The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized"

Transcription

1 W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 B87 Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 Brief article The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized Wim Gevers*, Bert Reynvoet, Wim Fias Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, H. Dunantlaan 2, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium Received 10 July 2002; accepted 13 November 2002 Abstract In the domain of numbers the existence of spatial components in the representation of numerical magnitude has been convincingly demonstrated by an association between number magnitude and response preference with faster left- than right-hand responses for small numbers and faster rightthan left-hand responses for large numbers (Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993) The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, ). Because numbers convey not only real or integer meaning but also ordinal meaning, the question of whether non-numerical ordinal information is spatially coded naturally follows. While previous research failed to show an association between ordinal position and spatial response preference, we present two experiments involving months (Experiment 1) and letters (Experiment 2) in which spatial coding is demonstrated. Furthermore, the response-side effect was obtained with two different stimulus-response mappings. The association occurred both when ordinal information was relevant and when it was irrelevant to the task, showing that the spatial component of the ordinal representation can be automatically activated. q 2003 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Spatial representation; Ordinal sequences 1. Introduction Although considerable advances have been made in the understanding of the internal representation and processing of ordinal information (see Leth-Steensen & Marley, 2000, for a recent review), one aspect of it, the association between order and space, has barely been investigated. This is surprising because numbers, which convey ordinal meaning * Corresponding author. Tel.: ; fax: addresses: wim.gevers@rug.ac.be (W. Gevers), bert.reynvoet@rug.ac.be (B. Reynvoet), wim.fias@rug.ac.be (W. Fias) /03/$ - see front matter q 2003 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. doi: /s (02)

2 B88 W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 (hierarchically implied by their real and integer meaning; Gallistel & Gelman, 2000), are generally accepted to be spatially coded. An explicit indication of an association between numbers and space was first demonstrated by a response-side effect in number comparison (Dehaene, Dupoux, & Mehler, 1990; Hinrichs, Yurko, & Hu, 1981). When participants had to indicate whether a number was smaller or larger than a predetermined reference number by pressing one of two response buttons, it was found that large responses were faster with the right hand than with the left hand. The reverse was true for small numbers. In a study investigating the mental processing of parity judgement, Dehaene, Bossini, and Giraux (1993) provided further evidence concerning the response-side effect (and called it SNARC effect for Spatial Numerical Association of Response Codes). In evaluating a number s parity status, shorter latencies were obtained for small numbers responded to with the left hand than with the right hand. Conversely, large numbers were responded to preferentially with the right hand. Because parity is a numerical property which alternates between consecutive numbers, this study extends the response-side effect by showing that the effect is not specific to the stimulus-response mapping of the comparison task. It also follows that the effect is not related to the use of a reference number for comparison. Importantly, the results also show that the numerical value is automatically activated, because in principle the parity judgement task needs no access to numerical magnitude information for correct task performance. Further support for the possibility of automatic activation of spatially coded numerical magnitudes comes from a series of experiments which showed that the SNARC effect was elicited, even when digits served merely as a background distracter with a non-numerical target superimposed in an orientation discrimination task (Fias, Lauwereyns, & Lammertyn, 2001). In the present paper we investigate whether a similar association exists between nonnumerical ordered sequences and spatial properties of the response. Dehaene et al. (1993; Experiment 4) already used letters in situations that were structurally analogous to the parity judgement task. In a first condition subjects had to classify the letters A F into the categories ACE or BDF. No SNARC effect was obtained. One could argue, however, that the categories along which the letters had to be classified were arbitrary. This may have induced an atypical way of performing the task. This assumption is supported by the fact that the pattern of response times (RTs) was compatible with a serial search strategy. In a second condition, letters were used in a consonant vowel classification task spanning almost the entire alphabet, but a significant SNARC effect was not obtained. However, there was a tendency toward a SNARC effect. Given the small number of subjects (n ¼ 10), one could argue that the experiment did not have enough statistical power. Most importantly, in both conditions the ordering of the stimuli was irrelevant to the task. One could imagine that the mental representation of order is spatially organized, but that, unlike numbers, this spatially organized representation is not accessed automatically. Taken together, there are a number of reasons why the conditions to test a possible spatial association of order and space were suboptimal. In the following experiments we investigate the possible spatial organization of two ordered sequences, namely months of the year (Experiment 1) and letters (Experiment 2). For each stimulus type, subjects performed both an order-relevant task (comparison with a

