Perception of complex sounds by the green treefrog, Hyla cinerea: envelope and fine-structure cues

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Perception of complex sounds by the green treefrog, Hyla cinerea: envelope and fine-structure cues"

Transcription

1 J Comp Physiol A (1993) 173: Journal of Comparative and Physiology A Behavioral Physiology 9 Springer-Verlag 1993 Perception of complex sounds by the green treefrog, Hyla cinerea: envelope and fine-structure cues A.M. Simmons, R.C. Buxbaum, M.P. Mirin Department of Psychology, Brown University, Box 1853, 89 Waterman St., Providence, RI 02912, USA Accepted: 30 April, 1993 Abstract. 1. The envelope periodicity of communication signals is an important feature distinguishing advertisement and aggressive calls for the green treefrog (Hyla cinerea). Envelope periodicity, a cue for periodicity pitch perception in humans, is affected by the fine-structure of the signal, a cue for timbre perception in humans. The present study examined perception of two acoustic features affecting waveform fine-structure - harmonic structure and phase structure - in male green treefrogs. 2. We analyzed evoked vocal responses of male green treefrogs living in laboratory arenas to playbacks of digitally-generated signals resembling either conspecific advertisement or aggressive calls in their first harmonic periodicity. Systematic changes in the harmonic structure of these signals were achieved by varying the harmonic relations between frequency components in the signals, and changes in phase structure were achieved by varying the starting phases of harmonically-related components. 3. Calling was significantly influenced by the first harmonic periodicity of the signals. Males vocalized more to signals with the periodicity of the advertisement than the aggressive call. There were no differences in response to harmonic and inharmonic signals with similar spectral content. Phase structure did not significantly influence vocal responses. 4. These results suggest that the fine-structure ("timbre") of complex acoustic signals is not a significant feature guiding behavior tested using a communication response in this species. Key words: Treefrog - Acoustic communication plex sounds - Harmonic periodicity Introduction Com- Many species of anuran amphibians (frogs and toads) communicate using complex, often harmonically-structured acoustic signals. Both spectral and temporal properties of these signals play important roles in species recognition and in mediating female choice of appro- Correspondence to." Dr. A.M. Simmons priate mates. Of these different cues, some of the more salient include the frequency distribution of harmonics in the signal, waveform periodicity, call duration, and calling rate (Allan and Simmons 1993; Forester and Czarnowsky 1985; Gerhardt 1978, 1981, 1987, 1988; Klump and Gerhardt 1987; Schwartz 1986; Straughan 1975; Sullivan 1983; Wells 1988). Waveform periodicity is a particularly important cue for distinguishing between different functional categories of vocalizations in several anuran species. Acoustic features affecting waveform periodicity (the amplitude-time envelope of the signal) include the rate and depth of amplitude modulation as well as the fundamental frequency itself. The importance of these envelope cues is particularly evident in examination of the vocal repertoire of the green treefrog (Hyla cinerea). Males of this species use two distinct calls for communication, the advertisement call and the aggressive call. Both signals are harmonically-structured, with dominant spectral peaks in a low-frequency range around 900 Hz, and in a high-frequency range around 2700 and 3000 Hz (the exact values of these peaks vary both among individual frogs, and between groups of frogs in different parts of their range; Gerhardt, personal communication). These signals differ primarily in their waveform periodicity: The advertisement call has a pulsatile beginning (one to two cycles of Hz modulation) followed by a smooth (unmodulated) envelope with a first harmonic periodicity of about 300 Hz, and the aggressive call is modulated at a rate of 50 Hz, leading to a 50 Hz periodicity as well as to extra sidebands produced by the modulation process (Gerhardt 1978). Both female (Gerhardt 1978) and male (Allan and Simmons 1993) green treefrogs respond differently to, and presumably distinguish between, natural and synthetic versions of these signals on the basis of these differences in waveform periodicity. For human listeners, waveform periodicity is an acoustic cue for the psychological percept of periodicity pitch (Moore 1989). This temporal feature can be influenced by spectral information, represented as the detailed structure of the waveform under the envelope - the fine-structure. Important cues affecting waveform fine-

2 322 A.M. Simmons et al.: Sound perception by treefrogs structure include the particular frequencies present in the sound, and their relative amplitudes and starting phases. These fine-structure cues, in humans, contribute to the psychological percept of timbre, the quality or tone color of a sound (Moore 1989; Plomp 1976). One way in which to affect the percept of timbre is by changing the starting phases of the individual harmonics in the sound; such manipulations in phase structure, by changing the shape of the waveform envelope, affect the salience of the periodicity pitch associated with the sound (Bilsen 1973 ; Lundeen and Small 1984; Moore 1977; Ritsma and Engel 1964). The psychological percept of timbre is also affected by changes in the harmonic relations between individual frequency components in the sound. Depending on the magnitude of the changes in harmonic structure and resulting changes in the shape of the waveform envelope, the sound can be considered to be quasiperiodic or inharmonic, and the salience and identity of the perceived periodicity pitch can be altered (de Boer 1956; Plomp 1976; Schouten et al. 1962). The perceptual salience of acoustic features related to timbre has been identified as important for understanding periodicity pitch perception in humans (Moore 1989; Patterson 1987; Schouten 1970). The focus of the experiments reported here is to examine the role of the fine-structure of complex, biologicallyrelevant vocal signals in mediating the perception of these signals by green treefrogs. Few studies (Gerhardt 1978) have examined the relative influence of envelope and fine-structure cues on call recognition, and have focussed primarily on the perceptibility of the waveform envelope itself (Allan and Simmons 1993; Gerhardt 1978). Whether anurans can perceive, and use for communication, acoustic features contributing to waveform fine-structure is important for two reasons. First, physiological studies suggest that the anuran auditory system represents a good model for studying the neural basis of periodicity phenomena. Eighth nerve fibers code the amplitude envelope of complex sounds (Rose and Capranica 1985; Schwartz and Simmons 1990; Simmons et al. 1992), and are sensitive to both harmonic structure (Simmons and Ferragamo 1993) and phase structure (Simmons et al. 1993). How and whether these physiological cues translate into behavior would provide crucial information in constraining models of complex sound perception in anurans. Second, several species of frogs are behaviorally sensitive to acoustic cues mediating the perception of pitch in humans, such as, as described above, waveform periodicity (Allan and Simmons 1993; Gerhardt 1978, 1988). Perception of phase structure by frogs has not been previously reported. Whether frogs can perceive differences in the harmonic structure of complex sounds is unresolved. Green treefrogs tested in a psychophysical paradigm show lower thresholds for detecting a harmonic than an inharmonic signal in noise (Simmons 1988). But, female green treefrogs do not apparently use this acoustic cue to guide their phonotaxis responses (Gerhardt et al. 1990). In these experiments, we test the ability of male green treefrogs to perceive acoustic cues related to timbre perception in humans. We assess the animals' recognition of synthetic versions of their advertisement and aggressive calls. The synthetic signals have the same waveform periodicity as natural calls of these animals but differ in either their phase structure or their harmonic structure. Perception of these cues is tested using the evoked calling technique, a behavioral assay based on a communication response of the animal. The use of this technique, rather than a psychophysical technique, allows us both to test the assertions by Gerhardt et al. (1990) on the usefulness of harmonic structure as a factor guiding communication and to measure the generalizability of the psychophysical results themselves. Experiments were conducted in the laboratory to allow precise control of the acoustic background. Materials and methods Six male green treefrogs were tested in these experiments. They were established in a circular arena (1.3 m diameter) housed in a soundattenuating room (3 x 3 m) lined with acoustic foam to minimize reverberations and extraneous noise. The room was maintained at temperatures of 23 to 25 ~ with a light/dark schedule of 14 h light/10 h darkness, simulating the natural breeding period of the green treefrog. Males were housed in the arena in groups of 2 to 5 males. In this colony, only one caller was present. If other males began vocalizing, either spontaneously or during the course of playbacks, they were removed in order to avoid vocal interactions between frogs. We noted that males housed singly seldom vocalized. The frogs were tested for about 1 h/day, 5 days/week, during the months of September-November 1990 and September-December Testing began near the beginning of the dark period in the light/dark cycle. Stimuli. Acoustic stimuli were digitally synthesized on an IBM PC/AT computer with a Data Translation DT2801A board at a sampling rate of 20 khz. Two different sets of stimuli were synthesized. For the experiments testing perception of harmonic structure, the stimuli were 4 two-tone complexes with frequencies of Hz (harmonic sound with periodicity of 300 Hz); Hz (harmonic sound with periodicity of 276 Hz); Hz (inharmonic sound without a stable first-harmonic periodicity); and Hz (inharmonic sound without a stable firstharmonic periodicity). For the first 3 stimuli, the changes in finestructure did not obviously change the shape of the waveform envelope, but the envelope shape was altered in the second inharmonic stimulus. Three of these stimuli were the same as those used by Simmons (1988) and two of them were used by Gerhardt et al. (1990). The frequencies in these signals were chosen either to match dominant spectral peaks in the advertisement call of the green treefrog, or to be within the same critical ratio-bands as these frequencies (Moss and Simmons 1986). The individual frequency components in the complexes were of equal amplitude, and were summed together in sine phase. Stimuli were 160 ms in duration with rise/fall times of 10 ms. For the experiments testing perception of phase structure, stimuli were synthetic 12-component harmonic complexes with frequencies based on green treefrog vocalizations recorded in the field in eastern Georgia. These natural vocalizations were digitized with an RC Electronics isc-67 interface board and accompanying software on an IBM PC/AT computer. Spectral analyses of the vocalizations were computed using ILS software from Signal Technology Inc. Twelve harmonics were chosen from this analysis (see Table 1). Their amplitudes and frequencies were set to be integer multiples of a (missing) 300 Hz fundamental frequency, and matched those of the digitized vocalizations. Six different variations of this basic stimulus were produced. Three stimuli were unmodulated, but maintained a waveform periodicity of 300 Hz to match that of the

