Chapter 3 Gender and Motivation for Achievement, Affiliation Intimacy, and Power

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Chapter 3 Gender and Motivation for Achievement, Affiliation Intimacy, and Power"

Transcription

1 Chapter 3 Gender and Motivation for Achievement, Affiliation Intimacy, and Power Lauren E. Duncan and Bill E. Peterson Research on achievement, affiliation intimacy, and power motivation is tied intimately to the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), co-developed by Christiana D. Morgan and Henry A. Murray (1935). In the TAT, respondents are shown pictures of people (often line drawings) and asked to write a story in response to each picture cue. Researchers assume that test takers will tell stories that reflect their own conscious and unconscious motives and impulses. As the methodology evolved, these stories (or thought samples) were content coded by experts for recurring themes that reflect major human motives. The three most studied motives, also referred to as social motives, are achievement, affiliation intimacy, and power. Early researchers conducted experiments with samples of college men to derive objective categories that would enable researchers to code stories for motive content (e.g., Atkinson, 1958; McClelland, Atkinson, Clark, & Lowell, 1953). For example, one group of men would be put in an achievement-arousing situation and another would be put in a neutral situation. Differences in the types of stories composed by men in the two groups were assumed to reflect differences in achievement motivation. Decades of research have shown that the resulting scoring definitions for each motive can be applied to written material in a reliable way by different researchers. In order to assess social motives, individuals write stories under neutral arousal conditions. Individuals are hypothesized to tell stories that reveal the motive concerns uppermost in their minds. For example, someone high in achievement motivation is expected to write stories filled with images of unique accomplishments under neutral arousal conditions; someone high in affiliation intimacy motivation is expected to write stories about maintaining close interpersonal relationships. This arousal methodology was used to develop and elaborate the constructs of achievement, affiliation intimacy, and power motivation. Achievement motivation is defined as a concern for standards of excellence and doing well on tasks. According to Winter (1991a), written imagery that indicates the presence of achievement motivation includes adjectives that evaluate positive performance, goals or performances that are described as successes, success in competition with others, unique accomplishments, and negative affect expressed in the face of failure. The affiliation intimacy motive is a concern for establishing, maintaining, and repairing friendly relationships as well as experiencing warm and close interactions with others. Key imagery includes companionate activities; nurturant acts; expressions of warm, positive, friendly feelings toward other people; and negative affect about the disruption of friendly relationships. Power motivation involves a heightened concern about having impact or influence over other people. Key written images that denote power motivation include strong vigorous actions that necessarily impact others, behaviors that arouse L.E. Duncan (B) Smith College, Northampton, MA, USA J.C. Chrisler, D.R. McCreary (eds.), Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology, DOI / _3, C Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

2 42 L.E. Duncan and B.E. Peterson strong emotions in another person, attempts to influence others, mentions of prestige or fame, control or regulation of other people, and giving help that is not explicitly solicited. Those who conduct research on achievement, affiliation intimacy, and power motivation are encouraged to use the term picture story exercise (PSE) to denote the systematic administration of picture cues to participants (Schultheiss & Pang, 2007). Older studies of implicit motives, though, may use the term TAT in honor of Morgan and Murray s contributions to understanding human personality. However, the contemporary use of the PSE recognizes the 70 years of experimental work that have gone into the refinement and assessment of achievement, affiliation intimacy, and power motivation. In order to distinguish PSE motives from other types of motivational constructs (e.g., the hunger motive), the terms social motives and implicit motives are often used to refer to PSE achievement, affiliation intimacy, and power as a group. For any given motive, n X might be used in place of X motivation. Thus, n Power refers to power motivation. Some researchers use abbreviations when referring to each motive (i.e., nach, naff, npow), but we will not do so here. In this chapter we examine how gender has been treated by social motive researchers. In our review, we emphasize research published since the early 1980s, when Stewart and Chester (1982) last reviewed research on gender and PSE-based motives in a comprehensive way. First, though, we set the stage by discussing recent trends in motive research, best practices for assessing motives in men and women, and the difference between implicit and explicit motives. Reemergence of Interest in Social Motives The peak of social motive research occurred between the 1950s and the 1970s (Atkinson, 1958; Heyns, Veroff, & Atkinson, 1958; McAdams, 1980; McClelland, 1975; McClelland et al., 1953; Winter, 1973; Winter & Stewart, 1978). Probably because of questions about reliability and the time-intensive nature involved in the coding of social motives, psychological research after the 1970s tended to focus more on Likert-scaled questionnaire items that assessed achievement, affiliation, and power as trait-like variables. However, in the past decade, there seems to have been a resurgence of interest in studying implicit social motives. One reason new research has increased in the past decade may be attributable to Winter s (1991a, 1991b) Manual for Scoring Motive Imagery in Running Text. This manual consolidated existing scoring systems for achievement, affiliation intimacy, and power in such a way that investigators can reliably learn to content code all three motives at one time. Furthermore, the coding system can be applied to any kind of written document (including archival materials) that is not purely technical (e.g., PSE stories, letters, novels). Although not all social motive researchers use the running text system, it has opened up new content areas for possible exploration. For example, in a recent study, Winter (2007) used the running text system to score political documents to examine the role of social motives in international conflict escalation. He coded the diplomatic exchanges, speeches, and media commentary made by key figures of opposing countries locked in potential military disputes and found that the documents related to crises that eventually led to armed conflict (e.g., the US Bay of Pigs Invasion) contained more power imagery by both sides involved, whereas documents related to crises that were resolved peacefully contained more achievement and affiliation intimacy imagery (e.g., the Cuban Missile Crisis). In addition to studying war and peace (Winter, 1993, 2003, 2005; see also Smith, 2008), Winter and his colleagues have used social motives to predict the behaviors of US presidents at the aggregate (e.g., Winter, 1987) and individual levels (e.g., Winter, 1998, 2005; Winter, Hermann, Weintraub, & Walker, 1991). Because most political documents recorded statements originally uttered or written by men, there is a dearth

3 3 Gender and Motivation for Achievement, Affiliation Intimacy, and Power 43 of research on women s political leadership and social motivation. The 2008 US presidential election, with the prominence of Republican Sarah Palin and Democrat Hillary Clinton, punctuates the fact that this is clearly an area too long neglected but ripe for investigation. Research on women business leaders and politicians is needed and would enhance our understanding of how social motives relate to the behavior of contemporary leaders. In addition to studying a leader s motivation at a distance by coding political (and other) documents, social motive investigators have increased their use of standard experimental and survey research designs. Much of this work has focused on establishing relationships between social motives and other existing constructs. For example, Woike (Woike, Gershkovich, Piorkowski, & Polo, 1999; Woike, Lavezzary, & Barsky, 2001; Woike & Polo, 2001) has focused attention on how implicit motives influence the content and structure of autobiographical memory. In general terms, her work demonstrates that variations in the content, structure, and affect of autobiographical memories are accounted for by individual differences in social motivation. For example, Woike et al. (1999) showed that high intimacy individuals (who were also low on achievement and power) discussed emotional memories in ways that emphasized integration of themes rather than differentiation. Other constructs recently studied in relation to social motives include creativity (Fodor & Carver, 2000), emotions (Fodor, Wick, & Hartsen, 2005; Zurbriggen & Sturman, 2002), agency and communion (Saragovi, Aube, Koestner, & Zuroff, 2002), identity (Hofer, Busch, Chasiotis, & Kiessling, 2006), and life satisfaction (Brunstein, Schultheiss, & Grassmann, 1998; Hofer & Chasiotis, 2003; Hofer, Chasiotis, & Campos, 2006). It is interesting that of the 16 studies conducted in the 11 articles cited in this paragraph, 2 of them sampled men only, 4 sampled women only, and 1 did not report the gender of the participants; only 9 sampled both women and men (women made up 54% of these mixed samples). Given the evidence of unbalanced gender ratios in some of these studies, researchers interested in the meaning of gender for the expression of social motives have much work ahead of them. For most variables the correlates of social motives do not differ for women and men; however, there may be some domains (e.g., types of achievement sanctioned by society and accessible to individuals) where it is important to sample both women and men in comprehensive ways. Best Practices for Measuring Social Motives in Women and Men Social motive researchers have spent decades developing reliable and valid coding systems for assessing achievement, affiliation intimacy, and power (Smith, 1992). As noted earlier, Winter (1991b) adapted these scoring systems into a running text system that allows researchers to code all three types of motives at one time. However, for those doing laboratory research with the PSE, comparable efforts have not yet been made to establish a set of procedures to evoke motive imagery. This means that laboratory researchers have had to use their own intuitions or advice from senior colleagues about the best picture cues to use to assess motives. Because different labs use different picture cues, the comparison of absolute levels of motive scores across (and sometimes within) labs becomes impossible. For example, in an ambitious study, Veroff, Depner, Kulka, and Douvan (1980) and Veroff, Reuman, and Feld (1984) assessed social motives in two US national samples in 1957 and Unfortunately, in part because no standardized set of cues was available, Veroff and his colleagues made the decision to use different sets of picture cues for men (featuring male protagonists) and women (featuring female protagonists). For example, Card 5 for men depicted seven men grouped around a conference table, whereas Card 5 for women showed two women preparing food in a kitchen. The rationale was that matching the gender of participants with the gender of people portrayed in the cues was necessary to allow respondents to project their own desires onto