3 W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 B89 fixed standard) and an order-irrelevant task (letter detection in Experiment 1 and consonant vowel classification in Experiment 2). The order-relevant task allows judging of the spatial coding of internally ordered information, and with the order-irrelevant task the automaticity of activation of the spatial codes can be evaluated. 2. Experiment 1: months 2.1. Method Twenty-five Dutch speaking participants (four left-handed, mean age: 21.4 years) completed both order-relevant and -irrelevant tasks. The range of stimuli consisted of eight months, ranging from January to April and from September to December, presented in Dutch ( januari, februari, maart, april, september, oktober, november and december ). During the order-relevant task, subjects were asked to judge the position of the months as coming before or after July. During the order-irrelevant task they were asked to judge whether the presented month ended with the letter R or not. Within each task subjects were tested twice: once with early months/months ending with R assigned to the left hand and once to the right hand. Order of task and response assignment were counterbalanced across subjects. In each trial # was presented as a fixation mark for 300 ms, immediately followed by the target month (width ranging from 2.98 to 5.98 depending on word length, height 0.88), until a response was given or 1200 ms elapsed. The screen then remained blank for 1500 ms after which a new trial was started. Every condition was preceded by a training list of 16 items and then each month was presented 20 times in random order with the restriction that the same month could never be presented in successive trials. The experiment was run on a PC-compatible computer Results and discussion Order-relevant task RTs more than three standard deviations from the mean were excluded from the analysis. As a result 2.63% of the data had to be removed. The average error rate over subjects was 4.06%. Mean correct RTs were 484, 513, 482, 479, 501, 497, 492 and 477 ms for the respective months. An ANOVA with Position (before or after reference), Distance from reference (1 4) and Side of response (left or right) as within-subjects factors revealed the following significant effects: a main effect of Distance (Fð3; 72Þ ¼13:06; MSE ¼ 975; P, 0:0001), a main effect of Side of response (Fð1; 24Þ ¼6:71; MSE ¼ 1552; P, 0:002) with faster right-hand responses (486 ms) than left-hand responses (496 ms), and an interaction between Distance and Position (Fð3; 72Þ ¼8:70; MSE ¼ 618; P, 0:0001) which is due to the fast responses to the short months March and April, and most importantly a Position Side of response interaction (Fð1; 24Þ ¼ 4:68; MSE ¼ 11304; P, 0:05), indicating a clear association between order and spatial co-ordinates of the response. To capture the nature of the Position Side of response interaction, a regression analy-

4 B90 W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 Fig. 1. Observed data and regression line representing RT differences between right-handed minus left-handed responses as a function of position in the sequence of months in (a) the order-relevant task and (b) the orderirrelevant task. sis was performed on the RT difference between right-hand minus left-hand responses (drt). From an association between order and space, a negative relation between position in the sequence and drt is predicted: if the beginning of the sequence is associated with left, responses will be faster with the left hand, resulting in a positive drt, whereas negative drts are expected towards the end of the sequence. For this purpose we used the regression analysis for repeated measures data recommended by Lorch and Myers (1990; Method 3) (for advantages of this method see Fias, Brysbaert, Geypens, & d Ydewalle, 1996). In a first step, for all participants the mean RT of the correct responses was computed for each number, separately for left and right responses. On the basis of these mean RTs, drts were computed by subtracting the RT for left-hand responses from the RT for right-hand responses. In a second step, for each individual participant, a regression analysis was computed with the absolute position of months in the complete sequence as the predictor variable (January to April: 1 4; September to December: 9 12). 1 In a third step, one-tailed t-tests were performed to test whether the regression weights of the group deviated significantly from zero. This resulted in the equation drt ¼ 26:17 2 5:60 (Position), with Position contributing significantly (tð24þ ¼ 22:30; SD ¼ 12:2; P, 0:05). This 1 Analyses were also performed with position coded relative to the used items of the sequence (values 1 8). Although intercept and regression weights were different, the patterns of results were the same for all four conditions in the present manuscript.

5 W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 B91 confirms that the Position Side of response interaction results from an association of the spatial components of the position in the ordered sequence and of the response (Fig. 1a). Regression analysis was also used to evaluate the nature of the distance effect with the effect of word length removed. For this purpose, the number of syllables and distance from the reference (1 4) were included as variables to predict RT. This resulted in the equation RT ¼ 476:4 1 10:53 (Number of syllables) (Distance). Both Number of syllables (tð24þ ¼ 4:89; SD ¼ 10:76; P, 0:0001) and Distance (tð24þ ¼ 23:93; SD ¼ 8:10; P, 0:001) contributed significantly. The distance effect shows that subjects solved the task by comparing the month to the reference and not by using a serial search strategy. In the latter case, distance is not predicted to affect comparison times for items positioned after the reference Order-irrelevant task Data-trimming removed 1.99% of the data. The average error rate was 4.13%. Mean correct RTs were 469, 488, 482, 461, 449, 458, 446 and 444 ms for the respective months. An ANOVA was performed with Position (beginning or end) and Side of response (left or right) as within-subjects factors and Order of tasks (first order-relevant or first orderirrelevant) as a between-subjects variable. Distance was not incorporated in the design because no reference was used in the order-irrelevant task. The Position factor perfectly coincided with the presence of the end-letter R (September December) or not (January April). For clarity we keep the same nomenclature as above. The between-subjects factor allows us to evaluate whether the possible Position Side of response interaction is a residue of the S-R mapping in the order-relevant task or not. There was a main effect of Position (Fð1; 23Þ ¼57:71; MSE ¼ 275; P, 0:0001) with faster responses to months positioned at the end (containing the final letter R; 449 ms) than in the beginning (not containing a final R; 475 ms) of the sequence. This probably reflects the general observation that target-present responses are faster than target-absent responses (e.g. Treisman & Gelade, 1980). The interaction between Position and Side of response (Fð1; 23Þ ¼16:61; MSE ¼ 639; P, 0:0005) was significant and was not modulated by Order of tasks (F, 1), from which it can be concluded that the association between spatial coding of Position and Side of response was not due to the persistence of the S-R mapping adopted in the order-relevant task. The regression of Position (coded 1 4 and 8 12) on drt (Fig. 1b) resulted in the equation drt ¼ 25:28 2 4:93 (Position). Position differed significantly from zero (tð1; 24Þ ¼ 24:21; SD ¼ 5:85; P, 0:0005). The regression slopes of Position in the order-irrelevant and the order-relevant task did not differ (dependent t-test: tð24þ ¼20:30). Because months ending in R were the last months of the year and vice versa, one could argue that participants solved the final letter identification task on the basis of ordinal position. If participants really employed this strategy, one would expect a distance effect in the orderirrelevant task. Because a regression analysis of distance on RT revealed no distance effect (Distance-slope ¼ 0:20; tð24þ ¼ 0:15), the strategy explanation can be rejected. 3. Experiment 2: letters Although the results of Experiment 1 are quite clear, one could argue that the conclusion

