THE PERCEPTION AND MEMORY OF STEREOSCOPIC DEPTH INFORMATION IN NATURALISTIC OBJECTS

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "THE PERCEPTION AND MEMORY OF STEREOSCOPIC DEPTH INFORMATION IN NATURALISTIC OBJECTS"

Transcription

1 THE PERCEPTION AND MEMORY OF STEREOSCOPIC DEPTH INFORMATION IN NATURALISTIC OBJECTS Technical Report June 20, 1996 Thomas A. Busey, Ph.D. Indiana University Please send correspondence to: Thomas A. Busey Department of Psychology Indiana University Bloomington, IN

2 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 1 Abstract In two experiments with 162 participants, we examined the role of stereoscopic depth cues in the memory for objects and scenes. Stereoscopic depth cues are formed by disparities in the locations of objects in the images formed on the two eyes, and are naturally used by most observers to recover the distances to objects in the world. Using naturalistic pictures, we determined that stereo depth cues do persist in memory, as long as the observer is given several seconds to view the image and extract the depth relations from the scene. However, we did not find direct evidence that stereoscopic depth cues increase the rate of information extraction from scenes. One implication of the present work is that objects that will be recognized in the real world should be initially presented using stereoscopic depth cues.

3 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 2 The world around us has three dimensions, although we often represent scenes using flat, 2D representations such as photographs or movies. However, by virtue of the geometry of space and the separation of our two eyes by several inches, we perceive two slightly different views of the real world. These different views introduce subtle changes in the locations of objects. Most observers can use these differences, or disparities, to recover the distances of objects in depth. The use of disparities as depth cues is known as stereoscopic depth, and these enable us to perceive objects in three dimensions. However, these stereoscopic depth cues by no means the only cue to depth. Texture, shading, relative size, position, accommodation and occlusion are all monocular depth cues, and in many cases, such as viewing a photograph, these monocular cues are sufficient to reconstruct the relative depth positions of objects in space. Thus while stereoscopic depth cues are available to observes, they are redundant in many respects, leaving open the question of whether observers even use stereoscopic depth cues during normal perception. The purpose of the present work is to examine how observers use stereoscopic depth cues, and how they influence memory for objects presented in 3D. The perception of stereo depth cues has been thoroughly addressed by a number of researchers, most notably Bela Julesz (Julesz, 1971), who demonstrated that observers can use stereoscopic cues for perception, both in the presence and absence of monocular depth cues. Despite this groundbreaking work, more recent investigations have yet to derive a satisfactory theory of stereopis (Shipley, 1987). Moreover, the utility of stereo depth cues has recently been challenged by Grimson (1993), who argued that stereoscopic depth cues are not used primarily as cues to depth relations, but are instead used for figure-ground segmentation. In this view, a first step towards object recognition is separating surfaces that belong to different objects. This figure-ground segmentation problem is solved quickly and accuracy by stereo depth cues, where different surfaces are assigned different depths in space and therefore are likely to belong to different objects. Grimson argues that use of stereoscopic disparities to recover accurate depth requires precise calibration of the viewing apparatus and such precision may be beyond the capacities of the human visual system. Figure-ground segmentation by stereo depth cues is indeed a very fast operation, although we can do figure-ground segmentation on scenes without disparities, such as photographs. To summarize, the role of stereo depth cues in perception is still an open question. Observers can use stereo depth cues to perceive objects, but whether they actually rely on these cues during normal perception is less certain. While the use of stereoscopic depth cues for perceiving objects has been widely studied, the representation of these depth cues in memory has received little attention, and

4 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 3 the question of whether stereoptic depth cues are preserved in memory is still unclear. Memory for pictures is by no means a distortion-free process, and considerable recoding of the visual information may take place. Images that are preserved in memory are subject to characteristic biases such as contraction to include objects and area outside the picture frame (Intraub, Bender & Mangels, 1992). In addition to these biases, a re-coding of the visual information to more abstract verbal information may take place, in which depth relations are lost. For example, an observer might remember that a scene contained a boy, a dog and a tree, but not the relative locations of each object in depth. Research on the use of stereoscopic depth cues in object recognition gives some clues to the representation of objects in memory, although very few experiments have addressed the long-term maintenance of these cues in memory. The debate centers around whether objects are represented in memory as a linked series of viewpoint-specific two-dimensional images (Tarr and Pinker, 1989) or as viewpoint-invariant three-dimensional representations (e.g. the geon-based GSD theory of Biederman and colleagues: Biederman and Gerhardstein, 1993; Hummel and Biederman, 1992). The former view is consistent with the storage of a relatively large number of raw images, from which little abstraction takes place. The later view holds that observers create three-dimensional mental representations that extract depth relations to create mental models that are more isomorphic to the physical object than to the perceptual image that generated the initial percept of the object. This new representation allows mental operations such as rotation and even structural changes that are analagous to the physical manipulations on real objects. Various models exist along the continuum defined by the above models. However, for the purposes of the current article the important issues are how objects are maintained in memory, and whether these representations include stereoscopic depth cues. Most experiments do not use stereoscopic depth cues, and thus any three-dimensional memory representation must be derived from monocular depth cues or inferred from multiple views of the same novel object. However, Edelman and Bulthoff (1992) examined the addition of stereoscopic depth cues to novel objects. Observers were trained on a series of novel objects under conditions of full stereoscopic depth cues. They were then tested with identical and similar objects in different orientations, and indicated whether each object was one they had previously seen. The error rate decreased when stereo cues were added during the test session by a factor of two (Edelman and Bulthoff, 1992), suggesting that depth cues were useful during object recognition. However, their overall results are at odds with theory that propose three-dimensional, viewpoint-invariant memory representations. While it does appear that observers construct abstractions based on different views of the same object,

