Running head: DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 1. Daily Experiences and Well-Being: Do Memories of Events Matter? William Tov

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Running head: DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 1. Daily Experiences and Well-Being: Do Memories of Events Matter? William Tov"

Transcription

1 Running head: DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 1 Daily Experiences and Well-Being: Do Memories of Events Matter? William Tov Singapore Management University William Tov School of Social Sciences Singapore Management University 90 Stamford Road, Level 4 Singapore , Republic of Singapore. williamtov@smu.edu.sg. Office: Fax: Keywords: well-being, emotion, satisfaction, episodic memory, events, daily diary This is an electronic version of an article published as: Tov, W. (2012). Daily experiences and well-being: Do memories of events matter? Cognition and Emotion, 26, The published version is available online at

2 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 2 Abstract Retrospective subjective well-being (SWB) refers to self-reported satisfaction and emotional experience over the past few weeks or months. Two studies investigated the mechanisms linking daily experiences to retrospective SWB. Participants reported events each day for 21 days (Study 1) or twice a week for two months (Study 2). The emotional intensity of each event was rated (i) when it had recently occurred (proximal intensity); and (ii) at the end of the event-reporting period (distal intensity). Both sets of ratings were then aggregated across events and used to predict retrospective SWB at the end of the study. Path analyses showed that proximal intensity predicted retrospective SWB whereas distal intensity did not. The effect remained even after controlling for trait happiness and neuroticism. These results suggest that daily experiences influence retrospective SWB primarily through abstract representations of the past few weeks or months (as measured by aggregated proximal intensity ratings) rather than the explicit recollection of individual events during the same period (as measured by aggregated distal intensity ratings). Retrospective SWB, in turn, mediated the effect of daily experiences on global SWB (i.e., self-reported satisfaction and emotional experiences in general).

3 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 3 Acknowledgements William Tov, School of Social Sciences, Singapore Management University. This research was supported by internal research grants 08-C242-SMU-020 and 09- C242-SMU-019 from the Office of Research at Singapore Management University. I thank Evelyn Au, Serena Wee, and Christie Scollon for helpful comments on previous drafts of this paper; and Ian Kwok, Isaac Chin, Jolene Tan, Euodia Chua, and Michelle Chen for assisting in data collection. Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to Will Tov, School of Social Sciences, Singapore Management University, 90 Stamford Road, Level 4, Singapore , Republic of Singapore. williamtov@smu.edu.sg.

4 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 4 Daily life is filled with ups and downs. A fun-filled dinner with close friends might be followed hours later by an upset stomach after eating something that did not agree with us. As positive and negative experiences accumulate over time, how do they contribute to our sense of well-being? Presumably, positive events make us happy and negative events make us unhappy. However, the relation between everyday experience and well-being is not always as clear-cut. As Schwarz and Strack (1991) proposed, events may not influence well-being judgments unless they are cognitively accessible at the time of reporting. For example, when students first reported how often they went on dates and then rated their life satisfaction, the correlation between the two items was.66 (Strack, Martin, & Schwarz, 1988). However, when the order was reversed, the correlation was not significant. Dating frequency was only related to wellbeing when it was made cognitively accessible prior to evaluating life satisfaction. One interpretation of the order effects identified by Strack et al. (1988) is that the experience of an event is not as important as whether we remember it when reporting our wellbeing. Though intriguing, this hypothesis also challenges the validity of well-being measures. As Schwarz and Strack (1991, 1999) concluded, well-being reports may not reflect stable internal states, but are judgments constructed at the time of query from whatever information is accessible and deemed relevant. Thus self-reported well-being can be misleading because it only reflects a subset of experiences that may not have been meaningfully accessible. The present research sought to clarify this issue, focusing specifically on the relation between everyday experience and subjective well-being (SWB). The latter consists of cognitive, satisfaction judgments as well as affective experiences (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). Drawing on the distinction between episodic and semantic memory (Robinson & Clore, 2002a; Tulving, 1993), I propose three potential mechanisms linking everyday experiences to SWB:

5 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 5 episodic memory, recent-past representations, and dispositional causation. Table 1 presents a summary of the key constructs and how they are operationalized in the present research. Episodic and Semantic Memory in Self-Reported Emotion Similar to Schwarz and Strack (1991, 1999), Robinson and Clore (2002a) proposed that people evaluate their emotional experiences using the most accessible information that is relevant to the judgment at hand. More specifically, when people are asked to make short-term evaluations of their emotional experiences (e.g., how happy they felt during the past day), they rely primarily on episodic memory their conscious recollection of the specific events they experienced during the referenced period. However, when people are asked to make global evaluations of their emotional experiences (e.g., how happy they feel in general), there are too many past events to recall and many remain inaccessible. In that case, people rely more on semantic memory stable, generalized beliefs about their personality or how they typically feel. Although daily experiences must somehow contribute to semantic beliefs, the latter are largely decontextualized from such experiences (Robinson & Clore, 2002a). Thus, people can quickly report that they are generally a happy person without retrieving concrete instances of their happy experiences. Thus short-term and global SWB are differentially informed by episodic and semantic memory, respectively. What remains unanswered is how people summarize their SWB over intermediate periods such as the past few weeks or months. In the present paper, these assessments of intermediate length are referred to as retrospective SWB. A better understanding of the processes underlying retrospective SWB has important implications. Research suggests that the transition from an episodic to a semantic retrieval strategy occurs for reference periods between the past few weeks and the past few months (Robinson & Clore, 2002b), though the nature of this shift is unknown. Is it an all-or-none shift (only episodic or only

6 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 6 semantic knowledge is used) or is it a mixture of both? An all-or-none shift would impose a boundary condition on the extent to which episodic memory informs SWB judgments. Mechanisms Underlying the Link Between Daily Experiences and Retrospective SWB Mediation by Episodic Memory A possible mechanism linking daily experiences to retrospective SWB is episodic memory. Positive and negative events may not contribute to retrospective SWB unless people remember them when they make their judgment. Thus, episodic memory mediates the relation between daily experiences and retrospective SWB. This model is consistent with the order effects observed by Strack et al. (1988). It could also explain why global SWB is better predicted from events that people have recently experienced than those that occurred long ago (Suh, Diener, & Fujita, 1996): recent events are more likely to be recalled. Episodic memory may affect retrospective SWB judgments in two ways. First, how events are remembered could influence judgments. Past events that are remembered more (or less) negatively than originally felt, could sway the judgment in the corresponding direction. Second, the subset of events that are remembered could influence judgments: recalling more positive than negative events should result in more positive assessments of retrospective SWB. Perhaps because of its intuitiveness, the episodic mediation model has rarely been tested directly. Past studies measured episodic memory using free recall of events (e.g., Seidlitz & Diener, 1993), but did not obtain prior measures of experienced events. Without such measures, it is impossible to evaluate how the memory of events diverges from the original experience of these events, and if so, whether it is truly the former that ultimately influences retrospective SWB as posited by the episodic mediation model. Recent-Past Representations

7 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 7 Daily experiences may also influence retrospective SWB through recent-past representations (RPRs). As positive and negative experiences accumulate over a period of time, people may develop abstract summary representations of how things have been going. A major aspect of RPRs is their hedonic tone, which may be positive or negative overall, depending on the balance of experiences. In effect, RPRs summarize the overall pleasantness or unpleasantness of a given period of time (e.g., the past few weeks) and may share qualities of both episodic and semantic memory. RPRs may include knowledge of events that occurred during the period in question, but with less specificity than episodic knowledge. For example, a specific argument with one s spouse at a particular time and place would reflect episodic knowledge. In contrast, the knowledge that one has recently had many arguments with one s spouse and that the relationship has soured over the past month would reflect one s RPR. Thus, RPRs are semantic in that they are abstracted from concrete events, but episodic in that they are circumscribed within a particular period of time. RPRs are akin to what other researchers have called personal semantic memory (Kopelman, Wilson, & Baddeley, 1989; Robinson & Clore, 2002a). Unlike general semantic knowledge (e.g., The capital of China is Beijing ), personal semantic memory references a particular period of one s life (e.g., where one lived as a child). Such factual knowledge shares features of both episodic and semantic memory (Kazui, Hashimoto, Hirono, & Mori, 2003). Kazui et al. (2003) suggested that specific episodic knowledge may develop into personal semantic memory over time as the information (e.g., one s childhood address) is repeatedly recalled across contexts (e.g., in filling out paperwork, writing letters, etc.) and becomes dissociated from any specific episode. Unlike personal semantic memory, which consists primarily of factual knowledge, RPRs

