INTERNAL VALIDITY, BIAS AND CONFOUNDING

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "INTERNAL VALIDITY, BIAS AND CONFOUNDING"

Transcription

1 OCW Epidemiology and Biostatistics, 2010 J. Forrester, PhD Tufts University School of Medicine October 6, 2010 INTERNAL VALIDITY, BIAS AND CONFOUNDING Learning objectives for this session: 1) Understand and distinguish the concepts of internal and external validity 2) Understand what is meant by the term bias 3) Distinguish bias from imprecision 4) Understand what is meant by the term confounding 5) Know the characteristics of a confounding variable 6) Understand what study design features are used to avoid confounding 7) Distinguish confounding and effect modification (interaction) Outside preparation: Gordis Chapter 15, pages Internal validity is the degree to which the study findings represent a true reflection of the exposure-outcome association in the target population. (e.g. How close the estimated relative risk in a study is to the true, but unknown relative risk) There are three steps to assess the internal validity of a study: 1) Rule out bias 2) Rule out confounding (a special kind of bias) 3) Rule out chance (by means of statistical tests which we will see later) What is bias? Bias is a distortion of the results of a study such that the results do not reflect the true (but unknown) exposure-outcome association. More specifically bias is a systematic error in the measurement of an association between two variables. 1

2 Bias vs. imprecision Bias can be thought of as the distance an observation is from truth. Precision can be thought of as reproducibility. Imprecision is lack of reproducibility. Each one of the s below can be thought of as a study result. The bull's eye in the target is the true exposure - outcome association that we are attempting to estimate (e.g. the true RR). No Bias & No Imprecision Bias & No Imprecision No bias & Imprecision Bias & Imprecision (Here, the average of the s will fall On the bull s eye) 2

3 There is no coherent taxonomy of biases. The same bias can have more than one name. Different textbooks classify biases in different ways. We use the classification below. There are three major types of bias. Most biases fall under one of these three types: 1) Confounding 2) Selection bias (also called "sample distortion bias") 3) Information or observation bias: There are three major types of information bias: 1) Random (non-differential) misclassification of exposure/outcome 2) Non-Random (differential) misclassification of exposure/outcome 3) Loss-to-follow-up bias Confounding The presence of confounding induces bias and is a threat to a study s internal validity. A confounder is a risk factor for the disease that is not the exposure under study and that is not evenly distributed among the exposure groups. Our goal is to compare groups that are alike with respect to all risk factors for the outcome except the exposure we are studying. "Imbalance" in risk factors among the exposure groups will result in an unfair comparison of groups because the group with more risk factors is bound to develop more disease. This imbalance in risk factors across study arms, regardless of the study design, is called confounding. Characteristics of a confounder: To be a confounder a variable must fulfill three conditions: 1) A confounder must be a risk factor for the outcome. Only an imbalance in risk factors for the outcome of interest can lead to confounding. We do not care about imbalance in factors that are not risk factors for the disease. For example, we do not care about the proportion of Red Sox fans in each group since being a Red Sox fan, as far as we know, is only a risk factor for disappointment. 2) A confounder must be associated with the study exposure in the study. The ways in which a confounder can be associated with the exposure in a study are as follows: In a RCT: Treatment and placebo groups are not balanced for disease risk factors after randomization (most often a problem in studies with small numbers of subjects). If the risk factor is balanced across the treatment and placebo groups then the risk factor is not a confounder because it is not associated with the exposure. 3

4 In an observational study: Exposed and non-exposed groups are not balanced for other disease risk factors once the groups are assembled. If the risk factor is balanced across the exposure groups then the risk factor is not a confounder because it is not associated with the exposure. 3) A confounder must not be an intermediate risk factor (a.k.a. a mediator) in the causal pathway between the exposure and outcome. Cholesterol is an example of an intermediate risk factor or mediator of the association between a high fat diet and cardiovascular disease: High Fat Diet Cholesterol Cardiovascular disease We will see more on mediators later. Methods to prevent and control confounding (i.e. to force the exposure groups to be balanced for disease risk factors) 1) Control of confounding in the study design: Randomization: Randomization helps to balance the risk factors across the study arms, especially if a large number of subjects are randomized. If the study is small (about 100 subjects or less would be considered a small RCT), randomization helps but does not guarantee balance. Restriction: You can control confounding by restricting the study sample to one level of the confounder. For example, if you are worried about confounding by gender you could choose to study only women. Clinical trials will often mention exclusion criteria that are motivated by a desire to reduce confounding. For example, it would be reasonable in a trial testing the effect of a new cholesterol-lowering drug on the incidence of cardiovascular disease to exclude persons who have been diagnosed with cancer because there are so many aspects of cancer that could confound a study of cardiovascular disease. While this strategy may eliminate confounding by cancer, it limits the generalizability of the study. You may want to treat high cholesterol in your patients with the new drug described above, including those patients who have had a diagnosis of cancer in their past. The restriction of the trial to persons without a cancer diagnosis may limit your ability to judge how the medication will act in these people. Matching: This method is used in observational study designs (not in RCTs). In a cohort study matching makes intuitive sense. If you want to control confounding by gender you recruit one exposed woman and match her to an unexposed woman, followed by one exposed man and an unexposed man, etc. Matching can be difficult if you have many variables to match for since it will be harder to find pairs of subjects with the same risk factor profile except the exposure under study. Matching is both expensive and inefficient because it is difficult and time consuming to recruit study subjects and you have to throw away recruited subjects for whom you have no match. In a case-control study matching is not intuitive. Suffice it to say that 4

5 matching cases and controls for important confounders helps us to make a more efficient adjustment for the confounders in the statistical analysis (described below). 2) Control of confounding in the statistical analyses: Stratification: This is done by calculating the measure of association (e.g. RR, OR) within the confounder groups. For example, if the confounder is gender, calculate the RR in women and, separately, calculate the RR in men. Each of these RRs is free of confounding by gender. If there is no evidence that the RRs differ in magnitude, you can collapse these two RRs into one using a weighted average of the RR in women and the RR in men. This average RR is free of confounding by gender. This was the old fashioned method used by epidemiologists before everyone had a PC with statistical software. It still works and it is a method used often by epidemiologists and statisticians to look at data before it is given to the computer to calculate. Multivariate Analysis: This is a fancy form of stratification. It is fancy because the computer can stratify by many variables simultaneously using multiple regression techniques, which will simultaneously adjust for many potential confounders, which we will see later. When reading the medical literature, look for the words adjustment or control for potential confounders in the statistical section or in the footnotes to the tables to see if the authors report adjusting for any imbalance in the groups being compared. A well-written article will list all of the factors that were included in the adjustment. How to recognize confounding Let s use the evaluation of alcohol as a confounder of the relationship between smoking (the exposure) and myocardial infarction (the outcome) in a study with 100 subjects. You suspect that alcohol could be a confounder because your preliminary analyses showed that the smokers were more likely to drink alcohol than the non-smokers (i.e. the exposure groups were imbalanced on drinking). Alcohol, in moderate amounts, is known to protect against cardiovascular disease. Step 1) Calculate the RR the usual way with drinkers and non-drinkers mixed all together: You find the overall "crude" RR of MI & smoking = 3.0 Is alcohol a confounder? In other words, is 3.0 the true, unbiased RR? 5