5 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 4 such abstractions appear to be collections of specific views of an object, assisted by limited depth information that provide a partial viewer-centered three-dimensional representation (Bulthoff, Edelman and Tarr; 1994). In summary, stereoscopic depth cues appear to assist the segregation of objects from each other, and provide relative depth information that is used to create abstractions or collections of views based on individual perceptual images. Under this view, object recognition is seen as a process of interpolation from the nearest image-based representation in memory to match the currently-viewed object. This leaves the role of stereoscopic depth cues in memory in a somewhat uncertain state: while they are clearly used for perception and can assist object recognition, whether they persist in memory is still an open question, one addressed by the current article. Two Models Based on the object recognition and binocular vision literature, we can construct two (non-exclusive) models of how stereoscopic depth cues might enhance perception and memory. Before describing these models, a brief description of the experimental paradigm is in order. We will then describe the models and derive predictions for each. The current experiments use three-dimensional photographs taken with a dual-camera stereo rig. These photographs are scanned and displayed on 21 color monitors with polarizing filter stereoscopic display capabilities. The experiments typically have two parts: a study session, in which 60 pictures are shown sequentially, and then a test session in which the 60 old pictures are intermixed with 60 new pictures. The participants in the experiment go through each test picture and indicate whether the picture was in the original list (old) or is a new picture (new). We add to this the stereo manipulation: a study picture could either be presented with stereoscopic depth cues (STEREO) or without these cues (FLAT). The flat pictures are generated by presenting either the left or right picture to both eyes. This stereo/flat manipulation is duplicated for the test pictures as well, giving four possible conditions as indicated by Table 1. Study Depth This design systematically varies the Test Depth Flat Stereo cues that are used to probe memory, and Study Flat Study Stereo Flat provide data that test the following two Test Flat Test Flat Study Flat Study Stereo models of the effects of stereoscopic depth Stereo Test Stereo Test Stereo cues. Table 1. Four conditions for Experiments 1 and 2. The Speed-Up Model Following the views of Julesz, Grimson and others who have argued that stereoscopic depth cues can be and are used in perception, one might predict that these additional cues

6 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 5 serve to make stereo pictures more memorable or more durable in memory. Under the Speed-Up Model, stereoscopic depth cues assist the initial perception of 3D scenes, either through figure-ground segregation or increasing the rate of information extraction. Thus even if stereoscopic depth cues do not persist in memory, they do make 3D pictures more memorable by providing more information in memory. The predictions of the Speed-Up Model are straightforward. Items that are studied in stereo are recognized more often than items that are studied flat, regardless of how these are tested. Thus this model predicts an overall main effect of studying a picture in stereo verses studying the picture flat. One issue that is relevant to the Speed-Up model is that of novelty: stereo pictures for our participants are novel, and therefore more interesting than the flat pictures. Thus participants may pay more attention to stereo pictures, which would give results consistent with the Speed-Up model. We tried to address this issue in three ways. First, we gave participants lots of experience with stereo pictures at the beginning of the experiment. We had them view a very well-composed scenic picture that had a great deal of depth, and we explained how stereo worked while they viewed this picture. We then gave a practice minisession in which they had an opportunity to see several other stereo pictures. Second, we told our subjects that half of the pictures will be in stereo and half will be flat, and we told them that they would have to recognize both kinds later on. While neither of these may have been sufficient to completely eliminate novelty effects, one could argue that the mechanism behind an increase in information processing is simply increased attention on the part of the participants. This would require an acknowledgment (perhaps long overdue) that participants bring expectations, interests and emotions to experiments, and that these can affect their data. The Depth Coding Model Under the Depth Coding model, stereoscopic depth cues are stored in memory, either as an intrinsic part of a three-dimensional representation (such as Biederman's Geon theory) or extrinsically as part of a list of features (e.g. the picture contained a boy, a dog and a tree, and it was in depth ). Here we assume that the stereoscopic depth cues which contribute to perception are relevant to the object and the task, and are thus stored in memory. The predictions of the Depth Coding model follow the Encoding Specificity Hypothesis of Tulving & Thomson (1973), which states that recognition memory will be best when the study and test conditions match. Thus recognition performance will be best in the study flat/test flat and study stereo/test stereo condition and poor in the study stereo/test

7 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 6 flat and study flat/test stereo conditions. The Depth Coding model would be supported by finding a crossover interaction between study and test conditions. Note that the Speed-Up and Depth Coding models are not exclusive: it is possible, even reasonable, to assume that stereo pictures at study would provide more information about an object, but also remain in memory and be probed best by a test stimulus that also contain stereo depth cues. In this case we would expect an overall main effect of study depth combined with a crossover interaction between study and test depth. Experiments 1 and 2 are designed to confirm or disconfirm each of these two models independently. Experiment 1 uses pictures of cars, trees and hallways presented for 5 seconds at study, while Experiment 2 uses the same stimuli but presented each picture for 600 milliseconds (ms) at study. To anticipate, in Experiment 1 we find evidence for the Depth Coding Model, but not for the Speed-Up Hypothesis. In Experiment 2 we find an overall main effect of test condition, suggesting that the development of a robust threedimensional memory representation may take several seconds. Experiment 1 Experiment 1 addresses two questions: do stereoscopic depth cues assist in the perception of naturalistic scenes, and do they remain in memory to make pictures viewed in stereo more memorable? Note that neither answer may be inferred a priori simply by the mere fact that the stimuli contain stereoscopic 3d cues: participants without the ability to perceive stereoscopic depth cues still perceive the pictures and still can do quite well in our experiments. Thus the real question is whether participants can and will use stereoscopic cues as aids to memory, or are they relying on more abstract representations devoid of the original depth cues? Methods Experiment 1 adopts a standard picture-memory paradigm using stereo and flat pictures of cars, trees and hallways. Participants viewed the scenes on two 21 color monitors that present 3D images using LCD circularly-polarizing panels. Stimuli and Apparatus The stereo and flat pictures were presented using two 21 color monitors equipped with Tektronix LCD stereoscopic panels and controlled by a Pentium computer. Participants wore passive polarizing glasses that allowed only either the left or right image through. The left and right images of a stereo pair were alternatively shown on the monitor at 45 Hz per image (giving a stereo rate of 90 Hz) and the panels were synchronized with these images such that each eye saw only one image, and when fused these two pictures give a vivid, flicker-free impression of depth.

