JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45,

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45,"

Transcription

1 JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45, NUMBER 3(FALL 2012) THE EFFECTS OF MATCHED STIMULATION AND RESPONSE INTERRUPTION AND REDIRECTION ON VOCAL STEREOTYPY JESSICA J. LOVE, CAIO F. MIGUEL, JONATHAN K. FERNAND, AND JILLIAN K. LABRIE CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, SACRAMENTO Stereotypy has been classified as repetitive behavior that does not serve any apparent function. Two procedures that have been found to reduce rates of vocal stereotypy effectively are response interruption and redirection (RIRD) and noncontingent access to matched stimulation (MS). The purpose of the current study was to evaluate the effects of RIRD alone, MS alone, and MS combined with RIRD. One participant s results suggested similar suppressive effects on vocal stereotypy across treatment conditions. For the second participant, a slightly greater suppression of stereotypy was associated with MS þ RIRD. In addition, both participants emitted a greater frequency of appropriate vocalizations in conditions with RIRD. Data suggest that the addition of MS might facilitate the implementation of RIRD in applied settings. Key words: autism, matched stimulation, response interruption and redirection, vocal stereotypy The term stereotypy is often used to classify either motor or vocal repetitive behavior that does not appear to serve an adaptive function (Lovaas, Newsom, & Hickman, 1987). Although stereotypy is not displayed exclusively by individuals with disabilities, an increased rate can be observed in this population, especially in those diagnosed with autism (American Psychiatric Association, 2005; Lewis & Bodfish, 1998; Repp & Barton, 1980). Stereotypy is typically targeted for reduction given that it may interfere with learning, compete with more functional responses, and be socially stigmatizing (e.g., Durand & Carr, 1987; Koegel & Covert, 1972; Matson, Kiely, & Bamburg, 1997; Repp & Barton, 1980). Vocal stereotypy specifically may compete with This study is based on a thesis submitted by the first author under the supervision of the second author to the Psychology Department at California State University, Sacramento in partial fulfillment of the requirements for an MA degree in Psychology: Applied Behavioral Analysis. Jessica Love is now at the University of Nevada, Las Vegas. We thank Timothy Fechter for his assistance with data collection and Tiffany Kodak for her editorial suggestions. Address correspondence to Caio Miguel, Department of Psychology, California State University, Sacramento, 6000 J St., Sacramento, California ( miguelc@csus.edu). doi: /jaba more adaptive forms of communication (e.g., mands) and also create disruptions for other people in the social environment (e.g., Athens, Vollmer, Sloman, & St. Peter Pipkin, 2008). Lovaas et al. (1987) suggested that stereotypical responding functions to provide sensory input to an individual. That is, it produces automatic reinforcement. Stereotypic behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement may be more difficult to treat because one does not have access to the exact source of reinforcement (Vollmer, 1994). Despite these challenges, interventions have emerged to treat automatically reinforced stereotypy. Two of the recently evaluated treatments are response interruption and redirection (RIRD; e.g., Ahearn, Clark, MacDonald, & Chung, 2007; Ahrens, Lerman, Kodak, Worsdell, & Keegan, 2011; Duffy- Cassella, Sidener, Sidener, & Progar, 2011; Liu-Gitz & Banda, 2010; Miguel, Clark, Tereshko, & Ahearn, 2009) and noncontingent access to matched stimulation (MS; e.g., Lanovaz, Fletcher, & Rapp, 2009; Rapp, 2007; Taylor, Hoch, & Weissman, 2005). Ahearn et al. (2007) evaluated RIRD with four participants who displayed automatically reinforced vocal stereotypy. During RIRD, the experimenter interrupted the participants vocal stereotypy and then redirected them to emit 549

2 550 JESSICA J. LOVE et al. appropriate vocalizations such as answering social questions. Results showed a decrease in vocal stereotypy for all four participants and an increase in the frequency of appropriate vocalizations for three of four participants. In a more applied extension, Liu-Gitz and Banda (2010) evaluated teacher-implemented RIRD in a natural classroom setting. Results showed significant reductions in vocal stereotypy and provided further evidence for the efficacy of RIRD in school settings. Recently, Ahrens et al. (2011) sought to isolate the mechanism that is responsible for the effects of RIRD by implementing a treatment integrity fading procedure modeled after Lerman and Iwata (1996) and Smith, Russo, and Le (1999). Results indicated that reductions in vocal stereotypy followed a pattern of responding indicative of positive punishment. Because vocal stereotypy may occur quite frequently, a high number of RIRD implementations may be required to achieve considerable reductions in behavior, which may prevent clinicians from adopting the procedure (Miguel et al., 2009). This limitation may be circumvented by potentially decreasing the motivating operation (MO) for engaging in stereotypy through the presentation of stimuli that produce the same hypothesized sensory consequence as the problem behavior (Piazza, Adelinis, Hanley, Goh, & Delia, 2000; Piazza et al., 1998). The addition of these stimuli (matched stimulation) to a treatment with RIRD may be a more efficient approach than RIRD alone, because decreasing the MO for the products of stereotypy may result in fewer implementations of RIRD. Piazza et al. (1998, 2000) investigated the effects of MS on pica, saliva manipulation, hand mouthing, jumping, and climbing, which were all maintained by automatic reinforcement. Participants received continuous access to items that produced either the same (matched) or different (unmatched) forms of stimulation. Results indicated a greater reduction in the target behaviors for most participants when they were given continuous and noncontingent access to items that were hypothesized to match the target behavior s sensory consequences. Fisher, Lindauer, Alterson, and Thompson (1998) also evaluated the effects of MS and unmatched stimulation on stereotypic object breaking plus tapping. Results indicated that matched toys alone decreased stereotypy for one of the two participants. Following the addition of response blocking to MS for one participant, rates of stereotypy further decreased and appropriate manipulation of the matched toys increased. Thus, previous literature on MS and noncontingent reinforcement highlights the potential benefits of supplementing a response-blocking procedure with MS. In light of the aforementioned findings, the present investigation sought to evaluate whether the use of MS would increase the effectiveness of RIRD. By lowering the MO for the products of stereotypy, the addition of MS could increase the feasibility of implementing RIRD in school or community settings if the inclusion of MS decreases the number of times RIRD must be implemented. In addition, the relative efficacy of MS and RIRD treatment components were evaluated both alone and as a treatment package. METHOD Participants, Setting, and Materials Participants were two children who had been diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder by independent psychologists. Both participants communicated vocally, could sit for a minimum of 5 min in the absence of aggression (e.g., hitting, kicking, punching) or self-injury (e.g., scratching, hitting, biting him- or herself), and engaged in vocal stereotypy that was at least partially maintained by automatic reinforcement, as determined by a functional analysis (Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & Richman, 1982/1994). Parents referred their children to participate in the study because their children s vocal stereotypy was considered to be intrusive in school or public settings.

3 MATCHED STIMULATION AND RIRD 551 Ivan was an 8-year-old boy who vocally communicated with others. He requested highly preferred items using I want. Ivan could tact colors and numbers. His fine motor skills were consistent with those of his typical peers. We conducted sessions in his bedroom (2 m by 3 m), which contained a bed, a small table and two chairs, various leisure items, and mirrored closet doors. Ivan did not take any medications throughout the duration of the study. Troy was a 9-year old boy who requested items using full sentences (e.g., I want Elmo, please ). He could also tact colors and identify numbers up to 30. Troy s parents reported that his general academic abilities were comparable to his peers. Troy did not take any medication for the duration of the study. We conducted sessions in a treatment room (2 m by 3 m) equipped with a table and two chairs, a bulletin board, a video camera, and toys. We conducted approximately three to four sessions per day, 2 to 3 days per week with both participants. Response Measurement Observers collected data on four dependent variables for each participant: vocal stereotypy, appropriate vocalizations, frequency of implementation of RIRD, and session length. For Ivan, vocal stereotypy was defined as any instance of noncontextual phrases or repetitions of noncontextual words, phrases, or sounds. It included noncontextual repetitions, defined as at least two occurrences of phonemes (two or more emissions of the same phoneme within a 5-s interval; e.g., mm mm or na na na ) or phrases (e.g., J is for jaguar ), delayed echolalia (e.g., We go to the doctor first, then Blockbuster ), lip popping, and repetitious sounds with a closed mouth (e.g., bbbbbb sound with mouth closed and lips vibrating). Sound effects in the absence of appropriate toys (e.g., shoo or vroom ), rhythmic breathing patterns (e.g., huh huh huh or heh heh heh ), and noncontextual repetitive blowing of air (e.g., blowing in the absence of candles) were also included. Troy s vocal stereotypy was defined as any instance of noncontextual or nonfunctional speech, including rhythmic or patterned breathing (e.g., huh huh huh, heh heh heh, or breathing inward through pursed lips) and single phonemes (e.g., mmm ). Repetitions of words, phonemes (e.g., kkk, tuh tuh tuh ), or noncontextual high-pitched squeals or squeaks, and reciting of movie or videogame scripts were also included. Ivan s and Troy s appropriate vocalizations included mands for items, attention, or breaks using the I want frame and tacts that included vocalizing the name of an item in its presence (e.g., tiger in the presence of a tiger figurine). For both participants, we did not score repetitive mands (e.g., I want break, I want break ) or tacts (e.g., tiger, tiger ) as vocal stereotypy unless the phrase was noncontextual (e.g., crocodile, crocodile when no crocodile was present). Furthermore, the repetitive tact was considered appropriate and contextual if the participant exhibited shared interaction with the experimenter. For example, if the participant looked at the tiger and looked at the experimenter while saying tiger during the second repetition of the word tiger, this behavior was scored as appropriate. We scored the above-mentioned examples of vocalizations as appropriate because it was assumed that joint attention with the experimenter during the vocalization might have served a social function. Moreover, if a participant repeated the same vocalization within 5 s (e.g., I want break, I want break ), we scored only one instance of an appropriate vocalization. RIRD implementation was defined as the experimenter saying the participant s name, the participant complying with three vocal imitation demands or social questions, and the experimenter delivering praise (e.g., Nice job talking ). We measured vocal stereotypy using duration recording. To calculate the percentage of stereotypy that occurred in each session, we divided the total number of seconds the participant engaged in vocal stereotypy by the total number of seconds in the session (i.e., 300 s) and converted this number to a percentage.

