Region- and edge-based configurational effects in texture segmentation

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Region- and edge-based configurational effects in texture segmentation"

Transcription

1 Vision Research 47 (2007) Region- and edge-based configurational effects in texture segmentation Enrico Giora *, Clara Casco Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Via Venezia 8, Padua, Italy Received 12 April 2006; received in revised form 13 January 2007 Abstract We have found a new configurational effect in texture segmentation. In addition to collinear facilitation at the edge, this effect results from contextual modulation within the texture-region, i.e. from texels not abutting the segmented edge. The largest facilitation was found when two conditions were fulfilled: (i) elements along the edge were parallel to the edge and collinear, (ii) elements in the texture-region were also collinear but non-parallel to the edge. We show that this facilitation occurs when there are groups of different orientation from the edge in the texture-region. We suggest two possible underlying mechanisms: either a region-based process that links collinear iso-oriented elements and locates the edge when the orientation changes, or else second-order filters tuned to orientation differences rather than orientation per se. Ó 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Texture segmentation; Lateral interactions; Grouping by collinearity; Energy models 1. Introduction * Corresponding author. Fax: address: enrico.giora@unipd.it (E. Giora). One of the first processing steps on the path to perception is the segregation of objects from the background. This operation has often been studied using texture images where, in the absence of mean luminance differences, a given texture-region can be segmented on the basis of discontinuities of some basic dimension such as orientation, spatial-frequency or motion. The most popular accounts of texture segmentation are edge-based segregation models (see Landy & Graham, 2004; for a review). Briefly, these models predict that texture segmentation results from a non-linear transformation of the output of local spatial filters, followed by a 2ndorder spatial filtering to enhance activity at the texturedefined contours, where the local filter response changes. According to these models, edge-based segregation is thought to result from both enhanced signal processing at the texture-edge and local inhibitory activity in the texture-region, where filter response is weak (Malik & Perona, 1990; Sagi, 1991, p. 406). However, the hypothesis of inhibition is difficult to conciliate with the phenomenological evidence that local properties of texture-region can be still salient after segmentation. Indeed, when observing a pair of zebras, say, we perceive a pair of zebra coats, not just the boundary between their bodies, and this can only result from textureregion information, with no feature gradient (Ben Shahar, 2006). Roelfsema, Lamme, Spekreijse, & Bosch (2002) attempt to accommodate phenomenology with visual processing. They propose that during texture segregation, locations where the properties of texture-elements change abruptly are assigned to boundaries, whereas image regions that are relatively homogeneous are not inhibited, but perceived as texture-regions by grouping elements together. Moreover, psychophysical data strongly suggest that the properties of texture-regions are perceived by mechanisms different from those responsible for extracting texture edges (Ariely, 2001; Parkes, Lund, Angelucci, Solomon, & Morgan, 2001). Lee (1995) and Roelfsema et al. (2002) suggested that edge- and region-based mechanisms operate at different levels of processing /$ - see front matter Ó 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi: /j.visres

2 880 E. Giora, C. Casco / Vision Research 47 (2007) However, there are data suggesting that not only are textures perceived, but they produce contextual effects in segmentation. A well known contextual effect is a facilitation when texels are collinear and parallel to the edge (Caputo & Casco, 1999; Casco, Campana, Grieco, & Fuggetta, 2004; Casco, Grieco, Campana, Corvino, & Caputo, 2005; Nothdurft, 1992; Olson & Attneave, 1970; Wolfson & Landy, 1995). The underlying physiological mechanism (see Lamme, 2004; for a review) is based on the familiar phenomenon of enhancement of neuronal firing rate resulting from contextual influences from outside the receptive field. These contextual influences affect several visual tasks in addition to texture segmentation. For example, they reduce contrast threshold for a single target bar (Polat & Sagi, 1993, 1994) and enhance detection of contours embedded in background noise (Field, Hayes, & Hess, 1993; Hess & Dakin, 1997; Li & Gilbert, 2002). An account for contextual effects from the region in edge segmentation is proposed by region-based models. According to these models, the visual system treats neighbouring texture-regions as belonging to the same texture if they are similar enough. In this way, individual elements are grouped by spreading neural activity emanating from highly stimulated detectors, and the edge is detected when linking operations interrupt because the output of local filters changes (Caelli, 1985). Interestingly, since linking between collinear elements is preferred, edge segmentation by such a mechanism results from spreading of activity in the direction non-parallel to the edge and cannot be assimilated to the mechanism accounting for the facilitation produced by collinear elements parallel to the texture-edge. An alternative approach conciliates the contextual effects with edge-based models of texture segmentation. Wolfson & Landy (1999), for example, suggest that reduced detection of a target element oriented differently from the surrounding region, when the background surrounding the region is iso-oriented with the target (Caputo, 1996), could depend on inhibitory connections between orientation and spatial-frequency selective linear-filters. This explanation is, however, local and does not account for facilitatory contextual effects resulting from grouping in the texture-region. Thus, contextual influences from elements parallel to the edge are well compatible with edge-based models (as shown by Wolfson & Landy, 1995) and can modulate either facilitating or inhibiting edge-based segregation. On the other hand, contextual influences from the texture-region are instead taken in account by a region-based mechanism, predicting that a texture-edge is detected not explicitly but rather implicitly when the growing of two different texture-regions causes their interaction. In this case, facilitation should occur when collinear elements are nonparallel to the texture-edge. Our experiments were designed to investigate the interaction between region- and edge-based mechanisms. We predicted that, if texture-edge segmentation depended on region-based analysis, then contextual influences from the texture-region should affect the saliency of the edge. To test this hypothesis we checked how the texture overall-orientation affected the discrimination of the edge. In order to distinguish between the effect of collinearity at the edge and region-based effects, we arranged the edge to segment a larger texture-region from a narrow one. Consequently, when elements in the narrow region were parallel to the edge they were also collinear, whereas collinear elements in the larger texture were either parallel or orthogonal to the edge. Results show a new facilitatory configurational effect resulting from grouping by collinearity in the textureregion, and independent of collinear facilitation at the edge. 2. Methods 2.1. Stimuli Stimuli were generated by using a VSG 2/3 Cambridge Research System graphic card with 12-bit luminance resolution and displayed on a gamma-corrected Sony Triniton monitor with a resolution of pixels refreshed at 100 Hz. Observers viewed the stimuli in a dark room at 57 cm viewing distance. In all experiments, we used textures composed of 8 8 ( deg of visual angle) matrices of cosine-phase [even] Gabor-elements with circular support or envelope. Each Gabor-patch was defined as a sinusoidal-modulated carrier with a wavelength [k] of.31 deg (spatial-frequency of 3.2 cycles/deg) multiplied by a Gaussian envelope with standard deviation [r] of.19 deg. Centre-to-centre elements distance was equal to 3.66 k. Mean luminance of a Gabor-element was equal to the background luminance (49 cd/m 2 ). By selecting two orientations of the Gabor-elements amongst four possible orientations (0, 45, 90 and 135 deg) we obtained, for each pattern, two texture sub-regions separated by a texture-edge (Fig. 1). The texture-edge was located either between the two extreme stripes of elements (the up/down rows or the left/right columns) or between the two central stripes. In the first case, we will refer to the area with the larger number of iso-oriented elements as the larger texture-region. Segmented textures were differentiated on the basis of two distinct configurational properties (Fig. 1): - collinearity at the edge: the texture-elements (texels) in one of the two stripes abutting the edge, were either iso-oriented and collinear to each other, (in this case parallel to the edge), or iso-oriented and non-collinear (in this case non-parallel to the edge). - congruency in the larger texture-region: the texels in the larger textureregion were always iso-oriented and collinear to each other (except in Experiment 4) but their orientation was either congruent (parallel to the edge) or non-congruent (non-parallel to the edge) Subjects Subjects were aged years, all volunteers with normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. All the participants, except the authors, were ignorant of the purposes of the experiments. Each of the twelve subjects executed two experiments in random order: six participated in Experiments 1 and 2 and six in Experiments 3 and 4. Two new naïve subjects and the authors participated in Experiment Task Subjects performed a binary classification task and were asked to discriminate, by pressing one of two alternative keys, the orientation of the texture-edge (horizontal vs vertical).