8 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 7 The pictures were taken with two 35 mm cameras with 50 mm lenses. The separation between the lenses was 12.7 cm, with is slightly larger than the typical separation between most observer s eyes. This resulted from physical limitations with the cameras, and to compensate for this we followed standard formulas within the stereo photography literature and made certain that the closest object was at least 15 feet from the camera. This prevents unresolvable disparities outside Panum s fusional limit (Julesz, 1971) and minimizes the slight depth exaggeration caused by the increased stereo base. These pictures were then scanned via a slide scanner and cropped to a screen resolution of 800x600 pixels shown in thousands of colors mode (16-bits per pixel). Up to 6 people could participate at once, and were arranged in two rows of desks facing two 21 color monitors. Participants responded on numeric keypads connected to a Macintosh computer that could determine which keypad corresponded to which participant. This computer also controlled the stimulus display computer via a serial cable. Design and Procedure The experiment consisted of three study-test sessions, in which participants viewed 20 pictures of one class of scenes (cars, trees or hallways) and then viewed 40 pictures, half of which were new and half were from the original list. Test pictures from the original list are Targets, while new pictures in the test session are Distracters. Participants indicated for each picture in the test session whether they thought the picture was in the original list (old) or a new picture (new). At study the pictures were shown either in stereo or flat, and the pictures were shown for 5 seconds each, with about 4 seconds in between pictures. Participants were told to expect a test session after each series of pictures, and were given a short practice session at the beginning of the experiment with 3 practice study pictures and 6 practice test pictures. Study and Test pictures could either be shown flat or stereo, giving the four conditions shown in Table 1. During the study session, each picture was preceded by a warning tone. During the test session, the picture remained on the screen until all participants had responded. Participants Participants were 80 Indiana University undergraduate students. Prior to the experiment each participant was given a stereo acuity test (Stereo Reindeer, Randot Corporation) and coded according to their ability to perceive various stereo acuity. Six participants complete the experiment but were excluded from the data analysis due to a lack of stereo depth perception. Results and Discussion

9 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 8 Results for Experiment 1 are shown in Figure 1. Performance is computed as a function of hit rates corrected for the false alarm rate. Individual false alarm rates were computed for each participant, and used to correct that individual's hit rates. Because there were two depth conditions in the test session, the distracters were also shown either in stereo or flat depths. This gives two separate false alarm rates, and these were used to correct the hit rates that corresponded to how that condition was tested (e.g. both the study flat/test stereo and study stereo/test stereo conditions were corrected with the stereo false alarm rate. The flat false alarm rate is (SEM = 0.011) and the stereo false alarm rate is (SEM = 0.016), which were not statistically significantly different. Overall performance was quite high in this experiment, due in part to the relatively long 5 second exposure durations during the study session. A two-way ANOVA reveals no significant main effect of study depth (F(1,60)= 0.28, p>0.05), but a significant interaction Recognition Accuracy (Hits-FAs)/(1-FAs) Stereo Experiment 1 Study Flat Study Stereo 0.50 Test Flat Test Stereo Test Condition Figure 1. Data from Experiment 1. The data show a crossover interaction that is consistent with the Depth Coding model, demonstrating that stereo depth cues do persist in memory and are used when recognizing a previously-seen image. There is no main effect of study condition, which fails to support the Speed-Up hypothesis. Apparently stereo depth cues do not provide an encoding benefit, at least for 5 second exposure durations. Error bars represent one standard error of the mean.

10 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 9 (F(1,60) = 5.4, p<0.05). Inspection of the Figure 1 data reveals that this interaction is a crossover interaction: memory for pictures is best when a picture is studied and tested at the same depth. Memory is poorest for conditions in which the study and test depth differ. The crossover interaction evident in the Experiment 1 data confirm the Depth Coding model, and demonstrate that stereoscopic depth cues are preserved in memory and assist in recognition memory. However, recognition for pictures that were studied in stereo was overall no better than recognition for pictures that were studied flat. This argues against the Speed-Up model, suggesting that stereoscopic depth cues do not increase the rate of information acquisition for picture-related information during study. Experiment 2 The Experiment 1 data support the Depth Coding model, although this effect is modest (around 10% benefit for pictures tested in the same depth as they were studied). In addition, the data do not support the Speed-Up model, which is surprising given Grimson s arguments for stereoscopic cues as aids to figure-ground segregation. However, it is likely that such speed-up effects would assist perception only during the initial stages of encoding a scene. Perhaps the long 5 second exposure duration minimized the encoding advantages of the stereo depth cues. In addition, the high recognition rates seen in the Experiment 1 data may have reduced the size of the crossover interaction through ceiling effects. In Experiment 2 we attempted to address both limitations of Experiment 1 by greatly reducing the exposure duration a study, from 5 seconds to 600 ms. If stereo depth cues give an advantage during encoding, we might see this advantage with such short exposure durations. We were concerned that reducing the exposure duration would eliminate the stereo percept entirely, and so we had participants give confidence ratings during the study session that indicated where they thought each picture was in stereo or not. Methods Experiment 2 is similar to Experiment 1, with the exceptions of the use of confidence ratings taken at study to determine if participants could perceive stereoscopic depth cues, and the much shorter, 600 ms exposure durations at study. Procedure During the study sessions, a trial was preceded by a warning tone, and then the target picture appeared in either flat or stereo depth conditions for 600 ms. Following stimulus offset, the participants were asked to give a confidence rating on a 5-point scale indicating how confident they were that the pictures were in depth (1 = very little depth, 3= some depth, 5 = a lot of depth ). Once all participants had responded the computer initiated the next trial.

11 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 10 The test session was identical to Experiment 1. Participants Participants were 82 Indiana University undergraduate students. Prior to the experiment they were administered the same stereo acuity test as in Experiment 1. Seven participants failed this acuity test and their data was excluded from the analysis as a result. Results and Discussion Participants had no trouble distinguishing between flat and stereo study pictures during the study session. The mean confidence rating for flat pictures was 2.23, and the mean confidence rating for stereo pictures was This difference is statistically significant (t(30) = 19.0, p<0.001). Of primary interest is whether this ability to perceive stereo translates into either an overall main effect of study pictures in stereo (vs. flat) or the encoding specificity effects seen in Experiment 1. The answer is no on both counts. Figure 2 shows the Experiment 2 data. Surprisingly, the data show a main effect of test depth: Recognition accuracy was highest when pictures were tested flat, regardless of how they were studied. A two way ANOVA reveals no main effect of study depth, and no significant interaction. The main effect of test depth was significant (F(1, 60) = 9.23, p < 0.01). This main effect, however, is attributable to the stereo false alarm rate: contrary to Experiment 1, the flat and stereo false alarm rates differed significantly (t (30) = 2.59, p< 0.01). In Experiment 2, flat false alarm rate was (SEM = 0.015) and the stereo false alarm rate was (SEM = 0.022). This difference accounts for the main effect of Test condition in Figure 1. When the raw hit rates for the four conditions are compared, they do not differ significantly, indicating that the decrease in performance with both stereo test conditions comes from a decrease in overall sensitivity when stereo pictures are used as cues, and also a bias shift to preserve the same overall hit rates.

