Coaching Climates and the Destructive Effects of Mastery- Avoidance Achievement Goals on Situational Motivation

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Coaching Climates and the Destructive Effects of Mastery- Avoidance Achievement Goals on Situational Motivation"

Transcription

1 JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2006, 28, Human Kinetics, Inc. Coaching Climates and the Destructive Effects of Mastery- Avoidance Achievement Goals on Situational Motivation David E. Conroy 1, Miranda P. Kaye 1, and J. Douglas Coatsworth 2 The Pennsylvania State University The present research tested a model of social-cognitive influences on situational motivation (i.e., youths reasons for participating in sport at a given moment in time) via youths 2 2 achievement goals. Boys and girls (N N = 165) participating in a summer swim league completed measures of their achievement goals and situational motivation on multiple occasions during a 6-week period; they also rated the coaching climate at the end of the season. All Situational Motivation Scale responses exhibited acceptable levels of longitudinal factorial invariance. Latent growth curve analyses revealed that intrinsic motivation and identified regulation did not appear to change over the course of the season; however, external regulation and amotivation increased significantly during that period. Youths perceptions of an avoidance-oriented coaching climate predicted corresponding residualized change in their own achievement goals over the season. Additionally, residualized change in youths mastery-avoidance goals (i.e., focus on avoiding self-referenced incompetence) was positively linked to the rate at which external regulation and amotivation scores changed. Key Words: competence, approach-avoidance, extrinsic motivation Coaches fill important roles in children s sport experiences as instructors, role models, or mentors. In addition to guiding skill development, coaches can have a pronounced impact on the self-concept, motivation, and affective experience of young athletes. Motivation is a particularly important youth outcome that coaches can influence because it can affect the quality of youths experience in an activity as well as their likelihood of participating in that activity in the future. Surprisingly little is known about the mechanisms by which coaches influence youth motivation as the course of their interactions unfolds. Research on youth sport motivation consistently finds that fun and competence related themes, such as skill development and challenge, are among the top reasons cited by youths for participating in sports (Weiss & Petlichkoff, 1989). Indeed, fun and competence motivation appear to be somewhat interdependent, as indicated by nonexperimental studies linking youths cognitive focus for demonstrating competence (i.e., achievement goals) with their intrinsic motivation (Cury, Elliot, 1 Dept. of Kinesiology and 2 Dept. of Human Development & Family Studies, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA

2 70 / Conroy, Kaye, and Coatsworth Sarrazin, Da Fonseca, & Rufo, 2002). Both fun and competence motivation for young athletes can be influenced by youths perceptions of social factors in their sport environment; however, relatively little is known about the dynamic interplay between social factors, competence motivation, and the reasons why youth engage in organized sports. The present research tested a short-term longitudinal model of social influences on situational motivation (i.e., youths reasons for participating in sport at a given time). Youth achievement goals are posited as links between their perceptions of the motivational climate created by their coaches (i.e., how they think the coach is evaluating their competence) and their situational motivation. This model frames important questions concerning associations between youths perceptions of the coaching climate, changes in youth achievement goals, and changes in situational motivation. Specifically, it addresses the following questions: Do youth perceptions of how their coaches are evaluating their competence influence changes in either (a) how youth evaluate their competence over the season, or (b) the reasons that youth give for participating in sport? Are changes in youths own achievement goals associated with changes in their reasons for participating in sport? Do those changes in youths achievement goals mediate any links between youth perceptions of the coaching climate and changes in their reasons for participating in sport? Situational Motivation in Sport Motivation in Youth Sport Motivation has been conceptualized at varying levels of specificity (e.g., global, contextual, situational), and the present research focuses on situational motivation, the most temporally and contextually specific level of motivation. Situational motivation represents the here-and-now of motivation and reflects the reasons an individual has for engaging in an activity (Vallerand, 2001). Three major forms of situational motivation have been described along a continuum of self-determination: intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and amotivation (Deci & Ryan, 1991; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Intrinsic motivation represents behaviors that are performed for rewards provided by the activity itself such as accomplishment, knowledge, and stimulation (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Vallerand et al., 1992). Extrinsic motivation describes behaviors performed to attain a separable outcome (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1991). Identified regulation and external regulation are two specific forms of extrinsic motivation. Identifi ed regulation characterizes extrinsically motivated behavior that is self-initiated, identified as valuable, and identified as important to the self despite not being intrinsically rewarding (Deci & Ryan, 1985). Similar to intrinsic motivation, it has been positively linked to concentration and enjoyment, albeit with slightly weaker effect sizes (Vallerand, 2001). External regulation reflects the classic definition of extrinsic motivation whereby individuals act to obtain external rewards or avoid external punishments (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1991). Amotivation describes relatively aimless behavior (Ryan & Deci, 2000). These four degrees of self-determined situational motivation encompass some of the most common reasons for participating in youth sports (e.g., having fun [intrinsic motivation]; it s good for me to be outside and active part of the day [identified regulation]; my parents are making me [external regulation]; I really don t know why I m doing this [amotivation]). These four forms of motivation also

3 Situational Motivation / 71 have been associated with unique profiles of affective, behavioral, and cognitive consequences (Vallerand, 2001; Vallerand & Rousseau, 2001). Self-determination theory proposes that social factors influence the selfdetermination of behavior and that those effects are mediated by the satisfaction of three basic psychological needs: autonomy, relatedness, competence (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1991). Given the prominence of competence related reasons for participating in youth sport (Weiss & Petlichkoff, 1989), we focused our attention on a competence related mechanism for social influences on the self-determination of situational motivation, namely achievement goals. Achievement Goal Theory Achievement goals refer to the dynamic cognitive focus of an individual s achievement behavior (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Nicholls, 1984). An individual s goals create a framework for how he or she interprets, evaluates, experiences, and acts in achievement settings (Dweck, 1986; Nicholls, 1984). Historically these goals have been distinguished by their definition of competence, but recent research has demonstrated the value of distinguishing goals both by the definition of competence and the valence of the outcome (for a review, see Elliot, 2005). Defi nitions of Competence. Competence may be defined in terms of absolute (e.g., performing a task as well as possible), intrapersonal (e.g., learning and improvement), and interpersonal (e.g., normative comparisons) criteria (Elliot & McGregor, 2001). Most research has combined absolute and intrapersonal definitions of competence into a single mastery (or task) goal definition. In the sport domain, mastery goal involvement has been linked positively to competence, intrinsic motivation, effort, enjoyment, interest, and positive affect (Duda, 2005). Interpersonal definitions of competence have been identified as performance (or ego) goals and linked to a number of undesirable outcomes in sport such as reduced enjoyment and interest, elevated state anxiety, or decreased intrinsic motivation (Duda, 2005). Goal Valence. To resolve somewhat equivocal findings about relations between performance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation, Elliot (Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996; Rawsthorne & Elliot, 1999) proposed distinguishing performance goals focused on outperforming others (i.e., performance-approach goals) from performance goals focused on not being outperformed by others (i.e., performance-avoidance goals). Experimental studies in the laboratory and nonexperimental studies in naturalistic settings have linked intrinsic motivation positively to performance-approach goals (when mastery-approach goals are simultaneously high) and negatively to performance-avoidance goals (Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996; Rawsthorne & Elliot, 1999). Broadening the universe of achievement goals in sport research to consider both definitions of competence and goal valences may be similarly beneficial (Elliot & Conroy, 2005). Thus, a 2 2 framework comprising mastery-approach, mastery-avoidance, performance-approach, and performanceavoidance goals can be conceived for describing achievement goals (Elliot, 1999; Pintrich, 2000). The 2 2 Achievement Goal Framework. Mastery-approach (MAp) goals represent strivings to improve on a previous performance or to perform a task as well as possible. In sport, MAp goals have been linked with high levels of task absorption and low levels of state anxiety (Cury et al., 2002). Mastery-avoidance (MAv) goals are the most recent addition to the achievement goal literature and