4 552 JESSICA J. LOVE et al. Observers did not record data on vocal stereotypy or appropriate vocalizations when the experimenter implemented RIRD. Furthermore, the experimenter stopped the session clock each time he or she implemented the procedure to ensure that time spent implementing RIRD was taken out of the 5-min total session time. Data on appropriate vocalizations and implementation of RIRD were collected using frequency recording and expressed as cumulative frequency across sessions. At the conclusion of the study, we calculated and compared the average number of implementations of RIRD per session during the conditions of MS þ RIRD and RIRD alone for each participant. In addition, averages for each condition were compared across participants. Interobserver Agreement A second trained observer scored data during at least 33% of all conditions by watching the video footage. We calculated agreement on occurrences of stereotypy using a time-window analysis method (Mudford, Martin, Hui, & Taylor, 2009), which involved a second-by-second comparison across the two observers for both occurrence and nonoccurrence of stereotypy. We scored an agreement if both observers agreed that stereotypy occurred or did not occur for a 1-s interval. We divided the number of agreements by the number of agreements plus disagreements and converted this number to a percentage. We also calculated interobserver agreement for appropriate vocalizations using the total agreement method by comparing the cumulative frequencies recorded by observers. The smaller total from one observer was divided by the larger total from the second observer and this number was converted to a percentage. Mean agreement for vocal stereotypy and appropriate vocalizations was 96% (range, 89% to 100%) and 95% (range, 84% to 100%), respectively. Mean agreement for appropriate vocalizations was 100%. Treatment Integrity We collected treatment integrity on implementation of RIRD and MS during a portion of sessions in each condition of the treatment evaluation. Correct implementation of RIRD was scored if the experimenter delivered RIRD within 2 s of vocal stereotypy and ensured that the participant complied with three consecutive demands in the absence of stereotypy. We calculated integrity during RIRD by dividing the total number of correct implementations of RIRD by the total number of opportunities to implement RIRD in the session. The quotient was converted to a percentage of overall treatment integrity for the implementation of RIRD. Treatment integrity was also calculated during conditions that included the delivery of MS using 5-s partial-interval recording. An interval was scored as containing MS if the toys were making sounds at any time during the 5-s interval. We divided the number of intervals containing MS by the total number of intervals for each session (i.e., 60) and converted the result to a percentage to obtain an overall percentage of integrity. Session percentages were then added and averaged to yield a percentage of overall integrity of MS for each participant. Across both participants, we evaluated treatment integrity for implementation of RIRD during 41% of sessions and for implementation of MS during 36% of sessions. Treatment integrity averaged 96% (range, 92% to 100%) for RIRD and 99% (range, 93% to 100%) for MS. PREEXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES We conducted a stimulus preference assessment, a functional analysis, RIRD probes, and a matched stimuli assessment prior to the experimental evaluation. Preference Assessment The experimenter conducted three sessions of a multiple-stimulus without replacement (MSWO) preference assessment for both matched and unmatched toys based on proce-

5 MATCHED STIMULATION AND RIRD 553 dures described by DeLeon and Iwata (1996). Toys classified as matched were those that made sound, and toys classified as unmatched were toys that did not make sound. The presence or absence of these toys was planned for each condition, because vocal stereotypy was hypothesized to be maintained by auditory stimulation. Parents completed a modified Reinforcer Assessment for Individuals with Severe Disabilities (RAISD; Fisher, Piazza, Bowman, & Amari, 1996) to identify some of the items included in the preference assessments. Along with the items identified via the RAISD, the experimenter conducted an MSWO to identify additional items. Two preference assessments were conducted, one for 10 toys that produced sound and one for 10 toys that did not produce sound. Thereafter, one MSWO per day was conducted with the seven top-ranked items. Depending on the type of sessions conducted during a day, sometimes the experimenter conducted two preference assessments (one for matched and one for unmatched). The toys that produced sound were later tested (MS assessment) to see whether they would compete with the auditory product (i.e., automatic reinforcer) of vocal stereotypy. Items identified for inclusion in functional analysis sessions were highly and moderately preferred toys that did not produce sound. The use of these toys served to control for potential suppressive effects that sound may have exerted on stereotypy levels (e.g., Rapp, 2006, 2007). Anecdotal observations indicated that the highly preferred items used during sessions of the functional analysis did not suppress stereotypy. The experimenter randomly selected four items identified as moderately and highly preferred from the daily MSWO for baseline and treatment sessions to prevent potential satiation effects. Functional Analysis We conducted a functional analysis of vocal stereotypy according to procedures described by Roscoe, Carreau, MacDonald, and Pence (2008) and Athens et al. (2008). We rotated among seven highly and moderately preferred toys from the initial MSWO during sessions, with three toys per functional analysis session (that required toys). Toys were rotated to prevent satiation. The toys included in the previous session were not made available in subsequent sessions unless the participant requested an item or indicated sustained preference (e.g., continually looking at the item or reaching for it), in which case he received access to it during the next session. We conducted three conditions in the functional analysis in a fixed order: no consequence, attention, and demand. We conducted twice as many no-consequence sessions as demand and attention sessions, because we hypothesized that stereotypy was maintained by automatic reinforcement. An extended no-consequence condition was conducted for Ivan only, given that his performance was low and undifferentiated during the last few sessions of the multielement functional analysis. Sessions lasted 5 min. No consequence. Participants had continuous access to two toys identified as moderately preferred from the initial MSWO preference assessment. We included moderately preferred toys so that they would not compete with stereotypy (Ahearn, Clark, DeBar, & Florentino, 2005). The experimenter withheld social interactions by sitting in a chair and looking away from the participant. The person filming the session was across from the participant and also did not interact with him. No programmed consequences were provided for appropriate vocalizations. Attention. The participant and experimenter sat in a room in which two moderately preferred toys (that did not produce sound) were available on a table. Contingent on vocal stereotypy, the experimenter responded by lightly touching the participant on the shoulder and saying, Don t do that; it s loud or Please be quiet. Demand. The experimenter rotated the presentation of four tasks similar to those presented during the participant s school or

6 554 JESSICA J. LOVE et al. day-treatment program. We selected unmastered (e.g., less than 80% correct) and nonvocal tasks based on caregiver report. Demands for Ivan included hanging a shirt on a coat hanger, tying a shoe, folding a shirt, and buttoning a shirt. Demands for Troy included solving threedigit multiplication problems, tying a shoe, catching a ball, and putting pennies into a box with one hand. The experimenter used a leastto-most prompting hierarchy for all task presentations. Contingent on stereotypy, the experimenter said, Okay, you don t have to and removed the task for 15 s. No programmed consequences were provided for appropriate vocalizations. MS Assessment The purpose of this assessment was to demonstrate that the items used in conditions containing MS would suppress vocal stereotypy. We conducted the assessment using a multielement design and rapidly alternated among three conditions: baseline, toys with sound, and toys without sound. We alternated each baseline session with one of the toy conditions to avoid carryover effects. During Phase 1, we alternated between baseline sessions and toys with sound (i.e., matched stimulation). During Phase 2, we alternated between baseline and toys without sound. Phase 1 was then reinstated to replicate the effects of matched toys. Each session lasted 5 min. Baseline. The purpose of this condition was to evaluate levels of vocal stereotypy in the absence of toys and social interaction. The experimenter sat in the room with the participant but did not interact with him in any way. No leisure items or materials were present. Although sessions began with the participant seated in a chair, he was free to move about the room, and the experimenter did not redirect him to sit. Toys with sound. The purpose of this condition was to measure levels of vocal stereotypy in the presence of toys that produced sound, because we wanted to determine whether the auditory stimulation matched or effectively substituted for the sensory consequences typically produced by engaging in vocal stereotypy (Rapp, 2007). Sessions included two of four preferred items (identified in an MSWO). Thus, we evaluated two sets of toys. During the session, the participant sat at a table or on the floor and had access to a set of toys. Following 2 s without toy activation, the experimenter manipulated the item to produce auditory stimulation. This ensured consistent delivery of MS throughout the condition. After demonstrating that the presence of both sets of toys reduced stereotypy, the experimenter rotated the two sets during treatment conditions that included MS. Toys without sound. The purpose of this condition was to evaluate vocal stereotypy levels in the presence of toys that did not produce sound. We used the same toys as those in the toys-with-sound condition but removed the batteries (Taylor et al., 2005). This condition sought to isolate the effects of the sound produced by the toys on vocal stereotypy. RIRD Probes Prior to treatment, an assessment probe was conducted to identify appropriate vocal demands for use during conditions containing RIRD. Prior to the study, we conducted probes of tasks that required vocalizations across two different experimenters and two different settings for a minimum criterion of 89% accuracy. The results indicated that both participants were most consistent and accurate with vocal imitation of two-syllable words. From a master list of 15 words, we created three lists that contained 10 words and randomly rotated these lists across all sessions (lists available from the second author). We used the same word lists for both participants because of similar performances during the RIRD probes. The experimenter presented all words from the lists created during the probe session. The purpose of these lists was to ensure demand consistency across all experimenters. The experimenter randomly determined the order of words from each list by assigning