8 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 8 contain more subjective, hedonic elements such as how enjoyable the recent past has been. Nonetheless, RPRs may play a similar role in the formation of semantic knowledge from episodic knowledge. Frequent positive (or negative) experiences may gradually produce RPRs that are predominantly pleasant (or unpleasant). Such representations may provide an important route by which daily experience shapes retrospective SWB, independently of episodic memory. Dispositional Causation Past research has shown that SWB is consistently correlated with personality traits (DeNeve & Cooper, 1998; Heller, Watson, & Ilies, 2004). Costa and McCrae (1980) argued that traits such as extraversion and neuroticism cause SWB by predisposing people to experience positive and negative emotions. In its strongest form, the dispositional causation hypothesis states that daily experiences do not actually contribute to retrospective SWB. Instead, both experiences and retrospective SWB are produced by a top-down process driven by personality traits. To rule out this explanation, the present studies control for trait levels of happiness and neuroticism. Retrospective SWB as a Critical Link Between Daily Experiences and Global SWB The distinction between episodic and semantic memory underscores the divergent processes that people engage in when evaluating short-term versus global SWB. However, if the two memory sources are dissociable, it is unclear how global SWB arises from the everyday episodes that fill our lives. If global SWB is informed by semantic memory, and the latter is relatively more stable and resistant to change than episodic memory how do daily experiences affect one s global SWB or do they at all? Past research has shown that daily hassles are associated with self-reported health and global SWB (DeLongis, Coyne, Dakof, Folkman, & Lazarus, 1982; Zika & Chamberlain, 1987). Thus, there is support for such a link, but a lack of

9 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 9 theoretical models that shed light on the underlying processes. One such model is the mood-mediation model (Robinson, 2000) which posits that the accumulation of positive and negative daily experiences affects people s mood states, which in turn influences global SWB. This occurs because mood states serve a reactive function they provide a hedonic summary of one s recent experiences and current life situation. Mood states also serve a prospective function they indicate the potential direction in which one s life is headed. The conditions of the present and immediate future, in turn, affect global SWB. The present research conceptually extends the mood-mediation model. I propose that the reactive and prospective function of mood-states apply more generally to retrospective SWB. Daily experiences do not only influence recent affective states, but also how satisfied people have been with their current situation. Retrospective SWB then influences global SWB. Therefore, the present research also tests the hypothesis that retrospective SWB mediates the relation between daily experiences and global SWB. Overview of Hypotheses The current project involves two objectives. The first is to clarify the memory processes underlying judgments of retrospective SWB. Unlike previous research that used one-time measures of event checklists (e.g., Robinson, 2000; Seidlitz & Diener, 1993; Suh et al., 1996), the present studies enabled participants to record specific, personally relevant events for a period of weeks and measured their episodic memory of these same events at a later time. Participants rated the emotional intensity of these events twice: (1) proximal intensity ratings were completed in close proximity to the occurrence of each event (within a few days); and (2) distal intensity ratings were completed at the end of the event-reporting period (participants were instructed to remember how they felt about the event when it happened). By comparing both sets of ratings,

10 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 10 one can examine whether memory of the events has changed over time and if so, whether these changes are related to retrospective SWB at the end of the study. The distal intensity ratings are consistent with Wheeler, Stuss, and Tulving s (1997) theory that episodic memory involves mentally traveling back to re-experience the retrieved event [emphasis added] (p. 332). However, in Study 2, participants also completed a free recall task with the frequency of recalled events providing an additional measure of episodic memory. In contrast, RPRs were measured indirectly by computing the average proximal intensities of positive events minus those for negative events. This reflects the nature of RPRs as arising from the hedonic balance of positive and negative events that have accumulated over time, irrespective of whether individual events are consciously recollected. In addition, past research has shown that the preponderance of positive experiences over negative experiences predicts well-being more consistently than the average intensity or frequency of either measure alone (Larsen, Diener, & Emmons, 1985; Seidlitz & Diener, 1990). Figure 1 summarizes the three possible mechanisms linking daily experiences to retrospective SWB. Each mechanism can be articulated as a specific hypothesis. (Figure 1 about here) Hypothesis 1 (Mediation by Episodic Memory): Over time, positive and negative experiences contribute to episodic memory, which then predicts retrospective SWB. This implies that paths a and b in Figure 1 are significant, and path c is not. Hypothesis 2 (Recent-Past Representations Predict SWB): The balance of positive and negative experiences over time result in RPRs that are predominantly pleasant or unpleasant. These RPRs predict retrospective SWB independently of episodic memory and personality. This is implies that path c is significant.

11 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 11 Hypothesis 3 (Dispositional Causation): Both daily experiences and SWB are caused by dispositional variables. This implies that paths d and e are significant. A stronger form of this hypothesis further implies that paths b and c are not significant. In addition, the mediating role of retrospective SWB was also tested. Hypothesis 4: The balance of positive and negative experiences over time predicts retrospective SWB, which in turn, predicts global SWB. More formally, proximal intensity exerts a significant indirect effect on global SWB through retrospective SWB. If Hypothesis 4 is supported, this would provide important evidence for the cumulative impact of daily experiences on global SWB. Study 1 Method Participants. Two hundred and twenty-nine students at a small Asian university were paid (up to Singaporean $56) for a three-week daily diary study. Participants were excluded if they dropped out (n=9), listed invalid entries (e.g., nothing happened today ) for over half of their event listings (n=10), or tended to give the same ratings across all their events (n=4). The final sample consisted of 206 students (121 female) with a mean age of 21.6 years. 1 Materials and Procedure. Study 1 consisted of three phases. Phase 1. Students completed the personality measures in a one-hour survey session. Trait happiness was measured with the Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS; Lyubomirksy & Lepper, 1999), which consisted of four items (e.g., In general, I consider myself 1 [not a very happy person] to 7 [a very happy person]). Neuroticism was measured with the International Personality Item Pool (NEO-PI-R Broad Domains, The scale consisted of ten items (e.g., I worry

12 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 12 about things ) rated from 1 (very inaccurate) to 5 (very accurate). Phase 2. A few days after Phase 1, the 21-day event-reporting period began. At the end of each day (between 9pm and 3am), students logged into the study website and listed, in a randomized order, one positive and one negative event. Then they rated each event in terms of valence (1 = extremely negative; 10 = extremely positive) and their emotional reaction to the event on three 9-point bipolar scales with anchors of extremely happy extremely sad, extremely cheerful extremely depressed, and extremely pleased extremely upset. The four event ratings were rescaled from 0 to 10 and then averaged into a single score for each event. Ratings for negative events were reverse coded so that higher scores reflected greater negativity. A proximal intensity index was created by subtracting the average intensity of the negative events from the average intensity of the positive events. Phase 3. In a final computerized session, students completed measures of retrospective and global SWB. The retrospective SWB survey contained four scales referring to the past three weeks of the study (i.e., during Phase 2): (a) Overall Satisfaction (level of satisfaction dissatisfaction, and how terrible excellent the period was); (b) Domain Satisfaction (satisfaction with school, relationships, health, finances, leisure, friends, family, romantic life, learning in courses, grades, and campus activities); (c) Positive Emotions (extent to which one felt happy, pleased, proud, affectionate, relaxed, and cheerful); and (d) Negative Emotions (extent to which one felt sad, upset, ashamed, angry, stressed, and depressed). Satisfaction items were rated from 1 (extremely dissatisfied) to 7 (extremely satisfied); emotion items were rated from 0 (not at all) to 6 (a great deal). The global SWB items were identical to the retrospective items but referred to how participants felt in general. Next, in a randomized order, students were shown all the events they listed in Phase 2 (a

13 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 13 maximum of 42), and rated how they remembered feeling about each event the day it occurred. A distal intensity index was computed using similar items and procedures employed for the proximal intensity index. All SWB measures were administered before the distal ratings to avoid unduly influencing participants episodic memory. Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics and reliabilities for all measures. (Table 2 about here) Results Distal intensity did not differ from proximal intensity for positive events (M s = 7.27 vs 7.33) or negative events (M s = 6.81 vs 6.84), both t s Between-person correlations between distal and proximal intensities were quite high (r s =.87 and.85 for positive and negative events respectively). Analyses were also conducted within-subjects at the level of individual events using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM). Again, mean-level differences were not significant for either positive events, t(205) = 1.82, p =.07, or negative events, t(205) = 1.01, p =.32. The average event-level correlation was.54 for positive events and.59 for negative events, both p s <.001. This is notably smaller than the between-person correlations and suggests that memory for the average intensity across events is better than the specific intensity of each individual event. After subtracting negative ratings from positive ratings, all subsequent analyses were based on the proximal and distal intensity indices (see Method). 2 Mechanisms linking daily experiences and retrospective SWB. Event intensities and personality traits correlated with retrospective SWB (Table 3) and significantly accounted for variance in the latter: overall satisfaction (40.0%), domain satisfaction (54.2%), positive emotion (PE; 43.8%), and negative emotion (NE; 35.2%), p s < (Table 3 about here)

