Notes and Comment. Negative priming and multiple repetition: A reply to Grison and Strayer (2001)

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Notes and Comment. Negative priming and multiple repetition: A reply to Grison and Strayer (2001)"

Transcription

1 Perception & Psychophysics 2002, 64 (5), Notes and Comment Negative priming and multiple repetition: A reply to Grison and Strayer (2001) W. TRAMMELL NEILL State University of New York, Albany, New York and STEVE JOORDENS University of Toronto, Scarborough, Ontario, Canada Strayer and colleagues (Grison & Strayer, 2001; Malley & Strayer, 1995; Strayer& Grison, 1999) havereported experiments in which negative priming by ignored stimuli occurredonly for stimuli that were repeatedlysampled from small sets. These results were arguedto be inconsistentwith episodic/mismatch accounts of negative priming. We show here that a dependenceof negative priming on multiple repetition is wholly consistent with such theories. Furthermore, we argue that the inhibitory theory proposed by Strayer and colleagues cannot account for major findings regardingnegative priming and that anomalies in the data reported by Grison and Strayer are more parsimoniously explained by episodic/mismatch accounts. If one has recently attended or responded to an object, responses to a similar object are usually faster than responses to an unrepeated object ( positive priming). On the other hand, if one has recently ignored an object, responses to a similar object are often slower than responses to an unrepeated object (negative priming). The latter effect has emerged as one of the more controversial topics in the study of attention and repetition effects (see reviews by Fox, 1995; May, Kane, & Hasher, 1995; Neill & Mathis, 1998; Neill & Valdes, 1996; Neill, Valdes, & Terry, 1995). Although a variety of theoretical explanations have been proposed for the effect, they generally fall into two classes, described here as inhibitory and mismatch theories. Strayer and colleagues (Grison & Strayer, 2001; Malley & Strayer, 1995; Strayer & Grison, 1999) have reported a series of experiments in which positive priming by target target repetition occurred only if the stimuli were sampled infrequently from a large set. In contrast, negative priming by ignored distractors occurred only if the stimuli were sampled repeatedly from a small set throughoutthe experiment. Strayer and colleaguesargued Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to either W. T. Neill, Department of Psychology,State University of New York, Albany, NY 12222( neill@csc.albany.edu), or S. Joordens, Division of Life Sciences, University of Toronto, 1265 Military Trail, Scarborough, ON, M1C 1A4 Canada ( joordens@psych. utoronto.ca). that the latter result is incompatible with mismatch theories of negative priming and proposed a specific inhibitory theory to account for their data. Here, we provide a brief historical review of this debate and a critique of the theoretical conclusions drawn by Strayer and colleagues and conclude that their results are more parsimoniously explained by mismatch theories. Inhibitory Theories In the first publisheddemonstration of negative priming, Dalrymple-Alford and Budayr (1966)compared total color-naming time for a list of unrelated Stroop (1935) color words with that for a list in which each distractor word named the color of the next color word in the list (e.g., blue written in red ink, followed by yellow in blue ink). Color naming was slower in the latter condition. (Lowe, 1979, and Neill, 1977, replicated this effect in a randomized, discrete-trials version of the Stroop task.) Dalrymple-Alford and Budayr concluded that responding to each color necessitated suppression of the competing response elicited by the distractor word; if the suppressed response was then appropriate to the next color, naming was slowed. Subsequent research cast doubt on the overt response as the locus of suppression. Negative priming is obtained even if the trial in which the distractor is ignored (prime trial) requires a different response mode (vocal or manual) from the trial in which the ignored stimulus reappears as a target (probe trial; Tipper, MacQueen, & Brehaut, 1988). Negative priming also occurs even if ignored and unrelated stimuli require the same responses (e.g., DeSchepper & Treisman, 1996; Neill, Lissner, & Beck, 1990). A purely perceptual locus is unlikely as well, because negative priming occurs between perceptually dissimilar stimuli for example, between the word and the color in the Stroop task and between pictures and associatedwords (Tipper & Driver, 1988). Inhibitorytheories have therefore emphasized a central, cognitiverepresentation of the ignored stimulus. It is commonly assumed that interference by distracting stimuli is caused by activationof their cognitive representations.it is tempting, therefore, to suppose that inhibition reduces such activation.however, the deactivation hypothesis has been disconfirmed by the finding that negative priming often depends on whether the probe trial target is accompanied by a distractor. In some studies, prime trial distractors caused significant facilitation (positive priming) on nonconflict probe trials, even though they were randomly intermixed with conflict probe trials that yielded negative priming (Lowe, 1979; Milliken, Joordens, Merikle, & Seiffert, 1998; Neill & 855 Copyright 2002 Psychonomic Society, Inc.