7 MATCHED STIMULATION AND RIRD 555 numbers to each item and then drawing numbers from a hat. The list was placed out of the participant s view. A different list was used each day. Approximately three to four sessions were conducted per day. Procedure During all conditions, the participant had access to two toys (matched or unmatched) and had the opportunity to interact with the experimenter. The experimenter smiled and visually attended to the participant while he played, but did not say anything unless the participant emitted an appropriate vocalization. For example, if the participant said, Dinosaur! while playing with a dinosaur magnet, the experimenter responded by saying, You re right, that is a dinosaur! Nice talking! Baseline and MS-alone conditions lasted exactly 5 min. RIRD and MS þ RIRD conditions were longer than 5 min depending on the frequency of implementation of RIRD. There was no session cap for conditions with RIRD. If the participant left his chair at any point during a session in any condition, the experimenter provided a gestural prompt by pointing towards or tapping the seat of the chair. If the participant complied with sitting in the chair, the experimenter delivered praise. If he did not sit within 5 s of the prompt, the experimenter provided a vocal prompt, [Name], sit down please. Compliance resulted in praise. If the participant did not comply with the vocal prompt, the experimenter physically guided the participant back to his chair by touching his back and walking with him back to the chair. The two toys included in baseline and RIRD-alone sessions were identified as moderately preferred from the preference assessment and did not produce sound. We selected moderately preferred toys because previous research has shown that highly preferred items (although unmatched in stimulation) could occasion reductions in vocal stereotypy (Ahearn et al., 2005). The participant had the opportunity to manipulate only one toy at a time. He could gain access to the other toy by requesting it. All conditions, with the exception of baseline, included prompts to engage in a mand if a participant was looking at a toy (indicating interest) but did not ask for it. To facilitate discrimination between MS þ RIRD and RIRD-alone conditions, the experimenter wore a blue shirt during sessions in which matched stimulation was available (paired with the MS þ RIRD and MS-alone condition) and a black shirt during sessions in which MS was not available (paired with the RIRD-alone condition). The experimenter also paired the instruction We re going to play with toys that make noise with the blue shirt. With the black shirt, the experimenter paired the instruction, We re going to play with quiet toys. During baseline, the experimenter wore any color shirt other than blue or black and did not provide any instructions. Baseline. The participant had access to two unmatched toys in a room with the experimenter. No programmed consequences were provided contingent on vocal stereotypy. We conducted at least three sessions or continued conducting sessions until a steady state of vocal stereotypy was observed. If a steady state was not achieved within 10 sessions, then the next condition was introduced. This was also the criterion for introduction of all subsequent conditions. MS þ RIRD. The procedures included those described by Rapp (2007) for MS and Ahearn et al. (2007) for RIRD. The participant had continuous access to one of two toys that produced auditory stimulation. The experimenter rotated the two sets of toys when conducting more than one session per day. If the participant stopped engaging with the toy for longer than 2 s, the experimenter manipulated the toy to produce sound. The experimenter activated the toy approximately five times per session; however, in some sessions the toy was never activated.

8 556 JESSICA J. LOVE et al. Contingent on vocal stereotypy, the experimenter removed the toy, interrupted the participant s stereotypy, and redirected the participant by saying the boy s name in a neutral tone of voice before issuing a demand to engage in a vocal response (as described by Ahearn et al., 2007). The removal of the toys during RIRD functioned both to increase the likelihood of participants attending to demands during RIRD and to increase the MO for the emission of the request. If the toy produced sound at the time of RIRD implementation, the experimenter did not silence the toy. A secondary observer stopped the session clock during implementation of RIRD. The vocal demands during RIRD were a series of three vocal imitations of words (e.g., Troy say hopscotch, say binder, say sofa ) from a previously generated list of words. If the child engaged in stereotypy between appropriate responses, the experimenter continued to present instructions until he emitted three consecutive appropriate vocalizations without vocal stereotypy. If he emitted a mand during RIRD, the experimenter acknowledged the response and replied with, in a minute, we can have, and then continued to deliver instructions. After the participant complied with three consecutive instructions without engaging in vocal stereotypy, the experimenter delivered verbal praise (e.g., Nice job talking ) and started the session clock. In addition, the experimenter returned the leisure item to the participant contingent on a mand. We required the participant to mand to increase the likelihood that he contacted reinforcement for appropriate vocalizations (Ahearn et al., 2007). However, the experimenter activated a toy to produce auditory stimulation (MS) if the participant did not mand for an item or activate it within 2 s after completion of RIRD. If the participant engaged in vocal stereotypy when the experimenter returned the item, the experimenter immediately removed the item and reimplemented RIRD. No consequences were provided for behaviors other than vocal stereotypy and appropriate vocalizations (e.g., throwing items, hitting the table). Sessions ended when 5 min elapsed (timed by the session clock). RIRD alone. The procedures were similar to those described in the MS þ RIRD condition, although the two moderately preferred leisure items included in the sessions did not produce any sound. The purpose of this condition was to evaluate the effects of RIRD alone on vocal stereotypy when the MO for stereotypy was assumed to be present. We assumed the MO was present, because the toys did not make sounds. Therefore, sounds produced by toys did not have the opportunity to function as an abolishing operation for the auditory products of stereotypy. MS alone. The purpose of this condition was to evaluate levels of stereotypy in the presence of toys that produced MS (auditory stimulation). This condition was similar to the MS þ RIRD condition; however, RIRD was not implemented. That is, the participants had access to one of two toys that produced sound throughout the session, and the experimenter activated a toy if 2 s elapsed without the production of MS. No programmed consequences were delivered contingent on stereotypy. Experimental Design We compared the effects of RIRD alone, MS alone, and MS þ RIRD on vocal stereotypy and appropriate vocalizations in a multitreatment reversal design (Holcomb & Wolery, 1994). For both participants, we introduced MS þ RIRD after the initial baseline. We counterbalanced the introduction of MS alone and RIRD alone across participants. The experimenter completed the evaluation with the reinstatement of the treatment associated with the greatest suppression of vocal stereotypy. Social Validity We obtained two seperate measures of social validity from both participants parents at the

9 MATCHED STIMULATION AND RIRD 557 Figure 1. The percentage of vocal stereotypy during Ivan s and Troy s functional analysis. conclusion of the study. One measure evaluated parents opinion of the RIRD procedure, and the other measure evaluated validity of the MS procedure. The experimenter provided each measure after the parent viewed a brief video clip of an RIRD or MS treatment session. Scores for each question ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to5(strongly agree). A score of 3 was neutral. RESULTS The results of the functional analyses for Ivan and Troy are depicted in Figure 1. For both participants, vocal stereotypy occurred in the absence of social consequences. Ivan s vocal stereotypy persisted under extended exposure to the no-consequence condition, and Troy s vocal stereotypy was highest in the no-consequence condition during the second half of the

10 558 JESSICA J. LOVE et al. Figure 2. The percentage of vocal stereotypy during the matched stimuli assessment for Ivan and Troy for Toy Sets 1 (top) and 2 (bottom). functional analysis. Results from both participants functional analyses suggested that vocal stereotypy was maintained by automatic reinforcement. Figure 2 depicts the percentage of vocal stereotypy during the MS assessment for both Ivan and Troy. Results showed reduced stereotypy when toys with batteries were present. Figure 3 depicts the percentage of Ivan s vocal stereotypy during baseline and treatment conditions. Vocal stereotypy occurred at moderate levels during the initial baseline (M ¼ 37%), and Ivan emitted appropriate vocalizations infrequently (M ¼ 0.7). During the MS þ RIRD condition, vocal stereotypy rapidly decreased to low levels (M ¼ 2.1%, M ¼ 5.8%) and appropriate vocalizations increased but were highly variable (M ¼ 2.6, M ¼ 7) during both exposures to this condition. Vocal stereotypy was maintained at lower levels during the MS-alone condition (M ¼ 5.2%,

11 MATCHED STIMULATION AND RIRD 559 Figure 3. The percentage of vocal stereotypy and the frequency of appropriate vocalizations during treatment for Ivan. BL ¼ baseline; MS ¼ matched stimulation; RIRD ¼ response interruption and redirection; AV ¼ appropriate vocalization. M ¼ 11.8%), although appropriate vocalizations decreased to baseline levels (M ¼ 0.8, M ¼ 0.5) during both phases of this condition. RIRD alone was associated with increased levels of appropriate vocalizations (M ¼ 4.6, M ¼ 6, M ¼ 3.9) and low levels of vocal stereotypy (M ¼ 5%, M ¼ 3%, M ¼ 4.3%) during the three exposures to this condition. Ivan s final treatment phase was RIRD alone because this treatment was associated with moderate rates of appropriate vocalizations and low rates of stereotypy, comparable to the MS þ RIRD condition. Also, Ivan did not have regular access to toys producing sound in other environments (e.g., school), thereby decreasing the likelihood of parents or teachers implementing MS þ RIRD. Figure 4 depicts the percentage of vocal stereotypy for Troy during baseline and treatment conditions. Vocal stereotypy occurred at moderate levels during the initial baseline (M ¼ 16%), and he infrequently emitted appropriate vocalizations (M ¼ 1.3). During the MS þ RIRD condition, stereotypy was low (M ¼ 1%, M ¼ 0.7%, M ¼ 1.5%) and appropriate vocalizations increased (M ¼ 3.8, M ¼ 3.3, M ¼ 6.75) during the three phases of this condition. Stereotypy occurred at a slightly higher level during RIRD alone (M ¼ 6.7%) compared to MS alone (M ¼ 2.4%). A higher

12 560 JESSICA J. LOVE et al. Figure 4. The percentage of vocal stereotypy and frequency of appropriate vocalizations during treatment for Troy. BL ¼ baseline; MS ¼ matched stimulation; RIRD ¼ response interruption and redirection; AV ¼ appropriate vocalization. frequency of appropriate vocalizations was associated with RIRD alone (M ¼ 16.6) compared to MS alone (M ¼ 4.4). Troy s final treatment phase was MS þ RIRD because this condition was associated with the lowest frequency of implementations of RIRD (see further description below) and a moderate frequency of appropriate vocalizations. Figure 5 (top) displays the average number of occasions in which the experimenter implemented RIRD per session for each participant. Ivan s frequency of RIRD implementations was relatively equal across conditions. The MS þ RIRD condition averaged 4.5 implementations, and the RIRD-alone condition was slightly higher with an average of 5.8 implementations. We observed a larger discrepancy among treatments for Troy. The MS þ RIRD condition was associated with a considerably lower frequency of implementations, averaging 2.3 per session. In comparison, the RIRD-alone condition averaged 15 implementations per session. Figure 5 (bottom) displays the average session length for the MS þ RIRD and RIRD-alone conditions for each participant. The horizontal line at the 300-s mark indicates the programmed amount of time for the session. The portion of the bar above the line indicates the average amount of extra session time in each condition. Ivan s treatment conditions had similar session lengths. Overall, both partici-