3 E. Giora, C. Casco / Vision Research 47 (2007) Fig. 2. Events in a trial: first, a central fixation point was presented for 1000 ms on a grey background, followed (no interval) by a masking texture for 300 ms, and then (no interval) the test matrix, whose duration changed randomly in a block amongst five levels (20, 40, 60, 80, 100 ms); this was finally interrupted (no interval) by a second 300-ms mask. Fig. 1. Example stimuli. (a) Textures used in Experiment 1, with deg orientation contrast at the edge: elements in one side of the edge were either collinear (left) or non-collinear (right). (b) Textures used in Experiment 2, with an orientation contrast of 90 deg at the edge: texel overall-orientation in the larger region was either non-congruent (left) or congruent (right) with boundary orientation; in the symmetrical control stimulus (centre) the congruency distinction does not apply. (c) Textures used in Experiment 3: the conditions were as in Experiment 2 but with 45/ 135 deg orientation contrast at the edge. (d) Textures used in Experiment 4: non-congruent and congruent stimuli of Experiment 3 and the same patterns in jittered condition. (e) Textures used in Experiment 5: symmetrical pattern 0/90 deg of Experiment 2 and a new symmetrical pattern 45/135 deg Procedure The procedure is illustrated in Fig. 2. Each trial started with a central fixation point presented for 1000 ms on a grey background; the test matrix was preceded and followed (with no interval) by a 300 ms mask composed of randomly oriented Gabors. Test matrix durations were varied in five levels (20, 40, 60, 80 and 100 ms), randomly in a block. At the second mask offset, the screen turned black until pressing of a key by the subject to start a new trial. Each block consisted of a random presentation of eight repetitions of each orientation, duration level and configurational condition (collinear vs non-collinear in Experiments 1 and 5, in total 160 trials; congruent, control and non-congruent, in Experiments 2 and 3, in total 240 trials; congruent-collinear, congruent-jittered, non-congruent-collinear, noncongruent-jittered in Experiment 4, in total 320 trials). Before the experiment started, each subject performed a practice session in which visual feedback was given after each trial, depending on whether the response was correct or incorrect Data analysis Temporal thresholds, defined as the estimated duration for 75% accuracy, were calculated by Probit analysis (Finney, 1971). Repeated measures ANOVAs were used to compare individual thresholds. The sphericity of the data was tested with Mauchly s test (Howell, 2002). When the sphericity assumption was not supported by that test, Greenhouse Geisser correction was applied. Since the data for the two orientations were not statistically different, they were not treated separately. Post hoc comparisons were carried out using Bonferroni s correction. 3. Experiments and results 3.1. Experiment 1 It is well known that collinearity at the edge facilitates edge segmentation (Caputo & Casco, 1999; Casco et al., 2004, 2005; Wolfson & Landy, 1995). Experiment 1 was designed to confirm this facilitatory effect. In both stimuli, orientation contrast at the edge was the same (45 deg) and the texture field was filled with collinear elements oriented either at 45 or 135 deg, with equal probability. For both cases, elements in the extreme stripe were either collinear to each other and parallel to the edge (Fig. 1a, left) or non-collinear and non-parallel (Fig. 1a, right). Fig. 3 shows that accuracy increased with duration. Temporal thresholds were lower (31.9 vs 57.6) when texels in the furthest stripe were collinear with the edge [t (5) = 5.8, p <.005]. These results confirmed that segmentation is improved by collinearity at the edge Experiment 2 In Experiment 2, as in Experiment 1, elements in the furthest stripe were either collinear and parallel to the edge (Fig. 1b, left) or non-collinear and non-parallel (Fig. 1b, right). The collinear elements in the larger texture-region had 90 deg orientation contrast with those in the stripe. In this way, both stimuli had one or the other stripe abutting the edge having elements collinear and parallel to the edge. This should balance the collinearity effect of Experiment 1. However, the elements in the larger region were always collinear but either parallel (congruent) to the edge

