J S P R. Effects of self-disclosure role on liking, closeness, and other impressions in get-acquainted interactions

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "J S P R. Effects of self-disclosure role on liking, closeness, and other impressions in get-acquainted interactions"

Transcription

1 Article Effects of self-disclosure role on liking, closeness, and other impressions in get-acquainted interactions J S P R Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(4) ª The Author(s) 2012 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalspermissions.nav DOI: / spr.sagepub.com Susan Sprecher 1, Stanislav Treger 2, and Joshua D. Wondra 3 Abstract We experimentally examined whether giving or receiving self-disclosure leads to greater liking and other positive impressions (e.g., closeness) in initial interactions. We also contributed to a recent debate about the familiarity-attraction link by examining whether knowledge about another leads to greater (or lesser) degrees of liking and perceived similarity. Pairs of unacquainted undergraduates completed a structured self-disclosure task. We randomly assigned one participant to disclose while the other listened in a first interaction; participants switched roles for a second interaction. After the first interaction, listeners (vs. disclosers) reported more liking and other positive interpersonal impressions. These differences disappeared after participants switched roles in the second interaction. Furthermore, listening was associated with greater degrees of perceived similarity. Keywords Acquaintanceship, familiarity, liking, self-disclosure, similarity, social interaction Scholars have long recognized the importance of initial interactions in determining the likelihood that relationships develop (Berscheid & Regan, 2005). The first impressions 1 Illinois State University, USA 2 DePaul University, USA 3 University of Michigan, USA Corresponding author: Susan Sprecher, Department of Sociology & Anthropology, Illinois State University, Schroeder Hall, Normal, IL , USA. Sprecher@ilstu.edu

2 498 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(4) that occur in initial interactions can influence not only whether further contact is sought but also the trajectory of relationship development. Even brief one-time interactions are important because they can help satisfy the human need to belong and be connected with others (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and can elevate positive affect (Vittengl & Holt, 2000). Higher levels of self-disclosure and quality communication generally lead to more liking and closeness (e.g., Aron, Melinat, Aron, Vallone, & Bator, 1997; Sprecher & Duck, 1994). In the self-disclosure process, however, there are two roles: the role of the discloser and the role of the disclosure recipient (Collins & Miller, 1994; Derlega, Winstead, & Greene, 2009). The discloser shares personal information, whereas the recipient listens and receives information. Turn-taking or reciprocity in disclosure is common in interactions (e.g., Dindia, 2000; Rubin, Hill, Peplau, & Dunkel-Schetter, 1980), but selfdisclosure can also be imbalanced and such situations highlight the distinction between the two roles. Increasing Internet communication, including through dating websites, webcams, personal blogs, and social network forums and posts, can potentially lead to imbalances in self-disclosure and in knowledge exchanged in the get-acquainted process (Sprecher, 2009). In addition, even in interactions that involve relatively rapid switches between the two roles, how the two sides of disclosure differentially impact the development of attraction and the relationship is unknown. Although recent research has begun to explore the effects of communication medium on attraction and the acquaintance process (McKenna, Green, & Gleason, 2002; Ramirez & Zhang, 2007; Schouten, Valkenburg, & Peter, 2009; for a review, see Nguyen, Bin, & Campbell, 2012), how the role in self-disclosure (giving vs. receiving) affects attraction and closeness is relatively unexplored as yet. In the present study, we used the get-acquainted paradigm, which involved two actual participants, strangers to each other, interacting in a controlled situation. Our paired participants took turns being in each disclosure role while they were engaged in a structured self-disclosure exercise that was adapted from previous research (Aron et al., 1997; Sedikides, Campbell, Reeder, & Elliot, 1999). After each segment of the interaction, the participants completed measures of liking, closeness, enjoyment of the interaction, and perceived similarity. Systematically separating the discloser and recipient roles allowed us to address two issues. First, we address a gap in the self-disclosure literature on the differential effects of receiving versus giving disclosure on liking (e.g., Collins & Miller, 1994). Second, we contribute to a recent debate in the social psychological literature: whether more information (or knowledge) about another, such as that obtained through the other s self-disclosure, is associated with greater liking and perceived similarity, or alternatively, with less liking and greater perceived dissimilarity (Norton, Frost, & Ariely, 2007, 2011; Reis, Maniaci, Caprariello, Eastwick, & Finkel, 2011a, 2011b). We discuss both of these issues next. The self-disclosure liking effects Self-disclosure is associated with attraction and relationship satisfaction in both developing and developed relationships (for reviews, see Collins & Miller, 1994; Derlega et al., 2009). In a review of disclosure-liking links, Collins and Miller (1994)

3 Sprecher et al. 499 distinguished between the effect of liking those who disclose to us (other s disclosure! own liking; i.e., being in the recipient role) and the effect of liking those to whom we disclose (own disclosure! own liking; i.e., being in the discloser role). They also discussed a third disclosure-liking effect: people disclose to those whom they like. Although Collins and Miller (1994) found support for all three disclosure-liking effects in their meta-analysis, they noted that many studies confounded the effects. In addition, they identified only five studies that provided a test of the effect of own disclosure on own liking, and these studies were limited in various ways. For example, they primarily involved participants exchanging written messages or recordings to a confederate or participants being in the role of a passive observer to a disclosure exchange (Archer, Berg, & Runge, 1980; Berg & Archer, 1983; Burger, 1981; Kleinke, 1975; McAllister, 1980). The design of our study, which involved actual participants taking turns disclosing (and receiving disclosure), offered the ability to examine (in an ecologically valid way) the relative effects of receiving self-disclosure versus giving self-disclosure on liking and other interpersonal impressions. Several theoretical models can explain the effect of receiving disclosure on liking (Collins & Miller, 1994). One may propose, however, that these theories are nested within the general framework of familiarity-based liking or the most basic principle of attraction (Berscheid & Reis, 1998, p. 205). From an uncertainty reduction theory perspective (Berger & Calabrese, 1975), for example, receiving disclosure from another may lead to liking based on the degree to which uncertainty (i.e., ambiguity) about the other wanes throughout the interaction. Using an information-processing perspective, one may predict that the information gained from the other s disclosure, particularly when it is positive, can lead to positive beliefs or impressions of the discloser (Collins & Miller, 1994). From a social exchange perspective, receiving disclosure from another can be rewarding because the disclosure suggests liking from the other (Altman & Taylor, 1973). There are also theoretical explanations for the effect of giving disclosure on liking. As noted by Collins and Miller (1994), the process of self-disclosing can be intrinsically gratifying as well as cathartic, and these feelings can become associated with the target of one s disclosure and generate affectionate feelings toward the other (Jourard, 1971; Pennebaker, 1985). In addition, self-perception theory (e.g., Bem, 1972) suggests that people make inferences about their thoughts and feelings based on their behavior; therefore, the act of disclosing to another can lead to the inference that one likes and trusts the other (e.g., Chaikin & Derlega, 1974). When examined at a closer level, one side of the self-disclosure is likely to be more strongly linked to liking in get-acquainted interactions than the other; this side is receiving self-disclosure. Specifically, upon receiving disclosure from another, one is able to learn knowledge about the other, reduce uncertainty, and gain familiarity; in turn, liking is likely to increase. Being in the role of a discloser can be rewarding and gratifying, but cannot lead to the establishment of familiarity to the same extent. We propose, therefore, that receiving self-disclosure (in comparison to giving self-disclosure) leads to more liking and other positive interpersonal impressions (e.g., feelings of closeness). Findings from a correlational study of dating couples offer some support for this hypothesis. Sprecher (1987) asked both members of a sample of dating couples to indicate how much they had disclosed to their partner and how much they believed their partners had

