Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach"

Transcription

1 PQJE TECHSET COMPOSITION LTD, SALISBURY, U.K. 6/16/2006 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 0000, 00 (0), 1 17 Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach Maria de Lurdes Delgado and Sylvie Droit-Volet Blaise Pascal University, Clermont-Ferrand, France This study examined the effect of the variability of representation of durations in reference memory on temporal discrimination performance in children aged 5 and 8 years as well as in adults using a bisection (Experiment 1) and a generalization task (Experiment 2). In each task, the participants were familiarized before the blocks of tested trials with either the same referent duration values (fixed condition) or a distribution of referent duration values, with a mean equal to the referent durations used in the fixed condition and a.20 coefficient of variation (variable condition). The results showed that the sensitivity to duration was lower in the variable than in the fixed condition in the children and, to a lesser extent, in the adults. The modelling of the data indicated that this effect was due to the increase in the variability of the representation of durations in reference memory, but also to changes in the decisional processes. The memory of past event durations plays an important role in the judgement of current event durations. Comparing the presented duration with those stored in reference memory allows us to judge whether the former is similar, shorter, or longer than usual. The importance of the role of memory representations of duration in temporal judgement was suggested in scalar timing theory (Gibbon, 1977) and its associated information-processing models (Church, 1984; Gibbon & Church, 1984; Gibbon, Church, & Meck, 1984), which were first proposed for animal timing and have since been successfully applied to timing in human adults (e.g., Allan & Gibbon, 1991; Wearden, 1991, 1992). According to this theory, the raw material for time judgement comes from a pacemaker accumulator clock mechanism. More precisely, the subjective duration depends on the number of pulses emitted by a pacemaker and accumulated in a counter during the presented stimulus. However, this theory assumes that the final temporal judgement is governed by the comparison of the presented duration with the representation of important durations stored in reference memory, such as those associated with reinforcement in animal studies or identified as referent durations in human studies. Correspondence should be addressed to Sylvie Droit-Volet, Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale et Cognitive, CNRS (UMR 6024), Université Blaise Pascal, 34 avenue Carnot, Clermont-Ferrand Cedex, France. droit@srvpsy.univ-bpclermont.fr # 0000 The Experimental Psychology Society 1 DOI: /

2 DE LURDES DELGADO AND DROIT-VOLET Some principles of scalar timing theory have been tested in human adults with different temporal discrimination tasks (for a review see Meck, 2003). However, as stated by Allan (2002), the bisection task has become the preferred task for the study of reference memory in humans because it maximizes the dependence of temporal judgements on the memory of referent durations. In the prototypical version of the bisection task, the participants are familiarized with a short (S) and a long (L) referent duration either at the beginning (e.g., Lustig & Meck, 2001; Wearden & Ferrara, 1995, 1996) or at intervals throughout the blocks of test trials (e.g., Allan & Gibbon, 1991; Penney, Allan, Meck, & Gibbon, 1998). They are then presented with comparison durations that are equal to the referent durations or of intermediate value and are required to judge whether these durations are more similar to the short or to the long reference duration. In humans, as of the age of 3 years, the temporal bisection task yields psychophysical functions that are ogival in form, with the proportion of long responses (t identified as more similar to L than to S) increasing with the value of t. However, the slope of these bisection functions appears to be flatter in younger children, 8 years old being the age at which the curve slope approaches that found in adults (e.g., Droit- Q1 Volet & Clément, in press; Droit-Volet, Tourret, & Wearden, 2004; Droit-Volet & Wearden, 2001, 2002; McCormack, Brown, Maylor, Darby, & Green, 1999). The scalar-timing-based models used to simulate data in the bisection task have provided an excellent description of the temporal behaviour obtained in human adults as well as in animals (e.g., Allan & Gibbon, 1991; Gibbon, 1981; Penney, Gibbon, & Meck, 2000; Wearden, 1991). In these models, the referent durations are stored in memory in the form of Gaussian distributions, rather than single values, with means equal to the value of the referent duration and some coefficient of variation, c. On any test trial, the duration t is thus compared with samples taken from this memory representation, S Q2 and L. Sampling from these distributions therefore produces trial-by-trial variance. In general, the greater the coefficient of variation of these memory distributions, the flatter the psychophysical function will be. The variability of referent memory is thus the main sensitivity parameter controlling the slope of the psychophysical functions. In other words, the temporal reference memory is the main source of variability in temporal bisection judgement. Using the scalar timing models of bisection, Droit-Volet and Wearden (2001) thus found that the young children s flatter bisection functions are due to a greater variability in the representation of the referent durations in memory. Recently, the validity of the memory representation of referent durations as a major source of variance in temporal behaviour has been questioned. Rodriguez-Girones and Kacelnik (1995, 1998, 2001) used a variant of the bisection task (roving referent bisection) in which no temporal reference memory could be formed and called on for temporal judgement. The participants had to indicate whether a third duration (test duration) was more similar to one or the other of the two previously presented referent durations. The referent durations were varied from trial to trial in order to prevent subjects from using referent memory. In this new task, bisection took place as in the prototypical version. In line with this finding, Wearden and Ferrara (1995, 1996) obtained similar psychophysical functions when the participants either were or were not (partition bisection) familiarized with referent durations. More recently, McCormack, Wearden, Smith, and Brock Q3 (2005), using a generalization task, and Droit- Volet and Rattat (2006), using a bisection task, Q4 decided to eliminate the reference memory, and they found that the age-related differences in temporal judgements did not disappear, with the psychophysical functions again being flatter in the younger children. Given this body of results, the authors concluded that the comparison of the presented duration with the referents is not necessary for temporal bisection to occur. Since the reference memory was unavailable in these bisection conditions, the dominant source of variability in temporal judgement, and its age-related changes, 2 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)

3 REPRESENTATION OF TIME IN REFERENCE MEMORY would appear not to lie in the reference memory, but rather in the perceptual processes (McCormack et al., 1995, p. 3). In short, temporal bisection can occur without referent durations in memory. However, this does not exclude the possibility that when the referent durations are explicitly identified, and a direct comparison with the stimulus duration is required, the content of the referent memory could, to some extent, affect the slope of the psychophysical functions. However, the exact nature of the content of the reference memory is not clear in the scalar timing models and has been the object of little research to date (Jones & Wearden, 2003). More specifically, in these models, the variability of the memory representation of referent durations results not from noise in perceived time, the presented duration being processed accurately, but from noise in the memory storage process itself (Meck, 1983, 1996; Meck & Angell, 1992). The duration in the accumulator is multiplied by a memory storage constant k, which varies as a Gaussian distribution with a mean of 1.0 and a coefficient of variation. It is this resulting value that is entered in the memory distribution (for a review see Church, 1997; Rattat & Droit-Volet, 2005b). In animal studies, Meck (1983) has demonstrated that we can alter this storage component through the administration of drugs without altering the other components of temporal information processing. Nevertheless, the question is: How is this noise in temporal reference memory produced? We assume that the variability in the memory representation of referent durations does not result solely from variance in the memory storage process itself as suggested in the scalar timing theories, but also from noise prior to memorization that is, in the encoding of time. Indeed, the content of the reference memory comes from what was initially encoded that is, the evaluation of each presented instance of the referent duration. In bisection, the variability in the reference memory would be relatively low in adults, because the quantity of errors in their encoding of each duration would be reduced. In contrast, imprecision in the evaluation of each presented instance of the referent duration would be greater in young children, thus producing a higher level of variability in the temporal representation in memory. This would affect their bisection judgements by flattening the bisection curve. The aim of the present study was thus to examine the effect of noise in the memory representation of the referent durations on temporal performance in a bisection task involving children aged 5 and 8 years old and adults by directly introducing variance in the presentation examples of the short and the long referent duration. In one condition (fixed referent memory), the participants were thus presented with the same short or long referent durations. In the other (varied referent memory), they were presented with a set of values around the short and the long referent duration. These values were chosen such that the coefficient of variation of their distributions was higher in the varied than in the fixed condition, and their means were equal to the referent durations used in this fixed condition. We assumed that if each perceived example of the referent durations contributes to the memory representation of the referent durations in the form of distributions with means and some coefficient of variation, the psychophysical function slope would be flatter in the varied than in the fixed condition, thus indicating a lower sensitivity to time. Furthermore, the fixed varied memory difference would be greater in the younger children. The modelling of the data with a scalar timing model should indicate that this lower temporal sensitivity in the varied condition is due to a greater coefficient of variation of the representation of durations in reference memory. EXPERIMENT 1 Method Participants The sample consisted of 80 participants: 24 five-year-olds (13 females and 11 males, mean age ¼ 4.8 years, SD ¼ 0.41); 28 eight-year-olds (14 females and 14 males, mean age ¼ 7.9 years, SD ¼ 0.35); and 28 adults (21 females and THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 3

