Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology"

Transcription

1 Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Copy of Notification z2f1029 Your article proofs from Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology (Volume 72, Issue 3 (ms #12)) are ready for your review ===== Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Published by American Psychological Association Dear Author, The page proofs of your article (# 1029 ) are now ready for your final review before your article is published. Please access your proofs, proofread them carefully, and respond to the Production Editor within 48 hours. To access your proofs, please refer to this URL address Login: your address Password: ---- The site contains 1 file. You will need to have Adobe Acrobat Reader software (Version 4.0 or higher) to read this file. This is free software and is available for user downloading at If you have any problems with downloading your article from the Rapid Proof site, please contact rapidprooftech@cadmus.com. PLEASE INCLUDE YOUR ARTICLE NUMBER ( 1029 ) WITH ALL CORRESPONDENCE. This file contains: 1. Proofreading Instructions 2. Subscriptions and Special Offers information 3. Reprint Order Form 4. A copy of the page proofs for your article. The proof contains 10 pages. After printing the PDF file, and within 48 hours of receipt, please read the page proofs carefully against your copy of the copyedited manuscript and 1. indicate changes or corrections in the margin of the page proofs; 2. answer any and all queries that appear on the last page of the PDF proof; 3. proofread any tables carefully against your copy of the copyedited manuscript; 4. proofread any equations and all mathematical symbols; 5. proofread any figures and check figure and caption placement; 6. check that any non-english characters and symbols, especially µ (mu), have been typeset correctly. Please respond to the Production Editor as follows: Authors within the continental United States: - If you have no changes, the Production Editor that you have no changes. - If you have minimal changes, you may summarize them in an to the Production Editor. Please indicate the location of changes clearly. Example: "Page 2, left column, 3rd paragraph, in sentence Each niche..., infrared is misspelled." - If you have several changes and would prefer not to summarize them in an , please send clearly marked proofs to the postal address given at the end of this message. Authors outside the continental United States:

2 Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Copy of Notification z2f If you have no changes, the Production Editor that you have no changes. - If you have any changes, please list them in an to the Production Editor, or fax a clearly marked set of proofs to the Production Editor s attention. If you choose to the changes, please indicate the location of changes clearly. Example: "Page 2, left column, 3rd paragraph, in sentence Each niche..., infrared is misspelled." If you are ordering reprints, please fill out the Reprint Order Form and return it with payment/purchase order to Cadmus Reprints, Reprint Account Manager, P.O. Box , Charlotte, NC If you have questions or concerns about the editing of your proofs, please contact the Production Editor at APA. Sincerely, Production Editor Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology APA Journals Office 750 First Street, NE Washington, DC Tel: JCCP@apa.org FAX (for non-u.s. authors):

3

4

5

6 Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

7 Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 72, No. 3, X/04/$12.00 DOI: / X Stages of Change or Changes of Stage? Predicting Transitions in Transtheoretical Model Stages in Relation to Healthy Food Choice Christopher J. Armitage and Paschal Sheeran University of Sheffield Madelynne A. Arden Sheffield Hallam University Mark Conner University of Leeds Relatively little research has examined factors that account for transitions between transtheoretical model (TTM) stages of change. The present study (N 787) used sociodemographic, TTM, and theory of planned behavior (TPB) variables, as well as theory-driven interventions to predict changes in stage. Longitudinal analyses revealed that sociodemographic, TPB, and 1 of the interventions predicted transitions between most stages of change. In fact, only progression from the preparation stage was not predictable. However, given that this change of stage marks the transition between cognition and actual behavior, the identification of variables that bridge this gap is crucial for the development of interventions to promote stage transitions. Accumulated research findings show a direct link between excessive fat consumption and several leading causes of morbidity and mortality in industrialized nations. Predominant among these conditions are coronary heart disease (e.g., Martin, Hulley, Browner, Kuller, & Wentworth, 1986; Stamler, Wentworth, & Neaton, 1986) and cancer, at a number of sites, including the large bowel, lung, endometrium, and prostate (e.g., Shu et al., 1993; Wynder, Hebert, & Kabat, 1987). In response to these serious threats to health, governments have recommended that people limit their fat intake; the U.K. government, for example, recommends that people should derive no more than 35% of their total calorific intake from fat (Department of Health, 1992). However, recent figures from the National Food Survey Branch of the U.K. Ministry of Agriculture, Fisheries, and Food (2001; as cited in Conner & Armitage, 2002) suggest that, on average, U.K. citizens derive more than 38% of their food energy from fat. The importance of understanding food choice is underlined by evidence showing that a reduction of just one percentage point in the proportion of food energy derived from fat in the diet would result in 10,000 fewer deaths per year in the United States alone (Rose, 1985). There is a clear need to investigate the ways in which food choice behavior may be changed to enhance the effectiveness of interventions. One way of achieving this goal is to use psychological models of Christopher J. Armitage and Paschal Sheeran, Centre for Research in Social Attitudes, Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom; Mark Conner, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds, United Kingdom; Madelynne A. Arden, Centre for Research in Human Behaviour, School of Social Science and Law, Sheffield Hallam University, Sheffield, United Kingdom. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Christopher J. Armitage, Centre for Research in Social Attitudes, Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Western Bank, Sheffield S10 2TP, United Kingdom. c.j.armitage@sheffield.ac.uk behavior change to design and implement interventions that will be effective in promoting health. Prochaska and DiClemente s (e.g., 1983, 1984) transtheoretical model (TTM) provides a framework for understanding healthrelated behavior change. Central to the TTM is the idea that individuals pass through five stages in the process of changing their health behavior. The first stage, precontemplation, designates individuals who are not thinking about performing the health behavior in question and are not sufficiently aware of the health implications of their actions. The second stage is labeled contemplation, the stage at which persons start to think seriously about changing their behavior but have not yet acted. The third stage is called preparation and is characterized by people preparing themselves and their social world for a change in their behavior. Thus, people in the preparation stage are not actually performing the behavior in question, but they are orienting themselves toward it (e.g., by making an attempt at the target behavior). When individuals successfully and consistently perform the behavior in question, they are regarded as being in the action stage. Progression from the action stage to the maintenance stage occurs when the behavior in question has been performed for 6 months or more. The TTM is a model of health behavior change, because individuals can progress, regress, or remain static with respect to their initial TTM stage. The TTM proposes that decisional balance and self-efficacy are the key predictors of transitions between stages. Derived from work by Janis and Mann (1977), decisional balance concerns the pros and cons associated with performing a particular behavior; self-efficacy (from Bandura s, 1997, social cognitive theory) represents feelings of confidence in one s ability to perform that behavior. The TTM also includes processes of change, which are 10 strategies that people may use in efforts to progress to a subsequent stage of change or to prevent regression from their current stage of change. We refer to decisional balance and self- 1

8 2 ARMITAGE, SHEERAN, CONNER, AND ARDEN F1 efficacy as predictors of change because the influence of the processes of change should be mediated through pros and cons and self-efficacy (see Prochaska, Redding, Harlow, Rossi, & Velicer, 1994; see Rosen, 2000, for a meta-analysis examining the direct relationship between the stages and processes of change). Decisional balance and self-efficacy have been used to test whether TTM stages can be discriminated from one another in several cross-sectional studies. For example, DiClemente et al. (1991) examined changes in decisional balance and self-efficacy across TTM stages with respect to quitting smoking. As predicted, the number of pros associated with smoking decreased in more advanced stages of change, whereas the number of cons increased; self-efficacy with respect to quitting smoking also increased across the stages of change. More recently, studies have tested the ability of self-efficacy and decisional balance to predict stage transitions, or changes of stage. For example, Herzog, Abrams, Emmons, Linnan, and Shadel (1999) compared cross-sectional analyses of stages of change with prospective assessments of stage transitions in the domain of smoking cessation. Consistent with most research in this area, Herzog et al. s cross-sectional analyses supported the TTM: There were linear increases in the cons associated with smoking and comparable decreases in the pros of smoking across stages (cf. DiClemente et al., 1991; Prochaska et al., 1994). In contrast, the longitudinal analyses indicate that... the pros and cons of smoking do not predict progressive movement out of the precontemplation, contemplation, or preparation stages (Herzog et al., 1999, p. 374). Mixed findings concerning the utility of decisional balance and self-efficacy for predicting stage transitions were also obtained in other studies of changes of stage (e.g., de Vries & Mudde, 1998; DiClemente, Prochaska, & Gibertini, 1985; Plotnikoff, Hotz, Birkett, & Courneya, 2001; Prochaska, DiClemente, Velicer, Ginpil, & Norcross, 1985; Velicer, Norman, Fava, & Prochaska, 1999). One possible explanation for this pattern of findings is that the focus on decisional balance and self-efficacy as predictors of change has excluded a range of alternative variables, many of which have proven predictive validity. For example, the behavioral beliefs that are held to underpin attitudes in the theory of planned behavior (TPB; see Ajzen, 1991) are formally equivalent to the pros and cons of performing a particular (health) behavior. Thus, a measure of attitude can be regarded as providing a summary of the processes associated with decisional balance (see Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). Similarly, Ajzen (1998) argued that perceived behavioral control is synonymous with self-efficacy: I added the construct of self-efficacy or perceived behavioural control to the original theory of reasoned action when the work of Bandura and his associates made it clear that this construct was needed to deal with determinants of human behaviour that are not under complete volitional control. (p. 737) Attitude and perceived behavioral control therefore subsume decisional balance and self-efficacy from the TTM. It is important to note that Ajzen s (1991) TPB (see Figure 1) both mediates the effects of sociodemographic variables on health behavior (e.g., Blaxter, 1990) and incorporates two constructs that are not specified by the TTM, namely, subjective norm and behavioral intention. Subjective norm captures social influences on health decisions and refers to the social pressures experienced by Figure 1. The theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991). The dashed AQ: 3 versus solid line denotes that perceived behavioral control does not always predict behavior. individuals with respect to engaging or not engaging in a particular behavior. Behavioral intentions, on the other hand, are indicators of how hard people are willing to try, of how much effort they are planning to exert, in order to perform the behavior (Ajzen, 1991, p. 181). The TPB has been widely used to predict health behavior, largely because of the substantial body of evidence that supports its predictive validity (e.g., Armitage & Conner, 2001). Moreover, several studies indicate that TPB variables provide very good discrimination between stages of change. For example, Armitage and Arden (2002) found stage-by-stage increases in TPB variables with respect to eating a low-fat diet: Individuals in the maintenance stage had more positive attitudes, perceived greater social pressure, reported more control, and were more likely to intend to eat a low-fat diet than those in the precontemplation stage. This pattern of findings has been replicated a number of times (e.g., Armitage, Povey, & Arden, 2003; Courneya, 1995). Moreover, Courneya, Plotnikoff, Hotz, and Birkett (2001) found that TPB variables were predictive of most transitions between the stages of change for exercise. Although problems with measurement and sampling (see Courneya et al., 2001, pp ) restricted the generalizability of their findings, Courneya et al. concluded that the TPB is a useful framework for predicting... stage transitions (p. 143). The present study therefore aims to extend the findings of Courneya et al. to the domain of healthy food choice. Derivation of the Present Study and Research Questions The literature reviewed above provides the following rationale for the present study. First, although the ability to predict changes of stage is important for the development of theory-based interventions, it is not yet clear what variables predict progressions between (or regressions from) TTM stages. Second, despite the TPB s success in cross-sectional studies, we were able to locate just one study that provided an adequate test of whether TPB variables can predict (longitudinal) changes of stage. Third, only two behaviors smoking and exercise have been studied longitudinally, so tests of other health behaviors are required to enhance generality. The present study tries to answer two questions: (a) To what extent does people s food choice remain static, progress, or regress across TTM stages over an 8-month time period? and (b) Can the TPB predict both stages of change and changes of stage?