12 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 11 Clearly the test pictures containing stereo depth cues are reducing memory performance. However, there may be evidence for encoding specificity that would explain the significant main effect of test depth. In Experiment 2, the study pictures were on for only 600 ms, while at test the pictures remained visible until all participants had responded. Thus even in conditions where the study and test depths matched, the durations of the stimuli were different, and this difference may have resulted in an encoding specificity difference that produced the Experiment 2 data. While it is possible that 600 ms provides enough information to determine whether a picture is in depth or not, it may not provide enough information to extract out the rich depth relations provided by these depth cues. At test, participants were encouraged to make accurate responses, and they had ample time (up to 10 seconds) to decide whether they had seen a picture before or not. Thus not only could Stereo Experiment Recognition Accuracy (Hits-FAs)/(1-FAs) Study Flat Study Stereo 0.00 Test Flat Test Stereo Test Condition Figure 2. Data from Experiment 2. The data show a main effect of test condition, which is attributable to a loss in sensitivity and a shift in bias to preserve the same overall hit rate for slides tested stereo. This results in a higher false alarm rate for stereo distracters as well, which when used to correct targets tested in stereo results in reduced performance compared with testing with flat slides.

13 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 12 they perceive the stereoscopic depth cues, they also had ample opportunity to use these depth cues to extract out the rich depth relations of the objects in the scene. The 600 ms presentations at study did not allow for such extraction, and thus the representation engendered by the stereo test pictures were poor cues to memory. The lack of an interaction between study and test depth fails to support the Depth Encoding model that was confirmed by Experiment 1, but this failure may result from duration differences described above. The lack of a main effect of study depth fails to support the Speed-Up model, which is evident for both pictures tested flat and tested stereo. General Discussion To summarize the results from the two experiments presented here, we find evidence for the Depth Coding model for long (5 second) exposure durations but not for short (600 ms) presentations. This demonstrates that stereo depth cues persist in memory and can be used in later picture recognition tasks. We do not find evidence for the Speed-Up model at any exposure duration, despite the fact that the stereo percept develops, at least partially, for pictures presented at very short exposure durations (600 ms). To reconcile these findings, we suggest that when pictures with stereoscopic depth cues are viewed, two sets of processes occur. The extraction of stereo depth cues is a very fast process (Grimson, 1993) that enables observers to identify the existence of stereoscopic disparities in pictures presented for as brief as 600 ms (Experiment 2). However, such cues appear not to increase the overall information extraction rate, but instead may simply change what information is acquired. These depth cues, while clearly visible to participants, may only assist in the memory for pictures that receive a great deal of encoding (Experiment 1). This lengthy encoding period (5 seconds) may give participants an opportunity to develop an object-oriented representation that preserves depth relations in memory. Rather than remembering simply what objects were in the scene, the participant now stores the locations of these objects in depth and develops a rich mental model that is analagous to the physical object. Implications for Applied Technologies The findings reported here demonstrate that stereo depth cues are perceived, remain in memory, and influence future recognition of the same object. A useful metaphor would be that of a lock and key: the memory trace produced by perceiving an object is only accessible if the appropriate key is used to probe the trace. In this case when objects with stereo depth cues are placed in memory, retrieval will be enhanced when the objects are tested with the same depth cues present.

14 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 13 A common applied use of object recognition data such as this is that of television advertising and virtual reality. In these situations, viewing an object is analagous to our study session, and then recognizing it later on is analagous to the test session. These data suggest that for optimal recognition, objects should be studied with the same depth cues that they are tested with. This implies that since objects are recognized in the real world using stereo 3D cues, they should be presented on television or in virtual reality displays using stereoscopic depth cues. Using the analogy from above, this sets up a lock (a trace in memory) that will be best opened using the key that contains stereo 3D cues (a real-world object). The present work looked only at static images, but depth cues are often enhanced when displays include motion. Stereoscopic depth cues help observers build a 3D mental model of the structure of an object, including its shape and the locations of important features. However, such information is available to some degree through motion: a rotating object also appears to have depth. When motion and stereo depth cues are combined in the same display, they work synergistically to provide a vivid illusion of the actual object appearing in front of the display. As more cues to depth are added to a display, the more likely the person will develop a mental representation that is analagous to the real-world object, and thus they are more likely to correctly recognize the physical object. Thus these findings are not only applicable to moving displays, but they likely to have a larger effect on the encoding and storage of the objects.

15 STEREO PROJECT REPORT PAGE 14 References Biederman, I. & Gerhardstein, P. C. (1993). Recognizing depth-rotated objects: Evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, Bulthoff, H., Edelman, S. & Tarr, M. (1994). How are three-dimensional objects represented in the brain? (A. I. Memo No. 1479). MIT. Edelman, S. & Bulthoff, H. H. (1992). Orientation dependence in the recognition of familiar and novel views of 3D objects. Vision Research, 32, Grimson, W. E. (1993). Why stereo vision is not always about 3D reconstruction (A.I. Memo No. 1435). MIT. Hummel, J. & Biederman, I. (1992). Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition. Psychological Review, 99, Intraub, H., Bender, R. & Mangels, J. (1992). Looking at pictures but remembering scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, Julesz, B. (1971). Foundations of cyclopean perception. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Shipley, T. (1987). Field processes in stereovision. Documenta Opthalmologica, 66, Tarr, M. J. & Pinker, S. (1989). Mental rotation and orientation-dependence in shape recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 21, Tulving, E. & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding Specificity and Retrieval Processes in Episodic Memory. Psychological Review, 80,