4 72 / Conroy, Kaye, and Coatsworth refer to strivings to avoid performing worse than one previously performed (i.e., losing ability). Although no empirical evidence is available concerning associations between MAv goals and intrinsic motivation, they are likely to be inversely related to intrinsic motivation because of their focus on avoiding an unpleasant possibility. Performance-approach (PAp) goals describe strivings to outperform others (i.e., normative demonstrations of competence). In sport, individuals adopting PAp and mastery goals have been shown to display the same level of intrinsic motivation (Cury et al., 2002) and equivalent amounts of practice times (Cury, Da Fonséca, Rufo, Peres, & Sarrazin 2003). Performance-avoidance (PAv) goals represent strivings to avoid appearing incompetent in relation to others. Findings in sport and physical education linked PAv goals with reduced intrinsic motivation (Cury et al., 2002) and reduced preparation (Cury et al., 2003). 1 Relations Between 2 2 Goals and Situational Motivation Extant findings suggest the desirability of mastery relative to performance definitions of competence, and approach relative to avoidance goal valences when intrinsic motivation is the outcome of interest. Accordingly, intrinsic motivation in the present study was expected to be positively associated with MAp goals, negatively associated with PAv and MAv goals, and unassociated with PAp goals. The simplex-like structure of situational motivation suggests that relations with 2 2 goals will be opposite for amotivation than they were for intrinsic motivation (i.e., positive associations with PAv and MAv goals, negative associations with MAp goals, and nonsignificant associations with PAp goals). Identified regulations are more similar to intrinsic motivation than to amotivation, so the pattern of associations for goals was expected to resemble associations between goals and intrinsic motivation (but possibly to be weaker because identified regulations are less self-determined than intrinsic motivation). Likewise, external regulations are more similar to amotivation than to intrinsic motivation, so the pattern of associations for goals was expected to resemble associations between goals and amotivation, albeit weaker. Beyond static links between goals and situational motivation, this research attempted to capture dynamic aspects of motivation in youth sport. We propose that such motivation may change as a function of (a) changes in youth achievement goals over the course of the season, or (b) properties of the social ecology that catalyze changes in youths achievement goals. Social-Ecological Factors Influencing Youth Achievement Goals. Coaches, parents, and peers may all contribute to youths achievement motivation in organized sports, but coaches are thought to play an especially important role in this process because of their role as proximal adult models for young athletes. Coaching climates can influence youth states of goal involvement (Solmon, 1996; Theebom, De Knop, & Weiss, 1995; Treasure & Roberts, 2001). Changes in achievement goals also may stimulate changes in more distal processes such as intrinsic motivation (Cury et al., 2002; Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996). For example, Theebom et al. (1995) found that mastery-oriented coaching climates were linked to youth having more fun (a construct similar to intrinsic situational motivation) during martial arts training. Thus, young people s perceptions of their coaches goals were expected to lead to corresponding changes in their own achievement goals (e.g., perceptions of coaches PAv goals predict changes in athletes PAv goals) and to changes in situational

5 Situational Motivation / 73 motivation. In turn, changes in youths achievement goals were expected to influence changes in youth situational motivation over the course of the season. Purpose This research addressed three objectives. First, the study evaluated the longitudinal factorial invariance of four types of situational motivation that span the continuum of self-determination: intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation with identified regulation, extrinsic motivation with external regulation, and amotivation. Second, the pattern of latent growth in each form of situational motivation over the course of a youth sport season was characterized. Finally, conditional latent growth curve models were tested to determine the extent to which initial levels of situational motivation were associated with youth 2 2 achievement goals at the beginning of the season, and whether subsequent changes in situational motivation were associated with (a) changes in youth 2 2 achievement goals over the season, or (b) youth perceptions of the 2 2 achievement goals that coaches have for them. With regard to predicting situational motivation, youth with more optimal achievement goals (i.e., mastery-defined or approach-valenced) at the beginning of the season were expected to have greater intrinsic motivation and identified regulation and less external regulation and amotivation than youth with less optimal achievement goals (i.e., performance-defined or avoidance-valenced). Youth who perceived that their coaches espoused more optimal achievement goals were expected to exhibit the greatest increases in intrinsic motivation and identified regulation, and the greatest decreases in external regulation and amotivation. This coaching climate effect was hypothesized to occur through a process whereby youth achievement goals would become more similar to their perceptions of their coaches goals over the course of the season. Youth whose achievement goals became more optimal over the season were expected to increase their levels of intrinsic motivation and identified regulation, and to decrease their levels of external regulation and amotivation over the season. Participants Methods Participants were recruited from a swim league sponsored by the department of parks and recreation (participation rate = 54%). This convenience sample included 66 boys and 99 girls (N = 165). The mean age of participants was 11.2 years (SD = 2.2; range = 7 18 years). Consistent with the demographic composition of the area, participants were almost exclusively white (79.4%; African American 3%; Asian American 1.8%; Hispanic 0.6%; other 7.3%; did not respond 7.9%). The majority of participants swam on a team only in this league (60%), and participants had been swimming on their team an average of 3.01 years (SD = 2.33; range = 0 13 years). Instruments The Situational Motivation Scale (SMS; Guay, Vallerand, & Blanchard, 2000) is a 16-item measure of one s self-determination of his or her choice to participate in an activity, but two items were dropped from the present study because of previous research indicating a lack of factorial invariance for those items (Standage,

6 74 / Conroy, Kaye, and Coatsworth Treasure, Duda, & Prusak, 2003). The resulting 14-item SMS provided separate scores for intrinsic motivation (4 items), identified regulation (3 items), external regulation (3 items), and amotivation (4 items). Participants rated different reasons for participating in an activity on a scale ranging from corresponds not at all (1) to corresponds exactly (7). The SMS has displayed satisfactory factorial validity, internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and external validity with both psychological and behavioral indices of motivation. Guay et al. (2000) additionally demonstrated that the SMS is sensitive enough to detect changes within individuals in motivation as a function of changes in autonomy, relatedness, and competence. The 12-item 2 2 Achievement Goals Questionnaire for Sport (AGQ-S; Conroy, Elliot, & Hofer, 2003) was used to measure youth achievement goals. The AGQ-S was developed by modifying items from the original Achievement Goals Questionnaire (Elliot & McGregor, 2001) to make them more applicable to sport and increase the salience of normative comparisons in the PAv goal items. The AGQ-S provided scores for MAp ( It is important to me to perform as well as I possibly can ), MAv ( I worry that I may not perform as well as I possibly can ), PAp ( It is important to me to do well compared to others ), and PAv ( My goal is to avoid performing worse than everyone else ) achievement goals in sport. Participants were asked to consider their present goals for swimming and to rate each item on a scale ranging from not at all true of me ( 3) to very true of me (+3). In an unpublished study from a separate sample of young swimmers, responses to the AGQ-S scales demonstrated acceptable internal consistency, factorial validity, strict longitudinal factorial invariance, and acceptable temporal stability for latent mean scores (Conroy, Coatsworth, & Cassidy, 2005). Youth perceptions of the goals that their coaches used to evaluate their competence (i.e., coaching climate) were assessed by modifying stems for items on the AGQ-S. The Perceptions of Coaches Achievement Goals Questionnaire for Sport (PCAGQ-S) provided scores for current perceptions of coaches MAp ( My coaches believe that it is important for me to master all aspects of my performance ), MAv ( My coaches worry that I may not perform as well as I possibly can ), PAp ( My coaches believe that it is important for me to perform as well as I possibly can ), and PAv ( My coaches just want me to avoid performing worse than others ) achievement goals in sport. Participants were asked to rate each item on a scale ranging from not at all true ( 3) to very true (+3). Procedures Permission to conduct this study was received from the town council s parks and recreation board, the director of the department of parks and recreation, the league supervisor, and the university institutional review board. Participants were recruited during the first week of practice. Letters and parental consent forms were sent home informing parents of the nature of the study and requesting permission for their child s participation. After receiving parental consent, research assistants described the study to the youth and requested their informed assent (for participants age 13 or younger) or informed consent (for participants age 14 or older). During the first week of the season, participants completed the SMS and AGQ-S. During Week 3 they completed the SMS. During Week 6 (the final week that culminated in the league championship meet), participants completed the SMS, AGQ-S, and the PCAGQ-S. Research assistants were available at all times to assist any who were having difficulty understanding or completing the questionnaires.

7 Data Analysis Situational Motivation / 75 Data from this study were analyzed with AMOS 4.01 software (Arbuckle & Wothke, 1999), and independence models were estimated separately to provide a basis for calculating the correct relative fit indices. Missing data appeared to be a combination of an arbitrary pattern and wave nonresponse. Logistic regression analyses indicated that the odds of missing data at each occasion were not significantly associated with participants age, sex, or initial achievement goal or situational motivation scores. Based on this evidence, we concluded that data was missing at random and tested our models using full-information maximum likelihood estimation to capitalize fully on the available data (Enders & Bandalos, 2001). Longitudinal Factorial Invariance (LFI) Analyses. A series of nested models with increasingly restrictive constraints on model parameters were tested and compared (Meredith, 1993; Meredith & Horn, 2001). The configural invariance model was used as a baseline for subsequent comparisons. This model imposed the same factor structure at each measurement occasion but did not impose equality constraints on any parameters in the model. A weak factorial invariance model was estimated next by constraining corresponding item-factor regression coefficients to be equal across waves of measurement (all other specifications were identical to the configural invariance model). A strong factorial invariance model was tested by adding constraints to corresponding item intercepts across measurement occasions (in addition to the specifications for the weak factorial invariance model). The final and most restrictive model was the strict factorial invariance model, which was estimated by imposing equality constraints on corresponding uniquenesses across measurement occasions (in addition to all of the constraints on the strong factorial invariance model). For identification purposes, the item-factor regression coefficient for the first item on each factor was fixed to 1.0 (each factor variance was freely estimated in these models). Additionally, corresponding manifest variable uniquenesses (i.e., uniquenesses from a single item at different occasions) were permitted to correlate across waves of measurement. Latent Growth Curve (LGC) Analyses. A series of LGC analyses were conducted using the best fitting and most constrained longitudinal factorial invariance models identified in the analyses described above. Second-order intercept and slope factors were then added to these models (Sayer & Cumsille, 2001). Regression coefficients between the second-order intercept factor and the first-order goal factors for each occasion of measurement were fixed at 1.0. To identify the second-order factor means in this model, the intercepts for manifest variables and first-order factors were fixed to zero. A progression of unconditional LGC models was then tested for each of the four motivational profiles to ascertain the nature of changes in the latent means (Meredith & Tisak, 1990). The first two models assumed no changes in situational motivation scores over time. The first no-growth model assumed that youth started the season with equivalent scores for each situational motivation scale (fixed intercepts) whereas the second no-growth model allowed for interindividual variability in initial scores for each situational motivation scale (random intercepts). Next, a pair of lineartrajectory growth models were specified by adding a second-order slope factor to the random coefficients no-growth model (Sayer & Cumsille, 2001). Parameters between the slope factor and the three first-order motivation factors were fixed at 0, 2, and 5 to indicate the rate of weekly change relative to starting values (the