4 882 E. Giora, C. Casco / Vision Research 47 (2007) Fig. 3. Results of Experiment 1. Mean accuracy of the six subjects is plotted as a function of stimulus duration (ms), separately for collinear (continuous line) and non-collinear (dotted line) patterns. (in Fig. 1b, right) or orthogonal (non-congruent) to the edge (Fig. 1b, left). Thus, only the congruency factor could account for a difference between the two conditions. As a control for these stimuli with a larger texture-region made up of seven stripes, a stimulus was used presenting identical orientation contrast at the edge, but with two sub-regions of identical size four stripes on each side of the edge, either parallel or orthogonal to the edge (Fig. 1b, centre). If edge saliency depended on collinearity at the edge only, we predicted no difference in the three conditions. On the other hand, if discrimination was based on region-based analysis, as in Caelli (1985) account, we expected better performance for the non-congruent condition and worse for the congruent. Results, reported in Fig. 4, showed that accuracy increased with duration. The effect of stimulus was significant [F (2,10) = 7.4; p <.05]. Post hoc comparisons showed that temporal thresholds were lower in the non-congruent (29.7 ms) than in the congruent (52.2 ms) condition [p <.05]. Temporal thresholds in the control condition were in between (32.5 ms), indicating the congruent condition to be more difficult than the control, where the two sub-regions segmented by the edge had the same size. Instead, the difference between control and non-congruent was weak. These results support the idea in agreement with region-based models that collinear elements in the larger texture improve segmentation when non-parallel to the edge. Note that this effect of non-congruency cannot be confused with collinear facilitation in the smaller region. Indeed, by decreasing the size of the larger non-congruent region in the control pattern, despite a larger number of collinear stripes parallel to the edge in the other sub-region, edge saliency was slightly reduced. Finally, the results cannot be accounted for by a difference in eccentricity that would predict a better performance for the control than for both patterns with edge located at a more external stripe of elements Experiment 3 Since in Experiments 1 and 2 orientation contrast differed, we replicated Experiment 2 to check the effect of orientation contrast. One of the stimuli used in Experiment 3 consisted of the same collinear stimulus used in Experiment 1, which was also non-congruent (Fig. 1c, left), and its symmetrical version, which was congruent (Fig. 1c, right). In both stimuli the edge was defined by 45 deg orientation contrast. Moreover, as in Experiment 2, in both stimuli elements were collinear on one side and non-collinear on the other; this should lead to an identical facilitation of collinearity at the edge. Data obtained with these two stimuli with a larger texture-region made up of seven stripes (as in Experiment 2) were compared with those in a control condition (Fig. 1c, centre) with no larger texture-region (four stripes on each side of the edge). The results, given in Fig. 5, showed an effect of duration similar to that obtained in Experiment 2. The ANOVA revealed a significant effect of stimulus [F (2,10) = 5.6; Fig. 4. Results of Experiment 2. Mean accuracy of the six subjects is plotted as a function of stimulus duration (ms), separately for congruent (squares), non-congruent (circles) and control (triangles) stimuli. Fig. 5. Results of Experiment 3. Mean accuracy of the six subjects is plotted as a function of stimulus duration (ms), separately for congruent (squares), non-congruent (circles) and control (triangles) stimuli.

5 E. Giora, C. Casco / Vision Research 47 (2007) p <.05]. Post hoc comparisons showed that temporal thresholds were lower in the non-congruent (26 ms) than in the congruent (55 ms) condition [p <.02]. Temporal thresholds in the control condition were in between (34 ms). With these data we confirm that the difference in saliency cannot be explained by collinearity at the edge. The non-congruency effect instead reflects a facilitation from the larger texture-region. The results of Experiments 1, 2 and 3 suggest two distinct contextual and non-local effects. One results from elements along the edge being collinear and parallel. The other results from elements in the larger texture-region that, if collinear and non-parallel to the edge, facilitate segmentation an effect that increases with the elements being linked (i.e. with texture size becoming larger). This second effect is consistent with a region-based account, predicting that collinear elements in the larger texture facilitate detection when non-parallel to the edge. Note that this explanation would predict that perturbation of collinearity should impair edge segmentation. However, also a local inhibitory effect of individual remote texels oriented as the edge in the congruent condition could account for the reduced saliency of the edge. Indeed, close facilitatory elements are present in all the three conditions but remote, inhibitory (Wolfson & Landy, 1999) texels are more numerous in the congruent, less numerous in the control, and absent in the non-congruent. Consequently, for this local explanation the crucial factor would be the individual orientation of texels, independently of their groupings by collinearity. To test this possibility we repeated Experiment 3 by presenting jittered and non-jittered patterns with equal probability. The local account would predict a similar performance Experiment 4 In Experiment 4, we jittered the spatial position of each element by 10 arcmin, in a random direction, to impede element collinearity (Fig. 1d). If facilitation in edge segmentation resulted from grouping of collinear elements both along the edge and in the larger texture, we predicted a stronger effect of jittering in the non-congruent condition than in the congruent condition. Fig. 6 shows that accuracy increased with duration. The ANOVA revealed a significant effect of congruency [F (1,5) = 30.1; p <.005] and of the jittering congruency interaction [F (1,5) = 7.0; p <.05]. Post hoc showed that the effect of jittering was significant only in the non-congruent condition [p <.05]. Oddly, jittering seems to improve performance in the congruent condition, but the effect is consistent only at 60 ms. The results for the non-congruent condition rule out the possibility that the facilitation comes from local inhibitory effect, because of remote individual texels iso-oriented with the edge, independent of grouping by collinearity. The data instead suggest that the non-congruency effect is due to region analysis, based on contextual facilitation resulting Fig. 6. Results of Experiment 4. Mean accuracy of the six subjects is plotted as a function of stimulus duration (ms), separately for jittered (dotted lines) and non-jittered stimulus (continuous lines), both congruent (squares) and non-congruent (circles). from excitatory lateral interactions amongst co-axial filters (Lamme & Roelfsema, 2000) Experiment 5 Overall, the results of previous experiments suggest that the relationship between the orientation of stripes resulting from collinear grouping and boundary orientation is the crucial factor. Nonetheless, one can argue that the enhanced discrimination for the non-congruent condition could result from having collinearity facilitation in both regions, leading to a strengthening and linking of the 1ststage filter output. Therefore, a simple edge-based mechanism would predict an increment of gradient detector response following a stronger activation due to collinearity. Consequently, the final response of edge extraction would be higher in the non-congruent condition, where collinearity is present in both the larger region and the more external stripe. Another possibility is that the interaction between stripes and edge orientation could actually be important, simply because of an inhibitory effect from remote stripes parallel to the edge. To distinguish amongst alternative explanations, we compared the control condition of Experiment 2 with stripes in the texture-region, both parallel and orthogonal to the edge (symmetrical 0/90 deg, see Fig. 1e, left) with a pattern having texels in both regions collinear, but neither parallel nor orthogonal to the boundary (symmetrical 45/ 135 deg, see Fig. 1e, right). If facilitation in the non-congruent condition resulted simply from collinearity on both sides of the edge, one would not expect the performance to differ. Furthermore, the absence of inhibition from remote stripes parallel to the edge should enhance edge saliency in the symmetrical 45/135 deg. Only, if the higher saliency of the non-congruent stimulus resulted from both facilitation at the edge and facilitation by region-based analysis, would we expect better performance in the symmetrical 0/90 deg condition. Results are shown in Fig. 7. Data appear coherent with those already obtained by Wolfson & Landy (1995) with