6 Step 2) Divide the data into drinkers and non-drinkers and calculate the RR within drinkers and the RR within non-drinkers (say half of the 100 subjects are drinkers): In the 50 Drinkers: In the 50 Non-drinkers: RR of MI & Smoking = 3.9 RR of MI & Smoking = 4.1 Each of these two RRs is free of confounding by alcohol use because the smoking-mi association was evaluated separately within groups that drink or do not drink. Step 3) To calculate the overall unconfounded ( adjusted ) RR, take the average of the RR in drinkers and non-drinkers: Adjusted RR = ( ) / 2 = 4.0 (This adjusted RR applies to all 100 subjects) Our data demonstrate evidence that alcohol was confounding the association between smoking and MI because the crude RR (3.0) is quite different from the average (adjusted) RR calculated drinkers and non-drinkers (4.0). How different is different is a judgment call. Because drinking was providing some protection from MI to the smokers, when the effect of drinking was removed, the association of smoking with MI was even bigger an unadjusted (crude) RR of 3.0 became an adjusted RR of 4.0. The evidence of confounding: Crude RR of 3.0 not equal to adjusted RR of 4.0 To assess confounding: Compare the crude RR (or OR) to the adjusted RR (or OR) In a journal article, it is unusual to see the data laid out as above. When reading an article, to evaluate confounding, you should look for differences in the RR between the unadjusted analysis (the crude RR), and the RR following multivariable adjustment (the "adjusted" RR). This is usually shown in a table format. If the unadjusted and adjusted RRs differ by a lot, this indicates that at least one of the factors that was adjusted for was, indeed, a confounder. Regardless of the size of the difference between the crude and adjusted RR, the adjusted RR is always the most valid estimate of the true association between the exposure and outcome. For an example of this look at Table 2 of the article by Tsutsumi et al. on occupational stress and the risk of stroke (the assigned reading for small group #2). The authors present RRs comparing the risk of total stroke in men in high-strain jobs vs. men in low strain jobs (the reference group) adjusted for age and area. The crude RR is 28 /8808 divided by 7/5311 = Adjustment for age and area gives a RR of This is a slightly different value indicating some confounding by age and area. The value 2.62 is the more valid of the two RRs. 6

7 Table 2. Associations of Psychosocial Job Characteristics With Incident Stroke Hazard Ratio Men (N=3190) Person-years No. of Events Adjusted for Age and Area Total stroke Low-strain job (Reference) High-strain job From Tsutsumi et al. Prospective study on occupational Stress and Risk of Stroke Arch Intern Med 2009; 169: Other comments about confounding: Confounding is the only kind of bias that can be corrected easily after the study data are collected. However, you can only adjust (control) for risk factors you have measured. If you did not collect data on a particular risk factor, you will not know if the comparison groups are balanced with respect to that risk factor and you will not be able remove the factor s possible confounding influence by stratification or multivariate adjustment. Confounding that remains in a study after adjustment is called residual confounding If you measure a confounder using a poor quality instrument such that there is misclassification in the determination of the confounder, you will still have residual confounding after adjustment. For example, if alcohol is the potential confounder of concern and the data were not carefully collected or the subjects were not truthful in their response about their drinking habits there may be residual confounding after adjustment for alcohol in the analyses because of the poor capture of the alcohol data. Control for an intermediate risk factor (a mediator) in the causal pathway, will wash out the association of the exposure with the outcome and thereby underestimate the true exposure-disease relationship. For example, High Fat Diet Cholesterol Cardiovascular disease In a study of the relation between a high fat diet and cardiovascular disease (CVD) adjustment for cholesterol ( a mediator) will wash out the association between a high fat diet and CVD because higher cholesterol is one of the ways in which a high fat diet leads to CVD. Epidemiologists call this over adjusting or over controlling. Of course, this assumes something is known about the biologic pathway between a high fat diet and CVD. Often we don t know the pathway. In the cardiovascular literature it is now common to see analyses that include not just confounders but also mediators. The motive for examining mediators in the analysis is to explore the biologic pathways. Using the example above, say we did not know how a high fat diet increases the risk of CVD. We could explore the hypothesis that one of the mechanisms is by increasing serum cholesterol levels. We would adjust for cholesterol and find that the RR describing the high fat diet - CVD association is attenuated. This would suggest that the mechanism by which a high fat diet increases the risk of CVD works through an elevation in cholesterol. To see an example of an exploration for mediators look again at Table 2 of the article by Tsutsumi et al. on occupational stress and the risk of stroke. In the far right column of 7