2 856 NEILL AND JOORDENS Kahan, 1999; Tipper & Cranston, 1985). Therefore, the distractor representation must remain activated, but its manifestation in positive priming depends on probe target conditions. Tipper and Cranston concluded that the activated distractor representation is actively blocked from accessing response mechanisms as long as the subject maintains a selection set. If the probe target is not accompanied by a distractor, the selection set is relinquished, and facilitationemerges owing to the persisting activation. Ironically, more recent inhibitory theories have resurrected the notion that the distractor representation is directly deactivated(grison &Strayer, 2001;Houghton& Tipper, 1994; Malley & Strayer, 1995; Strayer & Grison, 1999). Houghton and Tipper proposed a connectionist model of negative priming in which stimuli present on a trial are compared with an internal template of the critical selection criteria. A mismatch between the distractor object and the template results in a spread of inhibition to represented features of the distractor, resulting in negative priming when that object reappears as a target. Strayer and colleagues proposed that selective attention can either increase activation of the target representation or inhibit activation of the distractor representation. The mode of selection depends on the overall activation levels. When stimuli are presented infrequently, overall activation is low, and selection can be effected through increasing activation of the target representation. However, when stimuli are presented frequently, both target and distractor activations will be near maximum, and so selection can be effected only through deactivationof the distractor representation.consequently, negative priming occurs only if a small set of stimuli are frequently repeated, whereas positive priming by repeated targets occurs only for large sets with infrequent repetition. Mismatch Theories Mismatch theories attribute negative priming to the comparison of the current target stimulus to a remembered recent encounter with a similar stimulus (hence, they are also episodic retrieval theories). Neill and Valdes (1992) suggested that negative priming could be caused by an instance retrieval process similar to that proposed by Logan (1988, 1990) to account for the acquisition of automaticity.as elaborated by Neill, Valdes, Terry, and Gorfein (1992), the episodic retrieval theory assumes that a target stimulus cues the retrieval from memory of recent encounters with similar stimuli. Following Logan (1988, 1990), they argued that if a retrieved instance includes appropriate response information, the subject can bypass slower algorithmic computation of the response. However, if the retrieved instance is one in which the stimulus was ignored, the absence of useful response information (or perhaps an explicit ignore this tag) delays selection of an appropriate response. Critical support for an episodic retrieval process can be found in the dependence of negative priming on contextual similarity between prime and probe trials (Fox & de Fockert, 1998; Neill, 1997). Whereas Neill et al. (1992) emphasized a mismatch of response information, other theories of negative priming have emphasized a mismatch of selection criteria. Park and Kanwisher (1994) proposed such a mismatch theory to account for location-specific negative priming. Tipper, Brehaut, and Driver (1990) had previously demonstrated negative priming for ignored locations in a task requiring subjects to press a key corresponding to the location of a while ignoring a distractor + in another location. Slowed responding to a probe target occurred if it appeared in the same location as the prime trial distractor (see also Neill et al., 1992). In a crucial experiment by Park and Kanwisher, subjects were instructed to locate a target O, ignoring X, on the prime trial. However, on the subsequent probe trial, the subjects had to locate X, ignoring O. Relative to a neutral-location target, responding was faster if the target appeared in the previous distractor location (object match) but was slower if it appeared in the previous target location (object mismatch) MacDonald and Joordens (2000; MacDonald, Joordens, & Seergobin, 1999) demonstrated that the effects of match or mismatch of selection criteria occur in identification tasks as well and that such criteria can be conceptual, rather than strictly perceptual. In one experiment, MacDonald et al. presented subjects with the names of two animals (e.g., mouse dog), requiring them to read aloud the name of the larger animal. Very large ( msec) negative priming was found if the name of the smaller animal reappeared as that of the larger animal on the next trial. Most important for the present discussion, MacDonald and Joordens (2000) found negative priming by the nontarget prime if the same selection criterion (e.g., name the larger animal) was required on both prime and probe trials, but not if selection criteria were switched (e.g., name the larger animal on the prime trial, but name the smaller animal on the probe trial). In the latter case, the selection attribute of the probe target was consistent with the prime trial (e.g., both smaller). And similar to Park and Kanwisher (1994), negative priming was caused by target target repetitionswhen the selection criteria were switched (e.g., if the larger animal named on the prime trial was the smaller animal named on the probe trial). Neither inhibitory theories of negative priming nor the episodic retrieval theory proposed by Neill et al. (1992) can account for negative priming by directly attended stimuli, as in target target repetitions (see also Wood & Milliken, 1998). In view of such results, Neill and Mathis (1998) proposed a modified episodic retrieval theory in which instance retrieval tends to reinstate similar processing (cf. Kolers, 1976; Kolers & Ostry, 1974). Negative priming occurs if the reinstated processing is transfer inappropriate to current task demands. A different but related approach is taken by the temporal discriminability theory proposed by Milliken

3 NOTES AND COMMENT 857 et al. (1998). According to this theory, recently ignored stimuli are often intermediate in familiarity, relative to recently attended stimuli (highly familiar) or less recently encountered stimuli (unfamiliar). This creates an ambiguity as to how to process the stimuli for example, whether or not to rely on episodic retrieval. Under some conditions (such as delay or change in task context), attended stimuli may similarly result in intermediate familiarity and may also cause negative priming. As was noted above, negative priming often depends on the presence of a distractor stimuluson the probe trial and may reverse to positive priming in the absence of interference. Similar to Tipper and Cranston (1985), mismatch theories can account for this result because they attribute negative priming to a later stage of processing than that at which cognitive activation is assumed to occur. Hence, if the processes necessary for mismatch effects fail to occur, performance should benefit from persisting activation. Neill (1997) demonstrated that the presence of a distractor on a prime trial, but not on the probe trial, can cause a contextualmismatch that reduces the likelihood of retrieving the prime-trial episode. In addition, algorithmic computation is likely to be faster for probe targets without distractors, reducing the opportunity for episodic retrieval to affect performance (cf. Logan, 1988, 1990). The temporal discriminability theory (Milliken et al., 1998) supposes that presence or absence of a distractor affects whether the temporal discrimination process is engaged; in the absence of interference, performance improves monotonically with the familiarity (activation) of the target. Effects of Multiple Repetition Over a set of experiments in which subjects named target words flanked by distractor words, Malley and Strayer (1995) found significantnegativeprimingby ignored distractors only when words were repeatedly sampled from a set of 16 words. In contrast, they found significant positive priming for target target repetitions only when new words were sampled for each prime probe trial pair. They argued that these results were incompatiblewith the episodic retrieval theory of negative priming, reasoning as follows. Over the course of the experiment, a multiply repeated stimulus would appear as an attended target or an ignored distractor with roughly equal frequency. To the extent that the current target cues the retrieval of past episodes involving that word, the retrieval of episodes in which the word was attended should counteract episodes in which that word was ignored. In contrast, if that word had appeared only once before, as an ignored distractor, that episode should exclusively affect performance. In accordance with this logic, the results of Strayer and Grison (1999) would appear even more damaging to the episodic retrieval theory: Their Experiment 3 showed an increase in negative priming with an increase in the number of prior repetitions as a target, but not with an increase in the number of repetitions as a distractor. In addition, Strayer and Grison were unable to replicate negative priming in a task requiring matching of completely novel nonsense shapes (DeSchepper & Treisman, 1996; Treisman & DeSchepper, 1996). DeSchepper and Treisman argued that negative priming for such figures had to reflect retrieval of specific perceptual instances, because there could be no preexisting cognitive representation to inhibit (see the further discussion by Neill, 1997; Neill & Mathis, 1998). Grison and Strayer (2001) expanded on these results in word-naming experiments in which either the prime trial target or the distractor was perceptually degraded. As in previous studies (Malley & Strayer, 1995;Strayer & Grison, 1999), they also varied whether the stimuli were sampled repeatedly or were unique to each prime probe trial pair. Again, positive priming for target target repetitions occurred only for novel words, whereas negative priming occurred only for multiply repeated words. Furthermore, positive priming was reduced if the target was degraded, whereas negative priming was reduced if the distractor was degraded. In addition to reiterating their arguments against the episodic retrieval theory, they argued that their results were also inconsistent with the connectionist model proposed by Houghtonand Tipper (1994). This model assumes that inhibition is proportional to distractor activation;on the assumption that an undegraded distractor should produce greater activation, it should also cause greater negative priming. Although this did occur for multiply repeated words, there was no effect for novelwords (at least in the reaction time measure). Critique Strayer and colleagues ignored critical evidence that cognitive activation and the mechanism underlying negative priming affect different stages of processing. As was discussed above, negative priming is often dependent on the presence or absence of a distractor on the probe trial, and a prime trial distractor may cause facilitation if it reappears as a probe target without a distractor (Lowe, 1979; Milliken et al., 1998; Neill & Kahan, 1999; Tipper & Cranston, 1985). Because the theory proposed by Strayer and colleagues attributes negative priming entirely to deactivationon the prime trial, it cannot account for a reversal of negative priming to positive priming when the probe trial does not include a distractor. Furthermore, any theory that does attribute activation and negative priming to different stages of processing as do mismatch theories, as well as the blocking theory proposed by Tipper and Cranston easily accounts for the effects of multiple repetition, as will be shown in the following. Let us subdivide processing of a probe trial target into those processes leading to categorization of the stimulus and postcategorical processes ultimately leading to a response. (We refer to these sets of processes as stages, but by no means do we imply that these stages cannot be decomposed into multiple distinguishable stages.) Let