3 A.M. Simmons et al. : Sound perception by treefrogs 323 Table 1. Composition of the stimuli in phase-shift experiments Frequency (Hz) Relative Phase shifts (degrees) Amplitude (db) SNE ALT RND SNE phase-coherent stimulus; ALT alternating phase stimulus; RND random phase stimulus. These 3 basic signals were left unmodulated to produce 300 Hz-type stimuli, or were modulated at 50 Hz to produce 50 Hz-type stimuli conspecific advertisement call. Three other stimuli were modulated at a rate of 50 Hz (depth 100%) to match the waveform periodicity of the conspecific aggressive call. The stimuli further differed in the starting phases of the individual harmonics. In one variant, all starting phases were set at sine phase (phase-coherent waveform, labeled SNE). In a second variant, starting phases varied between consecutive harmonics between sine and cosine phase (alternatingphase waveform, labeled ALT). In a third variant, starting phases were chosen using a 4th moment algorithm for flattening a waveform, based on that of Pumplin (1985). The resulting waveform is termed random-phase, labeled RND. Starting phases of each harmonic in the 6 stimuli are given in Table 1. Stimulus durations were 160 ms with 10 ms rise/fall times. Procedure. Stimulus presentation was controlled by an IBM PC/AT computer using custom-written software. The digitally-generated sounds were filtered (20 to 6000 Hz) with a Krohn-Hite model 3550 filter, attenuated (Hewlett-Packard audio attenuator and Coulbourn electronic attenuator), amplified (first channel of a Coulbourn $82-24 mixer/amplifier and Onkyo A-15 stereo amplifier), and presented to the frogs via a loudspeaker (Realistic, minimus-7) suspended 1 m above the center of the arena. The frog's responses were picked up by an Azden microphone placed at the approximate position of the calling male and recorded on the right channel of a Marantz (PMD 430) stereo tape deck. Stimuli were simultaneously recorded onto the left channel of the same tape deck. Calibration of RMS intensity levels of the acoustic stimuli was performed before each individual testing session. This was done using a Briiel & Kjaer 2230 digital precision sound level meter ("fast" setting, linear scale) placed at the approximate position of the calling male. The intensity levels of the stimuli did not vary more than 3 db for any position the frog took in the arena. Because animals were not restrained, the exact position of the caller throughout each individual testing session was unknown. We observed, however, that the caller appeared to remain within a small circumscribed area (< 0.3 m) during the experiment. Three frogs were tested for perception of harmonic structure. The 4 different stimuli were presented in 8 blocks (2 blocks for each stimulus) of 270 repetitions each. Interstimulus interval was 760 ms. Within each block, only one stimulus was presented. The order of stimulus presentation (blocks) was organized such that the same stimulus was not presented in consecutive blocks and all 4 stimuli were presented in each half of each playback session. Within these constraints, the order of presentation was randomized. There was a 3 min silent interval between blocks, during which spontaneous calling was measured. Stimulus intensity was set at 70 db SPL, a level at which males reliably vocalize in the laboratory (Allan and Simmons 1993). Background noise was present continually throughout each experimental block. This was generated with a Coulbourn $81-02 noise generator, lts level was set, in different experimental sessions, at spectrum levels of 15, 25 or 35 db RMS/ Hz, the latter two being the same used in the experiment of Simmons (1988). Three frogs were tested for perception of phase structure. The 6 different stimuli were presented in 6 blocks (1 block per stimulus) of 200 repetitions each, with an interstimulus interval of 760 ms. Each block lasted about 2-3 min. The order of stimulus presentation (blocks) was randomized. Silent intervals of 3 min duration separated individual blocks; spontaneous calling was measured during these intervals. Stimulus intensities were set at either 70 or 80 db SPL on separate testing sessions, and stimuli were presented under ambient noise conditions only. Analysis. The tape-recorded vocal responses to the playbacks were passed through a Schmitt trigger and digitized (IBM PC/AT computer with RC Electronics series 200 acquisition board, sampling rate 25 khz). They were analyzed by a custom-designed software program which converted each response into a digital display and computed the total number of responses evoked by each stimulus presentation; the latency of the first response to a given stimulus, measured from the onset of stimulus presentation; and the time-ofoccurrence of each vocal response with respect to each stimulus. The timing data were used to compute vector strength (VS), a measure of synchronization of response to stimulus (Batschelet 1981). VS varies between 0 (indicating no synchronization) and 1 (indicating perfect synchronization). The statistical significance of VS was calculated using the Rayleigh test of circular data (z distribution; Batschelet 1981). Differences in responding across stimulus type were analyzed using Friedman two-way analysis of variance by ranks (chi-square distribution) and the Kruskal-Wallis test. Results Perception of harmonic structure. We analyzed data only from those playback sessions (N= 21) during which the frogs responded to at least 4 blocks of stimuli. Across these 21 playback sessions, there were no significant differences in the proportion of blocks during which responding occurred between harmonic (87 % of blocks) or inharmonic (85% of blocks) stimuli, or between the two harmonic or the two inharmonic stimuli taken separately. There were 35 playback sessions in which the animals responded in at least one stimulus block (we removed from these analyses all sessions in which the frogs vocalized only during the first stimulus block). Within these 35 sessions, there were again no significant differences in proportion of blocks eliciting responding between harmonic (63%) or inharmonic (59%) stimuli. Figure 1 shows how the males' vocal responses were influenced by the harmonic structure of the stimuli and by the background noise level present during playbacks. Data were included only from those 21 sessions meeting our criteria for responding, and are shown as means (+ standard deviations) calculated from responses to the two harmonic stimuli and the two inharmonic stimuli summed together. There were no consistent individual differences between frogs in the pattern of response. The intensity of background noise significantly influenced the number of evoked vocal responses [H (31,63,58)=8.1, P<0.05; Fig. 1A]. Males called significantly more in