4 44 L.E. Duncan and B.E. Peterson the characters. However, PSE researchers have noted that different picture cards pull for, or evoke, different kinds of motive imagery. For example, the picture of a ship captain tends to elicit more images of power (rather than achievement or affiliation) from both women and men. Thus, when different PSE cards are used with women and men, it becomes difficult to compare absolute levels of motive imagery across gender. In the studies above, for example, it may be that the picture cues used with women (e.g., Card 5) pulled for more affiliation than the picture cues used with men did. The cues that depict men, by contrast, might have evoked more power or achievement imagery than those that depict women. In order to address the lack of a standardized set of picture cues to use with modern motive scoring systems, Schultheiss and Brunstein (2001) and Pang and Schultheiss (2005) focused their efforts on establishing baseline motive information on classic picture cues. They tested six picture cues often used by researchers, three of which depicted both men and women (couple on bench by river, trapeze artists, and nightclub scene). Of the remaining three cues, two featured male protagonists (ship captain, boxer) and one featured female protagonists (two women in lab coats in laboratory). In these two studies the authors addressed issues such as gender differences in motive strength (women wrote longer stories and scored higher than did men on affiliation intimacy, which is discussed later), cue strength in pulling for the expression of certain motives, and order effects in the presentation of picture cues. It is a good idea for those who want to conduct research on social motives to consult this work, especially Schultheiss and Pang (2007), who provided concrete advice about motive scoring systems, picture cues, test administration, and data management (see also Chusmir, 1983; Lundy, 1988). Schultheiss and Pang (2007) suggested that researchers administer between four and eight picture cues in relaxed conditions, use cues that pull for motives of interest, and use cues that are similar to the situations in which dependent variables are assessed. Schultheiss and Pang s (2007) efforts to standardize PSE administration in this way seem reasonable. Clearly, summarizing future work that examines social motives and gender will be made easier if researchers use the same picture cues for women and men across samples. Right now, fortunately, most researchers routinely use the same PSE cues for male and female study participants. (Note that the scoring definitions for achievement, affiliation intimacy, and power contained in Winter s running text system are the same for stories written by women and men. Ideally, the identity and gender of the respondent is completely masked before scoring begins.) Distinction Between Implicit and Explicit Measures One final topic needs discussion before we consider each social motive more closely. Researchers who use the PSE have agreed for some time that social motives correlate only weakly with selfreport scales of similar motives and traits (but see Thrash, Elliot, & Schultheiss, 2007, for recent work that questions this point). For example, in his meta-analysis, Spangler (1992) found that the average correlation between need for achievement and questionnaire measures of achievement was Findings like these led McClelland and his colleagues (Koestner, Weinberger, & McClelland, 1991; McClelland, 1980; McClelland, Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989) to argue that the PSE provides an operant measure of implicit motives, whereas questionnaire measures (where items are assessed on Likert scales) provide respondent measures of explicit motives. McClelland argued that PSEbased motives tap into people s nonconscious intrinsic desires to achieve, affiliate, or exercise power; expression of the motive is rewarding in and of itself. The PSE should be best at predicting long-term trends in behavior where people are free to choose their own activities. By contrast, questionnairebased motives (and traits) assess a person s explicitly articulated (i.e., conscious) image of self;

5 3 Gender and Motivation for Achievement, Affiliation Intimacy, and Power 45 people high in a self-attributed motive seek to maintain consistency in social situations where motive expression is expected. Thus, people who score high on a questionnaire measure of affiliation may find it important to act sociably at dinner parties with strangers. Because social motives are sometimes out of awareness, people high on need for affiliation intimacy would desire close relations with other people, but might or might not act sociably in a public setting depending upon how comfortable they felt with the other people at the parties. They value friendships and partnerships but choose them on their own terms rather than acting affiliative to maintain consistency between their behavior and their self-image. This distinction between implicit and explicit measures continues to be refined by contemporary researchers (e.g., Schultheiss, 2007a, 2008). Researchers have used the fact that PSE and questionnaire motives rarely correlate to study the interactive effects of implicit and explicit traits and motives. For example, in two samples of adult women, Winter, John, Stewart, Klohnen, and Duncan (1998) found that the trait of extraversion tended to facilitate the expression of affiliation and power motivation, whereas the trait of introversion tended to interfere with motive expression. In other work, Woike (2008) introduced a model that stresses the importance of the two-motive systems for understanding the formation of autobiographical memories. In King s (1995) examination of implicit and self-attributed motives, there was no relationship between the two types of measures. However, she found that personal strivings for power correlated with PSE power for men but not for women. Although these results were based on a small sample of men (N = 28), King suggested that the relationship of implicit and self-attributed motivation might be an interesting avenue in which to examine the relations between gender and motivation. The relationship of social expectations to the expression and satisfaction of motiverelated strivings is an area ripe for future research. For the purposes of this chapter, however, keep in mind that we are reviewing only PSE-based implicit motives that use variations of the PSE for assessment (McClelland et al., 1989; Sheldon, King, Houser-Marko, Osbaldiston, & Gunz, 2007; see Spangler, 1992, for a review of explicit measures of achievement, affiliation, and power motivation). Achievement Motivation The need for achievement was defined by McClelland (1985) as a concern with doing things better, with surpassing standards of excellence (p. 190). Decades of research have shown n Achievement to be related to moderate risk taking, responsiveness to feedback, future-time-orientation, personal responsibility for performance outcomes, and participation in entrepreneurial activity (see McClelland & Koestner, 1992, for an overview of correlates). It is not surprising that research on gender and achievement motivation has been influenced by prevailing views about gender differences. Stewart and Chester (1982) focused their review of these differences on two areas differential responses to achievement arousal and behavioral correlates of achievement. At the time their chapter was published, many researchers believed that it was difficult to arouse achievement motivation in women, most likely because achievement was seen as an exclusively masculine concern. It was also quite likely that because the original arousal studies had only male participants, key imagery to distinguish between high and low achievement-motivated women was left out of the coding categories. Stewart and Chester (1982) reviewed the early research on the arousal of n Achievement, and, after pointing out flaws in research design and missed opportunities to interpret main effects, they concluded that there is no consistent evidence that the motive was difficult or impossible to arouse in women. In particular, they argued that researchers, without realizing it, were finding that expressions of n Achievement for men were restricted to the traditional domains of work and leadership and that this is linked to a rigid masculine gender role. For women, on the

6 46 L.E. Duncan and B.E. Peterson other hand, n Achievement could be aroused with cues from a wide variety of domains because achievement opportunities in the traditional domain of work were less available to women at that time. In their review of the behavioral correlates of achievement motivation in women and men, Stewart and Chester (1982) concluded that there were no gender differences in the relationship between achievement and performance on laboratory tasks, behavior outside the laboratory, or scores on personality variables. For both women and men, n Achievement was positively related to quick and accurate performance on timed verbal and mathematical tests, career success in people who valued paid employment, and preference for taking academic classes with equally skilled classmates. Later research confirmed these early findings (e.g., McClelland & Franz, 1992; McClelland & Koestner, 1992). The expression of n Achievement, however, does seem sensitive to prevailing gender role norms. For example, Veroff (1982) examined two national data sets collected in 1957 and 1976 and found that, for men, scores on n Achievement remained consistent, whereas, for women, achievement motivation scores increased over that time period. Veroff argued that the increase in achievement motivation between 1957 and 1976 was directly attributable to the changes in gender role expectations and norms that resulted from the Women s Movement of the 1960s and the 1970s. Veroff also reported that college-educated women and men scored higher in achievement motivation than did their high school-educated peers, and that, for women, n Achievement varied depending on birth cohort. Taken together, these results imply that n Achievement is sensitive to the social context. To bolster this argument, Veroff (1982) reported a variety of positive correlates with achievement motivation and successful traditional gender role socialization in the same sample. For example, for men, high n Achievement was positively related to preferring work to leisure, seeing work as fulfilling a major life value, not seeing work as interfering with family but also with strong feelings of efficacy, happiness, marital satisfaction, low drug use, integration into the community, and good health. For women, high n Achievement was positively related to being the oldest child in the family, participating in challenging leisure time activities, seeing leisure as fulfilling a major life value, high marital interaction, and reporting many experiences of zest. Veroff argued that these findings show that for many women and men, n Achievement is correlated with excellence in traditionally gendered domains men in achieving at work, women in achieving at home, as wives and mothers. He concluded that n Achievement is related for many (but not all) people to successful gender role socialization. Consistent with Veroff s work, Elder and MacInnis (1983) found that high achievement women born in the early 1920s with family orientations tended to focus their adult lives on raising families, whereas high n Achievement women with work interests started families at a later age. It seems likely that achievement motivation is often channeled into traditionally gendered outlets but that if there are opportunities that allow excellence in non-traditional domains, some individuals will pursue achievement in those domains (see, e.g., Peterson & Stewart, 1993). For example, in research conducted in the 1980s, when women managers were relatively rare, Chusmir (1985) found that 62 female managers enrolled in an MBA program had higher achievement and power motivation than did 62 male managers enrolled in the same program (there were no gender differences in affiliation intimacy). Chusmir argued for a selection effect. That is, women who became managers in the 1970s and the 1980s probably had higher levels of achievement motivation than did men who became managers at the same time because management was a non-traditional occupation for women, and only those women looking for an occupational arena in which to express their n Achievement would seek out management positions. On the other hand, because management was a traditional career for men, those men who were motivated to pursue such a career would represent a wider range of n Achievement scores than would the women who pursed business careers (whose n Achievement scores should have been clustered at the top of the range). It might be