4 500 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(4) disclosed to them. Sprecher found that perceived disclosure received explained more unique variance in affection than perceived disclosure given. Could information learned in get-acquainted self-disclosure lead to less liking, on average? We now turn to our second issue, which is to address a controversy in recent literature concerning whether information-based familiarity could lead to less (rather than more) liking. Our prediction that receiving self-disclosure will lead to more liking than giving self-disclosure reflects the idea that familiarity is one of the foundations of liking (Berscheid & Reis, 1998; Laurenceau & Kleinman, 2006; Reis et al., 2011a, 2011b) and knowledge about another is one of the key components of having a close relationship (e.g., Miller, 2011). This basic principle, however, has recently been called into question. Norton et al. (2007) proposed that in a majority of initial interactions, learning more information about another will lead to perceptions of dissimilarity and a decrease in liking. Their less is more hypothesis proposes that a lack of knowledge about another person facilitates initial liking due to a lure of ambiguity. That is, initial impressions of others are often positive because limited information can lead to the assumption of similarity. Once information is obtained about the other, however, the lure of ambiguity subsides and perceptions of dissimilarity increase, leading to less liking for the other. As a consequence, the less information people know about a person in get-acquainted interactions, the more they will like him or her, on average. Norton et al. (2007) found support for their less is more hypothesis in five studies. In several trait evaluation studies, for example, people liked a hypothetical person less when they were given more traits describing that person (e.g., polite, reliable, and stubborn ); in another study, the researchers found evidence that this effect was mediated by perceived dissimilarity. In a more naturalistic study, they found that online daters who just went on a date liked the date less and perceived less similarity to that date (although reported more knowledge of the other) than those who were about to go on a date. Norton et al. concluded that greater knowledge of another will lead to perceptions of lesser (vs. greater) similarity and liking for the other. Although Norton et al. acknowledged that the positive association between familiarity and liking may stem mostly from naturalistic relationships such as those between partners and friends, they believed that if two strangers are randomly paired together, they should on average like one another less as they learn more about one another. Norton et al. s (2007) challenge of the classic idea that familiarity leads to attraction led to a debate of their findings. Reis et al. (2011a) questioned Norton et al. s research and conclusions, in part, because evaluating a stranger based on a decontextualized list of traits may misrepresent the impact of information (or familiarity) in naturalistic social interactions. 1 Reis et al. conducted two live-interaction experiments that tested the classic familiarity-liking principle. In their first study, previously unacquainted same-sex dyads reported more liking after discussing six versus two random topics. In their second study, the more times that previously unacquainted dyads briefly chatted online (e.g., 8 vs. 4), the more they liked one another. In addition, a greater number of chats was associated with greater perceived knowledge of the other, greater perception of being known

5 Sprecher et al. 501 by the other, greater satisfaction with the chats, greater desire to get together, and more actual attempts to get together. Reis et al. concluded that familiarity can still be considered the pantheon of social-psychological processes that promote attraction (2011a, p. 567). In a follow-up commentary, Norton et al. (2011) defended their findings by proposing that familiarity will lead to dislike on average but not in every case. They also acknowledged that more research is needed on this topic, including research that focuses on information-based familiarity (in contrast to a feeling of familiarity, which has been the focus of most prior research on familiarity effects; e.g., Zajonc, 1968). The current study In our study, participants took turns disclosing and receiving disclosure in a structured interaction. Participants were in one role (either discloser or recipient) for the first interaction segment; participants then switched roles for a second interaction segment. Because knowledge gained from receiving disclosure should increase familiarity and liking, we predicted that initial recipients would like their partners more than would initial disclosers; likewise, we expected that initial recipients would also report experiencing other positive interpersonal impressions (closeness and enjoyment) to a greater degree (Hypothesis 1). However, we expected that the differences in liking and other positive interpersonal impressions between participants in the two disclosure roles would disappear after the second segment of interaction (Hypothesis 2). We expected the difference to wane after interaction 2 for reasons similar to why we predicted a difference between disclosure roles after interaction 1. Initial disclosers would be unable to learn much about their partners; thus, they would report less liking than initial recipients. When the initial disclosers become recipients, however, they would be able to learn more about their partners and thus establish more liking, whereas disclosers (who were initial recipients) would not be able to learn more about their partners, which should lead their liking to plateau after the second interaction. Our first and second hypotheses reflect the theoretical proposition that familiarity is one of the foundations of interpersonal liking (Berscheid & Reis, 1998; Laurenceau & Kleinman, 2006; Reis et al., 2011a, 2011b). However, as noted above, some have recently questioned this familiarity-liking hypothesis (Norton et al., 2007, 2011). The design of our study offers a test of the effects of information-based familiarity on attraction within a live social interaction and with experimental controls. If there is support for Norton et al. s (2007, 2011) less is more hypothesis, then on average, disclosers should be liked less than recipients, given that the recipients are not providing any information about themselves. In addition, initial disclosers should report a decrease in liking for the other when they transition to being recipients, due to the increase in information received. However, we had earlier hypothesized just the opposite that initial disclosure recipients would experience more liking than would initial disclosers; and initial disclosers would increase in their liking when they become recipients. Therefore, we can test these competing hypotheses. To further test Norton et al. s (2007) hypothesis, we also consider how informationbased familiarity influences perceived similarity. Norton et al. (2007) argued that as people learn more information about another, their perception of similarity to the other will

6 502 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(4) decrease, on average. We expected, in contrast, that learning more information would be associated with increases (not decreases) in perceptions of similarity. We thus hypothesized that self-disclosure recipients would perceive greater similarity with the other than would disclosers (Hypothesis 3a). We also hypothesized that the more information recipients perceived to have received from the other, the more they would perceive that they are similar to the other (Hypothesis 3b). Method Sample Participants were undergraduate students (81.4% females) from a large Midwestern University in the United States, recruited primarily through the psychology participant pool. The mean age of the participants was years (SD ¼ 1.91). A majority (80.2%)of the participants identified themselves as White; 10.3% were Black, 5.2% were Hispanic/ Latino, and 4.3% were Asian. Although our sample was convenient, it was drawn from a student body that is fairly diverse; for example, students come from a Midwest US metropolis and its suburbs, as well as from rural Midwest communities. Procedure Overview. Because a large majority of the participants were female, only female female (n ¼ 37) and female male (n ¼ 22) dyads were formed. Two participants were scheduled to report for the experiment at the same time, each to a different room in a university building. Each participant was greeted by his or her own experimenter, who directed the participant to first read an informed consent form and a set of directions. Then, the experimenters instructed their participants to complete an online survey that asked for the background information (e.g., sex and race) and measured other variables not to be considered in this study. Upon completing the pre-interaction measures, the pair engaged in the first 12-min interaction segment conducted over Skype (web-based communication program). 3 After the first interaction segment, both participants completed a postinteraction survey (PS-1), which assessed their reactions to the first interaction. Then, a second 12-min interaction segment occurred (also over Skype), followed by another post-interaction survey (PS-2), which assessed their reactions to the second interaction. The self-disclosure task. We adapted items from both the Closeness-Generating Inventory (Aron et al., 1997) and the Relationship Closeness Induction Task (Sedikides et al., 1999) for the self-disclosure task used in this study. The questions were presented to the participants in three sets, with each set increasing in degree of intimacy. 4 Participants were given 4 mins for each set of questions; thus, two versions were created with three sets of items that increased in intimacy. One version was used for segment 1 (i.e., the first interaction) and the second version was used for segment 2 (i.e., the second interaction). Example questions included: If you could travel anywhere in the world, where would you go and why? (set 1), Is there something you ve dreamed of doing for a long time? Why haven t you done it? (set 2); and What is one of your biggest fears? (set 3).

7 Sprecher et al. 503 The disclosure role. Random assignment determined which participant in each pair was in the discloser role and which participant was in the recipient role during the first interaction segment. The roles were then reversed in the second segment. Participants read directions about what was expected in each role, including instructions for those in the recipient role to not disclose. Those in the recipient role, however, were told that they could engage in nonverbal communication, such as head nods, eye contact, and smiles. Participants were aware at the beginning of the study that they would be switching roles later in the session. The Skype interactions. When both the participants were ready to begin the interaction, the participant in the initial recipient role was given one version of the self-disclosure questions. The experimenters ensured that their participants were able to see and hear each other through Skype and then left the room to give participants privacy. Participants were given a maximum of 4 mins to interact using each set of questions; they were instructed to notify the experimenter if they completed the questions in less than 4 mins (although most dyads used the full 4 mins). Once the dyad completed a set of questions, an experimenter returned to direct the participant in the recipient role to advance to the second set of questions. 5 After the second set, the experimenter again instructed the recipient to advance to the third set of questions. After the participants had finished with the third set of items, they completed the first post-interaction questionnaire. When each participant had completed the questionnaire, the second interaction segment began. The segment 2 interaction repeated the same procedure, although the participants switched disclosure roles, and a second version of self-disclosure questions was used. At the end of the second interaction segment, participants completed the second post-interaction survey. Finally, they were debriefed and thanked for their participation. Measures The following measures were used in both post-interaction surveys (PS-1 and PS-2). Liking. We assessed liking for the other with three items. One item was: How much did you like the other? (1 ¼ not at all,7¼ a great deal). The other two items were adapted from Byrne s (1971) Interpersonal Judgment Scale. The first item asked about general feelings (1 ¼ I feel that I would probably dislike this person very much, 7¼ I feel that I would probably like this person very much), and the second item asked participants about their desire to work with the person again in an experiment (1 ¼ I believe that I would very much dislike working with this person in an experiment, 7¼ I believe that I would very much enjoy working with this person in an experiment). The a was.78 in PS-1 and.82 in PS-2. Closeness. Closeness to the other was assessed with the Inclusion of Other in the Self (IOS) Venn diagram (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992). This is a simple, validated, and commonly used scale to measure closeness (e.g., see Aron et al., 1992; Fraley & Aron, 2004). 6