4 324 A.M. Simmons et al.: Sound perception by treefrogs response to stimuli presented against a noise level of 25 db RMS/Hz compared to either lower or higher levels. The drop in calling at high levels of background noise suggests that male responses were not stimulated by the noise alone. Spontaneous calling was essentially nonexistent. There were no significant differences in number of responses attributable to differences between harmonic and inharmonic stimuli either across noise levels [Z 2 (3)= 1.1, P< 0.9] or within any particular noise level. Latency to first response also varied with noise intensity (Fig. 1B). For these analyses, a latency was included only for those blocks during which the frogs responded. This allowed us to test for consistent differences in latency apart from differences in number of responses, and it means that these data are based on unequal sample sizes. Latencies to responding were significantly shorter at a noise level of 25 db RMS/Hz [H (29,61,49)=22.2, P<0.01] than at either higher or lower noise levels. Latency did not differ significantly across noise levels with differences in harmonic structure [Z2(3)=1.5, P<0.75), nor within any noise level. Males tended to synchronize their responses to the playbacks, indicating that responding was not controlled by the presence of the continuous background noise alone (Fig. 1C). At all noise levels and for all stimuli, vector strength was statistically different from 0. (These data only include a value for vector strength when the frogs actually responded to a particular stimulus). Responding was significantly more synchronized at a noise level of 15 db RMS/Hz than at a noise level of 35 db RMS/Hz [H (29,61,49)=42.4, P<0.01]. Across noise levels, vector strength did not vary significantly with differences in harmonic structure [Z 2 (3) = 7.1, P < 0.1]. Perception of phase structure. Data could be analyzed from 35 separate experimental sessions during which the animals vocalized during at least one stimulus block, excluding the first. The frogs responded more often to playbacks at 80 db SPL than at 70 db SPL; however, the pattern of response (relative responding across stimuli) was similar across playback intensity, so data presented here are summed across intensity. The frogs tended to respond most consistently in response to playbacks of the 300 SNE stimulus, vocalizing to this stimulus on about 90% of the test sessions, as compared to the 50 SNE stimulus, to which responding occurred in only about 50% of the test sessions. Differences in the proportion of test sessions during which the frogs vocalized as a func- A ~, I loo 150 l - o 50 0 B, (J ]Harmonic A ~ 200 o ~ 150 ~ 1 O0 ~S 5o B ~" 60 o l"- C ~ 20 0 C 1.0 0,8,.- 0,6 ~ ~ 0,2 0,0 Noise!j!, )ectrum Level (db RMS/Hz) Fig. 1A-C. Influence of harmonic structure on evoked vocal responses. Data are means and standard deviations of responses to the sum of the 2 harmonic and the sum of the 2 inharmonic stimuli across different background noise levels. A Number of vocal responses. B Latency to first vocal response. C Vector strength of response o ~ -~ ~ 40 ~ c- o o t- ~.. m ~ e 20 _j rl I % ~ 0.8 o ~ 0.6 ~ 0.4 ~ ~ g g g Fig. 2A-C. Influence of phase structure on evoked vocal responses. Data are means (:k standard deviation) averaged over individual frogs and displayed over stimulus type. Stimuli differ in both waveform periodicity (50 and 300 groups) and in phase structure (SNE, ALT and RND groups). A Number of vocal responses. B Latency to first response. C Vector strength of response

5 A.M. Simmons et al.: Sound perception by treefrogs 325 tion of type of stimulus were, significantly different as a function of waveform periodicity [50 Hz vs 300 Hz groups of stimuli; Z~(5)= 11, P<0.05]. There were no significant differences within the 50Hz and 300 Hz groups according to the phase characteristics of the different stimuli. There were no significant differences among individual frogs in the pattern of responding. The evoked calling responses of the males were influenced by the waveform periodicity (50 or 300 Hz) rather than phase structure (SNE, ALT, RND variations) of the stimuli (Fig. 2). As shown in Fig. 2A, the frogs vocalized more in response to playbacks of stimuli modulated at 300 Hz than at 50 Hz. Statistical analyses revealed significant effects by stimuli [Z 2 (5)= 32.9, P < 0.001] attributable to differences between the 50 Hz and 300 Hz groups of stimuli. There were no differences in number of responses among the 3 types of 50 Hz modulated stimuli [Z 2 (2)= 1.95, P= 0.38] or among the 3 different types of 300 Hz stimuli [Z 2 (2)= 0.47, P = 0.06]. Latency data were included only for those testing sessions in which an animal responded to a particular stimulus. There were no significant differences in latency to respond across the different stimuli [Z2(5)=9.1, P=0.10; Fig. 2B]. The data in Fig. 2C show that when the frogs responded to a stimulus, they tended to respond consistently at a particular time within the interstimulus interval (high VS). Synchronization was significantly greater in response to the 300 Hz stimuli than to the 50 Hz stimuli [Z~z(5)=12.4, P< 0.05]. There were no differences in synchronization of responding that could be reliably attributed to differences in the phase structure of the stimuli (in the 50 Hz group of stimuli: Z2(2)=0.93, P=0.63; in the 300 Hz group of stimuli: Z2(2)=5.5, e = 0.06]. Discussion The two main signals in the male green treefrog's repertoire, the advertisement call and the aggressive call, differ in both their waveform periodicity (shape of the amplitude-time envelope) and in their fine-structure. These differences come about from differences in modulation rate superimposed on the complex waveform, which produce changes in waveform fine-structure from the extra sidebands produced by the modulation process. Both female (Gerhardt 1978) and male (Allan and Simmons 1993) green treefrogs perceive these differences in modulation rate and waveform periodicity. The present data (Fig. 2) confirm those of Allan and Simmons (1993) in showing that differences in waveform periodicity can significantly influence evoked calling behavior in the male green treefrog. In these experiments, we found no significant differences in male calling responses that could be attributed to differences in the fine-structure of the signal, whether expressed as shifts in harmonic structure (Fig. 1) or as shifts in phase structure (Fig. 2). Changing the harmonic relationship between components in a two-tone signal to an inharmonic relation (no stable first harmonic periodicity) did not significantly influence male responses to these signals when presented against background noise. When the starting phases of consecutive harmonics in a 12-component signal were shifted, the vocal responses of the males did not differ significantly with these changes in fine-structure. Previous research has dealt with the issue of perception of fine-structure in these animals, with mixed results. Gerhardt (1978) showed that female green treefrogs prefer a three-component synthetic call with a 300 Hz periodicity over a two-component synthetic call with the same periodicity. The differences in the fine-structure of these signals were reflected in the shape of the waveform envelope, and Gerhardt argued that it was the shape of the envelope, rather than the fine-structure difference itself, that motivated the females' responses. Consistent with this explanation, Gerhardt et al. (1990) found no preferences for harmonic over inharmonic signals when these changes in harmonic structure did not also produce obvious changes in the shape of the waveform envelope. The data presented here suggest that differences in harmonic structure, when individual frequency components remain within the same critical bands as in the harmonic situation, do not produce significant differences in evoked vocal responses. They suggest that the males' responses were guided by the amplitude-time envelope (waveform shape) and periodicity of the signals. The shifts in the envelope of one of the inharmonic stimuli were apparently not salient enough to affect vocal behavior when presented against background noise. The gross temporal features of the waveform thus appeared to have perceptually overshadowed any changes in waveform fine-structure. The differences in the salience of harmonic structure as a perceptual cue as now noted in different experiments may be due to several factors. First, these experiments all use different techniques with different underlying assumptions. Neither the study by Gerhardt et al. (1990), based on a selective phonotaxis response by females, nor the present study, based on an evoked calling response by males, can necessarily be used to answer questions about sound detection." if an animal tested using either of these two techniques does not respond to a particular stimulus, or does not seemingly differentiate between two stimuli, we cannot with certainty conclude that the animal does not detect differences between these stimuli. The animal's behavior suggests a lower or more inclusive criterion for responding in a communication situation than in a psychophysical situation. Second, both the study by Gerhardt et al. (1990) and the present study used playback levels above threshold in order to reliably motivate responding, while the study by Simmons (1988) was concerned with differences at threshold. We were unable to elicit evoked vocal responding at the lower playback levels where the psychophysical differences were found. Third, the negative results both of the present study and of the study by Gerhardt et al. (1990) may have been influenced by the presence of other acoustic features besides those being studied. Specifically, in these experiments, the harmonic and inharmonic stimuli were of the same duration as natural calls and were