4 858 NEILL AND JOORDENS C = total time for the categorization stage and P 5 total time for the postcategorical stage. Reaction time (RT), therefore, is C 1 P. Let us also define subscripts IR and UR to represent the conditions ignored repetition (distractor-to-target repetition) and unrelated (no repetition between prime and probe trials), respectively. Negative priming (NP) is defined as RT IR 2 RT UR. Therefore. NP 5 (C IR 1 P IR ) 2 (C UR 1 P UR ) 5 (C IR 2 C UR ) 1 (P IR 2 P UR ). We agree completely with Strayer and colleaguesthat multiple repetition probably pushes the activation levels of both IR and UR conditions to some maximum. Thus, on the assumption that identification time depends on activation,c IR is approximatelyequal to C UR. Therefore, approximately,np 5 P IR 2 P UR. With the exceptions of Houghton and Tipper (1994) and Strayer and colleagues, most theories of negative priming assume that the mechanism of negative priming operates at a stage after the probe trial target has been categorized that is, P IR is slowed relative to P UR. Therefore, negative priming is expected to occur. What if stimuli are not multiply repeated? The recent presentation of a stimulus is likely to substantially increase activation, and so C IR, C UR. The magnitude of negative priming will depend on the relative size of this effect. Increased facilitation of the categorization stage will necessarily attenuate negative priming. Indeed, if the facilitation of categorization is greater than the impediment of postcategorical processing, an ignored distractor should cause positive priming that is, NP 5 (C IR 2 C UR ) 1 (P IR 2 P UR ), 0. More generally, activation by the ignored distractor will tend to counteract the mechanism of negative priming, and the effect of activation should vary inversely with presentation frequency. It should be emphasized that this analysis follows regardless of what causes negative priming. That is, the postcategorical processes (P) might be slowed by inhibition, episodic retrieval, mismatching, temporal discriminability, or anythingelse. All that is assumed is that ignored distractors can have opposite effects on processes leading to categorization (C) and postcategorical processes (P). Although it may seem gratuitous to postulate opposing processes, this assumption is mandated empirically by the fact that a prime trial distractor can cause either positive priming or negative priming, depending on whether the probe trial target is also accompanied by a distractor (Lowe, 1979; Milliken et al., 1998; Neill & Kahan, 1999; Tipper & Cranston, 1985). This analysis also accounts for the finding by Strayer and Grison (1999, Experiment 3) that negative priming was greater for stimuli multiply repeated as targets than for stimuli multiply repeated as distractors. On the assumption that activation levels for stimuli repeated as targets should be higher than those for stimuli repeated as distractors, the former would be closer to asymptotic. Therefore, C IR 2 C UR should more closely approximate zero, allowing the postcategorical component P to predominate. As was acknowledged by Grison and Strayer (2001), episodic retrieval accounts easily for the finding that negative priming is reduced if the prime trial distractor is degraded:if the distractoridentityis less well encoded, it is less likely to be retrieved on the probe trial. However, Grison and Strayer argue that episodic retrieval cannot account for the effect of degradation on negative priming only for multiply repeated words; they expect that a similar effect should occur for novel words. But this prediction follows only if it is assumed that the probability of episodic retrieval is the same for the multiple-repetition and novel conditions. Although the formal analysis above emphasized the effect of multiple repetitionon categorization, it seems likely that multiple repetition should also affect postcategorical processes. It is not implausible, for example, that a novel (less familiar) word would be a less effective retrieval cue than a multiply repeated (more familiar) word. It is inherent in the instance retrieval theory put forth by Logan (1988, 1990) that the effect of episodic retrieval will also depend on the time taken for algorithmic computation. If algorithmic computation is speeded for novel targets, because they cause a greater increase in activation relative to baseline, episodic retrieval is less likely to occur. Consequently, degradation of the prime trial distractor would be less likely to have any effect on performance. Furthermore, in the design used by Strayer and colleagues, the prime-distractor/probe-target frequency is inherently confounded with probe distractor frequency (and prime target frequency) that is, all the words in a prime2probe pair are selected from the same size pool. Does an unfamiliar probe trial distractor cause less interference than a familiar probe trial distractor? If so, algorithmicprocessing might again be more efficient. We realize that in describing how mismatch theories can account for the results of Grison and Strayer (2001), the reader might be left with the inaccurate impression that the increased flexibility provided by our notions of positive versus negative priming would allow mismatch theories to account for any pattern and, therefore, to predict none. Why would such an impression be inaccurate? Our argument is simply that if positive-priming effects have not been saturated, the positive-priming effect occurring at the identification stage will mask the negative-priming effect that occurs at later stages. This is a clear and concrete claim, and we currently know a great deal about positivepriming effects. For example, we know that priming effects are larger for degraded targets (Meyer, Schvaneveldt, & Ruddy, 1975) and for low-frequency targets (e.g., Becker, 1979; Becker & Killion, 1977). Thus, in a context in which positive priming has not yet been saturated, we would predict less negative priming for targets that are more sensitive to positive priming (i.e., degraded, lowfrequency, or degraded low-frequency targets). Clearly, this is a testable prediction of our account.