13 MATCHED STIMULATION AND RIRD 561 Figure 5. The average frequency of response interruption and redirection (RIRD) implementations (top) and the average session length in seconds (bottom) per participant during RIRD alone and matched stimulation (MS) þ RIRD conditions. The area above the horizontal line indicates the average time required to implement the RIRD procedure. pants session lengths did not display any decreasing trends throughout the experiment. Table 1 displays the questions and average ratings across both parents on the social validity questionnaire. Overall, parents rated both the RIRD and MS procedures favorably. Parents were also asked to circle the name of the preferred procedure or none at all. If they preferred both procedures equally, they could circle both procedures. Both parents indicated a preference for MS þ RIRD. One parent indicated that she would like to use the combined procedure at home and school. DISCUSSION Our results indicated that RIRD decreased stereotypy and increased appropriate vocalizations for both participants. These findings are consistent with previous studies on RIRD (e.g., Ahearn et al., 2007; Liu-Gitz & Banda, 2010; Miguel et al., 2009). Both participants also emitted a higher frequency of appropriate

14 562 JESSICA J. LOVE et al. Table 1 Social Validity Questions and Average Parent Ratings for RIRD and Matched Stimulation Average rating Question RIRD MS 1. I liked the vocal RIRD þ MS procedure If trained, I could easily implement the vocal RIRD þ MS procedure in my home I am satisfied with my child s response to the vocal RIRD þ MS procedure Vocal RIRD þ MS reduced my child s stereotypy during the session I thought the vocal RIRD þ MS procedure was intrusive Vocal RIRD þ MS is a socially appropriate intervention method for my child I think vocal RIRD þ MS improved my child s rate of appropriate vocalizations during the session My child will benefit in the long run from the procedures implemented in this study Vocal RIRD þ MS produced negative emotional reactions in my child I feel vocal RIRD þ MS produced little change in my child s stereotypy rates during session. 3 3 Note. Items were rated on a Likert-type scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 5(strongly agree). Items 5, 9, and 10 were reverse coded. vocalizations in conditions that contained RIRD. In addition, we observed decreased levels of vocal stereotypy for both participants during conditions that contained MS. The observed reduction in vocal stereotypy in the presence of MS may have been a result of the toys auditory stimulation functioning as an abolishing operation for the product of vocal stereotypy. These findings support results obtained by Rapp (2006) that stereotypy is sensitive to the manipulation of MOs. RIRD alone and MS alone produced similar outcomes for both participants. For Troy, MS þ RIRD produced lower levels of stereotypy with a shorter session time and considerably fewer implementations of RIRD than RIRD alone. For Ivan, the treatment conditions were associated with similar levels of stereotypy, session lengths, and number of RIRD implementations. The comparable suppressive effects of RIRD alone and MS þ RIRD on Ivan s stereotypy may be attributed to the inclusion of RIRD. Moreover, he engaged in fewer appropriate vocalizations during MS alone than during conditions that contained RIRD. The increase in vocalizations during RIRD may be related to the toy-removal component embedded in our RIRD procedure. This component may have functioned as a conditioned transitive MO (Michael, 2003), establishing the value of the removed toy as a reinforcer and increasing the probability of appropriate requests. In addition, toy removal may have functioned as punishment (i.e., response cost) for inappropriate vocalizations (e.g., Falcomata, Roane, Hovanetz, & Kettering, 2004). In contrast, Troy s results suggested that MS þ RIRD was associated with the most positive treatment outcome. These effects are consistent with O Reilly et al. (2006, 2007, 2008), who found that the addition of noncontingent reinforcement increased the effectiveness of procedures designed to suppress problem behavior. Troy s results also are consistent with those of Fisher et al. (1998), who found that MS plus response blocking was more effective for reducing one participant s stereotypy than MS alone. This study adds to the literature evaluating MS in conjunction with response blocking. Specifically, the reduction in vocal stereotypy during MS þ RIRD and MS conditions was notable, because stereotypy could still occur during the manipulation of matched items. Participants in Fisher et al. (1998) had access to matched toys, but manipulating them was incompatible with their topographies of stereotypy. One limitation of RIRD is the effort that may be necessary to implement it consistently in applied settings. Troy s results indicate that the addition of MS to RIRD may lower the number of implementations needed to suppress stereotypy, which may increase the likelihood

15 MATCHED STIMULATION AND RIRD 563 that caregivers will implement RIRD consistently. Overall, the addition of MS may address a possible limitation of RIRD and make it more user friendly. However, MS requires intervention agents to identify stimuli that compete with stereotypy, and providing continuous access to toys may not be possible during all classroom activities. Thus, additional research is needed to evaluate the feasibility of implementing MS þ RIRD in more natural settings. Several additional limitations warrant consideration. First, because RIRD could be conceptualized as a treatment package in this study, it is difficult to discern which component of RIRD (e.g., vocal demands, response cost) was responsible for the change in behavior. Results of more recent RIRD studies (e.g., Ahrens et al., 2011; Duffy-Casella et al., 2011) indicate that RIRD functions as positive punishment. Toy removal contingent on stereotypy may function as negative punishment. In addition, Ivan s increased frequency of appropriate vocalizations under conditions with RIRD seemed to be correlated with the number of times the response-cost procedure was implemented. Specifically, the more times response cost was implemented, the more times he manded to get the item back. Response cost was not included in the MS-alone condition. The omission of response cost during this condition may have provided fewer opportunities for Ivan to emit mands, which may explain the observed decrease in appropriate vocalizations. Future studies may evaluate the use of response cost in all conditions. Second, we did not collect data on appropriate object manipulation. Anecdotally, participants manipulated the toys themselves during most of the session. Although experimenter activation of toys ensured consistency across sessions and conditions, adults may not always be available in natural settings to activate toys if the child does not do so independently. Finally, it may be helpful to examine more naturalistic demands during RIRD instead of using vocal imitation of arbitrary words. Examples might include taking the toy the child currently is playing with (e.g., a tiger toy) and prompting the appropriate tact (e.g., say tiger, or say yellow tiger ) More relevant demands may increase the likelihood that appropriate vocalizations will come under the control of the appropriate stimuli in the environment. Additional research on MS and RIRD is necessary to examine the long-term effects of these interventions. It would be helpful to gain information on whether post-rird sessions contained higher levels of stereotypy compared to presession levels. This information may be used to identify the utility of these procedures in natural settings. REFERENCES Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., DeBar, R., & Florentino, C. (2005). On the role of preference in response competition. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., MacDonald, R. P. F., & Chung, B. I. (2007). Assessing and treating vocal stereotypy in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, Ahrens, E. N., Lerman, D. C., Kodak, T., Worsdell, A. S., & Keegan, C. (2011). Further evaluation of response interruption and redirection as treatment for stereotypic behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, American Psychiatric Association. (2005). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Arlington, VA: Author. Athens, E. S., Vollmer, T. R., Sloman, K. N., & St. Peter Pipkin, C. (2008). An analysis of vocal stereotypy and therapist fading. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, DeLeon, I. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Evaluation of a multiple-stimulus presentation format for assessing reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, Duffy-Cassella, M., Sidener, T. M., Sidener, D. W., & Progar, P. R. (2011). Response interruption and redirection for vocal stereotypy in children with autism: A systematic replication. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1987). Social influences on self-stimulatory behavior: Analysis and treatment applications. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, Falcomata, T. S., Roane, H. S., Hovanetz, A. N., & Kettering, T. L. (2004). An evaluation of response cost in the treatment of inappropriate vocalizations

16 564 JESSICA J. LOVE et al. maintained by automatic reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, Fisher, W. W., Lindauer, S. E., Alterson, C. J., & Thompson, R. H. (1998). Assessment and treatment of destructive behavior maintained by stereotypic object manipulation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., & Amari, A. (1996). Integrating caregiver report with a systematic choice assessment to enhance reinforcer identification. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 10, Holcomb, A., & Wolery, M. (1994). Comparative single subject research: Description of designs and discussion of problems. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 14, Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3 20, 1982) Koegel, R. L., & Covert, A. (1972). The relationship of self-stimulation to learning in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, Lanovaz, M. J., Fletcher, S. E., & Rapp, J. T. (2009). Using relative measures to identify stimuli that alter immediate and subsequent levels of vocal stereotypy: A further analysis of functionally matched stimulation. Behavior Modification, 33, Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). A methodology for distinguishing between extinction and punishment effects associated with response blocking. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, Lewis, M., & Bodfish, J. (1998). Repetitive behavior disorders in autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 4, Liu-Gitz, L., & Banda, D. R. (2010). A replication of the RIRD strategy to decrease vocal stereotypy in a child with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 25, Lovaas, O. I., Newsom, C., & Hickman, C. (1987). Selfstimulatory behavior and perceptual reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, Matson, J. L., Kiely, S. L., & Bamburg, J. W. (1997). The effect of stereotypies on adaptive skills as assessed with the DASH-II and Vineland adaptive adaptive behavior scales. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18, Michael, J. L. (2003). Concepts and principles of behavior analysis (rev. ed.). Kalamazoo, MI: Association for Behavior Analysis International. Miguel, C. M., Clark, K., Tereshko, L., & Ahearn, W. H. (2009). The effects of response interruption and redirection and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (sertraline) on vocal stereotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, Mudford, O. C., Martin, N. T., Hui, J. K. Y., & Taylor, S. A. (2009). Assessing observer accuracy in continuous recording of rate and duration: Three algorithms compared. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, O Reilly, M., Edrisinha, C., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., Cannella, H., Machalicek, W., et al. (2007). Manipulating the evocative and abative effects of an establishing operation: Influences on challenging behavior during classroom instruction. Behavioral Interventions, 22, O Reilly, M. F., Sigafoos, J., Edrisinha, C., Lancioni, G., Cannela, H., Choi, H. Y., et al. (2006). A preliminary examination of the evocative effects of the establishing operation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, O Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., Rispoli, M., Lang, R., Chan, J., et al. (2008). Manipulating the behavior-altering effect of the motivating operation: Examination of the influence on challenging behavior during leisure activities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 29, Piazza, C. C., Adelinis, J. D., Hanley, G. P., Goh, H. L., & Delia, M. D. (2000). An evaluation of the effects of matched stimuli on behaviors maintained by automatic reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., LeBlanc, L. A., Worsdell, A. S., Lindauer, S. E., et al. (1998). Treatment of pica through multiple analyses of its reinforcing functions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, Rapp, J. T. (2006). Toward an empirical method for identifying matched stimulation for automatically reinforced behavior: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, Rapp, J. T. (2007). Further evaluation of methods to identify matched stimulation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, Repp, A. C., & Barton, L. E. (1980). Naturalistic observations of institutionalized retarded persons: A comparison of licensure decisions and behavioral observations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, Roscoe, E. M., Carreau, A., MacDonald, J., & Pence, S. T. (2008). Further evaluation of leisure items in the attention condition of functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, Smith, R. G., Russo, L., & Le, D. D. (1999). Distinguishing between extinction and punishment effects of response blocking: A replication. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, Taylor, B. A., Hoch, H., & Weissman, M. (2005). The analysis and treatment of vocal stereotypy in a child with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 20, Vollmer, T. R. (1994). The concept of automatic reinforcement: Implications for behavioral research in developmental disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 15, Received February 28, 2011 Final acceptance February 21, 2012 Action Editor, Tiffany Kodak

