Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)"

Transcription

1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc /94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Shaun P. Vecera Recently, R. Egly, J. Driver, and R. D. Rafal (1994) provided evidence for an object-based component of visual orienting in a simple cued reaction time task. However, the effects of objects on visual attention can be due to selection from either of two very different types of representations: (a) a truly object-based representation that codes for object structure or (b) a grouped array representation that codes for groups of spatial locations. Are Egly et al.'s results due to selection from an object-based representation or from a grouped array representation? This question was addressed by using a variant of Egly et al.'s task. The findings replicated those of Egly et al. and demonstrated that the selection in this task is mediated through a grouped array representation. The implications of these results for studies of attentional selection are discussed. In the past, the study of attentional selection has primarily focused on how visual attention selects stimuli on the basis of spatial location in the visual field. However, in recent years there has been an increase in the study of how visual attention selects stimuli on the basis of shape or structure. The former approaches have come to be known as spatial or location-based models of attention, and the latter have come to be known as object-based models of attention. (The literature concerning these two types of selection is quite large and continually growing, so it will not be reviewed here. The reader is referred to the following for reviews and characteristic positions: Duncan, 1984; Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994; Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974; Kramer & Jacobson, 1991; Posner, 1980; Posner, Snyder, & Davidson, 1980; Vecera & Farah, 1994) Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) used a clever procedure that was reported to show both location-based and objectbased components of visual selection in normal subjects as well as differential impairments in these attentional components in parietal-damaged patients. The results from the normal subjects are of primary interest here. Subjects were shown two rectangles that appeared either above and below fixation or to the left and right of fixation. One of the corners was cued by brightening, and a target followed. Subjects made a simple reaction time (RT) response to the onset of the target, a procedure similar to Posner's classic spatial cuing paradigm (see Posner This work was supported in part by a Sigma Xi Grants-in-Aid of Research. I thank Martha Farah for her comments and encouragement on this work, and I thank Marlene Behrmann, MaryLou Cheal, Roberta Klatzky, David Plaut, and Bob Rafal for further discussion of these ideas. Thanks also to Kendra Gilds for her assistance in running subjects and analyzing data. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shaun P. Vecera, Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania Electronic mail may be sent to 316 & Cohen, 1984). The cues were either valid (cue and target at same corner) or invalid (cue and target at different corners). Not surprisingly, Egly et al. found a validity effect: Subjects were faster to respond to validly cued trials than to invalidly cued trials. The finding of interest, however, comes from an analysis of the invalid trials. These trials involved movements of attention either within a rectangle or across rectangles. Although the spatial separation of these two conditions was identical, Egly et al. found that subjects were faster to respond to an invalidly cued target when that target was in the rectangle that had previously been cued (a withinrectangle shift) as compared with a target that appeared in the other, uncued rectangle (an across-rectangles shift). Egly et al. referred to this nonspatial component of orienting as object-based attentional selection. They then showed that these two aspects of attentional selection were dissociable in brain-damaged subjects. Patients with damage to the left parietal lobe showed a deficit in moving attention between objects in the contralesional visual field, whereas patients with damage to the right parietal lobe showed a deficit in moving attention between spatial locations in the contralesional field relative to the ipsilesional field. Although Egly et al.'s data convincingly showed a nonspatial aspect of attentional selection, is the term objectbased attention warranted? Recently it has become clear that the effects of objects in the visual field on attentional selection can be explained within a modified location-based framework (see Kramer & Jacobson, 1991; Vecera & Farah, 1994). It might be that these effects are due to selection from a location-based representation in which the locations have been parsed or grouped according to whether the locations belong to one object or another. This grouped array selection would not be object-based selection; rather, it would be a modified location-based selection in which groups of locations are selected. The underlying representational medium would remain spatial (or location-based). Kramer and Jacobson (1991) provided empirical evidence

2 COMMENT 317 for selection from a grouped array representation. Subjects performed a response competition task in which the target and distractors were embedded in the same object or in different objects. Subjects showed more competition from the distractors when targets were embedded in the same object as the distractors as compared with when the target and distractors were grouped in different objects. However, this effect was reduced if the target and distractors were moved farther away from one another, although grouping within the same object produced more interference than grouping within different objects. That is, the amount of response competition was a function not only of object grouping (same object vs. different object) but also of spatial position (distractors near the target vs. distractors far from the target). Although this grouped array account might explain all of the so-called object-based effects, Vecera and Farah (1994) recently demonstrated that certain types of selection may not be mediated through a grouped array. Using a variant of Duncan's (1984) object-selection task, they found a decrement in selecting from two objects as compared with selecting from a single object (as Duncan originally found), but this effect was independent of whether the objects were spatially adjacent (i.e., superimposed) or spatially separated. This finding suggests that selection in Duncan's task is truly object-based in that object structure, independent of spatial location, is selected. Given the findings of both grouped array selection and object-based selection, an important question is whether Egly et al.'s results were due to selection from a grouped array representation or from an object-based representation. This question is addressed in the experiment below. Egly et al.'s paradigm was used, with one minor modification namely, a spatial manipulation. That is, the corners of the rectangles could be equidistant, as in Egly et al.'s work (the far condition), or the corners of the two different rectangles could be closer than the two comers of a single rectangle (the near condition). The grouped array and object-based accounts make differing predictions as to subjects' performance in these two conditions. If selection in this task is truly object based, then the rectangle itself is being selected. When the cue is invalid, subjects should always be faster if the target is on the same (cued) rectangle (a within-rectangle shift of attention) than when the target is on the other (uncued) rectangle (an across-rectangles shift of attention). This finding would appear as a main effect for type of attention shift (within rectangle vs. across rectangles). The grouped array account, however, suggests that the underlying representation is location based. This account would predict that moving the rectangles closer together should reduce the amount of cost in switching attention from one rectangle to the other. This would appear as an interaction between rectangle separation (near vs. far) and type of attention shift (within rectangle vs. across rectangles). Subjects Method Subjects were 15 Carnegie Mellon University undergraduates. All reported having normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Stimuli The displays used in this experiment were similar to those used by Egly et al. and are shown in Figure 1. Two rectangles appeared A. Large Separation ("Far" Condition) B. Small Separation ("Near" Condition) Horizontal Orientation Vertical Orientation Figure 1. Examples of stimuli used in the experiment.

