FALSE MEMORY INCREASED BY SELF-GENERATION IN COMPARISONS OF RECOGNITION AND RECALL. Andrew M. McCullough

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "FALSE MEMORY INCREASED BY SELF-GENERATION IN COMPARISONS OF RECOGNITION AND RECALL. Andrew M. McCullough"

Transcription

1 FALSE MEMORY INCREASED BY SELF-GENERATION IN COMPARISONS OF RECOGNITION AND RECALL Andrew M. McCullough A Thesis Submitted to the University of North Carolina Wilmington in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts Department of Psychology University of North Carolina Wilmington 2010 Approved by Advisory Committee Julian R. Keith Dale J. Cohen Jeffrey P. Toth Chair Accepted by h Dean, Graduate School

2 TABLE OF CONTENTS ABSTRACT... iv LIST OF TABLES... v LIST OF FIGURES... vi INTRODUCTION... 1 COMPARING RECOGNITION AND RECALL... 4 THEORIES OF RECOGNITION AND RECALL... 7 Theories of Recognition... 9 Theories of Recall Metacognitive Processes Directly Comparing Recognition and Recall False Memory in Recognition and Recall OVERVIEW OF EXPERIMENTS EXPERIMENT Memory and Detection Theory Current Predictions Methods Participants and design Materials Procedure Results and Discussion Confidence ratings ii

3 Sensitivity and bias ROC-based measures EXPERIMENT Current Predictions Methods Participants, design, and procedure Results and Discussion Type 1 analyses Type 2 confidence Metamemory parameters ROC-based measures GENERAL DISCUSSION TABLES FIGURES REFERENCES iii

4 ABSTRACT The overarching goal of this research was to explore the relation between recognition and recall. A more proximate goal was to investigate a recent report of elevated false memories in cued recall relative to recognition. Two experiments are presented that compared performance on matched tests of recognition and cued recall, and replicated this increase in false memory in confidence ratings (Experiment 1) and binary judgments (Experiment 2). Type-1 signal detection analyses of memory performance (Experiments 1) revealed changes in both sensitivity and response bias. In Experiment 2, the effect on type-1 sensitivity was observed with no effect on response bias. Type-2 analyses (Experiment 2) revealed no effects of test condition on metamemory for studied-item trials, but significant effects on metamemory confidence bias for unstudied-item trials. These results clarify prior studies, showing that self-generation in cued recall impairs memory sensitivity, and imply that previous, inconclusive results were obscured by metacognitive processes. iv

5 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Proportions of Recollect, Familiar, and New Judgments for Recognition and Cued Recall Type-1 Signal Detection Measures and Parameters for Recognition and Cued Recall Type-1 Signal Detection Measures and Parameters for Recognition and Cued Recall in Experiment Mean Type-2 Confidence Type-2 Signal Detection Measures and Parameters v

6 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Classification model for Type-1 responses and theoretical signal detection distributions General interpretations of memory data according to signal detection theory Receiver operating characteristic curves for recognition and cued recall in experiment Classification model for Type-2 responses and theoretical signal detection distributions Type-2 receiver operating characteristic curves for recognition and cued recall in experiment vi

7 FALSE MEMORY INCREASED BY SELF-GENERATION IN COMPARISONS OF RECOGNITION AND RECALL INTRODUCTION Two methods for measuring memory performance are recognition and recall tasks, and both are analogous to everyday uses of memory. While recognition refers to an awareness of having previously experienced some target information, recall requires the (re-)production of previously experienced information. In recognition tasks, people are presented with an item (e.g., a word, a face, a melody) and are asked to judge whether they have experienced that item in the past, usually in some specific context (i.e., a study list). For recall tasks, people are reminded of a specific past event and asked to produce (i.e., self-generate) some items that occurred within it. Everyday life provides abundant examples of this contrast between recall and recognition. After attending a weekend party, for example, one might be asked by a friend who was unable to attend, "who was at the party?" (a recall question), followed by, "was John there?" (a recognition question). The distinction between recall and recognition is also critical for the scientific study of memory. However, although both are generally viewed as conscious, "explicit" forms of retrieval (Cabeza et al., 1997) they are often studied separately, and have engendered different methods, phenomena, and theories (Haist, Shimamura, & Squire, 1992). The ubiquity of the distinction between recall and recognition raises two important issues. The first concerns the extent to which recognition and recall engage similar underlying processes. As described more fully below, attempts to understand the relation between these two ways of expressing memory has a long history in psychology; however, it is difficult to find clear statements in the literature about exactly how these two tests are related. The second issue

8 concerns false memories. That is, to what extent are these two forms of memory susceptible to errors and distortion? The study of false memory has become a central issue in both cognitive psychology and cognitive neuroscience (e.g., Schacter & Tulving, 1994), not only because of its relevance in applied contexts such as eye-witness testimony (see Loftus, 1975), but also because it is viewed as providing clues to the more general operation of memory, both accurate and false (see Jacoby & Rhodes, 2006; Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993; Schacter, Norman, Koutstaal, 1998). The purpose of the present research was to address the two issues raised above. Two experiments are presented that directly compared recall and recognition in an attempt to better understand the relation between the two, and to investigate whether the generation process underlying recall makes this form of retrieval more susceptible to false memory. The research is based on unpublished studies by Toth (2005) and McCullough & Toth (2008), both of which found evidence for a high degree of similarity in performance on matched tests of recall and recognition, as well as evidence that, relative to recognition, recall increases false memory. Importantly, those studies employed a "remember/know" methodology (Tulving, 1985) that, while used extensively in the memory literature (see Gardiner, Ramponi, & Richardson-Klavehn, 2002; Gardiner & Richardson-Klavehn, 2000), has been criticized on theoretical grounds (Dunn, 2004; Rotello, Macmillan, Reeder, & Wong, 2005; Wixted & Stretch, 2004). Thus, one specific goal of the present research was to replicate those prior findings using a more standard memorytesting technique, confidence ratings. A second goal of the present research was to replicate and extend our prior findings by analyzing memory performance using Receiver Operator Characteristic (ROC) curves and other measures from both type-1 and type-2 Signal Detection Theory (SDT). Type-1 SDT is 2

9 commonly used in the analysis of recognition memory performance (for a review, see Yonelinas & Parks, 2007) where it provides measures of both sensitivity (i.e., the ability to discriminate between studied and unstudied items) and response bias (i.e., the overall tendency of a person to respond old ). The application of type-1 SDT to recall is less common, given the participants role in producing both studied and unstudied test items (Green & Swets, 1966), but there are exceptions (see Higham, Perfect, & Bruno, 2009). The present research examined recall in a paradigm that encouraged production of unstudied items, and therefore allows SDT-based measures to be computed. Although type-1 SDT provides a solid theoretical basis for analyzing memory performance, it does not incorporate metacognitive processes that have recently been argued to be critical for regulating mnemonic accuracy (Koriat & Goldsmith, 1996; Koriat, Goldsmith, Pansky, 2000; Perfect & Schwartz, 2002). However, Higham has recently shown how type-2 SDT can be used to measure metacognitive processes, both in recall (Higham & Tam, 2005) and recognition (Higham et al., 2009). In the context of memory, type-2 SDT provides two metacognitive measures of performance that are useful for describing the relationship between response accuracy and confidence (for a review of type-2 SDT, see Galvin, Podd, Drga, & Whitmore, 2003). Monitoring (analogous to sensitivity) reflects a person's ability to discriminate between correct and incorrect responses, while confidence bias (analogous to response bias) reflects the tendency to accept their answers as correct. The present research also employed type- 2 SDT methods in order to compare the metacognitive processes associated with recall and recognition, and to investigate their role in the production of false memories. The remainder of this introduction is designed to more fully explain and motivate the current research. I start by describing the research by Toth (2005) and McCullough and Toth 3

10 (2008) in more detail. I then provide a theoretical background for the two main issues addressed in the current research, that is, the relation between recognition and recall, and findings of false memories on these tests. One specific false memory phenomenon the revelation effect is described in particular detail because it bears a high similarity to the effects explored in the present research, and thusly motivated the use of ROC curves and SDT for these investigations. I conclude this introduction with an overview of the experiments. Details about the application of type-1 SDT to recognition and recall are provided in the introduction to Experiment 1; details about type-2 SDT are provided in the introduction to Experiment 2. COMPARING RECOGNITION AND RECALL IN THE REMEMBER/KNOW TASK Toth (2005) compared recognition and recall in six experiments using variants of the remember/know procedure initially introduced by Tulving (1985; see also Gardiner & Richardson-Klavehn, 2000). Experiment 1 by Toth provides an example of this procedure, along with the mixed-test design that was adapted for the current research. In this experiment, participants studied a list of common, 5-letter English words (e.g., truck, smart ), and were then given a memory test that included both recognition and recall trials. On recognition trials, participants were shown studied or nonstudied words which they were asked to classify as Recollected (R), Familiar (F), or New (N). Participants were told to classify a word as R when they could clearly remember details surrounding their initial study of the word, F when the word was familiar but they could not remember any specific study details, and N when the word neither elicited details nor was familiar. 1 On recall trials, participants were presented with word stems (e.g., tru--, sma-- ) which they were told to use as cues for recalling words studied 1 Rather than Familiar, many researchers use the term Know to denote items that are familiar but lack episodic detail. 4