Response Interruption and Redirection (RIRD) as a Behavioral Intervention for Vocal Stereotypy: A Systematic Review

Response Interruption and Redirection (RIRD) as a Behavioral Intervention for Vocal Stereotypy: A Systematic Review Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 2018, 53(1), 33 43 Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities Response Interruption and Redirection (RIRD) as a Behavioral Intervention

More information

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2011, 44, 903 907 NUMBER 4(WINTER 2011) DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL MARCUS AUTISM CENTER AND EMORY

More information

A Thesis Presented. Delna Bharucha. The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology. In partial fulfillment of the requirements

A Thesis Presented. Delna Bharucha. The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology. In partial fulfillment of the requirements Evaluating the Use of a Multiple Schedule for Identifying Treatment and Motivational Effects A Thesis Presented By Delna Bharucha The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial

More information

Increasing Independent Task Engagement in an Individual. with Autism. A thesis presented. Jessica Paredes

Increasing Independent Task Engagement in an Individual. with Autism. A thesis presented. Jessica Paredes Increasing Independent Task Engagement in an Individual with Autism A thesis presented by Jessica Paredes The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial fulfillment of the requirements

More information

Evaluation of Salient Stimulus Cues During. Brief Functional Analyses in a Classroom Setting. Kelly L. McConnell. B.S., University of Florida

Evaluation of Salient Stimulus Cues During. Brief Functional Analyses in a Classroom Setting. Kelly L. McConnell. B.S., University of Florida Running head: SALIENT STIMULUS CUES Evaluation of Salient Stimulus Cues During Brief Functional Analyses in a Classroom Setting by Kelly L. McConnell B.S., University of Florida Submitted in partial fulfillment

More information

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition A Thesis Presented By Robert Mark Grant In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science

More information

The Effect of Differential Reinforcement and Response Cost on Vocal Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Lauren K. Palmer

The Effect of Differential Reinforcement and Response Cost on Vocal Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Lauren K. Palmer i The Effect of Differential Reinforcement and Response Cost on Vocal Stereotypy A Thesis Presented by Lauren K. Palmer The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial fulfillment

More information

e-newsletter ABA Literature Summary topic : MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND COM- MUNICATION ...

e-newsletter ABA Literature Summary topic : MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND COM- MUNICATION ... ABA Literature Summary e-newsletter topic : MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND COM- MUNICATION March 2013 ISSUE 22............ Topic : Interventions for Stereotypy for Individuals with Autism by Laura Garrett, BA,

More information

2005 The New England Center for Children, Inc. (NECC); All Rights Reserved; No portion of this work is to be reproduced or distributed without the

2005 The New England Center for Children, Inc. (NECC); All Rights Reserved; No portion of this work is to be reproduced or distributed without the 2005 The New England Center for Children, Inc. (NECC); All Rights Reserved; No portion of this work is to be reproduced or distributed without the express written permission of NECC. What is autistic behavior?

More information

Szu-Yin Chu Assistant Professor, Dept. of Early Childhood Education, National Taitung University. Sonia Baker School Counselor, Taylor High School

Szu-Yin Chu Assistant Professor, Dept. of Early Childhood Education, National Taitung University. Sonia Baker School Counselor, Taylor High School 特殊教育研究學刊民 100,36 卷 1 期,109-127 頁.109. Effects of Noncontingent Reinforcement Plus Differential Reinforcement of Alternative Behavior and Response Interruption and Redirection on a Child s Vocal Stereotypy

More information

Running head: UTILITY OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS 1. An Evaluation of the Relative Utility of Two Demand Assessments

Running head: UTILITY OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS 1. An Evaluation of the Relative Utility of Two Demand Assessments Running head: UTILITY OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS 1 An Evaluation of the Relative Utility of Two Demand Assessments for Identifying Negative Reinforcers A Thesis Presented By Carly Cornelius In Partial fulfillment

More information

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45, NUMBER 1(SPRING 2012)

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45, NUMBER 1(SPRING 2012) JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45, 107 120 NUMBER 1(SPRING 2012) THE EFFECTS OF VERBAL OPERANT TRAINING AND RESPONSE INTERRUPTION AND REDIRECTION ON APPROPRIATE AND INAPPROPRIATE VOCALIZATIONS

More information

Expanding Functional Analysis of Automatically Reinforced Behavior Using a Three-Component Multiple-Schedule

Expanding Functional Analysis of Automatically Reinforced Behavior Using a Three-Component Multiple-Schedule EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 11, 17-27 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2010) 17 Expanding Functional Analysis of Automatically Reinforced Behavior Using a Three-Component Multiple-Schedule Marc J. Lanovaz

More information

Functional Communication Training with Demand Fading. To Reduce Self-Injurious Behavior. Introduction

Functional Communication Training with Demand Fading. To Reduce Self-Injurious Behavior. Introduction Functional Communication Training with Demand Fading To Reduce Self-Injurious Behavior Introduction Applied behavior analysts have adopted a two phase model of behavioral assessment and treatment which

More information

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF EXCESSIVE STRAIGHTENING AND DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR IN AN ADOLESCENT DIAGNOSED WITH AUTISM DAVID E. KUHN

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF EXCESSIVE STRAIGHTENING AND DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR IN AN ADOLESCENT DIAGNOSED WITH AUTISM DAVID E. KUHN JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2009, 42, 355 360 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 2009) ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF EXCESSIVE STRAIGHTENING AND DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR IN AN ADOLESCENT DIAGNOSED WITH AUTISM DAVID E.

More information

NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN NORTHEASTERN UNIVERSITY

NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN NORTHEASTERN UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2009, 42, 425 446 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 2009) RELATIVE CONTRIBUTIONS OF THREE DESCRIPTIVE METHODS: IMPLICATIONS FOR BEHAVIORAL ASSESSMENT SACHA T. PENCE, EILEEN M. ROSCOE,

More information

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2007, 40, 571 576 NUMBER 3(FALL 2007) ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY MARCUS

More information

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE AND KRIS M. KEENEY GWINNETT COUNTY PUBLIC SCHOOLS, GEORGIA

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE AND KRIS M. KEENEY GWINNETT COUNTY PUBLIC SCHOOLS, GEORGIA JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2004, 37, 83 87 NUMBER 1(SPRING 2004) AN EVALUATION OF RESPONSE COST IN THE TREATMENT OF INAPPROPRIATE VOCALIZATIONS MAINTAINED BY AUTOMATIC REINFORCEMENT TERRY S.

More information

---Evidence-base for Antecedent-based Intervention---

---Evidence-base for Antecedent-based Intervention--- ---Evidence-base for Antecedent-based Intervention--- The National Professional Development Center on ASD has adopted the following criteria to determine if a practice is evidence-based. The EBP Report

More information

Assessing the Effects of Matched and Unmatched Stimuli on the Persistence of Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Sarah Scamihorn

Assessing the Effects of Matched and Unmatched Stimuli on the Persistence of Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Sarah Scamihorn Assessing the Effects of Matched and Unmatched Stimuli on the Persistence of Stereotypy A Thesis Presented by Sarah Scamihorn The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial

More information

Idiosyncratic Functions: Severe Problem Behavior Maintained by Access to Ritualistic Behaviors

Idiosyncratic Functions: Severe Problem Behavior Maintained by Access to Ritualistic Behaviors EDUCATION AND TREATMENT OF CHILDREN Vol. 32 No. 1, 2009 Idiosyncratic Functions: Severe Problem Behavior Maintained by Access to Ritualistic Behaviors Nicole Hausman Kennedy Krieger Institute SungWoo Kahng

More information

The effects of a multiple schedule and RIRD intervention on the vocal stereotypy of a student with autism spectrum disorder

The effects of a multiple schedule and RIRD intervention on the vocal stereotypy of a student with autism spectrum disorder Rowan University Rowan Digital Works Theses and Dissertations 9-21-2016 The effects of a multiple schedule and RIRD intervention on the vocal stereotypy of a student with autism spectrum disorder Kathleen

More information

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2008, 41, 15 24 NUMBER 1(SPRING 2008) UTILITY OF EXTINCTION-INDUCED RESPONSE VARIABILITY FOR THE SELECTION OF MANDS LAURA L. GROW, MICHAEL E. KELLEY, HENRY S. ROANE,

More information

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45, 431 435 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 2012) COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO MARCUS

More information

POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT AS TREATMENT FOR PROBLEM BEHAVIOR MAINTAINED BY NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT STEVEN W. PAYNE AND CLAUDIA L.

POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT AS TREATMENT FOR PROBLEM BEHAVIOR MAINTAINED BY NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT STEVEN W. PAYNE AND CLAUDIA L. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2013, 46, 699 703 NUMBER 3(FALL 2013) POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT AS TREATMENT FOR PROBLEM BEHAVIOR MAINTAINED BY NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT STEVEN W. PAYNE AND CLAUDIA L. DOZIER

More information

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A. Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing Social Consequences as Reinforcers A Thesis Presented by Hilary A. Gibson The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational

More information

Comparison of Reinforcement Alone and in Combination with Redirection for. Treating Automatically-Reinforced Motor Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented

Comparison of Reinforcement Alone and in Combination with Redirection for. Treating Automatically-Reinforced Motor Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented Comparison of Reinforcement Alone and in Combination with Redirection for Treating Automatically-Reinforced Motor Stereotypy A Thesis Presented by Gesell Gavidia The Department of Counseling and Applied

More information

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders.

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders. P2 / Current Research on Treatment of Automatically Reinforced Problem Behavior at The New England Center for Children P3/ Do Persons with ASD Avoid Eye Contact? P4 5/ Research at The New England Center

More information

Effects of delays to response blocking when used as treatment for problem behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement

Effects of delays to response blocking when used as treatment for problem behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2010 Effects of delays to response blocking when used as treatment for problem behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement

More information

AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK

AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2016, 49, 1 6 170 175 NUMBER 1(SPRING) AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT UNIVERSITY

More information

TEST-SPECIFIC CONTROL CONDITIONS FOR FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES TARA A. FAHMIE BRIAN A. IWATA ANGIE C. QUERIM JILL M. HARPER

TEST-SPECIFIC CONTROL CONDITIONS FOR FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES TARA A. FAHMIE BRIAN A. IWATA ANGIE C. QUERIM JILL M. HARPER JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 213, 46, 61 7 NUMBER 1(SPRING 213) TEST-SPECIFIC CONTROL CONDITIONS FOR FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES TARA A. FAHMIE CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, NORTHRIDGE BRIAN A. IWATA UNIVERSITY

More information

6/11/2017. Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism. stereotypy. Gregory P. Hanley Ph.D.

6/11/2017. Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism. stereotypy. Gregory P. Hanley Ph.D. /11/17 Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism Gregory P. Hanley Ph.D., BCBA D For more information and materials, go to: www.practicalfunctionalassessment.com

More information

CARBONE CLINIC VIVIAN ATTANASIO VERBAL BEHAVIOR INSTITUTE AND TAMARA KASPER CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT

CARBONE CLINIC VIVIAN ATTANASIO VERBAL BEHAVIOR INSTITUTE AND TAMARA KASPER CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 43, 705 709 NUMBER 4(WINTER 2010) INCREASING THE VOCAL RESPONSES OF CHILDREN WITH AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES USING MANUAL SIGN MAND TRAINING AND PROMPT

More information

Behavioral Persistence of Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Joseph Michael Lambert. The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology

Behavioral Persistence of Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Joseph Michael Lambert. The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology The Effects of Matched and Unmatched Stimulation on the Behavioral Persistence of Stereotypy A Thesis Presented by Joseph Michael Lambert The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology

More information

University of Nevada, Reno. Functional Analysis of Motor and Vocal Stereotypy: It s Not Always Automatic

University of Nevada, Reno. Functional Analysis of Motor and Vocal Stereotypy: It s Not Always Automatic University of Nevada, Reno Functional Analysis of Motor and Vocal Stereotypy: It s Not Always Automatic A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

More information

The Durational Effects of a Free Operant Condition on Automatically Maintained Stereotypic Behavior and Discrete Trial Task Responding

The Durational Effects of a Free Operant Condition on Automatically Maintained Stereotypic Behavior and Discrete Trial Task Responding University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School September 2015 The Durational Effects of a Free Operant Condition on Automatically Maintained Stereotypic Behavior

More information

Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism

Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism Gregory P. Hanley. Ph.D., BCBA-D For more information go to: www.practicalfunctionalassessment.com THE GRODEN

More information

ABSTRACT ASSESSMENT-BASED INTERVENTIONS

ABSTRACT ASSESSMENT-BASED INTERVENTIONS Designing and Evaluating Assessment-Based Interventions to Reduce Stereotypy Among Adults With Autism in a Community Job Dennis H. Reid, Ph.D., BCBA Carolina Behavior Analysis and Support Center Marsha

More information

Preliminary Effects of Conditioned Establishing Operations on Stereotypy

Preliminary Effects of Conditioned Establishing Operations on Stereotypy Preliminary Effects of Conditioned Establishing Operations on Stereotypy Marc J. Lanovaz, John T. Rapp, Ethan S. Long, Sarah M. Richling & Regina A. Carroll The Psychological Record ISSN 0033-2933 DOI

More information

ABATEMENT OF INTRACTABLE VOCAL STEREOTYPY USING AN OVERCORRECTION PROCEDURE

ABATEMENT OF INTRACTABLE VOCAL STEREOTYPY USING AN OVERCORRECTION PROCEDURE Behavioral Interventions Behav. Intervent. 26: 134 146 (2011) Published online 22 March 2011 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).326 ABATEMENT OF INTRACTABLE VOCAL STEREOTYPY USING AN OVERCORRECTION

More information

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism A Thesis Presented by Melissa A. Ezold The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial fulfillment

More information

Overview. Environmental Causes of Problem Behaviours Exhibited by Children and Youth with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Assessment

Overview. Environmental Causes of Problem Behaviours Exhibited by Children and Youth with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Assessment Environmental Causes of Problem Behaviours Exhibited by Children and Youth with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Assessment SungWoo Kahng, Ph.D., BCBA-D University of Missouri Overview History of the development

More information

A Comparison of Functional Analysis Results When Conducted in Contrived and. Home Settings. A Thesis Presented. Dana DeIngenis

A Comparison of Functional Analysis Results When Conducted in Contrived and. Home Settings. A Thesis Presented. Dana DeIngenis A Comparison of Functional Analysis Results When Conducted in Contrived and Home Settings A Thesis Presented By Dana DeIngenis The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial

More information

Assessment and Treatment of Behavior Maintained by Automatic Reinforcement

Assessment and Treatment of Behavior Maintained by Automatic Reinforcement Rollins College Rollins Scholarship Online Thesis Projects Master s in Applied Behavior Analysis and Clinical Science Summer 2018 Assessment and Treatment of Behavior Maintained by Automatic Reinforcement

More information

AN INVESTIGATION OF DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF ALTERNATIVE BEHAVIOR WITHOUT EXTINCTION ELIZABETH S. ATHENS TIMOTHY R. VOLLMER

AN INVESTIGATION OF DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF ALTERNATIVE BEHAVIOR WITHOUT EXTINCTION ELIZABETH S. ATHENS TIMOTHY R. VOLLMER JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 43, 569 589 NUMBER 4(WINTER 2010) AN INVESTIGATION OF DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF ALTERNATIVE BEHAVIOR WITHOUT EXTINCTION ELIZABETH S. ATHENS MARCUS AUTISM

More information

AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC

AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 43, 279 283 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 2010) AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC AUBURN UNIVERSITY AND COURTNEY

More information

Functional Analysis and Reduction of Inappropriate Spitting

Functional Analysis and Reduction of Inappropriate Spitting Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 2007, 42(1), 59 64 Division on Developmental Disabilities Functional Analysis and Reduction of Inappropriate Spitting Stacy L. Carter and John J. Wheeler

More information

Lydon, Sinéad; Healy, Olive; McCoy, Anna.

Lydon, Sinéad; Healy, Olive; McCoy, Anna. Provided by the author(s) and NUI Galway in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite the published version when available. Title A systematic review and evaluation of response redirection as a treatment

More information

Practical Strategies to Address Challenging Behavior. Bridget A. Taylor, Psy.D., BCBA-D, Alpine Learning Group

Practical Strategies to Address Challenging Behavior. Bridget A. Taylor, Psy.D., BCBA-D, Alpine Learning Group Practical Strategies to Address Challenging Behavior Bridget A. Taylor, Psy.D., BCBA-D, Alpine Learning Group Today * Common behavior problems * Conditions that may occasion behavior problems * Assessment

More information

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN BY Copyright 2010 MATTHEW H. NEWQUIST Submitted to the graduate

More information

Jason Garner, M.A. ABA Clinical Director

Jason Garner, M.A. ABA Clinical Director Jason Garner, M.A. ABA Clinical Director Discuss Autism Definition Prevalence rates Discuss Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) Discuss challenging behavior Functions of Behavior Discuss behavior management

More information

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2008, 41, 125 130 NUMBER 1(SPRING 2008) A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER LOUISIANA STATE UNIVERSITY

More information

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF SOCIAL AVOIDANCE JILL M. HARPER

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF SOCIAL AVOIDANCE JILL M. HARPER JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 213, 46, 147 16 NUMBER 1(SPRING 213) ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF SOCIAL AVOIDANCE JILL M. HARPER MELMARK NEW ENGLAND AND BRIAN A. IWATA AND ERIN M. CAMP UNIVERSITY

More information

The Effects of a CABAS Conditioning Procedure To Increase Appropriate Toy Play and Decrease Stereotypy

The Effects of a CABAS Conditioning Procedure To Increase Appropriate Toy Play and Decrease Stereotypy The Effects of a CABAS Conditioning Procedure To Increase Appropriate Toy Play and Decrease Stereotypy Silvia Perini, Fabiola Casarini, Roberto Cattivelli Columbia University, Teachers College Università