8 Table 2, the authors present their data on the association of occupational stress and stroke adjusted for biologic risk factors in addition to age, area, socio-demographic and behavioral risk factors. They explored the extent to which the elevated risk of stroke in persons with high-strain jobs was mediated by its effect on biologic risk factors. As they noted in the text, the RR adjusted for age, area, socio-demographic and behavioral risk factors alone (RR of total stroke=2.73 in the men) was attenuated to a value of 2.53 by adjusting for biologic factors. They write in the comment section of the paper, "We found that adjustments for biologic risk factors slightly attenuated the association between job strain and an incident stroke. These findings suggest that the association between job strain and cardiovascular diseases is mediated by the presence of 1 or more chronic diseases, such as obesity, hypertension, glucose intolerance, and dyslipidemia. However, adjustments for such variables did not fully account for the associations between job strain and stroke." Large randomized controlled trials do not suffer from confounding because the randomization of large numbers of study participants balances the risk factor profile across the study arms. Being balanced, the risk factors for the outcome do not satisfy criterion #2 for confounding. Going back to the low-dose aspirin study by Ridker et al. you will see in Table 1 of the trial results that the risk factor profile of the aspirin group is almost identical to the risk factor profile of the placebo group the magic of randomization! Therefore, confounding is not a problem in this study and no adjustments were necessary. Confounding vs. Effect Modification While an assessment of confounding involves comparing the crude and adjusted RRs, effect modification is evaluated by comparing RR in sub-groups based on some third variable. To do this, you compare the RR in subgroup 1 with the RR in subgroup 2 (Here, the crude RR is irrelevant). For example, in the low-dose aspirin study by Ridker et al. the RR describing the association of low-dose aspirin with cardiovascular disease was evaluated in women of different age groups. As we saw, low-dose aspirin was most effective (lowest RR) in women > 65 years of age. We concluded that age was a modifier of the effect of low-dose aspirin on the association of low-dose aspirin and cardiovascular disease. Since the study was a large RCT, there was no concern for confounding. In the example of smoking as a risk factor for myocardial infarction, used above, you should compare the RR for smoking and MI in the drinkers vs. the RR for smoking and MI in the non-drinkers i.e. 3.9 vs If they are very different, then alcohol is an effect modifier. In this example, the RRs are not very different, so alcohol is not an effect modifier. However, note that alcohol was a confounder because the crude and adjusted RRs were quite different from each other (4.0 vs. 3.0). To assess confounding: Compare crude to the adjusted RR. If they are very different (by the eye-ball test ), there was confounding To assess effect modification: Compare subgroup RRs to each other. If they are very different, there is effect modification by the stratification variable 8

9 A confounder must be a risk factor for the disease. An effect modifier does not need to be a risk factor for the disease - just some other variable that is biologically related to the disease process. However, effect modifiers are often risk factors for the disease. External validity: Another important aspect to consider when evaluating a study is its external validity. External validity is the degree to which study findings can be generalized beyond the study target population, - i.e., to whom do the results of this study apply? For example, can you generalize a study done in young to middle aged Caucasian adults to minorities, children, or the elderly? Can you generalize the results of a study done in normal weight adults to obese adults? Can you generalize a study of aspirin and heart disease done in men to women? The question of generalizability requires common sense. Is there reason to believe that the biologic processes examined in the current study are the same or different in another group of individuals? Use your head scientists generalize from mice to men every day. However, sometimes generalization it is not appropriate. Confounding is an issue of internal validity, while effect modification is an issue of external validity. You will never see confounding or effect modification laid out in a paper as below. These numbers are given simply as a guide to the evaluation of confounding and effect modification. Example of confounding (with no effect modification) by hair color in a case-control study of eye color and risk of lupus (fictitious numbers: eyes and hair color are not known risk factors for lupus, but race - African American or Hispanic - is a known risk factor) Overall crude results: OR = 1.6 Brown eyes Blue eyes Hair color dark: OR= 1.0 Hair color light: OR= 1.0 Brown eyes Blue eyes Brown eyes Blue eyes Overall adjusted OR = 1.0 (550/1000 * /1000*1.0) the weighted average of stratum-specific ORs). Note the ORs in the dark and light hair are identical, indicating no 9

10 effect modification. Interpretation: Hair color was confounding the association of eye color and the risk of lupus. Once hair color was adjusted, there was no association between eye color and lupus. Example of effect modification by hair color in a case-control study of eye color and risk of lupus (fictitious numbers: eyes and hair color are not known risk factors for lupus, but race - African American or Hispanic - is a known risk factor) Overall results: OR = 2.3 Brown eyes Blue eyes Hair color dark: OR= 4.7 Brown eyes Blue eyes 5 55 Hair color light: OR= 1.4 Brown eyes Blue eyes Interpretation: The effect of eye color on the risk of lupus depends on hair color. The risk associated with having brown eyes is greater if you also have dark hair. Hair color modifies the association of eye color and the risk of lupus. 10

Confounding and Bias

Confounding and Bias 28 th International Conference on Pharmacoepidemiology and Therapeutic Risk Management Barcelona, Spain August 22, 2012 Confounding and Bias Tobias Gerhard, PhD Assistant Professor, Ernest Mario School

More information

115 remained abstinent. 140 remained abstinent. Relapsed Remained abstinent Total

115 remained abstinent. 140 remained abstinent. Relapsed Remained abstinent Total Chapter 10 Exercises 1. Intent-to-treat analysis: Example 1 In a randomized controlled trial to determine whether the nicotine patch reduces the risk of relapse among smokers who have committed to quit,

More information

Confounding and Interaction

Confounding and Interaction Confounding and Interaction Why did you do clinical research? To find a better diagnosis tool To determine risk factor of disease To identify prognosis factor To evaluate effectiveness of therapy To decide

More information

Biases in clinical research. Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University

Biases in clinical research. Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University Biases in clinical research Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University Learning objectives Describe the threats to causal inferences in clinical studies Understand the role of

More information

Confounding Bias: Stratification

Confounding Bias: Stratification OUTLINE: Confounding- cont. Generalizability Reproducibility Effect modification Confounding Bias: Stratification Example 1: Association between place of residence & Chronic bronchitis Residence Chronic

More information

Biases in clinical research. Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University

Biases in clinical research. Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University Biases in clinical research Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University Learning objectives Describe the threats to causal inferences in clinical studies Understand the role of

More information

Main objective of Epidemiology. Statistical Inference. Statistical Inference: Example. Statistical Inference: Example

Main objective of Epidemiology. Statistical Inference. Statistical Inference: Example. Statistical Inference: Example Main objective of Epidemiology Inference to a population Example: Treatment of hypertension: Research question (hypothesis): Is treatment A better than treatment B for patients with hypertension? Study

More information

Introduction. Step 2: Student Role - Your Plan of Action. Confounding. Good luck and have fun!