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces N. F. Troje* and D. Kersten *Max-Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik, Spemannstr. 38, 72076 Tübingen, Germany Department of Psychology, University

More information

Viewpoint-dependent recognition of familiar faces

Viewpoint-dependent recognition of familiar faces Perception, 1999, volume 28, pages 483 ^ 487 DOI:10.1068/p2901 Viewpoint-dependent recognition of familiar faces Nikolaus F Trojeô Max-Planck Institut fïr biologische Kybernetik, Spemannstrasse 38, 72076

More information

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2008, 61 (4), 552 557 Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them Jeffrey S. Bowers and Keely W. Jones University of Bristol, Bristol,

More information

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler Natural Scene Statistics and Perception W.S. Geisler Some Important Visual Tasks Identification of objects and materials Navigation through the environment Estimation of motion trajectories and speeds

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction Tracey Miser (miser.4@osu.edu) Department of Psychology, 1835 Neil Avenue Columbus, OH43210 USA Vladimir Sloutsky (sloutsky.1@osu.edu)

More information

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Anna V. Fisher (fisher.449@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University

More information

Testing Conditions for Viewpoint Invariance in Object Recognition

Testing Conditions for Viewpoint Invariance in Object Recognition loiiiliil of Experimental Piychokwy: Human Perception and Pufaniaiice 1997, MM. 23, No. 5, 1511-1521 Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/97/1300 Testing Conditions

More information

SENG 412: Ergonomics. Announcement. Readings required for exam. Types of exam questions. Visual sensory system

SENG 412: Ergonomics. Announcement. Readings required for exam. Types of exam questions. Visual sensory system 1 SENG 412: Ergonomics Lecture 11. Midterm revision Announcement Midterm will take place: on Monday, June 25 th, 1:00-2:15 pm in ECS 116 (not our usual classroom!) 2 Readings required for exam Lecture

More information

Sensation & Perception PSYC420 Thomas E. Van Cantfort, Ph.D.

Sensation & Perception PSYC420 Thomas E. Van Cantfort, Ph.D. Sensation & Perception PSYC420 Thomas E. Van Cantfort, Ph.D. Objects & Forms When we look out into the world we are able to see things as trees, cars, people, books, etc. A wide variety of objects and

More information

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth

Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth Journal of Vision (2010) 10(4):1, 1 27 http://journalofvision.org/10/4/1/ 1 Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth Anna Seydell David C. Knill Julia Trommershäuser Department

More information

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses?

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses? Mental Imagery What is Imagery? What is nature of the images? Exact copy of original images? Represented in terms of meaning? If so, then how does the subjective sensation of an image arise? What is the

More information

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity S. B. Stevenson *, L. K. Cormack Vision Research 40, 2881-2884. (2000) * University of Houston College of Optometry, Houston TX 77204

More information

How Many Colors Can You Remember? Capacity is about Conscious vs unconscious memories

How Many Colors Can You Remember? Capacity is about Conscious vs unconscious memories Science B44 Lecture 18 Visual Memory Memory 1. Afterimage, persistence, iconic sensory memory 2. Conscious vs unconscious memories 3. Short and long term memories 4. Where are memories seen 5. Flashbulb

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article. Materials

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article. Materials Research Article TO WHAT EXTENT DO UNIQUE PARTS INFLUENCE RECOGNITION ACROSS CHANGES IN VIEWPOINT? Michael J. Tarr,1 Heinrich H. Bulthoff,2 Marion Zabinski,3 and Volker Blanz2 1 Brown University, 2 Max-Planck

More information

Differing views on views: response to Hayward and Tarr (2000)

Differing views on views: response to Hayward and Tarr (2000) Vision Research 40 (2000) 3901 3905 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Differing views on views: response to Hayward and Tarr (2000) Irving Biederman a, *, Moshe Bar b a Department of Psychology and Neuroscience

More information

Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information

Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information Peter Kalocsai and Irving Biederman Department of Psychology and Computer Science University of Southern California Los

More information

Discrete Resource Allocation in Visual Working Memory

Discrete Resource Allocation in Visual Working Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 5, 1359 1367 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015792 Discrete Resource

More information

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH Laurence D. Smith University of Maine ldsmith@maine.maine.edu Ronald M. Pickett Marjan Trutschl Institute for Visualization and Perception

More information

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get IAT 355 Perception 1 Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get Why we need to understand perception The ability of viewers to interpret visual (graphical) encodings of information and

More information

Scene recognition following locomotion around a scene

Scene recognition following locomotion around a scene Perception, 2006, volume 35, pages 1507 ^ 1520 DOI:10.1068/p5459 Scene recognition following locomotion around a scene Michael A Motes, Cory A Finlay, Maria Kozhevnikovô Department of Psychology, Rutgers

More information

Shape memory for intrinsic versus accidental holes

Shape memory for intrinsic versus accidental holes Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2009, 71 (1), 200-206 doi:10.3758/app.71.1.200 Shape memory for intrinsic versus accidental holes ROLF NELSON Wheaton College, Norton, Massachusetts AND JESSICA THIERMAN

More information

Sensation and Perception

Sensation and Perception Sensation and Perception 1 Chapters 4 of the required textbook Introduction to Psychology International Edition bv James Kalat (2010) 9 th Edition EXPECTED LEARNING OUTCOMES After studying this chapter,

More information

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING George J. Andersen, Rui Ni Department of Psychology University of California Riverside Riverside, California, USA E-mail: Andersen@ucr.edu E-mail: ruini@ucr.edu

More information

Gist of the Scene. Aude Oliva ABSTRACT II. THE NATURE OF THE GIST I. WHAT IS THE GIST OF A SCENE? A. Conceptual Gist CHAPTER

Gist of the Scene. Aude Oliva ABSTRACT II. THE NATURE OF THE GIST I. WHAT IS THE GIST OF A SCENE? A. Conceptual Gist CHAPTER INO041 10/18/04 6:15 PM Page 251 CHAPTER 41 Gist of the Scene Aude Oliva ABSTRACT Studies in scene perception have shown that observers recognize a real-world scene at a single glance. During this expeditious

More information

Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items

Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items Journal of Vision (2003) 3, 6-21 http://journalofvision.org/3/1/2/ 6 Feature binding in object-file representations of multiple moving items Jun Saiki PRESTO, JST, Kawaguchi, Japan; and Graduate School

More information

Normative Representation of Objects: Evidence for an Ecological Bias in Object Perception and Memory

Normative Representation of Objects: Evidence for an Ecological Bias in Object Perception and Memory Normative Representation of Objects: Evidence for an Ecological Bias in Object Perception and Memory Talia Konkle (tkonkle@mit.edu) Aude Oliva (oliva@mit.edu) Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences

More information

To what extent do unique parts influence recognition across changes in viewpoint?