14 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 14 Four path models were estimated one for each SWB measure. All models are summarized in Figure 2. For example, proximal intensity predicted retrospective PE (path c =.47), which predicted global PE (path g =.76). The path from neuroticism to proximal intensity was not significant in any model and was dropped for the sake of parsimony. All path coefficients corrected for measurement error in the personality and SWB measures, using the reliability of each scale to estimate the amount of error variance. (Figure 2 about here) Fit statistics are presented in Table 4. With the exception of overall satisfaction, all models fit the data well as indicated by non-significant chi-square values and RMSEA s below.06 (Hu & Bentler, 1999). As discussed later, the model for overall satisfaction required an additional modification. For now, the initial models are used as a starting point to evaluate each of the proposed mechanisms linking daily experiences to retrospective SWB (paths a to e2). (Table 4 about here) Mediation by episodic memory. Although proximal intensity was significantly related to distal intensity (path a), the latter did not predict retrospective SWB as path b was not significant in any of the models. Moreover, dropping path b had little effect on model fit. The change in chi-square (Δχ 2 ) for each model was not significant: overall satisfaction (0.00), domain satisfaction (0.19), PE (0.00), and NE (1.70), all df s = 1, all p s >.19. These results suggest that episodic memory does not mediate the effects of daily experiences on retrospective SWB. Recent-past representations. With the exception of negative emotions, proximal intensity significantly predicted retrospective SWB. Path c was significant even after controlling for the effects of trait happiness, neuroticism, and distal intensity. (This holds true even when the path from neuroticism to proximal intensity is estimated). The increment in variance

15 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 15 accounted for by proximal intensity was significant for overall satisfaction (+6.2%), domain satisfaction (+8.2%), and PE (+5.6%), p s <.001; but not NE (+0.1%), p =.11. These findings provide initial support for the hypothesis that RPRs contribute uniquely to retrospective SWB. Dispositional causation. There was mixed support for dispositional causation. In particular, trait happiness predicted daily experiences (path d1) and retrospective domain satisfaction and PE (path e1). In contrast, neuroticism did not predict proximal ratings, and only predicted retrospective NE and PE (path e2) though the latter was unexpectedly in the positive direction. The stronger form of the hypothesis was also tested by dropping paths b and c. This alternative model assumes that the relation between proximal intensity and retrospective SWB can be fully accounted for by personality traits. This model had worse fit than the initial model; the increase in χ 2 was significant in all cases: overall satisfaction (+67.70); domain satisfaction (+88.71); PE (+44.05); and NE (+21.64); all df s = 2, all p s <.001. Retrospective SWB as a mediator. Together personality traits, event intensities, and retrospective SWB accounted for significant variance in global SWB: overall satisfaction (62.1%), domain satisfaction (74.3%), PE (64.4%), and NE (69.8%), p s <.001. Even after controlling for personality traits and event intensities, retrospective SWB accounted for additional variance in global SWB: overall satisfaction (+5.2%), domain satisfaction (+23.1%), PE (+23.0%), and NE (+32.9%), p s <.001. The indirect effect from proximal intensity to retrospective SWB to global SWB was tested by computing the product of paths c and g. As such mediated effects are not always normally distributed, bootstrap analyses were conducted (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). For each model, 1000 bootstrap samples were generated and a biascorrected 95% confidence interval (CI) was constructed around the indirect effect of proximal intensity. Significant indirect effects were observed for domain satisfaction (.31;.74) and PE

16 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 16 (.16;.58), but not NE (-.40;.07) as the latter CI includes zero. The initial model for overall satisfaction was significantly improved by adding a path from proximal ratings to global satisfaction, Δχ 2 (1) = -8.31, p =.004. This model implies a direct effect of proximal intensity on global satisfaction and fit the data well, χ 2 (4) = 3.60, p =.46, RMSEA = 0.0, RMSEA 90% CI (0.0; 0.10). Bootstrap analyses performed on this modified model also revealed a significant indirect effect of proximal intensity through retrospective satisfaction, 95% CI(.11;.40). Thus the latter serves as a partial mediator. Discussion Personality traits, proximal intensity, and distal intensity all correlated with retrospective SWB (see Table 3). However, after controlling for proximal intensity and personality traits, distal intensity no longer predicted retrospective SWB. This result is inconsistent with the hypothesis that episodic memory mediates the relation between daily experiences and retrospective SWB. How participants consciously remembered past events did not predict retrospective SWB beyond the cumulative impact of those events as they were experienced. Trait happiness and neuroticism were distinctively related to different components of SWB. Specifically, trait happiness tended to be a stronger predictor of PE and satisfaction, whereas neuroticism tended to be a stronger predictor of NE. This pattern replicates past findings linking positive and negative affect to distinct personality traits such as extraversion and neuroticism, respectively (Costa & McCrae, 1980; DeNeve & Cooper, 1998). Although there were effects of trait happiness on both proximal intensity and retrospective SWB, a model that assumed complete dispositional causation fit the data poorly. This finding supports the notion that people develop RPRs that are distinct from semantic personality knowledge and episodic knowledge of concrete events. Finally, retrospective SWB mediated the link between daily

17 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 17 experiences and all measures of global SWB except global NE. In general, the balance of positive and negative experiences predicts retrospective SWB (i.e., how people summarized their well-being over the recent past), which then predicts global SWB. Study 2 One limitation of Study 1 was the relatively short three-week event-sampling period. Students memory for how they felt about the events (on average) was relatively accurate over this period as distal intensity ratings correlated with proximal intensity ratings above.80 at the betweenpersons level. A greater amount of time between event reporting and distal ratings could allow for more change in memory and larger effects on retrospective SWB. In addition, the extent to which the distal intensity measure reflects episodic memory may be questioned. Participants were simply shown and asked to rate all the events they had listed in Phase 2. Thus, it is not clear which events participants may have spontaneously recalled and used to evaluate their SWB. Even worse, participants may have supplied ratings for events they had no recollection of adding more error to the distal intensity ratings. Study 2 was designed to address these limitations. The event-reporting period was extended from three weeks to two months, after which a free-recall procedure was introduced. Participants recalled as many events as possible before rating the distal intensity of all their events. Two additional procedures were added. First, participants indicated how clearly they remembered each event they listed so that poorly recollected events could be excluded from the distal intensity index. Second, after completing the distal ratings, participants were shown the events they listed during free-recall and asked to match these events to the ones that were entered during the event-reporting period. This made it possible to identify the number of positive versus negative events that were recalled. This recall frequency measure offered an alternate

18 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 18 measure of episodic memory based on which events people remember (i.e., mostly positive or mostly negative) and not how they remember those events. Finally, two measures of retrospective SWB (immediate and delayed) were administered. The immediate retrospective measures were given in Phase 3 (following the event-reporting period). The delayed retrospective measures were given in Phase 4 (approximately one month after the event-reporting period). Both measures referenced the same two-month reporting period. The delayed assessment allowed for another test of the episodic mediation model. For example, it is possible that events that are freely recalled or clearly remembered in Phase 3 are also highly memorable. If so, such events should be particularly accessible and could influence retrospective SWB given a greater delay in time. If proximal intensity predicts even delayed retrospective SWB, this would imply that RPRs have some degree of stability, as they should if they reflect cumulative summaries circumscribed by a particular period of time. Method Participants. One hundred sixty-six students participated in a paid (up to Singaporean $64), four-month study. Participants were excluded if they dropped out (n=14), provided invalid entries for more than half of their event listings (n=6), or tended to give the same ratings across all their events (n=7). For example, some respondents provided extreme ratings for positive and negative emotions across all events. The final sample consisted of 139 students (91 females) with mean age of 21.3 years. Materials and Procedure. Study 2 consisted of four phases. Phase 1. Students completed the SHS and the IPIP Neuroticism scale in a one-hour survey session. Phase 2. Over the next eight weeks, students logged into the study website every

19 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 19 Wednesday and Sunday. On Wednesdays, students listed two events (one positive and one negative) that occurred from Sunday to Tuesday. On Sundays, students listed two events that occurred from Wednesday to Saturday. Thus, the listing procedure attempted to capture events from both halves of the week. Students evaluated the proximal intensity of each event by rating its valence (1 = extremely negative; 11 = extremely positive) and how much the event made them feel happy, pleased, sad, and upset (0 = not at all, 6 = a great deal). These items were recoded and combined into proximal intensity index following similar procedures as Study 1. Phase 3. The week after Phase 2, students attended a one-hour computerized survey session. First, they completed the immediate retrospective SWB measures, which referenced the past two months (i.e., Phase 2): Overall Satisfaction (see Study 1 for items); Domain Satisfaction (satisfaction with health, finances, leisure, friends, family, romantic life, learning, grades, and progress in completing assignments); Positive Emotions (extent to which they felt happy, pleased, relaxed, and cheerful); and Negative Emotions (extent to which they felt sad, upset, angry, and stressed). Response scales were identical to those used in Study 1. Next, students were asked to recall the events they had listed over the past two months (a maximum of 32). They were given 3.5 minutes to enter their events, after which a message appeared on-screen instructing them to continue with the survey. This time limit was determined using past research as a guide. Seidlitz and Diener (1993) allowed three minutes each to recall positive and negative life events; thus their recall procedure spanned a total of six minutes. As the reference period for Study 2 was much shorter, a concern was that allowing too much time would result in a ceiling effect across participants. Feedback from three research assistants indicated that 3.5 minutes was sufficient time for them to recall and report meaningful events that occurred over the past two months.