Introduction. Step 2: Student Role - Your Plan of Action. Confounding. Good luck and have fun! Confounding Introduction You have learned that random error and bias must be considered as possible explanations for an observed association between an exposure and disease. This week we will examine the

More information

Observational Study Designs. Review. Today. Measures of disease occurrence. Cohort Studies

Observational Study Designs. Review. Today. Measures of disease occurrence. Cohort Studies Observational Study Designs Denise Boudreau, PhD Center for Health Studies Group Health Cooperative Today Review cohort studies Case-control studies Design Identifying cases and controls Measuring exposure

More information

Confounding. Confounding and effect modification. Example (after Rothman, 1998) Beer and Rectal Ca. Confounding (after Rothman, 1998)

Confounding. Confounding and effect modification. Example (after Rothman, 1998) Beer and Rectal Ca. Confounding (after Rothman, 1998) Confounding Confounding and effect modification Epidemiology 511 W. A. Kukull vember 23 2004 A function of the complex interrelationships between various exposures and disease. Occurs when the disease

More information

Confounding, Effect modification, and Stratification

Confounding, Effect modification, and Stratification Confounding, Effect modification, and Stratification Tunisia, 30th October 2014 Acknowledgment: Kostas Danis Takis Panagiotopoulos National Schoool of Public Health, Athens, Greece takis.panagiotopoulos@gmail.com

More information

You can t fix by analysis what you bungled by design. Fancy analysis can t fix a poorly designed study.

You can t fix by analysis what you bungled by design. Fancy analysis can t fix a poorly designed study. You can t fix by analysis what you bungled by design. Light, Singer and Willett Or, not as catchy but perhaps more accurate: Fancy analysis can t fix a poorly designed study. Producing Data The Role of

More information

Biases in clinical research. Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University

Biases in clinical research. Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University Biases in clinical research Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University Learning objectives Understand goal of measurement and definition of accuracy Describe the threats to causal

More information

Bias and confounding. Mads Kamper-Jørgensen, associate professor, Section of Social Medicine

Bias and confounding. Mads Kamper-Jørgensen, associate professor, Section of Social Medicine Bias and confounding Mads Kamper-Jørgensen, associate professor, maka@sund.ku.dk PhD-course in Epidemiology l 7 February 2017 l Slide number 1 The world according to an epidemiologist Exposure Outcome

More information

Welcome to this series focused on sources of bias in epidemiologic studies. In this first module, I will provide a general overview of bias.

Welcome to this series focused on sources of bias in epidemiologic studies. In this first module, I will provide a general overview of bias. Welcome to this series focused on sources of bias in epidemiologic studies. In this first module, I will provide a general overview of bias. In the second module, we will focus on selection bias and in

More information

BIOSTATISTICAL METHODS

BIOSTATISTICAL METHODS BIOSTATISTICAL METHODS FOR TRANSLATIONAL & CLINICAL RESEARCH PROPENSITY SCORE Confounding Definition: A situation in which the effect or association between an exposure (a predictor or risk factor) and

More information

University of Wollongong. Research Online. Australian Health Services Research Institute

University of Wollongong. Research Online. Australian Health Services Research Institute University of Wollongong Research Online Australian Health Services Research Institute Faculty of Business 2011 Measurement of error Janet E. Sansoni University of Wollongong, jans@uow.edu.au Publication

More information

INTERPRETATION OF STUDY FINDINGS: PART I. Julie E. Buring, ScD Harvard School of Public Health Boston, MA

INTERPRETATION OF STUDY FINDINGS: PART I. Julie E. Buring, ScD Harvard School of Public Health Boston, MA INTERPRETATION OF STUDY FINDINGS: PART I Julie E. Buring, ScD Harvard School of Public Health Boston, MA Drawing Conclusions TRUTH IN THE UNIVERSE Infer TRUTH IN THE STUDY Infer FINDINGS IN THE STUDY Designing

More information

Confounding and Effect Modification. John McGready Johns Hopkins University

Confounding and Effect Modification. John McGready Johns Hopkins University This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike License. Your use of this material constitutes acceptance of that license and the conditions of use of materials on this

More information

Intermediate Methods in Epidemiology Exercise No. 4 - Passive smoking and atherosclerosis

Intermediate Methods in Epidemiology Exercise No. 4 - Passive smoking and atherosclerosis Intermediate Methods in Epidemiology 2008 Exercise No. 4 - Passive smoking and atherosclerosis The purpose of this exercise is to allow students to recapitulate issues discussed throughout the course which

More information

To evaluate a single epidemiological article we need to know and discuss the methods used in the underlying study.

To evaluate a single epidemiological article we need to know and discuss the methods used in the underlying study. Critical reading 45 6 Critical reading As already mentioned in previous chapters, there are always effects that occur by chance, as well as systematic biases that can falsify the results in population

More information

Trial Designs. Professor Peter Cameron

Trial Designs. Professor Peter Cameron Trial Designs Professor Peter Cameron OVERVIEW Review of Observational methods Principles of experimental design applied to observational studies Population Selection Looking for bias Inference Analysis

More information

Two-sample Categorical data: Measuring association

Two-sample Categorical data: Measuring association Two-sample Categorical data: Measuring association Patrick Breheny October 27 Patrick Breheny University of Iowa Biostatistical Methods I (BIOS 5710) 1 / 40 Introduction Study designs leading to contingency

More information

EPIDEMIOLOGY-BIOSTATISTICS EXAM Midterm 2004 PRINT YOUR LEGAL NAME:

EPIDEMIOLOGY-BIOSTATISTICS EXAM Midterm 2004 PRINT YOUR LEGAL NAME: EPIDEMIOLOGY-BIOSTATISTICS EXAM Midterm 2004 PRINT YOUR LEGAL NAME: Instructions: This exam is 20% of your course grade. The maximum number of points for the course is 1,000; hence, this exam is worth

More information

CHL 5225 H Advanced Statistical Methods for Clinical Trials. CHL 5225 H The Language of Clinical Trials

CHL 5225 H Advanced Statistical Methods for Clinical Trials. CHL 5225 H The Language of Clinical Trials CHL 5225 H Advanced Statistical Methods for Clinical Trials Two sources for course material 1. Electronic blackboard required readings 2. www.andywillan.com/chl5225h code of conduct course outline schedule

More information

Challenges of Observational and Retrospective Studies

Challenges of Observational and Retrospective Studies Challenges of Observational and Retrospective Studies Kyoungmi Kim, Ph.D. March 8, 2017 This seminar is jointly supported by the following NIH-funded centers: Background There are several methods in which

More information

Smoking among shift workers: more than a confounding factor

Smoking among shift workers: more than a confounding factor Smoking among shift workers: more an a confounding factor Ludovic van Amelsvoort Nicole Jansen IJmert Kant Occupational Heal Epidemiology Maastricht University The Neerlands. Background Shift work: Associated

More information

Controlling Bias & Confounding

Controlling Bias & Confounding Controlling Bias & Confounding Chihaya Koriyama August 5 th, 2015 QUESTIONS FOR BIAS Key concepts Bias Should be minimized at the designing stage. Random errors We can do nothing at Is the nature the of

More information

Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis

Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis EFSA/EBTC Colloquium, 25 October 2017 Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis Julian Higgins University of Bristol 1 Introduction to concepts Standard

More information

Chapter 13 Summary Experiments and Observational Studies

Chapter 13 Summary Experiments and Observational Studies Chapter 13 Summary Experiments and Observational Studies What have we learned? We can recognize sample surveys, observational studies, and randomized comparative experiments. o These methods collect data

More information

5 Bias and confounding

5 Bias and confounding Bias and confounding 37 5 Bias and confounding Learning objectives In this chapter students learn about Most important factors that can systematically impair the results of a population based study, like

More information

Chapter 13. Experiments and Observational Studies. Copyright 2012, 2008, 2005 Pearson Education, Inc.