4 DE LURDES DELGADO AND DROIT-VOLET 7 males, mean age ¼ years, SD ¼ 1.39). The children came from nursery and primary schools, and the adults were first-year psychology students from Blaise Pascal University, in Auvergne, France. Materials The participants were tested individually in a quiet room in their school (in the case of the children) or in the university psychology department (adults). The experiment was run on a PowerMacintosh computer, which controlled stimulus presentation and recorded the data via PsyScope. The stimulus, whose duration was varied, was a 4.5-cm diameter blue circle presented in the centre of the computer screen located at about cm and cm from the children s and the adults eyes, respectively. The participants responded to the stimulus by pressing the K or the D key on the computer keyboard. Procedure In each age group, half the participants were randomly assigned to the fixed referent memory condition and the other half to the variable referent memory condition. The procedure was similar in these two conditions, except for the value of the duration presented on each trial during the familiarization phase with the referent durations, and was given before each test block. In this familiarization phase, the participants were alternately presented with the short and the long referent five times each. In the fixed condition, the stimulus duration was always 1 s for the short and 7 s for the long referent duration. In the variable condition, the stimulus duration was randomly chosen on each trial from the five duration values 0.750, 0.875, 1.000, 1.125, and s for the short and the five duration values 5.250, 6.250, 7.000, 7.750, and s for the long referent, with a mean equal to the referent value used in the fixed condition and a coefficient of variation of.20. In the fixed as well as in the variable condition, the experimenter said on each trial: Look, it s the short/long circle. It stays on for a short/long time. Each familiarization phase was immediately followed by a test block. There were four test blocks of 21 trials, 3 trials for each of the seven comparison durations: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7 s. The total number of trials was thus 84 trials. The trials were presented in a random order within each block. The intertrial interval was also randomly chosen between 500 ms and 2 s. In the test blocks, the participant pressed one key ( D ) after a comparison duration judged to be similar to the short referent duration and the other key ( K ) after one judged similar to the long referent, the key press order being counterbalanced. The participants were instructed not to count and were required to repeat aloud blablabla... as fast as possible in order to preclude vocal and subvocal counting (Gallistel & Gelman, 2000). The experimenter monitored the continuity of this oral activity. Results Data analysis Figure 1 shows the mean proportion of long responses plotted against comparison durations in the fixed and the variable referent condition. The upper panel shows data from the 5-year-olds, the centre panel data from the 8-year-olds, and the lower panel data from the adults. An examination of Figure 1 suggests that the psychophysical functions were flatter in the variable than in the fixed condition, but to a greater extent in the younger children. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) 1 was run on the proportion of long responses with two between-subjects factors (age and referent memory) and one within-subject factor (comparison duration). The ANOVA found a significant effect of comparison duration, F(6, 444) ¼ , p ¼.0001, and a marginally significant effect of age, F(2, 74) ¼ 2.90, p ¼.06. The main effect of referent memory was not significant, F(1, 74) ¼ 0.45, p ¼.51. However, there was a Q5 1 Previous analyses revealed neither a significant main effect nor any interaction involving the sex and button order factors. Thus, these factors were not included in the statistical analyses. 4 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)

5 REPRESENTATION OF TIME IN REFERENCE MEMORY Q6 Figure 1. Mean proportion of long responses plotted against stimulus duration for the fixed and the variable referent memory condition. The upper panel shows data from the 5-year-olds, the centre panel from the 8-year-olds, and lower panel data from the adults. significant Age Comparison duration interaction, F(12, 444) ¼ 6.85, p ¼.0001, indicating that the steepness of the bisection functions increased with age. Furthermore, the Referent Memory Comparison Duration interaction, F(6, 444) ¼ 2.30, p ¼.03, and the Referent Memory Age Comparison Duration interaction, F(12, 444) ¼ 2.75, p ¼.001, were significant, with the Referent Memory Age interaction close to significance, F(2, 74) ¼ 2.89, p ¼.06. We therefore decided to run an ANOVA on the proportion of long responses for each age group taken separately. In both the 5 and the 8-year-olds, there was a significant interaction between the referent memory and the comparison duration, F(6, 132) ¼ 3.84, p ¼.001, F(6, 156) ¼ 2.64, p ¼.02, respectively, which subsumed a significant main effect of comparison duration, F(6, 132) ¼ 52.83, F(6, 156) ¼ , all p ¼.0001, with no significant main effect of referent memory, F(1, 22) ¼ 2.85, F(1, 26) ¼ 2.44, all p..05. Unlike in the children, there was only a main effect of comparison duration in the adults, F(6, 156) ¼ , p ¼ In the adults, neither the effect of referent memory, F(1, 26) ¼ 1.10, p ¼.31, nor the Referent Memory Comparison Duration interaction, F(6, 156) ¼ 0.70, p ¼.65, were significant. These results suggest that increasing the variability of the memory representation of the referent durations, while holding the means similar between the two memory conditions, flattened the bisection functions in the children, with the effect being more pronounced in the 5-year-olds than in the 8-year-olds, but not significantly in the adults. In order to further investigate similarities and differences between the psychological functions in the different referent memory conditions, we calculated two new indexes for each participant: a Weber ratio and a bisection point. The Weber ratio is a measure of the steepness of the psychophysical function, which is in turn an index of temporal sensitivity. The higher the Weber ratio value is, the lower the temporal sensitivity. It is the difference limen (half the difference between the comparison duration giving rise to 75 and 25% long responses) divided by the bisection point. The bisection point is the point of subjective equality that is to say, an index of the localization of the bisection criterion, the boundary below which the subjects mostly respond short and above which they mostly respond long. It is thus THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 5