6 B92 W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 Fig. 2. Observed data and regression line representing RT differences between right-handed minus left-handed responses as a function of position in the alphabet in (a) the order-relevant task and (b) the order-irrelevant task. of spatial coding of ordinal sequences is not justified because it is common practice to numerically code the months of the year. Thus, in principle it is possible that the spatial coding derives from numerical recoding. While numerical recoding is indeed well-learned in the case of months, it is not a feasible option for other sequences. Assigning numbers to the letters of the alphabet, for instance, has recently been shown to require at least 2000 ms (Jou & Aldridge, 1999). To evaluate whether spatial effects can be obtained in the absence of numerical recoding we used letters instead of months in Experiment Method Twenty-four native Dutch speaking subjects (three left-handed, average age: 20.8 years) participated in the experiment. Participants performed an order-relevant comparison task (before or after O) and an order-irrelevant task (consonant vowel classification). We made sure that in the subset of letters used (E G I L R U W Y; width ranging from 0.68 to 4.68, height 3.48) distance from the reference was the same before and after the reference letter O and that consonants and vowels alternated successively. Therefore, the order-irrelevant task allows the investigation of the spatial coding of ordinal structure with a completely different stimulus-response mapping. It should be noted that in Dutch, unlike in English, Y is considered to be a vowel /i/. In all other respects, stimulus presentation and design were the same as in Experiment 1.

7 3.2. Results and discussion W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 B Order-relevant task Due to data-trimming 3.50% of the data was removed. The average error rate was 2.62%. Average RTs for the respective letters were 560, 619, 568, 653, 624, 619, 550 and 583 ms. The response latencies are far below those required to convert letters to numbers (Jou & Aldridge, 1999). Hence, numerical recoding can be rejected as an explanation for the results described below. An ANOVA was performed, including Position (before or after reference), Distance from reference (1 4) and Side of response (left or right) as repeated measures. There were main effects of Distance (Fð3; 69Þ ¼18:74; MSE ¼ 4341; P, 0:0001) and of Side of response (Fð1; 23Þ ¼12:59; MSE ¼ 2069; P, 0:005; RT left ¼ 605 ms; RT right ¼ 589 ms). There was a significant interaction between Position and Distance (Fð3; 69Þ ¼24:98; MSE ¼ 2745; P, 0:0001). Inspection of RTs shows that the interaction is not due to the absence of a distance effect at one or the other side of the reference. Thus, there is no evidence that participants used a serial search strategy. Most importantly, the Position Side of response interaction was significant (Fð1; 23Þ ¼ 10:95; MSE ¼ 13240; P, 0:005). The nature of this interaction was determined by regression of Position (coded according to the absolute letter position in the alphabet) on drt (Fig. 2a), which resulted in the equation drt ¼ 58:05 2 4:97 (Position). Position differed significantly from zero (tð1; 23Þ ¼23:49; SD ¼ 6:98; P, 0:001). Hence, the results provide clear evidence in support of the spatial coding of letter positions Order-irrelevant task Data-trimming removed 0.76% of the data. The average error rate was 2.04%. RTs for the respective letters were 488, 513, 483, 540, 497, 508, 484 and 541 ms. An ANOVA with Position (1: E G, 2: I L, 3: R U or4:w Y), Letter category (consonant or vowel) and Side of response (left or right) as within-subjects variables and Order of task as a between-subjects variable led to main effects of Position (Fð3; 66Þ ¼5:34; MSE ¼ 690; P, 0:005) and of Side of response (Fð1; 22Þ ¼15:04; MSE ¼ 1333; P, 0:001). Position interacted with Letter category (Fð3; 66Þ ¼ 59:81; MSE ¼ 956; P, 0:0001) and, most importantly, with Side of response (Fð3; 66Þ ¼3:55; MSE ¼ 2546; P, 0:01). Thus, even with a radically different response mapping as in the order-relevant task and under conditions where this ordinal information is irrelevant to the task, the spatial component of the ordinal representation manifests itself. Finally, the spatial coding is not due to perseveration of the order-relevant task, because there was no Order of task Position Side of response interaction (F, 1). The regression analyses support the conclusions of the ANOVA (Fig. 2b): drt ¼ 2:37 2 1:12 (Position). Position differed significantly from zero (tð1; 23Þ ¼22:42; SD ¼ 2:26; P, 0:05) which was not due solely to the letter R, because the slope still was reliable when calculated omitting the letter R from the analysis (tð1; 23Þ ¼21:95; SD ¼ 2:38; P, 0:05). It should be noted that the effect size is weaker than in the orderrelevant task (dependent t-test: tð23þ ¼ 22:39; SD ¼ 7:9; P, 0:05). The intercept was also different (dependent t-test: tð23þ ¼ 2:22; SD ¼ 122:80, P, 0:036). The more complex alternating Position to response mapping might be at the basis of these differ-