3 318 COMMENT either above and below or to the left and right of fixation. There were two amounts of spatial separation between the rectangles; they could be far from one another or they could be near one another. The far condition was a replication of Egly et al.'s experiment, and the near condition was used to test whether selection was modified in part by location. Note that the name of the "far" condition is a bit of a misnomer, because the corners of the rectangles were actually equidistant. Finally, the cues could be either valid (cue and target appearing at same corner) or invalid (cue and target appearing at different corners). Also note that there were two types of invalid cues, those in which the cue and target were within the same rectangle (within-rectangle shifts of attention) and those in which the cue and target were across rectangles (across-rectangle shifts of attention). All figures (e.g., the fixation point and rectangles) were white drawn on a black background (i.e., the inverse of Figure 1). The fixation point measured 0.5 cm X 0.5 cm (0.48 X 0.48 of visual angle, respectively). The horizontal and vertical rectangles were identical except for orientation. Each of the rectangles measured 10 cm X 3.5 cm (9.46 X 3.34 of visual angle). The lines that composed the rectangles were 3 pixels wide. The cue consisted of the brightening of one of the ends of a rectangle. This brightening was achieved by having the width of the line increase from its original 3 pixels to 8 pixels. Note that this cuing procedure is slightly different from the one used by Egly et al., in which the corner switched from a gray line to a white line. This procedural difference does not have any theoretical implications for the present article, assuming that Egly et al.'s results are replicated in the far condition. Finally, the target was a gray box that appeared at one of the corners. The target measured 1.9 cm X 2.3 cm (1.81 X 2.2 of visual angle). When the rectangles were far from one another, the center of the rectangle was 2.95 cm (2.81 ) from the center of fixation. In this condition the four corners of the two rectangles were equidistant from one another; this distance measured 6.1 cm (5.81 of visual angle) from the center of the target in each corner. When the rectangles were near one another, the center of the rectangle was 2.05 cm (1.96 ) from the center of fixation. In this condition the ends of the two rectangles were closer to one another (3.2 cm or 3.05 from the center of the target in each corner) than were the ends of a single rectangle (6.1 cm; see Figure 1). Thus, in the near condition, subjects would move attention a shorter spatial distance when switching from one rectangle to the other, whereas in the far condition the spatial distance was the same when moving either within or across rectangles. Procedure All stimuli were presented on a Macintosh Plus computer. Subjects sat approximately 60 cm from the monitor. Subjects first participated in 80 practice trials in which their eye movements were monitored by the experimenter through a mirror. Subjects were told that they should not make any eye movements. By the end of the practice, subjects made no eye movements. After this practice, subjects received 640 experimental presentations that were divided into 8 blocks of 80 trials each. Subjects were allowed to rest between blocks. An individual trial began with a 1,000 ms fixation display that contained the fixation point and the two rectangles. After this display the cue was presented for 100 ms. The fixation display was then presented for another 200 ms (i.e., the interstimulus interval between the cue and target was 200 ms). Finally, the target appeared and remained visible until the subject responded. Subjects responded by pressing the space bar on a standard keyboard. Half of the subjects responded with their left hand and half responded with their right hand. After each response the screen was blank for 500 ms before the next trial began. The trials were distributed as follows. Half of the time the rectangles were horizontal and half of the time they were vertical. RTs to horizontal and vertical rectangles were averaged because Egly et al. found no effect for horizontal versus vertical presentation. Half of the time the rectangles were far from one another and half of the time they were near one another. Finally, 20% of the 640 trials were catch trials in which no target appeared. On these trials the fixation display followed the cue for 2,000 ms. Subjects were told to withhold responses on the catch trials. They were also asked to say "error" if they made a false alarm; all subjects complied with this request. For the remaining trials in which a target was presented, 75% of the time the cue was valid, and 25% of the time it was invalid. Half of the invalid trials involved shifting attention within the same rectangle and half of the time they involved shifting attention across the two rectangles. Presentation of the trials was random. Results First, any RTs that were over 1,000 ms or less than 100 ms were excluded from the analyses. This trimming eliminated less than 1.5% of all of the data. For each subject the median RT for each condition was calculated; these median RTs were then analyzed with a within-subject analysis of variance (ANOVA). All analyses were conducted using the SuperANOVA software on the Macintosh. The RTs for the far condition were analyzed first. Any subject who did not show faster responses to invalid cues within a rectangle as compared with those across rectangles was excluded from further analyses. This exclusion was done because these subjects showed a spatial selection strategy; that is, they did not show the nonspatial component described by Egly et al. Excluding these subjects should make it more difficult to find spatial effects in the near condition. Three subjects were excluded on the basis of this criterion. These subjects may have explicitly used a spatial strategy to predict the onset of the target; however, it is also possible that there are individual differences in this task. The mean RTs for cue type and spatial separation for 12 subjects appear in Figure 2. Subjects made false alarms on less than 1.5% of the total number of catch trials. As is evident from Figure 2, subjects were faster to respond to validly cued trials as compared with all invalidly cued trials, F( 1,11) = 19.74, p <.001. (Note that the invalid trials have not been separated by within-rectangle vs. across-rectangle shifts. The invalid RTs are due to all invalidly cued targets.) The main effect for spatial separation was not significant, F(l, 11) = 2.17, p >.15, suggesting that the slight spatial distance difference between the near and far conditions did not reliably influence attentional selection (see also Cheal & Lyon, 1989; Reuter-Lorenz & Fendrich, 1992, for consistent findings). Finally, the interaction between these two variables was not significant, F(l, 11) < 1, suggesting that the validity effects in the near and far conditions did not differ significantly from one another. Next, the invalid cues were divided into within-rectangle shifts and across-object shifts. The mean RTs for invalid cue