7 3 Gender and Motivation for Achievement, Affiliation Intimacy, and Power 47 worthwhile to note that similar arguments can be made for women entering science and engineering professions today (i.e., research shows that most women who enter those professions have very high standardized math test scores, whereas men who enter those professions have a much wider range of standardized math test scores; Maple & Stage, 1991). In longitudinal research with women who graduated from college in 1967, Jenkins (1987) argued that in addition to entrepreneurial activity, which was seen as the classic achievement-compatible career in men, teaching could serve the same purpose for women, in that it involved moderate challenges, autonomy, and rapid performance feedback. She further argued that college teaching in particular could be achievement arousing. Consistent with her arguments, Jenkins (1987) found that senior year n Achievement scores were related to employment in teaching 14 years later. In terms of career values, high achievement women working in achievement-compatible positions (i.e., college teaching, supervisory business positions) valued future status mobility and competition with a standard of excellence. On the other hand, high achievement women working in positions incompatible with achievement goals (i.e., non-college teaching, non-supervisory positions) valued other, non-achievement aspects of their jobs such as working with people and wielding power. Women employed in achievement-compatible positions showed an increase in achievement motivation over 14 years compared with women employed in achievement-incompatible professions. Furthermore, Jenkins found that high achievement women working in achievement-incompatible positions were more likely than high achievement women working in achievement-compatible positions to value advancement less, to perceive fewer status mobility routes, to have interrupted careers, and to be more involved in homemaking and mothering than in work. She argued that the high achievement women working in achievement-incompatible professions pursued excellence through their families, hobbies, or volunteer work, rather than through ambitious career goals. Jenkins concluded that life structure interacts with n Achievement to affect life outcomes and life satisfaction in both women and men. One s life structure could act specifically to arouse or suppress achievement motivation or channel it in different directions. Motive to Avoid Success Measures of the motive to avoid success (fear of success) were developed in response to early researchers who thought it was difficult to arouse achievement motivation in women. The fear of success had its analogue in early conceptualizations of power motivation, which included both a hope for power and a fear of power (Veroff, 1992). Fear of success was assessed by asking women (and men) to write a story in response to the verbal cue, After first term finals, Anne (John) finds herself (himself) at the top of her (his) medical school class. Stories were coded for absence of instrumental activity, lack of responsibility for goal attainment, interpersonal engagement, and negative consequences (Fleming, 1982). Fear of success is a learned, latent, stable characteristic of the personality acquired early in life in conjunction with the learning of sex role standards and other learned motives; is more prevalent and much more easily aroused in women than in men; is not equally important for all women; is much more strongly aroused in competitive achievement situations reflecting intelligence and leadership ability than in noncompetitive settings; will function, once aroused, as a negative inhibitory tendency acting to reduce the expression of the positive tendency to achieve; and is presumed to interact with other motivational and personality variables as a complex function of motive strength, incentive value, and probability of success (Fleming & Horner, 1992, pp ). Horner s (1968) early research showed that the expectation of negative consequences of achievement was related to anxiety in women study participants (Fleming & Horner, 1992). Although the

8 48 L.E. Duncan and B.E. Peterson PSEs of men who participated in these studies also showed evidence of fear of success, the negative effects on performance were far greater for women. The early studies were criticized on methodological grounds, and Horner and her colleagues worked to develop a more reliable coding system for the motive (Fleming, 1982; Fleming & Horner, 1992). The original interpretation of this construct located fear of success as a dispositional characteristic created by gender socialization. Other researchers critiqued the dispositional argument and posited instead that women were simply reflecting in PSEs their knowledge of the very real negative consequences of non-traditional achievement for women (see Fleming, 1982; Fleming & Horner, 1992, for reviews). In general, it appears that motive researchers have largely abandoned the construct of fear of success as assessed via PSEs (in a search of PsycINFO conducted in May 2008, the most recent article found was published in 1998, although Schultheiss and colleagues continue to reference the construct in theoretical models; e.g., Schultheiss & Pang, 2007). However, researchers have not abandoned the construct altogether; rather, the research appears to have transformed in two ways. First, Metzler and Conroy (2004) have attempted to resurrect the construct with a questionnaire measure developed by Zuckerman and Allison (1976) to assess fear of success in sport. Note that this line of research rejects the gendered assessment of fear of success and instead focuses equally on the anxieties male and female athletes experience while seeking success. Second, a more closely related construct in social psychology, stereotype threat, has captured the imaginations of many researchers. Stereotype threat (Steele, 1997) was developed to explain the underperformance of African Americans (relative to European Americans) on standardized English tests and the underperformance of women (relative to men) on standardized math tests. Defined as the fear of confirming a negative stereotype about a group to which one belongs, stereotype threat can be elicited by simply making salient the group membership that is associated with a negative stereotype. Further, removing that stereotype as a possible explanation for poor performance has the effect of equalizing performance on these tests. Similar to the motive to avoid success, stereotype threat is related to performance decrements for members of subordinate groups. However, unlike fear of success, it is assumed to be a situational variable, not a personality characteristic, and thus fairly malleable. Relationship of Implicit Measures to Explicit Measures of n Achievement Contemporary researchers appear to utilize questionnaire, or self-attribution measures, of n Achievement more frequently than implicit PSE measures (see Senko, Durik, & Harackiewicz, 2008, for a current review of theory and measurement of explicit achievement motivation). As mentioned earlier, research has established that these two types of measures are usually unrelated and predict different phenomena. In two meta-analyses of the validity of implicit (PSE) versus self-attributed (questionnaire) measures of achievement motivation, Spangler (1992) found that PSE measures of achievement were positively correlated with career success measured in the presence of intrinsic, or task-related, achievement incentives (e.g., farm or industry output). On the other hand, extrinsic questionnaire measures of achievement were positively correlated with outcomes, particularly in the presence of external or social achievement incentives (e.g., school grades; see also Brunstein & Maier, 2005). Spangler also found that, on average, PSE correlations were larger than questionnaire correlations. More recent research has shown that implicit and explicit measures of achievement motivation may not be as unrelated as they have seemed from previous studies. Thrash et al. (2007) found that when implicit and explicit measures of n Achievement were matched on content, there was a

9 3 Gender and Motivation for Achievement, Affiliation Intimacy, and Power 49 small positive correlation (r = 0.17). In addition, they found that for people who scored high on private body consciousness (i.e., awareness of their own physiological arousal), low on self-monitoring (i.e., less concerned about how they appeared to others), and high on need for consistency, implicit and explicit achievement motivation were correlated. They concluded that implicit and explicit motives are more closely related than previously thought, but that the relationship between the two is often attenuated by methodological inconsistency and moderated by certain individual difference variables. In sum, research on gender and achievement motivation has reflected the social-evaluative and competitive aspects of the construct and indicates that people who score high on achievement motivation are interested in living up to their own internal standards and that these standards are influenced by prevailing social values about gender. If the construct were to be reinvented today, researchers would need not only to include women in derivation experiments but also to focus on aspects of excellence relevant to both women and men. That is, the original derivation experiments put men in traditional academically competitive situations and emphasized competition with others. Certainly women have been able to hold their own in academic settings; however, what might achievement motivation look like if the competitive with others aspect of the construct was deemphasized and setting a goal for oneself was emphasized? Early studies on fear of success make it clear that women s awareness of the negative consequences of surpassing others means that an achievement motivation defined mostly on competition with others is probably less likely to capture the full spectrum of achievement motivation, especially in groups stigmatized for achieving success. Affiliation Intimacy Motivation As with the need for achievement, the coding categories for the need for affiliation were developed with male participants in the initial derivation studies (Heyns et al., 1958). However, as noted by Stewart and Chester (1982), subsequent research showed that women s and men s need for affiliation could be aroused in the same way and that similar patterns of correlates across gender exist for the affiliation motive. Stewart and Chester (1982) reported that women sometimes scored higher on n Affiliation relative to men but that these findings were often confounded by the gender of protagonists in picture cues, and so direct comparisons were not possible. In early studies, women were more likely to be shown PSE picture cues featuring girls and women rather than boys and men. Due to social stereotypes about women s communal interests, the presence of female characters may have led any test taker, man or woman, to express more affiliation imagery. Koestner and McClelland (1992) provided a general summary of research on affiliation motivation. Classic studies indicate that people high in n Affiliation (compared to those low in affiliation) spent more time interacting with people, learned social networks quickly, expressed sympathy toward others (and accommodated them), and had an aversion to interpersonal conflict, although it is interesting that n Affiliation seemed to be negatively (albeit weakly) correlated with popularity among peers. The latter finding and others like it led researchers to argue that a fear of rejection often underlies the behaviors and actions of people high in affiliation (Boyatzis, 1973). That is, highly affiliative people (especially under stress) may try too hard to establish connections and avoid rejection, which can lead other people to move away from them. Critics of the affiliation motive note that the original derivation studies often involved arousal of the motive by placing people in situations where rejection or acceptance by peers was emphasized. Thus, the coding categories for affiliation motivation may have inadvertently picked up on a fear of rejection embodied by the arousal manipulation. In order to develop a measure of affiliation that tapped more purely the positive aspects of interpersonal exchange, McAdams derived a new PSE measure of intimacy motivation.

