wearing spectacles were excluded from participation due to possible interference with the eyetracker software). Subjects were tested individually in a quiet and dimly lit university chamber. Stimuli and Apparatus Participants were seated in a comfortable chair with response buttons attached to both arm rests, at a distance of 55 cm in front of a computer screen on which stimuli where shown against a black background. A small white cross (subtending a visual angle of approximately 0.5 o x 0.5 o ) served as a fixation point during the experiment. On each trial four green circles (diameter 1.6 o ) were presented appearing in four out of six possible locations. The configuration of circles was constrained in such a way that at least two circles were presented in each half (left/right) of the screen, leaving one possible location open. Vertical distance from the fixation point was either 0 o or 2.5 o of visual angle above or below the fixation point. Horizontal distance from the fixation point was 10.8 o of visual angle to the left or the right of the fixation point. During each trial, three of the four circles changed from a green to an equiluminant blue color. In addition one of the circles changed position to the empty location in the same half of the screen (if the empty spot was situated above or below the fixation point, the circle changing position was always the one closest to it). The circle that changed position was never the one that retained its original color. Figure 1 depicts an experimental trial. 51

13 Figure 1. A possible congruent experimental trial (left) with a Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA) of 0 ms and an incongruent experimental trial (right) with a SOA of greater than 0 ms. In the congruent example the circle positioned center right moves to the bottom right position, thus forming the distractor. In the incongruent example the circle positioned top left moves to the center left position. In both examples the circle positioned top right is the only one not to change to an equiluminant blue color, forming the target. On each trial participants were asked to respond to the circle that did not change color (sustained color singleton target). The circle displacement was irrelevant for the execution of the task (offset onset distractor). Trials in which the target and the distractor were presented within the same hemifield are designated congruent trials; in all other cases the trial is labelled incongruent. The target and distractor could either be presented simultaneously (a SOA of 0 ms) or the distractor could be presented 120 or 240 ms in advance of the target. In each experimental block all possible starting configurations of circles appeared with one exception. The possible configuration where initially both open locations were located at the same height as the fixation point on both sides was excluded to limit the total duration of an experimental block. Since more distractor options are available when the starting configuration of circles features an open location on the same height as the fixation point (the distractor on that side can move either upwards from below or downwards from above) these starting configurations were presented more frequently than configurations in which the open locations were above or below the fixation point on both sides (with a ratio of 3:2). For each of the starting configurations all possible combinations of color changes and circle displacements occurred once for incongruent trials, or twice for congruent trials. Given 52

14 that the target was constrained not to appear on the circle that moved, the number of options for the target location is limited to one when both target and distractor appear in the same half of the screen (congruent trials), while two options are available to incongruent trials. To keep the ratio between congruent and incongruent trials at 1:1 all possible congruent targetdistractor combinations appeared twice. The sequence of starting configurations and distractor and target positions was randomised for each experimental block. As a consequence congruent and incongruent trials were presented in randomised order. In one half of the trial blocks subjects indicated their response by pressing one of the two response keys with their index fingers. In the other half no manual response was requested and participants responded by merely making an eye movement to the target stimulus. Saccades were recorded using an infrared based iview eyetracker (SMI, Berlin, Germany) with 50 Hz temporal resolution and a <0.1 o spatial resolution. The head was stabilized with a comfortable chin rest. Design Each of the experimental blocks contained 240 trials, 120 of which were congruent and 120 incongruent. Each of the congruence conditions contained 40 trials per SOA condition. A total of four experimental blocks were completed, two of which were manual response blocks and two were oculomotor response blocks. The sequence of response mode blocktypes was counterbalanced over subjects with the constraint that a manual block was always followed by an oculomotor block and vice versa. Procedure The experiment involved a single session which lasted approximately 60 minutes. Participants fixated four series of six calibration targets at the start of the session. Calibration targets appeared at the locations which would later be occupied by the experimental stimuli. After calibration participants practiced the experimental task. Practice ended when at least 20 trials 53