4 COMMENT 319 I I 370 Valid Versus Invalid Cues Near Distance Figure 2. Mean reaction times to valid and invalid cues for the near and far conditions. Note that the error bars contain betweensubject variability. type and spatial separation appear in Figure 3. The main effect for the shift of attention was significant, F(l, 11) = 45.33, p <.0001, with within-rectangle shifts resulting in faster RTs than across-rectangle shifts. The main effect for separation was not significant, as in the first analysis, F(l, 11) = 1.37, p >.25. Finally, and most important, the interaction between the two variables was significant, F(l, 11) = 9.50, p <.02, suggesting that the cost in shifting from one rectangle to another was influenced by the separation of the two rectangles. Planned comparisons were conducted on the invalid trials to test within- versus across-rectangle shifts for the near and far conditions separately. For the far condition, the difference was significant, f(ll) = , p <.0001, replicating Egly et al.'s findings. Similarly, the difference for the near condition was also significant, f(ll) = 45.83, p < This finding suggests that the structure that the rectangles imposed on the visual field still influenced the allocation of visual attention. This finding is discussed further below. The second important finding calls into question the use of the term "object-based" in reference to this task. When the rectangles were moved closer to one another (the near condition), subjects were again faster to respond to validly cued targets than to invalidly cued targets, and this validity effect did not differ from the validity effect in the far condition. However, within the invalid trials, the movement of attention (within a rectangle vs. across rectangles) interacted with the distance of the two rectangles. That is, the cost in shifting between the two rectangles was significantly reduced when the two rectangles were moved closer to one another. This finding demonstrates that the nonspatial finding reported by Egly et al. can indeed be influenced by a spatial manipulation. What implication does this interaction have for Egly et al.'s findings? The major implication of this result is that it suggests that the object-based selection argued for by Egly et al. is not object-based selection, assuming that object-based selection is unaffected by location manipulations (Vecera & Farah, 1994). If selection in this task were truly object-based, then manipulating the distance between the ends of the rectangles should have little if any effect on the movement of attention within and across rectangles. In particular, the advantage for within-rectangle shifts relative to between-rectangles shifts should have been the same in the far and near conditions, resulting in a main effect of attention shift but no interaction. Given this, what type of selection is occurring in this task? The present results are consistent with attentional selection from a grouped location-based (or grouped array format) representation. This type of representation contains features or locations (or both) that have been grouped according to whether they belong to the same shape or not (see Kramer & Jacobson, Shifts Within Versus Between Rectangles Within Rectangle Across Rectangles Discussion There are several findings of theoretical relevance from this study. First, the results of the far condition replicate Egly et al.'s results. Subjects were faster to respond to targets that followed a valid cue than to those that followed an invalid cue. This finding confirms a spatial component of attentional selection. However, on the invalid trials alone, subjects were faster to respond when the cue and target were located within the same object than when they were located across different objects. This finding confirms a nonspatial component of attentional selection. Recall that Egly et al. referred to this component as object-based. Figure 3. Mean reaction times for the invalid cues, broken down by whether the shift of attention was within a rectangle or between rectangles. Note that the error bars contain betweensubject variability.