20 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 20 Students then rated the distal intensity of all events listed in Phase 2. In a randomized order, they were instructed to remember how they felt about each event using the same five items from the proximal intensity measure. They also indicated whether their memory of the event was clear (the details and specific context in which it occurred are remembered), vague (some memory remains but the exact details are unclear), or whether they have no memory of the event (most details are forgotten). The last task of the session consisted of a recall-matching task. Students were shown the events they had recalled earlier, along with the events they reported in Phase 2. They were instructed to match (if possible) each recalled event to at least one of the previously listed events. This made is possible to determine how many recalled events were originally reported as positive versus negative events. Phase 4. Approximately 3 weeks after Phase 3, students reported their global SWB using the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985), and modified versions of the domain satisfaction, PE, and NE scales used in Phase 3. Next, students completed the delayed retrospective SWB measure. To ensure that the same two-month period was referenced, the dates of the event-reporting period (January 13 to March 7, 2010) were presented throughout the measures. Reliabilities and descriptive statistics for all measures in Study 2 are reported in Table 2. Results In Phase 3, students freely recalled 5.99 positive events and 5.70 negative events, t(138) = 1.16, p =.25. More positive events were marked as clearly remembered than were negative events (M s = 9.32 vs 7.97), t(134) = 5.38, d =.44, p <.001. On average, the distal intensity of positive events (M = 8.25) did not differ from proximal intensity (M = 8.28), t < However,

21 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 21 the distal intensity of negative events (M = 7.70) overestimated the proximal intensity (M = 7.46), t(138) = 5.17, d =.24, p <.001. Between-person correlations were high for both positive (r =.87) and negative (r =.84) events. These analyses were repeated within-subjects using HLM. Replicating the between-person results, the distal intensity of negative events overestimated proximal intensity, t(138) = 5.28, p <.001; but no difference was found for positive events. As in Study 1, average event-level correlations were smaller than betweenperson correlations: r s =.51 for positive events and.57 for negative events, p s <.001. All subsequent analyses employed the proximal and distal intensity indices with the latter computed from clearly remembered events only. Four more participants were excluded because they did not clearly remember any of their events. In addition, recall frequency (the difference in the number of positive versus negative events recalled) served as another measure of episodic memory (see Seidlitz & Diener, 1993, for a similar application). Mechanisms linking daily experiences and retrospective SWB. Together, personality traits and event intensities significantly accounted for variance in immediate retrospective SWB: overall satisfaction (25.8%), domain satisfaction (29.9%), PE (32.6%), NE (25.3%); and delayed retrospective SWB: overall satisfaction (27.8%), domain satisfaction (31.3%), PE (28.9%), NE (20.4%), p s <.001. Intercorrelations among these variables are displayed in Table 3. Again, four path models were estimated (see Figure 3). These models employed personality traits, proximal intensity, and distal intensity (of clearly remembered events) as predictors of immediate retrospective SWB. All models fit the data well (see Table 4). Below, the three mechanisms are evaluated in light of these initial path models. (Figure 3 about here) Mediation by episodic memory. Distal intensity did not predict retrospective SWB in

22 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 22 any model. Further, dropping path b did not reduce the fit of any model. The Δχ 2 s were: overall satisfaction (+0.02), domain satisfaction (+0.78), PE (+0.08), and NE (+0.23), all df s = 1, all p s >.37. These results are inconsistent with the episodic mediation model. Recent-past representations. In all models, proximal intensity predicted retrospective SWB (path c) even after controlling for the effects of trait happiness, neuroticism, and distal ratings. The increment in variance accounted for by proximal intensity was as follows: overall satisfaction (+6.1%); domain satisfaction (+7.7%); PE (+5.4%); and NE (+4.1%), p s <.01. Dispositional causation. Dispositional causation was only partially supported: trait happiness predicted both proximal intensity and retrospective SWB (except NE). However, after controlling for trait happiness, neuroticism predicted neither proximal intensity nor retrospective SWB. No support was found for the stronger form of dispositional causation as dropping paths b and c led to significantly worse fit across all models. The Δχ 2 s were: overall satisfaction (+13.49), domain satisfaction (+16.86), PE (+12.94), and NE (+9.48), all df s = 2, all p s <.01. Similar results were obtained when recall frequency was used in place of distal intensity: proximal intensity predicted immediate retrospective SWB, whereas recall frequency did not. Retrospective SWB as a mediator. Together, personality traits, event intensities, and immediate retrospective SWB significantly accounted for variance in global SWB: overall satisfaction (41.0%), domain satisfaction (69.4%), PE (59.2%), and NE (38.6%), p s <.001. The increment in variance accounted for by immediate retrospective SWB was also significant: overall satisfaction (+18.7%); domain satisfaction (+39.1%); PE (+28.1%); and NE (+14.4%), p s <.001. Bias-corrected 95% CI s (derived from bootstrapping) revealed significant indirect effects of proximal intensity on global SWB (via retrospective SWB): overall satisfaction (.08;.40); domain satisfaction (.23;.64); PE (.05;.40); and NE (-.38; -.07). Thus, immediate

23 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 23 retrospective SWB significantly mediated the effects of proximal intensity on global SWB. Predicting delayed retrospective SWB from personality and events. Given more time, episodic memory may predict retrospective SWB more strongly. To evaluate this possibility, the delayed retrospective measures were regressed on both proximal and distal intensity, controlling for trait happiness and neuroticism (Table 5, Model 1). (Table 5 about here) Proximal intensity predicted all delayed measures except NE (β = -.23, p =.06). Distal intensity did not predict delayed retrospective SWB. Interestingly, when recall frequency was substituted for distal intensity, the former predicted delayed retrospective SWB above and beyond proximal intensity and personality (Table 5, Model 2); only the effect on domain satisfaction was not significant (β =.11, p =.13). Discussion With few exceptions, proximal intensity predicted retrospective SWB beyond the effects of personality traits and distal intensity. In contrast, distal intensity did not predict retrospective SWB even when ratings of poorly remembered events were excluded. This pattern of findings is inconsistent with the episodic mediation model. Moreover, proximal intensity and not distal intensity predicted delayed retrospective SWB. This result is particularly noteworthy given that the distal intensity ratings were completed closer in time to the delayed measures and should have been more accessible in participants memory than the proximal intensity ratings. Nevertheless, the latter were still better predictors. This implies that RPRs have some degree of stability that is not fully accounted for by trait happiness and neuroticism. Recall frequency was the only measure of episodic memory to predict retrospective SWB but only for the delayed assessment (see General Discussion). Perhaps if participants

24 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 24 were given more time (> 3.5 minutes), the recall frequency measure would predict SWB more strongly. Although this possibility cannot be ruled out, recall frequency correlated significantly with immediate retrospective SWB (Table 3), suggesting that the amount of time was sufficient to capture meaningful differences in SWB. Nevertheless, future studies could investigate the optimal time for such recall tasks; such studies might also benefit from the increased power of a larger sample size (see Footnote 1). Finally, Study 2 provided additional support for the mediating role of retrospective SWB in linking daily experiences to global SWB. Retrospective SWB is informed by the cumulative impact of daily experiences, and shapes global SWB. General Discussion The present studies expand our understanding of how daily experiences relate to selfreported SWB, clarifying the role that memory plays in such judgments. The hypothesis that these experiences are mediated by episodic memory was rigorously tested and, somewhat surprisingly, was not supported. The results must be interpreted cautiously: it is not the case that episodic memory is unrelated to SWB. First, distal intensity ratings were significantly correlated with retrospective SWB. Only after controlling for proximal intensity was no relationship observed. Thus, the remembered intensity of past events is associated with retrospective SWB, but only to the extent that the memory corresponds with the original reactions to those events (i.e., proximal intensity). Discrepancies between distal and proximal intensity do not seem to bias retrospective SWB judgments. Second, under some conditions, an episodic retrieval strategy may still contribute to retrospective SWB. For example, in Study 2, recall frequency predicted delayed retrospective SWB. Perhaps when the reference period is somewhat removed or discontinuous with the present, episodic memory may be relied upon to outline the events that occurred during the period in question (but see Limitations).