11 earlier, guessing if necessary. After completing the stem, they then provided an R/F/N judgment, just as they had for recognition trials. The data from this experiment, mean proportions of R, F, and N judgments for recognition and recall trials, are shown in Table 1a. Note that, for recognition trials, participants provided a response to every studied item, and an equal number of unstudied items, and thus these proportions sum to 1.0. For the recall trials, in contrast, participants did not have the opportunity to respond to every item in the stimulus set because they sometimes failed to generate these words. The recall data is thus presented in two ways, as unconditionalized and conditionalized proportions. The unconditionalized proportions (bottom row) represent the proportions of R, F, and N responses out of all possible trials (i.e., out of all possible studied and unstudied items cued in recall); thus, these values sum to the mean proportions of studied and unstudied items that were output in response to all of the recall cues. For the conditionalized proportions, the number of R, F, and N responses is divided by the number of studied or unstudied words output. 2 Note that this correction is analogous to that used by Koriat and colleagues to compute memory accuracy (see Koriat & Goldsmith, 1994, 1996). More relevant for present purposes, it puts the recall data on the same scale as recognition (i.e., the conditionalized recall proportions sum to 1.0) thereby allowing a direct comparison of performance in the two test types. Two interesting results emerged from this experiment. First, looking at studied items (left panel of Table 1a), one can see that performance was nearly identical between recognition and conditionalized recall. These data suggest the processes underlying recognition and recall are highly similar, differing primarily in initial access to (i.e., the source of generation of) 2 As in the priming literature, restricting analyses to the critical set of target words allows the effects of studying an item to be compared to a baseline of outputting the same item when it had not been studied. 5

12 relevant target information. Once this difference is removed, by conditionalizing recall proportions on output of test items, recognition and recall performance are nearly identical. Despite this evidence of similarity between processes underlying recognition and recall (see also, Hamilton & Rajaram, 2003), the data revealed an important difference between test formats. In particular, the proportion of F responses to unstudied words (right panel of Table 1a) was much higher in conditionalized recall than in recognition. This elevation in false alarms for cued recall, replicated in five additional experiments by Toth (2005), suggests that the process of generating items increased their subjective familiarity, compared to when those items were presented externally (i.e., by the computer) on the recognition memory trials. McCullough and Toth (2008) replicated the above findings using a novel sample (UNCW undergraduates), a novel set of stimuli, and a slightly different test procedure asking participants to first make an old/new (i.e., studied/unstudied) judgment, followed by a recollect/familiar classification only for the items judged old. As shown in Table 1b, both patterns described above were replicated. That is, for studied items, there was a close correspondence of response proportions across test type (all differences n.s.) while, for unstudied items, there was a significant increase in F responses in recall as compared to recognition. 3 This consistent finding is particularly interesting when one considers that the set of (generated) items in which the false alarms increase are all words that are counterbalanced to recognition trials. Consequently, the procedure allows one to conclude that something inherent to self-generation is increasing false memory in cued recall. What explains the increased false alarm rate in cued recall observed by Toth (2005) and McCullough and Toth (2008)? One possibility is that, relative to recognition, self-generation of 3 Note that conditionalized recall proportions are necessarily interdependent (i.e., constrained to sum to 1.0); thus, the increased proportion of F responses is accompanied by a reduced proportion of N responses. 6

13 items in recall increases the processing fluency, and thus the perceived familiarity, of these items. Fluency has been identified as an important factor affecting recognition memory and other judgments (Jacoby & Whitehouse, 1989; see also Oppenheimer, 2008; Whittlesea, 1993; Whittlesea & Williams, 2001). However, research shows that processing fluency affects response criterion placement (Whittlesea; see also Jacoby & Whitehouse), suggesting that effects would be similar for studied and unstudied items. The consistent finding that hit rates do not differ between recognition and cued recall (Toth; McCullough & Toth) refutes a simple fluency-based explanation of the increased false alarm rate. The finding that an independent variable affects unstudied, but not studied, items is unusual and relevant to theories of memory (see Jacoby, Shimizu, Daniels, & Rhodes, 2005). One potential way of explaining this pattern is to assume that self-generation in recall produced a shift in the strength or familiarity distributions underlying old and/or new items, perhaps in addition to a change in response bias. Indeed, an explanation of this sort has been put forth to explain a similar pattern of data (i.e., a memory effect selective to unstudied items) in the revelation effect literature (Verde & Rotello, 2004). One of the goals of the present research is to evaluate whether a similar distribution-shift explanation can explain the difference in false alarm rates for recall vs. recognition. I return to this issue in the False Memory section below. First, however, I provide some background on the nature of the processes thought to underlie recognition and recall. THEORIES OF RECOGNITION AND RECALL What are the processes that underlie recognition and recall performance? An early idea was that the two forms of retrieval reflected the same memory process activation of an 7

14 underlying memory trace and that performance differences between the two simply reflected the superiority of recognition in activating the trace (Hunt & Ellis, 2004). This idea was consistent with the fact that recognition is generally easier than recall, and often results in higher memory scores. It is also consistent with Tulving's (1983) claim that recognition is similar to recall, but involves a highly specific copy cue for retrieval. However, subsequent research cast doubt on at least a simple form of this single-process hypothesis, which predicts that (a) recognition will always be higher than recall, and (b) recognition and recall will be affected similarly by manipulations that affect memory strength. Both of these predictions have been refuted. Specifically, recall can exceed recognition, as in the case of recognition failure of recallable words (Tulving & Thomson, 1973; see also, Watkins, 1974), and some experimental variables, such as word frequency (see Gregg, 1976), have opposite effects on recognition and recall performance. Given these findings, most of the research over the last two decades has progressed as independent domains and there are few clear statements about the relationship between recognition and recall. However, from those decades of research, a variety of theories have been proposed to explain each of these types of retrieval separately. In the following subsections, I provide brief overviews of these theories. I then describe recent research and theory on the role of metacognitive processes in memory tasks. These developments provide motivation for the current Experiment 2, which used type-2 SDT to explore metacognitive processes in recognition and recall. I conclude this section by describing research that has directly compared recognition and recall, noting the advantages and disadvantages of the mixed-test approach used in the present research. 8

15 Theories of Recognition Early theories proposed a single, evaluative process to underlie recognition (see Bernbach, 1967), such that test items are evaluated along a psychological scale of evidence often characterized as memory strength or familiarity. This "single-process" view of recognition is embodied in Global-Matching Models, such as SAM (Gillund & Shiffrin, 1984) and MINERVA 2 (Hintzman, 1988), and in the application of detection theory (Green & Swets, 1966) to recognition (e.g., Donaldson, 1992; Donaldson & Good, 1996; MacMillan & Creelman, 2005; Mickes, Wixted, Wais, 2007). However, numerous studies suggest that, in addition to a sense of memory strength or familiarity, recognition can also invoke a process of controlled retrieval that results in lucid memory of contextual details from the encoding event (e.g., Tulving & Thomson, 1973; see also Jacoby et al., 2005; Mandler, 1980). Stated differently, there is evidence that recognition cannot be fully described as involving only a process of evaluation, because encoding details or source information may also be retrieved or generated from memory (Gardiner, Ramponi, & Richardson-Klavehn, 1998; Gardiner, Richardson-Klavehn, & Ramponi, 1998; Johnson et al., 1993; Vilberg & Rugg, 2007). The above findings have led to the development of dual-process models of recognition (e.g., Jacoby, 1991; Jacoby & Dallas, 1981; Mandler, 1980) which propose a controlled-retrieval or recollection process that works in parallel with a more strength-based process of familiarity evaluation. In fact, hybrid theories such as the dual-process/signal-detection model have been supported by ROC data from recognition tasks in normal (Yonelinas, 1994) and amnesiac populations (Yonelinas, Kroll, Dobbins, Lazzara, & Knight, 1998), as well as by a wealth of neuro-imaging data (Eichenbaum, Yonelinas, & Ranganath, 2007; Elfman, Parks, & Yonelinas, 2008; Vilberg & Rugg, 2007). 9

16 The current project will not settle the ongoing debate between single-process and dual-process theories of recognition, as a variety of research has revealed compelling evidence on both sides (cf. Parks & Yonelinas, 2007; Wixted, 2007). In later sections, I describe the motivation of the use of signal detection theory (SDT) to analyze memory performance. Given the application of SDT, the experiments are most easily viewed in terms of a single, strengthbased recognition process. At the same time, the use of type-2 SDT in Experiment 2 tacitly assumes the involvement of metacognitive processes in task performance, thus, going beyond a simple single-process view (see also Bellezza, 2003). Moreover, in the General Discussion, I consider interpretations of the present data if a controlled retrieval process (e.g., Recollection) was assumed to affect performance, as is proposed by dual-process theories. Theories of Recall As with recognition, a variety of theories have been developed from studies of recall. One of the earliest accounts suggested that two main processes underlie recall performance: generation and recognition. First, potential memory information is covertly generated, and then generated candidates undergo a recognition-like process of evaluation (e.g., Bodner, Masson, Caldwell, 2000). These Generate-Recognize (GR) theories can be traced to Kintsch (1970; see also Anderson & Bower, 1972; Bahrick, 1970), and in simplest form, predict that a recognition test probe will incite better performance than any recall cue. However, as Jacoby & Hollingshead (1990) discuss, Tulving & colleagues (e.g., Tulving & Thomson, 1973) refuted early GR theories based on experimental findings that, in some situations, recall can exceed recognition. Such limitations of early GR theories led to proposals that recall may be accomplished by the Direct Retrieval (DR) of information from memory (e.g., Guynn & McDaniel, 1999; Jacoby, 1998). Theories of DR posit that the cues available for recall can constrain processing such that 10