15 were responded to correctly. Four experimental blocks were completed in this fashion, each lasting approximately 12 minutes. In blocks where the subject was asked to make a manual response, the instruction was given to push the button corresponding to the side of the screen in which the target was shown. Eye movements were not recorded in manual blocks. Blocks where a saccade was requested were accompanied by an instruction to move the eyes to the target as soon as it was presented on the screen. Instructions to the participant were given verbally and were reiterated in written form on an instruction screen at the start of each experimental block. Speed was emphasized over accuracy. It was stressed to the participant that the eyes should be directed to the fixation point at the start of each trial and should remain fixated until a target was presented on the screen. The starting configuration of circles was presented on the screen for 600 ms. After that the distractor appeared simultaneously with or quickly followed by the target according to the SOA condition. Target and distractor were shown for an additional period of 1200 ms after the presentation of the distractor. All stimuli then disappeared and a black screen with only the fixaton point visible was displayed for 1000 ms until the start of the next trial. Data Analysis In the analysis of the manual response data, trials in which the RT was below 100 ms were excluded (these were assumed be fast guesses). In addition responses slower then 1000 ms were excluded (misses). This resulted in a loss of 0.40 % of the trials, all of which were fast guesses. For the oculomotor response data an identical time window existed outside of which saccadic RTs were excluded from further analysis (loss 2.27% for fast guesses; 1.03% for misses). Movement artefacts or eye blinks occurring within this time window were also grounds for excluding the trial from further analysis (loss 1.59%). Furthermore trials were excluded if the eyes were not directed at the fixation point at the time the target was presented on the screen (loss 4.11%). To determine whether the eyes were at fixation at that time, a small time window was defined beginning 40 ms before target appearance and extending 40 ms after target 54

16 appearance (5 samples). If the average position signal in this window did not exceed 2 o of visual angle relative to the fixation position, the eyes were considered to be at fixation at target presentation. Outliers were removed from the data using a recursive procedure adopted from McCormick (1997), which involved temporarily removing the fastest and slowest RTs from each condition for each participant. After removal the mean and standard deviation for the remaining data was calculated. If either of the two removed data points fell outside an interval bounded by 4 SDs from the mean, that data point was removed permanently. If the data point fell within the interval it was returned to the data set. This procedure continued until no more data points were removed permanently. In the manual blocks 1.26% of the data was removed as outlier, in the saccade blocks 0.88 % of the data was removed. In off line analysis saccadic reaction times (SRTs) were defined as the time at which the velocity of the eye movement exceeded 100 o /s, but only if the movement resulted in a horizontal position signal exceeding 2.5 o of visual angle relative to the fixation position. For saccade accuracy purposes, trials were scored as an error trial if the first eye movement that satisfied the above criteria with regards to velocity and position moved away from the target on the horizontal axis. Note that for both manual and oculomotor blocks a reaction that is based solely on the location of the distractor is scored as correct when the trial is congruent. For each subject mean RT and overall accuracy were computed for congruent and incongruent trials aggregated over blocks. Two way analyses of variance using SPSS s GLM repeated measures feature were conducted with congruence and SOA as within subjects factors and RT and accuracy as the dependent variables. This was done separately for manual and oculomotor blocks. To capture the dynamics of the congruence effect, the RTs for each subject were rankordered for each SOA and congruence condition. Only correct trials were used for this purpose. The RTs were then divided into five equal size speed bins, thus comprising vincentized CDFs. For each of these bins the congruence effect was computed and plotted against the average RT collapsed across congruence conditions for that speed bin and SOA condition. 55

17 Results All reported p values were corrected using the Greenhouse Geisser adjustment of the degrees of freedom when necessary. Overall Performance Manual response speed and accuracy. Figure 2 shows the mean RTs for correct responses and accuracy levels in the manual response condition. Congruent trials were responded to significantly faster in comparison with incongruent trials. ANOVA revealed this difference to be significant (F(1,25)= 25.02, p < 0.001). Subjects made more errors when distractor and target appeared at opposite halves of the screen (F(1,25)= 6.63, p = 0.016). Figure 2. Mean reaction times (RTs; line graphs) and error rates (bar graphs) in congruent (gray) and incongruent (black) trials for the three stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) conditions, displayed separately for the manual (left) and oculomotor (right) blocks. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. 56