Chapter 13. Experiments and Observational Studies. Copyright 2012, 2008, 2005 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 13 Experiments and Observational Studies Copyright 2012, 2008, 2005 Pearson Education, Inc. Observational Studies In an observational study, researchers don t assign choices; they simply observe

More information

Study design. Chapter 64. Research Methodology S.B. MARTINS, A. ZIN, W. ZIN

Study design. Chapter 64. Research Methodology S.B. MARTINS, A. ZIN, W. ZIN Chapter 64 Study design S.B. MARTINS, A. ZIN, W. ZIN Research Methodology Scientific methodology comprises a set of rules and procedures to investigate the phenomena of interest. These rules and procedures

More information

Higher Psychology RESEARCH REVISION

Higher Psychology RESEARCH REVISION Higher Psychology RESEARCH REVISION 1 The biggest change from the old Higher course (up to 2014) is the possibility of an analysis and evaluation question (8-10) marks asking you to comment on aspects

More information

Welcome to this third module in a three-part series focused on epidemiologic measures of association and impact.

Welcome to this third module in a three-part series focused on epidemiologic measures of association and impact. Welcome to this third module in a three-part series focused on epidemiologic measures of association and impact. 1 This three-part series focuses on the estimation of the association between exposures

More information

RESEARCH. Dagfinn Aune, 1,2 Abhijit Sen, 1 Manya Prasad, 3 Teresa Norat, 2 Imre Janszky, 1 Serena Tonstad, 3 Pål Romundstad, 1 Lars J Vatten 1

RESEARCH. Dagfinn Aune, 1,2 Abhijit Sen, 1 Manya Prasad, 3 Teresa Norat, 2 Imre Janszky, 1 Serena Tonstad, 3 Pål Romundstad, 1 Lars J Vatten 1 open access BMI and all cause mortality: systematic review and non-linear dose-response meta-analysis of 230 cohort studies with 3.74 million deaths among 30.3 million participants Dagfinn Aune, 1,2 Abhijit

More information

The population attributable fraction and confounding: buyer beware

The population attributable fraction and confounding: buyer beware SPECIAL ISSUE: PERSPECTIVE The population attributable fraction and confounding: buyer beware Linked Comment: www.youtube.com/ijcpeditorial Linked Comment: Ghaemi & Thommi. Int J Clin Pract 2010; 64: 1009

More information

Purpose. Study Designs. Objectives. Observational Studies. Analytic Studies

Purpose. Study Designs. Objectives. Observational Studies. Analytic Studies Purpose Study Designs H.S. Teitelbaum, DO, PhD, MPH, FAOCOPM AOCOPM Annual Meeting Introduce notions of study design Clarify common terminology used with description and interpretation of information collected

More information

An Introduction to Epidemiology

An Introduction to Epidemiology An Introduction to Epidemiology Wei Liu, MPH Biostatistics Core Pennington Biomedical Research Center Baton Rouge, LA Last edited: January, 14 th, 2014 TABLE OF CONTENTS Introduction.................................................................

More information

Improved control for confounding using propensity scores and instrumental variables?

Improved control for confounding using propensity scores and instrumental variables? Improved control for confounding using propensity scores and instrumental variables? Dr. Olaf H.Klungel Dept. of Pharmacoepidemiology & Clinical Pharmacology, Utrecht Institute of Pharmaceutical Sciences

More information

Types of Biomedical Research

Types of Biomedical Research INTRODUCTION & MEASUREMENT IN CLINICAL RESEARCH Sakda Arj Ong Vallipakorn, MD MSIT, MA (Information Science) Pediatrics, Pediatric Cardiology Emergency Medicine, Ped Emergency Family Medicine Section of

More information

PubH 7405: REGRESSION ANALYSIS. Propensity Score

PubH 7405: REGRESSION ANALYSIS. Propensity Score PubH 7405: REGRESSION ANALYSIS Propensity Score INTRODUCTION: There is a growing interest in using observational (or nonrandomized) studies to estimate the effects of treatments on outcomes. In observational

More information

Instrumental Variables Estimation: An Introduction

Instrumental Variables Estimation: An Introduction Instrumental Variables Estimation: An Introduction Susan L. Ettner, Ph.D. Professor Division of General Internal Medicine and Health Services Research, UCLA The Problem The Problem Suppose you wish to

More information

12/26/2013. Types of Biomedical Research. Clinical Research. 7Steps to do research INTRODUCTION & MEASUREMENT IN CLINICAL RESEARCH S T A T I S T I C

12/26/2013. Types of Biomedical Research. Clinical Research. 7Steps to do research INTRODUCTION & MEASUREMENT IN CLINICAL RESEARCH S T A T I S T I C Types of Biomedical Research INTRODUCTION & MEASUREMENT IN CLINICAL RESEARCH Sakda Arj Ong Vallipakorn, MD MSIT, MA (Information Science) Pediatrics, Pediatric Cardiology Emergency Medicine, Ped Emergency

More information

DECLARATION OF CONFLICT OF INTEREST

DECLARATION OF CONFLICT OF INTEREST DECLARATION OF CONFLICT OF INTEREST Warfarin and the risk of major bleeding events in patients with atrial fibrillation: a population-based study Laurent Azoulay PhD 1,2, Sophie Dell Aniello MSc 1, Teresa

More information

Is There An Association?