8 76 / Conroy, Kaye, and Coatsworth availability of three measurement occasions limited us to testing linear growth trajectories). The two linear-trajectory growth models differed such that one assumed no interindividual variability in slopes (fixed slopes) whereas the other allowed for interindividual variability in slopes, thus the intercept represented each individual s latent mean at the start of the season and slope represented the subsequent weekly rate of change in motivation. Evaluating Model Fit. A combination of absolute (i.e., χ 2, RMSEA) and relative (i.e., NFI, NNFI, CFI) fit indices were used to evaluate model fit. Values above.90 for the latter indices typically indicate acceptable model fit whereas values above.95 indicate good model fit. RMSEA values of.08,.05, and.00 were interpreted as indicating acceptable, good, and exact fit. To evaluate the fit of nested invariance models, we examined changes in absolute and relative fit indices. A significant reduction in model fit (via chi-square difference tests or changes greater than.01 in relative fit indices) indicated that the null hypothesis of invariance should be rejected and that the less constrained model is more appropriate (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002). Results Descriptive statistics and internal consistency estimates for the SMS items and scales at each time point are listed in Table 1. Table 2 presents descriptive statistics and internal consistency estimates for the achievement goals and coaching climate scales. Each scale exhibited acceptable internal consistency. At the beginning of the season, older participants endorsed higher levels of PAp goals than younger participants (r r =.30, p <.05), and boys endorsed higher levels of PAv goals than girls (r r =.22, p <.05). Neither age nor sex was associated with any of the other situational motivation or achievement goal scores at the beginning of the study (p >.05), so subsequent analyses employed data from the entire sample. Three main questions derived directly from the study s three objectives were examined: (1) Did the measure of situational motivation tap the same construct over time (longitudinal factorial invariance)? (2) How should patterns of latent growth in situational motivation scores be modeled (unconditional latent growth)? (3) How do the coaching climate and youth achievement goals relate to levels of and influence changes in situational motivation (conditional latent growth)? 1. Did the Measure of Situational Motivation Tap the Same Construct Over Time? The first step in testing the proposed social-cognitive model of situational motivation dynamics involved evaluating the longitudinal factorial invariance of situational motivation responses over a 6-week interval. As seen in Table 3, responses to the intrinsic motivation scale exhibited strict factorial invariance, responses to the two extrinsic motivation scales exhibited weak factorial invariance, and responses to the amotivation scale exhibited configural factorial invariance. Further examination revealed that only two parameters in the amotivation model varied noticeably (i.e., Item 8 at Time 2 and Item 12 at Time 3). When those parameters were left unconstrained, the amotivation model achieved partial weak factorial invariance ( χ 2 [4] = 0.47, p <.05) and even partial strong factorial invariance ( χ 2 [8] = 17.10, p <.05).

9 Situational Motivation / 77 Table 1 Descriptive Statistics for Situational Motivation Items Across 3 Waves Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3 N M SD α N M SD α N M SD α Intrinsic Motivation Item Item Item Item Identified Regulation Item Item Item External Regulation Item Item Item Amotivation Item Item Item Item

10 78 / Conroy, Kaye, and Coatsworth Table 2 Descriptive Statistics for Achievement Goal and Coaching Climate Scores Scale N M SD α Achievement Goals Questionnaire for Sport (Week 1) MAp MAv PAp PAv Achievement Goals Questionnaire for Sport (Week 6) MAp MAv PAp PAv Perceptions of Coaches Achievement Goals Questionnaire for Sport MAp MAv PAp PAv Despite changes in the magnitude of two item-factor regression coefficients, the unconstrained parameter estimates were in the expected direction and the latent factor accounted for at least 42% of the variance in responses to each of those two items (compared to 35 79% for the other items). We concluded that the differences in these two model parameters should be modeled by releasing them from the weak invariance constraints, but they were unlikely to jeopardize the validity of our conclusions regarding changes in amotivation scores. To enhance our confidence in conclusions about changes in amotivation scores, we applied the strong invariance constraints to all other item-factor regression coefficients in the amotivation model. 2. How Should Changes in Situational Motivation be Modeled? The second set of analyses addressed the question of how changes in situational motivation should be modeled. These latent growth curve models employed the longitudinal invariance constraints identified in our earlier analyses. Table 4 depicts the fit comparisons for the no-growth and linear-growth models. Intrinsic Motivation. The random-intercept, no-growth model fit the data significantly better than the fixed-intercept, no-growth model for intrinsic motivation. There was significant interindividual variability in participants initial intrinsic motivation and, on average, there was no significant change in youths intrinsic motivation over the course of the season. The random coefficients model had a negative variance estimate for the slope factor so that the parameter was fixed to zero and the remaining models were not tested.

11 Situational Motivation / 79 Table 3 Fit Indices for Longitudinal Factorial Invariance Tests of Situational Motivation df χ 2 NFI NNFI CFI RMSEA (90% CI) Intrinsic Independence Configural (.08.13) Weak (.07.12) Strong (.07.11) Strict (.06.10) Identified Independence Configural (.00.09) Weak (.00.09) Strong (.01.09) Strict (.02.09) External Independence Configural (.00.08) Weak (.00.06) Strong (.00.09) Strict (.00.08) Amotivation Independence Configural (.04.09) Weak (.05.10) Strong (.05.10) Strict (.08.11) Model Comparisons df difference χ 2 difference Intrinsic Model Weak Configural Strong Weak Strict Strong Identified Model Weak Configural Strong Weak * Strict Strong External Model Weak Configural Strong Weak * Strict Strong Amotivation Model Weak Configural * Strong Weak Strict Strong *p <.05, one-tailed.

12 80 / Conroy, Kaye, and Coatsworth Table 4 Fit Indices and Parameter Estimates for No-Growth and Longitudinal Growth Curve Models of Situational Motivation RMSEA Intercept Slope df χ 2 NFI NNFI CFI (90% CI) M (var) M (var) Intrinsic Motivation (strict invariance constraints) No-Growth Models Fixed effects (.15.18) 5.61** (0) 0 (0) Random effects (.07.11) 5.61** (0.89**) 0 (0) Linear Trajectory Models Fixed slope (.73.11) 5.59** (0.89**) 0.01 (0) Identifi ed Regulation (weak invariance constraints) No-Growth Models Fixed effects (.15.19) 4.90** (0) 0 (0) Random effects (.00.09) 4.88** (0.83**) 0 (0) Linear Trajectory Models Fixed slope (.01.09) 4.90** (0.83**) 0.01(0) Random slope (.02.09) 4.90** (0.82**) 0.01 (0.01) External Regulation (weak invariance constraints) No-Growth Models Fixed effects (.17.22) 2.37** (0) 0 (0) Random effects (.01.09) 2.32** (1.59**) 0 (0) Linear Trajectory Models Fixed slope (.00.07) 2.21** (1.59**) 0.08** (0) Random slope (.00.07) 2.21** (1.57**) 0.08** (0.02)

13 Situational Motivation / 81 Amotivation (partial strong invariance constraints) No-Growth Models Fixed effects (.15.19) 1.98** (0) 0 (0) Random effects (.06.10) 2.01** (0.95**) 0 (0) Linear Trajectory Models Fixed slope (.05.10) 1.91** (1.08**) 0.05* (0) Random slope (.05.09) 1.89** (0.98**) 0.05* (0.02*) Model Comparisons df difference χ 2 difference Intrinsic No Growth (fixed) No Growth (random) No Growth (random) Linear Trajectory (fixed) * Identified No Growth (fixed) No Growth (random) No Growth (random) Linear Trajectory (fixed) * Linear Trajectory (fixed) Linear Trajectory (random) * External No Growth (fixed) No Growth (random) No Growth (random) Linear Trajectory (fixed) Linear Trajectory (fixed) Linear Trajectory (random) * Amotivation No Growth (fixed) No Growth (random) No Growth (random) Linear Trajectory (fixed) Linear Trajectory (fixed) Linear Trajectory (random) *p <.05, **p * <.01