25 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 25 Proximal Intensity Ratings as a Proxy for Recent-Past Representations The significant effects of proximal intensity ratings on retrospective SWB suggest that when aggregated over a period of time they reflect RPRs, a knowledge structure that is distinct from episodic and semantic memory. Even when the assessment was delayed, retrospective SWB was better predicted from proximal intensity than distal intensity. This implies that RPRs are somewhat stable as they reflect the cumulative impact of recent experiences rather than any single event. It may seem counterintuitive that events can have emotional effects beyond the ability to consciously recall them. However, recent experiments have shown that amnesics exhibit mood effects from sad and happy film clips even after their factual memory of the clips had dissipated (Feinstein, Duff, & Tranel, 2010). Thus, there may be more to daily experiences than what people are able to explicitly remember about them. Aggregated proximal intensities may reflect past experiences more broadly than do distal intensities. Though both measures are highly correlated (r s >.80), proximal ratings appear to capture something that distal ratings lack. One possibility is that our emotional reactions to events during a given period of time trigger a chain of subsequent events within the same period. An argument with a friend might produce negative mood, which then fosters procrastination with work and a hectic rush to meet deadlines two days later. These collateral effects would enter into the RPR for the period but might not be reflected in the distal intensity of the specific event (i.e., the argument). Distal intensity ratings could also be influenced by situation-specific beliefs ( I was at a party so I must have been pretty happy ). According to Robinson and Clore (2002a), when some episodic details are lost, such beliefs should influence distal ratings more than one s personality. However, situation-specific beliefs may not exist for all situations. Moreover, given that such beliefs are specific to the nature of each event, they may not clearly bias ratings in any single

26 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 26 direction. This may explain why the residual variance in distal intensity (after partialing out proximal intensity) is not systematically related to retrospective SWB. The Mediating Role of Retrospective SWB Robinson and Clore (2002a) suggested that people shift retrieval strategies from episodic to semantic memory for retrospective judgments greater than the past three weeks. The results support this hypothesis but also reveal that even for reference periods of two months (Study 2), semantic memory is not the only source of influence on retrospective SWB; RPRs explained additional variance. Independent of personality traits, people whose recent past experiences were more positive than negative tended to report greater retrospective SWB and ultimately greater global SWB. Thus, retrospective SWB provides a critical link between daily experiences and overall well-being (see also Robinson, 2000). The significant indirect effect of daily experience on global SWB has important policy implications. First, global SWB measures are widely administered in national and cross-national surveys (Tov & Au, in press). Second, there is increasing interest in using such measures to inform public policy decisions (Diener, Lucas, Schimmack, & Helliwell, 2009; Diener & Tov, in press). In both cases, the assumption is that respondents general feelings of happiness and life satisfaction reflect their experiences in society. However, if global measures are derived from people s semantic beliefs about their feelings rather than their daily experiences, then it is unclear whether public policy has any role in facilitating SWB. An undesirable implication is that global SWB is solely driven by top-down processes guided by personality traits. The present findings offer an alternative, bottom-up process that contributes to global SWB independently of trait happiness. Thus, policymakers interested in tracking the effects of new policy could administer retrospective SWB measures after the change has been implemented. Such measures

27 DAILY EXPERIENCES & WELL-BEING 27 will be more sensitive to daily experience and ultimately predict global SWB. Implications for the Validity of Self-Report SWB Measures Schwarz and Strack (1991, 1999) proposed that SWB judgments are not reflections of stable internal states; rather they are constructed from information that is cognitively accessible at the time of assessment and deemed relevant to the judgment at hand. The present findings do not contradict the importance of cognitive accessibility. Regardless of which knowledge source informs SWB judgments (semantic, episodic, or RPRs), it must somehow be accessible to the person making the judgment. However, past research has not clearly articulated the extent to which information must be consciously accessible as the classic demonstration of order effects suggests (Strack et al., 1988). The lack of consistent effects for distal intensity and recall frequency indicates that this need not be the case. Moreover, the conclusion that SWB judgments do not reflect stable internal states is not supported by the present research. Even when assessment is delayed, retrospective SWB was predicted from proximal intensity of past experiences. This is not to deny that self-reported SWB is susceptible to context effects such as item order, mood, and social comparison (Schwarz & Strack, 1999). Nevertheless, a considerable amount of the variance in global SWB is accounted for by retrospective SWB and the latter is sensitive to the gradual accumulation of daily experiences even if those experiences cannot be fully recalled at a conscious level. These findings are in line with other studies suggesting that life satisfaction is influenced more by relatively stable sources of information than context effects (Schimmack & Oishi, 2005). It should not be concluded from the above that global or retrospective SWB measures capture all we would want to know about well-being. Instead, what the above findings highlight is that different measures of SWB tap into distinct sources of knowledge. No single measure can

Life-Satisfaction Is a Momentary Judgment and a Stable Personality Characteristic: The Use of Chronically Accessible and Stable Sources

Life-Satisfaction Is a Momentary Judgment and a Stable Personality Characteristic: The Use of Chronically Accessible and Stable Sources Life-Satisfaction Is a Momentary Judgment and a Stable Personality Characteristic: The Use of Chronically Accessible and Stable Sources Ulrich Schimmack University of Toronto, Mississauga Ed Diener University

More information

1. Family context. a) Positive Disengaged

1. Family context. a) Positive Disengaged Online Supplementary Materials for Emotion manuscript 015-197 Emotions and Concerns: Situational Evidence for their Systematic Co-Occurrence. by Jozefien De Leersnyder, Peter Koval, Peter Kuppens, & Batja

More information

Measuring Psychological Wealth: Your Well-Being Balance Sheet

Measuring Psychological Wealth: Your Well-Being Balance Sheet Putting It All Together 14 Measuring Psychological Wealth: Your Well-Being Balance Sheet Measuring Your Satisfaction With Life Below are five statements with which you may agree or disagree. Using the

More information

The Structure of Subjective Wellbeing

The Structure of Subjective Wellbeing Structure of Subjective Wellbeing 1 The Structure of Subjective Wellbeing Ulrich Schimmack University of Toronto, Mississauga Department of Psychology, UTM 3359 Mississauga Road North Mississauga, Ontario,

More information

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1060 1066 Brief Report Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies

More information

The Role of Domain Satisfaction in Explaining the Paradoxical Association Between Life Satisfaction and Age

The Role of Domain Satisfaction in Explaining the Paradoxical Association Between Life Satisfaction and Age Boise State University ScholarWorks Psychological Sciences Faculty Publications and Presentations Department of Psychological Science 11-1-2012 The Role of Domain Satisfaction in Explaining the Paradoxical

More information

Personality and Social Psychology

Personality and Social Psychology Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2011, 52, 43 48 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00826.x Personality and Social Psychology Be yourself, believe in yourself, and be happy: Self-efficacy as a mediator between

More information

AFFECT MEASUREMENT IN EXPERIENCE SAMPLING RESEARCH

AFFECT MEASUREMENT IN EXPERIENCE SAMPLING RESEARCH ULRICH SCHIMMACK AFFECT MEASUREMENT IN EXPERIENCE SAMPLING RESEARCH ABSTRACT. Experience-sampling studies are used to study the emotional component of subjective well-being (hedonic balance). This manuscript

More information

Detecting Well-being via Computerized Content Analysis of Brief Diary Entries

Detecting Well-being via Computerized Content Analysis of Brief Diary Entries Singapore Management University Institutional Knowledge at Singapore Management University Research Collection School of Social Sciences School of Social Sciences 2013 Detecting Well-being via Computerized

More information

Validity of a happiness Implicit Association Test as a measure of subjective well-being q

Validity of a happiness Implicit Association Test as a measure of subjective well-being q Journal of Research in Personality xxx (2007) xxx xxx www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp Brief Report Validity of a happiness Implicit Association Test as a measure of subjective well-being q Simone S. Walker

More information

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY http://www.uiowa.edu/crisp Submitted: May 3, 2016 First Revision: June 3, 2016 Second Revision: August 26, 2016 Accepted: August 30, 2016 WHY DO EXTRAVERTS FEEL MORE

More information

The extraverted and the neurotic glasses are of different colors

The extraverted and the neurotic glasses are of different colors Personality and Individual Differences 41 (2006) 745 754 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid The extraverted and the neurotic glasses are of different colors Liad Uziel * School of Business Administration, The

More information

Amelie Gamble Tommy Gärling University of Gothenburg, Göteborg, Sweden

Amelie Gamble Tommy Gärling University of Gothenburg, Göteborg, Sweden Amelie Gamble Tommy Gärling University of Gothenburg, Göteborg, Sweden Subjective Well-Being (SWB) Definitions SWB has a cognitive and an affective component. The cognitive component is a global judgement

More information

Women s Symptom Memories: More Accessible Than Men s?