8 504 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(4) Enjoyment of interaction. We assessed the enjoyment of interaction with four items as follows: (1) How much did you enjoy the interaction in Stage 1 (or 2)? (2) How much did you enjoy your role in the interaction? (3) How much did you and the other laugh during the interaction? ; and (4) How much fun was the interaction? (1 ¼ not at all, 7¼ a great deal). The a was.84 in PS-1 and.85 in PS-2. Perceived similarity. We assessed perceived similarity with two items: How much do you think you have in common with the other person? (1 ¼ nothing or almost nothing,7¼ a great deal) and How similar do you think you and the other are likely to be? (1 ¼ not at all, 7¼ a great deal). The a was.82 in PS-1 and.92 in PS-2. Perceived information received and given. We measured the perceived information received (for those in the recipient role) with three items as follows: (1) How much did the other tell you about himself or herself? ; (2) How much personal or intimate information did the other share with you? ; and (3) How honest and open was the other in his or her responses? (1 ¼ not at all,7¼ a great deal). We measured perceived information given (for those in the discloser role) with a similar set of three items (e.g., How much personal or intimate information did you share with the other? ). The a was.75 for perceived disclosure received and.69 for perceived disclosure given in PS-1. The as were.79 and.75, respectively, in PS-2. Results Preliminary analyses After the first segment of interaction, participants, on the whole, reported a moderate amount of liking for the other (M ¼ 5.59, SD ¼ 0.76), felt at least some closeness to the other (M ¼ 3.60, SD ¼ 1.45), and generally enjoyed the interaction (M ¼ 4.76, SD ¼ 1.07). In preliminary analyses, female female pairs were found to have higher scores on liking, closeness, and perceived similarity than male female pairs, but neither the sex composition of the dyad nor the sex of participant was found to interact with any of the major variables in our analysis, and therefore, we ignore sex in the analyses below. 7 Does self-disclosure received or self-disclosure given lead to more liking? We hypothesized that initial recipients would like their partners more, report more enjoyment, and feel closer to their partners after the first segment of interaction than would initial disclosers (H1). We also predicted that after the participants switched roles, their interpersonal impressions would not differ because initial disclosers would increase in their liking for the other after they became a recipient of disclosure (H2). Thus, we expected a disclosure role interaction segment interaction. Given the statistical interdependence of the recipients and disclosers responses, we treated the dyad as the unit of analysis. Therefore, we tested our hypotheses with a repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). Initial disclosure role (discloser vs. recipient)

9 Sprecher et al. 505 Table 1. Comparisons of initial disclosure recipients and initial disclosers across both interaction segments. Segment Measure Initial recipient M (SE) Initial discloser M (SE) Effect size (d) 1 Liking 5.73 (0.10) 5.43 (0.11) 0.32 Enjoyment 4.98 (0.14) 4.56 (0.14) 0.34 Closeness 3.95 (0.20) 3.25 (0.18) Liking 5.83 (0.09) 5.77 (0.12) 0.07 Enjoyment 5.17 (0.14) 5.28 (0.12) 0.11 Closeness 4.37 (0.19) 3.98 (0.19) 0.21 Note. The initial recipient during segment 1 became the discloser during segment 2, and vice versa for the initial discloser. In segment 1, all ps.012; in segment 2, all ps.096. Table 2. Increases in the degree of interpersonal impressions from segment 1 to segment 2 for participants in each disclosure role, as identified by effect size d. Measured d (Initial recipient) d (Initial discloser) Liking ** Enjoyment ** Closeness 0.43* 0.73** Note. Initial recipient was the person who received disclosure during segment 1 and disclosed during segment 2. Original discloser was the person who disclosed during segment 1 and received disclosure during segment 2. *p <.01 **p <.001, as indicated by results of a dependent samples test. and interaction segment (segment 1 vs. segment 2) were treated as within-dyad independent variables. Ratings of liking, enjoyment, and closeness were the collective dependent variables. As expected, we found a statistically significant and fairly large interaction effect of disclosure role and interaction segment, Wilks L ¼.80, F(3, 54) ¼ 4.54, p ¼.007, partial Z 2 ¼.20. In support of H1, simple effects analyses revealed that after interaction segment 1, recipients reported liking their partners more, enjoying the interaction more, and feeling closer to their partners than did disclosers (see Table 1). In support of H2, however, these differences disappeared after segment 2. Initial disclosers generally experienced medium-sized increases in the degree of all three interpersonal impressions between segments 1 and 2 (see Table 2). Initial recipients, however, generally experienced relatively small-sized increases in the degree of their interpersonal impressions between segments 1 and 2 (see Table 2). Further supporting the conclusion that the initial disclosers catch up to the initial recipients in their liking after they switch roles, the main effect of initial disclosure role was nonsignificant. That is, there were no differences in the average reports of liking, closeness, and enjoyment, and perceived similarity between initial disclosers and initial recipients across the two interaction segments, Wilks L ¼.89, F(3, 54) ¼ 2.26, p ¼.09, partial Z 2 ¼.11. In addition, the main effect of interaction segment was, as expected, statistically significant and large, Wilks L ¼.53, F(3, 54) ¼ 15.80, p <.001, partial Z 2 ¼.47. The degree of liking and other

10 506 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(4) measures of interpersonal impressions increased over time. More specifically, compared to the participants scores after interaction 1, participants scores after interaction 2 were higher for liking (Ms ¼ 5.58 vs. 5.80; d ¼ 0.38), closeness (Ms ¼ 3.60 vs. 4.18; d ¼ 0.58), and enjoyment of the interaction (Ms ¼ 4.77 vs. 5.22; d ¼ 0.44). This main effect, however, was qualified by the interaction effect discussed above. Does information-based familiarity lead to more or less liking? The analyses we described above also serve as initial tests of the second purpose of this study, which was to examine whether information-based familiarity leads to greater or lesser liking. Given that initial recipients reported more liking and other positive interpersonal impressions than initial disclosers, our results support the familiarity-based liking hypothesis (Reis et al., 2011a). To further test Norton et al. s (2007) hypothesis, we also considered how informationbased familiarity influences perceived similarity. Norton et al. (2007) argued that as people learn more information about another, their perception of similarity to the other will decrease, on average. We hypothesized, in contrast, that initial disclosure recipients would perceive greater similarity with the other than would initial disclosers (H3a) and that the more information disclosure recipients perceived that they received, the more similarity they would perceive with the other (H3b). In support of H3a, initial recipients (M ¼ 4.29, SD ¼ 0.92) reported more perceived similarity after the first interaction than did initial disclosers (M ¼ 3.75, SD ¼ 1.12), t(57) ¼ 3.13, p ¼.003, d ¼ Given the statistical interdependence of the data, we tested H3b by examining the correlation between the recipients perceived disclosure received and perceived similarity (i.e., actor effects) while controlling for disclosers perceived disclosure given and perceived similarity (i.e., partner effects). In support of H3b, the more that those in the recipient condition perceived that the other had disclosed to them, the more they reported being similar to the other, r partial (52) ¼.28, p ¼.043. To examine Norton et al. s (2007) less is more hypothesis in more detail, we conducted a mediation analysis to explore whether perceived similarity mediated the relationship between the degree of information that those in the disclosure recipient condition perceived that they received and their degree of liking for the other. If the less is more hypothesis holds, we should observe that greater degree of disclosure leads to less similarity, which should in turn lead to less liking. If support for the classic, or more is more hypothesis holds, as we expect, then we should observe that the degree of disclosure is positively related to degree of similarity, which in turn should lead to greater liking. We tested these competing ideas with a bootstrapped mediation test using 20,000 resamples (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). Given the statistical interdependence in the data, and given our interest in the actor effects of the recipients, we controlled for partner effects (i.e., degree of disclosers disclosure given, perceived similarity, and liking). The mediation test revealed support for the classic view of familiarity-induced liking. Perceived similarity indeed partially mediated the positive relation between the perceived degree of disclosure received and liking (95% confidence interval of indirect effect: 0.01, 0.19). That is, the more disclosure people perceived to have received from their