Is There An Association? Is There An Association? Exposure (Risk Factor) Outcome Exposures Risk factors Preventive measures Management strategy Independent variables Outcomes Dependent variable Disease occurrence Lack of exercise

More information

Rapid appraisal of the literature: Identifying study biases

Rapid appraisal of the literature: Identifying study biases Rapid appraisal of the literature: Identifying study biases Rita Popat, PhD Clinical Assistant Professor Division of Epidemiology Stanford University School of Medicine August 7, 2007 What is critical

More information

observational studies Descriptive studies

observational studies Descriptive studies form one stage within this broader sequence, which begins with laboratory studies using animal models, thence to human testing: Phase I: The new drug or treatment is tested in a small group of people for

More information

Bias. Sam Bracebridge

Bias. Sam Bracebridge Bias Sam Bracebridge Bias Errors in epidemiological measurements Based on the slides of Sam Bracebridge By the end of the lecture fellows will be able to Define bias Identify different types of bias Explain

More information

Epidemiologic Methods and Counting Infections: The Basics of Surveillance

Epidemiologic Methods and Counting Infections: The Basics of Surveillance Epidemiologic Methods and Counting Infections: The Basics of Surveillance Ebbing Lautenbach, MD, MPH, MSCE University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine Nothing to disclose PENN Outline Definitions / Historical

More information

Lecture 5 Conducting Interviews and Focus Groups

Lecture 5 Conducting Interviews and Focus Groups Lecture 5 Conducting Interviews and Focus Groups Talking to participants enables in-depth information about the experiences of health and illness; and of factors that influence health and illness behaviour

More information

Introduction to Observational Studies. Jane Pinelis

Introduction to Observational Studies. Jane Pinelis Introduction to Observational Studies Jane Pinelis 22 March 2018 Outline Motivating example Observational studies vs. randomized experiments Observational studies: basics Some adjustment strategies Matching

More information

Understanding Statistics for Research Staff!

Understanding Statistics for Research Staff! Statistics for Dummies? Understanding Statistics for Research Staff! Those of us who DO the research, but not the statistics. Rachel Enriquez, RN PhD Epidemiologist Why do we do Clinical Research? Epidemiology

More information

John J.P. Kastelein MD PhD Professor of Medicine Dept. of Vascular Medicine Academic Medial Center / University of Amsterdam

John J.P. Kastelein MD PhD Professor of Medicine Dept. of Vascular Medicine Academic Medial Center / University of Amsterdam Latest Insights from the JUPITER Study John J.P. Kastelein MD PhD Professor of Medicine Dept. of Vascular Medicine Academic Medial Center / University of Amsterdam Inflammation, hscrp, and Vascular Prevention

More information

Intervention vs. Observational Trials:

Intervention vs. Observational Trials: Intervention vs. Observational Trials: A Brief Introduction Introduction Just like an architect translates a vision for a building into blueprints, so an investigator translates their research idea into

More information

Understanding Confounding in Research Kantahyanee W. Murray and Anne Duggan. DOI: /pir

Understanding Confounding in Research Kantahyanee W. Murray and Anne Duggan. DOI: /pir Understanding Confounding in Research Kantahyanee W. Murray and Anne Duggan Pediatr. Rev. 2010;31;124-126 DOI: 10.1542/pir.31-3-124 The online version of this article, along with updated information and

More information

The role of Randomized Controlled Trials

The role of Randomized Controlled Trials The role of Randomized Controlled Trials Dr. Georgia Salanti Lecturer in Epidemiology University of Ioannina School of Medicine Outline Understanding study designs and the role of confounding Observational

More information

Analysis methods for improved external validity

Analysis methods for improved external validity Analysis methods for improved external validity Elizabeth Stuart Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health Department of Mental Health Department of Biostatistics www.biostat.jhsph.edu/ estuart estuart@jhsph.edu

More information

Instrumental Variables I (cont.)

Instrumental Variables I (cont.) Review Instrumental Variables Observational Studies Cross Sectional Regressions Omitted Variables, Reverse causation Randomized Control Trials Difference in Difference Time invariant omitted variables

More information

Stratified Tables. Example: Effect of seat belt use on accident fatality

Stratified Tables. Example: Effect of seat belt use on accident fatality Stratified Tables Often, a third measure influences the relationship between the two primary measures (i.e. disease and exposure). How do we remove or control for the effect of the third measure? Issues

More information

RANDOMIZED CLINICAL TRIALS. Study Designs. Randomized Clinical Trials (RCT) ASSIGN EXPOSURE Follow up Check for OUTCOME. Experimental studies

RANDOMIZED CLINICAL TRIALS. Study Designs. Randomized Clinical Trials (RCT) ASSIGN EXPOSURE Follow up Check for OUTCOME. Experimental studies RANDOMIZED CLINICAL TRIALS Study Designs Observational studies Analytic studies Descriptive studies Randomized Controlled trials Case control Cohort Cross sectional Randomized Clinical Trials (RCT) Assign

More information

Sensitivity Analysis in Observational Research: Introducing the E-value

Sensitivity Analysis in Observational Research: Introducing the E-value Sensitivity Analysis in Observational Research: Introducing the E-value Tyler J. VanderWeele Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health Departments of Epidemiology and Biostatistics 1 Plan of Presentation

More information

AP Statistics Exam Review: Strand 2: Sampling and Experimentation Date:

AP Statistics Exam Review: Strand 2: Sampling and Experimentation Date: AP Statistics NAME: Exam Review: Strand 2: Sampling and Experimentation Date: Block: II. Sampling and Experimentation: Planning and conducting a study (10%-15%) Data must be collected according to a well-developed

More information

A NEW TRIAL DESIGN FULLY INTEGRATING BIOMARKER INFORMATION FOR THE EVALUATION OF TREATMENT-EFFECT MECHANISMS IN PERSONALISED MEDICINE

A NEW TRIAL DESIGN FULLY INTEGRATING BIOMARKER INFORMATION FOR THE EVALUATION OF TREATMENT-EFFECT MECHANISMS IN PERSONALISED MEDICINE A NEW TRIAL DESIGN FULLY INTEGRATING BIOMARKER INFORMATION FOR THE EVALUATION OF TREATMENT-EFFECT MECHANISMS IN PERSONALISED MEDICINE Dr Richard Emsley Centre for Biostatistics, Institute of Population

More information

Trial designs fully integrating biomarker information for the evaluation of treatment-effect mechanisms in stratified medicine

Trial designs fully integrating biomarker information for the evaluation of treatment-effect mechanisms in stratified medicine Trial designs fully integrating biomarker information for the evaluation of treatment-effect mechanisms in stratified medicine Dr Richard Emsley Centre for Biostatistics, Institute of Population Health,