Women s Symptom Memories: More Accessible Than Men s? Women s Symptom Memories: More Accessible Than Men s? J.A. Skelton & Elizabeth Corrigan Dickinson College Men are more likely than women to die at virtually every age, but women report greater numbers

More information

Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students

Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students Gary Felsten Stress Reactivity and Depressed Mood 1 Department of Psychology, Indiana University Purdue University Columbus 4601

More information

Underlying Theory & Basic Issues

Underlying Theory & Basic Issues Underlying Theory & Basic Issues Dewayne E Perry ENS 623 Perry@ece.utexas.edu 1 All Too True 2 Validity In software engineering, we worry about various issues: E-Type systems: Usefulness is it doing what

More information

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter Lawrence Feinstein Jyllian Call University of California, Los Angeles December, 1986 Further information on the instrument can be obtained

More information

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2014, 42(7), 1105-1116 Society for Personality Research http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2014.42.7.1105 ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN

More information

3/24/09. The Happy Personality: A Meta-Analysis of 137 Personality Traits and Subjective Well-Being. Kristina M. DeNeve & Harris Cooper 1998

3/24/09. The Happy Personality: A Meta-Analysis of 137 Personality Traits and Subjective Well-Being. Kristina M. DeNeve & Harris Cooper 1998 Examined 137 distinct personality constructs as correlates of Subjective Well- Being (SWB) Majority of studies of SWB have focused on biosocial indicators such as sex and age. Several narrative reviews

More information

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 1633 1639 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Short Communication The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic

More information

FACETS AND WELL-BEING 1. Predicting Psychological and Subjective Well-Being from Personality: Incremental Prediction from 30 Facets over the Big 5

FACETS AND WELL-BEING 1. Predicting Psychological and Subjective Well-Being from Personality: Incremental Prediction from 30 Facets over the Big 5 FACETS AND WELL-BEING 1 Predicting Psychological and Subjective Well-Being from Personality: Incremental Prediction from 30 Facets over the Big 5 Jeromy Anglim, Sharon Grant 1 Citation Information: Anglim,

More information

Development of Positive Psychology: Contributions from Traditional Psychological Disciplines

Development of Positive Psychology: Contributions from Traditional Psychological Disciplines Development of Positive Psychology: Contributions from Traditional Psychological Disciplines Canadian Conference on Positive Psychology June 17, 2016 Weiting Ng What is positive ology? Positive Psychology

More information

The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016

The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016 The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016 This course does not cover how to perform statistical tests on SPSS or any other computer program. There are several courses

More information

Pooling Subjective Confidence Intervals

Pooling Subjective Confidence Intervals Spring, 1999 1 Administrative Things Pooling Subjective Confidence Intervals Assignment 7 due Friday You should consider only two indices, the S&P and the Nikkei. Sorry for causing the confusion. Reading

More information

BACK TO THE FUTURE AND FORWARD TO THE PAST: EXPLORING RETROSPECTIVE BIASES AND CULTURE

BACK TO THE FUTURE AND FORWARD TO THE PAST: EXPLORING RETROSPECTIVE BIASES AND CULTURE BACK TO THE FUTURE AND FORWARD TO THE PAST: EXPLORING RETROSPECTIVE BIASES AND CULTURE Therese A. Louie, Lucas College and Graduate School of Business, San José State University, One Washington Square,

More information

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES (2018-19) PSYCHOLOGY Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology OUTLINE OF THE CHAPTER (i) Scientific Methods in Psychology -observation, case study, surveys, psychological tests, experimentation

More information

Personality Traits and Life Satisfaction among Working Men and Women

Personality Traits and Life Satisfaction among Working Men and Women Personality Traits and Life Satisfaction among Working Men and Women Azra Khanam Education University Lahore Campus, Pakistan Abstract The present research investigated personality traits and life satisfaction

More information

Tourists' Emotions: A Longitudinal Study

Tourists' Emotions: A Longitudinal Study University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Travel and Tourism Research Association: Advancing Tourism Research Globally 2012 ttra International Conference Tourists' Emotions: A Longitudinal

More information

The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories

The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories Wai Keen Vong (waikeen.vong@adelaide.edu.au) Andrew T. Hendrickson (drew.hendrickson@adelaide.edu.au) Amy Perfors (amy.perfors@adelaide.edu.au)

More information

According to annual surveys with nationally representative. The Experiencing and Remembering of Well-Being: A Cross-Cultural Analysis

According to annual surveys with nationally representative. The Experiencing and Remembering of Well-Being: A Cross-Cultural Analysis 10.1177/014616702236871 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN Oishi / REMEMBERING The Experiencing and Remembering of Well-Being: A Cross-Cultural Analysis Shigehiro Oishi University of Minnesota

More information

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto Session VI, September 20 2017 Learning objectives 1. Get familiar with the basic idea

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Multiple Act criterion:

Multiple Act criterion: Common Features of Trait Theories Generality and Stability of Traits: Trait theorists all use consistencies in an individual s behavior and explain why persons respond in different ways to the same stimulus

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism, Stressor and Stress Response

The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism, Stressor and Stress Response International Journal of Economics and Finance; Vol. 7, No. 8; 2015 ISSN 1916-971X E-ISSN 1916-9728 Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism,

More information

Evaluating the Causal Role of Unobserved Variables

Evaluating the Causal Role of Unobserved Variables Evaluating the Causal Role of Unobserved Variables Christian C. Luhmann (christian.luhmann@vanderbilt.edu) Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University 301 Wilson Hall, Nashville, TN 37203 USA Woo-kyoung

More information

DON M. PALLAIS, CPA 14 Dahlgren Road Richmond, Virginia Telephone: (804) Fax: (804)

DON M. PALLAIS, CPA 14 Dahlgren Road Richmond, Virginia Telephone: (804) Fax: (804) DON M. PALLAIS, CPA 14 Dahlgren Road Richmond, Virginia 23233 Telephone: (804) 784-0884 Fax: (804) 784-0885 Office of the Secretary PCAOB 1666 K Street, NW Washington, DC 20006-2083 Gentlemen: November

More information

WHO Quality of Life. health other than the cause of a disease or the side effects that come along with it. These other

WHO Quality of Life. health other than the cause of a disease or the side effects that come along with it. These other WHO Quality of Life Overview of the WHO Quality of Life As healthcare progresses globally, so does that evolution of scientific research on healthcare assessments and practices. Healthcare services have

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. The evolving concept of subjective well-being: the multifaceted nature of happiness

ARTICLE IN PRESS. The evolving concept of subjective well-being: the multifaceted nature of happiness Creator: Administrator/cipl-u1-3b2-1 Date/Time: 11.8.2003/6:48pm Page: 187/220 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44

More information

Income s Association with Judgments of Life Versus Feelings

Income s Association with Judgments of Life Versus Feelings Income s Association with Judgments of Life Versus Feelings Ed Diener University of Illinois Daniel Kahneman Princeton University William Tov Singapore Management University Raksha Arora The Gallup Organization

More information

Self Determination Theory, COACHE, and Faculty Outcomes in Higher Education. Lisa M. Larson Mack C. Shelley Sandra W. Gahn Matthew Seipel

Self Determination Theory, COACHE, and Faculty Outcomes in Higher Education. Lisa M. Larson Mack C. Shelley Sandra W. Gahn Matthew Seipel Self Determination Theory, COACHE, and Faculty Outcomes in Higher Education Lisa M. Larson Mack C. Shelley Sandra W. Gahn Matthew Seipel Overview ISU COACHE survey through the lens of Self- Determination

More information

Sins of Omission. There are many parts of the brain that determine who a person truly is, but one of the

Sins of Omission. There are many parts of the brain that determine who a person truly is, but one of the Danielle Wickingson Psychology 1010 Dana Erskine Ph.D. 30 November 2012 Sins of Omission There are many parts of the brain that determine who a person truly is, but one of the most important is memory.