17 only relevant information is retrieved, irrespective of whether retrieval is controlled or automatic. Admittedly, the basic theories of GR and DR are just starting points for exploring recall performance, and both have evolved into many forms. Though the models may seem to be in opposition, there is a general consensus that both strategies are used in different situations (Curran & Hintzman, 1995; Jacoby, 1998). Variations of GR theories have been proposed that do not succumb to the refutations by Tulving and colleagues described above. Moreover, recent models propose that DR could be described as a contextually-constrained version of GR (e.g., Higham & Tam, 2005; Jacoby & Hollingshead, 1990). That is, if recall cues are sufficient to specify a particular prior experience (e.g., a study list), those cues could limit the generation process to information from only that specific experience. Such a theory implies that the varied strategies employed across the wide spectrum of recall tasks are not easily divided into two distinct categories. Most relevant to the current research, Toth (2005, Experiment 2) compared GR and DR strategies in cued recall by directly manipulating test instructions, but observed little difference between the two conditions. This result, coupled with the high degree of correspondence across test format between R/F/N judgments for studied items, led Toth to conclude that, at least for the stimuli and retrieval cues he employed, recall performance was most consistent with a GR theory (see also Jacoby & Hollingshead, 1990). The present experiments will also bear on the issue of how best to characterize cued recall performance. Metacognitive Processes in Recognition and Recall Metacognition refers to thoughts, feelings, and beliefs about one s own cognitive processing; metamemory describes metacognitive phenomena related to remembering. Metamemory is typically investigated by collecting some type of performance judgment during 11

18 the study phase (e.g., judgments of learning) or test procedure (e.g., feelings of knowing, ratings of confidence-in-accuracy) (Hunt & Ellis, 2004). To paraphrase Dunlosky & Bjork (2008), any attempt to investigate memory or metamemory alone will likely fall short, as they are intricately linked. Moreover, the complex interplay of memory and metamemory is likely related to the ideas of consciousness, and particularly subjective awareness. Tulving (1985) reminded memory researchers that conscious remembering is an intrinsically subjective phenomenon, and encouraged the development of methods for exploring the subjective awareness of memory and other cognitive phenomena. This general view permeates contemporary theories of memory and metamemory, as evidenced by Koriat, Goldsmith, and Pansky s (2000) description of the human participant as an active agent that has at his/her disposal an arsenal of cognitive strategies and devices that can be flexibly applied in order to reach certain goals. The choice of such strategies as well as their online regulation is based on the subjective monitoring of these processes. As theorists have begun to explore how subjective awareness affects task performance, a great deal of effort has been directed at developing methods to better assess the separate roles of memory and metamemory processes in overall performance (e.g., Higham & Tam, 2005; Koriat & Goldsmith, 1994, 1996). Koriat and colleagues have designed a number of studies to elucidate mnemonic processes at the metamemory level. Noting that remembering in the real-world is often more concerned with accuracy than with quantity, Koriat & Goldsmith (1994) manipulated response option (i.e., free/forced) and test format (i.e., recognition/recall) to examine monitoring and control of memory accuracy. The authors developed a framework that distinguishes between measures of memory that are quantity-based and accuracy-based, noting that effects shown between recognition and recall are often confounded with response option. That is, forced 12

19 recognition (i.e., a quantity-based, or input-bound measure) is most frequently compared with free recall (i.e., an accuracy-based, or output-bound measure). The authors suggest that many comparisons of recognition and recall have focused on memory quantity, while overlooking memory accuracy. Subsequent research has shown that forcing output in recall tasks reduces memory accuracy without affecting memory quantity (Koriat & Goldsmith, 1996). Moreover, the increased accuracy in free recall is a function of a reduction in errors of commission, compared to forced report (Koriat & Goldsmith, 1996; Higham & Tam, 2005). In other words, when given an opportunity to withhold responses in recall tasks, participants use metacognitive processes to monitor generated items and control output of those items in an adaptive manner. While the current research differs considerably from studies comparing free and forced report procedures, the research described above does inform the current investigation of recognition and cued recall. In particular, the current research explored a reported increase in false alarms, or errors of commission, in cued recall. The current paradigm did not include a free-report phase, but Experiment 2 did explore metacognitive processes in recognition and cued recall using a paradigm that I describe below. So despite differences between the paradigms which preclude direct comparisons between the current results and studies comparing free- and forced-report procedures work by Koriat & colleagues (Koriat & Goldsmith, 1994, 1996; see also Higham & Tam, 2005) provides the foundation for basic predictions in the current Experiment 2. Higham and colleagues (2009) have examined metamemory in recognition, using a variant of signal detection theory (SDT). While type-1 SDT is commonly used to analyze recognition performance, Higham et al. suggest that type-2 SDT is well suited to examine the relation between memory accuracy and confidence. This relationship is necessarily 13

20 metacognitive, linking memory performance with processes that could allow for the monitoring and control of responding. I describe type-2 SDT in the overview to Experiment 2; for now, note that the current investigations are related to both of these methods for exploring metamemory. Specifically, the current Experiment 1 compares memory performance within subjects on tests of recognition and cued recall with a measure of memory accuracy analogous to that used by Koriat and Goldsmith (1996). Moreover, the current method uses confidence ratings, which allows for the application of Higham et al. s method of exploring metamemory with type-2 SDT (see Experiment 2). I describe the benefits of the current research more fully in the next section. Directly Comparing Recognition and Recall Given the extensive literatures on recall and recognition, it is surprising that there is not more research directly comparing these two forms of retrieval. A series of "successive testing" studies by Tulving and others - primarily directed at the phenomenon of recognition failure of recallable words - probably constitutes the most concerted effort to understand relations between recognition and recall. This research is broadly summarized by the empirically-based Tulving- Wiseman function (Tulving & Wiseman, 1975; see also Flexser & Tulving, 1978; Wiseman & Tulving, 1976) which predicts a moderate but consistent association between recognition and recall. However, it is important to note that the Tulving-Wiseman function does not directly relate performance on the tests, but rather, shows the predictive relation between recognition and the proportion of recallable words that will also be recognized. That is, the Tulving-Wiseman function was primarily designed to describe the phenomenon of recognition failure of recallable words. Moreover, that paradigm, in which the same items are used in successive tests of recognition and recall, has been strongly criticized on statistical grounds (e.g., Simpson s paradox, see Hintzman, 1980, 1992, 1993); it also has the drawback that responses to a memory 14

21 probe in one test format (e.g., recall) may influence responses to that same item in a subsequent test format (e.g., recognition). Aside from the recognition failure studies by Tulving and colleagues, the majority of studies directly comparing performance across test format find that recognition is typically better than recall (Aggleton & Shaw, 1996). Studies have also revealed dissociations between recognition and recall performance, for example, as a function of word frequency (see Gregg, 1976; Reder, Anderson, & Bjork, 1974). A number of studies of neurological patients have shown that amnesiacs recall performance is often impaired to a much greater degree than is their recognition performance (e.g., Haist, et al., 1992; Warrington & Weiskrantz, 1968), but again, these results may have been influenced by the quantity/accuracy distinction described above (see Koriat & Goldsmith, 1994, 1996). Craik (1979) has used these patterns to suggest that, while recognition performance is typically better, the critical difference between recognition and recall tasks is the amount of effortful, self-initiated processing required. As described above, the present study is an extension of Toth s (2005) direct comparisons of recognition and cued recall, which used a remember/know (R/K) procedure (Tulving, 1985). Tulving s first use of the R/K procedure compared four memory tasks recognition, free recall, and two forms of cued recall and showed that states of subjective awareness vary across tasks. Hamilton & Rajaram (2003) replicated his work, using a between-groups design to obviate the criticisms of successive testing, and specifically examined recollective experience across tests. In four experiments, the authors observed no difference between recognition and cued recall for the proportion of retrieved items that were also given remember responses (cf. Tables 1 and 2). Note that, Hamilton and Rajaram compared the dependent measure Remember/total Retrieved, which is analogous to the transformation of cued recall data presented by Toth. 15