To what extent do unique parts influence recognition across changes in viewpoint? Psychological Science 1997, Vol. 8, No. 4, 282-289 To what extent do unique parts influence recognition across changes in viewpoint? Michael J. Tarr Brown University Heinrich H. Bülthoff, Marion Zabinski,

More information

August 30, Alternative to the Mishkin-Ungerleider model

August 30, Alternative to the Mishkin-Ungerleider model 1 Visual Cognition August 30, 2007 2 3 Overview of Visual Cognition Visual system: mission critical Multivariate inputs, unitary experience Multiple types of vision means distributed visual network Segregating

More information

Neural codes PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 12. COC illusion

Neural codes PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 12. COC illusion Neural codes PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 12 Is 100 billion neurons enough? COC illusion The COC illusion looks like real squares because the neural responses are similar True squares COC squares Ganglion

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

The synergy of top-down and bottom-up attention in complex task: going beyond saliency models.

The synergy of top-down and bottom-up attention in complex task: going beyond saliency models. The synergy of top-down and bottom-up attention in complex task: going beyond saliency models. Enkhbold Nyamsuren (e.nyamsuren@rug.nl) Niels A. Taatgen (n.a.taatgen@rug.nl) Department of Artificial Intelligence,

More information

Differing views on views: comments on Biederman and Bar (1999)

Differing views on views: comments on Biederman and Bar (1999) Vision Research 40 (2000) 3895 3899 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Differing views on views: comments on Biederman and Bar (1999) William G. Hayward a, *, Michael J. Tarr b,1 a Department of Psychology,

More information

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay*

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay* Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay* JACOB BECK and BRUCE AMBLER University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 other extreme, when no masking

More information

Sensory Cue Integration

Sensory Cue Integration Sensory Cue Integration Summary by Byoung-Hee Kim Computer Science and Engineering (CSE) http://bi.snu.ac.kr/ Presentation Guideline Quiz on the gist of the chapter (5 min) Presenters: prepare one main

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Laboratory for Shape and Depth/Distance Perception

Laboratory for Shape and Depth/Distance Perception Name Laboratory for Shape and Depth/Distance Perception 1. Pictorial Depth Cues [Monocular Cues] a. Depth Cue Where it appears in the painting What time of day might be depicted in the painting and what

More information

CHAPTER 6: Memory model Practice questions at - text book pages 112 to 113

CHAPTER 6: Memory model Practice questions at - text book pages 112 to 113 QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS CHAPTER 6: Memory model Practice questions at - text book pages 112 to 113 1) Which of the following sequences reflects the order in which the human brain processes

More information

How does attention spread across objects oriented in depth?

How does attention spread across objects oriented in depth? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72 (4), 912-925 doi:.3758/app.72.4.912 How does attention spread across objects oriented in depth? Irene Reppa Swansea University, Swansea, Wales Daryl Fougnie Vanderbilt

More information

Task and object learning in visual recognition

Task and object learning in visual recognition A.I. Memo No. 1348 C.B.1.P Memo No. 63 MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE LABORATORY and CENTER FOR BIOLOGICAL INFORMATION PROCESSING WHITAKER COLLEGE Task and object learning

More information

Gaze Bias Learning II. Linking neuroscience, computational modeling, and cognitive development. Tokyo, Japan March 12, 2012

Gaze Bias Learning II. Linking neuroscience, computational modeling, and cognitive development. Tokyo, Japan March 12, 2012 Gaze Bias Learning II Linking neuroscience, computational modeling, and cognitive development Tokyo, Japan March 12, 2012 Tamagawa University Research & Management Building Meeting room 507 This workshop

More information

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making Jan M. Wiener (jwiener@bournemouth.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University Poole, BH12 5BB, UK Olivier De

More information

Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory

Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory 1 Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory Imagine that you are watching a game of tennis on television and someone accidentally mutes the sound. You will probably notice that following the game becomes

More information

Viewpoint Dependence in Human Spatial Memory

Viewpoint Dependence in Human Spatial Memory From: AAAI Technical Report SS-96-03. Compilation copyright 1996, AAAI (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved. Viewpoint Dependence in Human Spatial Memory Timothy P. McNamara Vaibhav A. Diwadkar Department

More information

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 J'OURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 2, 446-452 (1963) Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1 A visual configuration, or pattern, has qualities over and above those which can be specified

More information

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3 Perception Chapter 8, Section 3 Principles of Perceptual Organization The perception process helps us to comprehend the confusion of the stimuli bombarding our senses Our brain takes the bits and pieces

More information

Aging and the Detection of Collision Events in Fog

Aging and the Detection of Collision Events in Fog University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2009 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 23rd, 12:00 AM Aging and the Detection of Collision Events in Fog Zheng Bian University of California,

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2007

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2007 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2007 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect, which you ll acknowledge when you turn in your exam. Each multiple-choice question is worth 1 point and the value of the other

More information

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception Paul E. Hemeren (paul.hemeren@his.se) University of Skövde, School of Humanities and Informatics

More information

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E. In D. Algom, D. Zakay, E. Chajut, S. Shaki, Y. Mama, & V. Shakuf (Eds.). (2011). Fechner Day 2011: Proceedings of the 27 th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 89-94). Raanana,

More information

Measurement of Individual Changes in the Performance of Human Stereoscopic Vision for Disparities at the Limits of the Zone of Comfortable Viewing

Measurement of Individual Changes in the Performance of Human Stereoscopic Vision for Disparities at the Limits of the Zone of Comfortable Viewing Measurement of Individual Changes in the Performance of Human Stereoscopic Vision for Disparities at the Limits of the Zone of Comfortable Viewing Jan Paulus Pattern Recognition Lab Institute of Photonic