18 The length of the SOA had a clear effect on both RT (F(2,50)= , p < 0.001) and accuracy (F(2,50)= 18.77, p < 0.001). Participants responded faster at the longer SOAs but also were more prone to make an error. For the longer SOAs differences in reaction speeds between congruence conditions were reduced (F(2,50)= 6.82, p = 0.004). No interaction between congruence and SOA condition was found on accuracy (F(2,50)= 1.88, p = 0.18). Oculomotor response speed and accuracy. In contrast to the manual response mode blocks oculomotor reaction speed did not seem to be hampered by the distractor appearing in a position opposite to that of the target. The main effect of congruence on SRTs was not significant (F(1,25)= 1.13, p = 0.30). Longer SOAs however did enhance subjects reaction speeds (F(2,50)= , p < 0.001). The effect of congruence on SRTs did not differ significantly between SOA conditions (F(2,50)= 1.04, p = 0.36). Accuracy was affected by congruence with more errors being made on incongruent trials (F(1,25)= 8.81, p = 0.007). Longer intervals between the target and the distractor also elicited more errors (F(2,50)= , p < 0.001). The congruence effect on accuracy did not differ significantly between the SOA conditions (interaction between SOA and congruence (F(2,50)= 2.27, p = 0.11)). Figure 3. Cumulative density functions (CDFs) and delta plots (top left corner) for the three stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) conditions, displayed separately for the manual (left) and oculomotor (right) blocks. Separate CDFs are shown for the congruent (CG; thick lines) and incongruent (IC; thin striped lines) conditions. Black diamonds are used for the shortest SOA, gray diamonds are used for the intermediate SOA (120 ms), white diamonds are used for the longest SOA (240 ms). 57

19 Distributional Analysis CDFs, which plot the probability of responding as a function of response speed, were constructed for five speed bins. From these delta plots were extracted, which plot difference scores (incongruent minus congruent) for each of the five speed bins against the average of both conditions in that speed bin. This was done for each subject separately and then averaged across subjects (see Figure 3). The CDFs and delta plots allow us to examine whether the presence and absence of congruence effects (in ideo and oculomotor versions of the task, respectively) remained stable in magnitude across the entire RT distribution. The plots for the manual response mode depict a positive congruence effect for all of the speed bins in the 0 ms and 120 ms SOA conditions, and a positive effect in the 240 ms SOA condition for the faster responses. The congruence effect gradually declines or remains roughly constant in all of the SOA conditions when responses become slower. In the longest SOA condition this even leads to a negative congruence effect. To investigate the significance of mean differences in the CDFs we conducted three way ANOVAs with congruence, SOA, and quantile as within subject factors and mean RT as the dependent variable. This was done for both the motor and the oculomotor CDFs. For the motor CDFs, a significant interaction was found between SOA and quantile (F(8,200)=11.69, p<.001), which appears to reflect that the variance in the RT distribution associated with the longest SOA is higher compared to the distributions of the other two SOA conditions. No interaction was observed between quantile and congruence condition (F(4,100)=0.10, p=.84) or between all three factors (F(8,200)=0.77, p=.52). This pattern indicates that the slopes of the delta plots do not deviate from zero, in any of the SOA conditions. For the oculomotor CDFs, a significant interaction was found between SOA and quantile (F(8,200)=14.82, p<.001), again pointing to the pattern that the variance in the RT distribution associated with the longest SOA is higher than in the other two SOA conditions. Delta plot slopes for this response modality showed a strong decline in the effect of congruence when subjects responded slower. A significant two way interaction between quantile and congruence condition (F(8,200)=16.58, p<.001) confirmed this pattern. The sharpest decline in congruence effects was seen in the longest SOA condition; the shortest SOA 58

20 condition showed a similar decline but less steep; and, unexpectedly, the middle SOA condition slopes downward sharply for the first two speed bins but then levels out for the remaining bins. Delta plots for the different SOA conditions differed significantly from each other as indicated by a three way interaction between all of the factors (F(8,200)=3.96, p=.013). Delta plot slopes for the oculomotor data seemed more sharply negative going for the oculomotor data compared to the motor condition. A four way ANOVA with congruence, SOA, quantile and response modality as within subject factors and mean RT as the dependent variable was conducted to examine whether these differences between the response modalities could have been generated by chance. The analysis revealed a significant three way interaction between response modality, congruence and quantile (F(4,100)=9.57, p<.001), confirming the more pronounced decline in the congruence effect for the oculomotor data as compared to the motor data, which is more stable across the RT distribution. Discussion The aim of this study was to explore response activation and selective response suppression in motor and oculomotor versions of a conflict task. More specifically, we set out to verify whether the dynamics of response suppression would be expressed in similar ways in motor and oculomotor RT distributions. To that end, a conflict task was designed that allowed us to record manual and saccadic responses from the same subjects using identical task and stimulus formats. An additional element of our approach was to include an experimental manipulation that is known to influence the expression of response inhibition in RT distribution functions (in this case SOA, or the interval between distractor and target onset; Eimer, 1999; Burle et al, 2005), and to examine whether this manipulation had similar effects on the distributions of saccadic RTs. The results can be summarized as follows. Congruent trials elicited faster and more accurate manual responses than incongruent trials, and this congruence effect tended to decrease with longer SOA s. For oculomotor responses these patterns were less pronounced or absent, at least at the level of overall performance. Closer inspection of the performance data using distribution analysis, however, revealed different patterns. As anticipated, the 59