9 STAGES OF CHANGE OR CHANGES OF STAGE? 3 Design and Setting Method The Leeds University Fat Assessment Trial was a large-scale study designed to test the effectiveness of two types of low-intensity intervention (belief-based change and tailored feedback) in reducing fat intake. The sample comprised nurses, nursing assistants, and administrative staff working at two hospitals in the U.K. Participants were randomized to a 3 (attitude change, perceived behavioral control change, information only control group) 2 (feedback versus no feedback) intervention design. The interventions were evaluated using measures taken pre- and postintervention. Participants and Procedure One thousand ninety-one hospital workers were sent a letter inviting them to participate in a study about their beliefs and attitudes toward eating a low-fat diet. Accompanying the letter was a brief questionnaire designed to measure key variables and to provide a baseline comparison (see the Measures section). Potential respondents were informed that participation was voluntary, and they were asked to complete the questionnaire and return it (via internal mail) to the Occupational Health Department if they wished to participate in the study. The data provided during this first wave of the study were used to design the interventions. The interventions were distributed to participants (in each of the six conditions) via the internal mail system 3 months postbaseline. Follow-up questionnaires were distributed 5 months postintervention (i.e., 8 months postbaseline). Interventions The interventions were administered using brief (four-page) leaflets that were designed specifically for the study. The information intervention included current U.K. government recommendations concerning fat intake as well as a list of foods that are high in fat. This information was provided to all participants. The belief-based interventions were informed by the work of Fishbein, Ajzen, and Bandura (e.g., Ajzen, 1991; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Bandura, 1997; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975).Fishbein and Ajzen (1975) argued that attitude change requires an understanding of the way in which attitudes are formed, and they therefore recommended targeting salient underlying beliefs. Thus, the attitude change intervention included a brief message designed to change the beliefs that discriminated individuals who were intending to eat a low-fat diet from those who were not intending to eat a low-fat diet (see Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). Thus, messages designed to persuade people that eating a low-fat diet would maintain fitness, control weight, and enhance feelings of health were developed (see Armitage & Conner, 1999). Given that Ajzen (1991, 1998) regarded perceived behavioral control and self-efficacy as synonymous, the perceived behavioral control change intervention was based on Bandura (1997). Bandura (1997) argued that there are four ways in which self-efficacy may be enhanced: Personal mastery, persuasion, modeling, and relaxation techniques. In order to match the perceived behavioral control intervention with the attitude change intervention in terms of intensity, we focused on personal mastery and persuasion techniques. Personal mastery can be achieved by setting individuals achievable subtasks, and so we provided participants with low-fat options that would be simple to incorporate within their existing lifestyle, as well as recipes for low-fat meals that were cheap, easy to prepare, and not time consuming. We also incorporated a persuasive message based on the control beliefs that are held to underpin the perceived behavioral control construct (see Ajzen, 1991). This message attempted to persuade participants that eating a low-fat diet was not expensive, that information was freely available, and that it does not necessarily involve a great deal of will-power (see Armitage & Conner, 1999). All participants received a standard letter accompanying their intervention leaflet. The letters of individuals who were randomized to receive tailored feedback contained one additional sentence that informed them of their current dietary fat intake as assessed by a 63-item validated food frequency measure (Margetts, Cade, & Osmond, 1989). Accumulated evidence shows that even such a low-intensity technique can exert significant positive effects on people s behavior (Brug, Campbell, & van Assema, 1999). Measures Measures of age, gender, and socioeconomic status (derived from participants occupations) were taken at baseline. These and the following measures were included in the questionnaires at both baseline and follow-up. TPB. A measure of attitude was taken using a semantic differential scale. Respondents were presented with the stem: My eating a low-fat diet in the future is.... Eight pairs of adjectives were rated, each on a bipolar scale ( 3 to 3; e.g., bad good, unfavorable favorable, harmful beneficial). The mean of the eight items was used as the measure of attitude (Cronbach s s.87 and.90 for baseline and follow-up, respectively). Subjective norm was measured using three items (e.g., People who are important to me think I should not eat a low-fat diet should eat a low-fat diet ). All three items were measured on 7-point unipolar (1 to 7) scales. Cronbach s s were.80 (baseline) and.83 (follow-up). Perceived behavioral control was measured using four items (e.g., I believe I have the ability to eat a low-fat diet in the future definitely do not definitely do ). Cronbach s s indicated good internal reliability ( s.86 and.83 for baseline and follow-up, respectively). Behavioral intention with respect to eating a low-fat diet was assessed using three items, each on a bipolar ( 3 to 3) scale (e.g., I intend to eat a low-fat diet in the future definitely do not definitely do ). The mean of these items produced a scale. Cronbach s for the baseline measure was.78; for the follow-up, it was.84. The psychometric properties of each of these measures have been tested in earlier work (Armitage & Conner, 1999). TTM. The measure of transtheoretical stage was adapted from Courneya (1995; Courneya et al., 2001; see also Armitage & Arden, 2002). Respondents were asked to indicate which of five statements best described their current dietary behavior: I currently do not eat a low-fat diet and I am not thinking about starting (precontemplation); I currently do not eat a low-fat diet but I am thinking about starting (contemplation); I currently eat a low-fat diet but not on a regular basis (preparation); I currently eat a low-fat diet but I have only begun to do so in the last 6 months (action); and I currently eat a low-fat diet and I have done so for longer than 6 months (maintenance). These statements map closely onto accepted definitions of the TTM stages and provide a parsimonious method of classifying individuals into stages. In the present study, the test retest correlation across 8 months for the stages-of-change measure was strong (r.71, p.01). Construct validity was confirmed by using the data from Margetts et al. s (1989) food frequency measure to examine differences in the proportion of calories derived from fat across the stages of change. As expected, fat intake declined across the stages of change at both baseline (precontemplation M 40.33; maintenance M 32.16) and follow-up (precontemplation M 40.79; maintenance M 32.19). Analyses of variance confirmed that these decreases in fat intake were significant, F(4, 786) 44.33, p.01, at baseline, and F(4, 786) 44.33, p.01, at follow-up. As recommended by Sutton (2000), these effects were further tested using polynomial-based contrast analyses (with an adjustment for unequal sample sizes); these analyses tested linear, quadratic, and cubic relationships between the stages of change and fat intake. For the baseline data, there were significant linear, F(1, 786) , p.01; quadratic, F(1, 786) 7.85, p.01; and