6 DE LURDES DELGADO AND DROIT-VOLET the stimulus duration giving rise to 50% long responses. The Weber ratio and the bisection point were calculated on the basis of the slope and intercept obtained from the linear regression performed on the steepest part of the psychophysical function for each individual (for the method used, see Church & Deluty, 1977; Droit-Volet & Wearden, 2001; Wearden, 1991). It was impossible to calculate an individual Weber ratio value for four of the 5-year-olds in the variable condition because their temporal performance was totally impaired by the increase in variability in the presentation of the referent durations. We therefore decided to exclude these participants from the subsequent statistical analyses of the Weber ratio and the bisection point (Table 1). The overall two-way ANOVA run on the individual Weber ratios revealed a main effect of age, F(2, 70) ¼ 33.11, p ¼ However, there was also a significant effect of referent memory, F(1, 70) ¼ 15.71, p ¼.0001, as well as a significant interaction between age and referent memory, F(2, 70) ¼ 3.61, p ¼.03. The one-way ANOVA carried out on the Weber ratio in the fixed and the variable conditions taken separately showed a significant effect of age in each condition, F(2, 37) ¼ 20.58, F(2, 35) ¼ 15.75, respectively, all p ¼ As indicated by the Sheffe post hoc tests, in both the fixed and the variable referent memory condition, the Weber ratio was higher in the 5-year-olds than in either the 8-year-olds or the adults (all p..01), whereas it was similar in these two older age groups (p..05). This is entirely consistent with Table 1. Weber ratio and bisection point in the fixed and the variable referent memory condition for the three age groups Weber ratio Bisection point Mean SD Mean SD 5-year-olds Fixed Variable year-olds Fixed Variable Adults Fixed Variable the data found in previous studies using bisection tasks, which have revealed an improvement in time sensitivity between 5 and 8 years of age (Droit-Volet, 2003; Droit-Volet et al., 2004; Droit-Volet & Wearden, 2001; McCormack et al., 1999). In addition, the statistical analysis revealed that the Weber ratio value was higher in the variable than in the fixed referent condition for both the 5- and the 8-year-olds, t(18) ¼ 2.70, p ¼.02, t(26) ¼ 2.39, p ¼.02, respectively, but not for the adults, t(26) ¼ 0.76, p ¼.46. In line with the statistical analyses of the proportion of long responses, these data suggest that increasing the variability in the memory representation of the referent durations decreased the children s sensitivity to duration in the bisection task. However, unlike the children, the adults maintained a high sensitivity to duration whatever the referent memory condition. The overall ANOVA run on the individual bisection points also revealed a significant interaction between age and referent memory, F(2, 70) ¼ 4.76, p ¼.01, whereas the main effect of age, F(2, 70) ¼ 2.48, p ¼.09, and of referent memory, F(1, 70) ¼ 0.12, p ¼.74, were not significant. The post hoc statistical analyses indicated that this significant interaction was mainly due to the 5-year-olds who produced a significantly lower bisection point in the variable than in the fixed referent condition, t(18) ¼ 2.10, p ¼.05. This difference between the fixed and the variable condition failed to reach significance in the 8-year-olds, t(26) ¼ 1.93, p ¼.07, and the adults, t(26) ¼ 1.26, p ¼.23, respectively. Thus, for the adults and the 8-year-olds, the bisection point was located closer to the arithmetic mean of the short and the long referent (i.e., 4 s) than to the geometric mean (2.65), and this finding did not change significantly with the increase in the variability of the memory representation of the referent durations. In contrast, in the 5-year-olds, the bisection clearly fell beyond the arithmetic mean of the short and the long referent as found in previous studies (Droit-Volet & Wearden, 2001). However, it moved toward the geometric mean as the variability in the presentation of the reference duration values increased. 6 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)

7 REPRESENTATION OF TIME IN REFERENCE MEMORY Modelling In order to identity the mechanism underlying the effect of our experimental manipulation of reference memory on bisection performance, we decided to use the developmental version of Wearden s modified difference model (1991), which has provided a good fit with bisection data obtained from human adults in a large number of studies. Like most bisection models (see Introduction), this model suggests that the referent durations presented in the familiarization phase (i.e., the short, s, and the long, l ) are stored in reference memory in the form of Gaussian distributions, with means equal to the values of the short and long referent and some coefficient of variation, c. The stimulus duration, t, is thus compared with samples drawn from the reference distributions (i.e., s and l ). Consequently, the higher the coefficient of variation, c, the fuzzier the memory representation of the referent duration is. As illustrated in Figure 2a, increasing the coefficient of variation c, while the other parameters are held constant, flattens the bisection curve but does so to a greater extent for the longer than for the shorter stimulus durations. The coefficient of the remembered time is thus a sensitivity parameter controlling the slope of the psychophysical function. Added to this memory parameter, there are two other parameters: b, a bias toward long responses, and p, a probability of random responses. The bisection model assumes that the participants classify a comparison stimulus, t, as more similar to the short or the long referent by calculating two Q6 Figure 2. Data derived from simulations using the developmental version of the modified difference model (MDM) discussed in the text, when a parameter value is varying while the other parameter are kept constant. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 7

8 DE LURDES DELGADO AND DROIT-VOLET differences: D(s, t) and D(l, t), the absolute difference between t and a sample taken from the memory of the short and the long referent duration, s, l, which differ from trial to trial and are drawn from Gaussian distributions. If the difference between D(s, t) and D(l, t) is less than a threshold value, b, the model responds long. That is to say, the model responds long when faced with ambiguous cases that is, when it cannot tell whether t is closer to the short or the long referent duration. In contrast, when this difference is greater than b, and thus clearly differentiated, the model responds short if D(s, t), D(l, t) and long if D(s, t). D(l, t); b is thus a sort of bias toward responding long (for more details, see Droit-Volet & Wearden, 2001). Increasing the value of b thus shifts the proportion of long responses for intermediate stimulus durations to the left that is, toward the geometric mean of the two anchor durations without altering the slope of the psychophysical function. However, as we can see in Figure 2b, this shift remains small even when the bias value is large (i.e., from 0 to 0.40) and was not observed for the shortest and longest comparison durations. The last parameter, p, is the proportion of responses emitted at random on each trial that is, without reference to the stimulus duration values. Figure 2c shows that increasing p flattens the bisection curve just as increasing c does. However, the increase in the proportion of long responses is greater for the short comparison durations. Droit-Volet and Wearden (2001) have demonstrated that the removal of the parameter p impaired the fit between the data and the model in the youngest children. In the present study, in order to capture any possible distortion of the representation of the reference duration, we added a fourth parameter, k that is, a memory distortion parameter which serves as a multiplier of the remembered short and long referent duration. Thus, if k is 1.0 then the referent values are remembered correctly. If k, 1, the referent values are remembered as shorter than they are in reality, and if k. 1 then they are remembered as longer than they are. The effect of a decrease in the value of k on psychological functions is shown in Figure 2d. Obviously, decreasing k shifts the proportion of long responses to the left for short and intermediate stimulus durations. This model, implemented in a computer program written in Visual Basic 6 (Microsoft Corporation), was run for 1,000 trials, and c, b, p, and k were varied over a wide range in order to obtain the best fitting simulation for the data in terms of mean absolute deviation (MAD) that is, the sum of the absolute deviation between the data and the data derived from the modelling divided by 7. Our model fits the data well with a MAD smaller than.05 (Table 2, Figure 3). Furthermore, it replicates some findings obtained in previous studies in children, notably a decrease in the c parameter value with increasing age, thus indicating that the coefficient of variation of the memory representation of the referent durations decreases with age (for a discussion, see Droit-Volet, Delgado, & Rattat, 2006). However, and most interestingly here, the model shows that the value of the k parameter was quite similar in the two referent memory conditions. Thus, increasing the variability in the Table 2. Parameter values for the fits of the developmental version of the MDM with the data obtained in the bisection task in the fixed and variable referent memory conditions in the 5-year-olds, the 8-year-olds and the adults c b p k MAD 5-year-olds Fixed Variable year-olds Fixed Variable Adults Fixed Variable Note: MDM is the modified difference model used by Wearden (1991) to model data obtained in adults in the bisection task; c is the coefficient of variation of the memory representation of the short and long referent; b is the bias toward long responses; p is the probability of random responding; and k is a distortion of the memory representation of the short and long referent. MAD is the mean absolute deviation, which is the sum of absolute differences between the data points and the fitted function divided by 7 (i.e. the number of data points). 8 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)