5 NOTES AND COMMENT 859 Finally, we note two anomalies in the data reported by Grison and Strayer (2001) thatare more easilyaccounted for by mismatch theories than by their inhibitory theory. First, in their Experiment 1, error rates yielded a significant negative-priming effect for ignored novel words in the degraded-target (undegraded-distractor) condition; this reversed to significant positive priming in the degraded-distractor (undegraded-target) condition.this effect actually belies two conclusions by Grison and Strayer. First, it is untrue that negative priming did not occur for novel words. Second, it is untrue that degradation did not have similar qualitative effects on negative priming for novel and repeated words. The effects were manifested in error rates, rather than in reaction times. Although negative priming is most often demonstrated in reaction time, there is no a priori justification for ignoring similar effects on accuracy. A second anomaly in Experiment 1 of Grison and Strayer (2001) is thatreactiontime in the attendedrepetition condition yielded significant negative priming for multiplyrepeated words in the degraded-targetcondition. Although the inhibitory theory proposed by Strayer and colleagues predicts little or no positive priming for multiply repeated words, it cannotaccount for negative priming in target2target repetitions. However, this effect is easily accommodated by mismatch theories, insofar as a mismatch between a degraded prime target and a undegraded probe target may cause interference. The same condition did not produce negative priming in their Experiment 2, although the degree of degradation was increased. However, the latter experiment generally showed larger positive priming and smaller negative priming than did the first experiment. In terms of the formal analysis above, Experiment 2 appears to have a greater contribution of the C component,consistent with the reduced number of repetitions in that experiment. Conclusions To summarize, the experiments reported by Strayer and colleagues (Grison & Strayer, 2001; Malley & Strayer, 1995;Strayer & Grison, 1999) do not adequately resolve whether negative priming is caused by inhibition, episodic retrieval, or some other mechanism. For the most part, all that is required to account for the dependence of negative priming on multiple repetition is the assumption that an ignored distractor can facilitate one stage of processing for a subsequent target but can impede another stage. Hence, the magnitude of overt negative or positive priming necessarily depends on the balance of facilitatory and dilatory effects. Although this assumption may seem theoretically cumbersome, it is mandated by the dependenceof priming effects on probe trial conditions,as well as on prime trial conditions.and it is this frequently replicated effect that ultimately undermines the theory proposed by Strayer and colleagues. The theories proposed by Strayer and colleagues and by Houghton and Tipper (1994) are arguably the most fully articulated inhibitory theories of negative priming. Grison and Strayer (2001) assert that the latter theory cannot account for their data. Yet the theory proposed by Strayer and colleagues cannot account for a major attribute of negative priming that it can be reversed to positive by nonconflict probes. Their theory also cannot account for anomalies in their own reported data. Given that mismatch theories can account for the data of Strayer and colleagues, as well as for the dependence of negative priming on probe trial conditions, we can only conclude that the data of Strayer and colleagues contribute further support to mismatch theories. To readers not immersed in the negative priming literature, this debate may seem akin to that concerning how many angels can dance on the head of a pin. However, the issue has important consequences beyond abstract theory: Many studies have found that negative priming is diminished in various groups characterized by cognitive impairments for example, the elderly, children, schizophrenics, poor readers, and error-prone individuals(see reviews by May et al., 1995; Neill et al., 1995). Proponents of the inhibitory view have leapt to the conclusion that such groups are deficient in the ability to inhibit distracting information and that this, in turn, accounts for their impairments. However, if negative priming does not index inhibition but, rather, some process like memory retrieval, the interpretation (and possible remedy) of cognitive impairments shown by such groups clearly requires reevaluation. REFERENCES Becker,C. A. (1979). Semantic context and word frequency effects in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 4, Becker, C. A., & Killion, T. H. (1977). Interaction of visual and cognitive effects in word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, Dalrymple-Alford, E. C., & Budayr, D. (1966). Examination of some aspects of the Stroop color word test. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 23, DeSchepper, B., & Treisman, A. (1996). Visual memory for novel shapes: Implicit coding without attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 22, Fox, E. (1995). Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, Fox, E., & de Fockert, J. W. (1998). Negative priming depends on prime probe similarity: Evidence for episodic retrieval. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, Grison, S., & Strayer, D. L. (2001).Negative priming and perceptual fluency: More than what meets the eye. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, Houghton, G., & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp ). San Diego: Academic Press. Kolers, P. A. (1976). Reading a year later. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 2, Kolers, P. A., & Ostry, D. J. (1974). Time course of loss of information regarding pattern analyzing operations. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 13, Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, Logan, G. D. (1990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? Cognitive Psychology, 7, Lowe, D. G. (1979). Strategies, context, and the mechanism of response inhibition. Memory & Cognition, 7, MacDonald, P. A., & Joordens, S. (2000). Investigating a memory-