10 4 ARMITAGE, SHEERAN, CONNER, AND ARDEN cubic, F(1, 786) 4.56, p.05, relationships between the stages of change and fat intake, although the linear relationship had by far the largest effect size (linear r.42; quadratic r.10; cubic r.08). For the follow-up data, there was a significant linear relationship between the stages of change and fat intake, F(1, 498) 86.65, p.01, but nonsignificant quadratic and cubic relationships, Fs(1, 498) 2.65 and 0.16, ns, respectively. These data confirm the construct validity of the stages-ofchange measure and demonstrate that transitions between stages are related to changes in actual fat intake. Representativeness Check Results Of the 1,091 people who were contacted initially, 787 (72% response rate) returned completed questionnaires. Analyses indicated that nonrespondents were no different to respondents in terms of age, z 1.38, ns, or gender, 2 (1, N 1,091) 3.33, ns. Follow-up measures were taken 5 months postintervention. Of the 787 individuals who responded at baseline, we successfully contacted 499 at follow-up (63% response rate). Analysis of people who responded at follow-up versus those who did not revealed significant differences on some of the measures: Stage of change, F(1, 735) 17.16, p.05; perceived behavioral control, F(1, 735) 6.04, p.05; and age, F(1, 735) 4.28, p.05. Those who responded were in more advanced stages of change, reported greater perceived behavioral control, and were older than those who did not respond. We return to the significance of these findings in the Discussion section. Randomization Check Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) indicated that randomization of participants to the intervention conditions was successful: There were no significant differences between any of the six conditions on any of the measured variables at baseline, F(18, 1448) 0.85, ns. Univariate tests were all nonsignificant. Table 1 Cross-Sectional Analysis of Stage-of-Change Data: Baseline (N 787) TPB variables Precontemplation (n 81) Contemplation (n 117) Cross-Sectional Analyses: Predicting Stages of Change Preliminary analysis of the data indicated that participants were spread across the stages at both time points (see Tables 1 and 2). At baseline, 10% were in precontemplation, 15% were in contemplation, 40% were in preparation, 10% were in the action stage, and 25% reported that they were in the maintenance stage (see Table 1). At follow-up, 9% were in precontemplation, 8% were in contemplation, 42% were in preparation, 9% were in the action stage, and 33% reported that they were in the maintenance stage (see Table 2). Tables 1 and 2 also presents the means and standard deviations for TPB variables across each of the stages for baseline (Table 1) and follow-up (Table 2). These data were analyzed using discriminant function analysis. Discriminant function analysis is mathematically identical to MANOVA but emphasizes the prediction of group membership on the basis of a range of predictors (in this case, predicting TTM stage from TPB and demographic variables) rather than according to whether group membership produces reliable differences on a combination of DVs [dependent variables] (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989, p. 505). Thus, the baseline and follow-up data were analyzed using separate discriminant function analyses, with stage of change as the grouping variable and demographic (age, gender, socioeconomic status) and TPB variables as the independent variables. It was necessary to dummy code the TPB-based interventions because intervention constitutes a categorical variable with three levels (information, attitude change, perceived behavioral control change) and, therefore, cannot be used as an independent variable in discriminant function analysis. The three new variables were information-only intervention (0 did not receive,1 did receive), attitude change intervention (0 did not receive, 1 did receive) and perceived behavioral control change intervention (0 did not receive, 1 did receive). These dummy-coded intervention variables (as well as the presence or absence of personalized feedback, coded 1 and 0, respectively) were included in all subsequent multivariate analyses. Preparation (n 311) Action (n 79) Maintenance (n 199) M SD N M SD N M SD N M SD N M SD N Attitude 0.57 a b b c c 0.74 Subjective norm 4.07 a b,d b c c,d 1.24 Perceived behavioral 4.05 a b c d e 0.83 Control (PBC) Behavioral intention 0.37 a b b c c 0.94 Age Women Men Socioeconomic status Attitude change condition PBC change condition Control condition Received feedback Did not receive feedback T1-2 Note. Values with different subscripts within a row indicate significant ( p.05) differences between stages, on the basis of Newman Keuls post hoc tests. TPB theory of planned behavior; PBC perceived behavioral control.

11 STAGES OF CHANGE OR CHANGES OF STAGE? 5 Table 2 Cross-Sectional Analysis of Stage-of-Change Data: Follow-Up (N 499) Precontemplation (n 42) Contemplation (n 37) Preparation (n 210) Action (n 44) Maintenance (n 166) TPB variables M SD N M SD N M SD N M SD N M SD N AQ: T1 Attitude 0.77 a b c a e 0.66 Subjective norm 4.09 a a b,c c 1.19 Perceived behavioral 4.38 a b c Control (PBC) Behavioral intention 0.67 a b b c c 0.99 Age Women Men Socioeconomic status Attitude change condition PBC change condition Control condition Received feedback Did not receive feedback Note. All multivariate and univariate Fs associated with TPB variables are significant ( p.01) at both baseline (dfs 16, 2331) and follow-up (dfs 16, 1485); Fs range from to 95.54). Values with different subscripts within a row indicate significant ( p.05) differences between stages, on the basis of Newman Keuls post hoc tests. TPB theory of planned behavior; PBC perceived behavioral control. Analysis of the interventions produced few significant effects in the sample as a whole. In spite of this, it was deemed important to control for any potential influence of the interventions on transitions between the stages of change. It is also worth noting that two-way interactions between TPB and feedback interventions were tested in each of the analyses reported below. However, because these interactions were all nonsignificant, findings are not presented to aid clarity of presentation. Baseline. At baseline, the analyses revealed three significant discriminant functions, with a combined 2 (12, N 755) , p.01, that accounted for 91.39%, 7.54%, and 1.07% of the explained variance, respectively, and the first function had the largest group centroid compared with the other functions for four out of the five stages of change. Behavioral intention (r.92), perceived behavioral control (r.79), attitude (r.62), and subjective norm (r.32) were all significantly correlated with the first function. Perceived behavioral control and gender were most closely related to the second derived function (rs.61 and.39, respectively), and behavioral intention (r.32) and gender (r.89) were most strongly related to the third derived function. The predictors correctly classified 55.66% of the participants, which is 27.29% better than would be anticipated by chance alone (see Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989, p. 544). Discriminant function analysis also allows the researcher to test for significant differences between each of the groups across all of the predictor variables using pairwise Fs. In terms of the present study, the pairwise Fs take each stage in turn and contrast it with subsequent stages (e.g., contemplation vs. preparation, action, and maintenance). Thus, precontemplation was discriminated from contemplation, preparation, action, and maintenance, Fs(3, 730) , ps.01; contemplation was distinct from preparation, action, and maintenance, Fs(3, 730) , ps.01; preparation was distinguished from action and maintenance, Fs(3, 730) and 51.65, ps.01, respectively; and action was discriminated from maintenance, F(3, 730) 2.97, p.01. Findings indicate that participants in later stages of change generally scored higher on the measures of attitude, subjective norm, perceived behavioral control, and behavioral intention compared with participants in earlier stages of change. However, the discriminant function analysis indicates that behavioral intention and perceived behavioral control on their own provide the best combination of variables for distinguishing between stages of change. Follow-up. Discriminant function analysis of the follow-up data included the dummy-coded intervention variables in addition to the demographic and TPB variables. Two significant functions were derived, with a combined 2 (8, N 485) , p.01. The first derived function accounted for 95.78% of the explained variance, and the second derived function accounted for 4.22% of the explained variance, 2 (3, N 485) 13.84, p.01. Behavioral intention (r.99), attitude (r.58), subjective norm (r.37), and perceived behavioral control (r.69) were significantly correlated with the first derived function; perceived behavioral control (r.73) was most strongly correlated with the second derived function. The predictors correctly classified 54.31% of participants, which is 23.67% better than would be expected by chance alone. Consistent with the analysis of the baseline data, there were significant differences in pairwise Fs between most of the stages. More specifically, precontemplation was discriminated from contemplation, preparation, action, and maintenance, Fs(2, 479) , ps.01; contemplation was distinct from action and maintenance, Fs(2, 479) and 29.80, ps.01, respectively; preparation was also distinguished from action and maintenance, Fs(2, 479) and 46.79, ps.01, respectively. In contrast to the baseline analysis, the contemplation and preparation stages were not discriminated, F(2, 479) 2.59, p.10, neither were the action and maintenance stages, F(2, 479) 2.05, p.13. However, these latter findings only narrowly missed conventional statistical significance, and probably reflect the loss in power associated with the smaller sample size at follow-up.