6 326 A.M. Simmons et al. : Sound perception by treefrogs presented in series at species-typical calling rates. In the psychophysical experiment, stimuli were presented singly and aperiodically, at interstimulus intervals ranging from 20 to 40 s, well outside the species-typical calling rate. Call rate is an important acoustic feature for green treefrogs and other anurans (Forester and Czanrowsky 1985; Gerhardt 1988; Klump and Gerhardt 1987; Schwartz 1986; Wells 1988). It may be a salient enough cue to override the subtler differences in the signals tested in these studies. Indeed, Allan and Simmons (1993) observed that male green treefrogs will vocalize in response to unmodulated noise bursts presented at a species-typical calling rate and duration. Fourth, the psychophysical results were strongly influenced by individual differences among animals. The inhibition level functions presented by Simmons (1988; Fig. 2) differed in their slope both across animals and across stimuli, and the actual magnitude of the inhibition effects, although statistically significant, was small and also differed across individual animals. Neither the study by Gerhardt et al. (1990) nor the present study observed any reliable individual differences among the animals being tested. Other evidence relevant to the issue of the importance ofwaveform fine-structure is available. The vocalizations of individual male green treefrogs, whether recorded in the natural environment or under laboratory conditions, are relatively stereotyped within individuals but can vary between individuals even within the same geographic area (Gerhardt, personal communication; Allan, personal communication). For example, the dominant lowfrequency spectral peak in the advertisement call of 29 individual males at the same calling site was found to vary from around 700 to 1160 Hz (Gerhardt et al. 1987), and males often produce signals that are aperiodic (Gerhardt et al. 1990). These differences in fine-structure cues are potentially available for recognition of specific individual frogs, while envelope cues might be more stereotyped and useful for species and call recognition. How any of these acoustic features influence mating success is uncertain (Gerhardt et al. 1987). Moreover, fine-structure cues may be seriously degraded by acoustic interference in natural choruses (Schwartz 1987). These observations argue against specific reliance on fine-structure cues in natural choruses, particularly on the part of male frogs where such fine discriminations may not confer any reproductive advantage. In a series of studies on the coding of the conspecific advertisement call by auditory nerve fibers in the bullfrog, Schwartz and Simmons (1990) and Simmons et al. (1992, 1993) showed that acoustic features mediating pitch and timbre perception in humans are reflected in temporal responses of the anuran eighth nerve. Specifically, at sound intensities of 70 to 90 db SPL, most fibers, regardless of their frequency tuning, preferentially synchronize to the waveform periodicity (amplitude envelope) of the synthetic call. At high intensities, or at low signal-to-noise ratios (high levels of background noise), many fibers shift their synchronization away from the waveform periodicity to a low-frequency spectral peak in the stimulus. This means that the coding strategy changes from one of envelope detection to one of fine-structure detection. Changing the phase relations of harmonics within the signal also affects fiber response most consistently at high sound levels. Simmons and Ferragamo (1993) further showed that bullfrog eighth nerve fibers are sensitive to the harmonic structure of complex sounds. Shifts in fine-structure related to shifts in the harmonic relationship between frequency components are evident in fiber response to signals with lowfrequency components. These data suggest that, at least in the bullfrog, phase and harmonic structure are coded by the eighth nerve, and are potentially available for central nervous system processing. How this neural code affects sound perception and communication behavior is still uncertain but worthy of further study. Acknowledgements. This research was supported by NIH grant NS28565 to AMS. We thank Susan Allan for providing the fieldrecorded treefrog vocalizations, and Yan Shen for programming assistance. References Allan SE, Simmons AM (1993) Temporal features mediating call recognition in the green treefrog, Hyla cinerea: Amplitude modulation. Anim Behav (in press) Batschelet E (1981) Circular statistics in biology. Academic Press, London Bilsen FA (1973) On the influence of the number and phase of harmonics on the perceptibility of the pitch of complex signals. Acustica 28 : Boer E de (1956) Pitch of inharmonic signals. Nature 178: Forester DM, Czarnowsky R (1985) Sexual selection in the spring peeper, Hyla erucifer (Amphibia, Anura) : role of the advertisement call. Behaviour 92: Gerhardt HC (1978) Mating call recognition in the green treefrog (Hyla einerea) : The significance of some fine-temporal properties. J Exp Biol 74:59-73 Gerhardt H C (1981) Mating call recognition in the barking treefrog (Hyla gratiosa) : responses to synthetic mating calls and comparisons with the green treefrog (Hyla cinerea). J Comp Physiol 144:17-25 Gerhardt HC (1987) Evolutionary and neurobiological implications of selective phonotaxis in the green treefrog, Hyla einerea. Anim Behav 35: Gerhardt HC (1988) Acoustic properties used in call recognition by frogs and toads. In: Fritzsch B, Ryan M J, Wilczynski W, Hetherington TE, Walkowiak W (eds) The evolution of the amphibian auditory system. Wiley, New York, pp Gerhardt HC, Daniel RE, Perrill SA, Schramm S (1987) Mating behaviour and male mating success in the green treefrog. Anim Behav 35 : Gerhardt HC, Allan S, Schwartz J (1990) Female green treefrogs (Hyla cinerea) do not selectively respond to signals with a harmonic structure in noise. J Comp Physiol A 166: Klump GM, Gerhardt HC (1987) Use of non-arbitrary acoustic criteria in mate choice by female gray treefrogs. Nature 326 : Lundeen C, Small AM (1984) The influence of temporal cues on the strength of periodicity pitches. J Acoust Soc Am 75: Moore BCJ (1977) Effects of relative phase of the components on the pitch of three-component complex tones. In: Evans EF, Wilson JP (eds) Psychophysics and physiology of hearing. Academic Press, New York, pp Moore BCJ (1989) An introduction to the psychology of hearing. Academic Press, New York

7 A.M. Simmons et al. : Sound perception by treefrogs 327 Moss CF, Simmons AM (1986) Frequency selectivity of hearing in the green treefrog (Hyla cinerea). J Comp Physiol A 159: Patterson RD (1987) A pulse ribbon model of peripheral auditory processing. In: Yost WA, Watson CS (eds) Auditory processing of complex sounds. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale New Jersey, pp Plomp R (1976) Aspects of tone sensation. Academic~Press, New York Pumplin J (1985) Low-noise noise. J Acoust Soc Am 88: Ritsma R, Engel F (1964) Pitch of frequency-modulated signals. J Acoust Soc Am 34: Rose G J, Capranica RR (1985) Sensitivity to amplitude modulated sounds in the anuran auditory nervous system. J Neurophysiol 53 : 446M65 Schouten JF (1970) The residue revisited. In: Plomp R, Smoorenburg GF (eds) Frequency analysis and periodicity detection in hearing. Sijthoff, Leiden, pp Schouten JF, Ritsma RJ, Cardozo BL (1962) Pitch of the residue. J Acoust Soc Am 34: Schwartz JJ (1986) Male calling behavior and female choice in the neotropical treefrog Hyla microcephala. Ethology 73: Schwartz JJ (1987) The function of call alternation in anuran amphibians: a test of three hypotheses. Evolution 41: Schwartz J J, Simmons AM (1990) Encoding of a spectrally-complex communication sound in the bullfrog's auditory nerve. J Comp Physiol A 166: Simmons AM (1988) Selectivity for harmonic structure in complex sounds by the green treefrog (Hyla cinerea). J Comp Physiol A 162: Simmons AM, Ferragamo M (1993) Periodicity extraction in the anuran auditory nerve. I. "Pitch-shift" effects. J Comp Physiol A 172:57-69 Simmons AM, Schwartz JJ, Ferragamo M (1992) Auditory nerve representation of a complex communication sound in background noise. J Acoust Soc Am 91: Simmons AM, Reese G, Ferragamo M (1993) Periodicity extraction in the anuran auditory nerve. II. Phase and temporal fine-structure. J Acoust Soc Am 93: Straughan IR (1975) An analysis of the mechanisms of mating call discrimination in the frogs Hyla regilla and H. cadaverina. Copeia 1975 : Sullivan BK (1983) Sexual selection in Woodhouse's toad (Bufo woodhousei). II. Female choice. Anim Behav 31 : Wells KD (1988) The effect of social interactions on anuran vocal behavior. In: Fritzsch B, Ryan M J, Wilczynski W, Hetherington TE, Walkowiak W (eds) The evolution of the amphibian auditory system. Wiley, New York, pp