22 The current paradigm alleviates a number of concerns that have plagued previous comparisons of recognition and recall. Many prior studies employed a successive-testing procedure, in which participants take recognition and recall tests for the same material in succession. By randomizing recognition and recall trials within a single test list, the current mixed-test paradigm avoids confounds that limit successive-testing procedures, including effects of test order, response strategy changes between tests, and memory interference from having already been tested on an item (in the other format). Additionally, because all stimuli are counterbalanced across test format and study condition, the paradigm allows for within-subject comparisons of recognition and recall performance. The final advantage of the current paradigm is the simplicity with which recognition and recall are scaled for comparison. The current research uses a confidence rating method to explore effects of memory that were previously examined with a R/K methodology. Yonelinas (2001) empirically compared the R/K and confidence rating methods (as well as the process-dissociation procedure, Jacoby, 1991), and found strong evidence that measures derived from each methodology converged upon similar results about underlying mnemonic processes. His study suggests that experimental results will generalize across test methodologies, and thusly motivated the current research. Nevertheless, the current paradigm does not come without limitations. Most notably, there are items tested in recognition that are not tested in recall (i.e., not generated to a cue), thus raising questions about the assumptions underlying the application of SDT. One of the goals of the present research is to address this issue by directly comparing SDT parameters derived from recall to those derived from recognition, in which all items are tested. A major goal of this comparison is to better understand the elevated false memory rate observed by Toth (2005) and McCullough & Toth (2008). As described in the next section, the current project will inform 16

23 false memory research, as the presently-explored effect may reflect the operation of a novel mechanism underlying false memory (namely, self-generation). False Memory in Recognition and Recall False memory has become a central issue in memory research, both for applied reasons and because false memory effects are thought to reveal fundamental characteristics of the processes underlying normal, accurate memory performance (for a review, see Schacter et al., 1998). There are a handful of mechanisms thought to underlie the occurrence of false memory (Schacter, 1999), including spreading activation (as shown in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm; Roediger & McDermott, 1995), fluency misattributions (as shown in the false fame effect; Jacoby, Woloshyn, & Kelley, 1989), and source confusion (as shown in the misinformation effect; Loftus, 1975). The revelation effect, an increase in false recognition of revealed items (Watkins & Peynirciouglu, 1990), has recently been used to constrain theories of memory (e.g., Verde & Rotello, 2004). In this line of research, performance on standard recognition trials is compared to trials in which test words are revealed from some degraded form (e.g., presenting test words letter-by-letter, or as an anagram along with a solution code). Using a variety of revelation tasks, many researchers have shown increases in false memory compared to standard recognition tasks, but failed to elucidate the underlying nature of this effect (see Verde & Rotello, 2003). Verde & Rotello (2004) divided two decades of revelation studies into two categories, astutely noting a critical difference. In some studies the revealed item was different than the actual recognition test item (cf. Westerman & Greene, 1996), while in other studies the revelation manipulation was applied to the test items themselves (cf. Peynirciouglu & Tekcan, 1993). The authors thus explored the effects of unrelated-item and same-item revelation tasks, 17

24 using Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) curves to compare each condition to standard recognition. 4 Verde & Rotello (2004) found that, compared to standard recognition, the unrelated-item revelation condition produced an increase in old responses to both studied and unstudied items. In contrast, same-item revelation was found to increase old responses only for unstudied items. Under a signal detection framework, these results suggest that revealing an unrelated item prior to presenting a recognition (test) probe causes a liberal change in response bias, but that revealing the test item itself (prior to recognition judgment) causes a true decrement to memory sensitivity (Verde & Rotello, 2004). In the following section, I describe these theoretical explanations in relation to the current research. Importantly, the authors used confidence-based ROCs to elucidate memory phenomena that could not be understood via simpler applications of SDT. Perhaps most important, the effect of the same-item revelation condition is similar to the effect shown by Toth (2005), as both effects are shown only with unstudied items, and neither effect is well described by any of the mechanisms of false memory noted above. Thus, the methods and analyses presented by Verde & Rotello (2004) guided the current project for two specific reasons: (a) the afore-mentioned similarities in methods and findings, and (b) the opportunity to explore the effects of self-generation using confidence based-roc curves. The application of confidence-based ROC curves (i.e., type-1 SDT) is common in memory research, and is described in the following section. 4 The authors had previously shown that more common measures of performance (e.g., d ) are unable to account for the revelation effect, and argued that ROC-based d a is a better measure of memory sensitivity (Verde & Rotello, 2003). 18

25 OVERVIEW OF EXPERIMENTS The goals of the current research are to replicate and further examine the effect of false familiarity due to self-generation. While the increase in false memory in cued recall has only been shown with the remember/know method, the confidence rating method may confer measurement advantages (see Donaldson & Good, 1996), and has probably shaped theories of memory more so than any other experimental methodology (Parks & Yonelinas, 2007). Moreover, Yonelinas (2001) has shown that the remember/know method captures similar mnemonic phenomena as the confidence rating method, and the latter has recently helped illuminate a similar effect of false memory (Verde & Rotello, 2004). Thus, the current research employed a confidence rating method and two analytic approaches based in signal detection theory (SDT) to investigate the relation between recognition and cued recall, and the increase in false memory shown in cued recall (Toth, 2005). Experiment 1 employed analyses based in type- 1 SDT to compare recognition and cued recall, collecting confidence ratings rather than R/F/N judgments, as done previously by Toth. By subjecting the effect of false familiarity due to selfgeneration to rigorous signal detection analyses, the present research seeks to bridge the gap between this effect and other mechanisms of false memory. As discussed above, metamemory parameters have permeated theories of memory performance. Thus, Experiment 2 employed analyses based in type-2 SDT to investigate metamemory processes in the current paradigm. Prior to each experiment, I describe each method and relevant predictions in detail. EXPERIMENT 1 The goals of Experiment 1 are twofold: (a) to replicate the two patterns observed by Toth (2005) and McCullough & Toth (2008), that is, comparable performance on studied items and 19

26 increased false alarms in cued recall, and (b) to further elucidate the nature of the false memory effect by employing type-1 signal detection theory to compare ROC curves. The first important question this experiment will answer is whether the increase in false memory will extend to confidence ratings. The second theoretical issue concerns the relation between present and past investigations of the false memory effect (i.e., exactly how will the cued recall-based increase in false alarms manifest in confidence ratings?). A third issue addressed by this experiment is how the present effect relates to other false memory phenomena, such as the revelation effect. Before presenting specific predictions, I first describe the application of signal detection theory to memory research and briefly explore theoretical interpretations of memory phenomena. I then present the methods and results of Experiment 1. Memory and Detection Theory Signal Detection Theory (SDT) has proven a useful statistical framework for collecting and analyzing recognition data. Many factors led quite naturally to the use of SDT in characterizing recognition memory; most notably, (a) the recognition test situation is analogous to a signal detection task, with the signal being oldness or familiarity ; and (b) recognition performance is well capture by the 2x2 combination of test-probe status (studied, unstudied) and the participant s response ( old, new ). These combinations yield the standard SDT measures of hits, misses, false alarms, and correct rejections (see Figure 1a). Theoretically, as shown in Figure 1b, two distributions of items (studied, unstudied) lie on an axis of increasing evidence, trace strength, or familiarity. The overlapping distributions are presumably separated by the effects of the study experience. Participants are thought to place a response criterion (C) along the strength axis, dividing the distributions into four areas that correspond to the dependent measures. That is, the proportions of responses ( old, new ) to studied and unstudied items 20

27 correspond to the four areas under the theoretical distributions. Useful measures of sensitivity (e.g., d ) and response bias (e.g., B D ), whose formulas are derived from the underlying theory, can then be calculated (see Macmillan & Creelman, 2005). Sensitivity (i.e., the ability to discriminate studied and unstudied items) is indexed by the distance between the means of the distributions, while response bias (i.e., the tendency to respond old ) is commonly measured relative to the point where the curves intersect. Recall data is generally not analyzed with SDT, due to the fact that, in most recall paradigms, misses and correct rejections are not reported (i.e. participants withhold them). As such, proportional hit and false alarm rates cannot be computed (Green & Swets, 1966). Critical to the present research, proportional hit and false alarm rates can be calculated, specifically, because participants provide misses and correct rejections (i.e., new responses to relevant items generated in cued recall). In recent literature, SDT has proven particularly useful in exploring a false memory phenomenon. Verde and Rotello (2004) used a confidence rating procedure to explore the nature of the revelation effect. Figure 2b depicts Verde and Rotello s theoretical interpretation of the same-item revelation condition. As compared to standard recognition (Figure 2a) in which the study experience has separated the distributions by two (arbitrarily drawn) units in the revelation condition, the act of revealing the test item is thought to shift the distribution of unstudied items to the right along the axis of evidence. Thus, sensitivity (as indexed by the distance between the means of the distributions) is reduced, and response bias (as measured relative to the intersection of the two curves) can become more liberal. In comparing Figures 2a and 2b, note that the area representing false alarms differs, while the area for hits remains constant. Thus, a shift in the unstudied-item distribution could also explain the findings of increased false alarms in cued recall, compared to recognition (Toth, 2005). 21