6 860 NEILL AND JOORDENS based account of negative priming: Support for selection-feature mismatch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, MacDonald, P. A., Joordens, S., & Seergobin,K. N. (1999). Negative priming effects that are bigger than a breadbox: Attending to distractors does not eliminate negative priming, it enhances it. Memory & Cognition, 27, Malley, G. B., & Strayer, D. L. (1995).Effect of stimulus repetition on positive and negative identity priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, May, C. P., Kane, M. J., & Hasher, L. (1995). Determinants of negative priming. Psychological Bulletin, 118, Meyer, D. E., Schvaneveldt,R. W., & Ruddy, M. G. (1975). Loci of context effects in visual word recognition. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and Performance V (pp ). New York: Academic Press. Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P., & Seiffert, A. (1998). Selective attention: A re-evaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychological Review, 105, Neill, W. T. (1977). Inhibitory and facilitatory processes in selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 3, Neill, W. T. (1979). Switching attention within and between categories: Evidence for intracategory inhibition.memory & Cognition, 7, Neill, W. T. (1997). Episodicretrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 23, Neill, W. T., & Kahan, T. A. (1999).Response conflict reverses priming: A replication. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, Neill, W. T., Lissner, L. S., & Beck, J. L. (1990). Negative priming in same different matching: Further evidence for a central locus of inhibition. Perception & Psychophysics, 48, Neill, W. T., & Mathis, K. M. (1998). Transfer-inappropriate processing: Negative priming and related phenomena. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 38, pp. 1-44). San Diego: Academic Press. Neill, W. T., & Valdes, L. A. (1992). The persistence of negative priming: Steady-state or decay? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, Neill, W. T., & Valdes, L. A. (1996). Facilitatory and inhibitory aspects of attention. In A. F. Kramer, M. G. H. Coles, & G. D. Logan (Eds.), Converging operationsin the study of visual selective attention (pp ). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., & Terry, K. M. (1995). Selective attention and the inhibitory control of cognition. In F. N. Dempster & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp ). San Diego: Academic Press. Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). The persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodictrace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, Park, J., & Kanwisher, N. (1994). Negative priming for spatial locations: Identity mismatching,not distractor inhibition.journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, Strayer,D. L., & Grison, S. (1999).Negative identity priming is contingent on stimulus repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 25, Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, Tipper, S. P., Brehaut, J. C., & Driver, J. (1990). Selection of moving and static objects for the control of spatially directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 16, Tipper, S. P., & Cranston, M. (1985). Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignored primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, Tipper, S. P., & Driver, J. (1988). Negative priming between pictures and words in a selective attention task: Evidence for semantic processing of ignored stimuli. Memory & Cognition, 16, Tipper, S. P., MacQueen, G. M., & Brehaut, J. C. (1988). Negative priming between response modalities: Evidence for the central locus of inhibitionin selective attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 43, Treisman, A. M., & DeSchepper, B. (1996). Object tokens, attention, and visual memory. In T. Inui & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Attention and performance XVI: Information integration in perception and communication (pp ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, Bradford Books. Wood, T. J., & Milliken, B. (1998). Negative priming without ignoring. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, (Manuscript received December 31, 2000; accepted for publication September 18, 2001.)

Negative priming and perceptual fluency: More than what meets the eye

Negative priming and perceptual fluency: More than what meets the eye Perception & Psychophysics 2001, 63 (6), 1063-1071 Negative priming and perceptual fluency: More than what meets the eye SARAH GRISON University of Wales, Banger, Wales and DAVID L. STRAYER University

More information

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (3), 504-513 Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks Christian Frings Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany and Peter Wühr Friedrich

More information

Negative Priming as a Memory Phenomenon

Negative Priming as a Memory Phenomenon Zeitschrift S. Mayr & für A. Psychologie Buchner: / Negative JournalofPriming Psychology 2007as Hogrefe a 2007; Memory & Vol. Huber Phenomenon 215(1):35 51 Publishers Negative Priming as a Memory Phenomenon

More information

Running head: NP: ENCODING AND RETRIEVAL SPECIFIC EFFECTS. Response Retrieval and Negative Priming: Encoding and Retrieval Specific Effects

Running head: NP: ENCODING AND RETRIEVAL SPECIFIC EFFECTS. Response Retrieval and Negative Priming: Encoding and Retrieval Specific Effects NP: Encoding and Retrieval Specific Effects 1 Running head: NP: ENCODING AND RETRIEVAL SPECIFIC EFFECTS Response Retrieval and Negative Priming: Encoding and Retrieval Specific Effects Matthias Ihrke Max-Planck-Institute

More information

The effect of asymmetrical association on positive and negative semantic priming

The effect of asymmetrical association on positive and negative semantic priming Memory & Cognition 2002, 30 (8), 1263-1276 The effect of asymmetrical association on positive and negative semantic priming KEITH A. HUTCHISON State University of New York, Albany, New York One s actively

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Effects of Contextual Similarity and Target-Repetition Proportion on Negative Priming in RT Distributional Analyses Chi-Shing Tse, Keith

More information

priming disappear, while stays intact? Why does long-term positive long-term negative priming

priming disappear, while stays intact? Why does long-term positive long-term negative priming By Sandila Tanveer Dr Ewald Neumann Department of Psychology University of Canterbury, NZ Why does long-term positive priming disappear, while long-term negative priming stays intact? Priming Actions are

More information

Long and Short-term Negative and Positive Priming

Long and Short-term Negative and Positive Priming Submitted for publication 19 August 2005 Do not quote without permission c 2004, 2005 by Michael A. Erickson $Id: neg-prime.tex,v 2.0 2005/08/19 17:11:21 erickson Exp $ Long and Short-term Negative and

More information

Perceptron Example: Computational model for the Stroop Task. Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio and Brain Engineering, KAIST