12 6 ARMITAGE, SHEERAN, CONNER, AND ARDEN T3 AQ: 1 T4 Longitudinal Analyses: Predicting Changes of Stage In order to assess stage transitions, the baseline stage of change measure was subtracted from the follow-up stage measure. Positive values indicated progression (i.e., those that moved to a later stage), zero indicated no movement ( static ), and negative values signified regression (i.e., participants who moved to an earlier stage). Table 3 presents the frequency counts for individuals divided into the stages of change and the three change of stage categories (i.e., regress, static, progress). Overall, 61.52% of individuals remained in the same stage at both time points, 24.25% progressed and 14.23% regressed. However, these figures hide considerable variability between different stages. Chi-square tests comparing static versus regress and static versus progress confirmed this view, 2 (3, N 378) 75.30, p.05, and 2 (3, N 428) 50.56, p.05, respectively. Closer examination of Table 3 reveals that, in general, lower proportions of people regressed than either remained static or progressed. One exception was the action stage: 41.07% regressed, whereas 19.64% remained static and 39.29% progressed, suggesting that this stage is relatively volatile. Contemplators were particularly likely to progress from their stage: 66.67% of them did so. In contrast, more individuals remained static than progressed in both the precontemplation and preparation stages. We focused on predicting adjacent changes of stage: For each stage of change, we contrasted remaining in the same stage (static) with regression from that stage, and static versus progression from that stage using two methods. First, we conducted discriminant function analyses for transitions relating to each of the five stages of change and computed pairwise Fs to determine whether regression and progression could be distinguished from static using TPB, demographic, and (dummy coded) intervention variables as predictors. Second, we conducted planned comparisons (MANOVAs and Newman Keuls post hoc tests) on the variables that were most strongly associated with the discriminant functions (according to F to remove analyses, see Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989) in order to determine the best predictors of stage transitions. For each change of stage, the predictors correctly classified participants in excess of 10% better than would be expected by chance (range 10.76% 17.48% improvement in classification). The pairwise F analyses in Table 4 show that regression from the preparation, action, and maintenance stages were predicted from the demographic, TPB, and intervention variables (all ps.05). Progression from the precontemplation, contemplation, and action stages was also predicted (all ps.05). The findings indicate that of the seven possible stage transitions examined here, only progression from the preparation stage was not reliably predicted, F(1, 186) 3.45, ns. Planned comparisons presented in Table 5 confirm the results of the pairwise F analyses: Progression from the preparation stage was the only change of stage that was not reliably predicted. Looking at the predictors of transitions for each stage in turn indicates that having been the recipient of the attitude change intervention was the most important predictor of progress from the precontemplation stage. Analysis of the contemplation stage revealed that older participants and participants with greater perceived behavioral control were likely to progress. Behavioral intention differed significantly between individuals who regressed, remained static, and progressed from the preparation stage ( p.05). Newman Keuls post hoc tests revealed that behavioral intention was predictive of regression, but not progression, from the preparation stage (Ms 0.89, 1.44, and 1.80 for regress, static, and progress, respectively). Age and perceived behavioral control were associated with transitions in relation to the action stage: Younger individuals were more likely to remain static (M 30.18) than regress (M 42.56) or progress (M 40.91) from this stage whereas greater perceived behavioral control was predictive of progression but not regression from the action stage. Finally, regression from the maintenance stage was predicted by weak behavioral intentions to eat a low-fat diet. In summary, the findings show that the attitude change intervention (but not the other interventions), behavioral intentions, and perceived behavioral control from the TPB, and age reliably predicted regression from all stages and predicted progression from Table 3 Number of Participants Regressing, Remaining Static, or Progressing From Each Stage Stage Table 4 Pairwise Fs Testing Differences Between Static and Regress Progress Stage of change Regress Static Progress Total N % N % N % N % dfs Regress vs. static Static vs. progress Precontemplation (1, 40) 5.84* Contemplation a (1, 51) 10.08** Preparation (1,186) 4.20* 3.45 Action (2, 49) 5.07** 8.40** Maintenance (1,136) 5.84* a This category was removed from the Regress vs. static analyses due to the small cell size (n 4). * p.05. ** p.01. AQ: T2 AQ: T3 T5 Precontemplation Contemplation a Preparation Action Maintenance Total a This category was removed from subsequent Regress analyses due to the small cell size.

13 STAGES OF CHANGE OR CHANGES OF STAGE? 7 Table 5 Means and F Ratios for Variables That Predict Stage Transitions AQ: T4-T5 Predictor of stage transitions dfs Regress Static Progress Univariate Fs AQ: t6 Precontemplation stage (1, 45) ACI * Contemplation stage (1, 54) Age ** Perceived behavioral control * Preparation stage (2,191) Behavioral intention 0.89 a 1.44 b 1.80 b 4.66* Action stage (2, 53) Age a b a 5.37** Perceived behavioral control 5.70 a 5.43 a 6.41 b 5.71** Maintenance stage (1,142) Behavioral intention * Note. Variables were derived from analysis of Wilk s lambda coefficients. Values with different subscripts within a row indicate significant ( p.05) differences between groups, on the basis of Newman Keuls post hoc tests. ACI the attitude change intervention (dummy coded: 0 did not receive; 1 did receive). * p.05. ** p.01. the majority of the stages. The one exception to this pattern of findings was the failure to predict progression from the preparation stage. Discussion The present findings corroborate a considerable body of research that has examined differences in social cognitive variables across the stages of change (e.g., DiClemente et al., 1991; Herzog et al., 1999). Consistent with previous studies, the values of TPB variables increased in a linear fashion in successive stages of change (cf. Armitage & Arden, 2002; Courneya, 1995). Thus, participants had more positive evaluations of eating a low-fat diet, perceived greater social pressure to do so, were more confident in their dieting ability, and had stronger intentions in later, compared with earlier, stages of change. However, because cross-sectional analyses can provide only limited support for the TTM, longitudinal analyses were used to examine transitions between stages. Consistent with Courneya et al. (2001), the present study demonstrated that transitions between the stages of change could be predicted. More specifically, the attitude change intervention successfully advanced individuals from the precontemplation stage, and age and perceived behavioral control predicted progression from both the contemplation and action stages. Behavioral intention predicted regression from the preparation and maintenance stages, and age predicted regression from the action stage. Thus, two variables derived from the TPB, namely, behavioral intention and perceived behavioral control, were predictive of a number of stage transitions. Behavioral intention predicted regression from the preparation and maintenance stages, suggesting that interventions designed to bolster existing intentions might be effective in preventing regression from these stages of change. Perceived behavioral control was predictive of progression from the contemplation and action stages: Enhancing perceived behavioral control might therefore increase the likelihood that individuals would progress from these stages (cf. Bandura, 1997). Contrary to predictions, age was also a predictor of progression from the contemplation stage and of regression and progression from the action stage; in both cases, younger people remained static. This finding was not anticipated because the TPB is regarded as a complete model of social behavior, meaning that the effects of age on stage transitions should be mediated through TPB variables (cf. Ajzen, 1991). The implication is that the TPB may not be a sufficient model of dietary change and that additional variables are needed to explain the relationship between age and change of stage. It is particularly curious that age was positively associated with both progression and regression from the action stage. One explanation of this finding is that older people ( 30 years old) are more likely to change their diets, especially in response to medical information (see Haslam et al., 2000). Possibly, therefore, a measure of the number of previous failed attempts to change diet could have mediated the effects of age on transitions relating to the action stage. Future research should examine this possibility. Although TPB variables were generally effective at predicting both regression and progression, the model did not account for all stage transitions, and so further work is required to identify which variables are predictive. Of particular note is the fact that progression from the preparation stage was not predicted. Progression from the preparation stage is crucial because it involves the transition to actual performance of the behavior unlike most other stage transitions which involve changes in mental readiness for action. Individuals in the preparation stage are characterized by vague attempts to perform the behavior in question (e.g., purchasing foods that are lower in fat), in contrast with actors and maintainers, who have been performing the behavior for some time. Thus, the transition from preparation to action maintenance marks the translation of cognition into action. The implication is that the TPB cannot fully account for this gap between cognition ( preparation ) and action. In fact, several commentators have identified the failure to address processes that might explain the intention behavior gap as a significant limitation of the TPB (e.g., Sheeran, 2002). For example, Gollwitzer (1993) argued that implementation intentions are important in ensuring that cognitions are translated into action. Implementation intentions are plans that specify the conditions under which target behaviors will be performed and help to ensure that the decision is acted upon. Thus,