5 320 COMMENT 1991; Vecera & Farah, 1994). However, this type of representation is still location based, because if the shape changes ocations, the group of locations also changes. Selection from this type of representation would still be location based (and thus would show spatial effects, as in the present experiment), although it would not be simple location-based selection, as with a spotlight, zoom lens, or other mechanism that does not respect perceptual groupings. It should also be noted that location-invariant object-based selection is not merely a theoretical construct. Recent results have supported object-based selection irrespective of object location (Vecera & Farah, 1994). Thus, a simple spatial manipulation can be diagnostic in determining whether selection is occurring from a grouped array or from an object-based representation. Next, it is important to note that even in the near condition there was a significant difference between moving attention within an object as compared with moving attention between objects: Movements of attention within a rectangle were faster than movements across rectangles. (Note that this finding does not compromise the above finding and conclusion, because an object-based theory would predict no interaction between distance and movement of attention within and between rectangles.) This finding does, however, suggest that perceptual groupings are extremely strong in the influence they have on locationbased attentional selection. In the near condition, even though a movement across objects was spatially smaller than a movement within an object, there was still an advantage for moving within an object. This suggests that the perceptual groups established by early and intermediate vision can dramatically influence the processing of subsequent visual mechanisms, such as spatial attention. Finally, what implications do the present results have for the work that Egly et al. conducted with neurological patients? With respect to this work, the present findings suggest that left-parietal-damaged subjects may not have a deficit in object-based attention per se, but rather they may have a deficit in attending to grouped array representations. Right-parietal-damaged subjects may have deficits in allocating location-based attention to spatial locations, independent of the structure in the visual field, as Egly et al. suggested. The present experiment does not address these issues, but it offers testable predictions based on the grouped array-object-based distinction. References Cheal, M. L., & Lyon, D. (1989). Attention effects on form discrimination at different eccentricities. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, Kramer, A. F., & Jacobson, A. (1991). Perceptual organization and focused attention: The role of objects and proximity in visual processing. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32A, Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X (pp ). London: Erlbaum. Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Fendrich, R. (1992). Oculomotor readiness and covert orienting: Differences between central and peripheral precues. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, Vecera, S. P., & Farah, M. J. (1994). Does visual attention select objects or locations? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, Received December 1, 1993 Revision received January 30, 1994 Accepted February 2, 1994

Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles

Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (5), 945-949 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.5.945 Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles Xingshan Li and Gordon D. Logan Vanderbilt

More information

Selective attention to the parts of an object

Selective attention to the parts of an object Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2000, 7 (2), 301-308 Selective attention to the parts of an object SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa MARLENE BEHRMANN Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh,

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Exp Brain Res (2002) 142:349 353 DOI 10.1007/s00221-001-0899-2 RESEARCH ARTICLE Richard A. Abrams Mark B. Law Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Received: 10 May 2000 / Accepted: 4 September

More information

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1 The attentional window configures to object boundaries University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 2 ABSTRACT When

More information

Object-based selection in the two-rectangles method is not an artifact of the three-sided directional cue

Object-based selection in the two-rectangles method is not an artifact of the three-sided directional cue Perception & Psychophysics 2003, 65 (7), 1103-1109 Object-based selection in the two-rectangles method is not an artifact of the three-sided directional cue MARK T. MARRARA and CATHLEEN M. MOORE Pennsylvania

More information

Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the processes underlying the object advantage

Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the processes underlying the object advantage Perception & Psychophysics 2007, 69 (4), 606-618 Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the processes underlying the object advantage JAMES M. BROWN AND HOPE I. DENNEY University of Georgia,

More information

Attentional Spreading in Object-Based Attention

Attentional Spreading in Object-Based Attention Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2008, Vol. 34, No. 4, 842 853 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.842

More information

How does attention spread across objects oriented in depth?

How does attention spread across objects oriented in depth? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72 (4), 912-925 doi:.3758/app.72.4.912 How does attention spread across objects oriented in depth? Irene Reppa Swansea University, Swansea, Wales Daryl Fougnie Vanderbilt

More information

Hemispheric performance in object-based attention

Hemispheric performance in object-based attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (1), 84-91 Hemispheric performance in object-based attention MONICA A. VALSANGKAR-SMYTH University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada CHRISTA-LYNN DONOVAN University

More information

The Spatial Distribution of Attention Within and Across Objects

The Spatial Distribution of Attention Within and Across Objects Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 1, 135 151 2011 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0024463 The Spatial Distribution

More information

Object-based attention: Shifting or uncertainty?

Object-based attention: Shifting or uncertainty? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (7), 1743-1755 doi:10.3758/app.72.7.1743 Research Articles Object-based attention: Shifting or uncertainty? Leslie Drummond and Sarah Shomstein George Washington

More information

Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty

Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty Perception & Psychophysics 2008, 70 (8), 1435-1443 doi: 10.3758/PP.70.8.1435 Object-based attention with endogenous cuing and positional certainty ZHE CHEN University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand

More information

Strength of object representation: its key role in object-based attention for determining the competition result between Gestalt and top-down objects

Strength of object representation: its key role in object-based attention for determining the competition result between Gestalt and top-down objects Atten Percept Psychophys (2015) 77:2284 2292 DOI 10.3758/s13414-015-0922-5 Strength of object representation: its key role in object-based attention for determining the competition result between Gestalt

More information

Retinal location and its effect on the spatial distribution of visual attention. by Paula Goolkasian