More information

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction Marc Pomplun and Sindhura Sunkara Department of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts at Boston 100 Morrissey

More information

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit Vision Research 45 (2005) 1877 1884 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit Mark Edwards *, John A. Greenwood School of Psychology, Australian National

More information

Gestalt Principles of Grouping

Gestalt Principles of Grouping Gestalt Principles of Grouping Ch 4C depth and gestalt 1 There appears to be some inherent cognitive process to organize information in a simple manner (nativist perspective). Without some sort of mental

More information

Types of questions. You need to know. Short question. Short question. Measurement Scale: Ordinal Scale

Types of questions. You need to know. Short question. Short question. Measurement Scale: Ordinal Scale You need to know Materials in the slides Materials in the 5 coglab presented in class Textbooks chapters Information/explanation given in class you can have all these documents with you + your notes during

More information

Categorical Perception

Categorical Perception Categorical Perception Discrimination for some speech contrasts is poor within phonetic categories and good between categories. Unusual, not found for most perceptual contrasts. Influenced by task, expectations,

More information

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004 ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2004 As always, the Skidmore Honor Code is in effect. Read each question carefully and answer it completely. Multiple-choice questions are worth one point each, other questions

More information

Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons

Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons Entire Set of Printable Figures For Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons Cook In one experiment, Cerella (1980) trained pigeons to discriminate intact drawings of Charlie Brown from normal drawings

More information

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice Multiple Choice 1. Which structure is not part of the visual pathway in the brain? a. occipital lobe b. optic chiasm c. lateral geniculate nucleus *d. frontal lobe Answer location: Visual Pathways 2. Which

More information

Systematic perceptual distortion of 3D slant by disconjugate eye movements

Systematic perceptual distortion of 3D slant by disconjugate eye movements Vision Research 46 (2006) 2328 2335 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Systematic perceptual distortion of 3D slant by disconjugate eye movements Hyung-Chul O. Li * Department of Industrial Psychology, Kwangwoon

More information

Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system

Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system Vision Research 39 (1999) 4333 4340 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system Mark Edwards *, Clifton M. Schor School of Optometry, Uni ersity of California, Berkeley,

More information

Memory for moving and static images

Memory for moving and static images Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 27, 14 (5), 989-993 Memory for moving and static images W. J. MATTHEWS University of Warwick, Coventry, England AND CLARE BENJAMIN AND CLAIRE OSBORNE University of Leicester,

More information

Selective attention and cyclopean motion processing

Selective attention and cyclopean motion processing Vision Research 45 (2005) 2601 2607 Brief communication Selective attention and cyclopean motion processing Robert Patterson *, Lisa R. Fournier, Matt Wiediger, Greg Vavrek, Cheryl Becker-Dippman, Ivan

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

View-Specific Coding of Face Shape Linda Jeffery, 1 Gillian Rhodes, 1 and Tom Busey 2

View-Specific Coding of Face Shape Linda Jeffery, 1 Gillian Rhodes, 1 and Tom Busey 2 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article View-Specific Coding of Face Shape Linda Jeffery, 1 Gillian Rhodes, 1 and Tom Busey 2 1 University of Western Australia, Crawley, Australia, and 2 Indiana University,

More information

What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of

What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of What Effect Do Schemas Have On The Recall Of Memories AIM:Bartlett aimed to investigate the effect of culture on memory. memory ability/ability to recall and have advantageous effects if asked to do a

More information

Today: Visual perception, leading to higher-level vision: object recognition, word perception.

Today: Visual perception, leading to higher-level vision: object recognition, word perception. 9.65 - Cognitive Processes - Spring 2004 MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences Course Instructor: Professor Mary C. Potter 9.65 February 9, 2004 Object recognition HANDOUT I. Why is object recognition

More information

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences 2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences Stanislas Dehaene Chair of Experimental Cognitive Psychology Lecture n 5 Bayesian Decision-Making Lecture material translated

More information

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni VISUAL AGNOSIA -damage to the extrastriate visual regions (occipital, parietal and temporal lobes) disrupts recognition of complex visual stimuli

More information

Spatial updating during locomotion does not eliminate viewpoint-dependent visual object processing

Spatial updating during locomotion does not eliminate viewpoint-dependent visual object processing VISUAL COGNITION, 2007, 15 (4), 402419 Spatial updating during locomotion does not eliminate viewpoint-dependent visual object processing Mintao Zhao State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science,

More information

Attention and Scene Perception

Attention and Scene Perception Theories of attention Techniques for studying scene perception Physiological basis of attention Attention and single cells Disorders of attention Scene recognition attention any of a large set of selection

More information

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1 Vision and Action Our ability to move thru our environment is closely tied to visual perception. Simple examples include standing one one foot. It is easier to maintain balance with the eyes open than

More information

Distinctiveness and the Recognition Mirror Effect: Evidence for an Item-Based Criterion Placement Heuristic

Distinctiveness and the Recognition Mirror Effect: Evidence for an Item-Based Criterion Placement Heuristic Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2005, Vol. 31, No. 6, 1186 1198 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1186

More information

Object recognition under sequential viewing conditions: evidence for viewpoint-specific recognition procedures

Object recognition under sequential viewing conditions: evidence for viewpoint-specific recognition procedures Perception, 1994, volume 23, pages 595-614 Object recognition under sequential viewing conditions: evidence for viewpoint-specific recognition procedures Rebecca Lawson, Glyn W Humphreys, Derrick G Watson

More information

Encoding `regular' and `random' sequences of views of novel three-dimensional objects

Encoding `regular' and `random' sequences of views of novel three-dimensional objects Perception, 1999, volume 28, pages 601 ^ 615 DOI:10.1068/p2924 Encoding `regular' and `random' sequences of views of novel three-dimensional objects Karin L Harman, G Keith Humphreyô Department of Psychology,

More information

Image generation in a letter-classification task

Image generation in a letter-classification task Perception & Psychophysics 1976, Vol. 20 (3),215-219 Image generation in a letter-classification task THOMAS R. HERZOG Grand Valley State Colleges, Allandale, Michigan 49401 Subjects classified briefly