10 50 L.E. Duncan and B.E. Peterson McAdams (1992) summarized his validation efforts in the late 1970s and the early 1980s. In the end, his measure seemed to tap a preference for warm, close, and communicative interaction with other people, and it emphasized only the positive aspects of interpersonal communion. As reported by McAdams (1992), scores on affiliation and intimacy tended to correlate positively within samples in the range. Although early studies suggested that men and women were equally likely to express intimacy motivation in PSE stories (e.g., McAdams & Constantian, 1983), later research showed that women tended to score higher than men. In an article devoted to the topic of gender differences, McAdams, Lester, Brand, McNamara, and Lensky (1988) provided evidence from a sample of 1,500 undergraduate students that women scored significantly higher than men on most of the subcategories that define the intimacy coding system. The magnitudes of the effects for each of the coding subcategories were not always large but were reliable. Women seemed to be more likely to express intimacy themes in response to PSE picture cues. This gender difference could reflect women s greater socialization as girls to express intimacy or their training in the maintenance of close relationships. Regardless of any gender differences in absolute levels of n Intimacy, the correlates of intimacy motivation are generally the same for men and women. Thus, for example, McAdams and Constantian (1983) showed, through the use of experience sampling (using pagers), that individuals high on intimacy motivation were more likely to be caught thinking about or interacting with other people than were those low on intimacy. In a later study, McAdams, Hoffman, Mansfield, and Day (1996) found no gender differences in how people high in intimacy motivation reconstructed important autobiographical memories in communal terms. It seems reasonable to conclude that women probably have higher levels of implicit intimacy motivation but that not all studies show a gender difference. Furthermore, the patterns of correlates for n Intimacy are virtually the same for men and women. Because need for affiliation and need for intimacy tend to overlap, Winter (1991a) simplified and combined the two measures into a single scoring system that assesses affiliation intimacy motivation in running text. Researchers partial to either affiliation or intimacy have not objected strongly. Rather, many PSE researchers seem to recognize the value of such a merging. However, they also recognize the advantage of using the older system if one is interested in the more anxious aspects of affiliative need and of using the new intimacy system if one is interested solely in the positive aspects of interpersonal union. Contemporary research on gender and affiliation intimacy has focused on relationships and physiological correlates. Affiliation Intimacy and Relationships Although people high on affiliation intimacy are interested in establishing and maintaining close relationships with others, research has shown that, under conditions of threat, highly affiliative people act prickly or defensively (Boyatzis, 1973; Winter & Carlson, 1988). Mason and Blankenship (1987) assessed affiliation motivation, life stress, and activity inhibition (a measure of restraint) in undergraduate students who were married or in dating relationships. They found that highly affiliative women who were low in activity inhibition were more likely than any other group to inflict physical and psychological abuse on their male partners when experiencing high levels of life stress. There were no main effects or interactions with men s affiliation motivation, although the impact of men s power motivation (also measured in the study) on abuse is discussed later. Similar findings regarding affiliation intimacy motivation were found in research by Zurbriggen (2000), who studied motives and aggressive sexual behavior. Affiliation intimacy in men was uncorrelated with self-reports of sexual aggressiveness toward women. For women, however,

11 3 Gender and Motivation for Achievement, Affiliation Intimacy, and Power 51 affiliation intimacy was correlated positively with using sexual coercion and seduction in their relationships with men. The positive relationship between affiliation and coercion was most prominent for women who made cognitive associations between power and sex. That is, women centrally concerned with maintaining romantic relationships who associated dominance with intimate relationships tended to try to maintain those relationships by using manipulation and coercion. In interpreting these relationships results, keep in mind that the PSE measures used involved affiliation intimacy (or affiliation alone), which includes its defensive aspects. The McAdams (1992) measure of intimacy motivation has not, to date, been associated with problematic romantic behavior. (In fact, McAdams and Vaillant, 1982, showed in a longitudinal sample of male Harvard graduates that age 30 PSE intimacy predicted marital satisfaction in the mid-40s, r = 0.38.) All of the findings discussed about relationships need to be further qualified by the fact that only heterosexual relationships were examined. How affiliation intimacy motivation might direct the relationship strategies of lesbians and gay men, absent heterosexual power sex linkages, is a question for future research. Much work remains to be done to clarify for all people how affiliation intimacy motivation enhances romantic unions and how it might create fractures under some conditions. Physiological Correlates of Affiliation Intimacy As discussed in more detail later, recent work on the physiological correlates of affiliation intimacy has focused on the gonadal (sex) hormones such as progesterone. However, McClelland s (1987) early work on the biological substrates of affiliation intimacy examined other types of hormones and cellular activity. To give two brief examples, Jemmott et al. (1990) showed that low stress people high in affiliation had the greatest levels of natural killer cell activity (a major component of the body s immune system). In another study, McClelland, Patel, Stier, and Brown (1987) had people watch films saturated with affiliation images. After participants viewed these films, a positive correlation emerged between levels of dopamine and PSE affiliation motivation. Although both of these studies included both women and men as participants, neither article reported results by gender. It is unclear whether McClelland and his colleagues found no gender differences or whether they did not think participant gender would influence the relationships between affiliation motivation, natural killer cell activity, and the release of dopamine. More recent work on the relationships between affiliation intimacy and sex hormones, by contrast, has focused on gender because levels of these hormones differ in the bodies of men and women. For example, Schultheiss, Dargel, and Rohde (2003) examined how implicit motives were related to the presence of gonadal steroid hormones in women. They separated women into two groups 18 women who were using oral contraceptives and 18 with normal menstrual cycles. A group of 18 men was also examined and tested at what would be menstrual, midcycle, and premenstrual phases in normally cycling women. Two findings from this study are illustrative. One, women using oral contraceptives scored higher on affiliation intimacy than did people in the other two groups. Two, in normally cycling women, levels of progesterone and affiliation intimacy were positively correlated. With regard to the first finding, the authors suggested that gender differences in levels of affiliation intimacy that are found in some studies may be due to the numbers of women who were taking birth control pills in these samples. The authors argued that the high level of gestagens (synthetic progesterone) in oral contraceptives presumably induces increased levels of affiliation intimacy in women. However, it is also quite likely that women high in affiliation intimacy motivation are more likely than those low in affiliation intimacy to be involved in committed intimate relationships and using birth control pills as contraception. Also interesting in this study is that progesterone levels

12 52 L.E. Duncan and B.E. Peterson in men were negatively related to affiliation intimacy. The authors explained that progesterone is related to reduced sex drive in men and argued that affiliation intimacy reflects to some extent sexual motivation (p. 300) for both men and women, so low progesterone should be related to high affiliation intimacy in men. (See also Schultheiss, Wirth, and Stanton, 2004, and Wirth and Schultheiss, 2006, for additional studies of the relations between progesterone, testosterone, cortisol, and affiliation intimacy arousal.) The implications of Schultheiss work on the physiological correlates of affiliation intimacy are not yet clear. Schultheiss and colleagues are working at the level of sex differences and similarities in physiological arousal. Biological sex, however, is not always a good marker of psychological gender. Right now researchers working on the biology of motives seem rather agnostic in their approach to gender. They pay close attention to gender when dealing with sex hormones, but collapse across gender for other physiological variables (e.g., natural killer cell activity). In sum, researchers have found that women score higher than men in affiliation intimacy motivation in many studies. However, few gender differences have been found in the correlates of affiliation intimacy. Why gender differences have been found in absolute motive levels is unclear. Some possible explanations are that women are better socialized at intimacy expression than are men or that greater levels of circulating progesterone increase affiliation intimacy motivation. A third potential explanation could relate to the particular picture cues used to elicit imagery. A useful avenue of future research might test alternative cues meant to elicit affiliation imagery in men. For example, stories written in response to a picture of a father and a son interacting in an affiliative manner might elicit more affiliation imagery in men. (Other ideas include a picture of a group of men playing a friendly game or a man proposing marriage to a woman.) Another explanation for these consistent gender differences is related to women s lower social status. That is, people in low power positions need, for their own safety and survival, to be attentive to the needs of others and to avoid interpersonal conflict. This is an intriguing possibility, one that could be tested experimentally by manipulating status and subsequently measuring motives. This would be most interesting, of course, if it were tested in both people of color and White women and men. Similar strategies were used successfully to show that status, rather than gender, was implicated in the differential use of influence strategies (Sagrestano, 1992) and indirect aggression (Duncan & Owen-Smith, 2006). Research of this sort may also shed light on why women (but not men) high in affiliation intimacy are more likely to behave in problematic ways when romantic relationships are under stress. Power Motivation Fairly early on, researchers devoted theoretical attention to the question of gender similarities and differences in the expression of power motivation. For this reason, psychologists now have a fairly sophisticated understanding of how need for power operates in the lives of women and men. Two overlapping but distinct types of power motivation were part of the original conceptualization of the construct: fear of weakness and hope for power (Veroff, 1992; Winter, 1992). Over the years, research on power motivation has emphasized the latter, or people s desires to attain and wield personal power. In fact, the scoring definitions for Winter s (1991a) running text system focus on power as impact over others rather than concerns with personal autonomy or weakness in the face of another person s power. A recent review of key findings in the power motive literature can be found in Winter (in press). The classic correlates indicate that power motivation at the individual level is related to (among other things) gaining formal social influence (e.g., through elected offices), ownership of prestigious