Retinal location and its effect on the spatial distribution of visual attention. by Paula Goolkasian American Journal of Psychology Summer 1999 v112 i2 p187(2) Page 1 by Paula Goolkasian A series of studies tested for distractor compatibility effects across wide target/distractor distances (0.6 [degrees]

More information

Spatial attention: normal processes and their breakdown

Spatial attention: normal processes and their breakdown Neurol Clin N Am 21 (2003) 575 607 Spatial attention: normal processes and their breakdown Shaun P. Vecera, PhD a, *, Matthew Rizzo, MD b a Department of Psychology, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242,

More information

Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect

Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect Q. Gao, Z. Chen, P. Russell Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect Quanying Gao, Zhe Chen, & Paul Russell University of Canterbury Although the effect of working memory (WM) load on the

More information

Experience-Dependent Perceptual Grouping and Object-Based Attention

Experience-Dependent Perceptual Grouping and Object-Based Attention Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 1, 202 217 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.1.202

More information

Grouping does not require attention

Grouping does not require attention Perception & Psychophysics 2006, 68 (1), 17-31 Grouping does not require attention DOMINIQUE LAMY, HANNAH SEGAL, and LITAL RUDERMAN Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel Many theories of visual perception

More information

ATTENTION AND PERCEPTUAL ORGANIZATION: WHAT THE OBJECT EFFECT REVEALS ABOUT THE REPRESENTATION OF OBJECTS IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM HOPE DENNEY

ATTENTION AND PERCEPTUAL ORGANIZATION: WHAT THE OBJECT EFFECT REVEALS ABOUT THE REPRESENTATION OF OBJECTS IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM HOPE DENNEY ATTENTION AND PERCEPTUAL ORGANIZATION: WHAT THE OBJECT EFFECT REVEALS ABOUT THE REPRESENTATION OF OBJECTS IN THE VISUAL SYSTEM by HOPE DENNEY (Under the direction of James M. Brown) Abstract This study

More information

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load Psychonomic Bulletin & Review In press Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load JOSHUA D. COSMAN AND SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Attentional capture by abrupt onsets can be

More information

Chen, Z. (2009). Not all features are created equal: Processing asymmetries between

Chen, Z. (2009). Not all features are created equal: Processing asymmetries between 1 Chen, Z. (2009). Not all features are created equal: Processing asymmetries between location and object features. Vision Research, 49, 1481-1491. Not All Features Are Created Equal: Processing Asymmetries

More information

Attention to Overlapping Objects: Detection and Discrimination of Luminance Changes

Attention to Overlapping Objects: Detection and Discrimination of Luminance Changes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Human Perception and Performance 0096-1523/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.26.1.342 2000, Vol. 26, No.

More information

Automatic focusing of attention on object size and shape

Automatic focusing of attention on object size and shape Psicológica (2005), 26, 147-160. Automatic focusing of attention on object size and shape Cesar Galera 1*, Michael von Grünau 2 and Afroditi Panagopoulos 2 1 FFCLRP-Universidade de São Paulo (Brazil) 2

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff Operator Characteristics of Endogenous and Exogenous Covert Orienting of Attention

Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff Operator Characteristics of Endogenous and Exogenous Covert Orienting of Attention Review Article TheScientificWorldJOURNAL (2005) 5, 128 146 ISSN 1537-744X; DOI 10.1100/tsw.2005.20 Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff Operator Characteristics of Endogenous and Exogenous Covert Orienting of Attention

More information

Size scaling and spatial factors in visual attention. by Paula Goolkasian

Size scaling and spatial factors in visual attention. by Paula Goolkasian American Journal of Psychology Fall 1997 v110 n3 p397(19) Page 1 by Paula Goolkasian This study investigates the effect of the visibility of near and far letter distractors on target processing by scaling

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff Operator Characteristics of Endogenous and Exogenous Covert Orienting of Attention.

Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff Operator Characteristics of Endogenous and Exogenous Covert Orienting of Attention. McCormick & Francis 1 Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff Operator Characteristics of Endogenous and Exogenous Covert Orienting of Attention. Peter A. McCormick* 1 and Lori Francis 2 1 St. Francis Xavier University

More information

Object-Based Attention and Occlusion: Evidence From Normal Participants and a Computational Model

Object-Based Attention and Occlusion: Evidence From Normal Participants and a Computational Model Joernal of ~ t a l Psychology: Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Human Perception and Pe~ormance 0096-1523/98/$3.00 1998, Vol. 24, No. 4, 1011-1036 Object-Based Attention and

More information

OBSERVATIONS. Occlusion, Symmetry, and Object-Based Attention: Reply to Saiki (2000)

OBSERVATIONS. Occlusion, Symmetry, and Object-Based Attention: Reply to Saiki (2000) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2000, Vol. 2& No. 4, 1497-1505 Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/00/S5.00 DOI: 1Q.1037//0096-1523.26.4.1497

More information

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The number line effect reflects top-down control Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),862-868???-??? The number line effect reflects top-down control JELENA RISTIC University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set In press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set Timothy J. Wright 1, Walter

More information

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2001, 8 (2), 315-323 Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return ANA B. VIVAS City Liberal Studies: Affiliated Institution of the University of Sheffield, Thessaloniki,