Spatially mediated release from auditory masking in an anuran amphibian

Spatially mediated release from auditory masking in an anuran amphibian J Comp Physiol A (1989) 166:37~41,,lelmlJ ef Senm~ry, eural, 9 Springer-Verlag 1989 Spatially mediated release from auditory masking in an anuran amphibian Joshua J. Schwartz and H. Carl Gerhardt* Department

More information

HERPETOLOGICA THRESHOLDS FOR EVOKED VOCAL RESPONSES OF EUPSOPHUS EMILIOPUGINI (AMPHIBIA, LEPTODACTYLIDAE)

HERPETOLOGICA THRESHOLDS FOR EVOKED VOCAL RESPONSES OF EUPSOPHUS EMILIOPUGINI (AMPHIBIA, LEPTODACTYLIDAE) HERPETOLOGICA VOL. 61 MARCH 2005 NO. 1 Herpetologica, 61(1), 2005, 1 8 Ó 2005 by The Herpetologists League, Inc. THRESHOLDS FOR EVOKED VOCAL RESPONSES OF EUPSOPHUS EMILIOPUGINI (AMPHIBIA, LEPTODACTYLIDAE)

More information

Jitter, Shimmer, and Noise in Pathological Voice Quality Perception

Jitter, Shimmer, and Noise in Pathological Voice Quality Perception ISCA Archive VOQUAL'03, Geneva, August 27-29, 2003 Jitter, Shimmer, and Noise in Pathological Voice Quality Perception Jody Kreiman and Bruce R. Gerratt Division of Head and Neck Surgery, School of Medicine

More information

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA) Comments Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA) Is phase locking to transposed stimuli as good as phase locking to low-frequency

More information

TESTS FOR CALL RESTORATION IN THE GRAY TREEFROG HYLA VERSICOLOR

TESTS FOR CALL RESTORATION IN THE GRAY TREEFROG HYLA VERSICOLOR Bioacoustics The International Journal of Animal Sound and its Recording, 2010, Vol. 20, pp. 59 86 2010 AB Academic Publishers 59 TESTS FOR CALL RESTORATION IN THE GRAY TREEFROG HYLA VERSICOLOR JOSHUA

More information

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions Binaural Hearing Why two ears? Locating sounds in space: acuity is poorer than in vision by up to two orders of magnitude, but extends in all directions. Role in alerting and orienting? Separating sound

More information

Sound localization psychophysics

Sound localization psychophysics Sound localization psychophysics Eric Young A good reference: B.C.J. Moore An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing Chapter 7, Space Perception. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 233-267 (2004). Sound localization:

More information

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear Pitch & Binaural listening Review 25 20 15 10 5 0-5 100 1000 10000 25 20 15 10 5 0-5 100 1000 10000 Part I: Auditory frequency selectivity Tuning

More information

Discrimination of temporal fine structure by birds and mammals

Discrimination of temporal fine structure by birds and mammals Auditory Signal Processing: Physiology, Psychoacoustics, and Models. Pressnitzer, D., de Cheveigné, A., McAdams, S.,and Collet, L. (Eds). Springer Verlag, 24. Discrimination of temporal fine structure

More information

Responses of male cricket frogs (Acris crepitans) to attenuated and degraded advertisement calls

Responses of male cricket frogs (Acris crepitans) to attenuated and degraded advertisement calls Received: 18 November 2016 Accepted: 14 February 2017 DOI: 10.1111/eth.12602 Responses of male cricket frogs (Acris crepitans) to attenuated and degraded advertisement calls Kurt R. Venator 1 Michael J.

More information

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080 Perceptual segregation of a harmonic from a vowel by interaural time difference in conjunction with mistuning and onset asynchrony C. J. Darwin and R. W. Hukin Experimental Psychology, University of Sussex,

More information

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Volume 19, 2013 http://acousticalsociety.org/ ICA 2013 Montreal Montreal, Canada 2-7 June 2013 Speech Communication Session 4aSCb: Voice and F0 Across Tasks (Poster

More information

Female preferences for temporal order of call components in the túngara frog: a Bayesian analysis

Female preferences for temporal order of call components in the túngara frog: a Bayesian analysis ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1999, 58, 841 851 Article No. anbe.1999.1208, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Female preferences for temporal order of call components in the túngara frog: a Bayesian

More information

Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005

Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu 24.963 Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms. 24.963 Linguistic Phonetics

More information

Perceptual pitch shift for sounds with similar waveform autocorrelation

Perceptual pitch shift for sounds with similar waveform autocorrelation Pressnitzer et al.: Acoustics Research Letters Online [DOI./.4667] Published Online 4 October Perceptual pitch shift for sounds with similar waveform autocorrelation Daniel Pressnitzer, Alain de Cheveigné

More information

Auditory nerve representation of a complex communication sound in background noise

Auditory nerve representation of a complex communication sound in background noise Auditory nerve representation of a complex communication sound in background noise Andrea Megela Simmons, Joshua J. Schwartz, ") and Michael Ferragamo Department of Psychology and Graduate Program in Neuroscience,

More information

MATERIALS AND METHODS

MATERIALS AND METHODS 48 M. J. RYAN amount used to produce the signal. However, because of the difficulty in determining energy expenditures associated with acoustic signalling, this comparison has only been made with insects

More information

Role of F0 differences in source segregation

Role of F0 differences in source segregation Role of F0 differences in source segregation Andrew J. Oxenham Research Laboratory of Electronics, MIT and Harvard-MIT Speech and Hearing Bioscience and Technology Program Rationale Many aspects of segregation

More information

Linguistic Phonetics. Basic Audition. Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions.

Linguistic Phonetics. Basic Audition. Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions. 24.963 Linguistic Phonetics Basic Audition Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions. 1 Reading: Keating 1985 24.963 also read Flemming 2001 Assignment 1 - basic acoustics. Due 9/22.

More information

An Auditory-Model-Based Electrical Stimulation Strategy Incorporating Tonal Information for Cochlear Implant

An Auditory-Model-Based Electrical Stimulation Strategy Incorporating Tonal Information for Cochlear Implant Annual Progress Report An Auditory-Model-Based Electrical Stimulation Strategy Incorporating Tonal Information for Cochlear Implant Joint Research Centre for Biomedical Engineering Mar.7, 26 Types of Hearing

More information

The development of a modified spectral ripple test

The development of a modified spectral ripple test The development of a modified spectral ripple test Justin M. Aronoff a) and David M. Landsberger Communication and Neuroscience Division, House Research Institute, 2100 West 3rd Street, Los Angeles, California

More information

Effect of anomalous pulse timing on call discrimination by females of the gray treefrog (Hyla versicolor): behavioral correlates of neurobiology

Effect of anomalous pulse timing on call discrimination by females of the gray treefrog (Hyla versicolor): behavioral correlates of neurobiology 2066 The Journal of Experimental Biology 213, 2066-2072 2010. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd doi:10.1242/jeb.043372 Effect of anomalous pulse timing on call discrimination by females of the

More information

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

HCS 7367 Speech Perception Long-term spectrum of speech HCS 7367 Speech Perception Connected speech Absolute threshold Males Dr. Peter Assmann Fall 212 Females Long-term spectrum of speech Vowels Males Females 2) Absolute threshold

More information

Computational Perception /785. Auditory Scene Analysis

Computational Perception /785. Auditory Scene Analysis Computational Perception 15-485/785 Auditory Scene Analysis A framework for auditory scene analysis Auditory scene analysis involves low and high level cues Low level acoustic cues are often result in

More information

Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation

Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation Neural correlates of the perception of sound source separation Mitchell L. Day 1,2 * and Bertrand Delgutte 1,2,3 1 Department of Otology and Laryngology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, USA.