14 82 / Conroy, Kaye, and Coatsworth Identifi ed Regulation. The random-intercept, no-growth model fit the data significantly better than the fixed-intercept, no-growth model for identified regulation. Fit did not improve significantly when a linear growth trajectory was modeled. Thus there was significant interindividual variability in participants initial scores and, on average, there was no significant change in identified regulation over the course of the season. Extrinsic Regulation. The random-intercept, no-growth model fit the data significantly better than the fixed-coefficients, no-growth model, suggesting that there was significant interindividual variability in participants initial external regulation. Linear growth models fit the data better than the no-growth models; there was, on average, a significant increase in youths extrinsic regulation over the course of the season. Amotivation. The random-intercept, no-growth model fit the data significantly better than the fixed-coefficients, no-growth model for amotivation as well. There was significant interindividual variability in participants initial amotivated regulation scores. A linear growth model fit better than the no-growth models, with the random linear effect model fitting the best. There were, on average, slight but significant increases in youths amotivation over the course of the season, and significant variability characterized the rate at which youths amotivation scores changed. 3. How Did the Coaching Climate and Youth Achievement Goals Predict Initial Status or Subsequent Changes in Situational Motivation? The final set of analyses employed conditional LGC modeling to test the substantive model posited in the introduction. Separate models were estimated for each form of situational motivation. As reported earlier, intrinsic motivation and identified regulation scores neither changed over the course of the season (on average) nor exhibited interindividual variability in the rate of change. Accordingly, those models tested relationhips between achievement goals in Week 1 and those two forms of situational motivation at the same time and did not include any predictors of the second-order slope term. For external regulation and amotivation, both intercepts and slopes were predicted. Each analysis was conducted in two steps so that parameter estimates from the two models could be compared. The first step tested the direct effects of (a) youths achievement goals in Week 1 on external regulation or amotivation intercepts, and (b) the perceived coaching climate on external regulation or amotivation slopes. In the second step, residualized change in youth achievement goals were added as potential mediators or pathways for indirect effects of the perceived coaching climate on youth external regulation and amotivation. These residualized change scores were operationally defined as the disturbances resulting when a Week 1 goal score predicted a corresponding Week 6 goal score. Prior to testing this substantive model, age and gender were tested as potential predictors of situational motivation intercepts, situational motivation slopes, achievement goals in Week 1, and residualized change in achievement goals over the season. Neither age nor gender significantly predicted intercepts or slopes for any of the situational motivation scores (p >.01). Additionally, no age or gender differences were evident in either initial levels of or residualized changes in MAp or MAv goals (p >.01). Older youth started the season with higher PAp goals (β =

15 Situational Motivation / 83.22, p <.01), but age was not associated with residualized change in PAp goals (p >.01). There were no gender differences in either initial PAp goals or the residualized change in those goals (p >.01). There were no age differences in initial levels of or residualized change in PAv goals. Boys started the season with slightly higher levels of PAv goals (β =.80, p <.01) and increased those goals faster than girls (β =.77, p <.01). Intrinsic Motivation. Intrinsic motivation intercepts were positively associated with MAp goals (β = 0.58, p <.01), negatively associated with MAv (β = 0.17, p <.05) and PAv (β = 0.16, p <.05) goals, and unassociated with PAp goals (p >.05; R 2 =.39). Identified Regulation. Identified regulation intercepts were positively associated with Week 1 MAp goals (β = 0.50, p <.01), but not associated with Week 1 MAv, PAp, or PAv goals (p >.05; R 2 =.27). External Regulation. External regulation intercepts were positively associated with Week 1 MAv, PAp, and PAv goals, and negatively associated with Week 1 MAp goals (R 2 =.29). When the perceived coaching climate was modeled as the only predictor of external regulation slopes, youth perceptions of their coaches goals for them were unrelated to their external regulation slopes (p >.05). Figure 1 presents parameter estimates for the model after youth achievement goals were added as potential predictors of external regulation slopes and as potential pathways for indirect effects of the coaching climate. In this model, perceptions of an avoidance-oriented coaching climate positively predicted residualized changes in youth avoidance-oriented achievement goals (R 2 MAv =.36; R 2 PAv =.45), but perceptions of an approach-oriented coaching climate did not predict residualized changes in youth approach-oriented achievement goals (i.e., MAp, PAp; p >.05). 2 Changes in youth MAv goals were positively associated with external regulation slopes; however, changes in youth MAp, PAp, and PAv goals were not associated with external regulation slopes. In the full model with residualized changes in achievement goals, perceptions of coaches MAp, PAp, and PAv goals were unassociated with external regulation slopes, but perceptions of coaches MAv goals were negatively associated with external regulation slopes (β = 0.39, p <.05). Thus, perceptions of a MAv coaching climate had an inverse relation with variance in external regulation slopes after controlling for changes in youth achievement goals (R 2 =.39). Amotivation. Amotivation intercepts were positively associated with Week 1 PAp goals, negatively associated with Week 1 MAp goals, and not associated with Week 1 MAv and PAv goals (R 2 =.22). When the coaching climate was modeled as the only predictor of amotivation slopes, youth perceptions of their coaches MAp goals (β = 0.42, p <.01) predicted amotivation slopes, but youth perceptions of their coaches MAv, PAp, and PAv goals did not predict amotivation slopes (p >.05; R 2 =.25). Figure 2 presents parameter estimates for the model after youth achievement goals were added as potential mediators or pathways for indirect effects of the perceived coaching climate on amotivation slopes. Changes in youth MAv goals were positively associated with amotivation slopes; however, changes in youth MAp, PAp, and PAv goals were not associated with amotivation slopes. With youth achievement goals (and residualized changes therein) as a part of this model, the standardized path between youth perceptions of coaches MAp goals and amotivation slopes was reduced to 0.31 (p <.01); none of the remaining

16 84 / Conroy, Kaye, and Coatsworth Figure 1 Structural model of perceived coach achievement goals predicting residualized change in youth achievement goals, and the rate at which extrinsic motivation (external regulation) scores changed during a youth sport season. Unstandardized parameters between Week 6 achievement goal scores and corresponding residualized change scores were fixed at 1.0 to estimate the variance in the disturbance term representing residualized changes in youth achievement goals (values in the figure are the standardized coefficients). Solid and dashed lines represent relations that were and were not statistically significant, respectively. *p <.05, **p* <.01

17 Situational Motivation / 85 Figure 2 Structural model of perceived coach achievement goals predicting residualized change in youth achievement goals, and the rate at which amotivation scores changed during a youth sport season. Unstandardized parameters between Week 6 achievement goal scores and corresponding residualized change scores were fixed at 1.0 to estimate the variance in the disturbance term representing residualized changes in youth achievement goals (values in the figure are the standardized coefficients). Solid and dashed lines represent relations that were and were not statistically significant, respectively. *p <.05, **p* <.01

18 86 / Conroy, Kaye, and Coatsworth coaching climate variables were significantly associated with amotivation slopes. Thus, perceptions of MAp coaching climates and increases in youth MAv goals were positively linked to amotivation slopes (R 2 =.37). Discussion This study tested a model of social-ecological influence on young swimmers situational motivation, and achievement motivation theory was posited as a potential explanation for observed changes in situational motivation. Major findings from this investigation revolved around three research questions concerning measurement, the nature of observed change, and covariates of observed change. The first set of findings emerged from LFI analyses and helped to rule out changes in model parameters (e.g., item-factor regression coefficients) as plausible explanations for differences in latent factor scores. Most analyses of change data do not test LFI but rather assume equivalent factor structures and item-factor regression coefficients over time (i.e., weak invariance). To the extent that this assumption is incorrect, the statistical conclusion validity of findings will be threatened. In the present study, intrinsic motivation scores exhibited the highest level of LFI (i.e., strict invariance), the two extrinsic motivation scores exhibited moderate levels of LFI (i.e., weak invariance), and amotivation scores exhibited a slightly lower but nevertheless acceptable level of LFI (i.e., partial weak invariance). These results strengthened our confidence that differences in latent situational motivation scores were meaningful and not merely statistical artifacts attributable to large differences in model parameters. Previous research has found that the 14-item Situational Motivation Scale yields data with invariant item-factor regression coefficients, internal consistency, and the proposed simplex pattern of interrelationships between groups of male youth soccer players, middle school PE students, and participants in college physical activity classes (Standage et al., 2003). The present findings were the first to investigate the LFI of responses to the Situational Motivation Scale. In combination with the internal consistency estimates reported for each scale, these results suggest a high level of validity for situational motivation scores in the present study. The second major set of findings from this study concerned the nature of changes in situational motivation scores over the course of the season. Across individuals, uniform stability in intrinsic motivation and identified regulation over the course of the season was evident. In contrast, both external regulation and amotivation scores increased on average over the course of the season, with the biggest increases apparent in external regulation. These findings indicated a modest deterioration in self-determined motivation over the 6 weeks of data collection. This finding contrasted with the apparent stability of situational motivation scores during a 10-day physical education unit with middle school girls (Prusak, Treasure, Darst, & Pangrazi, 2004). 3 Previous research has not documented how situational motivation changes for youth sport participants, so these findings provide valuable insight into how young athletes situational motivation can deteriorate over the course of a youth sport season of moderate length. Further research is needed to examine whether these patterns of change are similar or different (e.g., curvilinear) during longer sport seasons. Achievement goal theory provides one potential explanation for the degradation in self-determined situational motivation during the youth sport season. Previous