More information

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting

The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting The Influence of the Attention Set on Exogenous Orienting Ahnate Lim (ahnate@hawaii.edu) Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii at Manoa 2530 Dole Street, Honolulu, HI 96822 USA Scott Sinnett (ssinnett@hawaii.edu)

More information

Chen, Z., & O Neill, P. (2001). Processing demand modulates the effect of spatial

Chen, Z., & O Neill, P. (2001). Processing demand modulates the effect of spatial 1 Chen, Z., & O Neill, P. (2001). Processing demand modulates the effect of spatial attention on the judged duration of a brief stimulus. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 1229-1238. Processing Demand Modulates

More information

The role of closure in defining the objects of object-based attention

The role of closure in defining the objects of object-based attention Journal Perception & Psychophysics 2005,?? 67 (?), (7),???-??? 1140-1149 The role of closure in defining the objects of object-based attention ALEXANDRIA C. MARINO and BRIAN J. SCHOLL Yale University,

More information

Grab it! Biased attention in functional hand and tool space

Grab it! Biased attention in functional hand and tool space Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (1), 236-245 doi:10.3758/app.72.1.236 Grab it! Biased attention in functional hand and tool space CATHERINE L. REED University of Denver, Denver, Colorado

More information

Reflexive Visual Orienting in Response to the Social Attention of Others

Reflexive Visual Orienting in Response to the Social Attention of Others Reflexive Visual Orienting in Response to the Social Attention of Others Stephen R. H. Langton and Vicki Bruce Department of Psychology, University of Stirling Running Head: VISUAL ORIENTING AND SOCIAL

More information

Object-based selection is contingent on attentional control settings

Object-based selection is contingent on attentional control settings TSpace Research Repository tspace.library.utoronto.ca Object-based selection is contingent on attentional control settings J. Eric T. Taylor, Jason Rajsic, Jay Pratt Version Post-print/accepted manuscript

More information

Perceptual organization influences visual working memory

Perceptual organization influences visual working memory Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2003, 10 (1), 80-87 Perceptual organization influences visual working memory GEOFFREY F. WOODMAN, SHAUN P. VECERA, and STEVEN J. LUCK University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Previous

More information

Searching through subsets:

Searching through subsets: Searching through subsets: A test of the Visual Indexing Hypothesis Burkell, Jacquelyn A. and Pylyshyn, Zenon W. University of Western Ontario and Rutgers University [1] Short Title: Searching through

More information

Attentional Window and Global/Local Processing

Attentional Window and Global/Local Processing University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 6-16-2016 Attentional Window and Global/Local Processing Steven Peter Schultz University of South Florida,

More information

Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 1483 1490 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Object-based attention and visual area LO Lee

More information

Attention and Scene Perception

Attention and Scene Perception Theories of attention Techniques for studying scene perception Physiological basis of attention Attention and single cells Disorders of attention Scene recognition attention any of a large set of selection

More information

SHIFTS OF COVERT VISUAL ATTENTION: A FUNCTION OF MERIDIA, DISTANCE, AND CUE TYPE MELI MACCURDY

SHIFTS OF COVERT VISUAL ATTENTION: A FUNCTION OF MERIDIA, DISTANCE, AND CUE TYPE MELI MACCURDY SHIFTS OF COVERT VISUAL ATTENTION: A FUNCTION OF MERIDIA, DISTANCE, AND CUE TYPE MELI MACCURDY Abstract: Shifts of covert attention differ depending on whether the shift is exogenously or endogenously

More information

Gaze cues evoke both spatial and object-centered shifts of attention

Gaze cues evoke both spatial and object-centered shifts of attention Journal Perception & Psychophysics 2006,?? 68 (?), (2),???-??? 310-318 Gaze cues evoke both spatial and object-centered shifts of attention ANDREW P. BAYLISS and STEVEN P. TIPPER University of Wales, Bangor,

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH Laurence D. Smith University of Maine ldsmith@maine.maine.edu Ronald M. Pickett Marjan Trutschl Institute for Visualization and Perception

More information

Conscious awareness of methodological choices: A reply to Milberg and McGlinchey (2010)

Conscious awareness of methodological choices: A reply to Milberg and McGlinchey (2010) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (3), 622-627 doi:10.3758/app.72.3.622 Conscious awareness of methodological choices: A reply to Milberg and McGlinchey (2010) MARLENE BEHRMANN Carnegie Mellon

More information

Posner s Attention Test

Posner s Attention Test iworx Physiology Lab Experiment Experiment HP-18 Posner s Attention Test Background Setup Lab Note: The lab presented here is intended for evaluation purposes only. iworx users should refer to the User

More information

NOISE EXCLUSION IN SPATIAL ATTENTION

NOISE EXCLUSION IN SPATIAL ATTENTION Research Article NOISE EXCLUSION IN SPATIAL ATTENTION Barbara Anne Dosher 1 and Zhong-Lin Lu 2 1 Department of Cognitive Sciences, Institute of Mathematical Behavioral Sciences, and Center for Neurobiology

More information

How Does Attention Select and Track Spatially Extended Objects? New Effects of Attentional Concentration and Amplification

How Does Attention Select and Track Spatially Extended Objects? New Effects of Attentional Concentration and Amplification Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 134, No. 4, 461 476 0096-3445/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.461 How Does Attention

More information

Selective Attention. Chapter Definition and Domain. Contents

Selective Attention. Chapter Definition and Domain. Contents Chapter 2 Selective Attention Contents 2.1 Definition and Domain............................. 5 2.2 Spatial Cueing.................................. 9 2.3 Partially-Valid Cueing.............................