8 B94 W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 ences. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that equal effect sizes and intercepts were obtained in Experiment 1, where the mapping was the same in both tasks. The regression of Distance on RT did not produce a significant distance effect (Distance-slope ¼ 20:67; tð23þ ¼ 20:76). Hence, there was no implicit comparison involved in solving the task. 4. General discussion In line with the findings of Dehaene et al. (1990, 1993) in the domain of numbers, we obtained clear evidence that the mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially coded. In Experiment 1 we found that months from the beginning of the year are responded to faster with the left hand than with the right hand, whereas the reverse pattern was obtained for months towards the end of the year. This was true not only in a task in which ordinal position had to be compared to a reference position, but also in a letterdetection task which requires only superficial processing of the month names and for which, consequently, the ordinal position was irrelevant. The latter result shows that the spatial coding is not limited to the explicit use of a reference and that the spatial components of the sequence representation can be activated automatically. Moreover, the orderirrelevant condition does not produce a distance effect, on the basis of which an automatically induced implicit comparison process can be ruled out as the underlying factor of the response-side effect. With letters instead of months, Experiment 2 confirmed and extended the conclusions obtained in Experiment 1: the same spatial response bias was observed. The mental representation of ordinal information is spatially defined and can affect performance automatically. These conclusions are strengthened by the fact that the spatial effects are not attributable to the translation of positions to numerical values, as may have been the case for months, because the RTs were much shorter than the 2000 ms reported by Jou and Aldridge (1999) to be required for this translation process. Moreover, the presence of the response-side effect in the order-irrelevant task, where the S-R mapping alternated between successive items, shows that the spatial coding is more precise than a crude beginning-left and end-right classification. Because a similar spatial coding has been established in the domain of numbers (Dehaene et al., 1993) and because numbers convey ordinal meaning, one could wonder whether non-numerical ordered sequences and numbers share the same processing mechanism or, alternatively, use different mechanisms with similar properties. While the present experiments do not allow these two possibilities to be disentangled, the performance of neurological patients might be informative. In particular, Dehaene and Cohen (1997) describe a double dissociation in two patients. Patient BOO performed normally in tasks involving quantitative aspects of number processing but was impaired in tasks involving ordered sequences like reciting the alphabet, musical notes, and so on. Patient MAR, however, was impaired in quantitative number processing but was intact in tasks (e.g. bisection) involving ordered series. Understanding of both number and ordinal sequence processing may benefit from charting the commonalities and differences established by systematically and directly

9 W. Gevers et al. / Cognition 87 (2003) B87 B95 B95 contrasting number to order. The research presented here was an effort in this respect and shows that not only numerical sequences but also ordinal sequences are associated with space. Hence, the association observed in numbers should not necessarily be attributed to their number-specific properties at integer or real level, as has been postulated before (Dehaene et al., 1993). Acknowledgements This research is supported by IUAP P5/04 and by grant D of the Flemish Fund for Scientific Research. Bert Reynvoet is a post-doc researcher supported by the Flemish Fund for Scientific Research. The authors wish to thank Sam Gilbert for checking the English. References Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1997). Cerebral pathways for calculation: double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex, 33 (2), Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16 (3), Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2, Fias, W., Lauwereyns, J., & Lammertyn, J. (2001). Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. Cognitive Brain Research, 12 (3), Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (2000). Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4 (2), Hinrichs, J. V., Yurko, D. S., & Hu, J. M. (1981). Two-digit number comparison: use of place information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, Jou, J., & Aldridge, J. W. (1999). Memory representation of alphabetic position and interval information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25 (3), Leth-Steensen, C., & Marley, A. A. J. (2000). A model of response time effects in symbolic comparison. Psychological Review, 107 (1), Lorch Jr., R. F., & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognition research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12,

Numbers and Space: A Computational Model of the SNARC Effect

Numbers and Space: A Computational Model of the SNARC Effect Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 1, 32 44 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.32

More information

THE SPATIAL REPRESENTATION OF NON-SYMBOLIC NUMERICAL QUANTITIES

THE SPATIAL REPRESENTATION OF NON-SYMBOLIC NUMERICAL QUANTITIES THE SPATIAL REPRESENTATION OF NON-SYMBOLIC NUMERICAL QUANTITIES Riccardo Luccio, Antonia Fumarola, Giorgia Tamburini, & Tiziano Agostini Department of Life Sciences, University of Trieste Abstract. Dehaene

More information

Shared Spatial Representations for Numbers and Space: The Reversal of the SNARC and the Simon Effects

Shared Spatial Representations for Numbers and Space: The Reversal of the SNARC and the Simon Effects Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 5, 1197 1207 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.5.1197

More information

Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers

Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Cognition 106 (2008) 1441 1450 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers Yi-hui

More information

Rational numbers: Componential versus holistic representation of fractions in a magnitude comparison task