6 884 E. Giora, C. Casco / Vision Research 47 (2007) Fig. 7. Results of Experiment 5. Mean accuracy of the four subjects is plotted as a function of stimulus duration, separately for the symmetrical 0/90 deg (continuous lines) and 45/135 deg (dotted line) patterns. similar patterns. Temporal thresholds are much lower in the symmetrical 0/90 deg (31.5 ms) condition than in the symmetrical 45/135 deg (68 ms) condition [t (3) = 6.62, p <.01]. These results rule out the possibility that a facilitation depended on the presence of collinearity in both regions. Moreover, the data do not indicate an inhibitory effect resulting from parallel stripes remote from the edge. Instead, the important variable is the relationship between the orientation of stripes resulting from collinear grouping and the orientation of the boundary. 4. Discussion In summary, the five experiments showed that segmentation was affected by configurational factors both at the edge and from the texture-region. Experiment 1 showed higher saliency for edges with abutting texels on one side iso-oriented and parallel to it. Experiment 2 showed a new facilitatory effect resulting when collinear elements in the larger texture are non-parallel to the edge. This result, confirmed by Experiment 3, is compatible with a regionbased mechanism. The finding that jittering reduces the facilitation from the region (Experiment 4) rules out explanations based on local effects of individual texels, independent of grouping by collinearity (Wolfson & Landy, 1999). Experiment 5, showing better performance in the symmetrical 0/90 deg than in the symmetrical 45/135 deg, indicates that facilitation results not just from a simple effect of collinearity per se in both regions, but because of the specific relationship between the orientation of stripes resulting from collinear grouping and the orientation of the boundary. It has also been shown that remote parallel stripes do not have an inhibitory effect on edge extraction. Overall, the main finding is that texture segmentation is facilitated not only when abutting elements are parallel to the edge but also from groupings in the larger texture, non-parallel to edge orientation. The two facilitatory effects cannot be interpreted within the framework of energy models based on Filter-Rectify- Filter operations. The underlying assumption of these models is that the detection of texture differences does not occur locally (Nothdurft, 1992) but results from a global operation that is based on relative (not absolute) orientation at the level of local 1st-order and global 2nd-order filtering. The dependency on global orientation contrast, predicted by Filter-Rectify-Filter models, does not, however, manage to explain the new finding of a facilitation from the texture-region, because these models should be indifferent to whether texture is larger or smaller. Therefore, our effects are necessarily based on the facilitatory interaction between spatial channels and a model explicitly based on these interactions has to be considered to account for them. The idea that spatial channels, rather than being independent, spatially interact, has often been contemplated by models of texture perception. For example, Caelli s (1985) model proposed linking operations that follow the Gestalt rules for grouping e.g., proximity, similarity and good continuation (Beck, Prazdny, & Rosenfeld, 1983) based on spread of neural activity emanating from highly stimulated detectors. It is known from the pioneering work of Gestalt psychology (Wertheimer, 1922) that iso-oriented and collinear elements group together, and recent investigations demonstrate that lateral interactions in the primary visual cortex can account for grouping phenomena (Gilbert, Ito, Kapadia, & Westheimer, 2000; Stettler, Das, Bennett, & Gilbert, 2002; Zipser, Lamme, & Schiller, 1996). These recent neurophysiological data lead to the assumption that regionbased mechanisms treat neighbouring texture-regions as belonging to the same texture if texels are both iso-oriented and collinear (i.e. texels and receptive field orientation are co-axial). A region-based mechanism could account for edge segmentation at a 1st-stage of processing with no need for energy-gradient extraction, explaining the non-congruent facilitation. On the other hand, that mechanism would fail to detect the edge in the congruent condition, because spreading of the neural activation when texels are iso-oriented and collinear would occur in the same direction for all stripes and produce, in the stripe with no collinear elements, less salient groups. Because of this failure of the region-based mechanism, activation of an edge-based mechanism is required, at a successive stage of processing, to account for edge segmentation in the congruent condition. Note that this two-stages explanation is based on the strong assumption that region-based analysis precedes edge extraction. Further data suggest that grouping by collinearity occurs before edge extraction (Giora & Casco, 2006). Lateral connectivity occurring in V1 could well be the underlying neural mechanism (Stettler et al., 2002). However, there is another possible interpretation for the facilitation at the edge and from the larger texture. At the edge, grouping between elements parallel to the edge can modulate the output of the edge-based mechanisms at some stage during the Filter-Rectify-Filter operations, probably at the level of the 1st-stage filtering or after rectification. Instead, facilitation from the region could reflect

7 E. Giora, C. Casco / Vision Research 47 (2007) activation of 2nd-order filters selective for orientation, spatial-frequency and length: facilitation could result when 2nd-order filters are stimulated by elongated groups of texels, formed by collinear linking with the same spatialfrequency and length but different orientation on either side of the edge. Several sets of data suggest that 2nd-order channels are orientation and spatial-frequency tuned (Arsenault, Wilkinson, & Kingdom, 1999; Kingdom & Keeble, 1996; Kwan & Regan, 1998). Landy & Oruç (2002) proposed that the 2nd-stage filters receive differently-oriented 1st-stage inputs to the centre and surround of the receptive field. They are therefore tuned to orientation differences rather than orientation per se. If these 2nd-order mechanisms were also end-stopped (Yu & Levi, 1997) they would prefer stimuli on the two sides of their receptive field having different orientation but identical spatial-frequency and length. Assuming groupings of iso-oriented and collinear elements before 2nd-order filtering, their optimal activation would occur when groups of appropriate orientation contrast (matching the orientation contrast preferred by a filter) fell on the receptive field. This hypothesis of 2ndorder filters selective not only for spatial-frequency and orientation contrast but also for length, would explain why congruent is worse than control and control is worse than non-congruent. Indeed, only in the non-congruent condition do oriented groups have the same orientation and length as the edge, whereas in the control and congruent conditions the length of groups is shorter in one side of the edge. To conclude, our account contrasts with the prominent view that segmentation is a first stage in the hierarchical organization of visual processing modules of increasing complexity edges, surfaces, objects (Marr, 1982). Our results indicate that overall-orientation information biases texture segmentation, thereby supporting the suggestion (Oliva & Torralba, 2001) that the holistic representation of an artificial image in the laboratory, as well as a scene in the real world, is an operation taking place earlier than the segmentation itself. Acknowledgments The work described in this paper was supported by a Grant (PRIN 2003) for the Ph.D. programme of the first author. We would like to thank Dr. G. Campana, Prof. M.S. Landy and Prof. M.J. Morgan for a critical reading of the manuscript and their useful suggestions. References Ariely, D. (2001). Seeing sets: representation by statistical properties. Psychological Science, 12, Arsenault, A. S., Wilkinson, F., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (1999). Modulation frequency and orientation tuning of second-order texture mechanisms. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 16, Beck, J., Prazdny, K., & Rosenfeld, A. (1983). A theory of textural segmentation. In J. Beck, B. Hope, & A. Rosenfeld (Eds.), Human and machine vision (pp. 1 38). New York: Academic. Ben Shahar, O. (2006). Visual saliency and texture segregation without feature gradient. Proceedings of the National Academy of the Sciences of the United States of America, 103, Caelli, T. (1985). Three processing characteristics of visual texture segmentation. Spatial Vision, 1, Caputo, G. (1996). The role of the background: texture segmentation and figure-ground segmentation. Vision Research, 36, Caputo, G., & Casco, C. (1999). A visual evoked potential correlate of global figure-ground segmentation. Vision Research, 39, Casco, C., Campana, G., Grieco, A., & Fuggetta, G. (2004). Perceptual learning modulates electrophysiological and psychophysical response to visual texture segmentation in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 371, Casco, C., Grieco, A., Campana, G., Corvino, M. P., & Caputo, G. (2005). Attention modulates psychophysical and electrophysiological response to visual texture segmentation in humans. Vision Research, 45, Field, D. J., Hayes, A., & Hess, R. F. (1993). Contour integration by the human visual system: evidence for a local association field. Vision Research, 33, Finney, D. J. (1971). Probit analysis. London: Cambridge University Press. Gilbert, C., Ito, M., Kapadia, M., & Westheimer, G. (2000). Interactions between attention, context and learning in primary visual cortex. Vision Research, 40, Giora, E., & Casco, C. (2006). Seeing texture figures before boundaries. Perception, 35(Suppl.), 74. Hess, R. F., & Dakin, S. C. (1997). Absence of contour linking in peripheral vision. Nature, 390, Howell, D. C. (2002). Statistical methods for psychology (5th ed). Belmont, CA: Duxberry Press. Kingdom, F. A. A., & Keeble, D. R. T. (1996). A linear systems approach to the detection of both abrupt and smooth spatial variations in orientation-defined textures. Vision Research, 36, Kwan, L., & Regan, D. (1998). Orientation-tuned spatial filters for texture-defined form. Vision Research, 38, Lamme, V. A. F. (2004). Beyond the classical receptive field: contextual modulation of V1 responses. In L. M. Chalupa & J. S. Werner (Eds.). The visual neurosciences (Vol. I, pp ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Lamme, V. A. F., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23, Landy, M. S., & Graham, N. (2004). Visual perception of texture. In L. M. Chalupa & J. S. Werner (Eds.). The visual neurosciences (Vol. II, pp ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Landy, M. S., & Oruç, I. _ (2002). Properties of second-order spatial frequency channels. Vision Research, 42, Lee, T. S. (1995). A Bayesan framework for understanding texture segmentation in the primary visual cortex. Vision Research, 35, Li, W., & Gilbert, C. D. (2002). Global contour saliency and local colinear interactions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88, Malik, J., & Perona, P. (1990). Preattentive texture discrimination with early vision mechanisms. Journal of Optical Society of America A, 7, Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: Calif.: Freeman. Nothdurft, H. C. (1992). Feature analysis and the role of similarity in preattentive vision. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, Oliva, A., & Torralba, A. (2001). Modeling the shape of the scene: a holistic representation of the spatial envelope. International Journal of Computer Vision, 42, Olson, R. K., & Attneave, F. (1970). What variables produce similarity grouping? American Journal of Psychology, 83, 1 21.