9 REPRESENTATION OF TIME IN REFERENCE MEMORY Q6 the 8-year-olds, although this variation was greatest in the youngest children. In comparison with the children, the difference in c values between the two memory conditions appeared to be relatively small in the adults. Furthermore, our model suggests that the manipulation of the reference memory affected not only the parameter c, but also other parameters. Indeed, in the 5-year-olds, both the p and the b parameter values were greater in the variable than in the fixed condition. This suggests that when the representation of durations in reference memory is fuzzier, the young children find it more difficult to produce a temporal decision. Faced with more ambiguous cases, they produce more random responses and more bias toward long responses. In contrast, in the 8-year-olds and the adults, the b parameter value tended to be smaller in the variable than in the fixed condition, thus suggesting a change in the decisional criterion as discussed below. Figure 3. Temporal bisection data from Experiment 1 in the fixed and variable referent memory condition (line) and values predicted by the best fitting modified difference model (MDM; circle) for the three age groups. presentation of examples of the referent durations did not alter their mean value in memory, regardless of the age groups. In contrast, the coefficient of variation of the memory representation of the referent durations, c, was greater in the variable than in the fixed condition in both the 5- and Discussion The results of the present study show that increasing the variability in the examples of the referent durations flattened the bisection functions and increased the Weber ratio in the children, and more particularly in the 5-year-olds. Our bisection model explains this in terms of an increase in the coefficient of variation of the memory representation of referent durations. This, therefore, provides clear evidence that, in temporal bisection, the children did not store single values in reference memory, but instead distributions of values together that allow obtaining of means and some coefficient of variation. Consequently, each example of the two referent durations, or at least the majority of these examples, would seem to be encoded and transferred into reference memory. This provides clear support for the suggestion that temporal reference memory results from the encoding of important times (Meck, 1983; Meck & Angell, 1992; Meck, Church, & Olton, 1984). However, the comparison between age groups allowed us to show that the effect of our THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 9

10 DE LURDES DELGADO AND DROIT-VOLET experimental manipulation of reference memory on temporal bisection behaviour was different in the children and the adults. Indeed, unlike in the children, in the adults an increased variability in the presentation of the examples of the reference durations did not significantly affect the features of the psychophysical functions. In another temporal discrimination task, Jones and Wearden (2003) varied the number of presentations of standard durations from 1 to 5 presentations and also failed to find an effect of the number of presentations on temporal performance in human adults. They concluded that the different examples of the standard duration are not stored separately in reference memory and then sampled, since otherwise a fuzzier memory representation would have been obtained with 5 presentations. According to these authors, this challenges the idea that reference memory contains more than one value. Allan and Gerhardt (2001) suggested that, in a temporal bisection task, adults do not use comparisons with the referent durations, but a unique duration criterion. However, the number of presentations of the reference durations used by Jones and Wearden (2003) (and in our experiment) might have been too low to produce sufficient noise in the reference memories of human adults to result in a modification of temporal judgement. Studies of animals have used long training sessions with the referent durations. However, our study showed that, unlike in the adults, a greater number of presentations of the referent durations was not necessary in the young children for the effect of variability of reference memory on temporal judgement to occur. This is probably due to their memory referent representation that was already fuzzy in the fixed memory condition. Thus, the comparison of the age groups has allowed to us to gain a better understanding of the fundamental nature of the content of the reference memory in humans in the bisection task. To confirm and extend our results, we decided to run the same experiment with another temporal task that is, the temporal generalization task. In this task, the participants are not required to categorize the stimulus durations as a function of their similarity with the short or the long referent duration, but to judge the similarity between the stimulus durations and a unique referent duration. In this condition, a direct comparison between the stimulus durations and the referent duration appears to be obligatory. Consequently, the effect on temporal judgement of the increasing variability of the reference durations in the examples should be observed both in the adults and in the children, although to a lesser extent in the former. EXPERIMENT 2 Method Participants The sample consisted of 86 new participants: 30 five-year-olds (17 girls and 13 boys; mean age ¼ 5.70 years, SD ¼ 0.76); 30 eight-year-olds (19 girls and 11 boys; mean age ¼ 8.45 years, SD ¼ 0.21), and 26 students (26 women; mean age ¼ years, SD ¼ 0.85). The children were recruited from nursery and primary schools other than those involved in Experiment 1, and the adults were first-year psychology students from Blaise Pascal University. Materials and procedure The materials were the same as those in Experiment 1, and the procedure was similar. In each age group, the participants were randomly assigned to one of the two referent memory conditions that is, fixed or variable. In each condition, they were presented with the referent duration five times (familiarization phase) before each test block. In the fixed referent condition, the referent duration was always 4 s while in the variable condition it was randomly chosen on each trial from the five following values: 3, 3.5, 4, 4.5, 5. The mean of the referent durations presented in the familiarization phase was 4 s and the coefficient of variation.20. In each condition, the experimenter said: Look, it s your circle. It stays on for a certain time. After each familiarization phase, the participants saw a test block. There were four test blocks of 21 trials each, 3 trials for each of the seven comparison durations 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, and 10 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)

11 REPRESENTATION OF TIME IN REFERENCE MEMORY 7 s. The participant s task was to press one key if they judged the comparison duration to be similar to the referent duration (yes response) and the other key if they judged that it was not similar to the referent duration (no response). The press button order was counterbalanced. The comparison durations were presented in random order within each block, and the intertrial interval value was also randomly chosen between 500 ms and 2 s. As in Experiment 1, the experimenter told the participants not to count and controlled the continuity of the verbal activity used to prevent counting. Results Data analysis Figure 4 shows the mean proportion of yes responses plotted against the comparison duration for the fixed and the variable referent memory condition in the 5-year-olds (top panel), the 8-year-olds (centre panel), and the adults (bottom panel). The ANOVA run on the proportion of yes responses with age, referent memory condition, and comparison duration as subject factors found a significant effect of age, F(2, 80) ¼ 20.63, p ¼.0001, and of comparison duration, F(6, 480) ¼ , p ¼ The interaction between age and comparison duration was also significant, F(12, 480) ¼ 6.25, p ¼ In each age group, we calculated the mean proportion of yes responses for the shortest (the three comparison duration values shorter than the reference duration value) and the longest comparison duration values (the three comparison duration values longer than the reference duration value). The effect of age appeared to be significant for both the shortest, F(2, 83) ¼ 19.29, p ¼.001, and the longest durations, F(2, 83) ¼ 5.97, p ¼.004. This is due to the proportion of yes responses being greater in the 5-year-olds than in the 8-year-olds and the adults for the shortest durations (Sheffe post hoc, all p,.05) and greater than in the adults for the longest durations ( p,.05). The 8-year-olds also produced more yes responses than the adults did for the shortest as well as for the longest durations ( p,.05). These various results therefore indicate Figure 4. Proportion of yes responses plotted against stimulus duration in the fixed and the variable referent memory condition for the 5-year-olds (upper panel), the 8-year-olds (centre panel), and the adults (lower panel). that the steepness of the generalization gradient increased with age. The overall ANOVA on the proportion of yes responses also showed a significant main effect of referent memory, F(1, 80) ¼ 10.29, p ¼.002, as Q6 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 11