More information

Implied body action directs spatial attention

Implied body action directs spatial attention Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (6), 1437-1443 doi:10.3758/app.72.6.1437 Brief Reports Implied body action directs spatial attention Will M. Gervais University of Denver, Denver, Colorado

More information

Perceptual grouping in change detection

Perceptual grouping in change detection 1 Perceptual grouping in change detection Yuhong Jiang Massachusetts Institute of Technology Marvin M. Chun Yale University Ingrid R. Olson University of Pennsylvania [In Press: Perception & Psychophysics,

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return

Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (6), 935-944 doi:10.3758/mc.37.6.935 Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return JONG MOON CHOI

More information

Enhanced visual perception near the hands

Enhanced visual perception near the hands Enhanced visual perception near the hands Bachelor thesis Marina Meinert (s0163430) Supervisors: 1 st supervisor: Prof. Dr. Ing. W. B. Verwey 2 nd supervisor: Dr. M. L. Noordzij External supervisor: Dr.

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

Perceptual grouping in change detection

Perceptual grouping in change detection Perception & Psychophysics 2004, 66 (3), 446-453 Perceptual grouping in change detection YUHONG JIANG Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts MARVIN M. CHUN Yale University, New

More information

Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search

Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search Perception & Psychophysics 2005, 67 (7), 1252-1268 Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search JOY J. GENG Carnegie Mellon University and Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Pittsburgh,

More information

Follow this and additional works at: Part of the Neurosciences Commons

Follow this and additional works at:   Part of the Neurosciences Commons University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Open Access Dissertations 9-2012 Gradients and Ranges of Visually Selective Attention Based on Location, Objects, Color, and Size: Gradients

More information

Attention capacity and task difficulty in visual search

Attention capacity and task difficulty in visual search Cognition 94 (2005) B101 B111 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Attention capacity and task difficulty in visual search Liqiang Huang, Harold Pashler* Department of Psychology 0109, University

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

Perceptual grouping determines the locus of attentional selection

Perceptual grouping determines the locus of attentional selection 1506 OPAM 2010 REPORT when finding an additional target was more likely. This was observed both within participants (with a main effect of number of targets found), and between groups (with an interaction

More information

Validity of Haptic Cues and Its Effect on Priming Visual Spatial Attention

Validity of Haptic Cues and Its Effect on Priming Visual Spatial Attention Validity of Haptic Cues and Its Effect on Priming Visual Spatial Attention J. Jay Young & Hong Z. Tan Haptic Interface Research Laboratory Purdue University 1285 EE Building West Lafayette, IN 47907 {youngj,

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return David J. Prime and Pierre Jolicoeur Abstract & Here we examined the relationship between inhibition of return (IOR) and response-selection

More information

Competition Between Endogenous and Exogenous Orienting of Visual Attention

Competition Between Endogenous and Exogenous Orienting of Visual Attention Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 134, No. 2, 207 221 0096-3445/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.207 Competition Between

More information

Spatially Diffuse Inhibition Affects Multiple Locations: A Reply to Tipper, Weaver, and Watson (1996)

Spatially Diffuse Inhibition Affects Multiple Locations: A Reply to Tipper, Weaver, and Watson (1996) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1996, Vol. 22, No. 5, 1294-1298 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/%/$3.00 Spatially Diffuse Inhibition

More information

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (2), 327-331 The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR THOMAS M. SPALEK AND VINCENT DI LOLLO Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia,

More information

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory

Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Rapid Resumption of Interrupted Visual Search New Insights on the Interaction Between Vision and Memory Alejandro Lleras, 1 Ronald A. Rensink, 2 and James T. Enns

More information

Which way is which? Examining symbolic control of attention with compound arrow cues

Which way is which? Examining symbolic control of attention with compound arrow cues University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 2016 Which way is which? Examining symbolic control

More information

Inhibition of Return to an Occluded Object Depends on Expectation

Inhibition of Return to an Occluded Object Depends on Expectation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2005, Vol. 31, No. 6, 1224 1233 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1224

More information

Novelty is not always the best policy: Inhibition of return and facilitation of return. as a function of visual task. Michael D.