8 886 E. Giora, C. Casco / Vision Research 47 (2007) Parkes, L., Lund, J., Angelucci, A., Solomon, J. A., & Morgan, M. (2001). Compulsory averaging of crowded orientation signals in human vision. Nature Neuroscience, 4, Polat, U., & Sagi, D. (1993). Lateral interactions between spatial channels: suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments. Vision Research, 33, Polat, U., & Sagi, D. (1994). The architecture of perceptual spatial interactions. Vision Research, 34, Roelfsema, P. R., Lamme, V. A., Spekreijse, H., & Bosch, H. (2002). Figure-ground segregation in a recurrent network architecture. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, Sagi, D. (1991). Spatial filters in texture segmentation tasks. In B. Blum (Ed.), Channels in the visual nervous system: neurophysiology, psychophysics and models (pp ). London and Tel Aviv: Freund Publishing House. Stettler, D. D., Das, A., Bennett, J., & Gilbert, C. D. (2002). Lateral connectivity and contextual interactions in macaque primary visual cortex. Neuron, 36, Yu, C., & Levi, D. M. (1997). End stopping and length tuning in psychophysical spatial filters. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 14, Wertheimer, M. (1922). Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt I. Psychologische Forschung, 1, Wolfson, S. S., & Landy, M. S. (1995). Discrimination of orientationdefined texture edges. Vision Research, 35, Wolfson, S. S., & Landy, M. S. (1999). Long range interactions between oriented texture elements. Vision Research, 39, Zipser, K., Lamme, V. A. F., & Schiller, P. H. (1996). Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 16,

Dynamics of snakes and ladders

Dynamics of snakes and ladders Journal of Vision (2007) 7(12):13, 1 9 http://journalofvision.org/7/12/13/ 1 Dynamics of snakes and ladders Keith A. May Robert F. Hess McGill Vision Research Unit, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill

More information

Contextual influences on orientation discrimination: binding local and global cues

Contextual influences on orientation discrimination: binding local and global cues Vision Research 41 (2001) 1915 1930 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Contextual influences on orientation discrimination: binding local and global cues Isabelle Mareschal *, Michael P. Sceniak 1, Robert

More information

Vision Research 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 50 (2010) 473 478 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Specificity of fast perceptual learning in shape localisation

More information

Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system

Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system Vision Research 39 (1999) 4333 4340 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system Mark Edwards *, Clifton M. Schor School of Optometry, Uni ersity of California, Berkeley,

More information

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity S. B. Stevenson *, L. K. Cormack Vision Research 40, 2881-2884. (2000) * University of Houston College of Optometry, Houston TX 77204

More information

Transducer model produces facilitation from opposite-sign flanks

Transducer model produces facilitation from opposite-sign flanks Vision Research 39 (1999) 987 992 Transducer model produces facilitation from opposite-sign flanks Joshua A. Solomon a, *, Andrew B. Watson b, Michael J. Morgan a a Institute of Ophthalmology, Bath Street,

More information

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli Vision Research 39 (1999) 3849 3854 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli A. Johnston a, *, C.P.

More information

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit Vision Research 45 (2005) 1877 1884 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit Mark Edwards *, John A. Greenwood School of Psychology, Australian National

More information

Luminance spatial frequency differences facilitate the segmentation of superimposed textures

Luminance spatial frequency differences facilitate the segmentation of superimposed textures Vision Research 40 (2000) 1077 1087 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Luminance spatial frequency differences facilitate the segmentation of superimposed textures Frederick A.A. Kingdom a, *, David R.T. Keeble

More information

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing C Contextual Influences in Visual Processing TAI SING LEE Computer Science Department and Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Synonyms Surround influence;

More information

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Javier R. Movellan Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0515 movellan@inc.ucsd.edu Thomas

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

Spatial Distribution of Contextual Interactions in Primary Visual Cortex and in Visual Perception

Spatial Distribution of Contextual Interactions in Primary Visual Cortex and in Visual Perception Spatial Distribution of Contextual Interactions in Primary Visual Cortex and in Visual Perception MITESH K. KAPADIA, 1,2 GERALD WESTHEIMER, 1 AND CHARLES D. GILBERT 1 1 The Rockefeller University, New

More information

Seeing circles: what limits shape perception?