21 congruence effects on RT did not increase monotonically as a function of response speed. For manual responses, the effect remained stable in magnitude across the entire RT distribution in each SOA condition. For oculomotor responses, the effect in fact decreased as a function of response speed, and more so at long than at short SOA s. In general, these patterns are in accordance with the predictions derived from the activation suppression model (Burle et al., 2002, 2005; Eimer, 1999; Ridderinkhof, 2002). This type of model assumes that distractor stimuli may initially activate a response, but that this response activation is subsequently inhibited by a process of selective suppression. This selective suppression may result in relative facilitation of the response driven by the target, to such an extent that in some cases responses to incongruent stimuli are in fact faster than responses to congruent stimuli (Burle et al., 2005; Eimer, 1999). Selective suppression is slow to build up, such that responses at the slow tail of the RT distribution benefit more from this inhibition than responses at the faster end. The results of the distribution analyses suggest that selective response suppression is at play in the oculomotor version of the conflict task as much as in the motor version. Even though a congruence effect was not detectable when looking at overall saccadic RTs, such an effect was revealed in the distribution of RTs. Fast saccadic responses, presumably being less influenced by a slowly building inhibition process, showed a relatively large congruence effect, but for slower responses the congruence effect declined and reversed in all three SOA conditions. We take this pattern to indicate the evolving presence of active top down inhibition processes, which selectively suppresses automatic oculomotor responses to the distractor. If anything, suppression manifested itself more prominently in oculomotor than manual responses, as the delta plots ran more negatively for saccadic RTs than for manual RTs. In fact, for saccadic responses, the suppression was so strong as to produce reversed congruence effects in the slowest speed quantiles. For manual responses the congruence effect did not seem to decrease strongly in any of the SOA conditions. The resulting delta plots which look more or less flat should not be taken to indicate an absence of response inhibition. Such absence would be reflected in a monotonous increase of the congruence effect with increasing RT and thus a positively sloped delta plot. A linear increase in RT differences between conditions is a standard 60

22 statistical property of RT distributions, following directly from the fact that the variance increases with the mean (Wagenmakers, Grasman & Molenaar, 2005). Departures from this pattern such as flat or negative going delta plots, appear only when response inhibition is brought into the equation. The finding that the effects of congruence decrease with increasing SOA length concur with other studies that introduced an SOA between the appearance of irrelevant and relevant stimuli or stimulus aspects. In the inhibition of return (IOR) paradigm (Posner & Cohen, 1984; Posner, Rafal, Choate, & Vaughan, 1985; for a review, see Klein, 2000) a task irrelevant cue is presented prior to the presentation of a target. As long as the SOA between cue and target is relatively short, subjects benefit from the cue if it appears at the location of subsequent target appearance. When SOAs are increased, benefits in RT turn into costs, thus decreasing and reversing effects of congruence between cue and target. Eimer and Schlaghecken (1998; Eimer, 1999; Schlaghecken & Eimer, 2000) observed similar performance benefits for (central and subliminally presented) cues that validly point out the response required by the imminent target (somewhat similar to our congruent trials), as long as the interval between the two was relatively short. When the interval was lengthened the benefits decreased and even turned into performance costs. This pattern was interpreted to reflect initial activation (based in direct perceptuomotor links) followed by active inhibition of the initial response tendency. The decrease of the congruence effect with longer SOAs suggests that the effectiveness of inhibition is a function of the degree of temporal overlap between the responses activated by distractor and target. When the target is presented while inhibition is still in its early stages, the system can take little advantage from it, resulting in long RTs for incongruent trials. When the target is presented later, at a time when inhibition has more fully developed, incongruent trials benefit more from the distractor related inhibition. Not only responses to incongruent stimuli, but to a lesser extent also responses to congruent stimuli benefited from a longer SOA. Schlaghecken and Eimer (2000) found a similar effect when (subliminal) arrow cues where presented peripherally instead of centrally. They concluded that while centrally presented cues give rise to an activation followed by inhibition effect, peripheral cues produce an activation only effect, with the cue causing a burst of 61