More information

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES Varinthira Duangudom and David V Anderson School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Georgia Institute of Technology Atlanta, GA 30332

More information

Evoked Vocal Responses Change with Experience in Male Physalaemus pustulosus

Evoked Vocal Responses Change with Experience in Male Physalaemus pustulosus Evoked Vocal Responses with Experience in Male Physalaemus pustulosus Author(s): Beth Dawson and Michael J. Ryan Source: Copeia, 2012(4):678-682. 2012. Published By: The American Society of Ichthyologists

More information

Topics in Linguistic Theory: Laboratory Phonology Spring 2007

Topics in Linguistic Theory: Laboratory Phonology Spring 2007 MIT OpenCourseWare http://ocw.mit.edu 24.91 Topics in Linguistic Theory: Laboratory Phonology Spring 27 For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.

More information

Essential feature. Who are cochlear implants for? People with little or no hearing. substitute for faulty or missing inner hair

Essential feature. Who are cochlear implants for? People with little or no hearing. substitute for faulty or missing inner hair Who are cochlear implants for? Essential feature People with little or no hearing and little conductive component to the loss who receive little or no benefit from a hearing aid. Implants seem to work

More information

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100 (4), Pt. 1, October /96/100(4)/2352/13/$ Acoustical Society of America 2352

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100 (4), Pt. 1, October /96/100(4)/2352/13/$ Acoustical Society of America 2352 Lateralization of a perturbed harmonic: Effects of onset asynchrony and mistuning a) Nicholas I. Hill and C. J. Darwin Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QG, United

More information

Hearing the Universal Language: Music and Cochlear Implants

Hearing the Universal Language: Music and Cochlear Implants Hearing the Universal Language: Music and Cochlear Implants Professor Hugh McDermott Deputy Director (Research) The Bionics Institute of Australia, Professorial Fellow The University of Melbourne Overview?

More information

Lecture 3: Perception

Lecture 3: Perception ELEN E4896 MUSIC SIGNAL PROCESSING Lecture 3: Perception 1. Ear Physiology 2. Auditory Psychophysics 3. Pitch Perception 4. Music Perception Dan Ellis Dept. Electrical Engineering, Columbia University

More information

Who are cochlear implants for?

Who are cochlear implants for? Who are cochlear implants for? People with little or no hearing and little conductive component to the loss who receive little or no benefit from a hearing aid. Implants seem to work best in adults who

More information

Interference Risk and the Function of Dynamic Shifts in Calling in the Gray Treefrog (Hyla versicolor)

Interference Risk and the Function of Dynamic Shifts in Calling in the Gray Treefrog (Hyla versicolor) Journal of Comparative Psychology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 2008, Vol. 122, No. 3, 283 288 0735-7036/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.122.3.283 Interference Risk and the

More information

What you re in for. Who are cochlear implants for? The bottom line. Speech processing schemes for

What you re in for. Who are cochlear implants for? The bottom line. Speech processing schemes for What you re in for Speech processing schemes for cochlear implants Stuart Rosen Professor of Speech and Hearing Science Speech, Hearing and Phonetic Sciences Division of Psychology & Language Sciences

More information

Speech (Sound) Processing

Speech (Sound) Processing 7 Speech (Sound) Processing Acoustic Human communication is achieved when thought is transformed through language into speech. The sounds of speech are initiated by activity in the central nervous system,

More information

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean?

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean? 3-D Sound and Spatial Audio What do these terms mean? Both terms are very general. 3-D sound usually implies the perception of point sources in 3-D space (could also be 2-D plane) whether the audio reproduction

More information

whether or not the fundamental is actually present.

whether or not the fundamental is actually present. 1) Which of the following uses a computer CPU to combine various pure tones to generate interesting sounds or music? 1) _ A) MIDI standard. B) colored-noise generator, C) white-noise generator, D) digital

More information

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17 Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17 related lectures: Applied and Virtual Acoustics (Winter Term) Advanced Psychoacoustics (Summer Term) Sound Perception 2 Frequency and Level Range of Human Hearing Source:

More information

Hearing Lectures. Acoustics of Speech and Hearing. Auditory Lighthouse. Facts about Timbre. Analysis of Complex Sounds

Hearing Lectures. Acoustics of Speech and Hearing. Auditory Lighthouse. Facts about Timbre. Analysis of Complex Sounds Hearing Lectures Acoustics of Speech and Hearing Week 2-10 Hearing 3: Auditory Filtering 1. Loudness of sinusoids mainly (see Web tutorial for more) 2. Pitch of sinusoids mainly (see Web tutorial for more)

More information

On the influence of interaural differences on onset detection in auditory object formation. 1 Introduction

On the influence of interaural differences on onset detection in auditory object formation. 1 Introduction On the influence of interaural differences on onset detection in auditory object formation Othmar Schimmel Eindhoven University of Technology, P.O. Box 513 / Building IPO 1.26, 56 MD Eindhoven, The Netherlands,

More information

Animal Behaviour 79 (2010) 1317e1328. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage:

Animal Behaviour 79 (2010) 1317e1328. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage: Animal Behaviour 79 (21) 1317e1328 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav Testing an auditory illusion in frogs: perceptual restoration

More information

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization Acoust. Sci. & Tech. 24, 2 (23) PAPER The role of low components in median plane localization Masayuki Morimoto 1;, Motoki Yairi 1, Kazuhiro Iida 2 and Motokuni Itoh 1 1 Environmental Acoustics Laboratory,

More information

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS 19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 27 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS PACS: 43.66.Pn Seeber, Bernhard U. Auditory Perception Lab, Dept.

More information

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency. (Some slides are adapted from Zhiyao Duan s course slides on Computer Audition and Its Applications in Music)

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency. (Some slides are adapted from Zhiyao Duan s course slides on Computer Audition and Its Applications in Music) Topic 4 Pitch & Frequency (Some slides are adapted from Zhiyao Duan s course slides on Computer Audition and Its Applications in Music) A musical interlude KOMBU This solo by Kaigal-ool of Huun-Huur-Tu

More information

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception Overview of Questions What is it that makes sounds high pitched or low pitched? How do sound vibrations inside the ear lead to the perception

More information

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion Perception & Psychophysics 1980.28 (5). 465-470 Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion GREGORY M. CORSO Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia 30332 A pair of pure-tone sine

More information

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Volume 19, 2013 http://acousticalsociety.org/ ICA 2013 Montreal Montreal, Canada 2-7 June 2013 Noise Session 3aNSa: Wind Turbine Noise I 3aNSa5. Can wind turbine sound

More information

Auditory Scene Analysis

Auditory Scene Analysis 1 Auditory Scene Analysis Albert S. Bregman Department of Psychology McGill University 1205 Docteur Penfield Avenue Montreal, QC Canada H3A 1B1 E-mail: bregman@hebb.psych.mcgill.ca To appear in N.J. Smelzer

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors

The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors PAPER The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors Waka Fujisaki 1;2;* and Makio Kashino 2; { 1 Graduate School of Humanities and Sciences, Ochanomizu University, 2 1 1 Ootsuka, Bunkyo-ku,

More information

Separate What and Where Decision Mechanisms In Processing a Dichotic Tonal Sequence

Separate What and Where Decision Mechanisms In Processing a Dichotic Tonal Sequence Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1976, Vol. 2, No. 1, 23-29 Separate What and Where Decision Mechanisms In Processing a Dichotic Tonal Sequence Diana Deutsch and Philip

More information

HST.723J, Spring 2005 Theme 3 Report

HST.723J, Spring 2005 Theme 3 Report HST.723J, Spring 2005 Theme 3 Report Madhu Shashanka shashanka@cns.bu.edu Introduction The theme of this report is binaural interactions. Binaural interactions of sound stimuli enable humans (and other

More information

Representation of sound in the auditory nerve

Representation of sound in the auditory nerve Representation of sound in the auditory nerve Eric D. Young Department of Biomedical Engineering Johns Hopkins University Young, ED. Neural representation of spectral and temporal information in speech.