Novelty is not always the best policy: Inhibition of return and facilitation of return. as a function of visual task. Michael D. IOR, task set 1 Running head: INHIBITION OF RETURN, TASK SET Novelty is not always the best policy: Inhibition of return and facilitation of return as a function of visual task Michael D. Dodd University

More information

The Contribution of Covert Attention to the Set-Size and Eccentricity Effects in Visual Search

The Contribution of Covert Attention to the Set-Size and Eccentricity Effects in Visual Search Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Human Perception and Performance 1998, Vol. 24, No. 2, 673-692 0096-1523/98/$3.00 The Contribution of

More information

Novelty Is Not Always the Best Policy Inhibition of Return and Facilitation of Return as a Function of Visual Task

Novelty Is Not Always the Best Policy Inhibition of Return and Facilitation of Return as a Function of Visual Task PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Novelty Is Not Always the Best Policy Inhibition of Return and Facilitation of Return as a Function of Visual Task Michael D. Dodd, 1 Stefan Van der Stigchel, 2 and

More information

Spatial Attention and the Apprehension of Spatial Relations

Spatial Attention and the Apprehension of Spatial Relations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1994, Vol. 20, No. 5, 1015-1036 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/94/S3.00 Spatial Attention

More information

Enhanced discrimination in autism

Enhanced discrimination in autism THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2001, 54A (4), 961 979 Enhanced discrimination in autism Michelle O Riordan and Kate Plaisted University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Children with autism

More information

Splitting Visual Focal Attention? It Probably Depends on Who You Are

Splitting Visual Focal Attention? It Probably Depends on Who You Are Splitting Visual Focal Attention? It Probably Depends on Who You Are Jit Yong Yap (yapjityong@gmail.com) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore 9 Arts Link, Singapore 117570 Stephen

More information

Attentional templates regulate competitive interactions among attended visual objects

Attentional templates regulate competitive interactions among attended visual objects Perception & Psychophysics 2007, 69 (2), 209-217 Attentional templates regulate competitive interactions among attended visual objects JEFFREY R. W. MOUNTS State University of New York, Geneseo, New York

More information

Contextual cost: When a visual-search target is not where it should be. Tal Makovski Yuhong V. Jiang

Contextual cost: When a visual-search target is not where it should be. Tal Makovski Yuhong V. Jiang [August 2009; In press, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology] 1 Contextual cost: When a visual-search target is not where it should be Tal Makovski Yuhong V. Jiang Department of Psychology and

More information

Early, Involuntary Top-Down Guidance of Attention From Working Memory

Early, Involuntary Top-Down Guidance of Attention From Working Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2005, Vol. 31, No. 2, 248 261 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.248

More information

Voluntary and involuntary attention have different consequences: The effect of perceptual difficulty

Voluntary and involuntary attention have different consequences: The effect of perceptual difficulty THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2009, 62 (2), 352 369 Voluntary and involuntary attention have different consequences: The effect of perceptual difficulty William Prinzmetal, Aleksey Zvinyatskovskiy,

More information

Which Way Is Which? Examining Global/Local Processing With Symbolic Cues

Which Way Is Which? Examining Global/Local Processing With Symbolic Cues University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 2014 Which Way Is Which? Examining Global/Local

More information

Taking control of reflexive social attention

Taking control of reflexive social attention Cognition 94 (2005) B55 B65 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit Brief article Taking control of reflexive social attention Jelena Ristic*, Alan Kingstone Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia,

More information

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (7), 1236-1247 doi: 10.3758/MC/36.7.1236 An action sequence held in memory can interfere with response selection of a target stimulus, but does not interfere with response activation

More information

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 490-495 The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze CHRIS KELLAND FRIESEN and ALAN KINGSTONE University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta,

More information

The effects of unilateral pulvinar damage in humans on reflexive orienting and filtering of irrelevant information

The effects of unilateral pulvinar damage in humans on reflexive orienting and filtering of irrelevant information Behavioural Neurology 13 (2001/2002) 95 104 95 IOS Press The effects of unilateral pulvinar damage in humans on reflexive orienting and filtering of irrelevant information Shai Danziger a,, Robert Ward

More information

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? University of Iowa Honors Theses University of Iowa Honors Program Spring 2017 Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? Ann Walsh University of Iowa Follow this and additional works at:

More information

Limitations of Object-Based Feature Encoding in Visual Short-Term Memory

Limitations of Object-Based Feature Encoding in Visual Short-Term Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 458 468 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.458

More information

Temporal expectancy modulates inhibition of return in a discrimination task

Temporal expectancy modulates inhibition of return in a discrimination task Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2010, 17 (1), 47-51 doi:10.3758/pbr.17.1.47 Temporal expectancy modulates inhibition of return in a discrimination task SHAI GABAY AND AVISHAI HENIK Ben-Gurion University

More information

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze Psychology, 2014, 5, 451-460 Published Online April 2014 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.55055 Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift

More information

Inhibition of return with rapid serial shifts of attention: Implications for memory and visual search

Inhibition of return with rapid serial shifts of attention: Implications for memory and visual search Perception & Psychophysics 2003, 65 (7), 1126-1135 Inhibition of return with rapid serial shifts of attention: Implications for memory and visual search MICHAEL D. DODD, ALAN D. CASTEL, and JAY PRATT University

More information

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles?

Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize Gestalt Principles? Carnegie Mellon University Research Showcase @ CMU Dietrich College Honors Theses Dietrich College of Humanities and Social Sciences 2006 Are there Hemispheric Differences in Visual Processes that Utilize

More information

The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze

The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 49-495 The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze CHRIS KELLAND FRIESEN and ALAN KINGSTONE University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta,

More information