23 response activation that dissipates passively over time. However, passive dissipation cannot explain the decrease and reversal of the congruence effect that we see in our data. Thus, the small benefit for congruent trials appears to reflect a more general preparation effect associated with longer SOAs. The present SOA manipulation appeared to result in a speed/accuracy tradeoff: speed benefits from the longer SOAs were accompanied by accuracy effects in the opposite direction (i.e., more errors). Importantly, however, the finding that increasing SOAs are associated with increasing congruence effects on accuracy but with decreasing congruence effects on RT likely reflects details of the RT distributions rather than speed/accuracy trade off. On the one hand, long SOAs were shown to be associated with the strongest response inhibition, explaining the decrease of congruence effects on RT. On the other hand, long SOAs are accompanied by relatively many fast responses (cf. Figure 2), many of which may be distractor driven and therefore result in error. Strong response inhibition in this condition might not be able to prevent these fast errors because, in line with the activation suppression hypothesis, the inhibition takes some time to develop and would exert its influence (be it on RT or accuracy) only on trials within the slower tail of the RT distribution. Although the patterns in the delta plots were largely in line with the predictions based on the activation suppression hypothesis, and were not predicted by alternative frameworks, one remaining concern is the possibility that the presently observed patterns are not typical for just any kind of manipulation within our task set up, but instead are specific to our critical experimental manipulation of congruence. In a separate study (Wijnen & Ridderinkhof, submitted) 1, two experimental tasks were designed to be as similar to each other and to the present one as possible. One version included a conflict element (presumably invoking the process of selective response suppression, as in the present task), whereas the other task version lacked this conflict element. For the conflict task we observed negative going delta plots, as in the present case, but for the control task we established that, as much as in any non conflict RT task, effect size increased (rather than decreased) with slower responses. This pattern, which was observed for both the manual and oculomotor response modalities, attests 1 See Chapter 3 62

24 to the specificity of the RT distribution effect as an effect ensuing from inhibitory processes triggered by the need to overcome automatic stimulus driven activation, and strengthens further our interpretation in terms of activation suppression models. In conclusion, despite massive differences in the neural circuitry underlying motor control and oculomotor control, the present study indicates that the processes of direct response activation and selective response suppression have several functional and dynamical characteristics in common. While the expression of response inhibition in the RT distributions was more pronounced for saccadic responses, this pattern appears to reflect gradual differences rather than fundamental discrepancies. This is an important observation, since it implies that various global functional features of the response inhibition system, as modeled in the dual process architecture and accompanying activation suppression model, can be studied in the motor and oculomotor system alike. This is not to say that results (e.g., clinical individual differences) observed in one modality can readily be generalized to the other; but effects seen in one modality at the very least may directly inform hypotheses about whether and how such effects might come to expression in the other. Another implication is that relatively simple RT distribution analysis techniques may be applied in the field of oculomotor control to examine the dynamics of oculomotor inhibition. This supplementary technique, combined with the available (reasonably detailed) knowledge about the anatomical and physiological properties of the oculomotor system, may prove valuable in advancing our understanding of the nature of response inhibition processes. 63

25 References Atkinson, R.C. & Shiffrin, R.M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 2, Botvinick, M.M., Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M., Carter, C.S. & Cohen, J.D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review 108(3), Bub, D.N., Masson, M.E.J. & Lalonde, C.E. (2006). Cognitive control in children: Stroop interference and suppression of word reading. Psychological Science, 17(4), Burle, B., Possamaï, C. A., Vidal, F., Bonnet, M. & Hasbroucq, T. (2002). Executive control in the Simon effect: An electromyographic and distributional analysis. Psychological Research, 66, Burle, B., van den Wildenberg, W.P.M. & Ridderinkhof, K.R. (2005). Dynamics of facilitation and interference in cue priming and Simon tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17(5), Cohen, J.D., Dunbar, K. & McClelland, J.L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing model of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97(3), Eimer, M. (1999). Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. Acta Psychologica, 101(2 3), Eimer, M., Hommel, B. & Prinz, W. (1995). S R compatibility and response selection. Acta Psychologica, 90, Eimer, M. & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimantal Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, Eimer, M. & Schlaghecken, F. (2001). Response facilitation and inhibition in manual, vocal and oculomotor performance; Evidence for a modality unspecific mechanism. Journal of motor behaviour, 33(1), Eriksen, B.A. & Eriksen, C.W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of targetletters in a non search task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16,

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set In press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set Timothy J. Wright 1, Walter

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load Psychonomic Bulletin & Review In press Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load JOSHUA D. COSMAN AND SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Attentional capture by abrupt onsets can be

More information

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2009, Vol. 35, No. 2, 434 445 2009 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014529 Influence of Visual Stimulus

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Shared Spatial Representations for Numbers and Space: The Reversal of the SNARC and the Simon Effects

Shared Spatial Representations for Numbers and Space: The Reversal of the SNARC and the Simon Effects Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 5, 1197 1207 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.5.1197