19 Situational Motivation / 87 research linking achievement goals with situational motivation has focused exclusively on definitions of competence (e.g., mastery vs. performance) and implicitly delimited attention to approach-valenced goals (e.g., Parish & Treasure, 2003; Standage & Treasure, 2002). Consistent with previous findings, MAp goals at the beginning of the season were positively associated with the more self-determined forms of situational motivation (i.e., intrinsic motivation, identified regulation), and PAp goals at the beginning of the season were positively (albeit more weakly) associated with the less self-determined forms of motivation (i.e., external regulation, amotivation). These findings mirrored previous reports with middle school physical education students (Parish & Treasure, 2003; Standage & Treasure, 2002). These findings departed somewhat from previous studies that distinguished approach from avoidance goals because PAp goals were not associated with intrinsic motivation in the present study whereas PAp goals have been positively associated with intrinsic motivation in previous research (Cury et al., 2002). 4 This finding may reflect developmental differences between our sample of children and adolescents and previous samples of college students. With age, normative sources of information about competence become more salient (Horn, 2004), so it is possible that relations between PAp goals and intrinsic motivation are attenuated in younger children and increase with age. Previous reports in the physical domain (e.g., sport, physical education) have not examined relations between avoidance achievement goals and the different forms of situational motivation per se. In this study, both avoidance goals (i.e., MAv, PAv) exhibited negative relations with intrinsic motivation and positive relations with external regulation, and neither avoidance goal was associated with identified regulation or amotivation. It appears that focusing on undesirable possibilities (i.e., incompetence) reduces the self-determination of situational motivation. Whether that occurred because avoidance goal involvement inhibited competence-need satisfaction requires further research. Overall, these results clearly indicated that goal valence was at least as critical as definitions of competence in determining situational motivation. Achievement goals are commonly employed in the prospective prediction of motivational outcomes such as intrinsic motivation. The present study was unique because it examined how changes in these dynamic foci (i.e., goals) over a youth sport season might be linked to changes in situational motivation during the same period. Changes in MAv goals were positively associated with external regulation and amotivation slopes. These findings extend previous research reporting negative relations between avoidance goals and an individual s level of self-determination (Church, Elliot, & Gable, 2001; Cury et al., 2002; McGregor & Elliot, 2002). It was somewhat surprising that changes in PAv goals over the course of the season did not predict changes in external regulation and amotivation during that same period. Given that boys had greater residualized change scores for PAv goals than girls did, sex may moderate the dynamic relations between changes in PAv goals and changes in these forms of situational motivation. We did not plan on testing that hypothesis and did not recruit a large enough sample to conduct the necessary multigroup analyses. Future research may benefit from recruiting large groups of both sexes to test this moderation hypothesis. Changes in approach goals were not associated with external regulation or amotivation. Although the difference was not statistically significant, residualized change scores for MAp and PAp goals exhibited slightly less variance (0.74 and

Antecedents and consequences of approach and avoidance achievement goals: A test of gender invariance

Antecedents and consequences of approach and avoidance achievement goals: A test of gender invariance Psychology of Sport and Exercise 9 (2008) 352 372 www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport Antecedents and consequences of approach and avoidance achievement goals: A test of gender invariance Chiao-Lin Nien

More information

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science Ghali Hassan Faculty of Education, Queensland University of Technology, Australia Abstract The purpose of this exploratory study was

More information

The Roles of Competence Beliefs and Goal Orientations for Change in Intrinsic Motivation

The Roles of Competence Beliefs and Goal Orientations for Change in Intrinsic Motivation Journal of Educational Psychology 2012 American Psychological Association 2012, Vol. 104, No. 4, 1135 1148 0022-0663/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0028115 The Roles of Competence Beliefs and Goal Orientations

More information

A Modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to Include an Assessment of Amotivation

A Modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to Include an Assessment of Amotivation JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2004, 26, 191-196 2004 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. A Modification to the Behavioural Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire to Include an Assessment of Amotivation

More information

Motivation in physical education across the primary-secondary school transition

Motivation in physical education across the primary-secondary school transition Loughborough University Institutional Repository Motivation in physical education across the primary-secondary school transition This item was submitted to Loughborough University's Institutional Repository

More information

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model 50 Educational Measurement and Evaluation Review (2012), Vol. 3, 50-59 2012 Philippine Educational Measurement and Evaluation Association Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model Adonis

More information

Running head: PREDICTING PARTICIPATION IN OPTIONAL PHYSICAL. A Prospective Study of Participation in Optional School Physical Education Using a

Running head: PREDICTING PARTICIPATION IN OPTIONAL PHYSICAL. A Prospective Study of Participation in Optional School Physical Education Using a Running head: PREDICTING PARTICIPATION IN OPTIONAL PHYSICAL EDUCATION A Prospective Study of Participation in Optional School Physical Education Using a Self-Determination Theory Framework Address for

More information

Psychology of Sport and Exercise

Psychology of Sport and Exercise Psychology of Sport and Exercise 13 (01) 491e499 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Psychology of Sport and Exercise journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport Goal orientations,

More information

ACHIEVEMENT GOALS AND MOTIVATIONAL CLIMATE IN COMPETITIVE GYMNASTICS CLASSES

ACHIEVEMENT GOALS AND MOTIVATIONAL CLIMATE IN COMPETITIVE GYMNASTICS CLASSES ACHIEVEMENT GOALS AND MOTIVATIONAL CLIMATE IN COMPETITIVE GYMNASTICS CLASSES Trevor Dowdell Queensland, Australia Abstract Original research article This paper is a preliminary investigation into the motivational

More information

Achievement goal profiles and sportspersonship behaviour of high school cricketers. Laura Singh University of Cape Town

Achievement goal profiles and sportspersonship behaviour of high school cricketers. Laura Singh University of Cape Town 1 Achievement goal profiles and sportspersonship behaviour of high school cricketers. Laura Singh University of Cape Town Supervisor: Prof. Johann Louw Word Count: Abstract: 166 Main body: 9900 2 Abstract

More information

SPORT PSYCHOLOGY JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 25, Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.

SPORT PSYCHOLOGY JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 25, Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. SPORT PSYCHOLOGY JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 25, 19-43 2003 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. The Situational Motivation Scale / 19 Validity, Reliability, and Invariance of the Situational

More information

THE EFFECTS OF WIN:LOSS [W:L] ON INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATION AND PERFORMANCE

THE EFFECTS OF WIN:LOSS [W:L] ON INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATION AND PERFORMANCE THE EFFECTS OF WIN:LOSS [W:L] ON INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATION AND PERFORMANCE Presented By: Stephanie P. Bailey SUNY Old Westbury Faculty Mentor: Lorenz S. Neuwirth, Ph.D. Discussion Points Background Overview

More information

Competitive orientations and motives of adult sport and exercise participants

Competitive orientations and motives of adult sport and exercise participants Competitive orientations and motives of adult sport and exercise participants By: Diane L. Gill, Lavon Williams, Deborah A. Dowd, Christina M. Beaudoin, and Jeffrey J. Martin Gill, D.L., Williams, L.,

More information

Sports Science & Coaching Volume 4 Number

Sports Science & Coaching Volume 4 Number An Intra-Individual Analysis of Players Perceived Coaching Behaviours, Psychological Needs, and Achievement Goals by C.K. John Wang, Koon Teck Koh and Nikos Chatzisarantis Reprinted from International

More information

Assessing Measurement Invariance in the Attitude to Marriage Scale across East Asian Societies. Xiaowen Zhu. Xi an Jiaotong University.

Assessing Measurement Invariance in the Attitude to Marriage Scale across East Asian Societies. Xiaowen Zhu. Xi an Jiaotong University. Running head: ASSESS MEASUREMENT INVARIANCE Assessing Measurement Invariance in the Attitude to Marriage Scale across East Asian Societies Xiaowen Zhu Xi an Jiaotong University Yanjie Bian Xi an Jiaotong

More information

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto Session VI, September 20 2017 Learning objectives 1. Get familiar with the basic idea

More information

Publications David E. Conroy, Ph.D.

Publications David E. Conroy, Ph.D. Publications David E. Conroy, Ph.D. References in appear in chronological order. Peer-reviewed publications are denoted by an asterisk. Underlined author names are or were students. In Press Conroy, D.

More information

Follow this and additional works at: Part of the Exercise Science Commons

Follow this and additional works at:   Part of the Exercise Science Commons The College at Brockport: State University of New York Digital Commons @Brockport Senior Honors Theses Master's Theses and Honors Projects 12-8-2016 Who Matters? The Perceived Motivational Climates Created

More information

The Effect of Autonomy-Supportive Behaviors of Coaches on Need Satisfaction and Sport Commitment of Elite Female Players in Handball Premier League

The Effect of Autonomy-Supportive Behaviors of Coaches on Need Satisfaction and Sport Commitment of Elite Female Players in Handball Premier League The Effect of Autonomy-Supportive Behaviors of Coaches on Need Satisfaction and Sport Commitment of Elite Female Players in Handball Premier League Sedigheh Hosseinpoor Delavar Member, Islamic Azad University,

More information

Relations between sports motivation in physical education classes and dropping out of organized kinesiological engagement in adolescence

Relations between sports motivation in physical education classes and dropping out of organized kinesiological engagement in adolescence 1 Relations between sports motivation in physical education classes and dropping out of organized kinesiological engagement in adolescence Original scientific paper Zoran Grgantov,, Mirjana Milić*, Boris

More information

Volitional Autonomy and Relatedness: Mediators explaining Non Tenure Track Faculty Job. Satisfaction