13 3 Gender and Motivation for Achievement, Affiliation Intimacy, and Power 53 possessions (as defined by one s peer group), taking risks to get noticed, a preference for jobs where one controls the behavior of other people, a somewhat negative self-image, aggressiveness, and impulsivity. With regard to gender, Stewart and Chester (1982) noted that power motivation is aroused in women and men in the same way (e.g., inspirational speeches, watching a hypnotist control another person) and that women and men do not differ on levels of power motivation expressed in the PSE. Based upon our review of research since 1982, the observations of Stewart and Chester seem to hold true as much now as they did then. Furthermore, the behavioral correlates of power motivation also continue to show similarities across women and men. For example, Fodor and Carver (2000) found that negative feedback from a person in power compromised the creativity of power-motivated women and men. Schultheiss and Brunstein (2002) found that when highly power-motivated women and men (who were also high in activity inhibition) were placed in a power-arousing situation, they were more verbally fluent and used heightened gesturing and eyebrow lifts to make their arguments more convincing to others. In both of these studies the authors reported no significant gender differences. However, consistent gender differences are found in the relationship of power motivation to profligate or impulsive behavior, though these relationships are moderated by individual differences in levels of responsibility. Profligacy and Responsibility Training As discussed by Winter and Barenbaum (1985), women and men high in power motivation are adept at gaining formal social power and influence. Alongside this socialized form of power, however, evidence also shows a relationship, especially in men, between high power motivation and excessive alcohol use, drug use, gambling, physical and verbal aggression, and the exploitation and repression of women. Given this pattern of findings, it is not surprising that heterosexual men high in power experience significant trouble in their intimate relationships with women (Stewart & Rubin, 1976). Indeed, research has shown that power motivation in men is related to sexual aggressiveness (Yost & Zurbriggen, 2006) and the infliction of physical abuse on women (Mason & Blankenship, 1987). Evidence suggests that these correlates are enhanced in men who have formed cognitive associations between power and sex (Zurbriggen, 2000). Heterosexual women high in power do not have this record of negative relationships with men. Winter and Barenbaum (1985) found that individual differences in personal responsibility moderated the negative effects of power motivation in both women and men. That is, women and men who were high in responsibility exercised power in prosocial ways, whereas women and men low in responsibility were more likely to engage in profligate behaviors. Winter (1988) also showed that formal responsibility training at home during childhood (e.g., taking care of younger siblings, frequency of household chores) also channeled power motivation away from profligacy for both women and men. Alternatively, the absence of responsibility training led high-powered adults toward profligate expressions of the motive. Winter (1988) argued that high-powered women in general do not behave in profligate ways because girls in contemporary society are given more responsibility training while growing up than are boys: Thus, the differences between responsible and profligate expressions of the power motive may have to do with variables that reflect socialization rather than sex as such (Winter, 1988, p. 518). Furthermore, because power is intimately tied to privilege, it may be that individual differences in motive expression reflect individual differences in levels of entitlement in both women and men. Future researchers could examine how social status affects individual differences in the desire to have an impact on others or on the world at large. Much like Jenkins (1987)

The Thematic Apperception Test: Toward a Standard Measure of the Big Three Motives

The Thematic Apperception Test: Toward a Standard Measure of the Big Three Motives MEASURING LANGAN-FOX THE BIG AND THREE GRANT MOTIVES JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT, 87(3), 277 291 Copyright 2006, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Thematic Apperception Test: Toward a Standard

More information

Traits and Personality. Chapter 10

Traits and Personality. Chapter 10 Traits and Personality Chapter 10 Gordon Allport 1897-1967 1967 Grew up near Cleveland Spent much of his childhood alone College at Harvard A visit to Freud led him to find a new approach Young boy with

More information

THEORIES OF PERSONALITY II Psychodynamic Assessment 1/1/2014 SESSION 6 PSYCHODYNAMIC ASSESSMENT

THEORIES OF PERSONALITY II Psychodynamic Assessment 1/1/2014 SESSION 6 PSYCHODYNAMIC ASSESSMENT THEORIES OF PERSONALITY II Psychodynamic Assessment 1/1/2014 SESSION 6 PSYCHODYNAMIC ASSESSMENT THEORIES OF PERSONALITY II SESSION 6: Psychodynamic Assessment Psychodynamic Assessment Assessing the specific

More information

Lecture 9. Control and Personality. Professor Ian Robertson

Lecture 9. Control and Personality. Professor Ian Robertson Lecture 9 Control and Personality Professor Ian Robertson SEE KELTNER ET AL PSYCH REVIEW (FROM LECTURE 8) Why does Power increase behavioural activation? Power is correlated with increased resources. Powerful

More information

Implicit motives and hemispheric processing differences are critical for understanding. personality disorders: A Commentary on Hopwood

Implicit motives and hemispheric processing differences are critical for understanding. personality disorders: A Commentary on Hopwood Schultheiss commentary 1 Running head: Schultheiss commentary Implicit motives and hemispheric processing differences are critical for understanding personality disorders: A Commentary on Hopwood Oliver

More information

CHAPTER 6 BASIS MOTIVATION CONCEPTS

CHAPTER 6 BASIS MOTIVATION CONCEPTS CHAPTER 6 BASIS MOTIVATION CONCEPTS WHAT IS MOTIVATION? "Maybe the place to begin is to say what motivation isn't. Many people incorrectly view motivation as a personal trait that is, some have it and

More information

Assessing implicit motives with an online version of the picture story exercise

Assessing implicit motives with an online version of the picture story exercise Motiv Emot (2011) 35:251 266 DOI 10.1007/s11031-010-9175-8 ORIGINAL PAPER Assessing implicit motives with an online version of the picture story exercise Katharina Bernecker Veronika Job Published online:

More information

The Thematic Apperception Test. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.

The Thematic Apperception Test. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. + The Thematic Apperception Test This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. + INTRODUCTION: The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) is a projective personality

More information

c. Mageerrie A. Langner b

c. Mageerrie A. Langner b How personalized and socialized power motivation facilitate antisocial and prosocial decision-making Joe a b c. Mageerrie A. Langner b Robert F. Wagner Graduate School of Public Service. New York University,

More information

Personality. Chapter 13

Personality. Chapter 13 + Personality Chapter 13 + Personality An individual s characteristic pattern of thinking, feeling, and acting. Each Dwarf has a distinct personality. + Psychodynamic Perspective Freud s clinical experience

More information

Motivation CHAPTER FIFTEEN INTRODUCTION DETAILED LECTURE OUTLINE

Motivation CHAPTER FIFTEEN INTRODUCTION DETAILED LECTURE OUTLINE CHAPTER FIFTEEN Motivation INTRODUCTION Many of us have unrealized abilities. Some of us could run marathons, others could write novels, and still others could get straight A s in management classes. But

More information

C O N T E N T S ... v vi. Job Tasks 38 Job Satisfaction 39. Group Development 6. Leisure Activities 41. Values 44. Instructions 9.

C O N T E N T S ... v vi. Job Tasks 38 Job Satisfaction 39. Group Development 6. Leisure Activities 41. Values 44. Instructions 9. C O N T E N T S LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES v vi INTRODUCTION TO THE FIRO-B INSTRUMENT 1 Overview of Uses 1 THEORY OF INTERPERSONAL NEEDS 3 The Interpersonal Needs 3 Expressed and Wanted Needs 4 The

More information

STUDENT GUIDE NAVEDTRA A CHAPTER 9 BASIC NEEDS AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR

STUDENT GUIDE NAVEDTRA A CHAPTER 9 BASIC NEEDS AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR CHAPTER 9 BASIC NEEDS AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR 1. Introduction The purpose of this chapter is to familiarize you with basic theories on motivation and how they relate to performance. We begin by describing the

More information

Chapter 2: Purpose in Life

Chapter 2: Purpose in Life Chapter 2: Purpose in Life Multiple Choice 1. Yalom identified four topics for existential psychology. They did not include: a. purpose in life. b. choice and responsibility. c. change and impermanence.