28 Figure 2c presents a theoretical depiction of an alternative data pattern, for comparison to the standard recognition depiction (Figure 2a) and the most plausible interpretation of the effects of self-generation (Figure 2b). Figure 2c represents a fluency-based hypothesis, which suggests that the process of generating items increases the processing fluency of those items. Note in this depiction how an increase in fluency would shift both distributions and affect the hit and false alarm rates to similar degrees, thus manifesting as a change in response bias. Figure 2c also depicts a criterion-shift hypothesis, which suggests that the act of generating test items in cued recall causes participants to adopt a more conservative response criterion. Much like the fluency hypothesis, a simple criterion shift would affect the hit and false alarm rates similarly, and thus a simple effect increase of processing fluency or change in response bias does not fit the observed effects of self-generation. Keep in mind, these are only basic interpretations. For example, selfgeneration might increase the familiarity of studied and unstudied items to different degrees, or it could affect response criterion placement in addition to moving one or both of the distributions. While signal detection theory describes recognition performance well, the application of SDT to memory can be intricate, particularly in regard to the assumptions underlying various parameters. Though the debate is beyond the scope of this thesis, many theorists support empirical methods based in SDT, but caution against interpretations based solely on SDT parameters, particularly those that don t reflect performance across a range of response criteria (e.g., Donaldson & Good, 1996; Verde & Rotello, 2004; Yonelinas, 1994, 2001; Yonelinas & Parks, 2007). A confidence-based receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve is a plot of proportions of responses to studied items against proportions of responses to unstudied items across a range of response confidence. ROCs are typically created by having participants make memory decisions on a scale of confidence (e.g., 1 = sure new to 6 = sure studied) and plotting 22

29 cumulative response proportions for studied vs. unstudied items at each level of confidence. That is, the proportion of 6 responses to studied items is plotted against the proportion of 6 responses to unstudied items. Next, the cumulative proportion of 5 and 6 responses to studied items is plotted against the same cumulative proportion for unstudied items, and so on through the proportions for confidence level 1. 5 Because the resultant curve reflects memory performance across a range of subjective confidence, measures derived from ROCs have been supported over simpler measures (see Verde & Rotello, 2003). However, despite the utility of ROC analyses, ROC-derived parameters also require theoretical assumptions, and so do not necessarily obviate the criticisms of applying detection theory to memory research. Nonetheless, a major goal of this research project was to investigate the false memory effect via SDT. Because debate continues over which is most appropriate, I calculated a number of measures of sensitivity. Current Predictions In previous research, Toth (2005) observed increased false memory in cued recall, relative to recognition. The current Experiment 1 seeks to replicate the effect in a confidence rating procedure, which allows for a number of predictions at varying levels of analysis. Toward the primary goal of replication, the current empirical design tests two predictions. The first prediction, Hypothesis 1a, is that mean confidence for studied items will not differ significantly across test formats, whereas mean confidence for unstudied items will be significantly higher in cued recall relative to recognition. In prior studies, the effect of false memory was revealed by comparing proportions of responses that are analogous to the SDT-based measures of hit rate (HR) and false alarm rate (FAR). Thus, for Hypothesis 1b, I predict that proportions of high 5 Note that the cumulative proportion of confidence judgments at the lowest level of confidence includes all trials, assuming participants must provide a confidence judgment for every test trial. 23

30 confidence responses to studied items (i.e., simple HR) will be equal across test formats, but that proportions of high confidence responses to unstudied items (i.e., simple FAR) will be significantly increased in cued recall. 6 The two predictions above are proposed to most directly replicate the results of Toth (2005); however, a secondary goal of this project was to vigorously explore the false memory effect using SDT. While Hypotheses 1a and 1b will satisfy the goal of replication, the former is not a signal detection analysis, and the latter is based upon measures that have been criticized in studies of memory (e.g., Yonelinas, 2001). Drawing on the signal detection interpretations described above, increased confidence selective to unstudied items (in cued recall) is indicative of a decrease in memory sensitivity (see Figure 2a). Thus, for Hypothesis 2, I predict that sensitivity will be significantly reduced in cued recall, compared to recognition. There are a number of ways to test this prediction, which I discuss below. In addition to a change in sensitivity, Toth observed a change in response bias in five of eight conditions, such that responding was significantly more liberal in cued recall relative to recognition. The effect was highly variable in size, and, in some cases, was found despite observing a significant effect for only unstudied items. Thus, for Hypothesis 3, I predict that responding will be liberal in cued recall, compared to recognition. As the primary effect of self-generation is not thought to be on response bias, I predict only a trend toward more liberal responding in cued recall, as measured by B D (Donaldson, 1992). In prior studies, Toth (2005) observed a significant effect of test format on sensitivity in seven of eight conditions. However, because those studies employed a remember/know methodology, only the simplest measures of sensitivity could be computed. However, the SDT- 6 To compute simple hit and false alarm rates from confidence ratings, a common method was used in which the response scale (1-6) is bifurcated to represent responses of new (1-3) and old (4-6). 24

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval

BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005,?? 12 (?), (5),???-??? 852-857 BRIEF REPORTS Modes of cognitive control in recognition and source memory: Depth of retrieval LARRY L. JACOBY, YUJIRO SHIMIZU,

More information

Remember-Know: A Matter of Confidence

Remember-Know: A Matter of Confidence Help INDIANA UNIV LIBRARIES Close Window Print E-mail Save Formats: HTML Full Text Citation Title: Remember-Know: A Matter of Confidence April 1, 2004, Vol. 111, Issue 2 Database: PsycARTICLES, By: John

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Strong memories obscure weak memories in associative recognition

Strong memories obscure weak memories in associative recognition Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (6), 1062-1066 Strong memories obscure weak memories in associative recognition MICHAEL F. VERDE and CAREN M. ROTELLO University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Massachusetts

More information

Assessing the influence of recollection and familiarity in memory for own- vs. other-race faces

Assessing the influence of recollection and familiarity in memory for own- vs. other-race faces Iowa State University From the SelectedWorks of Christian A. Meissner, Ph.D. 2009 Assessing the influence of recollection and familiarity in memory for own- vs. other-race faces Jessica L Marcon, University

More information

Recollection Can Be Weak and Familiarity Can Be Strong

Recollection Can Be Weak and Familiarity Can Be Strong Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2012, Vol. 38, No. 2, 325 339 2011 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0025483 Recollection Can Be Weak

More information

Memory strength and the decision process in recognition memory

Memory strength and the decision process in recognition memory Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (2), 254-262 Memory strength and the decision process in recognition memory MICHAEL F. VERDE University of Plymouth, Plymouth, England AND CAREN M. ROTELLO University of Massachusetts,

More information

How cue-dependent is memory?: Internal reinstatement and cueing effects in recognition and source memory

How cue-dependent is memory?: Internal reinstatement and cueing effects in recognition and source memory Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2006 How cue-dependent is memory?: Internal reinstatement and cueing effects in recognition and source memory Jeffrey

More information

The Effects of Unitization on Familiarity-Based Source Memory: Testing a Behavioral Prediction Derived From Neuroimaging Data

The Effects of Unitization on Familiarity-Based Source Memory: Testing a Behavioral Prediction Derived From Neuroimaging Data Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2008, Vol. 34, No. 4, 730 740 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.730

More information

MEASURING CONSCIOUS MEMORY 0

MEASURING CONSCIOUS MEMORY 0 MEASURING CONSCIOUS MEMORY 0 Dawn M. McBride Methods for Measuring Conscious and Automatic Memory A Brief Review Abstract: Memory researchers have discussed the relationship between consciousness and memory

More information

JOHN M. GARDINER University of Sussex, Brighton, England

JOHN M. GARDINER University of Sussex, Brighton, England Memory & Cognition 2006, 34 (2), 227-239 Recognition memory and awareness: Occurrence of perceptual effects in remembering or in knowing depends on conscious resources at encoding, but not at retrieval

More information

Journal of Memory and Language

Journal of Memory and Language Journal of Memory and Language 68 (2013) 333 349 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Journal of Memory and Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jml ethinking familiarity: emember/know

More information

Strength-based mirror effects in item and associative recognition: Evidence for within-list criterion changes

Strength-based mirror effects in item and associative recognition: Evidence for within-list criterion changes Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (4), 679-688 Strength-based mirror effects in item and associative recognition: Evidence for within-list criterion changes WILLIAM E. HOCKLEY Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo,

More information

A dual-process account of the list-length and strength-based mirror effects in recognition q

A dual-process account of the list-length and strength-based mirror effects in recognition q Journal of Memory and Language 49 (2003) 231 248 Journal of Memory and Language www.elsevier.com/locate/jml A dual-process account of the list-length and strength-based mirror effects in recognition q

More information

Shayne Loft, Michael S. Humphreys, & Susannah J. Whitney. The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia

Shayne Loft, Michael S. Humphreys, & Susannah J. Whitney. The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia Memory Access Task Interference 1 Running Head: Memory Access Task Interference Control of Access to Memory: The use of Task Interference as a Behavioral Probe Shayne Loft, Michael S. Humphreys, & Susannah

More information

Response speeding mediates the contributions of cue familiarity and target retrievability to metamnemonic judgments

Response speeding mediates the contributions of cue familiarity and target retrievability to metamnemonic judgments Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 25,?? 12 (?), (5),???-??? 874-879 Response speeding mediates the contributions of cue familiarity and target retrievability to metamnemonic judgments AARON S. BENJAMIN