11 Sprecher et al. 507 partners, the more similar they perceived them to be (b ¼ 0.24, SE ¼ 0.11, p ¼ 0.038), which in turn bolstered liking (b ¼ 0.29, SE ¼ 0.10, p ¼.004; c path of direct effect: b ¼ 0.25, SE ¼.08, p <.001; c-prime path of direct effect: b ¼ 0.21, SE ¼ 0.08, p ¼.01). Our mediation analysis thus suggests that familiarity may breed liking due in part to greater degrees of perceived similarity. Discussion Self-disclosure and liking In this experiment, we examined the relationship between self-disclosure and liking. Our study extended the literature on the get-acquainted process by examining which side of the self-disclosure process receiving or giving is more positively associated with liking (e.g., Collins & Miller, 1994; Sprecher, 1987). We also examined whether the two sides of disclosure lead to differential degrees of enjoyment of the interaction and closeness to the other. We hypothesized that being in the recipient role would lead to greater liking (and other positive impressions) than being in the role of discloser. In support of our first hypothesis, after the first segment of the interaction, participants who received disclosure liked the partner more, enjoyed the interaction more, and felt closer to the partner than participants who disclosed. We further hypothesized that the differential effects of giving versus receiving disclosure on liking would lead to differences in interpersonal outcomes, and impressions between partners would disappear after switching roles. In support of our second hypothesis, participants who began in the disclosure role experienced greater liking, enjoyment, and closeness after they shifted to receiving disclosure during the second segment of the interaction; those who began in the recipient role and disclosed during the second segment experienced negligible changes in liking and enjoyment. Thus, we found that receiving disclosure leads to more liking than giving disclosure. Our experiment was the first (to our knowledge) to systematically separate the two sides of self-disclosure in a way that did not confound the two roles and that involved actual participants (rather than one side of the disclosure being fabricated written material or presented by a confederate). This was done by randomly assigning participants to one of the two disclosure roles for an initial interaction, with participants switching disclosure roles for a second interaction. We assessed liking and other impressions after each interaction. Our results replicate the findings of previous correlational work (Sprecher, 1987) that receiving self-disclosure is uniquely associated with more liking than giving self-disclosure. Prior self-disclosure literature (e.g., Collins & Miller, 1994) has provided theoretical proposals suggesting that both sides of self-disclosure should be rewarding and lead to liking, closeness, and enjoyment. Likewise, the results of our study indicated that both partners generally experienced high levels of liking for the other and enjoyment of the interaction. Furthermore, replicating previous research on generating closeness within brief social interactions (e.g., Aron et al., 1992, 1997; Fraley & Aron, 2004), at least some closeness was reported by the participants after the structured self-disclosure activity. Although both sides of self-disclosure may be rewarding (Altman & Taylor, 1973), receiving disclosure leads to knowledge and information-based familiarity of the other.

12 508 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(4) Familiarity has been identified as one of the most essential components in the development of intimacy (Laurenceau & Kleinman, 2006). In addition, the other s sharing of information can lead to liking by increasing positive beliefs about the other, including about the other s similarity to oneself. To further discuss this point, we turn to the less is more hypothesis (Norton et al., 2007, 2011). Does learning more about others lead to more or less liking? Norton et al. s (2007, 2011) less is more hypothesis makes the prediction that more knowledge acquired about another typically leads to less liking due to decreases in perceived similarity. More specifically, they posit that when people first meet others they assume more similarity than may be present. These initial perceptions of similarity lead to strong initial liking (Berscheid & Reis, 1998; Montoya, Horton, & Kirchner, 2008), but as one learns more about the other the initial illusion of similarity vanishes and liking decreases. In contrast, we hypothesized that disclosure recipients, who learned more about their interaction partners than disclosers, would report greater perceived similarity with their partners. We also hypothesized that more perceived information received would be related to greater perceived similarity. Our results demonstrated in multiple ways that a greater degree of disclosure (or information) from the other was associated with greater (not lesser) liking and perceived similarity. First, those who were in the initial recipient role perceived greater similarity to the other than those in the initial discloser role. Second, the more that those in the initial recipient role perceived that the other had disclosed to them, the greater their perceptions of similarity to their disclosing partners. In other words, information received from the other was associated with greater and not lesser perceived similarity. Finally, participants who started in the discloser role increased in their degree of perceived similarity and liking as they transitioned to the second segment of the interaction, when they learned more about their partners through the disclosure process. Thus, our findings seem to be incongruent with Norton et al. s (2007) less is more hypothesis. Similar to Reis et al. (2011a), we found that, on average, greater information acquisition about another person leads to greater liking. Reis et al. noted that their study was the first true experiment that examined the familiarity-leading-to attraction effect in a context that involved actual participants interacting. Our experimental study, using the role in disclosure (receiving vs. giving) as the manipulation (rather than the number of chats, as did Reis et al.) provides another strong experimental test that familiarity (particularly information-based familiarity) is associated with greater liking. Limitations and directions for future research Although we found that both sides of self-disclosure generally lead to positive interpersonal impressions, we do not doubt that there may be certain contexts, stages of relationship development, or types of information that could result in a weaker positive association or even a negative association between more information (disclosure) and perceptions of similarity and liking. For example, one question that we did not address is how

13 Sprecher et al. 509 interaction-relevant goals affect perceptions of similarity and liking. More specifically, the standard by which similarity is judged may differ in early interactions with a person considered for a casual acquaintance versus a potential romantic partner. It is plausible that the standards for potential romantic partners, especially committed or long-term partners, are quite stringent (e.g., Buunk, Dijkstra, Fetchenhauer, & Kenrick, 2002; Sprecher & Regan, 2002). There may be few people who approach the standard of similar enough when they are considered as potential romantic partners. Most people may be perceived as too dissimilar to be a compatible romantic partner. Furthermore, the variability in similarity dissimilarity in our sample may have been restricted based on the relative homogeneity of the participants in terms of gender, ethnicity, age, cultural upbringing, and other demographic factors. This homogeneity may have resulted in participants not engaging in conversations that would reveal major degrees of dissimilarity. Nevertheless, it is not clear that our participants were motivated to focus primarily on their similarities during the interaction. People are certainly not only motivated to fit in with a group but are also motivated to maintain a level of distinctiveness (Hornsey & Hogg, 1999; Leonardelli, Pickett, & Brewer, 2010). It is unclear to what extent disclosers would try to relate based on apparent similarities and to what extent they would try to distinguish themselves from those similarities. Our participants were somewhat constrained in their responses by the questions that were provided to add a degree of experimental control; these questions were selected from lists that are intended to induce interpersonal closeness (Aron et al., 1997; Sedikides et al., 1999). Does a relatively homogenous sample respond to structured questions with relatively homogenous answers? Future research can examine the role of connection and distinctiveness motives in initial acquaintance. Given that conversation in ecological settings is reciprocal (which may lead people to experience frustration when interacting with those who talk excessively), our legitimization of one-sided disclosure in an experimental context is another limitation. There are two important points that address this concern. First, we are not trying to tell a story about one-sided disclosure, rather, we were interested in examining the relative effects of giving and receiving disclosure on liking in general. The use of one-sided disclosure was of methodological importance to avoid confounding giving disclosure with receiving disclosure. The convergence of our findings with correlational research in natural communication settings (Sprecher, 1987) favors our interpretation that the effects of receiving disclosure on liking are stronger than the effects of giving disclosure. Second, although one could argue that legitimizing one-sided disclosure experimentally inflated the effects of disclosure on liking, the counterargument could be leveled that the observed effect was artificially weak because of listeners inferences about why their partners were disclosing. More specifically, in natural settings, listeners may feel that disclosers are communicating trust, a desire for intimacy, or liking by disclosing; in our study, listeners may have felt that disclosers were compelled to speak due to experimental demands. Nevertheless, social-psychological research on correspondence bias demonstrates that the participants are often insensitive to the experimental demands placed on the partners in a research setting (Gilbert & Jones, 1986). Therefore, it still may be plausible to study natural disclosure processes in experiments using one-sided disclosure methods.