14 8 ARMITAGE, SHEERAN, CONNER, AND ARDEN asking individuals in the preparation stage to form an implementation intention of the form I intend to perform behavior x in situation y could be effective in increasing the likelihood of progression to the action stage. In fact, Armitage (2004) recently demonstrated that an implementation intention that specified that people should plan to eat a low-fat diet paying particular attention to the situations in which they would implement those plans enabled participants to eat more healthily even after attitude, perceived behavioral control, and behavioral intention had been taken into account. There were a number of limitations of the present study that should be addressed. First, the theory-driven interventions exerted only limited impact on changes of stage. This may have been due to the fact that the interventions were relatively low intensity: For example, because the persuasive communications were distributed through the internal mail system, we had no control over the extent to which participants attended to, or elaborated, the arguments contained therein, which may have undermined their impact (cf. Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). Further work is required to examine the effective implementation of theory-driven interventions. That said, it is worthwhile noting that progression from the precontemplation stage was predicted by the attitude change intervention. This change of stage is important because participants in the precontemplation stage consumed at least 2% more fat (as a proportion of total energy intake) than participants in any other stage of change. A second limitation of the present study is that the predictive validity of the processes of change was not examined. Previous research has demonstrated consistent and robust relations among the processes and the stages of change (Rosen, 2000) and the inclusion of the processes in the present study may have substantially increased the predictive power, particularly in predicting progression from the preparation stage. A third limitation was that the sample comprised hospital employees, who may be more receptive to health information compared with other samples. However, this consideration does not seriously undermine the validity of our findings because (a) participants represented a range of occupations and (b) more than half of the sample were not in the action or maintenance stage at either time point, despite their potential receptiveness to health information. Similarly, although the level of attrition between baseline and follow-up poses another potential threat to the generalizability of the study, the attrition rate was similar to that reported in comparable studies (e.g., Courneya et al., 2001; Herzog et al., 1999; Velicer et al., 1999). Moreover, attrition in the present study is understandable, given that nonrespondents were more likely to be precontemplators and to have lower perceived behavioral control than respondents. In sum, although we do not believe that there are serious grounds for suggesting that the present sample is unrepresentative, caution is probably warranted before making generalizations to other samples. In conclusion, the present study provides the first test of TTM stage transitions in the domain of health-related food choice. The distribution of participants across stages at each time point and the extent of participants movement between stages over time were comparable to that found in previous research. Also consistent with previous research was the finding that TPB variables discriminated between the stages of change (at both time points). However, a unique feature of the present study was that we identified variables that were predictive of transitions between the stages of change, for the first time, variables that predict changes of state were determined. Participants who received an attitude change intervention progressed from the precontemplation stage; age and perceived behavioral control predicted progression from the contemplation stage; behavioral intention predicted regression from the preparation stage; age predicted action stage transitions, and perceived behavioral control predicted progression from the action stage; and behavioral intention predicted regression from the maintenance stage. The present research also identified a serious limitation of the TPB (and, by extension, of decisional balance and self-efficacy), namely, that it was not possible to predict progression from preparation to action. Future studies should investigate the utility of Gollwitzer s (1993) concept of implementation intentions in helping people make this crucial transition from cognition to action. References Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, Ajzen, I. (1998). Models of human social behavior and their application to health psychology. Psychology and Health, 13, Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Armitage, C. J. (2004). Evidence that implementation intentions reduce dietary fat intake: A randomized trial. Health Psychology, 23, XXX XXX. Armitage, C. J., & Arden, M. A. (2002). Exploring discontinuity patterns in the transtheoretical model: An application of the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (1999). Distinguishing perceptions of control from self-efficacy: Predicting consumption of a low-fat diet using the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, Armitage, C. J., Povey, R., & Arden, M. A. (2003). Evidence for discontinuity patterns across the stages of change: A role for attitudinal ambivalence. Psychology and Health, 18, Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman. Blaxter, M. (1990). Health and lifestyles. London: Routledge. Brug, J., Campbell, M., & van Assema, P. (1999). The application and impact of computer-generated personalized nutrition education: A review of the literature. Patient Education and Counseling, 36, Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (2002). The social psychology of food. Buckingham, U.K.: Open University Press. Courneya, K. S. (1995). Understanding readiness for regular physical activity in older individuals: An application of the theory of planned behavior. Health Psychology, 14, Courneya, K. S., Plotnikoff, R. C., Hotz, S. B., & Birkett, N. J. (2001). Predicting exercise stage transitions over two consecutive 6-month periods: A test of the theory of planned behaviour in a population-based sample. British Journal of Health Psychology, 6, Department of Health. (1992). The health of the nation: A strategy for health in England. London: Her Majesty s Stationery Office. de Vries, H., & Mudde, A. N. (1998). Predicting stage transitions for smoking cessation applying the attitude social-influence efficacy model. Psychology and Health, 13, DiClemente, C. C., Prochaska, J. O., Fairhurst, S. K., Velicer, W. F., Velasquez, M. M., & Rossi, J. S. (1991). The process of smoking cessation: An analysis of precontemplation, contemplation, and prepa- AQ: 2

Predicting and explaining transtheoretical model stage transitions in relation to condom-carrying behaviour

Predicting and explaining transtheoretical model stage transitions in relation to condom-carrying behaviour Predicting and explaining transtheoretical model stage transitions in relation to condom-carrying behaviour ARDEN, M. A. and ARMITAGE, C. J. Available from Sheffield

More information

Intention to consent to living organ donation: an exploratory study. Christina Browne B.A. and Deirdre M. Desmond PhD

Intention to consent to living organ donation: an exploratory study. Christina Browne B.A. and Deirdre M. Desmond PhD Intention to consent to living organ donation: an exploratory study Christina Browne B.A. and Deirdre M. Desmond PhD Department of Psychology, John Hume Building, National University of Ireland Maynooth,

More information

Heavy Smokers', Light Smokers', and Nonsmokers' Beliefs About Cigarette Smoking

Heavy Smokers', Light Smokers', and Nonsmokers' Beliefs About Cigarette Smoking Journal of Applied Psychology 1982, Vol. 67, No. 5, 616-622 Copyright 1982 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 002I-9010/82/6705-0616S00.75 ', ', and Nonsmokers' Beliefs About Cigarette Smoking

More information

How useful are the stages of change for targeting interventions? randomized test of a brief intervention to reduce smoking

How useful are the stages of change for targeting interventions? randomized test of a brief intervention to reduce smoking How useful are the stages of change for targeting interventions? randomized test of a brief intervention to reduce smoking ARMITAGE, C. J. and ARDEN, M. A. Available

More information

Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology*

Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology* Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology* Timothy Teo & Chwee Beng Lee Nanyang Technology University Singapore This

More information

Relationships between stage of change for stress management behavior and perceived stress and coping

Relationships between stage of change for stress management behavior and perceived stress and coping Japanese Psychological Research 2010, Volume 52, No. 4, 291 297 doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2010.00444.x Short Report Relationships between stage of change for stress management behavior and perceived stress

More information

Subtypes of precontemplating smokers defined by different long-term plans to change their smoking behavior

Subtypes of precontemplating smokers defined by different long-term plans to change their smoking behavior HEALTH EDUCATION RESEARCH Vol.15 no.4 2000 Theory & Practice Pages 423 434 Subtypes of precontemplating smokers defined by different long-term plans to change their smoking behavior Arie Dijkstra and Hein

More information

Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self-Affirming Implementation Intention

Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self-Affirming Implementation Intention Enhancing the Effectiveness of Alcohol Warning Labels With a Self-Affirming Implementation Intention ARMITAGE, Christopher J and ARDEN, Madelynne A Available from

More information

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [University of Maastricht] On: 14 January 2009 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 781062704] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and

More information

Behavior Change Theories

Behavior Change Theories Behavior Change Theories Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD, FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry Behavioral change theories These theories explain the reasons behind alterations in individuals' behavioral patterns.

More information

Measurement of Constructs in Psychosocial Models of Health Behavior. March 26, 2012 Neil Steers, Ph.D.

Measurement of Constructs in Psychosocial Models of Health Behavior. March 26, 2012 Neil Steers, Ph.D. Measurement of Constructs in Psychosocial Models of Health Behavior March 26, 2012 Neil Steers, Ph.D. Importance of measurement in research testing psychosocial models Issues in measurement of psychosocial

More information

Why Is Theory Important?

Why Is Theory Important? Health Behavior Theories: Applying Theory to Practice Why Is Theory Important? Theory gives planners tools for moving beyond intuition. Consistent with the current emphasis on using evidence-based interventions

More information

CHANGING HEALTH BEHAVIOUR

CHANGING HEALTH BEHAVIOUR CHANGING HEALTH BEHAVIOUR Dr Sam Caton ScHARR s.caton@sheffield.ac.uk Learning objectives 1. Identify different types of heath behaviour 2. Identify the importance of understanding health behaviours 3.

More information

EPHE 575. Exercise Adherence. To Do. 8am Tuesday Presentations

EPHE 575. Exercise Adherence. To Do. 8am Tuesday Presentations EPHE 575 Exercise Adherence To Do 8am Tuesday Presentations Quiz Find an article on exercise adherence and do an article summary on it. (If you have already checked it off, I will have one for you to fill

More information

Boliang Guo 1, Paul Aveyard 1, Antony Fielding 2 & Stephen Sutton 3 RESEARCH REPORT ABSTRACT

Boliang Guo 1, Paul Aveyard 1, Antony Fielding 2 & Stephen Sutton 3 RESEARCH REPORT ABSTRACT RESEARCH REPORT doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02519.x Do the Transtheoretical Model processes of change, decisional balance and temptation predict stage movement? Evidence from smoking cessation in adolescents

More information

Smoking and the Ø pattern; predictors of transitions through the stages of change

Smoking and the Ø pattern; predictors of transitions through the stages of change HEALTH EDUCATION RESEARCH Vol.21 no.3 2006 Theory & Practice Pages 305 314 Advance Access publication 8 June 2006 Smoking and the Ø pattern; predictors of transitions through the stages of change E. F.

More information

Deakin Research Online Deakin University s institutional research repository DDeakin Research Online Research Online This is the published version:

Deakin Research Online Deakin University s institutional research repository DDeakin Research Online Research Online This is the published version: Deakin Research Online Deakin University s institutional research repository DDeakin Research Online Research Online This is the published version: Taghian, Mehdi and D'Souza, Clare 2007, A cross-cultural

More information

The impact of asking about interest in free nicotine patches on smoker s stated. intent to change: Real effect or artefact of question ordering?

The impact of asking about interest in free nicotine patches on smoker s stated. intent to change: Real effect or artefact of question ordering? Impact of asking about interest in free nicotine patches 1 The impact of asking about interest in free nicotine patches on smoker s stated intent to change: Real effect or artefact of question ordering?

More information

Health Behaviour Change: Applying Prochaska and DiClemente s Stages of Change Model

Health Behaviour Change: Applying Prochaska and DiClemente s Stages of Change Model Health Behaviour Change: Applying Prochaska and DiClemente s Stages of Change Model Image Sources http://www.clevelandseniors.com/forever/100 smoker.htm http://bacontoday.com/bacon flavored diet coke/

More information

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution 1 Robert D. Mather University of Central Oklahoma Charles M. Mather University of Science

More information

Understanding Tourist Environmental Behavior An Application of the Theories on Reasoned Action Approach

Understanding Tourist Environmental Behavior An Application of the Theories on Reasoned Action Approach University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Tourism Travel and Research Association: Advancing Tourism Research Globally 2012 ttra International Conference Understanding Tourist Environmental

More information

Nutrition Education Research Brief: Message Framing, Use of Interactive Technology to Tailor Messages, and Intervention Intensity.