More information

Generalized learning of visual-to-auditory substitution in sighted individuals

Generalized learning of visual-to-auditory substitution in sighted individuals available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Generalized learning of visual-to-auditory substitution in sighted individuals Jung-Kyong Kim, Robert J. Zatorre Department

More information

The Effect of Training Context on Fixations Made During Visual Discriminations

The Effect of Training Context on Fixations Made During Visual Discriminations The Effect of Training Context on Fixations Made During Visual Discriminations Randy J. Brou (Rbrou@Inst.Msstate.Edu) 1 Teena M. Garrison (Teenag@Cavs.Msstate.Edu) 2 Stephanie M. Doane (Sdoane@Doane.Inst.Msstate.Edu)

More information

Psychologische Forschung Springer-Verlag 1993

Psychologische Forschung Springer-Verlag 1993 Psychol Res (1993) 55:237-247 PsychoNgical Research Psychologische Forschung Springer-Verlag 1993 Codes and operations in picture matching G. Keith Humphrey and Stephen J. Lupker Department of Psychology,

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Fleishman s Taxonomy of Human Abilities

Fleishman s Taxonomy of Human Abilities Fleishman s Taxonomy of Human Abilities Ability Taxonomy: Introduction At I/O Solutions, we utilize research to build selection tools that are effective and valid. Part of this effort is to use an ability

More information

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Frank Tong Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544 Office: Room 3-N-2B Telephone: 609-258-2652 Fax: 609-258-1113 Email: ftong@princeton.edu Graduate School Applicants

More information

Principals of Object Perception

Principals of Object Perception Principals of Object Perception Elizabeth S. Spelke COGNITIVE SCIENCE 14, 29-56 (1990) Cornell University Summary Infants perceive object by analyzing tree-dimensional surface arrangements and motions.

More information

9.65 Sept. 12, 2001 Object recognition HANDOUT with additions in Section IV.b for parts of lecture that were omitted.

9.65 Sept. 12, 2001 Object recognition HANDOUT with additions in Section IV.b for parts of lecture that were omitted. 9.65 Sept. 12, 2001 Object recognition HANDOUT with additions in Section IV.b for parts of lecture that were omitted. I. Why is visual perception difficult? II. Basics of visual perception A. Gestalt principles,

More information

Optical Illusions 4/5. Optical Illusions 2/5. Optical Illusions 5/5 Optical Illusions 1/5. Reading. Reading. Fang Chen Spring 2004

Optical Illusions 4/5. Optical Illusions 2/5. Optical Illusions 5/5 Optical Illusions 1/5. Reading. Reading. Fang Chen Spring 2004 Optical Illusions 2/5 Optical Illusions 4/5 the Ponzo illusion the Muller Lyer illusion Optical Illusions 5/5 Optical Illusions 1/5 Mauritz Cornelis Escher Dutch 1898 1972 Graphical designer World s first

More information

The dependence of motion repulsion and rivalry on the distance between moving elements

The dependence of motion repulsion and rivalry on the distance between moving elements Vision Research 40 (2000) 2025 2036 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres The dependence of motion repulsion and rivalry on the distance between moving elements Nestor Matthews a, Bard J. Geesaman b, Ning Qian

More information

Organization of Visual Short-Term Memory

Organization of Visual Short-Term Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2000, Vol. 26, No. 3.683-702 Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393AKW5.00 DOT: 1O.1O37W0278-7393.26.3.683

More information

Cognitive Processes PSY 334. Chapter 2 Perception

Cognitive Processes PSY 334. Chapter 2 Perception Cognitive Processes PSY 334 Chapter 2 Perception Object Recognition Two stages: Early phase shapes and objects are extracted from background. Later phase shapes and objects are categorized, recognized,

More information

l3;~~?~~~,'0~'~~t~t:~:~~~~~~~~~~!,1

l3;~~?~~~,'0~'~~t~t:~:~~~~~~~~~~!,1 112 Sensation and Perception Line A should look longer, even though both lines are actually the same length. People who come from noncarpentered cultures that do not use right angles and corners often

More information

Real-World Objects Are Not Represented as Bound Units: Independent Forgetting of Different Object Details From Visual Memory

Real-World Objects Are Not Represented as Bound Units: Independent Forgetting of Different Object Details From Visual Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 2012 American Psychological Association 2012, Vol. 141, No. 4, 000 0096-3445/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0029649 Real-World Objects Are Not Represented as Bound

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth 1574 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/ Vol. 25, No. 7/ July 2008 Shioiri et al. Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth Satoshi Shioiri, 1, * Tomohiko

More information

Explicit and Implicit Memory for Rotating Objects

Explicit and Implicit Memory for Rotating Objects Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2003, Vol. 29, No. 4, 554 562 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.554

More information

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients Anna SORKIN¹, Avi PELED 2, Daphna WEINSHALL¹ 1 Interdisciplinary Center for Neural Computation, Hebrew University of Jerusalem,

More information

DIFFERENT CONFIDENCE-ACCURACY RELATIONSHIPS FOR FEATURE-BASED AND FAMILIARITY-BASED MEMORIES

DIFFERENT CONFIDENCE-ACCURACY RELATIONSHIPS FOR FEATURE-BASED AND FAMILIARITY-BASED MEMORIES DIFFERENT CONFIDENCE-ACCURACY RELATIONSHIPS FOR FEATURE-BASED AND FAMILIARITY-BASED MEMORIES Mark Tippens Reinitz University of Puget Sound William J. Peria University of Washington Julie Anne Séguin Victoria

More information

Accounts of blending, distinctiveness and typicality in the false recognition of faces

Accounts of blending, distinctiveness and typicality in the false recognition of faces IN PRESS: JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY: LEARNING, MEMORY & COGNITION Accounts of blending, distinctiveness and typicality in the false recognition of faces THOMAS A. BUSEY *, Indiana University,

More information

Sensation vs. Perception

Sensation vs. Perception PERCEPTION Sensation vs. Perception What s the difference? Sensation what the senses do Perception process of recognizing, organizing and dinterpreting ti information. What is Sensation? The process whereby

More information

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Experimental design for Cognitive fmri Alexa Morcom Edinburgh SPM course 2017 Thanks to Rik Henson, Thomas Wolbers, Jody Culham, and the SPM authors for slides Overview Categorical designs Factorial designs

More information