More information

The Happiness Index: Issues to be considered when developing a new measure of subjective well-being

The Happiness Index: Issues to be considered when developing a new measure of subjective well-being The Happiness Index: Issues to be considered when developing a new measure of subjective well-being Candidate: Daniel Harris Module: Patterns of Action Dissertation Supervisor: Prof David Clarke On the

More information

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) 1 VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) Thank you for providing us with the opportunity to revise our paper. We have revised the manuscript according to the editors and

More information

Happiness, Pleasure, and Satisfaction

Happiness, Pleasure, and Satisfaction Happiness, Pleasure, and Satisfaction Christopher Rice Lynn University In The Nature and Value of Happiness, Christine Vitrano defines happiness as a mental state in which people feel satisfaction about

More information

Draft Peer Recovery Workers: Guidelines and Practice Standards. Submission: Submitted by to:

Draft Peer Recovery Workers: Guidelines and Practice Standards. Submission: Submitted by  to: ACT Mental Health Consumer Network Inc. The Griffin Centre, Level 2, Room 11 20 Genge Street, Canberra City, 2601 P.O.BOX 469, Civic Square, ACT, 2608 Phone: 02 6230 5796 Fax: 02 6230 5790 Email: policy@actmhcn.org.au

More information

A Cross-cultural Analysis of the Structure of Subjective Well-Being

A Cross-cultural Analysis of the Structure of Subjective Well-Being 1 A Cross-cultural Analysis of the Structure of Subjective Well-Being William A. Stock Morris A. Okun Arizona State University, USA and Juana Gomez Benito University of Barcelona, Spain In order for investigations

More information

OECD work on Subjective Well-being

OECD work on Subjective Well-being OECD work on Subjective Well-being Asia Pacific Conference on Measuring Well- Being and Fostering the Progress of Societies Marco Mira d Ercole OECD Statistics Directorate Tokyo, 5-6 December 2011 Overview

More information

M.A. Alhad & S.S. Turnip Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, Depok, Indonesia

M.A. Alhad & S.S. Turnip Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, Depok, Indonesia Diversity in Unity: Perspectives from Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Ariyanto et al. (Eds) 2018 Taylor & Francis Group, London, ISBN 978-1-138-62665-2 The association between the five-factor model

More information

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity

PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity PLS 506 Mark T. Imperial, Ph.D. Lecture Notes: Reliability & Validity Measurement & Variables - Initial step is to conceptualize and clarify the concepts embedded in a hypothesis or research question with

More information

Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel. University of Miami. Jonah Berger. University of Pennsylvania

Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel. University of Miami. Jonah Berger. University of Pennsylvania Facebook Therapy 1 Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel University of Miami Jonah Berger University of Pennsylvania Word Count: 1000 Contact Information: Jonah

More information

Session 7: Introduction to Pleasant Events and your Mood

Session 7: Introduction to Pleasant Events and your Mood Session 7: Introduction to Pleasant Events and your Mood Session Plan 1. Review of Planning for the Future 2. How Events Affect Your Mood 3. How to Identify Pleasant Events 4. Creating a List of Pleasant

More information

Developing a Comprehensive and One-Dimensional Subjective Well-Being Measurement: Evidence from a Belgian Pilot Survey

Developing a Comprehensive and One-Dimensional Subjective Well-Being Measurement: Evidence from a Belgian Pilot Survey Developing a Comprehensive and One-Dimensional Subjective Well-Being Measurement: Evidence from a Belgian Pilot Survey Marc Hooghe 1 1 University of Leuven (Belgium), e-mail: Marc.Hooghe@soc.kuleuven.be

More information

Chapter 5. Doing Tools: Increasing Your Pleasant Events

Chapter 5. Doing Tools: Increasing Your Pleasant Events 66 Chapter 5. Doing Tools: Increasing Your Pleasant Events The importance of engaging in pleasant events We think most of you would agree that doing things you like typically has a positive effect on your

More information

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Supplemental Materials 1 Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment Adam Hahn 1 Bertram Gawronski 2 Word count: 20,754 excluding acknowledgements, abstract,

More information

Are we having a good time, boys and girls? The need for time and affect methodologies in understanding gendered wellbeing

Are we having a good time, boys and girls? The need for time and affect methodologies in understanding gendered wellbeing University of Wollongong Research Online Faculty of Arts - Papers (Archive) Faculty of Law, Humanities and the Arts 2010 Are we having a good time, boys and girls? The need for time and affect methodologies

More information

The Regression-Discontinuity Design

The Regression-Discontinuity Design Page 1 of 10 Home» Design» Quasi-Experimental Design» The Regression-Discontinuity Design The regression-discontinuity design. What a terrible name! In everyday language both parts of the term have connotations

More information

Flow in computer-human environment

Flow in computer-human environment Flow in computer-human environment Nataša Rogulja 1, Maja Katarina Tomić 2, Diana Kopačević 3 University of Zagreb, The Faculty of Teacher Education, Croatia 1 e-mail: natasa.rogulja@ufzg.hr 2 e-mail:

More information

The Myers Briggs Type Inventory

The Myers Briggs Type Inventory The Myers Briggs Type Inventory Charles C. Healy Professor of Education, UCLA In press with Kapes, J.T. et. al. (2001) A counselor s guide to Career Assessment Instruments. (4th Ed.) Alexandria, VA: National

More information

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing? Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC How are we doing? Prepared by Maura McCool, M.S. Office of Research, Evaluation and Assessment Metropolitan Community Colleges Fall 2003 1 General Education Assessment

More information

Introduction to Positive Psychology 23 rd February Jo Hennessy

Introduction to Positive Psychology 23 rd February Jo Hennessy Introduction to Positive Psychology 23 rd February 2018 Jo Hennessy Objectives To introduce you to the following ideas, with exercises, reflection and discussion: The Health Disease Continuum Aims of Positive

More information

multilevel modeling for social and personality psychology

multilevel modeling for social and personality psychology 1 Introduction Once you know that hierarchies exist, you see them everywhere. I have used this quote by Kreft and de Leeuw (1998) frequently when writing about why, when, and how to use multilevel models

More information

A Survey Method for Characterizing Daily Life Experience: The Day Reconstruction Method

A Survey Method for Characterizing Daily Life Experience: The Day Reconstruction Method A Survey Method for Characterizing Daily Life Experience: The Day Reconstruction Method By Daniel Kahneman, Alan B. Krueger, David A. Schkade, Norbert Schwarz, Arthur A. Stone Science 3 December 2004:

More information

Facets of Personality and the Phenomenology of Autobiographical Memory

Facets of Personality and the Phenomenology of Autobiographical Memory APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 18: 913 930 (2004) Published online 15 June 2004 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.1038 Facets of Personality and the

More information

Emotion regulation and well-being among Puerto Ricans and European Americans

Emotion regulation and well-being among Puerto Ricans and European Americans Emotion regulation and well-being among Puerto Ricans and European Americans Arielle Riutort, McNair Scholar The Pennsylvania State University McNair Faculty Research Advisor: Jose A. Soto, Ph.D Assistant

More information

Events and Subjective Weil-Being: Only Recent Events Matter

Events and Subjective Weil-Being: Only Recent Events Matter Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1996. Vol. 70. No. 5, 1091-1102 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0022-3514/96/S3.00 Events and Subjective Weil-Being: Only Recent

More information

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas 1 Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas Reference: Barchard, K.A. (2002, May). Three subfactors of the empathic personality. Poster presented

More information

10 steps to create more personal relationships and help your clients meet their goals

10 steps to create more personal relationships and help your clients meet their goals 10 steps to create more personal relationships and help your clients meet their goals Individuals are unique and so too are their retirement journeys. The Science of Happiness is about understanding what

More information

A Comparison of the Stability of Measures of Personality Traits, Self-Esteem, Affective Well-Being, and Cognitive Well-being

A Comparison of the Stability of Measures of Personality Traits, Self-Esteem, Affective Well-Being, and Cognitive Well-being A Comparison of the Stability of Measures of Personality Traits, Self-Esteem, Affective Well-Being, and Cognitive Well-being by Ivana Anusic A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the

More information

Likert Scaling: A how to do it guide As quoted from

Likert Scaling: A how to do it guide As quoted from Likert Scaling: A how to do it guide As quoted from www.drweedman.com/likert.doc Likert scaling is a process which relies heavily on computer processing of results and as a consequence is my favorite method

More information

THE SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING CONSTRUCT: A TEST OF ITS CONVERGENT, DISCRIMINANT, AND FACTORIAL VALIDITY

THE SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING CONSTRUCT: A TEST OF ITS CONVERGENT, DISCRIMINANT, AND FACTORIAL VALIDITY Social Indicators Research (2005) 74: 445 476 Ó Springer 2005 DOI 10.1007/s11205-004-8209-6 MARNE L. ARTHAUD-DAY, JOSEPH C. RODE, CHRISTINE H. MOONEY and JANET P. NEAR THE SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING CONSTRUCT:

More information

Application of Psychoanalytic Measures of Personality. the Study of Social Behavior^ Jeanne Watson. Research Center for Group Dynamics

Application of Psychoanalytic Measures of Personality. the Study of Social Behavior^ Jeanne Watson. Research Center for Group Dynamics J The Application of Psychoanalytic Measures of Personality to the Study of Social Behavior^ Jeanne Watson Research Center for Group Dynamics University of Michigan The research which I shall talk about

More information

Personal Growth Interpretation of Goal Attainment as a New Construct Relevant to Well-being

Personal Growth Interpretation of Goal Attainment as a New Construct Relevant to Well-being Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 185 ( 2015 ) 244 249 3rd World Conference on Psychology and Sociology, WCPS- 2014 Personal Growth Interpretation