14 510 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 30(4) We suspect that the context in which communication occurs introduces important moderators for the effects of information exchange on liking and general person perception. One conceptual umbrella that might fit this purpose is perceived partner responsiveness (e.g., Reis, Clark, & Holmes, 2004; Reis et al., 2011a). For example, if the discloser believes that a listener is responsive during an interaction, then the listener s responsiveness may facilitate rapport and even further disclosure (Reis & Patrick, 1996). Notably, the recent controversy surrounding the effects of receiving information on liking (Norton et al., 2007) has treated partner responsiveness and perceived similarity as potential mediators. The mediation models that have been tested imply that receiving information has a direct causal effect on partner responsiveness (Reis et al., 2011a) or perceived similarity (Norton et al., 2007), which in turn has a direct effect on liking. As an alternative, models of moderation could be examined in which the effects of information given or received on liking depend on the degree of responsiveness or perceived similarity in the interaction. As a general point, the effects of disclosure on liking have been presented independent from the content of disclosure (Collins & Miller, 1994; Sprecher, 1987). If that content is negative (e.g., disclosures about past problems, statements about things not liked about the other), then it may be the case that receiving disclosure would negatively impact liking more than giving disclosure. In other words, receiving disclosure may affect liking more than giving disclosure regardless of whether the change is positive or negative. In addition, another contextual variable to consider in future research is the medium of communication. All of our dyads interacted over web-based Skype. The Hyperpersonal model (Walther, 1996) suggests that the levels of self-disclosure can be more intense and impressions formed in interactions can be more idealized in computer-mediated communication than in face-to-face interactions. Although the theory was developed in regard to text-based computer-mediated communication, the same processes may occur in web-based video or audio communication. Future research could thus examine how the differential effects of receiving versus giving self-disclosure are affected by the medium of communication. More generally, future research can examine the many moderators, from medium of communication to content of disclosure, that are likely to affect the disclosure-liking relationship. Conclusion Self-disclosure is a natural and important facet of relationship development and maintenance, as it can promote liking and rapport between relationship partners, regardless of whether the two persons are strangers or acquaintances (Collins & Miller, 1994). Our study suggests that the one who listens to another s disclosure may experience more liking (and enjoyment, closeness) than the one who discloses. Our study also indicates, however, that once there is an opportunity to reciprocate self-disclosure, not only do these differences disappear but also liking further increases. Naturalistic conversations generally entail both sides of disclosure, which allows a target to simultaneously learn about the other while allowing the other to learn more about the target himself or herself. Thus, perhaps more actually is more; the more people know about others, the more they desire to be connected with them.

15 Sprecher et al. 511 Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank Lyndsey Estes, Jacqueline Gray, Ashley Salzman, and Amy Stupay for serving as research assistants on the project. S.T. and J.D.W. were graduate students at Illinois State University when the research was conducted. Funding This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. Notes 1. Reis et al. also questioned Norton et al. s comparison (in their final study) of online daters who were about to go on a date with those online daters who had just been on a date. As noted by Reis et al., the second group likely had returned to the website because a date had not gone well; and as a consequence, they were looking for a new partner. 2. This was after deleting 12 participants who had to be paired with a confederate because the other assigned participant did not arrive and deleting one pair because both participants indicated that they had interacted previously. Although the data from participants who were paired with a confederate were not included in the analyses, separate analyses that included these 12 participants yielded similar results to those presented. 3. We decided to have the communication occur over Skype, with the participants in different rooms, to reduce the likelihood that the participants will deviate from their roles and to retain the focus of their reactions on the verbal communication itself rather than on other aspects of face-to-face interactions, such as proximity to each other, the possibility that one would touch the other, and body language. The exchanges were not audio- or video-taped. 4. A few items overlapped between the two versions of disclosure prompts administered in segments 1 and 2. These items were presented in different orders (i.e., last in segment 1 vs. first in segment 2). There were also unique items, which were randomly assigned to the different versions. Thus, the intimacy level of the sets in two versions can be considered to be approximately equivalent. 5. We instructed participants to answer the disclosure-prompting questions in the order that they were presented; however, in order to give participants privacy, the experimenters were not present in the room when participants interacted and could not monitor the order in which participants answered questions. Still, we have no reason to believe that participants went out of order or skipped any questions in the set. 6. A general question on closeness was also included: How close do you feel toward the Other? (1 ¼ Not at all,7¼ A great deal). Because the reliability of the two-item composite was lower than desired (a was.55 in PS-1 and.66 in PS-2), we decided to conduct the analyses with the more common and validated Aron et al. s (1992) IOS. However, similar results were found when analyzing the IOS or a composite of the two items. 7. In supplementary analyses, we found that female female and male female dyads differed collectively in liking, enjoyment of the interaction, and closeness, Wilks L ¼.77, F (3, 53) ¼ 5.44, p ¼.002, partial Z 2 ¼.24. Collapsing across dyads and over both interaction segments, female female pairs (when compared to male female dyads) reported higher scores on liking (Ms ¼ 5.89 vs. 5.37; d ¼ 0.91) and closeness (4.15 vs. 3.47; d ¼ 0.68). The difference between female female and male female dyads in enjoyment of interaction, however, was small (5.05

Self-disclosure in intimate relationships: Associations with individual and relationship characteristics over time

Self-disclosure in intimate relationships: Associations with individual and relationship characteristics over time Illinois State University ISU ReD: Research and edata Faculty Publications Sociology and Anthropology Sociology and Anthropology 12-2004 Self-disclosure in intimate relationships: Associations with individual

More information

Acceptance of Help as a Function of Similarity of the Potential Helper and Opportunity To Repay'

Acceptance of Help as a Function of Similarity of the Potential Helper and Opportunity To Repay' Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1974.4, 3, pp. 224-229 Acceptance of Help as a Function of Similarity of the Potential Helper and Opportunity To Repay' MARGARET S. CLARK, CAROLYN C. GOTAY, AND JUDSON

More information

Running head: PRESERVING RELATIONSHIPS WITH AN INTERNET CONNECTION 1

Running head: PRESERVING RELATIONSHIPS WITH AN INTERNET CONNECTION 1 Running head: PRESERVING RELATIONSHIPS WITH AN INTERNET CONNECTION 1 Preserving Relationships With an Internet Connection Zachary Jones COMM 3050 Section 001 April 27, 2015 PRESERVING RELATIONSHIPS WITH

More information

Self-Disclosure in Initial Acquaintanceship Interactions. Peter Caprariello. Carnegie Mellon University

Self-Disclosure in Initial Acquaintanceship Interactions. Peter Caprariello. Carnegie Mellon University Self-Disclosure and Acquaintanceship 1 Self-Disclosure in Initial Acquaintanceship Interactions Peter Caprariello Carnegie Mellon University Running head: SELF-DISCLOSURE BETWEEN STRANGERS The study was

More information

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction Heather Dudley What influences people s willingness to interact with someone of a different cultural background from

More information

Social Penetration Theory

Social Penetration Theory 1 Social Penetration Theory Amanda Carpenter and Kathryn Greene Rutgers University, USA Social penetration theory was developed to explain how information exchange functions in the development and dissolution

More information

CHAPTER 1 Understanding Social Behavior

CHAPTER 1 Understanding Social Behavior CHAPTER 1 Understanding Social Behavior CHAPTER OVERVIEW Chapter 1 introduces you to the field of social psychology. The Chapter begins with a definition of social psychology and a discussion of how social

More information

Supplementary Material for Malle, Knobe, and Nelson (2007). Actor-observer asymmetries in explanations of behavior: New answers to an old question.

Supplementary Material for Malle, Knobe, and Nelson (2007). Actor-observer asymmetries in explanations of behavior: New answers to an old question. Supplementary Material for Malle, Knobe, and Nelson (2007). Actor-observer asymmetries in explanations of behavior: New answers to an old question. Study A1 Bertram F. Malle and Sarah E. Nelson In this

More information

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution 1 Robert D. Mather University of Central Oklahoma Charles M. Mather University of Science

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information

National Relay Service: The Deaf Perspective DISCUSSION PAPER

National Relay Service: The Deaf Perspective DISCUSSION PAPER National Relay Service: The Deaf Perspective DISCUSSION PAPER Purpose This discussion paper National Relay Service: The Deaf Perspective has been written with the intention of generating feedback from

More information

D DAVID PUBLISHING. Can Closeness Established Experimentally Impact Friendship Development and Homesickness? * Christopher Russo, Robin Valeri

D DAVID PUBLISHING. Can Closeness Established Experimentally Impact Friendship Development and Homesickness? * Christopher Russo, Robin Valeri Psychology Research, June 2015, Vol. 5, No. 6, 347-354 doi:10.17265/2159-5542/2015.06.001 D DAVID PUBLISHING Can Closeness Established Experimentally Impact Friendship Development and Homesickness? * Christopher

More information

More Than Just Talking: The Role of Self- Disclosure in the Fast Friends Procedure

More Than Just Talking: The Role of Self- Disclosure in the Fast Friends Procedure Seton Hall University erepository @ Seton Hall Seton Hall University Dissertations and Theses (ETDs) Seton Hall University Dissertations and Theses Winter 12-8-2017 More Than Just Talking: The Role of

More information

Affinity Through Instant Messaging

Affinity Through Instant Messaging [This document contains the author s accepted manuscript. For the publisher s version, see the link in the header of this document.] Please note that this is an author-produced PDF of an article accepted

More information

Running head: VARYING DEFINITIONS OF ONLINE COMMUNICATION 1. Varying Definitions of Online Communication and. Their Effects on Relationship Research

Running head: VARYING DEFINITIONS OF ONLINE COMMUNICATION 1. Varying Definitions of Online Communication and. Their Effects on Relationship Research Running head: VARYING DEFINITIONS OF ONLINE COMMUNICATION 1 The title should summarize the paper s main idea and identify the variables under discussion and the relationship between them. The title centered

More information

Personal Talent Skills Inventory

Personal Talent Skills Inventory Personal Talent Skills Inventory Sales Version Inside Sales Sample Co. 5-30-2013 Introduction Research suggests that the most effective people are those who understand themselves, both their strengths

More information

Do misery and happiness both love company? The emotional consequences of listening to experiences shared by others.