More information

Sound Texture Classification Using Statistics from an Auditory Model

Sound Texture Classification Using Statistics from an Auditory Model Sound Texture Classification Using Statistics from an Auditory Model Gabriele Carotti-Sha Evan Penn Daniel Villamizar Electrical Engineering Email: gcarotti@stanford.edu Mangement Science & Engineering

More information

Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, Auditory system. http:

Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, Auditory system. http: Systems Neuroscience Oct. 16, 2018 Auditory system http: www.ini.unizh.ch/~kiper/system_neurosci.html The physics of sound Measuring sound intensity We are sensitive to an enormous range of intensities,

More information

Behavioral measures of signal recognition thresholds in frogs in

Behavioral measures of signal recognition thresholds in frogs in Behavioral measures of signal recognition thresholds in frogs in the presence and absence of chorus-shaped noise Mark A. Bee a Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Behavior, University of Minnesota, 100

More information

Prelude Envelope and temporal fine. What's all the fuss? Modulating a wave. Decomposing waveforms. The psychophysics of cochlear

Prelude Envelope and temporal fine. What's all the fuss? Modulating a wave. Decomposing waveforms. The psychophysics of cochlear The psychophysics of cochlear implants Stuart Rosen Professor of Speech and Hearing Science Speech, Hearing and Phonetic Sciences Division of Psychology & Language Sciences Prelude Envelope and temporal

More information

Auditory Perception: Sense of Sound /785 Spring 2017

Auditory Perception: Sense of Sound /785 Spring 2017 Auditory Perception: Sense of Sound 85-385/785 Spring 2017 Professor: Laurie Heller Classroom: Baker Hall 342F (sometimes Cluster 332P) Time: Tuesdays and Thursdays 1:30-2:50 Office hour: Thursday 3:00-4:00,

More information

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency Topic 4 Pitch & Frequency A musical interlude KOMBU This solo by Kaigal-ool of Huun-Huur-Tu (accompanying himself on doshpuluur) demonstrates perfectly the characteristic sound of the Xorekteer voice An

More information

New Approaches to Studying Auditory Processing in Marine Mammals

New Approaches to Studying Auditory Processing in Marine Mammals DISTRIBUTION STATEMENT A. Approved for public release; distribution is unlimited. New Approaches to Studying Auditory Processing in Marine Mammals James J. Finneran Space and Naval Warfare Systems Center

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Auditory Phase Opponency: A Temporal Model for Masked Detection at Low Frequencies

Auditory Phase Opponency: A Temporal Model for Masked Detection at Low Frequencies ACTA ACUSTICA UNITED WITH ACUSTICA Vol. 88 (22) 334 347 Scientific Papers Auditory Phase Opponency: A Temporal Model for Masked Detection at Low Frequencies Laurel H. Carney, Michael G. Heinz, Mary E.

More information

EFFECTS OF TEMPORAL FINE STRUCTURE ON THE LOCALIZATION OF BROADBAND SOUNDS: POTENTIAL IMPLICATIONS FOR THE DESIGN OF SPATIAL AUDIO DISPLAYS

EFFECTS OF TEMPORAL FINE STRUCTURE ON THE LOCALIZATION OF BROADBAND SOUNDS: POTENTIAL IMPLICATIONS FOR THE DESIGN OF SPATIAL AUDIO DISPLAYS Proceedings of the 14 International Conference on Auditory Display, Paris, France June 24-27, 28 EFFECTS OF TEMPORAL FINE STRUCTURE ON THE LOCALIZATION OF BROADBAND SOUNDS: POTENTIAL IMPLICATIONS FOR THE

More information

Female preferences for socially variable call characters in the cricket frog, Acris crepitans

Female preferences for socially variable call characters in the cricket frog, Acris crepitans ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2004, 68, 1391 1399 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.01.020 Female preferences for socially variable call characters in the cricket frog, Acris crepitans NICOLE M. KIME*, SABRINA S. BURMEISTER

More information

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION Karin Zimmer, R. Duncan Luce and Wolfgang Ellermeier Institut für Kognitionsforschung der Universität Oldenburg, Germany Institute

More information

AUTOCORRELATION AND CROSS-CORRELARION ANALYSES OF ALPHA WAVES IN RELATION TO SUBJECTIVE PREFERENCE OF A FLICKERING LIGHT

AUTOCORRELATION AND CROSS-CORRELARION ANALYSES OF ALPHA WAVES IN RELATION TO SUBJECTIVE PREFERENCE OF A FLICKERING LIGHT AUTOCORRELATION AND CROSS-CORRELARION ANALYSES OF ALPHA WAVES IN RELATION TO SUBJECTIVE PREFERENCE OF A FLICKERING LIGHT Y. Soeta, S. Uetani, and Y. Ando Graduate School of Science and Technology, Kobe

More information

INTRODUCTION. Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA Electronic mail:

INTRODUCTION. Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA Electronic mail: Level discrimination of sinusoids as a function of duration and level for fixed-level, roving-level, and across-frequency conditions Andrew J. Oxenham a) Institute for Hearing, Speech, and Language, and

More information

Essential feature. Who are cochlear implants for? People with little or no hearing. substitute for faulty or missing inner hair

Essential feature. Who are cochlear implants for? People with little or no hearing. substitute for faulty or missing inner hair Who are cochlear implants for? Essential feature People with little or no hearing and little conductive component to the loss who receive little or no benefit from a hearing aid. Implants seem to work

More information

VOCAL COMMUNICATION IN A NEOTROPICAL TREEFROG,

VOCAL COMMUNICATION IN A NEOTROPICAL TREEFROG, Anim. Behav., 1984, 32, 405-420 VOCAL COMMUNICATION IN A NEOTROPICAL TREEFROG, HYLA EBRACCATA : ADVERTISEMENT CALLS BY KENTWOOD D. WELLS & JOSHUA J. SCHWARTZ Biological Sciences Group, The University of

More information

Spectrograms (revisited)

Spectrograms (revisited) Spectrograms (revisited) We begin the lecture by reviewing the units of spectrograms, which I had only glossed over when I covered spectrograms at the end of lecture 19. We then relate the blocks of a

More information

Chapter 40 Effects of Peripheral Tuning on the Auditory Nerve s Representation of Speech Envelope and Temporal Fine Structure Cues

Chapter 40 Effects of Peripheral Tuning on the Auditory Nerve s Representation of Speech Envelope and Temporal Fine Structure Cues Chapter 40 Effects of Peripheral Tuning on the Auditory Nerve s Representation of Speech Envelope and Temporal Fine Structure Cues Rasha A. Ibrahim and Ian C. Bruce Abstract A number of studies have explored

More information

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 30. Organ of Corti

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 30. Organ of Corti Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 30 Waves, waves, waves. Organ of Corti Tectorial membrane Sits on top Inner hair cells Outer hair cells The microphone for the brain 1 Hearing Perceptually,

More information

The Structure and Function of the Auditory Nerve

The Structure and Function of the Auditory Nerve The Structure and Function of the Auditory Nerve Brad May Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (BME 580.626) September 21, 2010 1 Objectives Anatomy Basic response patterns Frequency