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return

Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (6), 935-944 doi:10.3758/mc.37.6.935 Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return JONG MOON CHOI

More information

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2009, 16 (1), 57-61 doi:10.3758/pbr.16.1.57 The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words DANIEL SMILEK, GRAYDEN J. F. SOLMAN, PETER MURAWSKI, AND

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search VISUAL COGNITION, 0000, 00 (0), 111 Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search Walter R. Boot Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee,

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (7), 1236-1247 doi: 10.3758/MC/36.7.1236 An action sequence held in memory can interfere with response selection of a target stimulus, but does not interfere with response activation

More information

Cognitive Control in Children: Stroop Interference and Suppression of Word Reading

Cognitive Control in Children: Stroop Interference and Suppression of Word Reading Psychological Science (in press accepted May 2005) Cognitive Control in Children: Stroop Interference and Suppression of Word Reading Daniel N. Bub, Michael E. J. Masson, and Christopher E. Lalonde University

More information

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 396 409 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.396

More information

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the 1 Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 835-54. doi:10.1037/a0012726

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1 The attentional window configures to object boundaries University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 2 ABSTRACT When

More information

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SESORY COSTRAIS? TOWARD A UIFIED THEORY OF GEERAL TASK DIFFICULTY Hanna Benoni and Yehoshua Tsal Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University hannaben@post.tau.ac.il

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Dissociation of S-R Compatibility and Simon Effects With Mixed Tasks and Mappings

Dissociation of S-R Compatibility and Simon Effects With Mixed Tasks and Mappings Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 5, 000 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0029923 Dissociation of S-R Compatibility

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Den Bosch: Boxpress

Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Den Bosch: Boxpress UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Parigger, E.M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Cognitive Control in Children

Cognitive Control in Children PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Cognitive Control in Children Stroop Interference and Suppression of Word Reading Daniel N. Bub, Michael E.J. Masson, and Christopher E. Lalonde University of Victoria,

More information

Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task

Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task 2007 volume 3 no 3 409-417 Advances in Cognitive Psychology Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task John F. Stins 1,2,

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing

Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing Nooraini Yusoff 1, André Grüning 1 and Antony Browne 1 1 Department of Computing, Faculty of Engineering and Physical

More information

Selective attention and response set in the Stroop task

Selective attention and response set in the Stroop task Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (7), 893-904 doi:10.3758/mc.38.7.893 Selective attention and response set in the Stroop task MARTIJN J. M. LAMERS, ARDI ROELOFS, AND INGE M. RABELING-KEUS Radboud University

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations

Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 742-748 Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations Çağlar Akçay and Eliot Hazeltine University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Recent

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming

Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming Biological Psychology 64 (2003) 7 26 Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming Martin Eimer a,, Friederike Schlaghecken b a School of Psychology, Birkbeck College, University of London,

More information

Attentional Capture in Singleton-Detection and Feature-Search Modes

Attentional Capture in Singleton-Detection and Feature-Search Modes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2003, Vol. 29, No. 5, 1003 1020 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1003

More information

Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning

Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (3), 449 456 Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning Natacha Deroost and Eric Soetens Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels,

More information

A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions

A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions Neurocomputing 69 (2006) 1322 1326 www.elsevier.com/locate/neucom A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions Nicola De Pisapia, Todd S. Braver Cognitive

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Vascular factors in dementia and apathy Eurelings, Lisa. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Vascular factors in dementia and apathy Eurelings, Lisa. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Vascular factors in dementia and apathy Eurelings, Lisa Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Eurelings, L. S. M. (2016). Vascular factors in

More information

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze Psychology, 2014, 5, 451-460 Published Online April 2014 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.55055 Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Brain training improves recovery after stroke but waiting list improves equally: A multicenter randomized controlled trial of a computer-based cognitive flexibility

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Spatially Diffuse Inhibition Affects Multiple Locations: A Reply to Tipper, Weaver, and Watson (1996)

Spatially Diffuse Inhibition Affects Multiple Locations: A Reply to Tipper, Weaver, and Watson (1996) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1996, Vol. 22, No. 5, 1294-1298 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/%/$3.00 Spatially Diffuse Inhibition

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

Precueing Spatial S-R Correspondence: Is There Regulation of Expected Response Conflict?