Seeing circles: what limits shape perception? Vision Research 40 (240) 2329 2339 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Seeing circles: what limits shape perception? Dennis M. Levi a, *, Stanley A. Klein b a Uni ersity of Houston, College of Optometry, Houston,

More information

The Architecture of Perceptual Spatial Interactions

The Architecture of Perceptual Spatial Interactions Vision Res. Vol. 34, No. 1, pp. 73-78, 1994 Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved 0042-6989/94 $6.00 + 0.00 copyright 0 1993 Pergamon Press Ltd The Architecture of Perceptual Spatial Interactions

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

C ontextual modulation is a general phenomenon that relates to changes in the perceived appearance of

C ontextual modulation is a general phenomenon that relates to changes in the perceived appearance of OPEN SUBJECT AREAS: HUMAN BEHAVIOUR CORTEX Received 1 April 2014 Accepted 13 November 2014 Published 28 November 2014 Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to U.P. (urip@post.tau.

More information

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I Monocular Sensory Processes of Vision: Color Vision Mechanisms of Color Processing . Neural Mechanisms of Color Processing A. Parallel processing - M- & P- pathways B. Second

More information

Spatial or Temporal 2AFC May Give Different Results Depending on Context

Spatial or Temporal 2AFC May Give Different Results Depending on Context Spatial or Temporal 2AFC May Give Different Results Depending on Context E. Peli 1, M.A. García-Pérez 2, R.G. Giorgi 1, R.L. Woods 1 1 Schepens Eye Research Institute, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA

More information

Contour Integration by the Human Visual System: Evidence for a Local Association Field

Contour Integration by the Human Visual System: Evidence for a Local Association Field Vision Res. Vol. 33, No. 2, pp. 173-193, 1993 Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved 0042-6989/93 $5.00 + 0.00 Copyright 0 1993 Pergamon Press Ltd Contour Integration by the Human Visual System:

More information

Change detection is easier at texture border bars when they are parallel to the border: Evidence for V1 mechanisms of bottom ^ up salience

Change detection is easier at texture border bars when they are parallel to the border: Evidence for V1 mechanisms of bottom ^ up salience Perception, 2008, volume 37, pages 197 ^ 206 doi:10.1068/p5829 Change detection is easier at texture border bars when they are parallel to the border: Evidence for V1 mechanisms of bottom ^ up salience

More information

Contour grouping: closure effects are explained by good continuation and proximity

Contour grouping: closure effects are explained by good continuation and proximity Vision Research xxx (2004) xxx xxx www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Contour grouping: closure effects are explained by good continuation and proximity Tal Tversky a,b, Wilson S. Geisler a,c, *, Jeffrey S.

More information

Measurement and modeling of center-surround suppression and enhancement

Measurement and modeling of center-surround suppression and enhancement Vision Research 41 (2001) 571 583 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Measurement and modeling of center-surround suppression and enhancement Jing Xing, David J. Heeger * Department of Psychology, Jordan Hall,

More information

Response profiles to texture border patterns in area V1

Response profiles to texture border patterns in area V1 Visual Neuroscience (2000), 17, 421 436. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2000 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238000 $12.50 Response profiles to texture border patterns in area V1 HANS-CHRISTOPH NOTHDURFT,

More information

Spatial versus temporal grouping in a modified Ternus display

Spatial versus temporal grouping in a modified Ternus display Vision Research 47 (2007) 2353 2366 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Spatial versus temporal grouping in a modified Ternus display Julian M. Wallace *, Nicholas E. Scott-Samuel Department of Experimental

More information

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Current Biology, in press Supplemental Information: Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Grigorios Sotiropoulos, Aaron R. Seitz, and Peggy Seriès Supplemental Data Detailed

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

Orientation-selective adaptation to crowded illusory lines

Orientation-selective adaptation to crowded illusory lines Perception, 2003, volume 32, pages 1199 ^ 1210 DOI:10.1068/p76 Orientation-selective adaptation to crowded illusory lines Reza Rajimehr, Leila Montaser-Kouhsari, Seyed-Reza Afrazô Cognitive Neuroscience

More information

Perceptual Grouping in a Self-Organizing Map of Spiking Neurons

Perceptual Grouping in a Self-Organizing Map of Spiking Neurons Perceptual Grouping in a Self-Organizing Map of Spiking Neurons Yoonsuck Choe Department of Computer Sciences The University of Texas at Austin August 13, 2001 Perceptual Grouping Group Two! Longest Contour?

More information

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material Supplemental Material Recording technique Multi-unit activity (MUA) was recorded from electrodes that were chronically implanted (Teflon-coated platinum-iridium wires) in the primary visual cortex representing

More information

Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot

Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot Significance of Natural Scene Statistics in Understanding the Anisotropies of Perceptual Filling-in at the Blind Spot Rajani Raman* and Sandip Sarkar Saha Institute of Nuclear Physics, HBNI, 1/AF, Bidhannagar,

More information

Two Visual Contrast Processes: One New, One Old

Two Visual Contrast Processes: One New, One Old 1 Two Visual Contrast Processes: One New, One Old Norma Graham and S. Sabina Wolfson In everyday life, we occasionally look at blank, untextured regions of the world around us a blue unclouded sky, for

More information

City, University of London Institutional Repository. This version of the publication may differ from the final published version.

City, University of London Institutional Repository. This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Solomon, J. A. & Morgan, M. J. (2017). Orientation-defined boundaries are detected with low efficiency. Vision Research,

More information

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Baran Çürüklü Department of Computer Science and Engineering Mälardalen University Västerås S-721 23, Sweden Abstract. In this paper

More information

Attention enhances feature integration

Attention enhances feature integration Vision Research 43 (2003) 1793 1798 Rapid Communication Attention enhances feature integration www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Liza Paul *, Philippe G. Schyns Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow,

More information

Collinear facilitation: Effect of additive and multiplicative external noise

Collinear facilitation: Effect of additive and multiplicative external noise Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Vision Research 47 (2007) 3108 3119 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Collinear facilitation: Effect of additive and multiplicative external noise Pi-Chun Huang *,

More information

Effect of colour pop-out on the recognition of letters in crowding conditions

Effect of colour pop-out on the recognition of letters in crowding conditions Psychological Research (07) 71: 641 645 DOI 10.7/s00426-006-0053-7 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Endel Põder Effect of colour pop-out on the recognition of letters in crowding conditions Received: 2 March 05 / Accepted:

More information

Psychophysical tests of the hypothesis of a bottom-up saliency map in primary visual cortex

Psychophysical tests of the hypothesis of a bottom-up saliency map in primary visual cortex Psychophysical tests of the hypothesis of a bottom-up saliency map in primary visual cortex In press for Public Library of Science, Computational Biology, (2007). Citation: Zhaoping L. May KA (2007) Psychophysical