12 DE LURDES DELGADO AND DROIT-VOLET well as a significant interaction between the referent memory and the comparison duration, F(6, 480) ¼ 4.70, p ¼ This revealed that the shape of the generalization gradient was flatter in the variable than in the fixed referent condition, although the varied fixed difference was particularly more important for the longer stimulus durations. The difference in the mean proportion of yes responses between the shortest and the longest comparison durations appeared to be greater than zero for the fixed, t(42) ¼ 2.48, p ¼.02, and the varied condition, t(42) ¼ 7.11, p ¼.0001, thus indicating a right asymmetry in the generalization gradients in each memory condition. However, for the longest durations, the mean proportion of yes responses appeared to be greater in the varied than in the fixed condition, t(84) ¼ 3.17, p ¼.002. This suggested that the varied condition shifted the generalization gradients toward the right. The overall ANOVA found no significant interaction between referent memory and age, F(2, 80) ¼ 0.68, p ¼.53, nor between age, referent memory, and comparison duration, F(12, 480) ¼ 1.44, p ¼.15. This suggests that the effect of the variability in the examples of the reference durations on the generalization gradients did not significantly change with age. Modelling and discussion The model that we used was the generalization model originally used by Church and Gibbon (1982) with animals and later modified by Wearden (1992) for adults and more recently for children (Droit-Volet, 2002; Droit-Volet, Clément, & Wearden, 2001; Droit-Volet & Q7 Izaute, 2004). In this model, the referent duration is represented as a Gaussian distribution with a mean s and a coefficient of variation, c. For each trial, a value s was randomly sampled from this distribution. This model thus assumes that the participants respond yes when j(s t)/tj, b. The first parameter in this model is c. As far as bisection is concerned, c is the coefficient of variation of the memory representation of s. The threshold b was also represented as a Gaussian distribution with a mean b and the referent deviation held constant at 0.5b. Varying this last parameter produces no great improvement. The value b is a sample taken on each trial from the distribution of the threshold. The participants respond yes when t, the duration which has just been presented, is sufficiently close to s, with the decision being controlled by the threshold b. The second parameter in the model is thus b. The third parameter in the model is p, the proportion of responses given at random. On each trial, the model would be equally likely to make yes or no responses regardless of the stimulus duration. The fourth parameter is k, a memory distortion parameter, which is multiplied s over a range of values below 1.0 or above 1.0. If k were 1.0, the referent duration would be remembered correctly. If k were. 1.0, it would be remembered as longer than it really was, and if k were, 1.0, it would be remembered as shorter than it was. Figure 5 illustrates some proprieties of this model. In Panel a, the parameter c was varied while the other parameters were held constant. Increasing c make the reference memory fuzzier and flattens the generalization gradients, but few yes responses occur at the shortest stimulus durations. Panel b shows the effect of increasing b, while the other parameters were also held constant. Increasing b makes the decision as to whether to respond yes less conservative, and the overall proportion of yes responses increases, while the general shape of the gradient remain constant. Increasing p (Panel c) increases the proportion of yes responses occurring at each stimulus duration, even for the shortest stimulus durations. Increasing k shifts the gradient toward the right while decreasing k shifts it toward the left. This model was implemented in a computer program written in Visual Basic. As in Experiment 2, the different parameters were varied until we obtained the best fitting simulation in terms of mean absolute deviation that is, the sum of the absolute differences between the predictions from the simulation and the data (Figure 6). The model fits our data very well. Table 3 shows the parameter values obtained in this way. Our model captured the same Q8 Q9 12 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)

13 REPRESENTATION OF TIME IN REFERENCE MEMORY Q6 Figure 5. Data derived from simulations using the developmental version of the modified Church and Gibbon (MCG) model discussed in the text, when a parameter value is varying while the other parameter are kept constant. developmental trends as those found in previous generalization studies: (a) the age-related increase of the c value, which indicates a fuzzier memory representation of the referent duration in the younger children, and (b) a higher proportion of random responses (Droit-Volet, 2002; Droit-Volet et al., 2001; Droit-Volet & Izaute, Q7 2004). It also allows us to identify the mechanisms underlying the fixed varied differences in the generalization gradients. First, it indicates that the flatter generalization gradient in the varied condition is due to a greater coefficient of variation of the temporal representation in reference memory. Furthermore, our model suggests that introducing noise in the presentation of examples of the reference duration affects not only the content of the reference memory, but also the decisional processes, and particularly so in children. Indeed, the value of the b parameter was greater in the varied than in the fixed condition. This indicated that the participants became less conservative when the representation of durations in memory was fuzzier. This would explain why the temporal generalization gradients were more skewed to the right in the varied than in the fixed memory condition. Unlike the c and b parameters, the p (probability of random responses) and k parameters did not differ between the fixed and the variable memory condition. GENERAL DISCUSSION Experiment 1, which used a bisection task, and Experiment 2, which used a generalization task, THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 13

14 DE LURDES DELGADO AND DROIT-VOLET Table 3. Parameter values for the fits of the developmental version of the MCG model with the data obtained in the generalization task in the fixed and variable referent memory conditions in the 5-year-olds, the 8-year-olds and the adults c b p k MAD 5-year-olds Fixed Variable year-olds Fixed Variable Adults Fixed Variable Q6 Note: MCG is the modified Church and Gibbon model used bywearden (1992) to model data obtained in adults in the generalization task. c ¼ coefficient of variation of the memory representation of the standard. b ¼ decisional threshold. p ¼ probability of random responses. k ¼ distortion of the memory representation of the standard. MAD ¼ mean absolute difference between the data points and the fitted function divided by 7 (i.e. the number of data points). Figure 6. Temporal generalization data from Experiment 2 in the fixed and variable referent memory condition (line) and values predicted by the best fitting modified Church and Gibbon (MCG) model (circle) for the three age groups. provided consistent data showing that introducing noise in the presentation of examples of the referent durations altered our participants temporal judgements by decreasing their sensitivity to time, even though this alteration was systematically observed in the children and only in the generalization task in the adults. This provides evidence that each example of the referent duration was coded and stored in memory, thus forming a distribution of values with a mean and a coefficient of variation. As previously suggested, this supports the idea that temporal reference memory contains a set of important durations (e.g., Meck, 1996; Meck, Church, & Olton, 1984). These results also reveal that the main sources of noise in the memory reference do not reside solely in the storage processes themselves, as the scalar timing models suppose, but also in the initial imprecision in the perception of time, which is greater in the children than in the adults. In their model, McCormack et al. introduced a noise parameter representing an amount of noise added to the perceived duration. Their model showed that the proportion of noise in the perceived time was effectively higher in children than in adults. Consequently, the fuzzier memory representation of referent durations in children, initially identified by Droit-Volet and Wearden (2001) in the developmental version of the scalar timing models, would, to a large extent, be due to an initial noise in time encoding. However, it remains possible that certain memory Q10 14 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0)