Nutrition Education Research Brief: Message Framing, Use of Interactive Technology to Tailor Messages, and Intervention Intensity. The Office of Analysis, Nutrition and Evaluation Nutrition Education Research Brief: Message Framing, Use of Interactive Technology to Tailor Messages, and Intervention Intensity June 2007 United States

More information

Using Implementation Intentions to Increase Attendance for Cervical Cancer Screening

Using Implementation Intentions to Increase Attendance for Cervical Cancer Screening Health Psychology 2000. Vol. 19. No. 3. 283-289 Copyright 2000 by the American Piychohjgical Allocution, Inc. 0278-6133/00/S100 DOT: 10.1037«0278-«133.I9.3.283 Using Implementation Intentions to Increase

More information

Perceived usefulness. Intention Use E-filing. Attitude. Ease of use Perceived behavioral control. Subjective norm

Perceived usefulness. Intention Use E-filing. Attitude. Ease of use Perceived behavioral control. Subjective norm Project Guidelines Perceived usefulness Attitude Intention Use E-filing Ease of use Perceived behavioral control Subjective norm Introduction Introduction should include support/justification why the research

More information

The Roles of Behavioral and Implementation Intentions in Changing Physical Activity in Young Children With Low Socioeconomic Status

The Roles of Behavioral and Implementation Intentions in Changing Physical Activity in Young Children With Low Socioeconomic Status Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 2010, 32, 359-376 2010 Human Kinetics, Inc. The Roles of Behavioral and Implementation Intentions in Changing Physical Activity in Young Children With Low Socioeconomic

More information

Changing People s Behavior. Larry Wissow Professor Health, Behavior and Society Johns Hopkins School of Public Health

Changing People s Behavior. Larry Wissow Professor Health, Behavior and Society Johns Hopkins School of Public Health This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike License. Your use of this material constitutes acceptance of that license and the conditions of use of materials on this

More information

Comparing 3 Means- ANOVA

Comparing 3 Means- ANOVA Comparing 3 Means- ANOVA Evaluation Methods & Statistics- Lecture 7 Dr Benjamin Cowan Research Example- Theory of Planned Behaviour Ajzen & Fishbein (1981) One of the most prominent models of behaviour

More information

The SAGE Encyclopedia of Educational Research, Measurement, and Evaluation Multivariate Analysis of Variance

The SAGE Encyclopedia of Educational Research, Measurement, and Evaluation Multivariate Analysis of Variance The SAGE Encyclopedia of Educational Research, Measurement, Multivariate Analysis of Variance Contributors: David W. Stockburger Edited by: Bruce B. Frey Book Title: Chapter Title: "Multivariate Analysis

More information

Psychosocial Variables, External Barriers, and Stage of Mammography Adoption

Psychosocial Variables, External Barriers, and Stage of Mammography Adoption Health Psychology Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 22, No. 6, 649 653 0278-6133/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.649 Psychosocial Variables, External Barriers,

More information

Behavioral change theories : explain the reasons behind alterations in individuals' behavioral patterns. serve to understand better those factors

Behavioral change theories : explain the reasons behind alterations in individuals' behavioral patterns. serve to understand better those factors Behavioral change theories : explain the reasons behind alterations in individuals' behavioral patterns. serve to understand better those factors associated with maximizing adherence to positive physical

More information

User Experience: Findings from Patient Telehealth Survey

User Experience: Findings from Patient Telehealth Survey User Experience: Findings from Patient Telehealth Survey Sarah Gorst University of Sheffield NIHR CLAHRC for South Yorkshire MALT WS3: User experience Overcoming the Barriers to Mainstreaming Assisted

More information

Self-Efficacy, Decisional Balance and Stages of Change on Dietary Practices among Metabolic Syndrome Persons, Uthai Thani Province

Self-Efficacy, Decisional Balance and Stages of Change on Dietary Practices among Metabolic Syndrome Persons, Uthai Thani Province Self-Efficacy, Decisional Balance and Stages of Change on Dietary Practices among Metabolic Syndrome Persons, Uthai Thani Province Manirat Therawiwat PhD*, Nirat Imamee PhD*, Thaweesak Khamklueng MSc**

More information

PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH

PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH DANIEL Z. LEVIN Management and Global Business Dept. Rutgers Business School Newark and New Brunswick Rutgers

More information

Analyzing Determinants of Educational Methods in Environmental Education by using the Theory of Planned Behavior.

Analyzing Determinants of Educational Methods in Environmental Education by using the Theory of Planned Behavior. 1 Analyzing Determinants of Educational Methods in Environmental Education by using the Theory of Planned Behavior. Klee. R., Bamberg, S., Erten, S., Graf, D. 1. Goals of the Study. In environmental education

More information

Evaluation of the. Learn2Live Road Safety Intervention for Young People. Plymouth Pavilions event. 25 th November 2011.

Evaluation of the. Learn2Live Road Safety Intervention for Young People. Plymouth Pavilions event. 25 th November 2011. Evaluation of the Learn2Live Road Safety Intervention for Young People Plymouth Pavilions event 25 th November 2011 Research by Emma Dunnett, Amy Goodwin, Georgina Hobson, Claire Hubbard, Daniel Orton,

More information

Acknowledgments. TTM-Tailored Tailored Smoking

Acknowledgments. TTM-Tailored Tailored Smoking Successes and Failures in Changing Multiple Behaviors in Populations of Primary Care Patients, Employees, and Parents Acknowledgments This project was funded by: National Cancer Institute - NCI PO1 # CA

More information

Original Article. Evaluation of the Relationship between Causality Orientation and Decisional Balance at Different Stages of Exercise Behavior Change

Original Article. Evaluation of the Relationship between Causality Orientation and Decisional Balance at Different Stages of Exercise Behavior Change Journal of Physical Education and Sport (JPES), 17 Supplement issue 2, Art 69, pp. 458-465, 2017 online ISSN: 2247-806X; p-issn: 2247 8051; ISSN - L = 2247-8051 JPES Original Article Evaluation of the

More information

REDUCING FRIDAY ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION AMONG MODERATE, WOMEN DRINKERS: EVALUATION OF A BRIEF EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTION

REDUCING FRIDAY ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION AMONG MODERATE, WOMEN DRINKERS: EVALUATION OF A BRIEF EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTION Alcohol & Alcoholism Vol. 42, No. 1, pp. 37 41, 2007 Advance Access publication 27 November 2006 doi:10.1093/alcalc/agl083 REDUCING FRIDAY ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION AMONG MODERATE, WOMEN DRINKERS: EVALUATION

More information

Assessing Readiness To Change

Assessing Readiness To Change Assessing Readiness To Change Transtheoretical Model The Transtheoretical Model describes the stages of behavior prior to change. It focuses on the individual s decision making. This model involves the

More information

Ogden, J, Karim, L. Choudry, A., and Brown, K. (2007). Understanding successful

Ogden, J, Karim, L. Choudry, A., and Brown, K. (2007). Understanding successful 1 Ogden, J, Karim, L. Choudry, A., and Brown, K. (2007). Understanding successful behaviour change: the role of intentions, attitudes to the target and motivations and the example of diet. Health Education

More information

Modeling Health Behavior Change: How to Predict and Modify the Adoption and Maintenance of Health Behaviors

Modeling Health Behavior Change: How to Predict and Modify the Adoption and Maintenance of Health Behaviors Modeling Health Behavior Change, p. 1 To appear in: Applied Psychology: An International Review, 2007 Modeling Health Behavior Change: How to Predict and Modify the Adoption and Maintenance of Health Behaviors

More information

ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION

ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION Perceived Availability of and Attitudes toward Healthy Food Choices in Assisted-Living Facilities in Kansas Pei Liu Department of Hospitality Management and Dietetics Kansas State University and Junehee

More information

Extensive research has documented the determinants of

Extensive research has documented the determinants of EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PUBLIC HEALTH 2002; 12: 302 307 Cigarette smoking and stages of change among men and women in Kiev, Ukraine JENNA M. MCALLISTER, GORDON B. LINDSAY, RAY M. MERRILL, UGO A. PEREGO * 302

More information

Adolescent Regular Moderate-to-Vigorous Physical Activity

Adolescent Regular Moderate-to-Vigorous Physical Activity Running Head: TPB and adolescent physical activity Extending the Theory of Planned Behavior: The Role of Self and Social Influences in Predicting Adolescent Regular Moderate-to-Vigorous Physical Activity

More information

Michele Clements-Thompson and Robert C. Klesges University of Memphis Prevention Center. Harry Lando University of Minnesota

Michele Clements-Thompson and Robert C. Klesges University of Memphis Prevention Center. Harry Lando University of Minnesota Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 1998, Vol. 66, No 6, 1005-1011 Copyright 1998 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-006X/98/S3.0C Relationships Between Stages of Change in Cigarette

More information

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies Emma ter Mors 1,2, Mieneke Weenig 1, Naomi Ellemers 1, Dancker Daamen 1 1 Leiden University,

More information

Motivational Interviewing

Motivational Interviewing Motivational Interviewing Barbara M. Miller, RN, CEC Robin Seabury, MS WVU School of Nursing And Finally What Do You Want To Leave With Today? Change Remember, change is much larger than behavior.

More information

Author's response to reviews

Author's response to reviews Author's response to reviews Title: Gender differences in Greek centenarians. A cross-sectional nation-wide study, examining multiple socio-demographic and personality factors and health locus of control.