More information

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception EDGE DETECTION Edge Detectors Slide 2 Convolution & Feature Detection Slide 3 Finds the slope First derivative Direction dependent Need many edge detectors for all orientation Second order derivatives

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Perceived motion in orientational afterimages: direction and speed

Perceived motion in orientational afterimages: direction and speed Vision Research 41 (2001) 161 172 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Perceived motion in orientational afterimages: direction and speed Gregory Francis a, *, Hyungjun Kim b a Purdue Uni ersity, Department

More information

Mechanism independence for texture-modulation detection is consistent with a filter-rectify-filter mechanism

Mechanism independence for texture-modulation detection is consistent with a filter-rectify-filter mechanism Visual Neuroscience (2003), 20, 65 76. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2003 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238003 $16.00 DOI: 10.10170S0952523803201073 Mechanism independence for texture-modulation detection

More information

V isual crowding is the inability to recognize objects in clutter and sets a fundamental limit on conscious

V isual crowding is the inability to recognize objects in clutter and sets a fundamental limit on conscious OPEN SUBJECT AREAS: PSYCHOLOGY VISUAL SYSTEM OBJECT VISION PATTERN VISION Received 23 May 2013 Accepted 14 January 2014 Published 12 February 2014 Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed

More information

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities David P. McGovern, Andrew T. Astle, Sarah L. Clavin and Fiona N. Newell Figure S1: Group-averaged learning

More information

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay*

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay* Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay* JACOB BECK and BRUCE AMBLER University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 other extreme, when no masking

More information

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth 1574 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/ Vol. 25, No. 7/ July 2008 Shioiri et al. Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth Satoshi Shioiri, 1, * Tomohiko

More information

Segregation from direction differences in dynamic random-dot stimuli

Segregation from direction differences in dynamic random-dot stimuli Vision Research 43(2003) 171 180 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Segregation from direction differences in dynamic random-dot stimuli Scott N.J. Watamaniuk *, Jeff Flinn, R. Eric Stohr Department of Psychology,

More information

When crowding of crowding leads to uncrowding

When crowding of crowding leads to uncrowding Journal of Vision (2013) 13(13):10, 1 10 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/13/10 1 When crowding of crowding leads to uncrowding Laboratory of Psychophysics, Brain Mind Institute, Ecole Polytechnique

More information

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück The Binding Problem This lecture is based on following articles: Adina L. Roskies: The Binding Problem; Neuron 1999 24: 7 Charles M. Gray: The Temporal Correlation Hypothesis of Visual Feature Integration:

More information

Object recognition and hierarchical computation

Object recognition and hierarchical computation Object recognition and hierarchical computation Challenges in object recognition. Fukushima s Neocognitron View-based representations of objects Poggio s HMAX Forward and Feedback in visual hierarchy Hierarchical

More information

The processing of feature discontinuities for different cue types in primary visual cortex

The processing of feature discontinuities for different cue types in primary visual cortex BRAIN RESEARCH 1238 (2008) 59 74 available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report The processing of feature discontinuities for different cue types in primary visual

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system

Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system Vision Research 38 (1998) 3057 3068 Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system Clifton M. Schor *, Mark Edwards, David R. Pope School of Optometry, Uni ersity of California,

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Vision Research 61 (2012) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 61 (2012) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 1 (2012) ontents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Task relevancy and demand modulate double-training enabled transfer of

More information

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) What happens beyond the retina? What happens in Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)- 90% flow Visual cortex Information Flow Superior colliculus 10% flow Slide 2 Information

More information

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1 Announcements exam 1 this Thursday! review session: Wednesday, 5:00-6:30pm, Meliora 203 Bryce s office hours: Wednesday, 3:30-5:30pm, Gleason https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zdw7pvgz0um M Cells M cells

More information

Learning to find a shape

Learning to find a shape articles Learning to find a shape M. Sigman and C. D. Gilbert The Rockefeller University, 1230 York Avenue, New York, New York 10021-6399, USA Correspondence should be addressed to C.D.G. (gilbert@rockvax.rockefeller.edu)

More information

Orientation tuning of the transient-stereopsis system

Orientation tuning of the transient-stereopsis system Vision Research 39 (1999) 2717 2727 Orientation tuning of the transient-stereopsis system Mark Edwards *, David R. Pope, Clifton M. Schor School of Optometry, Uni ersity of California, Berkeley, California,

More information

The Structuralist Approach

The Structuralist Approach The Structuralist Approach Approach established by Wundt (1830-1920) States that perceptions are created by combining elements called sensations Popular in mid to late 19 th century Wundt studied conscious

More information

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK?

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK? CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK? Bobby Nguyen 1, Yan Zhuo 2 & Rui Ni 1 1 Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA 2 Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy

More information

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SESORY COSTRAIS? TOWARD A UIFIED THEORY OF GEERAL TASK DIFFICULTY Hanna Benoni and Yehoshua Tsal Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University hannaben@post.tau.ac.il

More information

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Paul E. Hemeren (paul.hemeren@his.se) University of Skövde, School of Humanities and Informatics

More information

Vision Research 67 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 67 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 67 (2012) 37 43 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Positional noise in Landolt-C stimuli reduces spatial

More information

Alignment of separated patches: multiple location tags

Alignment of separated patches: multiple location tags Vision Research 39 (1999) 789 801 Alignment of separated patches: multiple location tags Hiromi Akutsu a, Paul V. McGraw b, Dennis M. Levi a, * a Uniersity of Houston, College of Optometry, Houston, TX

More information

Principals of Object Perception

Principals of Object Perception Principals of Object Perception Elizabeth S. Spelke COGNITIVE SCIENCE 14, 29-56 (1990) Cornell University Summary Infants perceive object by analyzing tree-dimensional surface arrangements and motions.

More information

Is the straddle effect in contrast perception limited to secondorder spatial vision?

Is the straddle effect in contrast perception limited to secondorder spatial vision? Journal of Vision (2018) 18(5):15, 1 43 1 Is the straddle effect in contrast perception limited to secondorder spatial vision? Norma V. Graham Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY,

More information

Detecting disorder in spatial vision

Detecting disorder in spatial vision Vision Research 40 (2000) 2307 2327 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Detecting disorder in spatial vision Dennis M. Levi a, *, Stanley A. Klein b, Vineeta Sharma a,1, Lisa Nguyen a a Uni ersity of Houston,

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Vision Research 8 (8) 6 67 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Multi-component correlate for lateral collinear interactions in the

More information

Object vision (Chapter 4)

Object vision (Chapter 4) Object vision (Chapter 4) Lecture 8 Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Spring 2015 1 Outline for today: Chap 3: adaptation Chap 4: intro to object vision gestalt

More information

Perceptual consequences of centre-surround antagonism in visual motion processing