Volitional Autonomy and Relatedness: Mediators explaining Non Tenure Track Faculty Job. Satisfaction Autonomy and : Mediators explaining Non Tenure Track Faculty Job Satisfaction Non-tenure track (NTT) faculty are increasingly utilized in higher education and shoulder much of the teaching load within

More information

Why Do You Study? Complex Answers to a Simple Question. Mimi Bong. Department of Education Brain & Motivation Research Institute Korea University

Why Do You Study? Complex Answers to a Simple Question. Mimi Bong. Department of Education Brain & Motivation Research Institute Korea University Why Do You Study? Complex Answers to a Simple Question Mimi Bong Department of Education Brain & Motivation Research Institute Korea University 1 interest self-efficacy volition Self-defeating ego goals

More information

Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2

Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2 Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2 Gherdai Hassel a, Jeffrey John Milroy a, and Muhsin Michael Orsini a Adolescents who engage in regular physical

More information

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES Correlational Research Correlational Designs Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are

More information

Title: Autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the optimal functioning of adult male and female sport participants: A test of basic needs theory

Title: Autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the optimal functioning of adult male and female sport participants: A test of basic needs theory Editorial Manager(tm) for Motivation and Emotion Manuscript Draft Manuscript Number: MOEM1R Title: Autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the optimal functioning of adult male and female sport participants:

More information

Achievement Goal Orientation as a Predictor of Sport Involvement and Perceived Benefits of Sport Involvement: Examination of a Mixture Model

Achievement Goal Orientation as a Predictor of Sport Involvement and Perceived Benefits of Sport Involvement: Examination of a Mixture Model The Journal of SPORT Volume 3 Issue 1 Article 4 2014 Achievement Goal Orientation as a Predictor of Sport Involvement and Perceived Benefits of Sport Involvement: Examination of a Mixture Model Leeann

More information

Why Try? Achievement Motivation and Perceived Academic Climate among Latino Youth

Why Try? Achievement Motivation and Perceived Academic Climate among Latino Youth Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Psychology Theses Department of Psychology 4-20-2007 Why Try? Achievement Motivation and Perceived Academic Climate among Latino Youth Natalie

More information

Self Determination Theory, COACHE, and Faculty Outcomes in Higher Education. Lisa M. Larson Mack C. Shelley Sandra W. Gahn Matthew Seipel

Self Determination Theory, COACHE, and Faculty Outcomes in Higher Education. Lisa M. Larson Mack C. Shelley Sandra W. Gahn Matthew Seipel Self Determination Theory, COACHE, and Faculty Outcomes in Higher Education Lisa M. Larson Mack C. Shelley Sandra W. Gahn Matthew Seipel Overview ISU COACHE survey through the lens of Self- Determination

More information

The Reasons And Motivation For Pre-Service Teachers Choosing To Specialise In Primary Physical Education Teacher Education

The Reasons And Motivation For Pre-Service Teachers Choosing To Specialise In Primary Physical Education Teacher Education Australian Journal of Teacher Education Volume 39 Issue 5 Article 1 2014 The Reasons And Motivation For Pre-Service Teachers Choosing To Specialise In Primary Physical Education Teacher Education Sharna

More information

Predictors of Avoidance of Help-Seeking: Social Achievement Goal Orientation, Perceived Social Competence and Autonomy

Predictors of Avoidance of Help-Seeking: Social Achievement Goal Orientation, Perceived Social Competence and Autonomy World Applied Sciences Journal 17 (5): 637-642, 2012 ISSN 1818-4952 IDOSI Publications, 2012 Predictors of Avoidance of Help-Seeking: Social Achievement Goal Orientation, Perceived Social Competence and

More information

Running head: ACADEMIC REGRETS, MOTIVATION, AND EMOTIONS. A mixed method investigation of academic regrets, motivation, and emotions

Running head: ACADEMIC REGRETS, MOTIVATION, AND EMOTIONS. A mixed method investigation of academic regrets, motivation, and emotions Running head: ACADEMIC REGRETS, MOTIVATION, AND EMOTIONS A mixed method investigation of academic regrets, motivation, and emotions Problem Regrets are a negative emotion initiated by a counterfactual

More information

A Multilevel Approach to Motivational Climate in Physical Education and Sport Settings: An Individual or a Group Level Construct?

A Multilevel Approach to Motivational Climate in Physical Education and Sport Settings: An Individual or a Group Level Construct? 90 JOURNAL / Papaioannou, OF SPORT & Marsh, EXERCISE and PSYCHOLOGY, Theodorakis2004, 26, 90-118 2004 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. A Multilevel Approach to Motivational Climate in Physical Education

More information

Sources of Competence Information and Perceived Motivational Climate Among Adolescent Female Gymnasts Varying in Skill Level

Sources of Competence Information and Perceived Motivational Climate Among Adolescent Female Gymnasts Varying in Skill Level 396 JOURNAL / Halliburton OF SPORT & and EXERCISE Weiss PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 24, 396-419 2002 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. Sources of Competence Information and Perceived Motivational Climate Among Adolescent

More information

Human Motivation and Emotion

Human Motivation and Emotion Human Motivation and Emotion 46-332-01 Dr. Fuschia Sirois Lecture 7 Sept. 28, 2006 Lecture 8 Oct. 3, 2006 Types of Motivation INTRINSIC strive inwardly to be competent and self-determining in their quest

More information

Psychology of Sport and Exercise

Psychology of Sport and Exercise Psychology of Sport and Exercise 13 (2012) 51e59 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Psychology of Sport and Exercise journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport Perceived coach-autonomy

More information

Running Head: ROLE OF BOUNDARY GOALS 1. Setting Lower Limits High: The Role of Boundary Goals in Achievement Motivation

Running Head: ROLE OF BOUNDARY GOALS 1. Setting Lower Limits High: The Role of Boundary Goals in Achievement Motivation Running Head: ROLE OF BOUNDARY GOALS 1 Setting Lower Limits High: The Role of Boundary Goals in Achievement Motivation Katherine S. Corker M. Brent Donnellan Michigan State University Accepted for publication

More information

Original Article. Relationship between sport participation behavior and the two types of sport commitment of Japanese student athletes

Original Article. Relationship between sport participation behavior and the two types of sport commitment of Japanese student athletes Journal of Physical Education and Sport (JPES), 17(4), Art 267, pp. 2412-2416, 2017 online ISSN: 2247-806X; p-issn: 2247 8051; ISSN - L = 2247-8051 JPES Original Article Relationship between sport participation

More information

Path Analysis of a Self-Determination Model of Work Motivation in Vocational Rehabilitation

Path Analysis of a Self-Determination Model of Work Motivation in Vocational Rehabilitation Path Analysis of a Self-Determination Model of Work Motivation in Vocational Rehabilitation Timothy N. Tansey Jill Bezyak Kanako Iwanaga Cayte Anderson Nicole Ditchman This presentation is being offered

More information

COACHES INFLUENCE ON MALE ADOLESCENTS ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION, PSYCHOLOGICAL FACTORS, AND SPORT PARTICIPATION. Dustin M. Johnson, B.A., M.S.

COACHES INFLUENCE ON MALE ADOLESCENTS ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION, PSYCHOLOGICAL FACTORS, AND SPORT PARTICIPATION. Dustin M. Johnson, B.A., M.S. COACHES INFLUENCE ON MALE ADOLESCENTS ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION, PSYCHOLOGICAL FACTORS, AND SPORT PARTICIPATION Dustin M. Johnson, B.A., M.S. Dissertation Prepared for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY

More information

How to Achieve in Elite Training Centers without Burning Out? An Achievement Goal Theory Perspective

How to Achieve in Elite Training Centers without Burning Out? An Achievement Goal Theory Perspective How to Achieve in Elite Training Centers without Burning Out? An Achievement Goal Theory Perspective Sandrine Isoard-Gautheur, Emma Guillet-Descas, Joan Duda To cite this version: Sandrine Isoard-Gautheur,

More information

that fosters self-determined motivation among athletes. Coaching style influences athlete

that fosters self-determined motivation among athletes. Coaching style influences athlete BURRELL, CHELSEA J., M.S. The Relationship of Perceived Autonomy-Supportive Coaching Behavior with Motivation Among High School Athletes. (2016) Directed by Dr. Diane Gill. 94 pp. Recent literature suggests

More information

MOTIVATION OF FEMALE SECONDARY STUDENTS TOWARDS PARTICIPATION IN PHYSICAL ACTIVITIES IN HONG KONG

MOTIVATION OF FEMALE SECONDARY STUDENTS TOWARDS PARTICIPATION IN PHYSICAL ACTIVITIES IN HONG KONG physical education / Tjelesna i zdravstvena kultura Eric C.K. Tsang Department of Health and Physical Education The Hong Kong Institute of Education, Hong Kong Original scientific paper MOTIVATION OF FEMALE

More information

--Manuscript Draft-- achievement goals & academic motivations

--Manuscript Draft-- achievement goals & academic motivations Social Psychology of Education: An International Journal The pivotal role of effort beliefs in mediating implicit theories of intelligence and achievement goals & academic motivations --Manuscript Draft--

More information

A Meta-Analysis of Perceived Locus of Causality in Exercise, Sport, and Physical Education Contexts

A Meta-Analysis of Perceived Locus of Causality in Exercise, Sport, and Physical Education Contexts 284 JOURNAL / Chatzisarantis, OF SPORT & EXERCISE Hagger, PSYCHOLOGY, Biddle, Smith, 2003, and 25, Wang 284-306 2003 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. A Meta-Analysis of Perceived Locus of Causality in Exercise,

More information

Examination of the 3x2 Achievement Goal Model in Collegiate Recreation: Comparison Across Sport Programs

Examination of the 3x2 Achievement Goal Model in Collegiate Recreation: Comparison Across Sport Programs Examination of the 3x2 Achievement Goal Model in Collegiate Recreation: Comparison Across Sport Programs Leeann M. Lower 1 Brian A. Turner 2 1 Ball State University 2 The Ohio State University Sport is

More information

ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION IN TRAINING AND COMPETITION: DOES THE CONTEXT MATTER? PEPIJN KLAAS CHRISTIAAN VAN DE POL. A thesis submitted to

ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION IN TRAINING AND COMPETITION: DOES THE CONTEXT MATTER? PEPIJN KLAAS CHRISTIAAN VAN DE POL. A thesis submitted to ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION IN TRAINING AND COMPETITION: DOES THE CONTEXT MATTER? By PEPIJN KLAAS CHRISTIAAN VAN DE POL A thesis submitted to The University of Birmingham for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

More information

Where a licence is displayed above, please note the terms and conditions of the licence govern your use of this document.