More information

Causal Association : Cause To Effect. Dr. Akhilesh Bhargava MD, DHA, PGDHRM Prof. Community Medicine & Director-SIHFW, Jaipur

Causal Association : Cause To Effect. Dr. Akhilesh Bhargava MD, DHA, PGDHRM Prof. Community Medicine & Director-SIHFW, Jaipur Causal Association : Cause To Effect Dr. MD, DHA, PGDHRM Prof. Community Medicine & Director-SIHFW, Jaipur Measure of Association- Concepts If more disease occurs in a group that smokes compared to the

More information

FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS MINIMAL DATA SET (MDS)

FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS MINIMAL DATA SET (MDS) FREQUENTLY ASKED QUESTIONS MINIMAL DATA SET (MDS) Date in parentheses is the date the question was added to the list or updated. Last update 6/25/05 DEFINITIONS 1. What counts as the first call? (6/24/05)

More information

Comparing Proportions between Two Independent Populations. John McGready Johns Hopkins University

Comparing Proportions between Two Independent Populations. John McGready Johns Hopkins University This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike License. Your use of this material constitutes acceptance of that license and the conditions of use of materials on this

More information

M6728. Goals. Depression Scores. Research Designs

M6728. Goals. Depression Scores. Research Designs M6728 Research Designs Goals Match appropriate research designs to the study purpose and questions Differentiate between experimental and non-experimental studies Discuss epidemiologic designs Evaluate

More information

Determinants of quality: Factors that lower or increase the quality of evidence

Determinants of quality: Factors that lower or increase the quality of evidence Determinants of quality: Factors that lower or increase the quality of evidence GRADE Workshop CBO, NHG and Dutch Cochrane Centre CBO, April 17th, 2013 Outline The GRADE approach: step by step Factors

More information

UNIT 5 - Association Causation, Effect Modification and Validity

UNIT 5 - Association Causation, Effect Modification and Validity 5 UNIT 5 - Association Causation, Effect Modification and Validity Introduction In Unit 1 we introduced the concept of causality in epidemiology and presented different ways in which causes can be understood

More information

Lecture 4. Confounding

Lecture 4. Confounding Lecture 4 Confounding Learning Objectives In this set of lectures we will: - Formally define confounding and give explicit examples of it s impact - Define adjustment and adjusted estimates conceptually

More information

EVect of measurement error on epidemiological studies of environmental and occupational

EVect of measurement error on epidemiological studies of environmental and occupational Occup Environ Med 1998;55:651 656 651 METHODOLOGY Series editors: T C Aw, A Cockcroft, R McNamee Correspondence to: Dr Ben G Armstrong, Environmental Epidemiology Unit, London School of Hygiene and Tropical

More information

Aetiology versus Prediction - correct for Confounding? Friedo Dekker ERA-EDTA Registry / LUMC

Aetiology versus Prediction - correct for Confounding? Friedo Dekker ERA-EDTA Registry / LUMC Aetiology versus Prediction - correct for Confounding? Friedo Dekker ERA-EDTA Registry / LUMC Aetiology Study effect of a risk factor on an outcome Consider potential confounding: other risk factor for

More information

Glossary From Running Randomized Evaluations: A Practical Guide, by Rachel Glennerster and Kudzai Takavarasha

Glossary From Running Randomized Evaluations: A Practical Guide, by Rachel Glennerster and Kudzai Takavarasha Glossary From Running Randomized Evaluations: A Practical Guide, by Rachel Glennerster and Kudzai Takavarasha attrition: When data are missing because we are unable to measure the outcomes of some of the

More information

INTRODUCTION TO EPIDEMIOLOGICAL STUDY DESIGNS PHUNLERD PIYARAJ, MD., MHS., PHD.

INTRODUCTION TO EPIDEMIOLOGICAL STUDY DESIGNS PHUNLERD PIYARAJ, MD., MHS., PHD. INTRODUCTION TO EPIDEMIOLOGICAL STUDY DESIGNS PHUNLERD PIYARAJ, MD., MHS., PHD. 1 OBJECTIVES By the end of this section, you will be able to: Provide a definition of epidemiology Describe the major types

More information

Methodology for Non-Randomized Clinical Trials: Propensity Score Analysis Dan Conroy, Ph.D., inventiv Health, Burlington, MA

Methodology for Non-Randomized Clinical Trials: Propensity Score Analysis Dan Conroy, Ph.D., inventiv Health, Burlington, MA PharmaSUG 2014 - Paper SP08 Methodology for Non-Randomized Clinical Trials: Propensity Score Analysis Dan Conroy, Ph.D., inventiv Health, Burlington, MA ABSTRACT Randomized clinical trials serve as the

More information

Module 4 Introduction

Module 4 Introduction Module 4 Introduction Recall the Big Picture: We begin a statistical investigation with a research question. The investigation proceeds with the following steps: Produce Data: Determine what to measure,

More information

Incorporating Clinical Information into the Label

Incorporating Clinical Information into the Label ECC Population Health Group LLC expertise-driven consulting in global maternal-child health & pharmacoepidemiology Incorporating Clinical Information into the Label Labels without Categories: A Workshop

More information

Overview of Clinical Study Design Laura Lee Johnson, Ph.D. Statistician National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine US National

Overview of Clinical Study Design Laura Lee Johnson, Ph.D. Statistician National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine US National Overview of Clinical Study Design Laura Lee Johnson, Ph.D. Statistician National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine US National Institutes of Health 2012 How Many Taken a similar course

More information

Biostatistics and Epidemiology Step 1 Sample Questions Set 1

Biostatistics and Epidemiology Step 1 Sample Questions Set 1 Biostatistics and Epidemiology Step 1 Sample Questions Set 1 1. A study wishes to assess birth characteristics in a population. Which of the following variables describes the appropriate measurement scale

More information

Study Design STUDY DESIGN CASE SERIES AND CROSS-SECTIONAL STUDY DESIGN

Study Design STUDY DESIGN CASE SERIES AND CROSS-SECTIONAL STUDY DESIGN STUDY DESIGN CASE SERIES AND CROSS-SECTIONAL Daniel E. Ford, MD, MPH Vice Dean for Clinical Investigation Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Introduction to Clinical Research July 15, 2014 STUDY DESIGN Provides

More information

Is a Mediterranean diet best for preventing heart disease?