Perceptual consequences of centre-surround antagonism in visual motion processing 1 Perceptual consequences of centre-surround antagonism in visual motion processing Duje Tadin, Joseph S. Lappin, Lee A. Gilroy and Randolph Blake Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Vanderbilt University,

More information

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 49 (2009) 1006 1016 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres A new approach to the study of detail perception in Autism

More information

First- and second-order processing in transient stereopsis

First- and second-order processing in transient stereopsis Vision Research 40 (2000) 2645 2651 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres First- and second-order processing in transient stereopsis Mark Edwards *, David R. Pope, Clifton M. Schor School of Optometry, Uni ersity

More information

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15 s PSY 200 Greg Francis Lecture 06 How do you recognize your grandmother? Action potential With enough excitatory input, a cell produces an action potential that sends a signal down its axon to other cells

More information

The locus of attentional effects in texture segmentation

The locus of attentional effects in texture segmentation articles The locus of attentional effects in texture segmentation Yaffa Yeshurun 1 and Marisa Carrasco 1,2 1 Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York 10003, USA 2 Department of

More information

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 49 (2009) 553 568 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres A model of non-linear interactions between cortical top-down

More information

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017 V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8 Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017 Topography: mapping of objects in space onto the visual cortex contralateral representation

More information

Colin Ware Center for Coastal and Ocean Mapping University of New Hampshire.

Colin Ware Center for Coastal and Ocean Mapping University of New Hampshire. Abstract Towards a Perceptual Theory of Flow Visualization Colin Ware Center for Coastal and Ocean Mapping University of New Hampshire. At present there is very little attention paid to vision science

More information

You may not alter the pdf file, as changes to the published contribution are prohibited by copyright law.

You may not alter the pdf file, as changes to the published contribution are prohibited by copyright law. 12 Springer Dear Author: Please find attached the final pdf file of your contribution, which can be viewed using the Acrobat Reader, version 3.0 or higher. We would kindly like to draw your attention to

More information

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2015 Laboratory 1 Noisy Representations: The Oblique Effect

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2015 Laboratory 1 Noisy Representations: The Oblique Effect Psychology 4165, Laboratory 1 Noisy Representations: The Oblique Effect Probability of Clockwise Response 0.0 0.4 0.8 Orientation Discrimination at 0.0 Deg 0 deg -10-5 0 5 10 Orientation of Test Stimulus

More information

Low Level Constraints on Dynamic Contour Path Integration

Low Level Constraints on Dynamic Contour Path Integration Low Level Constraints on Dynamic Contour Path Integration Sophie Hall*, Patrick Bourke, Kun Guo School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, United Kingdom Abstract Contour integration is a fundamental

More information

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions Tsung-Ren Huang Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University trhuang@ntu.edu.tw Abstract Shibata et al. reported that humans could learn to

More information

Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation

Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation articles Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation Paul V. McGraw 1, Dennis M. Levi 2 and David Whitaker 1 1 Department of Optometry, University of Bradford,

More information

What is mid level vision? Mid Level Vision. What is mid level vision? Lightness perception as revealed by lightness illusions

What is mid level vision? Mid Level Vision. What is mid level vision? Lightness perception as revealed by lightness illusions What is mid level vision? Mid Level Vision March 18, 2004 Josh McDermott Perception involves inferring the structure of the world from measurements of energy generated by the world (in vision, this is

More information

Recognizing partially visible objects

Recognizing partially visible objects Vision Research 45 (2005) 1807 1814 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Recognizing partially visible objects Philip Servos a, *, Elizabeth S. Olds a, Peggy J. Planetta a, G. Keith Humphrey b a Department of

More information

Models of Attention. Models of Attention

Models of Attention. Models of Attention Models of Models of predictive: can we predict eye movements (bottom up attention)? [L. Itti and coll] pop out and saliency? [Z. Li] Readings: Maunsell & Cook, the role of attention in visual processing,

More information

Limitations of Object-Based Feature Encoding in Visual Short-Term Memory

Limitations of Object-Based Feature Encoding in Visual Short-Term Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 458 468 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.458

More information

Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces

Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces Vision Research 43 (2003) 1501 1505 Rapid communication Configural information is processed differently in perception and recognition of faces Adrian Schwaninger a,b, *, Stefan Ryf b, Franziska Hofer b

More information

Motion parallel to line orientation: Disambiguation of motion percepts

Motion parallel to line orientation: Disambiguation of motion percepts ion, 1999, volume 28, pages 1243 ^ 1255 DOI:.68/p298 Motion parallel to line orientation: Disambiguation of motion percepts Gregory Francis, Hyungjun Kim Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University,

More information

Coherent plaids are preattentively more than the sum of their parts

Coherent plaids are preattentively more than the sum of their parts Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 29, 71 (7), 1469-1477 doi:1.3758/app.71.7.1469 RESEARCH ARTICLES Coherent plaids are preattentively more than the sum of their parts JONG-HO NAM Catholic University

More information

Selective attention and cyclopean motion processing

Selective attention and cyclopean motion processing Vision Research 45 (2005) 2601 2607 Brief communication Selective attention and cyclopean motion processing Robert Patterson *, Lisa R. Fournier, Matt Wiediger, Greg Vavrek, Cheryl Becker-Dippman, Ivan

More information

Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies

Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies Supplemental Material for Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies Timo Stein, Kiley Seymour, Martin N. Hebart, and Philipp Sterzer Additional experimental details Participants Volunteers

More information

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2008, 61 (4), 552 557 Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them Jeffrey S. Bowers and Keely W. Jones University of Bristol, Bristol,

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance VOL. I I, NO. 6 DECEMBER 1985 Separability and Integrality of Global and Local Levels of Hierarchical Patterns Ruth Kimchi University

More information

B.A. II Psychology - Paper A. Form Perception. Dr. Neelam Rathee. Department of Psychology G.C.G.-11, Chandigarh

B.A. II Psychology - Paper A. Form Perception. Dr. Neelam Rathee. Department of Psychology G.C.G.-11, Chandigarh B.A. II Psychology - Paper A Form Perception Dr. Neelam Rathee Department of Psychology G.C.G.-11, Chandigarh Form Perception What it is? How do we recognize an object? (form perception) 2 Perception of

More information

Modeling the Deployment of Spatial Attention

Modeling the Deployment of Spatial Attention 17 Chapter 3 Modeling the Deployment of Spatial Attention 3.1 Introduction When looking at a complex scene, our visual system is confronted with a large amount of visual information that needs to be broken

More information

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes The Puzzle of Object and Scene Perception The stimulus on the receptors is ambiguous. Inverse projection problem: An image on the retina can be caused by an infinite

More information

Neural synergy in visual grouping: when good continuation meets common fate

Neural synergy in visual grouping: when good continuation meets common fate Vision Research 41 (2001) 2057 2064 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Neural synergy in visual grouping: when good continuation meets common fate Sang-Hun Lee, Randolph Blake * Vanderbilt Vision Research

More information