More information

Continuous Recollection Versus Unitized Familiarity in Associative Recognition

Continuous Recollection Versus Unitized Familiarity in Associative Recognition Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2010, Vol. 36, No. 4, 843 863 2010 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019755 Continuous Recollection

More information

Dissociations of Accuracy and Confidence Reveal the Role of Individual Items. and Distinctiveness in Recognition Memory and Metacognition

Dissociations of Accuracy and Confidence Reveal the Role of Individual Items. and Distinctiveness in Recognition Memory and Metacognition Dissociations of Accuracy and Confidence Reveal the Role of Individual Items and Distinctiveness in Recognition Memory and Metacognition Draft for Caren Rotello only Thomas A. Busey Anne Arici Indiana

More information

Remember/Know Judgments Probe Degrees of Recollection

Remember/Know Judgments Probe Degrees of Recollection Remember/Know Judgments Probe Degrees of Recollection Peter E. Wais, Laura Mickes, and John T. Wixted Abstract & Remembering and knowing are states of awareness that accompany the retrieval of facts, faces,

More information

Dissociating familiarity from recollection using rote rehearsal

Dissociating familiarity from recollection using rote rehearsal Memory & Cognition 2004, 32 (6), 932-944 Dissociating familiarity from recollection using rote rehearsal IAN G. DOBBINS Duke University, Durham, North Carolina and NEAL E. A. KROLL and ANDREW P. YONELINAS

More information

Distinctiveness and the Recognition Mirror Effect: Evidence for an Item-Based Criterion Placement Heuristic

Distinctiveness and the Recognition Mirror Effect: Evidence for an Item-Based Criterion Placement Heuristic Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2005, Vol. 31, No. 6, 1186 1198 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1186

More information

Feedback as a Source of Criterion Noise in Recognition Memory

Feedback as a Source of Criterion Noise in Recognition Memory Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2014 Feedback as a Source of Criterion Noise in Recognition Memory Bryan Franks Louisiana State University and Agricultural

More information

Source memory and the picture superiority effect

Source memory and the picture superiority effect Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2007 Source memory and the picture superiority effect Noelle L. Brown Louisiana State University and Agricultural and

More information

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions

Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Categorization and Memory: Representation of Category Information Increases Memory Intrusions Anna V. Fisher (fisher.449@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University

More information

Evidence for a Generate-Recognize Model of Episodic Influences on Word-Stem Completion

Evidence for a Generate-Recognize Model of Episodic Influences on Word-Stem Completion Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2000, Vol. 26, No. 2, 267-293 Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.2.267

More information

Global Subjective Memorability and the Strength-Based Mirror Effect in Recognition Memory. Davide Bruno. University of Southampton. Philip A.

Global Subjective Memorability and the Strength-Based Mirror Effect in Recognition Memory. Davide Bruno. University of Southampton. Philip A. The mirror effect and subjective memorability (C481) 1 Running Head: THE MIRROR EFFECT AND SUBJECTIVE MEMORABILITY Global Subjective Memorability and the Strength-Based Mirror Effect in Recognition Memory

More information

PART 1. Have I SEEN that before? Theories of recognition memory. Structure of talk. Memory strength theory. Signal detection theory.

PART 1. Have I SEEN that before? Theories of recognition memory. Structure of talk. Memory strength theory. Signal detection theory. Have I SEEN that before? Theories of recognition memory Structure of talk Memory strength theory Prof Tim Perfect Challenges to memory strength theory Dual-process theories RK theory Attributional theories

More information

Verde & Rotello 1. Running Head: FAMILIARITY AND THE REVELATION EFFECT. Does familiarity change in the revelation effect?

Verde & Rotello 1. Running Head: FAMILIARITY AND THE REVELATION EFFECT. Does familiarity change in the revelation effect? Verde & Rotello 1 Running Head: FAMILIARITY AND THE REVELATION EFFECT Does familiarity change in the revelation effect? Michael F. Verde and Caren M. Rotello University of Massachusetts - Amherst JEP:LMC,

More information

Measuring Recollection and Familiarity in the Medial Temporal Lobe

Measuring Recollection and Familiarity in the Medial Temporal Lobe Measuring Recollection and Familiarity in the Medial Temporal Lobe John T. Wixted, 1 * Laura Mickes, 1 and Larry R. Squire 1,2,3,4 HIPPOCAMPUS 20:1195 1205 (2010) ABSTRACT: Many recent studies have investigated

More information

Strong Memories Are Hard to Scale

Strong Memories Are Hard to Scale Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 211 American Psychological Association 211, Vol. 14, No. 2, 239 257 96-3445/11/$12. DOI: 1.137/a237 Strong Memories Are Hard to Scale Laura Mickes, Vivian Hwe,

More information

RUNNING HEAD: RECOGNITION MEMORY

RUNNING HEAD: RECOGNITION MEMORY RUNNING HEAD: RECOGNITION MEMORY Recognition Memory: A Review of the Critical Findings and an Integrated Proposal for Relating Them Kenneth J. Malmberg University of South Florida Send Correspondence to:

More information

Does Familiarity Change in the Revelation Effect?

Does Familiarity Change in the Revelation Effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2003, Vol. 29, No. 5, 739 746 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.5.739

More information

Simultaneous Versus Sequential Presentation in Testing Recognition Memory for Faces Made available courtesy of University of Illinois Press:

Simultaneous Versus Sequential Presentation in Testing Recognition Memory for Faces Made available courtesy of University of Illinois Press: Simultaneous Versus Sequential Presentation in Testing Recognition Memory for Faces By: Jason R. Finley, Henry L. Roediger III, Andrea D. Hughes, Christopher N. Wahlheim, Larry L. Jacoby Finley, J. R.,

More information

Evaluating models of remember know judgments: Complexity, mimicry, and discriminability

Evaluating models of remember know judgments: Complexity, mimicry, and discriminability Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 28, 15 (5), 96-926 doi: 1.3758/PBR.15.5.96 Evaluating models of remember know judgments: Complexity, mimicry, and discriminability Andrew L. Cohen, Caren M. Rotello, and Neil

More information

Confidence and Memory Accuracy: Do we know what we think we know? Bennett Brocka. University of Iowa

Confidence and Memory Accuracy: Do we know what we think we know? Bennett Brocka. University of Iowa Running Head: CONFIDENCE AND MEMORY ACCURACY 1 Confidence and Memory Accuracy: Do we know what we think we know? Bennett Brocka University of Iowa Running Head: CONFIDENCE AND MEMORY ACCURACY 2 Abstract

More information

The effects of categorical relatedness on judgements of learning (JOLs)

The effects of categorical relatedness on judgements of learning (JOLs) MEMORY, 2006, 14 (2), 253±261 The effects of categorical relatedness on judgements of learning (JOLs) Greg Matvey and John Dunlosky The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, USA Bennett L. Schwartz

More information

Reversing the picture superiority effect: A speed accuracy trade-off study of recognition memory

Reversing the picture superiority effect: A speed accuracy trade-off study of recognition memory Memory & Cognition 007, 35(1), 113-13 Reversing the picture superiority effect: A speed accuracy trade-off study of recognition memory ANGELA BOLDINI, RICCARDO RUSSO, SAHIBA PUNIA, AND S. E. AVONS University

More information

Suppressing False Recognition in Younger and Older Adults: The Distinctiveness Heuristic

Suppressing False Recognition in Younger and Older Adults: The Distinctiveness Heuristic Journal of Memory and Language 40, 1 24 (1999) Article ID jmla.1998.2611, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Suppressing False Recognition in Younger and Older Adults: The Distinctiveness

More information

Recognition without face identification

Recognition without face identification Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (7), 1610-1619 Recognition without face identification ANNE M. CLEARY Colorado State University, Fort Collins, Colorado AND LAURA E. SPECKER Williams College, Williamstown,

More information

Probabilistic Retroactive Interference: The Role of Accessibility Bias in Interference Effects

Probabilistic Retroactive Interference: The Role of Accessibility Bias in Interference Effects Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 136, No. 2, 200 216 0096-3445/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.200 Probabilistic Retroactive

More information

University of Illinois Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The American Journal of Psychology.

University of Illinois Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The American Journal of Psychology. Simultaneous Versus Sequential Presentation in Testing Recognition Memory for Faces Author(s): Jason R. Finley, Henry L. Roediger III, Andrea D. Hughes, Christopher N. Wahlheim and Larry L. Jacoby Source:

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Elaborative Retrieval: Do Semantic Mediators Improve Memory? Melissa Lehman and Jeffrey D. Karpicke Online First Publication, March 4,

More information

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory 1 Memory II October 2, 2008 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome Impaired new learning (anterograde amnesia), exacerbated by increasing retention delay Impaired recollection of events learned prior

More information

Retrieval-Induced Forgetting in Item Recognition: Evidence for a Reduction in General Memory Strength

Retrieval-Induced Forgetting in Item Recognition: Evidence for a Reduction in General Memory Strength Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 5, 863 875 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.863

More information

The contribution of recollection and familiarity to yes±no and forced-choice recognition tests in healthy subjects and amnesics

The contribution of recollection and familiarity to yes±no and forced-choice recognition tests in healthy subjects and amnesics Neuropsychologia 38 (2000) 1333±1341 www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia The contribution of recollection and familiarity to yes±no and forced-choice recognition tests in healthy subjects and amnesics

More information

Attributions of Familiarity in Amnesia: Evidence From a Fame Judgment Task

Attributions of Familiarity in Amnesia: Evidence From a Fame Judgment Task Neuropsychology 1993, Vol. 7, No. 4, 510-518 In (he public domain Attributions of Familiarity in Amnesia: Evidence From a Fame Judgment Task Laird S. Cermak, Mieke Verfaellie, Tracy Butler, and Larry L.