Is a Mediterranean diet best for preventing heart disease? Is a Mediterranean diet best for preventing heart disease? By Peter Attia, M.D. This week an article titled Primary Prevention of Cardiovascular Disease with a Mediterranean Diet was featured in the New

More information

Chapter 8 Statistical Principles of Design. Fall 2010

Chapter 8 Statistical Principles of Design. Fall 2010 Chapter 8 Statistical Principles of Design Fall 2010 Experimental Design Many interesting questions in biology involve relationships between response variables and one or more explanatory variables. Biology

More information

Study Designs. Randomized Clinical Trials (RCT) RCT: Example 1. RCT: Two by Two Table. Outcome. Exposure. Yes a b No c d

Study Designs. Randomized Clinical Trials (RCT) RCT: Example 1. RCT: Two by Two Table. Outcome. Exposure. Yes a b No c d Study Designs Randomized Clinical Trials (RCT) Assign Exposure Randomly Experimental studies Analytic studies Observational studies Descriptive studies POPULATION Population People without the outcome

More information

Propensity score methods to adjust for confounding in assessing treatment effects: bias and precision

Propensity score methods to adjust for confounding in assessing treatment effects: bias and precision ISPUB.COM The Internet Journal of Epidemiology Volume 7 Number 2 Propensity score methods to adjust for confounding in assessing treatment effects: bias and precision Z Wang Abstract There is an increasing

More information

OCW Epidemiology and Biostatistics, 2010 Michael D. Kneeland, MD November 18, 2010 SCREENING. Learning Objectives for this session:

OCW Epidemiology and Biostatistics, 2010 Michael D. Kneeland, MD November 18, 2010 SCREENING. Learning Objectives for this session: OCW Epidemiology and Biostatistics, 2010 Michael D. Kneeland, MD November 18, 2010 SCREENING Learning Objectives for this session: 1) Know the objectives of a screening program 2) Define and calculate

More information

Chapter 2 A Guide to Implementing Quantitative Bias Analysis

Chapter 2 A Guide to Implementing Quantitative Bias Analysis Chapter 2 A Guide to Implementing Quantitative Bias Analysis Introduction Estimates of association from nonrandomized epidemiologic studies are susceptible to two types of error: random error and systematic

More information

In this second module in the clinical trials series, we will focus on design considerations for Phase III clinical trials. Phase III clinical trials

In this second module in the clinical trials series, we will focus on design considerations for Phase III clinical trials. Phase III clinical trials In this second module in the clinical trials series, we will focus on design considerations for Phase III clinical trials. Phase III clinical trials are comparative, large scale studies that typically

More information

Chapter 6. Experiments in the Real World. Chapter 6 1

Chapter 6. Experiments in the Real World. Chapter 6 1 Chapter 6 Experiments in the Real World Chapter 6 1 Thought Question 1 Suppose you are interested in determining if drinking a glass of red wine each day helps prevent heartburn. You recruit 40 adults

More information

Choosing the right study design

Choosing the right study design Choosing the right study design Main types of study design Randomised controlled trial (RCT) Cohort study Case-control study Cross-sectional study Case series/case note review Expert opinion BEST QUALITY

More information

SAMPLING AND SAMPLE SIZE

SAMPLING AND SAMPLE SIZE SAMPLING AND SAMPLE SIZE Andrew Zeitlin Georgetown University and IGC Rwanda With slides from Ben Olken and the World Bank s Development Impact Evaluation Initiative 2 Review We want to learn how a program

More information

The ROBINS-I tool is reproduced from riskofbias.info with the permission of the authors. The tool should not be modified for use.

The ROBINS-I tool is reproduced from riskofbias.info with the permission of the authors. The tool should not be modified for use. Table A. The Risk Of Bias In Non-romized Studies of Interventions (ROBINS-I) I) assessment tool The ROBINS-I tool is reproduced from riskofbias.info with the permission of the auths. The tool should not

More information

Title: Home Exposure to Arabian Incense (Bakhour) and Asthma Symptoms in Children: A Community Survey in Two Regions in Oman

Title: Home Exposure to Arabian Incense (Bakhour) and Asthma Symptoms in Children: A Community Survey in Two Regions in Oman Author's response to reviews Title: Home Exposure to Arabian Incense (Bakhour) and Asthma Symptoms in Children: A Community Survey in Two Regions in Oman Authors: Omar A Al-Rawas (orawas@squ.edu.om) Abdullah

More information

Online Supplementary Material

Online Supplementary Material Section 1. Adapted Newcastle-Ottawa Scale The adaptation consisted of allowing case-control studies to earn a star when the case definition is based on record linkage, to liken the evaluation of case-control

More information

Complier Average Causal Effect (CACE)

Complier Average Causal Effect (CACE) Complier Average Causal Effect (CACE) Booil Jo Stanford University Methodological Advancement Meeting Innovative Directions in Estimating Impact Office of Planning, Research & Evaluation Administration

More information

Bias. A systematic error (caused by the investigator or the subjects) that causes an incorrect (overor under-) estimate of an association.

Bias. A systematic error (caused by the investigator or the subjects) that causes an incorrect (overor under-) estimate of an association. Bias A systematic error (caused by the investigator or the subjects) that causes an incorrect (overor under-) estimate of an association. Here, random error is small, but systematic errors have led to

More information

Experimental Design. Terminology. Chusak Okascharoen, MD, PhD September 19 th, Experimental study Clinical trial Randomized controlled trial

Experimental Design. Terminology. Chusak Okascharoen, MD, PhD September 19 th, Experimental study Clinical trial Randomized controlled trial Experimental Design Chusak Okascharoen, MD, PhD September 19 th, 2016 Terminology Experimental study Clinical trial Randomized controlled trial 1 PHASES OF CLINICAL TRIALS Phase I: First-time-in-man studies

More information

3. Factors such as race, age, sex, and a person s physiological state are all considered determinants of disease. a. True

3. Factors such as race, age, sex, and a person s physiological state are all considered determinants of disease. a. True / False 1. Epidemiology is the basic science of public health. LEARNING OBJECTIVES: CNIA.BOYL.17.2.1 - Define epidemiology. 2. Within the field of epidemiology, the term distribution refers to the relationship

More information

Risks and benefits of weight loss: challenges to obesity research

Risks and benefits of weight loss: challenges to obesity research European Heart Journal Supplements (2005) 7 (Supplement L), L27 L31 doi:10.1093/eurheartj/sui083 Risks and benefits of weight loss: challenges to obesity research Donna Ryan* Pennington Biomedical Research

More information