Do misery and happiness both love company? The emotional consequences of listening to experiences shared by others. Pursuit - The Journal of Undergraduate Research at the University of Tennessee Volume 6 Issue 1 Article 23 April 2015 Do misery and happiness both love company? The emotional consequences of listening

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Social Presence Measure

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Social Presence Measure Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Social Presence Measure Chad Harms, Frank Biocca Iowa State University, Michigan State University Harms@iastate.edu, Biocca@msu.edu Abstract

More information

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology Preface p. xv Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology p. 3 Sociology, Psychology, or Both? p.

More information

Funnelling Used to describe a process of narrowing down of focus within a literature review. So, the writer begins with a broad discussion providing b

Funnelling Used to describe a process of narrowing down of focus within a literature review. So, the writer begins with a broad discussion providing b Accidental sampling A lesser-used term for convenience sampling. Action research An approach that challenges the traditional conception of the researcher as separate from the real world. It is associated

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/322/5901/606/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Experiencing Physical Warmth Promotes Interpersonal Warmth This PDF file includes: Lawrence E. Williams * and John A.

More information

Chapter 13. Social Psychology

Chapter 13. Social Psychology Social Psychology Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne What s It For? Social Psychology Interpreting the Behavior of Others Behaving in the Presence of Others Establishing Relations With Others Social

More information

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT Study Focus Empathy s impact on: O Human Interactions O Consequences of those interactions O Empathy Development Study Purpose to examine the empathetic

More information

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Measure of Social Presence

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Measure of Social Presence Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Measure of Social Presence Chad Harms Iowa State University Frank Biocca Michigan State University Abstract This study sought to develop and

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information Baldwin and Lammers 10.1073/pnas.1610834113 SI Methods and Results The patterns of predicted results were not affected when age, race (non-white = 0, White = 1), sex (female = 0,

More information

Running head: VARYING DEFINITIONS OF ONLINE COMMUNICATION 1. Varying Definitions of Online Communication and. Their Effects on Relationship Research

Running head: VARYING DEFINITIONS OF ONLINE COMMUNICATION 1. Varying Definitions of Online Communication and. Their Effects on Relationship Research Running head: VARYING DEFINITIONS OF ONLINE COMMUNICATION 1 The title should summarize the paper s main idea and identify the variables under discussion and the relationship between them. The title centered

More information

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles

UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter. Lawrence Feinstein. Jyllian Call. University of California, Los Angeles UCLA Social Support Inventory * (UCLA-SSI) Christine Dunkel-Schetter Lawrence Feinstein Jyllian Call University of California, Los Angeles December, 1986 Further information on the instrument can be obtained

More information

FUNCTIONAL CONSISTENCY IN THE FACE OF TOPOGRAPHICAL CHANGE IN ARTICULATED THOUGHTS Kennon Kashima

FUNCTIONAL CONSISTENCY IN THE FACE OF TOPOGRAPHICAL CHANGE IN ARTICULATED THOUGHTS Kennon Kashima Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy Volume 7, Number 3, Fall 1989 FUNCTIONAL CONSISTENCY IN THE FACE OF TOPOGRAPHICAL CHANGE IN ARTICULATED THOUGHTS Kennon Kashima Goddard College

More information

Computers in Human Behavior

Computers in Human Behavior Computers in Human Behavior 24 (2008) 2965 2971 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Computers in Human Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh Social facilitation and human

More information

WARNING, DISTRACTION, AND RESISTANCE TO INFLUENCE 1

WARNING, DISTRACTION, AND RESISTANCE TO INFLUENCE 1 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1965, Vol. 1, No. 3, 262-266 WARNING, DISTRACTION, AND RESISTANCE TO INFLUENCE 1 JONATHAN L. FREEDMAN Stanford DAVID 0. SEARS of California, Los Angeles 2 hypotheses

More information

Incorporating Experimental Research Designs in Business Communication Research

Incorporating Experimental Research Designs in Business Communication Research Incorporating Experimental Research Designs in Business Communication Research Chris Lam, Matt Bauer Illinois Institute of Technology The authors would like to acknowledge Dr. Frank Parker for his help

More information

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY http://www.uiowa.edu/crisp Submitted: May 3, 2016 First Revision: June 3, 2016 Second Revision: August 26, 2016 Accepted: August 30, 2016 WHY DO EXTRAVERTS FEEL MORE

More information

At first sight: Persistent relational effects of get-acquainted conversations

At first sight: Persistent relational effects of get-acquainted conversations At first sight: Persistent relational effects of get-acquainted conversations Michael Sunnafrank University of Minnesota, Duluth Artemio Ramirez, Jr. The Ohio State University ABSTRACT Previous research

More information

PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH

PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH DANIEL Z. LEVIN Management and Global Business Dept. Rutgers Business School Newark and New Brunswick Rutgers

More information

Impression order effects as a function of the personal relevance of the object of description*

Impression order effects as a function of the personal relevance of the object of description* Memory & Cognition 1974, Vol. 2, No.3, 561-565 Impression order effects as a function of the personal relevance of the object of description* JOHN H. BRINK Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907

More information

The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016

The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016 The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016 This course does not cover how to perform statistical tests on SPSS or any other computer program. There are several courses

More information

Running head: PREDICTING SOCIAL INTIMACY 1. Predicting Social Intimacy: Exploring Contribution of Romantic Relationships and Interpersonal

Running head: PREDICTING SOCIAL INTIMACY 1. Predicting Social Intimacy: Exploring Contribution of Romantic Relationships and Interpersonal Running head: PREDICTING SOCIAL INTIMACY 1 Predicting Social Intimacy: Exploring Contribution of Romantic Relationships and Interpersonal Dependency to and Gender Differences in Social Intimacy Vanessa

More information

Temporalization In Causal Modeling. Jonathan Livengood & Karen Zwier TaCitS 06/07/2017

Temporalization In Causal Modeling. Jonathan Livengood & Karen Zwier TaCitS 06/07/2017 Temporalization In Causal Modeling Jonathan Livengood & Karen Zwier TaCitS 06/07/2017 Temporalization In Causal Modeling Jonathan Livengood & Karen Zwier TaCitS 06/07/2017 Introduction Causal influence,

More information

Several studies have researched the effects of framing on opinion formation and decision

Several studies have researched the effects of framing on opinion formation and decision Attitudes 1 Framing Effects on Attitudes Toward Pedophiles By: Larissa Smage, Advisor--- Joan Riedle The effects of framing techniques on attitudes toward a pedophile were investigated. Framing involves

More information

The Disclosure Intimacy Link in Computer-Mediated Communication: An Attributional Extension of the Hyperpersonal Model

The Disclosure Intimacy Link in Computer-Mediated Communication: An Attributional Extension of the Hyperpersonal Model Human Communication Research ISSN 0360-3989 ORIGINAL ARTICLE The Disclosure Intimacy Link in Computer-Mediated Communication: An Attributional Extension of the Hyperpersonal Model L. Crystal Jiang, Natalie

More information

Attachment Styles and Patterns of Self-Disclosure

Attachment Styles and Patterns of Self-Disclosure Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1991. Vol. 61, No. 2.321-331 Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0O22-3514/91/$3.OO Attachment Styles and Patterns of Self-Disclosure

More information

Gender Differences Associated With Memory Recall. By Lee Morgan Gunn. Oxford May 2014

Gender Differences Associated With Memory Recall. By Lee Morgan Gunn. Oxford May 2014 Gender Differences Associated With Memory Recall By Lee Morgan Gunn A thesis submitted to the faculty of The University of Mississippi in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the Sally McDonnell

More information

Book Review. Review of Cultural Psychology

Book Review. Review of Cultural Psychology The Journal of Social Psychology, 2010, 150(2), 231 234 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC Book Review Review of Cultural Psychology VSOC 0022-4545 The Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 150, No. 2,