More information

A Comparison of Forgetting for Conscious and Automatic Memory Processes in Word Fragment Completion Tasks

A Comparison of Forgetting for Conscious and Automatic Memory Processes in Word Fragment Completion Tasks Journal of Memory and Language 45, 585 615 (2001) doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2792, available online at http://www.academicpress.com on A Comparison of Forgetting for Conscious and Automatic Memory Processes

More information

Journal of Memory and Language

Journal of Memory and Language Journal of Memory and Language 63 (2010) 149 157 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Memory and Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jml Testing promotes eyewitness accuracy

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Modeling Experimentally Induced Strategy Shifts Scott Brown, 1 Mark Steyvers, 2 and Pernille Hemmer 2

Modeling Experimentally Induced Strategy Shifts Scott Brown, 1 Mark Steyvers, 2 and Pernille Hemmer 2 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Modeling Experimentally Induced Strategy Shifts Scott Brown, 1 Mark Steyvers, 2 and Pernille Hemmer 2 1 School of Psychology, University of Newcastle, Callaghan, New

More information

Sources of Bias in the Goodman Kruskal Gamma Coefficient Measure of Association: Implications for Studies of Metacognitive Processes

Sources of Bias in the Goodman Kruskal Gamma Coefficient Measure of Association: Implications for Studies of Metacognitive Processes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2009, Vol. 35, No. 2, 509 527 2009 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014876 Sources of Bias in the

More information

A multidimensional analysis of learning and forgetting in recognition memory. Michael Diaz and Aaron S. Benjamin

A multidimensional analysis of learning and forgetting in recognition memory. Michael Diaz and Aaron S. Benjamin Manuscript A multidimensional analysis of learning and forgetting in recognition memory Michael Diaz and Aaron S. Benjamin University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign Corresponding author: Michael Diaz Department

More information

Decision Rules for Recognition Memory Confidence Judgments

Decision Rules for Recognition Memory Confidence Judgments Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1998, Vol. 24, No. 6, 1397-1410 Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/98/S3.00 Decision Rules for

More information

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing This document contains 4 examples of writing and feedback comments from Level 2A lab reports, and 4 steps to help you apply

More information

Recollection and Familiarity: Examining Controversial Assumptions and New Directions

Recollection and Familiarity: Examining Controversial Assumptions and New Directions HIPPOCAMPUS 20:1178 1194 (2010) Recollection and Familiarity: Examining Controversial Assumptions and New Directions Andrew P. Yonelinas,* Mariam Aly, Wei-Chun Wang, and Joshua D. Koen ABSTRACT: It is

More information

Distinguishing between attributional and mnemonic sources of familiarity: The case of positive emotion bias

Distinguishing between attributional and mnemonic sources of familiarity: The case of positive emotion bias Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (2), 142-153 doi:10.3758/mc.38.2.142 Distinguishing between attributional and mnemonic sources of familiarity: The case of positive emotion bias MICHAEL F. VERDE, LAURA K. STONE,

More information

Testing Promotes Eyewitness Accuracy with a Warning Implications for Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility

Testing Promotes Eyewitness Accuracy with a Warning Implications for Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility Psychology Publications Psychology 8-2010 Testing Promotes Eyewitness Accuracy with a Warning Implications for Retrieval Enhanced Suggestibility Ayanna K. Thomas Tufts University John B. Bulevich Rhode

More information

UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY. Effects of Levels-of-processing and Test-list Context on Recognition and Pupil Dilation. Alexander Taikh A THESIS

UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY. Effects of Levels-of-processing and Test-list Context on Recognition and Pupil Dilation. Alexander Taikh A THESIS UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY Effects of Levels-of-processing and Test-list Context on Recognition and Pupil Dilation by Alexander Taikh A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT

More information

Are awareness questionnaires valid? Investigating the use of posttest questionnaires for assessing awareness in implicit memory tests

Are awareness questionnaires valid? Investigating the use of posttest questionnaires for assessing awareness in implicit memory tests Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (1), 53-64 doi: 10.3758/MC.36.1.53 Are awareness questionnaires valid? Investigating the use of posttest questionnaires for assessing awareness in implicit memory tests Terrence

More information

Separating Habit and Recollection: Memory Slips, Process Dissociations, and Probability Matching

Separating Habit and Recollection: Memory Slips, Process Dissociations, and Probability Matching Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1996, Vol. 22, No. 6,1323-1335 Copyright 19% by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/96/$3.00 Separating Habit and

More information

Recognition Memory, Familiarity, and Déjà vu Experiences Anne M. Cleary

Recognition Memory, Familiarity, and Déjà vu Experiences Anne M. Cleary CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Recognition Memory, Familiarity, and Déjà vu Experiences Anne M. Cleary Colorado State University ABSTRACT Déjà vu occurs when one feels as though a situation

More information

On the Role of Individual Items in Recognition Memory and Metacognition: Challenges for Signal Detection Theory

On the Role of Individual Items in Recognition Memory and Metacognition: Challenges for Signal Detection Theory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2009, Vol. 35, No. 5, 1123 1136 In the public domain DOI: 10.1037/a0016646 On the Role of Individual Items in Recognition Memory and

More information

The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research

The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research Journal of Memory and Language 46, 441 517 (2002) doi:10.1006/jmla.2002.2864, available online at http://www.academicpress.com on The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research

More information

A CONTINUOUS DUAL-PROCESS ACCUMULATION MODEL OF RECOGNITION JUDGMENTS NEHA SINHA

A CONTINUOUS DUAL-PROCESS ACCUMULATION MODEL OF RECOGNITION JUDGMENTS NEHA SINHA A CONTINUOUS DUAL-PROCESS ACCUMULATION MODEL OF RECOGNITION JUDGMENTS By NEHA SINHA A dissertation submitted to the Graduate School-New Brunswick Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey In partial

More information

Are remember And know The Same Process? a Perspective From Reaction Time Data

Are remember And know The Same Process? a Perspective From Reaction Time Data University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 January 2007 Are remember And know The Same Process? a Perspective From Reaction Time Data Min Zeng University

More information

Repetition Priming in Speeded Word Reading: Contributions of Perceptual and Conceptual Processing Episodes

Repetition Priming in Speeded Word Reading: Contributions of Perceptual and Conceptual Processing Episodes Journal of Memory and Language 42, 208 228 (2000) doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2674, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Repetition Priming in Speeded Word Reading: Contributions of Perceptual and

More information

Are There Two Kinds of Reasoning?

Are There Two Kinds of Reasoning? Are There Two Kinds of Reasoning? Evan Heit (E.Heit@warwick.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, University of Warwick Coventry CV4 7AL, UK Caren M. Rotello (caren@psych.umass.edu) Department of Psychology,

More information

Comparing Direct (Explicit) and Indirect (Implicit) Measures to Study Unconscious Memory

Comparing Direct (Explicit) and Indirect (Implicit) Measures to Study Unconscious Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association~ Inc. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/91/$3.00 1991, Vol. 17, No. 2, 224-233 Comparing Direct (Explicit)

More information

RECOGNITION MEMORY RESPONSE BIAS IS CONSERVATIVE FOR PAINTINGS AND WE DON T KNOW WHY

RECOGNITION MEMORY RESPONSE BIAS IS CONSERVATIVE FOR PAINTINGS AND WE DON T KNOW WHY 4 RECOGNITION MEMORY RESPONSE BIAS IS CONSERVATIVE FOR PAINTINGS AND WE DON T KNOW WHY D. Stephen Lindsay, Justin Kantner, and Kaitlyn M. Fallow Foreword by Steve Lindsay I became a Jacobean under the

More information

The effects of a levels-of-processing manipulation on false recall

The effects of a levels-of-processing manipulation on false recall Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2000, 7 (1), 158-162 The effects of a levels-of-processing manipulation on false recall MATTHEW G. RHODES and JEFFREY S. ANASTASI Francis Marion University, Florence, South

More information

Importance of Deficits

Importance of Deficits Importance of Deficits In complex systems the parts are often so integrated that they cannot be detected in normal operation Need to break the system to discover the components not just physical components

More information

The Effects of Individual Differences in Imagery Ability and Working Memory Capacity on False Memories for Imagined Actions

The Effects of Individual Differences in Imagery Ability and Working Memory Capacity on False Memories for Imagined Actions Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Psychology Theses Department of Psychology 8-11-2015 The Effects of Individual Differences in Imagery Ability and Working Memory Capacity