More information

How Can Employers Make a Difference

How Can Employers Make a Difference PATHWAYS TO SUCCESSFUL HEALTH BEHAVIOR CHANGE: How Can Employers Make a Difference CARLO C. DICLEMENTE, Ph.D. University of Maryland, Baltimore County www.umbc.edu/psych/habits www.mdquit.org diclemen@umbc.edu

More information

Motivating people to be physically active (Chapters 1 & 2)

Motivating people to be physically active (Chapters 1 & 2) + Motivating people to be physically active (Chapters 1 & 2) + Major premise Effective Program Motivation Higher Adherence + Goals of each component Effective Program Higher exercise self-efficacy Lower

More information

THE WHEN AND HOW OF COMMUNICATING CLIMATE CHANGE

THE WHEN AND HOW OF COMMUNICATING CLIMATE CHANGE THE WHEN AND HOW OF COMMUNICATING CLIMATE CHANGE CAROLINE WILSON KATHERINE N. IRVINE GREIG MILL cwilson@dmu.ac.uk De Montfort University (United Kingdom) ABSTRACT This investigation discovers how combined

More information

The Ecological Perspective: A Multilevel, Interactive Approach

The Ecological Perspective: A Multilevel, Interactive Approach 10 The Ecological Perspective: A Multilevel, Interactive Approach Contemporary health promotion involves more than simply educating individuals about healthy practices. It includes efforts to change organizational

More information

An Empirical Study of the Roles of Affective Variables in User Adoption of Search Engines

An Empirical Study of the Roles of Affective Variables in User Adoption of Search Engines An Empirical Study of the Roles of Affective Variables in User Adoption of Search Engines ABSTRACT Heshan Sun Syracuse University hesun@syr.edu The current study is built upon prior research and is an

More information

Transtheoretical Model

Transtheoretical Model History/Background: Transtheoretical Model Developed by the University of Rhode Island Cancer Prevention Research Center through several years of observing how people quit smoking. Its name was formed

More information

Motivational Interviewing Maureen P. Scahill, NP, MS Center for Health & Behavioral Training

Motivational Interviewing Maureen P. Scahill, NP, MS Center for Health & Behavioral Training Motivational Interviewing Maureen P. Scahill, NP, MS Center for Health & Behavioral Training Infectious Disease Division, Department of Medicine, University of Rochester Medical Center STD/HIV Program,

More information

Running head: THEORIES AND THEORISTS IN PUBLIC HEALTH RESPONSE T 1

Running head: THEORIES AND THEORISTS IN PUBLIC HEALTH RESPONSE T 1 Running head: THEORIES AND THEORISTS IN PUBLIC HEALTH RESPONSE T 1 Theories and Theorists in Public Health Response to Bioterrorism Patricia Bolivar Walden University Research Theory, Design, and Method

More information

This report summarizes the stakeholder feedback that was received through the online survey.

This report summarizes the stakeholder feedback that was received through the online survey. vember 15, 2016 Test Result Management Preliminary Consultation Online Survey Report and Analysis Introduction: The College s current Test Results Management policy is under review. This review is being

More information

SURVEY TOPIC INVOLVEMENT AND NONRESPONSE BIAS 1

SURVEY TOPIC INVOLVEMENT AND NONRESPONSE BIAS 1 SURVEY TOPIC INVOLVEMENT AND NONRESPONSE BIAS 1 Brian A. Kojetin (BLS), Eugene Borgida and Mark Snyder (University of Minnesota) Brian A. Kojetin, Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2 Massachusetts Ave. N.E.,

More information

Title: The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) and Texting While Driving Behavior in College Students MS # Manuscript ID GCPI

Title: The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) and Texting While Driving Behavior in College Students MS # Manuscript ID GCPI Title: The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) and Texting While Driving Behavior in College Students MS # Manuscript ID GCPI-2015-02298 Appendix 1 Role of TPB in changing other behaviors TPB has been applied

More information

Colon cancer information as a source of exercise motivation

Colon cancer information as a source of exercise motivation Psychology and Health December, 2006; 21(6): 739 755 Colon cancer information as a source of exercise motivation STACEY P. GRAHAM 1, HARRY PRAPAVESSIS 2, & LINDA D. CAMERON 1 1 Department of Psychological

More information

The Myers Briggs Type Inventory

The Myers Briggs Type Inventory The Myers Briggs Type Inventory Charles C. Healy Professor of Education, UCLA In press with Kapes, J.T. et. al. (2001) A counselor s guide to Career Assessment Instruments. (4th Ed.) Alexandria, VA: National

More information

Computer tailored persuasion: Working mechanisms. Arie Dijkstra University of Groningen The Netherlands

Computer tailored persuasion: Working mechanisms. Arie Dijkstra University of Groningen The Netherlands Computer tailored persuasion: Working mechanisms Arie Dijkstra University of Groningen The Netherlands Tailoring ingredients What in tailored interventions works; what elements are responsible for the

More information

THE USE OF MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS IN DEVELOPMENT THEORY: A CRITIQUE OF THE APPROACH ADOPTED BY ADELMAN AND MORRIS A. C. RAYNER

THE USE OF MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS IN DEVELOPMENT THEORY: A CRITIQUE OF THE APPROACH ADOPTED BY ADELMAN AND MORRIS A. C. RAYNER THE USE OF MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS IN DEVELOPMENT THEORY: A CRITIQUE OF THE APPROACH ADOPTED BY ADELMAN AND MORRIS A. C. RAYNER Introduction, 639. Factor analysis, 639. Discriminant analysis, 644. INTRODUCTION

More information

Understanding Tailoring Using The Integrative Model of Behavior Prediction

Understanding Tailoring Using The Integrative Model of Behavior Prediction Understanding Tailoring Using The Integrative Model of Behavior Prediction Brenda Curtis, PhD Martin Fishbein Memorial Lecture Annenberg Public Policy Center November 18, 2010 The Basic Question What elements

More information

Small Group Presentations

Small Group Presentations Admin Assignment 1 due next Tuesday at 3pm in the Psychology course centre. Matrix Quiz during the first hour of next lecture. Assignment 2 due 13 May at 10am. I will upload and distribute these at the

More information

Marketing a healthier choice: Exploring young people s perception of e-cigarettes

Marketing a healthier choice: Exploring young people s perception of e-cigarettes Marketing a healthier choice: Exploring young people s perception of e-cigarettes Abstract Background: As a consequence of insufficient evidence on the safety and efficacy of e- cigarettes, there has been

More information

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site SEUNGHO CHO shcho72@gmail.com LAURA RICHARDSON WALTON lwalton@comm.msstate.edu Mississippi

More information

Using the construct of perceived autonomy support to understand social influence within the theory of planned behavior

Using the construct of perceived autonomy support to understand social influence within the theory of planned behavior Psychology of Sport and Exercise 9 (2008) 27 44 www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport Using the construct of perceived autonomy support to understand social influence within the theory of planned behavior

More information

Issues in Information Systems Volume 17, Issue II, pp , 2016

Issues in Information Systems Volume 17, Issue II, pp , 2016 CONSUMER ADOPTION AND USE OF MOBILE APPLICATIONS: DO PRIVACY AND SECURITY CONCERNS MATTER? Gary Garrison, Belmont University, gary.garrison@belmont.edu Sang Hyun Kim, Kyungpook National University, ksh@knu.ac.kr

More information

Funnelling Used to describe a process of narrowing down of focus within a literature review. So, the writer begins with a broad discussion providing b

Funnelling Used to describe a process of narrowing down of focus within a literature review. So, the writer begins with a broad discussion providing b Accidental sampling A lesser-used term for convenience sampling. Action research An approach that challenges the traditional conception of the researcher as separate from the real world. It is associated

More information

Title: Healthy snacks at the checkout counter: A lab and field study on the impact of shelf arrangement and assortment structure on consumer choices

Title: Healthy snacks at the checkout counter: A lab and field study on the impact of shelf arrangement and assortment structure on consumer choices Author's response to reviews Title: Healthy snacks at the checkout counter: A lab and field study on the impact of shelf arrangement and assortment structure on consumer choices Authors: Ellen van Kleef

More information

External Variables and the Technology Acceptance Model

External Variables and the Technology Acceptance Model Association for Information Systems AIS Electronic Library (AISeL) AMCIS 1995 Proceedings Americas Conference on Information Systems (AMCIS) 8-25-1995 External Variables and the Technology Acceptance Model

More information

Determinants of older adults intentions to vaccinate against influenza: a theoretical application

Determinants of older adults intentions to vaccinate against influenza: a theoretical application Journal of Public Health Vol. 28, No. 2, pp. 139 144 doi:10.1093/pubmed/fdl008 Advance Access Publication 26 April 2006 Determinants of older adults intentions to vaccinate against influenza: a theoretical

More information

Cardiac rehabilitation: The psychological changes that predict health outcome and healthy behaviour

Cardiac rehabilitation: The psychological changes that predict health outcome and healthy behaviour Psychology, Health & Medicine, February 2005; 10(1): 88 95 Cardiac rehabilitation: The psychological changes that predict health outcome and healthy behaviour SUSAN MICHIE 1, DARYL O CONNOR 2, JULIAN BATH

More information

Strategic Approaches to Continuing Medical Education: Applying the Transtheoretical Model and Diffusion of Innovation Theory