Rational numbers: Componential versus holistic representation of fractions in a magnitude comparison task THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2009, 62 (8), 1598 1616 Rational numbers: Componential versus holistic representation of fractions in a magnitude comparison task Gaëlle Meert and Jacques

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003)

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003) Cognition 97 (2005) 89 97 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003) Filip Van Opstal a, Bert Reynvoet b, Tom Verguts a, * a Department

More information

The role of parity, physical size, and magnitude in numerical cognition: The SNARC effect revisited

The role of parity, physical size, and magnitude in numerical cognition: The SNARC effect revisited Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2009, 71 (1), 143-155 doi:10.3758/app.71.1.143 The role of parity, physical size, and magnitude in numerical cognition: The SNARC effect revisited DANIEL FITOUSI

More information

Visuospatial priming of the mental number line

Visuospatial priming of the mental number line Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Cognition 106 (2008) 770 779 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Visuospatial priming of the mental number line Ivilin Stoianov *, Peter Kramer, Carlo Umiltà, Marco

More information

The effect of orientation on number word processing

The effect of orientation on number word processing Acta Psychologica 124 (2007) 370 381 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The effect of orientation on number word processing Roi Cohen Kadosh *, Avishai Henik, Orly Rubinsten Department of Behavioral Sciences,

More information

Transfer of Magnitude and Spatial Mappings to the SNARC Effect for Parity Judgments

Transfer of Magnitude and Spatial Mappings to the SNARC Effect for Parity Judgments Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2009, Vol. 35, No. 6, 1506 1521 2009 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0017257 Transfer of Magnitude

More information

Supplemental material: Interference between number magnitude and parity: Discrete representation in number processing

Supplemental material: Interference between number magnitude and parity: Discrete representation in number processing Krajcsi, Lengyel, Laczkó: Interference between number and parity Supplemental material 1/7 Supplemental material: Interference between number magnitude and parity: Discrete representation in number processing

More information

Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements. Influence Spatial-Numerical Associations

Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements. Influence Spatial-Numerical Associations Running head: CODING STRATEGIES IN NUMBER SPACE Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements Influence Spatial-Numerical Associations Oliver Lindemann 1, 2, Juan M. Abolafia 1,3, Jay Pratt 4

More information

The Relational SNARC: Spatial Representation of Nonsymbolic Ratios?

The Relational SNARC: Spatial Representation of Nonsymbolic Ratios? The Relational SNARC: Spatial Representation of Nonsymbolic Ratios? Percival G. Matthews (pmatthews@wisc.edu) Rui Meng (rmeng5@wisc.edu) Elizabeth Y. Toomarian (toomarian@wisc.edu) Edward M. Hubbard (emhubbard@wisc.edu)

More information

Interhemispheric Stroop-Like Interference in Number Comparison: Evidence for Strong Interhemispheric Integration of Semantic Number Information

Interhemispheric Stroop-Like Interference in Number Comparison: Evidence for Strong Interhemispheric Integration of Semantic Number Information Neuropsychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 16, No. 2, 217 229 0894-4105/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0894-4105.16.2.217 Interhemispheric Stroop-Like Interference

More information

Dissociating between cardinal and ordinal and between the value and size magnitudes of coins

Dissociating between cardinal and ordinal and between the value and size magnitudes of coins Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2010, 17 (6), 889-894 doi:10.3758/pbr.17.6.889 Dissociating between cardinal and ordinal and between the value and size magnitudes of coins Daniel Fitousi Pennsylvania State

More information

Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments

Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2010, 17 (2), 219-223 doi:10.3758/pbr.17.2.219 Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments ROLF REBER University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway PASCAL WURTZ

More information

Automaticity of access to numerical magnitude and its spatial associations Cleland, Alexandra Alice; Bull, Rebecca

Automaticity of access to numerical magnitude and its spatial associations Cleland, Alexandra Alice; Bull, Rebecca University of Aberdeen Automaticity of access to numerical magnitude and its spatial associations Cleland, Alexandra Alice; Bull, Rebecca Published in: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory

More information

Repetition and the SNARC Effect With One- and Two-Digit Numbers

Repetition and the SNARC Effect With One- and Two-Digit Numbers Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 2011 Canadian Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 65, No. 2, 84 97 1196-1961/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0022368 Repetition and the SNARC Effect With One- and Two-Digit

More information

The influence of an external symbol system on number parity representation, or What s odd about 6?

The influence of an external symbol system on number parity representation, or What s odd about 6? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (4),???-??? 730-736 The influence of an external symbol system on number parity representation, or What s odd about 6? WIEBKE IVERSEN Collaborative

More information

Cognition. Post-error slowing: An orienting account. Wim Notebaert a, *, Femke Houtman a, Filip Van Opstal a, Wim Gevers b, Wim Fias a, Tom Verguts a

Cognition. Post-error slowing: An orienting account. Wim Notebaert a, *, Femke Houtman a, Filip Van Opstal a, Wim Gevers b, Wim Fias a, Tom Verguts a Cognition 111 (2009) 275 279 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Post-error slowing: An orienting account Wim Notebaert a,

More information

Similarities and differences in judgements in numerical and non-numerical domains

Similarities and differences in judgements in numerical and non-numerical domains Similarities and differences in judgements in numerical and non-numerical domains Filip Van Opstal Promotor: Prof. Dr. Tom Verguts Proefschrift ingediend tot het behalen van de academische graad van Doctor