15 REPRESENTATION OF TIME IN REFERENCE MEMORY problems specific to children may also be involved (Droit-Volet et al., 2006). Using an interfering task and deferred temporal tests in bisection, Rattat and Droit-Volet (2005a, 2005b) showed that the forgetting of reference durations was more important in the younger children. That increased the variability of their memory representation of the reference durations and flattened their bisection curves. Young children s errors in the encoding of each stimulus duration would therefore be introduced into reference memory. This in turn would produce imprecision in the judgement of the similarity of t to the referent duration. Recently, McCormack et al. (2005), using a generalization Q4 task, and Droit-Volet and Rattat (2006), using a bisection task, have shown that the age-related differences in temporal judgements do not disappear when temporal reference memory is disabled. This suggests that the main source of developmental differences in sensitivity to time does not lie in reference memory, otherwise children would be expected to obtain, at least, the same performance without reference memory. Our study, in which the content of reference memory was manipulated, provided additional data supporting this suggestion. Indeed, it demonstrated that the variability in the examples of the referent durations made their memory representations fuzzier, thus reducing sensitivity to duration as indicated by the flatter psychophysical functions in the varied than in the fixed condition. Our experiments, and more particularly Experiment 1 with the bisection task, also revealed a gap between the temporal behaviour of adults and that of children. Our interpretation is that this is mainly due to the development of selfregulatory functions, which are dependent on the acquisition of metacognitive knowledge and skills (for a review see Kuhl & Kraska, 1996). Young children are known to experience difficulties in identifying and using new strategies and therefore follow the instructions given by the experimenter. In our experiments, they thus compared the test durations with the referent durations even if their representation in memory was fuzzy. Unlike young children, human adults are able to use strategies other than those presented by the experimenter. According to Allan and Gerhardt (2001), in a bisection task, adults do not use the decision rule based on direct comparisons with the referent durations, but a unique criterion established on the basis of the values of the two referent durations. One possibility is that, unlike the young children, the adults were able to calculate the mean of the presented referent durations since there was no difference in the psychophysical functions between the fixed and the varied condition. Another possibility is that the adults did not use the referent durations in the bisection task but instead transformed it into a partition task in which they classified each presented duration into two categories namely, a short or a long one. This interpretation is consistent with Wearden and Ferrara s (1995, 1996) findings of similar data in the prototypical and the partition task in which no standard durations were presented. Whatever the case may be, the results of our study did not allow us to come to any conclusion about the type of strategies used by human adults in bisection, and these will have to be investigated in future studies. Our model only suggests that the b parameter changes in the condition in which the representation of reference durations in memory is fuzzier. Little research has been devoted to the role of decisional strategies in temporal judgement. Furthermore, in the models of temporal information processing, the decisional processes have always been considered independently of the other processes (Wearden & Grindrod, 2002) that is, independently of the perceived durations or the quality of the memory representation of durations. In contrast, our experiments have provided original data suggesting that, in children, and probably also in adults, the value of decisional parameters changes as a function of the representation (more or less fuzzy) of the referent durations in memory. Indeed, the children became less conservative when their temporal representation in memory became fuzzier. The participants awareness of the quality of the content of temporal reference memory would thus affect the decision and the strategy used in a temporal discrimination task. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 0000, 00 (0) 15

Perception of the duration of auditory and visual stimuli in children and adults

Perception of the duration of auditory and visual stimuli in children and adults Q283 QJEP(A)42 / Jun, 4 (Tue)/ [22 pages 3 Tables 3 Figures 2 Footnotes Appendices].. Centre single caption. cf. [no comma]. disk edited RJ,. Graphics from word/xcel into cdraw. Please check THE QUARTERLY

More information

Temporal Generalization in 3- to 8-Year-Old Children

Temporal Generalization in 3- to 8-Year-Old Children Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 80, 271 288 (2001) doi:10.1006/jecp.2001.2629, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Temporal Generalization in 3- to 8-Year-Old Children Sylvie Droit-Volet

More information

Short-term memory for time in children and adults: A behavioral study and a model

Short-term memory for time in children and adults: A behavioral study and a model Journal of Experimental Child Psychology xxx (2007) xxx xxx www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp Short-term memory for time in children and adults: A behavioral study and a model Sylvie Droit-Volet a, *, John

More information

Episodic temporal generalization: A developmental study

Episodic temporal generalization: A developmental study THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2005, 58A (4), 693 704 Episodic temporal generalization: A developmental study T. McCormack Queen s University, Belfast, Northern Ireland, UK J. H. Wearden

More information

Decision processes in models of timing

Decision processes in models of timing Acta Neurobiol Exp 2004, 64: xxx-xxx NEU OBIOLOGI E EXPE IMENT LIS Decision processes in models of timing John H. Wearden Review Department of Psychology, Manchester University, Manchester, M13 9PL, UK,

More information

Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection?

Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection? Learning and Motivation 33, 10 31 (2002) doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1097, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection? Lorraine G. Allan McMaster University,

More information

When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur?

When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur? THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (10), 1709 1724 When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur? J. H. Wearden Keele University, Keele, UK N. P. M. Todd and L.

More information

Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration. John Wearden Keele University U.K.

Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration. John Wearden Keele University U.K. Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration John Wearden Keele University U.K. In this talk I want to illustrate the use of the techniques of production, reproduction, and verbal estimation

More information

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? Keyed. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 55B (1), 1 25 Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? J.H. Wearden, A. Parry, and L. Stamp University of

More information

The perception of empty and filled time intervals by pigeons

The perception of empty and filled time intervals by pigeons pqjb04b-06.qxd 12/29/04 11:00 AM Page 31 THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2005, 58B (1), 31 45 The perception of empty and filled time intervals by pigeons Andrew Miki and Angelo Santi

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Chapter 2: Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges

Chapter 2: Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges Chapter 2: Applying the scalar timing model to human time psychology: Progress and challenges JOHN H. WEARDEN Abstract Scalar timing (or scalar expectancy) theory, SET, was originally developed as an explanation

More information

Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation

Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation Emily A. Williams (Emily.A.Williams@Manchester.ac.uk) Andrew J. Stewart (Andrew.J.Stewart@Manchester.ac.uk) Luke A. Jones (Luke.Jones@Manchester.ac.uk)

More information

SCALAR TIMING (EXPECTANCY) THEORY: A COMPARISON BETWEEN PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION. Abstract

SCALAR TIMING (EXPECTANCY) THEORY: A COMPARISON BETWEEN PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION. Abstract SCALAR TIMING (EXPECTANCY) THEORY: A COMPARISON BETWEEN PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION Hannes Eisler and Anna D. Eisler Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden he@psychology.su.se

More information

Why ``Sound s Are Jud ged Longer T han Ligh ts : Application of a Model of th e Internal Clock in Hu m ans

Why ``Sound s Are Jud ged Longer T han Ligh ts : Application of a Model of th e Internal Clock in Hu m ans Ó THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1998, 51B (2), 97± 120 Why ``Sound s Are Jud ged Longer T han Ligh ts : Application of a Model of th e Internal Clock in Hu m ans J. H. Wearden, H. Edwards,

More information

Age and IQ Effects on Stimulus and Response Timing

Age and IQ Effects on Stimulus and Response Timing Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Human Perception and Performance 96-1523/97/$3. 1997, Vol. 23, No. 4, 962-979 Age and IQ Effects on Stimulus

More information

How Liked and Disliked Foods Affect Time Perception

How Liked and Disliked Foods Affect Time Perception Emotion 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 9, No. 4, 457 463 1528-3542/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015751 How Liked and Disliked Foods Affect Time Perception Sandrine Gil Université Blaise

More information

NUMEROSITY DISCRIMINATION IN PRESCHOOL CHILDREN ALZIRA ALMEIDA, JOANA ARANTES, AND ARMANDO MACHADO

NUMEROSITY DISCRIMINATION IN PRESCHOOL CHILDREN ALZIRA ALMEIDA, JOANA ARANTES, AND ARMANDO MACHADO JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2007, 88, 339 354 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) NUMEROSITY DISCRIMINATION IN PRESCHOOL CHILDREN ALZIRA ALMEIDA, JOANA ARANTES, AND ARMANDO MACHADO UNIVERSITY OF MINHO,

More information

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific?