Where a licence is displayed above, please note the terms and conditions of the licence govern your use of this document. How to achieve in elite training centers without burning out? An achievement goal theory perspective Isoard-Gautheur, Sandrine; Guillet-Descas, Emma; Duda, Joan DOI: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.08.001 Document

More information

Ian M. Taylor, 1 Nikos Ntoumanis, 2 Martyn Standage, 3 and Christopher M. Spray 1

Ian M. Taylor, 1 Nikos Ntoumanis, 2 Martyn Standage, 3 and Christopher M. Spray 1 Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 010, 3, 99-10 010 Human Kinetics, Inc. Motivational Predictors of Physical Education Students Effort, Exercise Intentions, and Leisure-Time Physical Activity: A

More information

Testing a self-determination theory-based teaching style intervention in the exercise domain

Testing a self-determination theory-based teaching style intervention in the exercise domain European Journal of Social Psychology Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 375 388 (2008) Published online 18 September 2007 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).463 Testing a self-determination theory-based

More information

PETTLEP IMAGERY AND VIDEO-OBSERVATION: A MOTIVATION CASE STUDY OF FOUR BADMINTON PLAYERS PETTLEP and intrinsic motivated badminton performance

PETTLEP IMAGERY AND VIDEO-OBSERVATION: A MOTIVATION CASE STUDY OF FOUR BADMINTON PLAYERS PETTLEP and intrinsic motivated badminton performance 239 PETTLEP IMAGERY AND VIDEO-OBSERVATION: A MOTIVATION CASE STUDY OF FOUR BADMINTON PLAYERS PETTLEP and intrinsic motivated badminton performance FERNANDO RAMIREZ RAMIREZ, DAVE SMITH, PAUL HOLMES Manchester

More information

The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign

The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign Reed Larson 2 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign February 28,

More information

The Psychometric Properties of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 in Internet Gaming

The Psychometric Properties of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 in Internet Gaming Curr Psychol (2009) 28:194 201 DOI 10.1007/s12144-009-9058-x The Psychometric Properties of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 in Internet Gaming C. K. John Wang & W. C. Liu & A. Khoo Published online: 27 May

More information

Isabel Castillo, Inés Tomás, and Isabel Balaguer University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain

Isabel Castillo, Inés Tomás, and Isabel Balaguer University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain International Journal of Testing, : 21 32, 20 0 Copyright C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1530-5058 print / 1532-7574 online DOI: 10.1080/15305050903352107 The Task and Ego Orientation in Sport Questionnaire:

More information

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale Simon B. Sherry, 1 Paul L. Hewitt, 1 * Avi Besser, 2 Brandy J. McGee, 1 and Gordon L. Flett 3

More information

Using a sample of student-athletes (N=64) first year (seventh grade)

Using a sample of student-athletes (N=64) first year (seventh grade) Sport Science Review, vol. Sport XXV, Science No. Review, 3-4, September vol. XXV, no. 2016 3-4, 2016, 225-242 DOI: 10.1515/ssr-2016-0012 Relationships between ego-oriented peer climate, perceived competence

More information

Maarten Vansteenkiste, 1 Athanasios Mouratidis, 2 and Willy Lens 2. University of Gent; 2 University of Leuven

Maarten Vansteenkiste, 1 Athanasios Mouratidis, 2 and Willy Lens 2. University of Gent; 2 University of Leuven Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 2010, 32, 217-242 2010 Human Kinetics, Inc. Detaching Reasons From Aims: Fair Play and Well-Being in Soccer as a Function of Pursuing Performance- Approach Goals

More information

The Dynamic Processes of Influence Between Contextual and Situational Motivation: A Test of the Hierarchical Model in a Science Education Setting 1

The Dynamic Processes of Influence Between Contextual and Situational Motivation: A Test of the Hierarchical Model in a Science Education Setting 1 The Dynamic Processes of Influence Between Contextual and Situational Motivation: A Test of the Hierarchical Model in a Science Education Setting 1 Geneviève L. Lavigne and Robert J. Vallerand 2 Laboratoire

More information

Mastery, autonomy and transformational approaches to coaching: Common features and applications

Mastery, autonomy and transformational approaches to coaching: Common features and applications University of Wollongong Research Online Faculty of Social Sciences - Papers Faculty of Social Sciences 0 Mastery, autonomy and transformational approaches to coaching: Common features and applications

More information

Available from Deakin Research Online:

Available from Deakin Research Online: This is the published version: Richardson, Ben and Fuller Tyszkiewicz, Matthew 2014, The application of non linear multilevel models to experience sampling data, European health psychologist, vol. 16,

More information

Creating an Optimal Climate for Athlete Achievement & Enjoyment

Creating an Optimal Climate for Athlete Achievement & Enjoyment Creating an Optimal Climate for Athlete Achievement & Enjoyment Dr James Matthews, C. Psychol., Ps.S.I UCD School of Public Health, Physiotherapy and Sports Science IOC consensus statement on youth athletic

More information

Running head: Hierarchical Model of Achievement Motivation

Running head: Hierarchical Model of Achievement Motivation Page 1 Running head: Hierarchical Model of Achievement Motivation Fear of Failure, 2 x 2 Achievement Goal and Self-handicapping: An Examination of the Hierarchical Model of Achievement Motivation in Physical

More information

Between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Examination of reasons for academic study based on the theory of internalization

Between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Examination of reasons for academic study based on the theory of internalization Japanese Psychological Research 1997, Volume 39, No. 2, 98 108 Between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Examination of reasons for academic study based on the theory of internalization TOSHIHIKO HAYAMIZU

More information

A methodological perspective on the analysis of clinical and personality questionnaires Smits, Iris Anna Marije

A methodological perspective on the analysis of clinical and personality questionnaires Smits, Iris Anna Marije University of Groningen A methodological perspective on the analysis of clinical and personality questionnaires Smits, Iris Anna Mare IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version

More information

Achievement Goals, Social Goals, and Students Reported Persistence and Effort in High School Physical Education

Achievement Goals, Social Goals, and Students Reported Persistence and Effort in High School Physical Education Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 2006, 25, 58-74 2006 Human Kinetics, Inc. Achievement Goals, Social Goals, and Students Reported Persistence and Effort in High School Physical Education Jianmin

More information

Jumpstart Mplus 5. Data that are skewed, incomplete or categorical. Arielle Bonneville-Roussy Dr Gabriela Roman

Jumpstart Mplus 5. Data that are skewed, incomplete or categorical. Arielle Bonneville-Roussy Dr Gabriela Roman Jumpstart Mplus 5. Data that are skewed, incomplete or categorical Arielle Bonneville-Roussy Dr Gabriela Roman Questions How do I deal with missing values? How do I deal with non normal data? How do I

More information

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 294 303 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology Capturing age-group differences and developmental change

More information

Development of a New Fear of Hypoglycemia Scale: Preliminary Results

Development of a New Fear of Hypoglycemia Scale: Preliminary Results Development of a New Fear of Hypoglycemia Scale: Preliminary Results Jodi L. Kamps, 1 PHD, Michael C. Roberts, 2 PHD, ABPP, and R. Enrique Varela, 3 PHD 1 Children s Hospital of New Orleans, 2 University

More information

How to Move On and Keep Moving : Preparing Student-Athletes for Life after College Sports

How to Move On and Keep Moving : Preparing Student-Athletes for Life after College Sports How to Move On and Keep Moving : Preparing Student-Athletes for Life after College Sports Erin Reifsteck, Ph.D. Dr. Erin Reifsteck is a post-doctoral research fellow with IPAHW. She received her Ph.D.