More information

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan University of Alberta The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm by Shawn Tan A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial

More information

Making SNAP Judgments: Rethinking the Spatial Representation of Number

Making SNAP Judgments: Rethinking the Spatial Representation of Number Making SNAP Judgments: Rethinking the Spatial Representation of Number Tyler Marghetis (tmarghet@cogsci.ucsd.edu) Esther J. Walker (e1walker@cogsci.ucsd.edu) Benjamin K. Bergen (bkbergen@cogsci.ucsd.edu)

More information

Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Groningen, The Netherlands

Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Groningen, The Netherlands This article was downloaded by:[radboud University Nijmegan] On: 9 January 2008 Access Details: [subscription number 732592891] Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered

More information

Acta Psychologica. Conflict and error adaptation in the Simon task. Wim Notebaert, Tom Verguts. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Acta Psychologica. Conflict and error adaptation in the Simon task. Wim Notebaert, Tom Verguts. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica 136 (2011) 212 216 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www. e lsevier. com/ l ocate/ a ctpsy Conflict and error adaptation in the Simon task

More information

Two-digit Number Comparison:

Two-digit Number Comparison: Two-digit Number Comparison: Digit Similarity Effect on Reaction Time in Two-digit Number Comparison by Hyunmo Pang A thesis submitted to the Department of Psychology in conformity with requirements for

More information

On the mental representation of negative numbers: Context-dependent SNARC effects with comparative judgments

On the mental representation of negative numbers: Context-dependent SNARC effects with comparative judgments Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (5), 931-937 On the mental representation of negative numbers: Context-dependent SNARC effects with comparative judgments SAMUEL SHAKI and WILLIAM M. PETRUSIC Carleton

More information

Automaticity of Number Perception

Automaticity of Number Perception Automaticity of Number Perception Jessica M. Choplin (jessica.choplin@vanderbilt.edu) Gordon D. Logan (gordon.logan@vanderbilt.edu) Vanderbilt University Psychology Department 111 21 st Avenue South Nashville,

More information

THE SYMBOLIC DISTANCE EFFECT FOR ALPHABETIC ORDER JUDGEMENTS : A SUBJECTIVE REPORT AND REACTION TIME ANALYSIS

THE SYMBOLIC DISTANCE EFFECT FOR ALPHABETIC ORDER JUDGEMENTS : A SUBJECTIVE REPORT AND REACTION TIME ANALYSIS Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (1978) 30, 33-43. THE SYMBOLIC DISTANCE EFFECT FOR ALPHABETIC ORDER JUDGEMENTS : A SUBJECTIVE REPORT AND REACTION TIME ANALYSIS J. M. E. HAMILTON AND A. J.

More information

Look, no hands: A perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention

Look, no hands: A perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention Nicholls, Loftus & Gevers 1 Look, no hands: A perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention Michael E.R. Nicholls* Andrea M. Loftus* Wim Gevers # *Department of Psychology University

More information

On the cognitive link between space and number: a meta-analysis of the SNARC effect

On the cognitive link between space and number: a meta-analysis of the SNARC effect Psychology Science Quarterly, Volume 50, 2008 (4), pp. 489-525 On the cognitive link between space and number: a meta-analysis of the SNARC effect GUILHERME WOOD 1, KLAUS WILLMES 2, HANS-CHRISTOPH NUERK

More information

Symbolic Distance Between Numerosity and Identity Modulates Stroop-Like Interference

Symbolic Distance Between Numerosity and Identity Modulates Stroop-Like Interference Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (in press) SDE Modulates Stroop Interference 1 Symbolic Distance Between Numerosity and Identity Modulates Stroop-Like Interference

More information

Number magnitude to finger mapping is disembodied and topological

Number magnitude to finger mapping is disembodied and topological Exp Brain Res () 9:9 DOI.7/s---x RESEARCH ARTICLE Number magnitude to finger mapping is disembodied and topological Myrthe A. Plaisier Jeroen B. J. Smeets Received: 7 August / Accepted: 7 January / Published

More information

PSYC20007 READINGS AND NOTES

PSYC20007 READINGS AND NOTES Week 4 Lecture 4 Attention in Space and Time The Psychological Function of Spatial Attention To assign limited-capacity processing resources to relevant stimuli in environment - Must locate stimuli among

More information

Episodic and prototype models of category learning

Episodic and prototype models of category learning DOI 10.1007/s10339-011-0403-2 RESEARCH REPORT Episodic and prototype models of category learning Richard J. Tunney Gordon Fernie Received: 7 October 2010 / Accepted: 28 March 2011 Ó Marta Olivetti Belardinelli

More information

DOCTORATE OF PHILOSOPHY Dec 2008 Committee: Richard B. Anderson, Advisor Dale Klopfer Yiwei Chen Barbara Moses

DOCTORATE OF PHILOSOPHY Dec 2008 Committee: Richard B. Anderson, Advisor Dale Klopfer Yiwei Chen Barbara Moses THE INFLUENCE OF REPRESENTATIONAL PROCESSES ON THE NUMERICAL DISTANCE EFFECT Neil D. Berg A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling Green State University in partial fulfillment of the

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Priming the Mental Time Line Maria Grazia Di Bono, Marco Casarotti, Konstantinos Priftis, Lucia Gava, Carlo Umiltà, and Marco Zorzi