More information

Chapter 9-Sexuality-Psy222

Chapter 9-Sexuality-Psy222 Chapter 9-Sexuality-Psy222 Chapter 9-Sexuality There are few components of relationships that have seen as much evolution and change as that of sexuality Our attitudes about sex have become increasingly

More information

SMITH COLLEGE Curriculum Vitae (Abbreviated) July 11, 2016

SMITH COLLEGE Curriculum Vitae (Abbreviated) July 11, 2016 Lauren E. Duncan 1 SMITH COLLEGE Curriculum Vitae (Abbreviated) July 11, 2016 1. Name 2. Addresses 3. Degrees Lauren E. Duncan Campus: Department of Psychology Northampton, MA 01063 (413) 585-3763 Ph.D.

More information

What is Personality?

What is Personality? What is Personality? Free will or determinism? Masters of our own fate or biological, unconscious, external factors Nature or nurture? Heredity or environment Past, present, or future? Is it determined

More information

gender and violence 2 The incidence of violence varies dramatically by place and over time.

gender and violence 2 The incidence of violence varies dramatically by place and over time. gender and violence Everyone has the right to life, liberty, and the security of person (Article 3, UDHR, 1948; Articles 6.1 and 9.1, ICCPR, 1966). No one shall be subjected to... cruel, inhuman or degrading

More information

Attitude Measurement

Attitude Measurement Business Research Methods 9e Zikmund Babin Carr Griffin Attitude Measurement 14 Chapter 14 Attitude Measurement 2013 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be scanned, copied or duplicated, or

More information

Fear of Success in Males and Females Effects of Developmental Level and Sex-Linked Course of Study ~

Fear of Success in Males and Females Effects of Developmental Level and Sex-Linked Course of Study ~ Sex Roles, Vol. 2, No. 3, 1976 Fear of Success in Males and Females Effects of Developmental Level and Sex-Linked Course of Study ~ Betsy Kimball and Robert L. Leahy 2 The Catholic University of America

More information

Lesson 12. Understanding and Managing Individual Behavior

Lesson 12. Understanding and Managing Individual Behavior Lesson 12 Understanding and Managing Individual Behavior Learning Objectives 1. Identify the focus and goals of individual behavior within organizations. 2. Explain the role that attitudes play in job

More information

ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY MA 204-1

ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY MA 204-1 GENERAL OBJECTIVES OF THE SUBJECT At the end of the course, students will examine the principles of organizational psychology; apply them within companies; critically reflect emotional behavior within

More information

Character Education Framework

Character Education Framework Character Education Framework March, 2018 Character Education: Building Positive Ethical Strength Character education is the direct attempt to foster character virtues the principles that inform decisionmaking

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological needs & implicit motives Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2016 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 3 Psychological

More information

12 The biology of love

12 The biology of love The biology of love Motivation Neurological origins of passionate love begin in infancy when infants attach to mother. Certain neurotransmitters and hormones involved in pleasure and reward are activated

More information

CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK

CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK 2.0.0 INTRODUCTION The details about introduction, rationale of the present study, statement of the problem objectives of the study, hypotheses of the study, delimitation

More information

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR)

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR) Discussion Questions The concept of IR INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR) 1. Define interpersonal relationship. 2. List types of interpersonal relationship. 3. What are the advantages and disadvantages of

More information

My Notebook. A space for your private thoughts.

My Notebook. A space for your private thoughts. My Notebook A space for your private thoughts. 2 Ground rules: 1. Listen respectfully. 2. Speak your truth. And honor other people s truth. 3. If your conversations get off track, pause and restart. Say

More information

1/23/2012 PERSONALITY. Personality. THE JOURNEY OF ADULTHOOD Barbara R. Bjorklund

1/23/2012 PERSONALITY. Personality. THE JOURNEY OF ADULTHOOD Barbara R. Bjorklund THE JOURNEY OF ADULTHOOD Barbara R. Bjorklund Chapter 8 PERSONALITY Personality Personality: relatively enduring set of characteristics that define our individuality and affect our interactions with the

More information

Reflect on the Types of Organizational Structures. Hierarch of Needs Abraham Maslow (1970) Hierarchy of Needs

Reflect on the Types of Organizational Structures. Hierarch of Needs Abraham Maslow (1970) Hierarchy of Needs Reflect on the Types of Organizational Structures 1 Hierarch of Needs Abraham Maslow (1970) Self- Actualization or Self- Fulfillment Esteem Belonging, Love, and Social Activities Safety and Security Psychological

More information

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design 11-1 Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design Advantages and Limitations Comparing Two Groups Comparing t Test to ANOVA Independent Samples t Test Independent Samples ANOVA Comparing

More information

Cannabis Changes. Understanding Dynamics of Use and Dependence N. Liebregts

Cannabis Changes. Understanding Dynamics of Use and Dependence N. Liebregts Cannabis Changes. Understanding Dynamics of Use and Dependence N. Liebregts Cannabis changes. Understanding dynamics of use and dependence. Summary Nienke Liebregts Summary 201 Cannabis is the most widely

More information

Multiple Act criterion:

Multiple Act criterion: Common Features of Trait Theories Generality and Stability of Traits: Trait theorists all use consistencies in an individual s behavior and explain why persons respond in different ways to the same stimulus

More information

Cambridge Public Schools SEL Benchmarks K-12

Cambridge Public Schools SEL Benchmarks K-12 Cambridge Public Schools SEL Benchmarks K-12 OVERVIEW SEL Competencies Goal I: Develop selfawareness Goal II: Develop and Goal III: Develop social Goal IV: Demonstrate Goal V: Demonstrate skills to demonstrate

More information

MIDTERM EXAMINATION Spring 2009 MGT502- Organizational Behaviour (Session - 2) Question No: 1 ( Marks: 1 ) - Please choose one Which one of the following is NOT a characteristic of group in organization?

More information

Breaking the Bias Habit. Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute

Breaking the Bias Habit. Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute Breaking the Bias Habit Jennifer Sheridan, Ph.D. Executive & Research Director Women in Science & Engineering Leadership Institute P e r c e n t W o m e n Percent Women Bachelor's Degrees, Selected Fields

More information

Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs

Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2014 Image source 1 Reeve (2009, pp. 142-143) Psychological need An inherent source

More information

Meaning in Work and Life 6th Edition by Denis Waitley

Meaning in Work and Life 6th Edition by Denis Waitley Link download full: Test Bank for Psychology of Success Finding Meaning in Work and Life 6th Edition by Denis Waitley https://digitalcontentmarket.org/download/test-bank-for-psychology-ofsuccess-finding-meaning-in-work-and-life-6th-edition-by-denis-waitley

More information

draft Big Five 03/13/ HFM

draft Big Five 03/13/ HFM participant client HFM 03/13/201 This report was generated by the HFMtalentindex Online Assessment system. The data in this report are based on the answers given by the participant on one or more psychological

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2014 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 when people find

More information

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT

PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT Van Der Velde / Guide to Business Research Methods First Proof 6.11.2003 4:53pm page 1 Part I PLANNING THE RESEARCH PROJECT Van Der Velde / Guide to Business Research Methods First Proof 6.11.2003 4:53pm

More information

MARK SCHEME for the May/June 2011 question paper for the guidance of teachers 9699 SOCIOLOGY

MARK SCHEME for the May/June 2011 question paper for the guidance of teachers 9699 SOCIOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE INTERNATIONAL EXAMINATIONS GCE Advanced Subsidiary Level and GCE Advanced Level MARK SCHEME for the May/June 2011 question paper for the guidance of teachers 9699 SOCIOLOGY 9699/22

More information

Samantha Wright. September 03, 2003

Samantha Wright. September 03, 2003 BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory By Reuven Bar-On, Ph.D. Resource Report Name: ID: Admin. Date: Samantha Wright September 03, 2003 Copyright 2002 Multi-Health Systems Inc. All rights reserved. P.O. Box

More information

Emotional Quotient. Andrew Doe. Test Job Acme Acme Test Slogan Acme Company N. Pacesetter Way

Emotional Quotient. Andrew Doe. Test Job Acme Acme Test Slogan Acme Company N. Pacesetter Way Emotional Quotient Test Job Acme 2-16-2018 Acme Test Slogan test@reportengine.com Introduction The Emotional Quotient report looks at a person's emotional intelligence, which is the ability to sense, understand

More information

Motivation, Conflict, Emotion. Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry

Motivation, Conflict, Emotion. Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry Motivation, Conflict, Emotion Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry Motivation Motivation is the psychological feature that arouses an organism to action toward a desired goal and

More information

CHAPTER 7: Achievement motivation, attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence. Practice questions - text book pages