Precueing Spatial S-R Correspondence: Is There Regulation of Expected Response Conflict? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2008, Vol. 34, No. 4, 872 883 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.872

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction

More information

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The number line effect reflects top-down control Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),862-868???-??? The number line effect reflects top-down control JELENA RISTIC University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psych.colorado.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

The Temporal Dynamic of Automatic Inhibition of Irrelevant Actions

The Temporal Dynamic of Automatic Inhibition of Irrelevant Actions Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2015, Vol. 41, No. 1, 000 2015 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038654 The Temporal

More information

Gezinskenmerken: De constructie van de Vragenlijst Gezinskenmerken (VGK) Klijn, W.J.L.

Gezinskenmerken: De constructie van de Vragenlijst Gezinskenmerken (VGK) Klijn, W.J.L. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Gezinskenmerken: De constructie van de Vragenlijst Gezinskenmerken (VGK) Klijn, W.J.L. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Klijn, W. J. L. (2013).

More information

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 3, 580 584 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027885 OBSERVATION Templates

More information

The Simon effect in vocal responses

The Simon effect in vocal responses Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 210 226 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The Simon effect in vocal responses Peter Wühr * Friedrich Alexander Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg, Institut für Psychologie I, Kochstrasse

More information

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Nart Bedin Atalay (natalay@selcuk.edu.tr) Selcuk University, Konya, TURKEY Mine Misirlisoy

More information

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003)

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003) Cognition 97 (2005) 89 97 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003) Filip Van Opstal a, Bert Reynvoet b, Tom Verguts a, * a Department

More information

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory Alejandro Lleras, 1 Ronald A. Rensink, 2 and James T. Enns

More information

Dynamics, models, and mechanisms of the cognitive flexibility of preschoolers van Bers, B.M.C.W.

Dynamics, models, and mechanisms of the cognitive flexibility of preschoolers van Bers, B.M.C.W. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Dynamics, models, and mechanisms of the cognitive flexibility of preschoolers van Bers, B.M.C.W. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): van Bers,

More information

Assessing the Time Course of Stroop and Simon Effects. Michael S. Pratte, Jeffrey N. Rouder, Richard D. Morey, Kathleen E. Wenzlick, and.

Assessing the Time Course of Stroop and Simon Effects. Michael S. Pratte, Jeffrey N. Rouder, Richard D. Morey, Kathleen E. Wenzlick, and. Stroop vs. Simon 1 Running head: STROOP VS. SIMON Assessing the Time Course of Stroop and Simon Effects Michael S. Pratte, Jeffrey N. Rouder, Richard D. Morey, Kathleen E. Wenzlick, and Chuning Feng University

More information

Numbers and Space: A Computational Model of the SNARC Effect

Numbers and Space: A Computational Model of the SNARC Effect Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 1, 32 44 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.32

More information

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (4), 697-702 Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search JAN THEEUWES Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, the Netherlands and ARTHUR F. KRAMER and

More information

Altered time course of unconscious response priming in schizophrenia patients

Altered time course of unconscious response priming in schizophrenia patients Schizophrenia Research 150: 590-1 (2013) Altered time course of unconscious response priming in schizophrenia patients Markus Kiefer 1, Ph.D., Anna Morschett 1, M.D., Carlos Schönfeldt-Lecuona 1, M.D.,

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Standaarden voor kerndoelen basisonderwijs : de ontwikkeling van standaarden voor kerndoelen basisonderwijs op basis van resultaten uit peilingsonderzoek van der

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention

Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:1064 1070 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0172-z BRIEF REPORT Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention Sophie Forster & Nilli Lavie Published online: 12 October

More information

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08 Auditory Perception - Detection versus Discrimination - Localization versus Discrimination - - Electrophysiological Measurements Psychophysical Measurements Three Approaches to Researching Audition physiology

More information

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction Imitation and anticipation 1 It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction Roland Pfister 1, David Dignath 1, Bernhard Hommel 2, & Wilfried Kunde 1 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm

Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm VISUAL COGNITION, 2002, 9 (4/5), 421 457 Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm Peter Wühr and Jochen Müsseler Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research,

More information

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Masters of Science in Psychology By Quan Ying Gao University of Canterbury

More information

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Exp Brain Res (2002) 142:349 353 DOI 10.1007/s00221-001-0899-2 RESEARCH ARTICLE Richard A. Abrams Mark B. Law Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Received: 10 May 2000 / Accepted: 4 September

More information

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08 Auditory Perception - Detection versus Discrimination - Localization versus Discrimination - Electrophysiological Measurements - Psychophysical Measurements 1 Three Approaches to Researching Audition physiology

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments

Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments Robert W. Proctor & Motonori Yamaguchi Purdue University, W. Lafayette, Indiana MURI Grant W9112NF-05-1-0153 from

More information

Auditory Warning Signals Affect Mechanisms of Response Selection

Auditory Warning Signals Affect Mechanisms of Response Selection Research Article Auditory Warning Signals Affect Mechanisms of Response Selection Evidence from a Simon Task Rico Fischer, 1 Franziska Plessow, 1 and Andrea Kiesel 2 1 Technische Universität Dresden, Germany

More information

Emotion. Enhanced Attentional Capture in Trait Anxiety

Emotion. Enhanced Attentional Capture in Trait Anxiety Emotion Enhanced Attentional Capture in Trait Anxiety Jason S. Moser, Mark W. Becker, and Tim P. Moran Online First Publication, November 7, 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0026156 CITATION Moser, J. S., Becker, M.