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Evidence for Direct Retrieval of Relative Quantity Information in a Quantity Judgment Task: Decimals, Integers, and the Role of Physical

More information

Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression

Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression Supplementary Material Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression 1, 2, * Bahador Bahrami Petra Vetter 1, 3 Eva Spolaore 1, 4 Silvia Pagano 1, 4 Brian Butterworth 1,3 Geraint Rees 1,

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Journal of Experimental Psychology: General The Interplay Between Nonsymbolic Number and Its Continuous Visual Properties Titia Gebuis and Bert Reynvoet Online First Publication, November 14, 2011. doi:

More information

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation The description and presentation of results involves a number of topics. These include scales of measurement, descriptive statistics used to summarize

More information

How is Number Associated with Space? The Role of Working Memory

How is Number Associated with Space? The Role of Working Memory C H A P T E R 10 How is Number Associated with Space? The Role of Working Memory Wim Fias,* Jean-Philippe van Dijck* and Wim Gevers * Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Belgium Unité

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

Size congruity effects with two-digit numbers: Expanding the number line?

Size congruity effects with two-digit numbers: Expanding the number line? Memory & Cognition 2006, 34 (2), 445-457 Size congruity effects with two-digit numbers: Expanding the number line? DANIEL FITOUSI and DANIEL ALGOM Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel The size congruity

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits

Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits Cognitive Brain Research 12 (2001) 415 423 www.elsevier.com/ locate/ bres Research report Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits Wim Fias *, Johan

More information

Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli

Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli L. Naccache, S. Dehaene / Cognition 80 (2001) 215±229 215 COGNITION Cognition 80 (2001) 215±229 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli Lionel Naccache*,

More information

This article was published in an Elsevier journal. The attached copy is furnished to the author for non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the author s institution, sharing

More information

Masked Primes Can Be Genuinely Semantically Processed

Masked Primes Can Be Genuinely Semantically Processed Masked Primes Can Be Genuinely Semantically Processed A Picture Prime Study Eva Van den Bussche, Karolien Notebaert, and Bert Reynvoet Department of Psychology, University of Leuven, Belgium Abstract.

More information

Running head: FEATURE INTEGRATION AND TASK SWITCHING 1. Feature Integration and Task Switching:

Running head: FEATURE INTEGRATION AND TASK SWITCHING 1. Feature Integration and Task Switching: Running head: FEATURE INTEGRATION AND TASK SWITCHING 1 Feature Integration and Task Switching: Diminished Switch Costs After Controlling for Stimulus, Response, and Cue Repetitions James R. Schmidt and

More information

Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, in press. Physical similarity (and not quantity representation) drives perceptual comparison of

Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, in press. Physical similarity (and not quantity representation) drives perceptual comparison of Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, in press Physical similarity (and not quantity representation) drives perceptual comparison of numbers: Evidence from two Indian notations (in press; Psychonomic Bulletin

More information

When Numbers Get Heavy: Is the Mental Number Line Exclusively Numerical?

When Numbers Get Heavy: Is the Mental Number Line Exclusively Numerical? When Numbers Get Heavy: Is the Mental Number Line Exclusively Numerical? Kevin J. Holmes 1 *, Stella F. Lourenco 2 1 Department of Linguistics, University of California, Berkeley, California, United States

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Limited transfer of subliminal response priming to novel stimulus orientations and identities

Limited transfer of subliminal response priming to novel stimulus orientations and identities Limited transfer of subliminal response priming to novel stimulus orientations and identities Katrin Elsner Department of Psychology Martin-Luther-University Halle-Wittenberg Wilfried Kunde Department

More information

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1040-1050 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1040 Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching ERINA SAEKI AND SATORU

More information

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) 1 VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2) Thank you for providing us with the opportunity to revise our paper. We have revised the manuscript according to the editors and

More information

Running head: AUTOMATIC EFFECTS OF INSTRUCTIONS 1. Automatic effects of instructions do not require the intention to execute these instructions.

Running head: AUTOMATIC EFFECTS OF INSTRUCTIONS 1. Automatic effects of instructions do not require the intention to execute these instructions. Running head: AUTOMATIC EFFECTS OF INSTRUCTIONS 1 Automatic effects of instructions do not require the intention to execute these instructions. Baptist Liefooghe & Jan De Houwer Department of Experimental-Clinical

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation

Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (7), 1791-1802 doi:10.3758/app.72.7.1791 Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation SANDRA A J. THOMSON,

More information

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Stimulus conflict predicts conflict adaptation in a numerical flanker task

Stimulus conflict predicts conflict adaptation in a numerical flanker task Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006, 13 (6), 1078-1084 Stimulus conflict predicts conflict adaptation in a numerical flanker task WIM NOTEBAERT and TOM VERGUTS Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium Conflict

More information

Optimization of Computer presented Information for left-handed Observer

Optimization of Computer presented Information for left-handed Observer PsychNology Journal, 2003 Volume 1, Number 2, 151-163 Optimization of Computer presented Information for left-handed Observer Cecília Sikné Lányi Laboratory of Colour and Multimedia, Department of Image

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

Time causes forgetting from working memory

Time causes forgetting from working memory Psychon Bull Rev (2012) 19:87 92 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0192-8 BRIEF REPORT Time causes forgetting from working memory Pierre Barrouillet & Annick De Paepe & Naomi Langerock Published online: 20 December