Perceptron Example: Computational model for the Stroop Task. Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio and Brain Engineering, KAIST Perceptron Example: Computational model for the Stroop Task Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio and Brain Engineering, KAIST GREEN YELLOW BLUE BLUE YELLOW GREEN BLUE RED The Stroop Task Can Teach Us

More information

Unifying Negative Priming Theories

Unifying Negative Priming Theories Unifying Negative Priming Theories Hecke Schrobsdorff,2, Matthias Ihrke,3, Jörg Behrendt,3, Marcus Hasselhorn,3, Jan Michael Herrmann,2 hecke@nld.ds.mpg.de Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience

More information

Processing of attended and ignored words in the parafovea: Inhibitory aspects of semantic processing

Processing of attended and ignored words in the parafovea: Inhibitory aspects of semantic processing Psicológica (2000), 21, 233-257 Processing of attended and ignored words in the parafovea: Inhibitory aspects of semantic processing Juan Lupiáñez *, María Rosario Rueda, María Ruz and Pío Tudela Universidad

More information

Visual Selective Attention and the Effects of Monetary Rewards Chiara Della Libera and Leonardo Chelazzi

Visual Selective Attention and the Effects of Monetary Rewards Chiara Della Libera and Leonardo Chelazzi PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Visual Selective Attention and the Effects of Monetary Rewards Chiara Della Libera and Leonardo Chelazzi Department of Neurological and Vision Sciences, Section of

More information

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 3, 580 584 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027885 OBSERVATION Templates

More information

Negative priming 1985 to 2015: a measure of inhibition, the emergence of alternative accounts, and the multiple process challenge

Negative priming 1985 to 2015: a measure of inhibition, the emergence of alternative accounts, and the multiple process challenge THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2016 Vol. 69, No. 10, 1890 1909, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1173077 Negative priming 1985 to 2015: a measure of inhibition, the emergence

More information

Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review

Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1995,2(2), /45-173 Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review ELAINE FOX University ofessex, Colchester, England Ignoring a distractor on a prime

More information

Cognitive Control as Alternation of Activation and Suppression in the Stroop Task

Cognitive Control as Alternation of Activation and Suppression in the Stroop Task Cognitive Control as Alternation of Activation and Suppression in the Stroop Task Ion Juvina (ijuvina@andrew.cmu.edu) Niels A. Taatgen (taatgen@cmu.edu) Daniel Dickison (danieldickison@cmu.edu) Department

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

Contextual control over task-set retrieval

Contextual control over task-set retrieval Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (8), 2047-2053 doi:10.3758/app.72.8.2047 BRIEF REPORTS Contextual control over task-set retrieval MATTHEW J. C. CRUMP AND GORDON D. LOGAN Vanderbilt University,

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

Repetition Blindness, Forward and Backward

Repetition Blindness, Forward and Backward Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 1, 137 149 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.1.137

More information

Probe distractors can influence negative priming by perceptual grouping

Probe distractors can influence negative priming by perceptual grouping Perception & Psychophysics 2004, 66 (2), 208-218 Probe distractors can influence negative priming by perceptual grouping YEI-YU YEH and HSUAN-FU CHAO National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan Negative

More information

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 11 : 9 September 2011 ISSN

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 11 : 9 September 2011 ISSN LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume ISSN 1930-2940 Managing Editor: M. S. Thirumalai, Ph.D. Editors: B. Mallikarjun, Ph.D. Sam Mohanlal, Ph.D. B. A. Sharada, Ph.D.

More information

Prime Retrieval of Motor Responses in Negative Priming

Prime Retrieval of Motor Responses in Negative Priming Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 2, 408 423 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.35.2.408 Prime Retrieval

More information

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Masters of Science in Psychology By Quan Ying Gao University of Canterbury

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2008, 61 (4), 552 557 Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them Jeffrey S. Bowers and Keely W. Jones University of Bristol, Bristol,

More information

A Simulation of the Activation- Selection Model of Meaning. Gorfein, D.S. & Brown, V.R.

A Simulation of the Activation- Selection Model of Meaning. Gorfein, D.S. & Brown, V.R. A Simulation of the Activation- Selection Model of Meaning Gorfein, D.S. & Brown, V.R. Abstract The activation-selection model of determining the meaning of an ambiguous word or phrase (Gorfein, 2001)

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SESORY COSTRAIS? TOWARD A UIFIED THEORY OF GEERAL TASK DIFFICULTY Hanna Benoni and Yehoshua Tsal Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University hannaben@post.tau.ac.il

More information

Target localization among concurrent sound sources: No evidence for the inhibition of previous distractor responses

Target localization among concurrent sound sources: No evidence for the inhibition of previous distractor responses Atten Percept Psychophys (2013) 75:132 144 DOI 10.3758/s13414-012-0380-2 Target localization among concurrent sound sources: No evidence for the inhibition of previous distractor responses Malte Möller

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

Object-Based Perceptual Grouping Affects Negative Priming

Object-Based Perceptual Grouping Affects Negative Priming Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Human Perception and Performance 0096-1523/98/53.00 1998, Vol. 24, No. 2, 664-672 Object-Based Perceptual

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Surface Segmentation Cues Influence Negative Priming for Novel and Familiar Shapes

Surface Segmentation Cues Influence Negative Priming for Novel and Familiar Shapes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. l.,eaming, Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.4.929 2000, Vol. 26, No.