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Temporal Binding and Internal Clocks: Is Clock Slowing General or Specific? Richard Fereday (FeredayR@cardiff.ac.uk), Marc J. Buehner (BuehnerM@cardiff.ac.uk) Cardiff University, School of Psychology,

More information

Stimulus Range Effects in Temporal Bisection by Humans

Stimulus Range Effects in Temporal Bisection by Humans q THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1996, 49B (1), 24± 44 Stimulus Range Effects in Temporal Bisection by Humans J.H. Wearden Ma nchester University, Ma nchester, U.K. A. Ferrara University

More information

Unequal Weber fractions for the categorization of brief temporal intervals

Unequal Weber fractions for the categorization of brief temporal intervals Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 21, 72 (5), 1422-143 doi:1.3758/app.72.5.1422 Unequal Weber fractions for the categorization of brief temporal intervals SIMON GRONDIN Université Laval, Québec, Québec,

More information

Experimental and Computational Analyses of Strategy Usage in the Time-Left Task

Experimental and Computational Analyses of Strategy Usage in the Time-Left Task Experimental and Computational Analyses of Strategy Usage in the Time-Left Task Ben Meijering (B.Meijering@student.rug.nl) Hedderik van Rijn (D.H.van.Rijn@rug.nl) Department of Artificial Intelligence

More information

Regression Including the Interaction Between Quantitative Variables

Regression Including the Interaction Between Quantitative Variables Regression Including the Interaction Between Quantitative Variables The purpose of the study was to examine the inter-relationships among social skills, the complexity of the social situation, and performance

More information

What is timed in a fixed-interval temporal bisection procedure?

What is timed in a fixed-interval temporal bisection procedure? Learn Behav (216) 44:366 377 DOI 1.3758/s1342-16-228-z What is timed in a fixed-interval temporal bisection procedure? Adam E. Fox 1 & Katelyn E. Prue 1 & Elizabeth G. E. Kyonka 2 Published online: 29

More information

The Impact of Emotion on Numerical Estimation: A Developmental Perspective

The Impact of Emotion on Numerical Estimation: A Developmental Perspective The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ISSN: 1747-0218 (Print) 1747-0226 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pqje20 The Impact of Emotion on Numerical Estimation: A Developmental

More information

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons From M. L. Commons, R.J. Herrnstein, & A.R. Wagner (Eds.). 1983. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Discrimination Processes. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (Vol. IV, Chapter 1, pages 3-19). A Memory Model for

More information

Raquel Cocenas-Silva a, José Lino Oliveira Bueno a, Valérie Doyère b & Sylvie Droit-Volet c a University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, Brazil

Raquel Cocenas-Silva a, José Lino Oliveira Bueno a, Valérie Doyère b & Sylvie Droit-Volet c a University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, Brazil This article was downloaded by: [USP University of Sao Paulo] On: 14 March 2014, At: 07:34 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:

More information

Time flies when we read taboo words

Time flies when we read taboo words Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2010, 17 (4), 563-568 doi:10.3758/pbr.17.4.563 Time flies when we read taboo words JASON TIPPLES University of Hull, Hull, England Does time fly or stand still when one is

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Pigeons timing of an arbitrary and a naturalistic auditory stimulus: tone versus cooing

Pigeons timing of an arbitrary and a naturalistic auditory stimulus: tone versus cooing Behavioural Processes 53 (2001) 103 111 www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Pigeons timing of an arbitrary and a naturalistic auditory stimulus: tone versus cooing Andrew Miki, Angelo Santi * Department

More information

On the formation of an internal standard in discrimination experiments

On the formation of an internal standard in discrimination experiments Department of Psychology On the formation of an internal standard in discrimination experiments The Internal Reference Model 2012/10/04 K. M. Bausenhart, O. Dyjas, & R. Ulrich Why do we need an internal

More information

Method of Limits Worksheet

Method of Limits Worksheet 1 Method of Limits Worksheet Name: Date: Trial # Upper Threshold Lower Threshold 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Sum Mean Question 1: What is the upper threshold of Trial #1? Question 2: Compute the mean upper threshold.

More information

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS

TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS Simon Grondin, Vincent Laflamme, Nicolas Bisson, Emi Hasuo, Tsuyoshi Kuroda Université Laval, Québec, Canada simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca Abstract

More information

Time perception, depression and sadness

Time perception, depression and sadness Time perception, depression and sadness Sandrine Gil, Sylvie Droit-Volet - Blaise Pascal University, CNRS, France A paraître dans / To appear in : Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S.. Time perception, depression

More information

Categorical Perception

Categorical Perception Categorical Perception Discrimination for some speech contrasts is poor within phonetic categories and good between categories. Unusual, not found for most perceptual contrasts. Influenced by task, expectations,

More information

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL 1 SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL Response time and signal detection time distributions SM Fig. 1. Correct response time (thick solid green curve) and error response time densities (dashed red curve), averaged across

More information

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Fundamentals of Psychophysics Fundamentals of Psychophysics John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3045! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Visual neuroscience physiology stimulus How do we see the world? neuroimaging

More information

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Materials Supplementary Materials Supplementary Figure S1: Data of all 106 subjects in Experiment 1, with each rectangle corresponding to one subject. Data from each of the two identical sub-sessions are shown separately.

More information

Are Time and Action Dissociated in Young Children s Time Estimation?

Are Time and Action Dissociated in Young Children s Time Estimation? Cognitive Development, 14, 573 595 (1999) ISSN 0885-2014 2000 Elsevier Science Inc. All rights of reproduction reserved. Are Time and Action Dissociated in Young Children s Time Estimation? Sylvie Droit-Volet

More information

Time Interval Estimation: Internal Clock or Attentional Mechanism?

Time Interval Estimation: Internal Clock or Attentional Mechanism? Time Interval Estimation: Internal Clock or Attentional Mechanism? Niels Taatgen 1,2 (taatgen@cmu.edu), John Anderson 1 (ja@cmu.edu), Daniel Dickison 1 (danieldickison@cmu.edu) and Hedderik van Rijn 2

More information

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Current Biology, in press Supplemental Information: Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction Grigorios Sotiropoulos, Aaron R. Seitz, and Peggy Seriès Supplemental Data Detailed

More information

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title Context effects in temporal differentiation: Some data and a model Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/588544v0 Journal International Journal

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The differential recruitment of short-term memory and executive functions during time, number and length perception: An individual differences approach. Journal: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

More information

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions Readings: OpenStax Textbook - Chapters 1 5 (online) Appendix D & E (online) Plous - Chapters 1, 5, 6, 13 (online) Introductory comments Describe how familiarity with statistical methods can - be associated

More information

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE Isabelle Guay and Simon Grondin Université Laval, Québec, Canada Email: simon.grondin@psy.ulaval.ca ABSTRACT

More information

Nonverbal arithmetic in humans: Light from noise

Nonverbal arithmetic in humans: Light from noise Perception & Psychophysics 7, 69 (7), 1185-13 Nonverbal arithmetic in humans: Light from noise SARA CORDES Duke University, Durham, North Carolina C. R. GALLISTEL AND ROCHEL GELMAN Rutgers University,

More information

Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures

Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this

More information

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison Ethan D. Montag Rochester Institute of Technology Munsell Color Science Laboratory Chester F. Carlson Center for Imaging Science

More information

How Emotional Auditory Stimuli Modulate Time Perception

How Emotional Auditory Stimuli Modulate Time Perception Emotion Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 7, No. 4, 697 704 1528-3542/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.697 How Emotional Auditory Stimuli Modulate Time Perception