CHAPTER 7: Achievement motivation, attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence. Practice questions - text book pages QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS CHAPTER 7: Achievement motivation, attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence Practice questions - text book pages 111-112 1) Which one of the following best explains achievement

More information

BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory. Resource Report. John Morris. Name: ID: Admin. Date: December 15, 2010 (Online) 17 Minutes 22 Seconds

BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory. Resource Report. John Morris. Name: ID: Admin. Date: December 15, 2010 (Online) 17 Minutes 22 Seconds BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory By Reuven Bar-On, Ph.D. Resource Report Name: ID: Admin. Date: Duration: John Morris December 15, 2010 (Online) 17 Minutes 22 Seconds Copyright 2002 Multi-Health Systems

More information

Chapter Introduction Section 1: Theories of Motivation Section 2: Biological and Social Motives Section 3: Emotions. Chapter Menu

Chapter Introduction Section 1: Theories of Motivation Section 2: Biological and Social Motives Section 3: Emotions. Chapter Menu Chapter Introduction Section 1: Theories of Motivation Section 2: Biological and Social Motives Section 3: Emotions Chapter Menu Chapter Objectives Section 1 Theories of Motivation Explain motivation and

More information

c) Redraw the model and place on it relevant attributions for each of the four boxes.

c) Redraw the model and place on it relevant attributions for each of the four boxes. CHAPTER 6: Attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence, and leadership Practice questions - text book pages 107-108 1) a) Figure 6.21 partly illustrates Weiner s model of attribution. Explain the

More information

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1060 1066 Brief Report Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies

More information

Identity Development. Kate C. McLean Western Washington University. Moin Syed University of Minnesota

Identity Development. Kate C. McLean Western Washington University. Moin Syed University of Minnesota Identity Development 1 Identity Development Kate C. McLean Western Washington University Moin Syed University of Minnesota To appear in: M.H. Bornstein (Ed.), The SAGE Encyclopedia of Lifespan Human Development

More information

BEING A LEADER and LEADERSHIP

BEING A LEADER and LEADERSHIP LEADERSHIP : BEING A LEADER and LEADERSHIP Qemeru Dessalegn Learning good leadership skills and becoming a successful leader are not easy tasks. With this basic introduction of leadership, we can begin

More information

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16)

Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16) MSP Feedback Guide 2009 Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16) Feedback to Test Takers Introduction (page 2 of the report) The Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (EIQ16) measures aspects of your

More information

Creating a Positive Professional Image

Creating a Positive Professional Image Creating a Positive Professional Image Q&A with: Laura Morgan Roberts Published: June 20, 2005 Author: Mallory Stark As HBS professor Laura Morgan Roberts sees it, if you aren't managing your own professional

More information

Chapter 1 Applications and Consequences of Psychological Testing

Chapter 1 Applications and Consequences of Psychological Testing Chapter 1 Applications and Consequences of Psychological Testing Topic 1A The Nature and Uses of Psychological Testing The Consequences of Testing From birth to old age, people encounter tests at all most

More information

PSYCHOLOGY (413) Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D.

PSYCHOLOGY (413) Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D. PSYCHOLOGY (413) 662-5453 Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D. Email: S.Claffey@mcla.edu PROGRAMS AVAILABLE BACHELOR OF ARTS IN PSYCHOLOGY BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS MINOR PSYCHOLOGY MINOR TEACHER LICENSURE PSYCHOLOGY

More information

Focus of Today s Presentation. Partners in Healing Model. Partners in Healing: Background. Data Collection Tools. Research Design

Focus of Today s Presentation. Partners in Healing Model. Partners in Healing: Background. Data Collection Tools. Research Design Exploring the Impact of Delivering Mental Health Services in NYC After-School Programs Gerald Landsberg, DSW, MPA Stephanie-Smith Waterman, MSW, MS Ana Maria Pinter, M.A. Focus of Today s Presentation

More information

The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign

The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign Reed Larson 2 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign February 28,

More information

Insight Hogan Personality Inventory (HPI)

Insight Hogan Personality Inventory (HPI) Insight Hogan Personality Inventory (HPI) Report For: Sam Poole ID: HE764696 Date: 8.08.2018 2018 Hogan Assessment Systems Inc. Introduction The Hogan Personality Inventory is a measure of normal personality

More information

Discovering Diversity Profile Individual Report

Discovering Diversity Profile Individual Report Individual Report Respondent Name Monday, November 13, 2006 This report is provided by: Integro Leadership Institute 1380 Wilmington Pike, Suite 113B West Chester PA 19382 Toll Free (866) 468-3476 Phone

More information

Japanese Statistics and Happiness Measurement

Japanese Statistics and Happiness Measurement Japanese Statistics and Happiness Measurement Yoshizoe, Yasuto President, Statistics Council, Japan June 27, 2007 Second OECD World Forum on Statistics, Knowledge and Policy Istanbul 1 Summary Japanese

More information

TAT Responses in Relation to Induced Motivational Set and Reasoning Ability

TAT Responses in Relation to Induced Motivational Set and Reasoning Ability Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology July 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 235-240. TAT Responses in Relation to Induced Motivational Set and Reasoning Ability N. Annalakshmi Bharathiar University,

More information

TAT INTERPERSONAL DECENTERING AND SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING. James Nixon, B.S. Sharon Rae Jenkins, Ph. D. Brenton LaBrie, B.A.

TAT INTERPERSONAL DECENTERING AND SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING. James Nixon, B.S. Sharon Rae Jenkins, Ph. D. Brenton LaBrie, B.A. TAT INTERPERSONAL DECENTERING AND SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING James Nixon, B.S. Sharon Rae Jenkins, Ph. D. Brenton LaBrie, B.A. Abstract This study examined social understanding, defined as better attention to

More information

Motivation & Emotion. Outline Intrinsic & extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic-extrinsic motivations & goal-setting. Intrinsic motivation

Motivation & Emotion. Outline Intrinsic & extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic-extrinsic motivations & goal-setting. Intrinsic motivation Motivation & Emotion Intrinsic-extrinsic motivations & goal-setting Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2014 Image source 1 Outline Intrinsic & extrinsic motivation Intrinsic

More information

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: SOCIAL AFFILIATION CUES PRIME HELP-SEEKING INTENTIONS 1 This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows: Rubin, M. (2011). Social

More information

The Power to Change Your Life: Ten Keys to Resilient Living Robert Brooks, Ph.D.

The Power to Change Your Life: Ten Keys to Resilient Living Robert Brooks, Ph.D. The Power to Change Your Life: Ten Keys to Resilient Living Robert Brooks, Ph.D. The latest book I co-authored with my colleague Dr. Sam Goldstein was recently released. In contrast to our previous works

More information

MOTIVATION MODELS 6-9. OliveraNovitović, PhD 1, ZoranaVidić, MSc 2, BojanaŠaljić, MSc 3 1. INTRODUCTION

MOTIVATION MODELS 6-9. OliveraNovitović, PhD 1, ZoranaVidić, MSc 2, BojanaŠaljić, MSc 3 1. INTRODUCTION MOTIVATION MODELS OliveraNovitović, PhD 1, ZoranaVidić, MSc 2, BojanaŠaljić, MSc 3 1 Užice, SERBIA, nolivera@mts.rs 2 Užice, SERBIA, zvidic87@live.com 3 Belgrade business School, Belgrade, SERBIA Abstract:

More information

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE TEST-R

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE TEST-R We thank you for taking the test and for your support and participation. Your report is presented in multiple sections as given below: Menu Indicators Indicators specific to the test Personalized analysis

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 4, Number 1 Submitted: August 20, 2008 Revisions: October 16, 2008 Accepted: October 17, 2008 Publication Date: October 20, 2008 Start Today or the Very

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological & social needs Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2013 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6 3 Psychological

More information

CHAPTER 2. MEASURING AND DESCRIBING VARIABLES

CHAPTER 2. MEASURING AND DESCRIBING VARIABLES 4 Chapter 2 CHAPTER 2. MEASURING AND DESCRIBING VARIABLES 1. A. Age: name/interval; military dictatorship: value/nominal; strongly oppose: value/ ordinal; election year: name/interval; 62 percent: value/interval;

More information

Psychology 481. A.A. Degree: Psychology. Faculty & Offices. Degrees Awarded

Psychology 481. A.A. Degree: Psychology. Faculty & Offices. Degrees Awarded Psychology 481 Psychology Psychology is the social science discipline most concerned with studying the behavior, mental processes, growth and well-being of individuals. Psychological inquiry also examines

More information

Instrumental activity in achievement motivation1. Department of Child Study, Faculty of Home Economics, Japan Women's University, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo 112

Instrumental activity in achievement motivation1. Department of Child Study, Faculty of Home Economics, Japan Women's University, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo 112 Japanese Psychological Research 1981, Vol.23, No.2, 79-87 Instrumental activity in achievement motivation1 MISAKO MIYAMOTO2 Department of Child Study, Faculty of Home Economics, Japan Women's University,

More information

GCSE EXAMINERS' REPORTS

GCSE EXAMINERS' REPORTS GCSE EXAMINERS' REPORTS SOCIOLOGY SUMMER 2016 Grade boundary information for this subject is available on the WJEC public website at: https://www.wjecservices.co.uk/marktoums/default.aspx?l=en Online Results

More information

Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members

Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members Margaret E. Brooks, Eyal Grauer, Erin E. Thornbury, and Scott Highhouse Bowling Green State University The scientist-practitioner

More information

NORTH LONDON SCHOOL OF ENGLISH. Title: Personality. Read the text and fill in the gaps with the words from the box.