More information

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155 24 Motivation Internalized Motivation in the Classroom Kennon M. Sheldon The motivation that students bring to a classroom setting is critical in determining how much, and how well, they learn. This activity

More information

Articles. Development of the Free Time Motivation Scale for Adolescents

Articles. Development of the Free Time Motivation Scale for Adolescents Journal of Leisure Research Copyright 2003 2003, Vol. 35, No. 2, pp. 129-151 National Recreation and Park Association Articles Development of the Free Time Motivation Scale for Adolescents Cheryl K. Baldwin

More information

Best Practices for Coaching the Ego-Oriented Athlete

Best Practices for Coaching the Ego-Oriented Athlete Best Practices for Coaching the Ego-Oriented Athlete Charlotte R. Stith, M.S. Health and Human Performance Oklahoma State University Timothy Baghurst, Ph.D. Health and Human Performance Oklahoma State

More information

Goal Orientations and Performance: Differential Relationships Across Levels of Analysis and as a Function of Task Demands

Goal Orientations and Performance: Differential Relationships Across Levels of Analysis and as a Function of Task Demands Journal of Applied Psychology 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 94, No. 3, 710 726 0021-9010/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015044 Goal Orientations and Performance: Differential Relationships

More information

Trait motivational correlates of athleticism

Trait motivational correlates of athleticism Personality and Individual Differences 30 (2001) 1139±1145 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Trait motivational correlates of athleticism Steven Reiss *, James Wiltz, Michael Sherman Ohio State University,

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Self-Determination of Contextual Motivation, Inter-Context Dynamics and Adolescents Patterns of Sport Participation over Time

Self-Determination of Contextual Motivation, Inter-Context Dynamics and Adolescents Patterns of Sport Participation over Time Self-Determination of Contextual Motivation, Inter-Context Dynamics and Adolescents Patterns of Sport Participation over Time Julie Boiché, Philippe Sarrazin To cite this version: Julie Boiché, Philippe

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 4, Number 1 Submitted: August 20, 2008 Revisions: October 16, 2008 Accepted: October 17, 2008 Publication Date: October 20, 2008 Start Today or the Very

More information

This file was dowloaded from the institutional repository Brage NIH - brage.bibsys.no/nih

This file was dowloaded from the institutional repository Brage NIH - brage.bibsys.no/nih This file was dowloaded from the institutional repository Brage NIH - brage.bibsys.no/nih Buch, R., Nerstad, C. G. L., Säfvenbom, R. (2015). The interactive roles of mastery climate and performance climate

More information

This is the peer reviewed version of the following article: Jaakkola, T. and Ntoumanis, N. and Liukkonen, J Motivational climate, goal

This is the peer reviewed version of the following article: Jaakkola, T. and Ntoumanis, N. and Liukkonen, J Motivational climate, goal This is the peer reviewed version of the following article: Jaakkola, T. and Ntoumanis, N. and Liukkonen, J. 2015. Motivational climate, goal orientation, perceived sport ability, and enjoyment within

More information

From self-theories of intelligence to academic delay of gratification: The mediating role of achievement goals 1

From self-theories of intelligence to academic delay of gratification: The mediating role of achievement goals 1 ACADEMIC DELAY OF GRATIFICATION - ABD-EL-FATTAH & AL-NABHANI 93 Australian Journal of Educational & Developmental Psychology. Vol 12, 2012, pp. 93-107 From self-theories of intelligence to academic delay

More information

Effects of an intervention based on self-determination theory on self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation

Effects of an intervention based on self-determination theory on self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation Psychology and Health Vol. 24, No. 1, January 2009, 29 48 Effects of an intervention based on self-determination theory on self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation Nikos L.D. Chatzisarantis

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2013 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 3 Psychological

More information

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are highly extraverted people less afraid of rejection

More information

From Environmental Factors to Outcomes: A Test of an Integrated Motivational Sequence 1

From Environmental Factors to Outcomes: A Test of an Integrated Motivational Sequence 1 Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 28, No. 4, December 2004 ( C 2004) DOI: 10.1007/s11031-004-2387-z From Environmental Factors to Outcomes: A Test of an Integrated Motivational Sequence 1 Frederick M. E. Grouzet,

More information

T. Rene Jamison * and Jessica Oeth Schuttler

T. Rene Jamison * and Jessica Oeth Schuttler Jamison and Schuttler Molecular Autism (2015) 6:53 DOI 10.1186/s13229-015-0044-x RESEARCH Open Access Examining social competence, self-perception, quality of life, and internalizing and externalizing

More information

The effects of autonomous and controlled regulation of performance-approach goals on well-being: A process model

The effects of autonomous and controlled regulation of performance-approach goals on well-being: A process model 154 British Journal of Social Psychology (2014), 53, 154 174 2012 The British Psychological Society www.wileyonlinelibrary.com The effects of autonomous and controlled regulation of performance-approach

More information

Motivating children in and out of school: Research findings and practical implications

Motivating children in and out of school: Research findings and practical implications Motivating children in and out of school: Research findings and practical implications The Question Does promoting motivation toward activities in school lead to increased motivation toward activities

More information

Self-Determination in Physical Education: Designing Class Environments to Promote Active Lifestyles

Self-Determination in Physical Education: Designing Class Environments to Promote Active Lifestyles Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 2007, 26, 260-278 2007 Human Kinetics, Inc. Self-Determination in Physical Education: Designing Class Environments to Promote Active Lifestyles Charity L. Bryan

More information

Self-determination Theory and the psychology of exercise

Self-determination Theory and the psychology of exercise International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology Vol. 1, No. 1, March 2008, 79103 Self-determination Theory and the psychology of exercise Martin Hagger a * and Nikos Chatzisarantis b a School of

More information

Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs

Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2014 Image source 1 Reeve (2009, pp. 142-143) Psychological need An inherent source

More information

Application of Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling to Evaluate the Academic Motivation Scale

Application of Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling to Evaluate the Academic Motivation Scale The Journal of Experimental Education ISSN: 0022-0973 (Print) 1940-0683 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjxe20 Application of Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling to Evaluate

More information

Manipulating Achievement Motivation in Physical Education By Manipulating the Motivational Climate

Manipulating Achievement Motivation in Physical Education By Manipulating the Motivational Climate European Journal of Sport Science, vol. 2, issue 1 2002 by Human Kinetics Publishers and the European College of Sport Science Manipulating Achievement Motivation in Physical Education By Manipulating

More information

COMMUNITY-LEVEL EFFECTS OF INDIVIDUAL AND PEER RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS ON ADOLESCENT SUBSTANCE USE

COMMUNITY-LEVEL EFFECTS OF INDIVIDUAL AND PEER RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS ON ADOLESCENT SUBSTANCE USE A R T I C L E COMMUNITY-LEVEL EFFECTS OF INDIVIDUAL AND PEER RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS ON ADOLESCENT SUBSTANCE USE Kathryn Monahan and Elizabeth A. Egan University of Washington M. Lee Van Horn University

More information

The Why and How of Goal Pursuits: Effects of Global Autonomous Motivation and. Perceived Control on Emotional Well-being. E.

The Why and How of Goal Pursuits: Effects of Global Autonomous Motivation and. Perceived Control on Emotional Well-being. E. The Why and How of Goal Pursuits: Effects of Global Autonomous Motivation and Perceived Control on Emotional Well-being E. Gaëlle Hortop A Thesis in the Department of Psychology Presented in Partial Fulfillment

More information

2x2 Achievement Goals in Learning and Coping among High School Students. Yvonne Toh Monash University

2x2 Achievement Goals in Learning and Coping among High School Students. Yvonne Toh Monash University 2x2 Achievement Goals in Learning and Coping among High School Students Yvonne Toh Monash University yvonne.toh@education.monash.edu.au This study examines the 2 x 2 achievement goal frame-work (Elliot

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2014 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 when people find

More information

The MHSIP: A Tale of Three Centers

The MHSIP: A Tale of Three Centers The MHSIP: A Tale of Three Centers P. Antonio Olmos-Gallo, Ph.D. Kathryn DeRoche, M.A. Mental Health Center of Denver Richard Swanson, Ph.D., J.D. Aurora Research Institute John Mahalik, Ph.D., M.P.A.

More information

Effect of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation on Academic Performance

Effect of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation on Academic Performance Effect of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation on Academic Performance Nadia Ayub Education & Social Sciences Institute of Business Management nadia.ayub@iobm.edu.pk Abstract The aim of the present study

More information

Achievement Goals as Predictors of Achievement-Relevant Processes Prior to Task Engagement

Achievement Goals as Predictors of Achievement-Relevant Processes Prior to Task Engagement Journal of Educational Psychology Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2002, Vol. 94, No. 2, 381 395 0022-0663/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-0663.94.2.381 Achievement Goals as Predictors

More information

Perception of Coaching Behaviors, Coping, and Achievement in a Sport Competition

Perception of Coaching Behaviors, Coping, and Achievement in a Sport Competition Research Note Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 2011, 33, 460-468 2011 Human Kinetics, Inc. Perception of Coaching Behaviors, Coping, and Achievement in a Sport Competition Michel Nicolas, 1 Patrick

More information

Perfectionism and Achievement Goals in Athletes: Relations with Approach and Avoidance Orientations in Mastery and Performance Goals

Perfectionism and Achievement Goals in Athletes: Relations with Approach and Avoidance Orientations in Mastery and Performance Goals Stoeber, J., Stoll, O., Pescheck, E., & Otto, K. (2008). Perfectionism and achievement goals in athletes: Relations with approach and avoidance orientations in mastery and performance goals. Psychology

More information