More information

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE 이자료는이바로바스교수의응용행동수정강의를리차드손임상심리학박사가요약해서 정리한것입니다. Lovaas Method Philosophy Children stay with family at home If not working (no positive changes

More information

A COMPARISON OF PROCEDURES FOR UNPAIRING CONDITIONED REFLEXIVE ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS. Dissertation

A COMPARISON OF PROCEDURES FOR UNPAIRING CONDITIONED REFLEXIVE ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS. Dissertation A COMPARISON OF PROCEDURES FOR UNPAIRING CONDITIONED REFLEXIVE ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate

More information

ESCAPE BEHAVIOR DURING ACADEMIC TASKS: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS OF IDIOSYNCRATIC ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS JENNIFER MCCOMAS

ESCAPE BEHAVIOR DURING ACADEMIC TASKS: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS OF IDIOSYNCRATIC ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS JENNIFER MCCOMAS JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2000, 33, 479 493 NUMBER 4(WINTER 2000) ESCAPE BEHAVIOR DURING ACADEMIC TASKS: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS OF IDIOSYNCRATIC ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS JENNIFER MCCOMAS THE

More information

John Demanche & James T. Chok

John Demanche & James T. Chok The Use of Wrist Weights and Vibratory Stimulation to Treat Self-Injurious Behavior John Demanche & James T. Chok Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities ISSN 1056-263X DOI 10.1007/s10882-012-9304-2

More information

Evaluation of a presentation and measurement method for assessing activity preference

Evaluation of a presentation and measurement method for assessing activity preference University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2003 Evaluation of a presentation and measurement method for assessing activity preference Tara L. Lieblein

More information

The Concept of Automatic Reinforcement: Implications for Assessment and Intervention

The Concept of Automatic Reinforcement: Implications for Assessment and Intervention The Concept of Automatic Reinforcement: Implications for Assessment and Intervention Timothy R. Vollmer, Meghan Deshais, & Faris R. Kronfli University of Florida Overview Discuss what is meant by automatic

More information

THE EFFECTS OF VERBAL OPERANT TRAINING AND RESPONSE INTERRUPTION AND REDIRECTION ON VOCAL STEREOTYPY

THE EFFECTS OF VERBAL OPERANT TRAINING AND RESPONSE INTERRUPTION AND REDIRECTION ON VOCAL STEREOTYPY Southern Illinois University Carbondale OpenSIUC Theses Theses and Dissertations 8-1-2017 THE EFFECTS OF VERBAL OPERANT TRAINING AND RESPONSE INTERRUPTION AND REDIRECTION ON VOCAL STEREOTYPY Casey Kaye

More information

4/4/2017. Intervention Strategies for the Reduction of Repetitive Behavior in Persons with ASD

4/4/2017. Intervention Strategies for the Reduction of Repetitive Behavior in Persons with ASD Intervention Strategies for the Reduction of Repetitive Behavior in Persons with ASD David B. McAdam, PhD, BCBA-D Deborah A. Napolitano, PhD, BCBA-D Rochester Regional Center for Autism Spectrum Disorder

More information

Treatment of Pica Using a Pica Exchange Procedure with Increasing Response Effort

Treatment of Pica Using a Pica Exchange Procedure with Increasing Response Effort Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 2009, 44(1), 143 147 Division on Developmental Disabilities Treatment of Pica Using a Pica Exchange Procedure with Increasing Response Effort Stacy

More information

Use of Functional Analysis and Treatment Analysis in the Development of a Home-Based Pica Intervention

Use of Functional Analysis and Treatment Analysis in the Development of a Home-Based Pica Intervention Use of Functional Analysis and Treatment Analysis in the Development of a Home-Based Pica Intervention Meghan Clausen ABACS & Simmons College Kate Merrill ABACS Ashley Williams ABACS & Simmons College

More information

BRIEF (TEST-CONTROL) FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT EVALUATION OF AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR EVOKED BY DIVIDED ATTENTION

BRIEF (TEST-CONTROL) FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT EVALUATION OF AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR EVOKED BY DIVIDED ATTENTION Behavioral Interventions Behav. Intervent. (2014) Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).1394 BRIEF (TEST-CONTROL) FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT EVALUATION OF AGGRESSIVE

More information

SIDE EFFECTS OF EXTINCTION: PREVALENCE OF BURSTING AND AGGRESSION DURING THE TREATMENT OF SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR DOROTHEA C.

SIDE EFFECTS OF EXTINCTION: PREVALENCE OF BURSTING AND AGGRESSION DURING THE TREATMENT OF SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR DOROTHEA C. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1999, 32, 1 8 NUMBER 1(SPRING 1999) SIDE EFFECTS OF EXTINCTION: PREVALENCE OF BURSTING AND AGGRESSION DURING THE TREATMENT OF SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR DOROTHEA C. LERMAN

More information

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented 1 Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present A Thesis Presented by Lindsey M. Ryan The Department of Counseling and Applied

More information

An Evaluation of the Utility of Two Demand Assessments in Identifying Negative. Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Casey E. Bethay

An Evaluation of the Utility of Two Demand Assessments in Identifying Negative. Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Casey E. Bethay Running Head: EVALUATION OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS Thesis 1 An Evaluation of the Utility of Two Demand Assessments in Identifying Negative Reinforcers A Thesis Presented By Casey E. Bethay The Department

More information

EFFECTS OF ALTERNATIVE RESPONSES ON BEHAVIOR EXPOSED TO NONCONTINGENT REINFORCEMENT JAVIER VIRUES-ORTEGA BRIAN A. IWATA TARA A. FAHMIE JILL M.

EFFECTS OF ALTERNATIVE RESPONSES ON BEHAVIOR EXPOSED TO NONCONTINGENT REINFORCEMENT JAVIER VIRUES-ORTEGA BRIAN A. IWATA TARA A. FAHMIE JILL M. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 213, 46, 63 612 NUMBER 3(FALL 213) EFFECTS OF ALTERNATIVE RESPONSES ON BEHAVIOR EXPOSED TO NONCONTINGENT REINFORCEMENT JAVIER VIRUES-ORTEGA UNIVERSITY OF MANITOBA AND

More information

METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent

METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2009, 42, 827 832 NUMBER 4(WINTER 2009) COMPARISON OF DATA-COLLECTION METHODS IN A BEHAVIORAL INTERVENTION PROGRAM FOR CHILDREN WITH PERVASIVE DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS:

More information

The Effects of Specific Behavioral Interventions on Vocal Stereotypy: A Systematic Review

The Effects of Specific Behavioral Interventions on Vocal Stereotypy: A Systematic Review Nova Southeastern University NSUWorks Fischler College of Education: Theses and Dissertations Abraham S. Fischler College of Education 1-1-2018 The Effects of Specific Behavioral Interventions on Vocal

More information

Effects of Discrimination Abilities on Functional Analysis Outcomes. Abstract. Authors. Ashley Greenwald, Holly Seniuk, W.

Effects of Discrimination Abilities on Functional Analysis Outcomes. Abstract. Authors. Ashley Greenwald, Holly Seniuk, W. Effects of Discrimination Abilities on Functional Analysis Outcomes Abstract Volume 18, Number 3, 212 Authors Ashley Greenwald, Holly Seniuk, W. Larry Williams Behavior Analysis Program, University of

More information

Behavioral Development Bulletin. Mary E. McDonald 1, Sharon A. Reeve 2, and Erin J. Sparacio 3. Volume 19 Number 1 March 2014

Behavioral Development Bulletin. Mary E. McDonald 1, Sharon A. Reeve 2, and Erin J. Sparacio 3. Volume 19 Number 1 March 2014 14 American Psychological Association Behavioral Development Bulletin Volume 19 Number 1 March 14 Using a tactile prompt to increase instructor delivery of behaviorspecific praise and token reinforcement

More information

OBSERVATIONAL EFFECTS ON PREFERENCE SELECTION FOR FOUR CHILDREN ON THE AUTISM SPECTRUM: A REPLICATION

OBSERVATIONAL EFFECTS ON PREFERENCE SELECTION FOR FOUR CHILDREN ON THE AUTISM SPECTRUM: A REPLICATION Behavioral Interventions Behav. Intervent. 30: 256 269 (2015) Published online 8 May 2015 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com).1411 OBSERVATIONAL EFFECTS ON PREFERENCE SELECTION FOR FOUR CHILDREN

More information

REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE THINNING FOLLOWING TREATMENT WITH FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION TRAINING GREGORY P. HANLEY, BRIAN A. IWATA, AND RACHEL H.

REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE THINNING FOLLOWING TREATMENT WITH FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION TRAINING GREGORY P. HANLEY, BRIAN A. IWATA, AND RACHEL H. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2001, 34, 17 38 NUMBER 1(SPRING 2001) REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE THINNING FOLLOWING TREATMENT WITH FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION TRAINING GREGORY P. HANLEY, BRIAN A. IWATA,

More information

A Comparison of Sensory and Function-based Antecedent Approaches to Decreasing Out-of-Seat Behavior

A Comparison of Sensory and Function-based Antecedent Approaches to Decreasing Out-of-Seat Behavior A Comparison of Sensory and Functionbased Antecedent Approaches to Decreasing OutofSeat Behavior A Thesis Presented by Kate L. Desmond The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In

More information

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45,

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45, JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45, 809 813 NUMBER 4(WINTER 2012) AN EVALUATION OF THE NUMBER OF PRESENTATIONS OF TARGET SOUNDS DURING STIMULUS STIMULUS PAIRING TRIALS ADRIANE MILIOTIS, TINA

More information

A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Object Imitation: In-Vivo versus Video Modeling. A Thesis Presented. Amy N. Wick

A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Object Imitation: In-Vivo versus Video Modeling. A Thesis Presented. Amy N. Wick Imitation 1 A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Object Imitation: In-Vivo versus Video Modeling A Thesis Presented by Amy N. Wick The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial

More information

Differential Reinforcement of Prompted and Independent Responses: An Alternative. Procedure to Decrease Prompt Dependency. A Thesis Presented

Differential Reinforcement of Prompted and Independent Responses: An Alternative. Procedure to Decrease Prompt Dependency. A Thesis Presented Differential Reinforcement of Prompted and Independent Responses: An Alternative Procedure to Decrease Prompt Dependency A Thesis Presented by Catia Cividini-Motta The Department of Counseling and Applied

More information

Running head: EVALUATING INHIBITORY STIMULUS CONTROL. Increasing the Reinforcing Efficacy of Low-Preference Teachers and Caretakers

Running head: EVALUATING INHIBITORY STIMULUS CONTROL. Increasing the Reinforcing Efficacy of Low-Preference Teachers and Caretakers Reinforcing Efficacy 1 Running head: EVALUATING INHIBITORY STIMULUS CONTROL Increasing the Reinforcing Efficacy of Low-Preference Teachers and Caretakers for Individuals Diagnosed with Developmental Disabilities

More information

RESPONSE PATTERNS IN FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS. Christine M. Gibson, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

RESPONSE PATTERNS IN FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS. Christine M. Gibson, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE RESPONSE PATTERNS IN FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS Christine M. Gibson, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS August 2012 APPROVED: Richard G.

More information

THE EFFECTS OF BEHAVIORAL SKILLS TRAINING ON CAREGIVER IMPLEMENTATION OF GUIDED COMPLIANCE NICHOLAS I. MILES AND DAVID A. WILDER

THE EFFECTS OF BEHAVIORAL SKILLS TRAINING ON CAREGIVER IMPLEMENTATION OF GUIDED COMPLIANCE NICHOLAS I. MILES AND DAVID A. WILDER JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2009, 42, 405 410 NUMBER 2(SUMMER 2009) THE EFFECTS OF BEHAVIORAL SKILLS TRAINING ON CAREGIVER IMPLEMENTATION OF GUIDED COMPLIANCE NICHOLAS I. MILES AND DAVID A. WILDER

More information

The Use of Response Interruption Redirection (RIRD), Timeout, and Differential Reinforoement to Decrease Stereotypy

The Use of Response Interruption Redirection (RIRD), Timeout, and Differential Reinforoement to Decrease Stereotypy Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 12-2014 The Use of Response Interruption Redirection (RIRD), Timeout, and Differential Reinforoement to Decrease Stereotypy

More information

Bonnie Van Metre M.Ed., BCBA Kennedy Krieger Institute Center for Autism and Related Disorders

Bonnie Van Metre M.Ed., BCBA Kennedy Krieger Institute Center for Autism and Related Disorders Bonnie Van Metre M.Ed., BCBA Kennedy Krieger Institute Center for Autism and Related Disorders Principles of behavior/ functions of behavior Preventive strategies Consequences strategies Aggression Pinching

More information

POST-HOC ANALYSIS OF CHALLENGING BEHAVIOR BY FUNCTION: A COMPARISON OF MULTIPLE-RESPONDENT ANECDOTAL ASSESSMENTS, FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES, AND TREATMENTS

POST-HOC ANALYSIS OF CHALLENGING BEHAVIOR BY FUNCTION: A COMPARISON OF MULTIPLE-RESPONDENT ANECDOTAL ASSESSMENTS, FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES, AND TREATMENTS POST-HOC ANALYSIS OF CHALLENGING BEHAVIOR BY FUNCTION: A COMPARISON OF MULTIPLE-RESPONDENT ANECDOTAL ASSESSMENTS, FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES, AND TREATMENTS Kathleen Dignan, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree

More information

VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment 1. Negative Behaviors 0. Does not demonstrate any significant negative behaviors 1. Engages in some minor negative behaviors weekly, but recovery is quick 2. Emits a variety of minor negative behaviors

More information

EFFECTIVENESS OF THE IPAD WITH THE PROLOQUO2GO APPLICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Presented by, Melissa L. King, M.S.

EFFECTIVENESS OF THE IPAD WITH THE PROLOQUO2GO APPLICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Presented by, Melissa L. King, M.S. EFFECTIVENESS OF THE IPAD WITH THE PROLOQUO2GO APPLICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM Presented by, Melissa L. King, M.S. PURPOSE Determine the efficacy of using the ipad with the Proloquo2Go application,

More information

Thesis. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University

Thesis. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University The Effects of Response Interruption and Contingent Demands on Reducing Vocal Stereotypy in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for

More information

Abstract. Authors. Evan Anderson, Anjali Barretto, T. F. McLaughlin, T. McQuaid. Correspondence. Keywords. Volume 22, Number 1, 2016

Abstract. Authors. Evan Anderson, Anjali Barretto, T. F. McLaughlin, T. McQuaid. Correspondence. Keywords. Volume 22, Number 1, 2016 Volume 22, Number 1, 2016 Authors Evan Anderson, Anjali Barretto, T. F. McLaughlin, T. McQuaid Gonzaga University, Spokane, WA Correspondence barretto@gonzaga.edu Keywords functional communication training,

More information

USING PERCENTILE SCHEDULES TO INCREASE EYE CONTACT IN CHILDREN WITH FRAGILE X SYNDROME SCOTT S. HALL, NATALEE P. MAYNES, AND ALLAN L.

USING PERCENTILE SCHEDULES TO INCREASE EYE CONTACT IN CHILDREN WITH FRAGILE X SYNDROME SCOTT S. HALL, NATALEE P. MAYNES, AND ALLAN L. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2009, 42, 171 176 NUMBER 1(SPRING 2009) USING PERCENTILE SCHEDULES TO INCREASE EYE CONTACT IN CHILDREN WITH FRAGILE X SYNDROME SCOTT S. HALL, NATALEE P. MAYNES, AND

More information

Fixed-time schedules involved presenting reinforcers

Fixed-time schedules involved presenting reinforcers JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1998, 31, 529 542 NUMBER 4(WINTER 1998) FIXED-TIME SCHEDULES ATTENUATE EXTINCTION-INDUCED PHENOMENA IN THE TREATMENT OF SEVERE ABERRANT BEHAVIOR TIMOTHY R. VOLLMER,

More information

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction Linda LeBlanc Trumpet Behavioral Health Laura Grow University of British Columbia Importance of Effective Teaching

More information

ABA Basics: Basic Techniques Used to See Change. Presenter: Kail T. Graham, M.A., BCBA

ABA Basics: Basic Techniques Used to See Change. Presenter: Kail T. Graham, M.A., BCBA ABA Basics: Basic Techniques Used to See Change Presenter: Kail T. Graham, M.A., BCBA Topics of the Day The History of ABA What is ABA? The ABCs of ABA Reinforcement Attending and Ignoring Preview to the

More information

A Comparison of Edible, Social, and No Contrived Reinforcement on the. Acquisition of a Behavior Chain. A Thesis Presented.

A Comparison of Edible, Social, and No Contrived Reinforcement on the. Acquisition of a Behavior Chain. A Thesis Presented. Comparing Edible, Social, and No Contrived Reinforcement 1 A Comparison of Edible, Social, and No Contrived Reinforcement on the Acquisition of a Behavior Chain A Thesis Presented by Kimberly Flint Department

More information

Running Head: BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF CHOICE. A Behavioral Economic Analysis of Choice. A Thesis Presented. Jamie Leigh Lebowitz

Running Head: BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF CHOICE. A Behavioral Economic Analysis of Choice. A Thesis Presented. Jamie Leigh Lebowitz Choice 1 Running Head: BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF CHOICE A Behavioral Economic Analysis of Choice A Thesis Presented By Jamie Leigh Lebowitz The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology

More information

TREATMENT OF COVERT SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR IN INDIVIDUALS WITH PRADER-WILLI SYNDROME

TREATMENT OF COVERT SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR IN INDIVIDUALS WITH PRADER-WILLI SYNDROME TREATMENT OF COVERT SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR IN INDIVIDUALS WITH PRADER-WILLI SYNDROME By MEAGAN K. GREGORY A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information

Differences in Immediate and Subsequent Effects of Stimuli on Vocal Stereotypy Inform DRO and DRA Treatments. Kristen M. Brogan

Differences in Immediate and Subsequent Effects of Stimuli on Vocal Stereotypy Inform DRO and DRA Treatments. Kristen M. Brogan Differences in Immediate and Subsequent Effects of Stimuli on Vocal Stereotypy Inform DRO and DRA Treatments by Kristen M. Brogan A thesis submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Auburn University in partial

More information

PYRAMIDAL TRAINING FOR FAMILIES OF CHILDREN WITH PROBLEM BEHAVIOR STEPHANIE A. C. KUHN, DOROTHEA C. LERMAN, AND CHRISTINA M.

PYRAMIDAL TRAINING FOR FAMILIES OF CHILDREN WITH PROBLEM BEHAVIOR STEPHANIE A. C. KUHN, DOROTHEA C. LERMAN, AND CHRISTINA M. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2003, 36, 77 88 NUMBER 1(SPRING 2003) PYRAMIDAL TRAINING FOR FAMILIES OF CHILDREN WITH PROBLEM BEHAVIOR STEPHANIE A. C. KUHN, DOROTHEA C. LERMAN, AND CHRISTINA M. VORNDRAN

More information

The University of Iowa

The University of Iowa The Psychological Record, 213, 63, 3 2 An Evaluation of Resurgence During Functional Communication Training David P. Wacker, Jay W. Harding, Theresa A. Morgan, Wendy K. Berg, Kelly M. Schieltz, John F.

More information

12/19/2016. Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview. What is the Autism Spectrum? Autism Spectrum Disorder

12/19/2016. Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview. What is the Autism Spectrum? Autism Spectrum Disorder Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview What is the Autism Spectrum? DSM V (2013) included revisions with more specific diagnostic criteria and Further distinctions will

More information