Strategic Approaches to Continuing Medical Education: Applying the Transtheoretical Model and Diffusion of Innovation Theory Strategic Approaches to Continuing Medical Education: Applying the Transtheoretical Model and Diffusion of Innovation Theory Brian Russell, PharmD, Gerard Maher, 1 James O. Prochaska, PhD, 2 Sara S. Johnson,

More information

SELF-EFFICACY AND THE MODERATE (OR LESS) DRINKER: A SURVIVOR S GUIDE FOR COPING WITH HEAVY DRINKING ON (OR NEAR) COLLEGE CAMPUSES

SELF-EFFICACY AND THE MODERATE (OR LESS) DRINKER: A SURVIVOR S GUIDE FOR COPING WITH HEAVY DRINKING ON (OR NEAR) COLLEGE CAMPUSES SELF-EFFICACY AND THE MODERATE (OR LESS) DRINKER: A SURVIVOR S GUIDE FOR COPING WITH HEAVY DRINKING ON (OR NEAR) COLLEGE CAMPUSES Ronald B. Anderson, Stan Richards School of Advertising & Public Relations,

More information

Attitude = Belief + Evaluation. TRA/TPB and HBM. Theory of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior. TRA: Constructs TRA/TPB

Attitude = Belief + Evaluation. TRA/TPB and HBM. Theory of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior. TRA: Constructs TRA/TPB and HBM Theory of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior Both focus on rational, cognitive decision-making processes adds the social context to the basic ideas of the HBM 2 TRA: Constructs Behavioral Intention

More information

Behavior change in noise-producing activities a model

Behavior change in noise-producing activities a model Behavior change in noise-producing activities a model Irène SCHLACHTER a, Tommaso MELONI a, Elisabeth LAUPER b, Stephanie MOSER b a Federal office for the environment FOEN 3003 Berne, SWITZERLAND, e-mail:

More information

Faculty Influence and Other Factors Associated with Student Membership in Professional Organizations 1

Faculty Influence and Other Factors Associated with Student Membership in Professional Organizations 1 Faculty Influence and Other Factors Associated with Student Membership in Professional Organizations 1 Marion K. Slack and John E. Murphy College of Pharmacy, The University of Arizona Tucson AZ 85721

More information

Effects of a Brief Intervention Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior on Leisure-Time Physical Activity Participation

Effects of a Brief Intervention Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior on Leisure-Time Physical Activity Participation 470 / Chatzisarantis and Hagger JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 27, 470-487 2005 Human Kinetics, Inc. Effects of a Brief Intervention Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior on Leisure-Time

More information

Author s response to reviews

Author s response to reviews Author s response to reviews Title: The validity of a professional competence tool for physiotherapy students in simulationbased clinical education: a Rasch analysis Authors: Belinda Judd (belinda.judd@sydney.edu.au)

More information

Communication for Change A S H O R T G U I D E T O S O C I A L A N D B E H A V I O R C H A N G E ( S B C C ) T H E O R Y A N D M O D E L S

Communication for Change A S H O R T G U I D E T O S O C I A L A N D B E H A V I O R C H A N G E ( S B C C ) T H E O R Y A N D M O D E L S Communication for Change A S H O R T G U I D E T O S O C I A L A N D B E H A V I O R C H A N G E ( S B C C ) T H E O R Y A N D M O D E L S 2 Why use theories and models? Answers to key questions Why a

More information

Decisional Balance and Its Relationship with Stages of Change in Exercise Behavior among Employees of Gorgan: A Cross-Sectional Study

Decisional Balance and Its Relationship with Stages of Change in Exercise Behavior among Employees of Gorgan: A Cross-Sectional Study Journal of Clinical and Basic Research (JCBR) Original Research Article Decisional Balance and Its Relationship with Stages of Change in Exercise Behavior among Employees of Gorgan: A Cross-Sectional Study

More information

Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE)

Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE) Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE) Eachus, P and Cassidy, SF Title Authors Type URL Published Date 2004 Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE) Eachus, P and Cassidy,

More information

Journal of Health Psychology

Journal of Health Psychology Journal of Health Psychology http://hpq.sagepub.com Protection Motivation Theory and Stages of Change in Sun Protective Behavior Steven Prentice-Dunn, Ben F. Mcmath and Robert J. Cramer J Health Psychol

More information

A Comparison of Several Goodness-of-Fit Statistics

A Comparison of Several Goodness-of-Fit Statistics A Comparison of Several Goodness-of-Fit Statistics Robert L. McKinley The University of Toledo Craig N. Mills Educational Testing Service A study was conducted to evaluate four goodnessof-fit procedures

More information

Testing an extended theory of planned behavior to predict young people s intentions to join a bone marrow donor registry

Testing an extended theory of planned behavior to predict young people s intentions to join a bone marrow donor registry Testing an extended theory of planned behavior to predict young people s intentions to join a bone marrow donor registry Author K. Hyde, Melissa, M. White, Katherine Published 2013 Journal Title Journal

More information

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document. Copyright 2015 Edizioni Luigi Pozzi. Archived with permission of the publisher.

The Singapore Copyright Act applies to the use of this document. Copyright 2015 Edizioni Luigi Pozzi. Archived with permission of the publisher. Title Author(s) Developing and evaluating utility of school-based intervention programs in promoting leisure-time physical activity: An application of the theory of planned behavior Nikos L. D. Chatzisarantis,

More information

Predicting and Understanding Mothers' Infant-Feeding Intentions and Behavior: Testing the Theory of Reasoned Action

Predicting and Understanding Mothers' Infant-Feeding Intentions and Behavior: Testing the Theory of Reasoned Action Predicting and Understanding Mothers' Infant-Feeding Intentions and Behavior: Testing the Theory of Reasoned Action A. S. R. Manstead Department of Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester, England

More information

Health Psychology. The Health Belief Model. Health Belief Model. The Theory of Reasoned Action

Health Psychology. The Health Belief Model. Health Belief Model. The Theory of Reasoned Action Health Psychology Psychology 46.339 (01) Summer 2007 Instructor: Dr. Fuschia Sirois Wednesday July 11: Lecture 3 Prep. Guide 2 Health Belief Model Health Threat Present? YES Behavior can reduce threat?

More information

This is a repository copy of Applying the Reasoned Action approach to understanding health protection and health risk behaviors.

This is a repository copy of Applying the Reasoned Action approach to understanding health protection and health risk behaviors. This is a repository copy of Applying the Reasoned Action approach to understanding health protection and health risk behaviors. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/123136/

More information

Questionnaire on Anticipated Discrimination (QUAD)(1): is a self-complete measure comprising 14 items

Questionnaire on Anticipated Discrimination (QUAD)(1): is a self-complete measure comprising 14 items Online Supplement Data Supplement for Clement et al. (10.1176/appi.ps.201300448) Details of additional measures included in the analysis Questionnaire on Anticipated Discrimination (QUAD)(1): is a self-complete

More information

School orientation and mobility specialists School psychologists School social workers Speech language pathologists

School orientation and mobility specialists School psychologists School social workers Speech language pathologists 2013-14 Pilot Report Senate Bill 10-191, passed in 2010, restructured the way all licensed personnel in schools are supported and evaluated in Colorado. The ultimate goal is ensuring college and career

More information

Title: Reliability and validity of the adolescent stress questionnaire in a sample of European adolescents - the HELENA study

Title: Reliability and validity of the adolescent stress questionnaire in a sample of European adolescents - the HELENA study Author's response to reviews Title: Reliability and validity of the adolescent stress questionnaire in a sample of European adolescents - the HELENA study Authors: Tineke De Vriendt (tineke.devriendt@ugent.be)

More information

ELEMENTARY TEACHERS SCIENCE SELF EFFICACY BELIEFS IN THE EAST AZERBAIJAN PROVINCE OF IRAN

ELEMENTARY TEACHERS SCIENCE SELF EFFICACY BELIEFS IN THE EAST AZERBAIJAN PROVINCE OF IRAN ELEMENTARY TEACHERS SCIENCE SELF EFFICACY BELIEFS IN THE EAST AZERBAIJAN PROVINCE OF IRAN Eskandar Fathi-Azar College of Education and Psychology, University of Tabriz, Iran This study measures the self-efficacy

More information

Social Norms about a Health Issue in Work Group Networks

Social Norms about a Health Issue in Work Group Networks Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 11621-11639; doi:10.3390/ijerph120911621 Article International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health ISSN 1660-4601 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph

More information

September 14, 2018 James O. Prochaska, Ph.D.

September 14, 2018 James O. Prochaska, Ph.D. More Effective and Inclusive Care by Combining Practices for Individual Patients and Entire Populations September 14, 2018 James O. Prochaska, Ph.D. Director and Professor Cancer Prevention Research Center

More information

STAGE MODELS OF BEHAVIOUR CHANGE

STAGE MODELS OF BEHAVIOUR CHANGE STAGE MODELS OF BEHAVIOUR CHANGE Falko F Sniehotta 1 & Robert Aunger 2 1 School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, UK (f.sniehotta@abdn.ac.uk) 2 Disease Control and Vector Biology Unit, London School

More information

Changing for Life: Using the Stages of Change to Support the Recovery Process

Changing for Life: Using the Stages of Change to Support the Recovery Process Changing for Life: Using the Stages of Change to Support the Recovery Process Sara S. Johnson, Ph.D. Senior Vice President, Research & Product Development sjohnson@prochange.com Assessing your Readiness.

More information