More information

Modeling Source Memory Decision Bounds

Modeling Source Memory Decision Bounds University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 2010 Modeling Source Memory Decision Bounds Angela M. Pazzaglia University of Massachusetts Amherst Follow

More information

Misled Subjects May Know More Than Their Performance Implies

Misled Subjects May Know More Than Their Performance Implies Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1989, Vol. 15, No. 2, 246-255 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. O278-7393/89/S0O.75 Subjects May Know More

More information

Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: Evidence from ROC curves

Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: Evidence from ROC curves Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: Evidence from ROC curves Andrew Heathcote 1, Frances Raymond 1 and John Dunn 2 1 School of Psychology, Aviation Building, The University of Newcastle,

More information

On the Difference Between Strength-Based and Frequency-Based Mirror Effects in Recognition Memory

On the Difference Between Strength-Based and Frequency-Based Mirror Effects in Recognition Memory Jourllal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1998, Vol. 24, No. 6, 1379-1396 Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/98/$3.00 On the Difference

More information

The effects of free recall testing on subsequent source memory

The effects of free recall testing on subsequent source memory MEMORY, 2010, 18 (4), 385393 The effects of free recall testing on subsequent source memory Gene A. Brewer, Richard L. Marsh, Joseph T. Meeks, and Arlo Clark-Foos University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA

More information

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions Sarah J. Miles (smiles25@uwo.ca) and John Paul Minda (jpminda@uwo.ca) Department of Psychology The University of Western Ontario London, ON N6A 5C2 Abstract

More information

A Positive Generation Effect on Memory for Auditory Context

A Positive Generation Effect on Memory for Auditory Context DOI 10.3758/s13423-016-1169-4 BRIEF REPORT A Positive Generation Effect on Memory for Auditory Context Amy A. Overman 1 & Alison G. Richard 1 & Joseph D. W. Stephens 2 # Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2016

More information

THE EFFECT OF A REMINDER STIMULUS ON THE DECISION STRATEGY ADOPTED IN THE TWO-ALTERNATIVE FORCED-CHOICE PROCEDURE.

THE EFFECT OF A REMINDER STIMULUS ON THE DECISION STRATEGY ADOPTED IN THE TWO-ALTERNATIVE FORCED-CHOICE PROCEDURE. THE EFFECT OF A REMINDER STIMULUS ON THE DECISION STRATEGY ADOPTED IN THE TWO-ALTERNATIVE FORCED-CHOICE PROCEDURE. Michael J. Hautus, Daniel Shepherd, Mei Peng, Rebecca Philips and Veema Lodhia Department

More information

Recollection Is a Continuous Process Implications for Dual-Process Theories of Recognition Memory

Recollection Is a Continuous Process Implications for Dual-Process Theories of Recognition Memory PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Recollection Is a Continuous Process Implications for Dual-Process Theories of Recognition Memory Laura Mickes, Peter E. Wais, and John T. Wixted University of California,

More information

Relations Between Memory and Reasoning

Relations Between Memory and Reasoning CHAPTER TWO Relations Between Memory and Reasoning Evan Heit, Caren M. Rotello and Brett K. Hayes Contents 1. Introduction 58 2. Applying Memory Methods to Reasoning 60 2.1. Argument Length Effects on

More information

C242 Comparing Decay Rates 1. Running head: COMPARING DECAY RATES FOR FALSE MEMORIES. Comparing Decay Rates for Accurate and False Memories

C242 Comparing Decay Rates 1. Running head: COMPARING DECAY RATES FOR FALSE MEMORIES. Comparing Decay Rates for Accurate and False Memories Comparing Decay Rates 1 Running head: COMPARING DECAY RATES FOR FALSE MEMORIES Comparing Decay Rates for Accurate and False Memories in the DRM Paradigm Jorie M. Colbert 1 and Dawn M. McBride 2 1 University

More information

On the relationship between recognition familiarity and perceptual uency: Evidence for distinct mnemonic processes

On the relationship between recognition familiarity and perceptual uency: Evidence for distinct mnemonic processes Acta Psychologica 98 (1998) 211±230 On the relationship between recognition familiarity and perceptual uency: Evidence for distinct mnemonic processes Anthony D. Wagner *, John D.E. Gabrieli Department

More information

An event-related potential study of the revelation effect

An event-related potential study of the revelation effect Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 926-931 An event-related potential study of the revelation effect NAZANIN AZIMIAN-FARIDANI and EDWARD L. WILDING Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Event-related

More information

Implicit and Explicit Memory for Novel Visual Objects: Structure and Function

Implicit and Explicit Memory for Novel Visual Objects: Structure and Function Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, emory, and Cognition 1993, Vol. 19, No. 5, 995-1009 CoDYristht 1993 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/93/S3.00 Implicit and Explicit

More information

Eyewitness decisions in simultaneous and sequential lineups: A dual-process signal detection theory analysis

Eyewitness decisions in simultaneous and sequential lineups: A dual-process signal detection theory analysis Memory & Cognition 2005, 33 (5), 783-792 Eyewitness decisions in simultaneous and sequential lineups: A dual-process signal detection theory analysis CHRISTIAN A. MEISSNER Florida International University,

More information

The role of test structure in creating false memories

The role of test structure in creating false memories Journal Memory & Cognition 2006,?? 34 (?), (5),???-??? 1026-1036 The role of test structure in creating false memories JENNIFER H. COANE and DAWN M. MCBRIDE Illinois State University, Normal, Illinois

More information

Thompson, Valerie A, Ackerman, Rakefet, Sidi, Yael, Ball, Linden, Pennycook, Gordon and Prowse Turner, Jamie A

Thompson, Valerie A, Ackerman, Rakefet, Sidi, Yael, Ball, Linden, Pennycook, Gordon and Prowse Turner, Jamie A Article The role of answer fluency and perceptual fluency in the monitoring and control of reasoning: Reply to Alter, Oppenheimer, and Epley Thompson, Valerie A, Ackerman, Rakefet, Sidi, Yael, Ball, Linden,

More information

Neuropsychologia ] (]]]]) ]]] ]]] Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Neuropsychologia ] (]]]]) ]]] ]]] Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia Neuropsychologia ] (]]]]) ]]] ]]] Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Item memory, context memory and the hippocampus:

More information

Inducing and reducing false memories: A Swedish version of the Deese Roediger McDermott paradigm

Inducing and reducing false memories: A Swedish version of the Deese Roediger McDermott paradigm Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2002, 43, 369 383 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Inducing and reducing false memories: A Swedish version of the Deese Roediger McDermott paradigm MIKAEL JOHANSSON 1 and GEORG

More information

Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment Hypothesis: Comment on Belli and Reply to Tversky and Tuchin

Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment Hypothesis: Comment on Belli and Reply to Tversky and Tuchin Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1989, Vol. 118, No. 1,92-99 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Im 0096-3445/89/S00.7 Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment

More information

Can corrective feedback improve recognition memory?

Can corrective feedback improve recognition memory? Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (4), 389-406 doi:10.3758/mc.38.4.389 Can corrective feedback improve recognition memory? JUSTIN KANTNER AND D. STEPHEN LINDSAY University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia,

More information

When Words Collide: Facilitation and Interference in the Report of Repeated Words From Rapidly Presented Lists

When Words Collide: Facilitation and Interference in the Report of Repeated Words From Rapidly Presented Lists Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2004, Vol. 30, No. 6, 1279 1289 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.6.1279

More information

A Simplified Conjoint Recognition Paradigm for the Measurement of Gist and Verbatim Memory

A Simplified Conjoint Recognition Paradigm for the Measurement of Gist and Verbatim Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2008, Vol. 34, No. 3, 570 586 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.570

More information

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias Lewis O. Harvey, Jr. Department of Psychology University of Colorado Boulder, Colorado The Brain (Observable) Stimulus System (Observable) Response System

More information

Supplemental Information. Varying Target Prevalence Reveals. Two Dissociable Decision Criteria. in Visual Search

Supplemental Information. Varying Target Prevalence Reveals. Two Dissociable Decision Criteria. in Visual Search Current Biology, Volume 20 Supplemental Information Varying Target Prevalence Reveals Two Dissociable Decision Criteria in Visual Search Jeremy M. Wolfe and Michael J. Van Wert Figure S1. Figure S1. Modeling

More information

Metacognition and False Recognition in Alzheimer s Disease: Further Exploration of the Distinctiveness Heuristic

Metacognition and False Recognition in Alzheimer s Disease: Further Exploration of the Distinctiveness Heuristic Neuropsychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 19, No. 2, 253 258 0894-4105/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.2.253 Metacognition and False Recognition in Alzheimer

More information

Source Memory in the Absence of Successful Cued Recall

Source Memory in the Absence of Successful Cued Recall Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 4, 828 835 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.4.828

More information

Perceptual Imagination and Perceptual Memory

Perceptual Imagination and Perceptual Memory 1 Perceptual Imagination and Perceptual Memory An Overview Fiona Macpherson The essays in this volume explore the nature of perceptual imagination and perceptual memory. How do perceptual imagination and

More information