More information

Trait Meta-Mood and Subjective Happiness: A 7-week Prospective Study

Trait Meta-Mood and Subjective Happiness: A 7-week Prospective Study J Happiness Stud (2011) 12:509 517 DOI 10.1007/s10902-010-9233-7 Trait Meta-Mood and Subjective Happiness: A 7-week Prospective Study Natalio Extremera Jose Martín Salguero Pablo Fernández-Berrocal Published

More information

Value as a Moderator in Subjective Well-Being

Value as a Moderator in Subjective Well-Being Value as a Moderator in Subjective Well-Being Shigehiro Oishi Ed Diener Eunkook Suh Richard E. Lucas University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign ABSTRACT We investigated individual differences in the processes

More information

Marriage Matters: Spousal Similarity in Life Satisfaction

Marriage Matters: Spousal Similarity in Life Satisfaction Pfad: E:/BBU_Daten/ZE/Schmollers Jahrbuch/1-2007/Schimmack ua.3d Pl. 2 1. 2. 2007 Seite 1-7 Schmollers Jahrbuch 127 (2007), 1 7 Duncker & Humblot, Berlin Marriage Matters: Spousal Similarity in Life Satisfaction

More information

Minimizing Uncertainty in Property Casualty Loss Reserve Estimates Chris G. Gross, ACAS, MAAA

Minimizing Uncertainty in Property Casualty Loss Reserve Estimates Chris G. Gross, ACAS, MAAA Minimizing Uncertainty in Property Casualty Loss Reserve Estimates Chris G. Gross, ACAS, MAAA The uncertain nature of property casualty loss reserves Property Casualty loss reserves are inherently uncertain.

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

to Cues Present at Test

to Cues Present at Test 1st: Matching Cues Present at Study to Cues Present at Test 2nd: Introduction to Consolidation Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 05/03/2018: Lecture 06-4 Note: This Powerpoint

More information

Studying the effect of change on change : a different viewpoint

Studying the effect of change on change : a different viewpoint Studying the effect of change on change : a different viewpoint Eyal Shahar Professor, Division of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Mel and Enid Zuckerman College of Public Health, University of Arizona

More information

Well-Being on Planet Earth

Well-Being on Planet Earth Full text presented in this Bibliography of Happiness with permission of the author Psychological Topics 18 (2009), 2, 213-219 Original scientific article UDC 159.9.019.3 159.922.4 Well-Being on Planet

More information

Personality trait change and life satisfaction in adults: The roles of age and hedonic balance

Personality trait change and life satisfaction in adults: The roles of age and hedonic balance University of Wollongong Research Online Faculty of Social Sciences - Papers Faculty of Social Sciences 2013 Personality trait change and life satisfaction in adults: The roles of age and hedonic balance

More information

ISA 540, Auditing Accounting Estimates, Including Fair Value Accounting Estimates, and Related Disclosures Issues and Task Force Recommendations

ISA 540, Auditing Accounting Estimates, Including Fair Value Accounting Estimates, and Related Disclosures Issues and Task Force Recommendations Agenda Item 1-A ISA 540, Auditing Accounting Estimates, Including Fair Value Accounting Estimates, and Related Disclosures Issues and Task Force Recommendations Introduction 1. Since the September 2016

More information

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing This document contains 4 examples of writing and feedback comments from Level 2A lab reports, and 4 steps to help you apply

More information

Choosing an Approach for a Quantitative Dissertation: Strategies for Various Variable Types

Choosing an Approach for a Quantitative Dissertation: Strategies for Various Variable Types Choosing an Approach for a Quantitative Dissertation: Strategies for Various Variable Types Kuba Glazek, Ph.D. Methodology Expert National Center for Academic and Dissertation Excellence Outline Thesis

More information

Regression Discontinuity Analysis

Regression Discontinuity Analysis Regression Discontinuity Analysis A researcher wants to determine whether tutoring underachieving middle school students improves their math grades. Another wonders whether providing financial aid to low-income

More information

Thank you very much for your guidance on this revision. We look forward to hearing from you.

Thank you very much for your guidance on this revision. We look forward to hearing from you. 1 PAINTED BUNTING MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) We would like to submit a revision of our manuscript. As you requested, we have run new studies that employ additional

More information

An Assessment of Life Satisfaction Responses on Recent Statistics Canada Surveys

An Assessment of Life Satisfaction Responses on Recent Statistics Canada Surveys Soc Indic Res DOI 10.1007/s11205-013-0437-1 An Assessment of Life Satisfaction Responses on Recent Statistics Canada Surveys Aneta Bonikowska John F. Helliwell Feng Hou Grant Schellenberg Accepted: 2 September

More information

Play with Your Brain

Play with Your Brain Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level: 5.1 Flesch-Kincaid Reading Ease: 67.1 Drawspace Curriculum 5.2.A1-6 Pages and 1 Illustration Play with Your Brain Answer 50 fun questions to discover your brain s strengths

More information

Self-Evaluation Processes in Life Satisfaction: Uncovering Measurement Non-Equivalence and Age-Related Differences

Self-Evaluation Processes in Life Satisfaction: Uncovering Measurement Non-Equivalence and Age-Related Differences Soc Indic Res (2012) 105:39 61 DOI 10.1007/s11205-010-9762-9 Self-Evaluation Processes in Life Satisfaction: Uncovering Measurement Non-Equivalence and Age-Related Differences Heike Heidemeier Ursula M.

More information

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments Consciousness and Cognition 9, 215 219 (2000) doi:10.1006/ccog.2000.0438, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

More information

Supplementary Materials. Worksite Wellness Programs: Sticks (Not Carrots) Send Stigmatizing Signals

Supplementary Materials. Worksite Wellness Programs: Sticks (Not Carrots) Send Stigmatizing Signals Supplementary Materials Worksite Wellness Programs: Sticks (Not Carrots) Send Stigmatizing Signals David Tannenbaum, Chad J. Valasek, Eric D. Knowles, Peter H. Ditto Additional Mediation Analyses for Study

More information

Volitional Autonomy and Relatedness: Mediators explaining Non Tenure Track Faculty Job. Satisfaction

Volitional Autonomy and Relatedness: Mediators explaining Non Tenure Track Faculty Job. Satisfaction Autonomy and : Mediators explaining Non Tenure Track Faculty Job Satisfaction Non-tenure track (NTT) faculty are increasingly utilized in higher education and shoulder much of the teaching load within

More information

Reliability and Validity

Reliability and Validity Reliability and Validity Why Are They Important? Check out our opening graphics. In a nutshell, do you want that car? It's not reliable. Would you recommend that car magazine (Auto Tester Weakly) to a

More information

Shaping the Relative Importance of Needs from Recent to Early Autobiographical Experiences

Shaping the Relative Importance of Needs from Recent to Early Autobiographical Experiences OHIO JOURNAL OF SCIENCE 61 Shaping the Relative Importance of Needs from Recent to Early Autobiographical Experiences JOSEPH P. GREEN 1 and PATRICK J. CARROLL, The Ohio State University-Lima, Lima, OH,

More information

Big five personality traits and coping styles predict subjective wellbeing: A study with a Turkish Sample

Big five personality traits and coping styles predict subjective wellbeing: A study with a Turkish Sample Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 12 (2011) 426 435 577 581 International Conference on Education and Educational Psychology (ICEEPSY 2010) Big five personality

More information

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences University of Lethbridge Research Repository OPUS Faculty Research and Publications http://opus.uleth.ca Lalumière, Martin 2005 Behavioral genetics: The study of differences Lalumière, Martin L. Department

More information

Locus of Control and Psychological Well-Being: Separating the Measurement of Internal and External Constructs -- A Pilot Study

Locus of Control and Psychological Well-Being: Separating the Measurement of Internal and External Constructs -- A Pilot Study Eastern Kentucky University Encompass EKU Libraries Research Award for Undergraduates 2014 Locus of Control and Psychological Well-Being: Separating the Measurement of Internal and External Constructs

More information

Running Head: TRUST INACCURATE INFORMANTS 1. In the Absence of Conflicting Testimony Young Children Trust Inaccurate Informants

Running Head: TRUST INACCURATE INFORMANTS 1. In the Absence of Conflicting Testimony Young Children Trust Inaccurate Informants Running Head: TRUST INACCURATE INFORMANTS 1 In the Absence of Conflicting Testimony Young Children Trust Inaccurate Informants Kimberly E. Vanderbilt, Gail D. Heyman, and David Liu University of California,

More information

Is Happiness Different From Flourishing? Cross-Country Evidence from the ESS

Is Happiness Different From Flourishing? Cross-Country Evidence from the ESS Is Happiness Different From Flourishing? Cross-Country Evidence from the ESS Andrew E. Clark (Paris School of Economics and IZA) http://www.pse.ens.fr/clark/ ESRI Tokyo. November 30 th 2012. Economics

More information