More information

The importance of response type to the relationship between temporal order and numerical magnitude

The importance of response type to the relationship between temporal order and numerical magnitude Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:1604 1613 DOI 10.3758/s13414-011-0114-x The importance of response type to the relationship between temporal order and numerical magnitude Michael E. R. Nicholls & Megan

More information

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2002, Vol. 28, No. 3, 445 457 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.445

More information

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 4, 354-373 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association Inc 0096-3445/94/S3.00 Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition

The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (4), 603-609 The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition MIRIAM GADE Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany AND IRING

More information

Stroop Dilution Depends on the Nature of the Color Carrier but Not on Its Location

Stroop Dilution Depends on the Nature of the Color Carrier but Not on Its Location Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 4, 826 839 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.826

More information

Enhanced discrimination in autism

Enhanced discrimination in autism THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2001, 54A (4), 961 979 Enhanced discrimination in autism Michelle O Riordan and Kate Plaisted University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Children with autism

More information

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations

Influence of Visual Stimulus Mode on Transfer of Acquired Spatial Associations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2009, Vol. 35, No. 2, 434 445 2009 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014529 Influence of Visual Stimulus

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance

Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance Vencislav Popov (vencislav.popov@gmail.com) Lynne Reder (reder@cmu.edu) Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh,

More information

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY Thomas Lachmann *, Gunjan Khera * and Cees van Leeuwen # * Psychology II, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany # Laboratory

More information

Acta Psychologica. Haptic pop-out in a hand sweep. Myrthe A. Plaisier *, Wouter M. Bergmann Tiest, Astrid M.L. Kappers. abstract

Acta Psychologica. Haptic pop-out in a hand sweep. Myrthe A. Plaisier *, Wouter M. Bergmann Tiest, Astrid M.L. Kappers. abstract Acta Psychologica 128 (28) 368 377 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy Haptic pop-out in a hand sweep Myrthe A. Plaisier *, Wouter

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

LIFETIME OF MEMORY FOR SELF-GENERATED INFORMATION

LIFETIME OF MEMORY FOR SELF-GENERATED INFORMATION LIFETIME OF MEMORY FOR SELF-GENERATED INFORMATION Lutz Munka and Christian Kaernbach Universität Leipzig, Germany munka@uni-leipzig.de, christian@kaernbach.de Abstract In experiments concerning the lifetime

More information

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 3, 580 584 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027885 OBSERVATION Templates

More information

Spatial Organization of Magnitude in the Representation of Number and Emotion

Spatial Organization of Magnitude in the Representation of Number and Emotion Spatial Organization of Magnitude in the Representation of Number and Emotion Kevin J. Holmes (kevin.holmes@emory.edu) Stella F. Lourenco (slouren@emory.edu) Department of Psychology, Emory University

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 Supporting Information Variances and biases of absolute distributions were larger in the 2-line

More information

Cognition 121 (2011) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage:

Cognition 121 (2011) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage: Cognition 121 (2011) 363 385 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit The mental representation of integers: An abstract-to-concrete

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces

Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces Vision Research 43 (2003) 1501 1505 Rapid communication Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces Adrian Schwaninger a,b, *, Stefan Ryf b, Franziska Hofer b

More information

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching Ayla Barutchu 1 *, Stefanie I. Becker 2, Olivia Carter 3, Robert Hester 3, Neil L. Levy 1,4 1 Florey Institute

More information

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Nart Bedin Atalay (natalay@selcuk.edu.tr) Selcuk University, Konya, TURKEY Mine Misirlisoy

More information

Objective correlates of an unusual subjective experience: A single-case study of number form synaesthesia

Objective correlates of an unusual subjective experience: A single-case study of number form synaesthesia COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2006, 23 (8), 1162 1173 Objective correlates of an unusual subjective experience: A single-case study of number form synaesthesia M. Piazza and P. Pinel INSERM, CEA, Cognitive

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time David E. Huber (dhuber@psyc.umd.edu) Department of Psychology, 1147 Biology/Psychology Building College Park, MD 2742 USA Denis Cousineau (Denis.Cousineau@UMontreal.CA)

More information

Unconscious priming according to. multiple S-R rules

Unconscious priming according to. multiple S-R rules Cognition 104 (2007) 89 105 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Unconscious priming according to, multiple S-R rules Andrea Kiesel a,, Wilfried Kunde b, Joachim HoVmann a a Julius-Maximilians University of

More information

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755

Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Memory & Cognition 1974, Vol. 2, No.3, 533 538 Incorporating quantitative information into a linear ordering" GEORGE R. POTTS Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 Ss were required to learn linear

More information

Rajeev Raizada: Statement of research interests

Rajeev Raizada: Statement of research interests Rajeev Raizada: Statement of research interests Overall goal: explore how the structure of neural representations gives rise to behavioural abilities and disabilities There tends to be a split in the field

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Automatic stimulus response associations may be semantically mediated

Automatic stimulus response associations may be semantically mediated Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2002, 9 (1), 107 112 Automatic stimulus response associations may be semantically mediated BERT REYNVOET, BERNIE CAESSENS, and MARC BRYSBAERT Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium

More information

Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures

Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this

More information