More information

Effect of stimulus repetition on positive and negative identity priming

Effect of stimulus repetition on positive and negative identity priming Perception & Psychophysics 1995,57 (5), 657-667 Effect of stimulus repetition on positive and negative identity priming GREGORY B. MALLEY and DAVID L. STRAYER University ofutah, Salt Lake City, Utah Most

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load Psychonomic Bulletin & Review In press Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load JOSHUA D. COSMAN AND SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Attentional capture by abrupt onsets can be

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psych.colorado.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2009, 16 (1), 57-61 doi:10.3758/pbr.16.1.57 The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words DANIEL SMILEK, GRAYDEN J. F. SOLMAN, PETER MURAWSKI, AND

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

The spacing and lag effect in free recall The spacing and lag effect in free recall Michael J. Kahana, Bradley R. Wellington & Marc W. Howard Center for Complex Systems and Department of Psychology Brandeis University Send correspondence to: Michael

More information

Intentional versus unintentional use of contingencies between perceptual events

Intentional versus unintentional use of contingencies between perceptual events University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 1996 Intentional versus unintentional use of contingencies

More information

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2001, 8 (2), 315-323 Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return ANA B. VIVAS City Liberal Studies: Affiliated Institution of the University of Sheffield, Thessaloniki,

More information

Investigating the Temporal Course of Attentional Processing A Test of the Response-Retrieval Account of Negative Priming

Investigating the Temporal Course of Attentional Processing A Test of the Response-Retrieval Account of Negative Priming Investigating the Temporal Course of Attentional Processing A Test of the Response-Retrieval Account of Negative Priming Matthias Ihrke 1,3 Jörg Behrendt 1,2 Hecke Schrobsdorff 1,3 Michael Herrman 1,3

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Using real-world scenes as contextual cues for search

Using real-world scenes as contextual cues for search VISUAL COGNITION, 2006, 13 1), 99±108 Using real-world scenes as contextual cues for search James R. Brockmole and John M. Henderson Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science Program, Michigan State

More information

Short article Attention dependency in implicit learning of repeated search context

Short article Attention dependency in implicit learning of repeated search context THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 0000, 00 (0), 1 8 Short article Attention dependency in implicit learning of repeated search context Valeria Rausei Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA,

More information

Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect

Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect Q. Gao, Z. Chen, P. Russell Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect Quanying Gao, Zhe Chen, & Paul Russell University of Canterbury Although the effect of working memory (WM) load on the

More information

The Adaptive Character of the Attentional System: Statistical Sensitivity in a Target Localization Task

The Adaptive Character of the Attentional System: Statistical Sensitivity in a Target Localization Task Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2003, Vol. 29, No. 3, 631 649 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.3.631

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by:[cdl Journals Account] [CDL Journals Account] On: 24 May 2007 Access Details: [subscription number 770849126] Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England

More information

Finding Memory in Search: The Effect of Visual Working Memory Load on Visual Search. 1 Department of Psychology, University of Toronto

Finding Memory in Search: The Effect of Visual Working Memory Load on Visual Search. 1 Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Finding Memory 1 Running head: FINDING MEMORY IN SEARCH This is a preprint of an article submitted for consideration in the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 2010 Experimental Psychology Society;

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Why It Is too Early to Lose Control in Accounts of Item-Specific Proportion Congruency Effects Julie M. Bugg, Larry L. Jacoby, and Swati

More information

Center-Surround or Spreading Inhibition: Which Mechanism Caused The. Negative Effect From Repeated Masked Semantic Primes?

Center-Surround or Spreading Inhibition: Which Mechanism Caused The. Negative Effect From Repeated Masked Semantic Primes? Repeated Masked Semantic Priming 1 RUNNING HEAD: Negative Masked Semantic Priming Center-Surround or Spreading Inhibition: Which Mechanism Caused The Negative Effect From Repeated Masked Semantic Primes?

More information

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY Thomas Lachmann *, Gunjan Khera * and Cees van Leeuwen # * Psychology II, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany # Laboratory

More information

INHIBITORY DEFICITS IN RUMINATION: A NEGATIVE PRIMING STUDY

INHIBITORY DEFICITS IN RUMINATION: A NEGATIVE PRIMING STUDY INHIBITORY DEFICITS IN RUMINATION: A NEGATIVE PRIMING STUDY A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Master of Science in Psychology Caitlin Leigh Aberhart Department

More information

Five shades of grey: Generalization in distractor-based retrieval of S-R episodes

Five shades of grey: Generalization in distractor-based retrieval of S-R episodes Atten Percept Psychophys (2016) 78:2307 2312 DOI 10.3758/s13414-016-1210-8 SHORT REPORT Five shades of grey: Generalization in distractor-based retrieval of S-R episodes Tarini Singh 1 & Birte Moeller

More information

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Automatic detection - 1 Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1986, 24 (6), 431-434 SIU L. CHOW The University of Wollongong,

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

Task Switching. Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008

Task Switching. Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008 Task Switching Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008 Monsell, 2003 Task Switching Basic phenomena Reaction time (RT) cost for switching tasks Higher error rate Diminished if prepared for switch Slower RT

More information

When the visual format of the color carrier word does and does not modulate the Stroop effect

When the visual format of the color carrier word does and does not modulate the Stroop effect Memory & Cognition 2005, 33 (8), 1337-1344 When the visual format of the color carrier word does and does not modulate the Stroop effect CHRIS BLAIS and DEREK BESNER University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario,

More information

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 916-920 Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion JOHN PREDEBON University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia Two experiments

More information

Priming of Pop-out provides reliable measures of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention q

Priming of Pop-out provides reliable measures of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Vision Research 48 (2008) 30 41 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Priming of Pop-out provides reliable measures of target activation and distractor inhibition in

More information

Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control

Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2011, 64 (6), 1048 1055 Rapid communication Integrating working memory capacity and context-processing views of cognitive control Thomas S. Redick and Randall

More information

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005,?? 12 (?), (5),???-??? 852-857 BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval LARRY L. JACOBY, YUJIRO SHIMIZU,

More information

Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences

Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 970-976 Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences Darryl W. Schneider Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee Exploring the hierarchical control relationship

More information

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction Tracey Miser (miser.4@osu.edu) Department of Psychology, 1835 Neil Avenue Columbus, OH43210 USA Vladimir Sloutsky (sloutsky.1@osu.edu)

More information

Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck

Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 729-734 Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck PAUL E. DUX AND RENÉ MAROIS Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee The attentional blink

More information

A strategy disruption component to retrieval-induced forgetting

A strategy disruption component to retrieval-induced forgetting Memory & Cognition 2006, 34 (1), 102-111 A strategy disruption component to retrieval-induced forgetting MICHAEL D. DODD University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada ALAN D. CASTEL Washington University,