More information

Interest in Another s Consideration of One s Needs in Communal and Exchange Relationships

Interest in Another s Consideration of One s Needs in Communal and Exchange Relationships JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 34, 246 264 (1998) ARTICLE NO. JS981352 Interest in Another s Consideration of One s Needs in Communal and Exchange Relationships Margaret S. Clark and Parastu

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1995 The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the

More information

What is Beautiful is Good Online: Physical Attractiveness, Social Interactions and Perceived Social Desirability on Facebook Abstract

What is Beautiful is Good Online: Physical Attractiveness, Social Interactions and Perceived Social Desirability on Facebook Abstract What is Beautiful is Good Online: Physical Attractiveness, Social Interactions and Perceived Social Desirability on Facebook Abstract The present research examined whether physically attractive Facebook

More information

Issues That Should Not Be Overlooked in the Dominance Versus Ideal Point Controversy

Issues That Should Not Be Overlooked in the Dominance Versus Ideal Point Controversy Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 3 (2010), 489 493. Copyright 2010 Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. 1754-9426/10 Issues That Should Not Be Overlooked in the Dominance Versus

More information

ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR

ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR CHAPTER 6 ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR Several studies were done as part of the UTDFP that were based substantially on subjective data, reflecting travelers beliefs, attitudes, and intentions.

More information

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY http://www.uiowa.edu/~grpproc/crisp/crisp.html Volume 11, Number 16 Submitted: June 28, 2006 First Revision: August 15, 2006 Accepted: August 21, 2006 Published: August

More information

"You Are So Beautiful... To Me": Romantic Partners' Insight Into Others' Perceptions

You Are So Beautiful... To Me: Romantic Partners' Insight Into Others' Perceptions Washington University in St. Louis Washington University Open Scholarship All Theses and Dissertations (ETDs) 1-1-2012 "You Are So Beautiful... To Me": Romantic Partners' Insight Into Others' Perceptions

More information

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 1. THE HUMANISTIC FRAMEWORK FOR INTERVIEWER SKILLS

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 1. THE HUMANISTIC FRAMEWORK FOR INTERVIEWER SKILLS INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 1. THE HUMANISTIC FRAMEWORK FOR INTERVIEWER SKILLS 1.1. Foundation of the Humanistic Framework Research interviews have been portrayed in a variety of different ways,

More information

Running head: EFFECT OF HIGH ATTRACTIVENESS ON PERCEIVED INTELLIGENCE 1

Running head: EFFECT OF HIGH ATTRACTIVENESS ON PERCEIVED INTELLIGENCE 1 Running head: EFFECT OF HIGH ATTRACTIVENESS ON PERCEIVED INTELLIGENCE 1 The Effect of High Attractiveness on Perceived Intelligence Katlyn Hogue, Cara Mulhall, and Kara Stewart Hanover College PSY 344:

More information

Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI)

Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) Scale Description The Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) is a multidimensional measurement device intended to assess participantsõ subjective experience related to

More information

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16)

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16) MSP Feedback Guide 2009 Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16) Feedback to Test Takers Introduction (page 2 of the report) The Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16) measures aspects of your

More information

Consumer Perception Survey (Formerly Known as POQI)

Consumer Perception Survey (Formerly Known as POQI) Department of Behavioral Health Consumer Perception Survey (Formerly Known as POQI) CPS Comparison May 2017 On a semi-annual basis the County of Fresno, Department of Behavioral Health (DBH) conducts its

More information

JOSHUA DANIEL WONDRA

JOSHUA DANIEL WONDRA Josh Wondra p. 1 JOSHUA DANIEL WONDRA Social Psychology University of Michigan Department of Psychology 530 Church St. Ann Arbor, MI 48109 jdwondra@umich.edu www.joshwondra.com Education Ph.D. in Social

More information

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR)

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR) Discussion Questions The concept of IR INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR) 1. Define interpersonal relationship. 2. List types of interpersonal relationship. 3. What are the advantages and disadvantages of

More information

Choosing Life: Empowerment, Action, Results! CLEAR Menu Sessions. Health Care 3: Partnering In My Care and Treatment

Choosing Life: Empowerment, Action, Results! CLEAR Menu Sessions. Health Care 3: Partnering In My Care and Treatment Choosing Life: Empowerment, Action, Results! CLEAR Menu Sessions Health Care 3: Partnering In My Care and Treatment This page intentionally left blank. Session Aims: Partnering In My Care and Treatment

More information

Understanding Unconventional Methods of Self- Disclosure in New Relationships

Understanding Unconventional Methods of Self- Disclosure in New Relationships University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Honors Theses, University of Nebraska-Lincoln Honors Program Spring 3-8-2018 Understanding Unconventional Methods of Self-

More information

Being liked. Attraction. Research results. Reward theory. Includes a wide range of situations:

Being liked. Attraction. Research results. Reward theory. Includes a wide range of situations: Attraction Includes a wide range of situations: People we find appealing to work with People we enjoy hanging out with Friends Serious love attachments Being liked Americans are deeply concerned about

More information

CHAPTER 1: Studying Human Sexuality

CHAPTER 1: Studying Human Sexuality CHAPTER 1: Studying Human Sexuality Total Assessment Guide (T.A.G.) Topic Introduction to Chapter Question Factual Conceptual Application Type Multiple Choice 1 3 2 True/False 1-2 Short Answer 1 Essay

More information

multilevel modeling for social and personality psychology

multilevel modeling for social and personality psychology 1 Introduction Once you know that hierarchies exist, you see them everywhere. I have used this quote by Kreft and de Leeuw (1998) frequently when writing about why, when, and how to use multilevel models

More information

IT S A WONDER WE UNDERSTAND EACH OTHER AT ALL!

IT S A WONDER WE UNDERSTAND EACH OTHER AT ALL! It s a Wonder we Understand Each Other at All! Pre-Reading 1 Discuss the following questions before reading the text. 1. Do you think people from different cultures have different communication styles?

More information

Operation S.A.V.E Campus Edition

Operation S.A.V.E Campus Edition Operation S.A.V.E Campus Edition 1 Suicide Prevention Introduction Objectives: By participating in this training you will learn: The scope and importance of suicide prevention The negative impact of myths

More information

Downloaded from UvA-DARE, the institutional repository of the University of Amsterdam (UvA)

Downloaded from UvA-DARE, the institutional repository of the University of Amsterdam (UvA) Downloaded from UvA-DARE, the institutional repository of the University of Amsterdam (UvA) http://hdl.handle.net/11245/2.89264 File ID Filename Version uvapub:89264 323347.pdf final SOURCE (OR PART OF

More information

Getting acquainted in interracial interactions: Avoiding intimacy but approaching race. Camille S. Johnson. San Jose State University

Getting acquainted in interracial interactions: Avoiding intimacy but approaching race. Camille S. Johnson. San Jose State University Interracial Interactions 1 Getting acquainted in interracial interactions: Avoiding intimacy but approaching race Camille S. Johnson San Jose State University Michael A. Olson University of Tennessee Russell

More information

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about?

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about? 2 Asking and answering research questions What s it about? (Social Psychology pp. 24 54) Social psychologists strive to reach general conclusions by developing scientific theories about why people behave

More information

Transformations and the Development of Beliefs about Relationships

Transformations and the Development of Beliefs about Relationships Transformations and the Development of Beliefs about Relationships Catherine A. Surra, Melissa A. Curran, and Christine R. Gray The University of Texas at Austin Paper presented at the meeting of the International

More information

PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE?

PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE? Submitted: February 25, 2017 Revision: March 30, 2017 Accepted: April 27, 2017 PERCEIVED AUTONOMOUS HELP AND RECIPIENTS WELL-BEING: IS AUTONOMOUS HELP GOOD FOR EVERYONE? Masataka Takebe Koji Murata Graduate

More information

OSTRACISM IS LOST IN TRANSLATION

OSTRACISM IS LOST IN TRANSLATION OSTRACISM IS LOST IN TRANSLATION Student Author Han Kang is a senior in psychological sciences in the College of Health and Human Sciences. Supervised by Dr. Kipling D. Williams, she is currently enrolled

More information

Assignment 4: True or Quasi-Experiment

Assignment 4: True or Quasi-Experiment Assignment 4: True or Quasi-Experiment Objectives: After completing this assignment, you will be able to Evaluate when you must use an experiment to answer a research question Develop statistical hypotheses

More information

copyrighted material by PRO-ED, Inc.

copyrighted material by PRO-ED, Inc. Preface As a stutterer, as a speech language pathologist, and as a listener, I have grown to understand that stuttering is the absolute worst thing in this world... if we do not talk about it. When I speak

More information

Analysis of Confidence Rating Pilot Data: Executive Summary for the UKCAT Board

Analysis of Confidence Rating Pilot Data: Executive Summary for the UKCAT Board Analysis of Confidence Rating Pilot Data: Executive Summary for the UKCAT Board Paul Tiffin & Lewis Paton University of York Background Self-confidence may be the best non-cognitive predictor of future

More information

PERSON PERCEPTION AND INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION

PERSON PERCEPTION AND INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION Person Perception and Interpersonal Attraction MODULE-V 22 PERSON PERCEPTION AND INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION We have already noted, achieving a sense of self is an important achievement. A neonate may not

More information

Marketing a healthier choice: Exploring young people s perception of e-cigarettes

Marketing a healthier choice: Exploring young people s perception of e-cigarettes Marketing a healthier choice: Exploring young people s perception of e-cigarettes Abstract Background: As a consequence of insufficient evidence on the safety and efficacy of e- cigarettes, there has been

More information

Running head: How large denominators are leading to large errors 1

Running head: How large denominators are leading to large errors 1 Running head: How large denominators are leading to large errors 1 How large denominators are leading to large errors Nathan Thomas Kent State University How large denominators are leading to large errors

More information

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010.

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010. Sound Advice This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010. Dr. Patricia Manning-Courtney is a developmental pediatrician and is director of the Kelly O Leary Center for

More information

Effects of Interviewer-Respondent Familiarity on Contraceptive Use and Abortion Data

Effects of Interviewer-Respondent Familiarity on Contraceptive Use and Abortion Data Effects of Interviewer-Respondent Familiarity on Contraceptive Use and Abortion Data Guy Stecklov Alex Weinreb Mariano Sana Problems with reproductive health data in non-western settings are widely noted,

More information

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 1 Running Head: THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF-EFFICACY The Role of Modeling and Feedback in Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy Skidmore College Self-Efficacy 2 Abstract Participants

More information

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX 2 Web Appendix A: Panel data estimation approach As noted in the main

More information

Participants. 213 undergraduate students made up the total participants (including the reporter): gender. ethnicity. single/dating/married.

Participants. 213 undergraduate students made up the total participants (including the reporter): gender. ethnicity. single/dating/married. Rape Blame Rape Defined Interpretations of rape o being motivated by a need for power, control or dominance and sexual fulfillment on the part of the perpetrator (Muehlenhard and Linton, 1987) Date rape

More information

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance 1 Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Erica Behrens Winona State University Abstract This study examines the effects of self-consciousness

More information

Chapter 5. Doing Tools: Increasing Your Pleasant Events

Chapter 5. Doing Tools: Increasing Your Pleasant Events 66 Chapter 5. Doing Tools: Increasing Your Pleasant Events The importance of engaging in pleasant events We think most of you would agree that doing things you like typically has a positive effect on your

More information

You must answer question 1.

You must answer question 1. Research Methods and Statistics Specialty Area Exam October 28, 2015 Part I: Statistics Committee: Richard Williams (Chair), Elizabeth McClintock, Sarah Mustillo You must answer question 1. 1. Suppose

More information

Responsiveness in psychotherapy research and practice. William B. Stiles Jyväskylä, Finland, February 2018

Responsiveness in psychotherapy research and practice. William B. Stiles Jyväskylä, Finland, February 2018 Responsiveness in psychotherapy research and practice William B. Stiles Jyväskylä, Finland, February 2018 Responsiveness Behavior influenced by emerging context (i.e. by new events),

More information

MOTIVATIONAL INTERVIEWING

MOTIVATIONAL INTERVIEWING MOTIVATIONAL INTERVIEWING Facilitating Behaviour Change Dr Kate Hall MCCLP MAPS Senior Lecturer in Addiction and Mental Health School of Psychology, Faculty of Health, Deakin University. Lead, Treatment

More information

04/12/2014. Research Methods in Psychology. Chapter 6: Independent Groups Designs. What is your ideas? Testing

04/12/2014. Research Methods in Psychology. Chapter 6: Independent Groups Designs. What is your ideas? Testing Research Methods in Psychology Chapter 6: Independent Groups Designs 1 Why Psychologists Conduct Experiments? What is your ideas? 2 Why Psychologists Conduct Experiments? Testing Hypotheses derived from

More information

EMPOWERMENT STRATEGIES: WORKSHOP DESIGN By: Drs. Kathleen E. Allen and Cynthia Cherrey

EMPOWERMENT STRATEGIES: WORKSHOP DESIGN By: Drs. Kathleen E. Allen and Cynthia Cherrey EMPOWERMENT STRATEGIES: WORKSHOP DESIGN By: Drs. Kathleen E. Allen and Cynthia Cherrey I. The Concept: Breaking Down Barriers of Dependency Moving Towards Empowerment. A. In Dyads: Have participants introduce

More information

PERSON PERCEPTION September 25th, 2009 : Lecture 5

PERSON PERCEPTION September 25th, 2009 : Lecture 5 PERSON PERCEPTION September 25th, 2009 : Lecture 5 PERSON PERCEPTION Social Information Attribution Self-serving Biases Prediction SOCIAL INFORMATION BEHAVIOURAL INPUT What Goes Into Person Perception?

More information

Belief in the Immutability of Attitudes Both Increases and Decreases Advocacy. Omair Akhtar. Apple, Inc. S. Christian Wheeler. Stanford Univeristy

Belief in the Immutability of Attitudes Both Increases and Decreases Advocacy. Omair Akhtar. Apple, Inc. S. Christian Wheeler. Stanford Univeristy Running Head: IMPLICIT THEORIES AFFECT ADVOCACY Belief in the Immutability of Attitudes Both Increases and Decreases Advocacy Omair Akhtar Apple, Inc. S. Christian Wheeler Stanford Univeristy IN PRESS:

More information

Managing conversations around mental health. Blue Light Programme mind.org.uk/bluelight

Managing conversations around mental health. Blue Light Programme mind.org.uk/bluelight Managing conversations around mental health Blue Light Programme 1 Managing conversations around mental health Managing conversations about mental wellbeing Find a quiet place with an informal atmosphere,

More information

Understanding Social Norms, Enjoyment, and the Moderating Effect of Gender on E-Commerce Adoption

Understanding Social Norms, Enjoyment, and the Moderating Effect of Gender on E-Commerce Adoption Association for Information Systems AIS Electronic Library (AISeL) SAIS 2010 Proceedings Southern (SAIS) 3-1-2010 Understanding Social Norms, Enjoyment, and the Moderating Effect of Gender on E-Commerce

More information

All Types of Mortality Salience Are Not Equal: The Effect of Contemplating Natural versus Unnatural Death on. Materialism Behavior

All Types of Mortality Salience Are Not Equal: The Effect of Contemplating Natural versus Unnatural Death on. Materialism Behavior All Types of Mortality Salience Are Not Equal: The Effect of Contemplating Natural versus Unnatural Death on Materialism Behavior ABSTRACT Just as nothing can last forever, people die. However, people

More information

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies Emma ter Mors 1,2, Mieneke Weenig 1, Naomi Ellemers 1, Dancker Daamen 1 1 Leiden University,

More information

Why Is It That Men Can t Say What They Mean, Or Do What They Say? - An In Depth Explanation

Why Is It That Men Can t Say What They Mean, Or Do What They Say? - An In Depth Explanation Why Is It That Men Can t Say What They Mean, Or Do What They Say? - An In Depth Explanation It s that moment where you feel as though a man sounds downright hypocritical, dishonest, inconsiderate, deceptive,

More information

What is effective communication?

What is effective communication? Communication What is effective communication? - Communication: Any connection between humans - Both verbal & non-verbal. - You cannot not communicate. * We are ALWAYS communicating. What are the five

More information

The Good, the Bad and the Blameworthy: Understanding the Role of Evaluative Reasoning in Folk Psychology. Princeton University

The Good, the Bad and the Blameworthy: Understanding the Role of Evaluative Reasoning in Folk Psychology. Princeton University The Good, the Bad and the Blameworthy: Understanding the Role of Evaluative Reasoning in Folk Psychology Joshua Knobe Gabriel Mendlow Princeton University People ordinarily make sense of their own behavior

More information

Interviewing, or MI. Bear in mind that this is an introductory training. As

Interviewing, or MI. Bear in mind that this is an introductory training. As Motivational Interviewing Module 2 Slide Transcript Slide 1 In this module, you will be introduced to the basics of Motivational Interviewing, or MI. Bear in mind that this is an introductory training.

More information