More information

Hearing. Figure 1. The human ear (from Kessel and Kardon, 1979)

Hearing. Figure 1. The human ear (from Kessel and Kardon, 1979) Hearing The nervous system s cognitive response to sound stimuli is known as psychoacoustics: it is partly acoustics and partly psychology. Hearing is a feature resulting from our physiology that we tend

More information

Abstract. 1 Introduction

Abstract. 1 Introduction An optimal high-passfilteringtechnique to improve the detectability of evoked otoacoustic emissions G. Tognola, P. Ravazzani, F. Grandori System Theory Centre - CNR, Polytechnic of Milan, Abstract Evoked

More information

Binaural interference and auditory grouping

Binaural interference and auditory grouping Binaural interference and auditory grouping Virginia Best and Frederick J. Gallun Hearing Research Center, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts 02215 Simon Carlile Department of Physiology, University

More information

THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING

THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING Vanessa Surowiecki 1, vid Grayden 1, Richard Dowell

More information

Ultrasonic frogs show extraordinary sex differences in. auditory frequency sensitivity

Ultrasonic frogs show extraordinary sex differences in. auditory frequency sensitivity Ultrasonic frogs show extraordinary sex differences in auditory frequency sensitivity Jun-Xian Shen 1, Zhi-Min Xu 1, Zu-Lin Yu 1, Shuai Wang 2 & De-Zhi Zheng 2 1 State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive

More information

Hearing. and other senses

Hearing. and other senses Hearing and other senses Sound Sound: sensed variations in air pressure Frequency: number of peaks that pass a point per second (Hz) Pitch 2 Some Sound and Hearing Links Useful (and moderately entertaining)

More information

The role of periodicity in the perception of masked speech with simulated and real cochlear implants

The role of periodicity in the perception of masked speech with simulated and real cochlear implants The role of periodicity in the perception of masked speech with simulated and real cochlear implants Kurt Steinmetzger and Stuart Rosen UCL Speech, Hearing and Phonetic Sciences Heidelberg, 09. November

More information

ACOUSTIC AND PERCEPTUAL PROPERTIES OF ENGLISH FRICATIVES

ACOUSTIC AND PERCEPTUAL PROPERTIES OF ENGLISH FRICATIVES ISCA Archive ACOUSTIC AND PERCEPTUAL PROPERTIES OF ENGLISH FRICATIVES Allard Jongman 1, Yue Wang 2, and Joan Sereno 1 1 Linguistics Department, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 66045 U.S.A. 2 Department

More information

Most theoretical and empirical work on mate choice has

Most theoretical and empirical work on mate choice has Behavioral Ecology Vol. 13 No. 4: 472 480 Mate sampling by female barking treefrogs (Hyla gratiosa) Christopher G. Murphy a and H. Carl Gerhardt b a Department of Biology, James Madison University, Harrisonburg,

More information

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 27, , June tleseareh Note RELATION BETWEEN REACTION TIME AND LOUDNESS

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 27, , June tleseareh Note RELATION BETWEEN REACTION TIME AND LOUDNESS Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 27, 306-310, June 1984 tleseareh Note RELATION BETWEEN REACTION TIME AND LOUDNESS LARRY E. HUMES JAYNE B. AHLSTROM Vanderbilt University School of Medicine,

More information

Publication VI. c 2007 Audio Engineering Society. Reprinted with permission.

Publication VI. c 2007 Audio Engineering Society. Reprinted with permission. VI Publication VI Hirvonen, T. and Pulkki, V., Predicting Binaural Masking Level Difference and Dichotic Pitch Using Instantaneous ILD Model, AES 30th Int. Conference, 2007. c 2007 Audio Engineering Society.

More information

Central Processing of Communication Sounds in the Anuran Auditory System 1

Central Processing of Communication Sounds in the Anuran Auditory System 1 AMER. ZOOL., 34:670-684 (1994) Central Processing of Communication Sounds in the Anuran Auditory System 1 JIM C. HALL University of Tennessee, Department of Zoology, M313 Walters Life Sciences Building,

More information

The effect of wearing conventional and level-dependent hearing protectors on speech production in noise and quiet

The effect of wearing conventional and level-dependent hearing protectors on speech production in noise and quiet The effect of wearing conventional and level-dependent hearing protectors on speech production in noise and quiet Ghazaleh Vaziri Christian Giguère Hilmi R. Dajani Nicolas Ellaham Annual National Hearing

More information

Chapter 1: Introduction to digital audio

Chapter 1: Introduction to digital audio Chapter 1: Introduction to digital audio Applications: audio players (e.g. MP3), DVD-audio, digital audio broadcast, music synthesizer, digital amplifier and equalizer, 3D sound synthesis 1 Properties

More information

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths Jennifer J. Lentz a Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington,

More information

Spectral processing of two concurrent harmonic complexes

Spectral processing of two concurrent harmonic complexes Spectral processing of two concurrent harmonic complexes Yi Shen a) and Virginia M. Richards Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, California 92697-5100 (Received 7 April

More information

Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults

Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults Daniel Fogerty Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of South Carolina, Columbia, South

More information

2201 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107 (4), April /2000/107(4)/2201/8/$ Acoustical Society of America 2201

2201 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107 (4), April /2000/107(4)/2201/8/$ Acoustical Society of America 2201 Dichotic pitches as illusions of binaural unmasking. III. The existence region of the Fourcin pitch John F. Culling a) University Laboratory of Physiology, Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PT, United Kingdom Received

More information

Effect of musical training on pitch discrimination performance in older normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

Effect of musical training on pitch discrimination performance in older normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners Downloaded from orbit.dtu.dk on: Nov 03, Effect of musical training on pitch discrimination performance in older normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners Bianchi, Federica; Dau, Torsten; Santurette,

More information

FREQUENCY COMPRESSION AND FREQUENCY SHIFTING FOR THE HEARING IMPAIRED

FREQUENCY COMPRESSION AND FREQUENCY SHIFTING FOR THE HEARING IMPAIRED FREQUENCY COMPRESSION AND FREQUENCY SHIFTING FOR THE HEARING IMPAIRED Francisco J. Fraga, Alan M. Marotta National Institute of Telecommunications, Santa Rita do Sapucaí - MG, Brazil Abstract A considerable

More information

Aggressive thresholds in Dendropsophus ebraccatus: habituation and sensitization to different call types

Aggressive thresholds in Dendropsophus ebraccatus: habituation and sensitization to different call types Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:529 539 DOI 10.1007/s00265-009-0868-5 ORIGINAL PAPER Aggressive thresholds in Dendropsophus ebraccatus: habituation and sensitization to different call types Michael S. Reichert

More information

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis Journal of Sound and Vibration Manuscript Draft Manuscript Number: JSV-D-10-00826 Title: Does filled duration illusion occur for very short time intervals? Article Type: Rapid Communication Keywords: time

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Perceptual evaluation of noise reduction in hearing aids Brons, I. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Perceptual evaluation of noise reduction in hearing aids Brons, I. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Perceptual evaluation of noise reduction in hearing aids Brons, I. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Brons, I. (2013). Perceptual evaluation

More information

Loudness Processing of Time-Varying Sounds: Recent advances in psychophysics and challenges for future research

Loudness Processing of Time-Varying Sounds: Recent advances in psychophysics and challenges for future research Loudness Processing of Time-Varying Sounds: Recent advances in psychophysics and challenges for future research Emmanuel PONSOT 1 ; Patrick SUSINI 1 ; Sabine MEUNIER 2 1 STMS lab (Ircam, CNRS, UPMC), 1

More information

Learning to detect a tone in unpredictable noise

Learning to detect a tone in unpredictable noise Learning to detect a tone in unpredictable noise Pete R. Jones and David R. Moore MRC Institute of Hearing Research, University Park, Nottingham NG7 2RD, United Kingdom p.r.jones@ucl.ac.uk, david.moore2@cchmc.org

More information