More information

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2001, 8 (2), 315-323 Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return ANA B. VIVAS City Liberal Studies: Affiliated Institution of the University of Sheffield, Thessaloniki,

More information

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Current Biology, in press Supplemental Information: Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Grigorios Sotiropoulos, Aaron R. Seitz, and Peggy Seriès Supplemental Data Detailed

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Anna SORKIN¹, Avi PELED 2, Daphna WEINSHALL¹ 1 Interdisciplinary Center for Neural Computation, Hebrew University of Jerusalem,

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (2), 327-331 The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR THOMAS M. SPALEK AND VINCENT DI LOLLO Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia,

More information

Acta Psychologica 137 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica. journal homepage:

Acta Psychologica 137 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica. journal homepage: Acta Psychologica 137 (2011) 90 100 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/ locate/actpsy Attention control and the antisaccade task: A response

More information

PSYC20007 READINGS AND NOTES

PSYC20007 READINGS AND NOTES Week 4 Lecture 4 Attention in Space and Time The Psychological Function of Spatial Attention To assign limited-capacity processing resources to relevant stimuli in environment - Must locate stimuli among

More information

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting Ahnate Lim (ahnate@hawaii.edu) Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii at Manoa 2530 Dole Street, Honolulu, HI 96822 USA Scott Sinnett (ssinnett@hawaii.edu)

More information

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task? Bachelor s Thesis Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a sequential movement task? Nils Hatger Prof. Dr. Ing. Willem Verwey Jonathan Barnhoorn, PhD student BMS Faculty/Cognitive

More information

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC) Neural Computation Overview of Lecture Models of prefrontal cortex function Dr. Sam Gilbert Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience University College London E-mail: sam.gilbert@ucl.ac.uk Prefrontal cortex

More information

Masked prime stimuli can bias free choices between response alternatives

Masked prime stimuli can bias free choices between response alternatives Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 463-468 Masked prime stimuli can bias free choices between response alternatives FRIEDERIKE SCHLAGHECKEN University of Warwick, Coventry, England and MARTIN

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task

A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task A Drift Diffusion Model of Proactive and Reactive Control in a Context-Dependent Two-Alternative Forced Choice Task Olga Lositsky lositsky@princeton.edu Robert C. Wilson Department of Psychology University

More information

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan University of Alberta The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm by Shawn Tan A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect VISUAL COGNITION, 2007, 15 (2), 133148 Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect Jay Pratt University of Toronto,

More information

On the role of stimulus response and stimulus stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect

On the role of stimulus response and stimulus stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect Memory & Cognition 2003, 31 (3), 353-359 On the role of stimulus response and stimulus stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect JAN DE HOUWER Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium Stroop effects might be

More information

This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.

This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Treccani B., Cubelli R., Della Sala S., Umiltà, C. (2009). Flanker and Simon

More information

RT distribution analysis

RT distribution analysis Space positional and motion SRC effects: A comparison with the use of reaction time distribution analysis Piotr Styrkowiec 1 and Remigiusz Szczepanowski 2 1 Institute of Psychology, University of Wroclaw,

More information

The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure?

The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure? The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure? Contributed by Bruno Rossion, May 2013 Comparing the matching of same top face halves when they are aligned vs. misaligned with different bottom

More information

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Fundamentals of Psychophysics Fundamentals of Psychophysics John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3045! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Visual neuroscience physiology stimulus How do we see the world? neuroimaging

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Oderkerk, A. E. (1999). De preliminaire fase van het rechtsvergelijkend onderzoek Nijmegen: Ars Aequi Libri

Citation for published version (APA): Oderkerk, A. E. (1999). De preliminaire fase van het rechtsvergelijkend onderzoek Nijmegen: Ars Aequi Libri UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) De preliminaire fase van het rechtsvergelijkend onderzoek Oderkerk, A.E. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Oderkerk, A. E. (1999). De preliminaire

More information