NORTH LONDON SCHOOL OF ENGLISH. Title: Personality. Read the text and fill in the gaps with the words from the box. Title: Personality. Read the text and fill in the gaps with the words from the box. Pessimistic Sympathetic Impulsive Calm Ambitious Aggressive Toughminded Goaloriented Imaginative Patient Analytical Stubborn

More information

CHAPTER 8 SPORT AND EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY. Robin S. Vealey

CHAPTER 8 SPORT AND EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY. Robin S. Vealey CHAPTER 8 SPORT AND EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY Robin S. Vealey Chapter Objectives Discuss what scholars and professionals do in sport and exercise psychology. Describe how sport and exercise psychology evolved

More information

The four chapters in Part I set the stage. Chapter 1 moves from the implicit common sense theories of everyday life to explicit theories that are

The four chapters in Part I set the stage. Chapter 1 moves from the implicit common sense theories of everyday life to explicit theories that are Preface This volume is designed as a basic text for upper level and graduate courses in contemporary sociological theory. Most sociology programs require their majors to take at least one course in sociological

More information

Motivation & Conflict. Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry

Motivation & Conflict. Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry Motivation & Conflict Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry Motivation Motivation is the psychological feature that arouses an organism to action toward a desired goal and elicits,

More information

Behavioral EQ MULTI-RATER PROFILE. Prepared for: By: Session: 22 Jul Madeline Bertrand. Sample Organization

Behavioral EQ MULTI-RATER PROFILE. Prepared for: By: Session: 22 Jul Madeline Bertrand. Sample Organization Behavioral EQ MULTI-RATER PROFILE Prepared for: Madeline Bertrand By: Sample Organization Session: Improving Interpersonal Effectiveness 22 Jul 2014 Behavioral EQ, Putting Emotional Intelligence to Work,

More information

Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary. NLPcourses.com

Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary. NLPcourses.com Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE Summary NLPcourses.com Table of Contents Using Your Brain -- for a CHANGE by Richard Bandler Summary... 6 Chapter 1 Who s Driving the Bus?... 6 Chapter 2 Running Your Own

More information

The Art of Empowerment. Lynn E. Lawrence, CMSgt(ret), USAF CPOT, ABOC, COA, OSC Consultant

The Art of Empowerment. Lynn E. Lawrence, CMSgt(ret), USAF CPOT, ABOC, COA, OSC Consultant The Art of Empowerment Lynn E. Lawrence, CMSgt(ret), USAF CPOT, ABOC, COA, OSC Consultant Quote If your actions inspire others to dream more, learn more, do more and become more, you are a leader. John

More information

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Why do Psychologists Perform Research? PSY 102 1 PSY 102 Understanding and Thinking Critically About Psychological Research Thinking critically about research means knowing the right questions to ask to assess the validity or accuracy of a

More information

International School of Turin

International School of Turin International School of Turin Adapted from the IB PSE scope and sequence Personal, Social and Physical Education Scope and Sequence Identity An understanding of our own beliefs, values, attitudes, experiences

More information

Horticultural and hunting-gathering societies had much less gender discrimination than is present in contemporary societies.

Horticultural and hunting-gathering societies had much less gender discrimination than is present in contemporary societies. Sex and Gender 1. Gender stratification: men and women's unequal access to power, prestige, and property. 2. Sex: refers to biological characteristics 3. Gender is a social characteristic that varies from

More information

Empirical testing of evolutionary hypotheses has used to test many theories both directly and indirectly. Why do empirical testing?

Empirical testing of evolutionary hypotheses has used to test many theories both directly and indirectly. Why do empirical testing? Empirical testing of evolutionary hypotheses has used to test many theories both directly and indirectly. Why do empirical testing? In fact, the general theory of evolution has been accepted as more or

More information

Motivation and Emotion deals with the drives and incentives behind everyday thoughts and actions.

Motivation and Emotion deals with the drives and incentives behind everyday thoughts and actions. Motivation and Emotion deals with the drives and incentives behind everyday thoughts and actions. Motivation- A need or desire that energizes and directs behavior primarily based on our instincts that

More information

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE Personal Report JOHN SMITH 2017 MySkillsProfile. All rights reserved. Introduction The EIQ16 measures aspects of your emotional intelligence by asking you questions

More information

Chapter 1. Lesson 2. Leadership Reshuffled. What You Will Learn to Do. Linked Core Abilities. Skills and Knowledge You Will Gain Along the Way

Chapter 1. Lesson 2. Leadership Reshuffled. What You Will Learn to Do. Linked Core Abilities. Skills and Knowledge You Will Gain Along the Way Lesson 2 Leadership Reshuffled Key Terms approach What You Will Learn to Do Compare leadership styles Linked Core Abilities Take responsibility for your actions and choices Treat self and others with respect

More information

What is Social Cognition?

What is Social Cognition? Social Cognition What is Social Cognition? Social Psychology scientific study of how people s thoughts, feelings, and actions are influenced by social environment Cognitive Psychology scientific study

More information

THE INFLUENCE OF UNCONSCIOUS NEEDS ON COLLEGE PROGRAM CHOICE

THE INFLUENCE OF UNCONSCIOUS NEEDS ON COLLEGE PROGRAM CHOICE University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Tourism Travel and Research Association: Advancing Tourism Research Globally 2007 ttra International Conference THE INFLUENCE OF UNCONSCIOUS

More information

UNDERSTANDING YOUR COUPLE CHECKUP RESULTS

UNDERSTANDING YOUR COUPLE CHECKUP RESULTS UNDERSTANDING YOUR COUPLE CHECKUP RESULTS The steps you will take to grow both as individuals and as a couple OVER 35 YEARS OF RESEARCH MEANS YOU ARE RECEIVING VALID RESULTS THAT WILL MAKE YOU SCREAM.

More information

Kantor Behavioral Profiles

Kantor Behavioral Profiles Kantor Behavioral Profiles baseline name: date: Kantor Behavioral Profiles baseline INTRODUCTION Individual Behavioral Profile In our earliest social system the family individuals explore a range of behavioral

More information

Volunteerism for the Next Generation

Volunteerism for the Next Generation Motivation We really can t motivate others; we can only provide situations which help others motivate themselves. These keys to effective motivation in volunteer groups, from How to Motivate People in

More information

Personal Talent Skills Inventory

Personal Talent Skills Inventory Personal Talent Skills Inventory Sales Version Inside Sales Sample Co. 5-30-2013 Introduction Research suggests that the most effective people are those who understand themselves, both their strengths

More information

Achievement incentives determine the effects of achievement-motive incongruence on flow experience

Achievement incentives determine the effects of achievement-motive incongruence on flow experience Erschienen in: Motivation and Emotion ; 34 (2010), 1. - S. 2-14 https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-009-9150-4 Achievement incentives determine the effects of achievement-motive on flow experience Julia

More information

Chapter 1 WHAT IS PSYCHOLOGY

Chapter 1 WHAT IS PSYCHOLOGY Chapter 1 WHAT IS PSYCHOLOGY Section 1: Why Study Psychology? Section 2: What Psychologists Do Section 3: A History of Psychology Section 4: Contemporary Perspectives 1 We are social animals, therefore,

More information

Effect of sports participation on locus of control and personality traits and self -efficacy among interuniversity sportsperson

Effect of sports participation on locus of control and personality traits and self -efficacy among interuniversity sportsperson 2017; 2(2): 348-352 ISSN: 2456-4419 Impact Factor: (RJIF): 5.18 Yoga 2017; 2(2): 348-352 2017 Yoga www.theyogicjournal.com Received: 12-05-2017 Accepted: 13-06-2017 Kum Paramma B Kuravatti Research scholar,

More information

CHAPTER OUTLINE CHAPTER SUMMARY

CHAPTER OUTLINE CHAPTER SUMMARY CHAPTER 14 PERSONALITY IN PERSPECTIVE: OVERLAP AND INTEGRATION CHAPTER OUTLINE Similarities Among Perspectives Psychoanalysis and Evolutionary Psychology: The Structural Model Psychoanalysis and Evolutionary

More information

MOTIVATION AND BEHAVIOUR MOTIVATION

MOTIVATION AND BEHAVIOUR MOTIVATION MOTIVATION AND BEHAVIOUR MOTIVATION Motivation is the will to do, and the will to do well. Motivation may be self-directed, or one may be motivated by others and by the environment, and one can motivate

More information