More information

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Anna V. Fisher (fisher.449@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University

More information

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Nart Bedin Atalay (natalay@selcuk.edu.tr) Selcuk University, Konya, TURKEY Mine Misirlisoy

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

Word-Based Grouping Affects the Prime-Task Effect on Semantic Priming

Word-Based Grouping Affects the Prime-Task Effect on Semantic Priming Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2000, Vol. 26, No. 2,469-479 Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.26.2.469

More information

Selective Attention and the Suppression of Cognitive Noise

Selective Attention and the Suppression of Cognitive Noise Journal or Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1987, Vol. 13, No. 2, 327-334 Copyright 1987by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/87/$00.75 Selective Attention and

More information

Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments

Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments Robert W. Proctor & Motonori Yamaguchi Purdue University, W. Lafayette, Indiana MURI Grant W9112NF-05-1-0153 from

More information

The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the Rescorla-Wagner Predictions: Relative Validity

The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the Rescorla-Wagner Predictions: Relative Validity Methods of Psychological Research Online 4, Vol. 9, No. Internet: http://www.mpr-online.de Fachbereich Psychologie 4 Universität Koblenz-Landau The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the

More information

Perceptual grouping determines the locus of attentional selection

Perceptual grouping determines the locus of attentional selection 1506 OPAM 2010 REPORT when finding an additional target was more likely. This was observed both within participants (with a main effect of number of targets found), and between groups (with an interaction

More information

Episodic retrieval processes take place automatically in auditory negative priming

Episodic retrieval processes take place automatically in auditory negative priming EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2010, 22 (8), 11921221 Episodic retrieval processes take place automatically in auditory negative priming Susanne Mayr and Axel Buchner Heinrich-Heine-Universität

More information

On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink

On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 723-728 On the failure of distractor inhibition in the attentional blink Pau l E. Dux Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee and Irina M. Harris University

More information

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1994, 47A (1) 201-205 Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing Harold Pashler University of

More information

Learning, Memory, and Cognition /91/$ , Vol. 17, No. l, Age and Inhibition. Bart Rypma Duke University

Learning, Memory, and Cognition /91/$ , Vol. 17, No. l, Age and Inhibition. Bart Rypma Duke University Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1991 by the American PSychological Association+ Inc. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/91/$3.00 1991, Vol. 17, No. l, 163-169 Age and Inhibition Lynn

More information

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (4), 697-702 Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search JAN THEEUWES Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, the Netherlands and ARTHUR F. KRAMER and

More information

Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing

Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing Nooraini Yusoff 1, André Grüning 1 and Antony Browne 1 1 Department of Computing, Faculty of Engineering and Physical

More information

DRAFT: 9/27/01 Please do not circulate or quote without permission of the authors

DRAFT: 9/27/01 Please do not circulate or quote without permission of the authors DRAFT: 9/27/01 Please do not circulate or quote without permission of the authors Automaticity, Attention and the Strength of Processing: A Parallel Distributed Processing Account of the Stroop Effect

More information

Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance

Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance Vencislav Popov (vencislav.popov@gmail.com) Lynne Reder (reder@cmu.edu) Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh,

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1 INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1 5.1 Clinical Interviews: Background Information The clinical interview is a technique pioneered by Jean Piaget, in 1975,

More information

The Effect of Target Repetition on Semantic Priming in a Three-Target RSVP Task

The Effect of Target Repetition on Semantic Priming in a Three-Target RSVP Task International Journal of Psychological Studies; Vol. 7, No. 3; 2015 ISSN 1918-7211 E-ISSN 1918-722X Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education The Effect of Target Repetition on Semantic Priming

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

PSYC20007 READINGS AND NOTES

PSYC20007 READINGS AND NOTES Week 4 Lecture 4 Attention in Space and Time The Psychological Function of Spatial Attention To assign limited-capacity processing resources to relevant stimuli in environment - Must locate stimuli among

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

Alternative Explanations for Changes in Similarity Judgments and MDS Structure

Alternative Explanations for Changes in Similarity Judgments and MDS Structure Cornell University School of Hotel Administration The Scholarly Commons Articles and Chapters School of Hotel Administration Collection 8-1990 Alternative Explanations for Changes in Similarity Judgments

More information

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (2), 327-331 The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR THOMAS M. SPALEK AND VINCENT DI LOLLO Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia,

More information

Running head: CONTINGENCY AND CONFLICT ADAPTATION 1

Running head: CONTINGENCY AND CONFLICT ADAPTATION 1 Running head: CONTINGENCY AND CONFLICT ADAPTATION 1 The Parallel Episodic Processing (PEP) Model: Dissociating Contingency and Conflict Adaptation in the Item-Specific Proportion Congruent Paradigm James

More information

Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment Hypothesis: Comment on Belli and Reply to Tversky and Tuchin

Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment Hypothesis: Comment on Belli and Reply to Tversky and Tuchin Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1989, Vol. 118, No. 1,92-99 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Im 0096-3445/89/S00.7 Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment

More information

SAMENESS AND REDUNDANCY IN TARGET DETECTION AND TARGET COMPARISON. Boaz M. Ben-David and Daniel Algom Tel-Aviv University

SAMENESS AND REDUNDANCY IN TARGET DETECTION AND TARGET COMPARISON. Boaz M. Ben-David and Daniel Algom Tel-Aviv University SAMENESS AND REDUNDANCY IN TARGET DETECTION AND TARGET COMPARISON Boaz M. Ben-David and Daniel Algom Tel-Aviv University boazb@post.tau.ac.il Abstract Searching for targets, people perform better and reap

More information

Parallels between Perception without Attention and Perception without Awareness

Parallels between Perception without Attention and Perception without Awareness CONSCIOUSNESS AND COGNITION 6, 219 236 (1997) ARTICLE NO. CC970310 Parallels between Perception without Attention and Perception without Awareness Philip M. Merikle 1 and Steve Joordens 2 University of

More information