More information

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08 Auditory Perception - Detection versus Discrimination - Localization versus Discrimination - - Electrophysiological Measurements Psychophysical Measurements Three Approaches to Researching Audition physiology

More information

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale Elizabeth M. Brannon*, Courtney J. Wusthoff #, C.R. Gallistel^, and John Gibbon @ * Center for Cognitive Neuroscience

More information

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08 Auditory Perception - Detection versus Discrimination - Localization versus Discrimination - Electrophysiological Measurements - Psychophysical Measurements 1 Three Approaches to Researching Audition physiology

More information

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance Shoko Yamane, Naohiro Matsumura Faculty of Economics, Kinki University; Graduate School of Economics, Osaka University syamane@kindai.ac.jp Abstract We examined

More information

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.) 1 Outline Method of Adjustment Method of Limits Method of Constant Stimuli Probit Analysis 2 Method of Constant Stimuli A set of equally spaced levels of the stimulus

More information

ABOUT THE INFLUENCE OF PERCEIVING DISGUSTED, HAPPY AND SAD FACES ON TIME PERCEPTION

ABOUT THE INFLUENCE OF PERCEIVING DISGUSTED, HAPPY AND SAD FACES ON TIME PERCEPTION ABOUT THE INFLUENCE OF PERCEIVING DISGUSTED, HAPPY AND SAD FACES ON TIME PERCEPTION Noémie de la Sablonnière, Vincent Laflamme, Katherine Labonté and Simon Grondin Université Laval, Québec, Canada noemie.de-la-sablonniere.1@ulaval.ca

More information

Chapter 2 Norms and Basic Statistics for Testing MULTIPLE CHOICE

Chapter 2 Norms and Basic Statistics for Testing MULTIPLE CHOICE Chapter 2 Norms and Basic Statistics for Testing MULTIPLE CHOICE 1. When you assert that it is improbable that the mean intelligence test score of a particular group is 100, you are using. a. descriptive

More information

Packet theory of conditioning and timing

Packet theory of conditioning and timing Behavioural Processes 57 (2002) 89 106 www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Packet theory of conditioning and timing Kimberly Kirkpatrick Department of Psychology, Uni ersity of York, York YO10 5DD, UK Accepted

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 Supporting Information Variances and biases of absolute distributions were larger in the 2-line

More information

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1 BLL R. BROWN AND THOMAS J. REBBN2 UNlVERSTY OF LOUSVLLE This experiment required Ss to make Markov-generated histoforms that were

More information

Statistics 2. RCBD Review. Agriculture Innovation Program

Statistics 2. RCBD Review. Agriculture Innovation Program Statistics 2. RCBD Review 2014. Prepared by Lauren Pincus With input from Mark Bell and Richard Plant Agriculture Innovation Program 1 Table of Contents Questions for review... 3 Answers... 3 Materials

More information

Two-Way Independent Samples ANOVA with SPSS

Two-Way Independent Samples ANOVA with SPSS Two-Way Independent Samples ANOVA with SPSS Obtain the file ANOVA.SAV from my SPSS Data page. The data are those that appear in Table 17-3 of Howell s Fundamental statistics for the behavioral sciences

More information

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Infant and Child Development Inf. Child Dev. 10: 39 45 (2001) DOI: 10.1002/icd.245 Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age Olivier Pascalis a, *, Elisabeth

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

Chapter 2--Norms and Basic Statistics for Testing

Chapter 2--Norms and Basic Statistics for Testing Chapter 2--Norms and Basic Statistics for Testing Student: 1. Statistical procedures that summarize and describe a series of observations are called A. inferential statistics. B. descriptive statistics.

More information

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH Laurence D. Smith University of Maine ldsmith@maine.maine.edu Ronald M. Pickett Marjan Trutschl Institute for Visualization and Perception

More information

Laura N. Young a & Sara Cordes a a Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut

Laura N. Young a & Sara Cordes a a Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut This article was downloaded by: [Boston College] On: 08 November 2012, At: 09:04 Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Acta Psychologica xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Acta Psychologica xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica Acta Psychologica xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy Timing of multiple overlapping intervals: How many clocks

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Numerical cognition is the idea that a living being has a number sense similar to a

Numerical cognition is the idea that a living being has a number sense similar to a Sample Lab Report Jane Doe Intro to Cognitive Psych Lab Report #9 1/1/2007 Introduction Numerical cognition is the idea that a living being has a number sense similar to a sense of smell or touch. Much

More information

Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks

Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks Psychon Bull Rev (211) 18:331 338 DOI 1.3758/s13423-11-59-z Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks Dale J. Cohen & Daryn Blanc-Goldhammer Published online: 11 February 211 # Psychonomic

More information

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation The Role of in Categorisation Mark Suret (m.suret@psychol.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology; Downing Street Cambridge, CB2 3EB UK I.P.L. McLaren (iplm2@cus.cam.ac.uk) Department of Experimental

More information

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data Area Principle Bar Chart Boxplot Conditional Distribution Dotplot Empirical Rule Five Number Summary Frequency Distribution Frequency Polygon Histogram Interquartile

More information

Business Statistics Probability

Business Statistics Probability Business Statistics The following was provided by Dr. Suzanne Delaney, and is a comprehensive review of Business Statistics. The workshop instructor will provide relevant examples during the Skills Assessment

More information

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired

More information

Absolute Identification Is Relative: A Reply to Brown, Marley, and Lacouture (2007)

Absolute Identification Is Relative: A Reply to Brown, Marley, and Lacouture (2007) Psychological Review opyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 114, No. 2, 533 538 0033-295X/07/$120 DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.533 Absolute Identification Is Relative: A Reply

More information

The Relationship between the Perception of Axes of Symmetry and Spatial Memory during Early Childhood

The Relationship between the Perception of Axes of Symmetry and Spatial Memory during Early Childhood University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 2010 The Relationship between the Perception of

More information

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions Readings: OpenStax Textbook - Chapters 1 5 (online) Appendix D & E (online) Plous - Chapters 1, 5, 6, 13 (online) Introductory comments Describe how familiarity with statistical methods can - be associated

More information

ELEMENTS OF PSYCHOPHYSICS Sections VII and XVI. Gustav Theodor Fechner (1860/1912)

ELEMENTS OF PSYCHOPHYSICS Sections VII and XVI. Gustav Theodor Fechner (1860/1912) ELEMENTS OF PSYCHOPHYSICS Sections VII and XVI Gustav Theodor Fechner (1860/1912) Translated by Herbert Sidney Langfeld (1912) [Classics Editor's note: This translation of these passages from Fechner's

More information

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation The description and presentation of results involves a number of topics. These include scales of measurement, descriptive statistics used to summarize

More information

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory Antonio H. Lara & Jonathan D. Wallis Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral measures of

More information

CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS IN INFORMATION INTEGRATION

CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS IN INFORMATION INTEGRATION Journal ol Experimental Psychology 1971, Vol. 88, No. 2, 18-170 CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS IN INFORMATION INTEGRATION MICHAEL H. BIRNBAUM,* ALLEN PARDUCCI, AND ROBERT K. GIFFORD University of California, Los Angeles

More information

Time Changes with the Embodiment of Another s Body Posture

Time Changes with the Embodiment of Another s Body Posture Time Changes with the Embodiment of Another s Body Posture Francisco C. Nather 1 *, José L. O. Bueno 1, Emmanuel Bigand 2, Sylvie Droit-Volet 3 * 1 University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, Brazil, 2 Institut

More information

Psychology Research Process

Psychology Research Process Psychology Research Process Logical Processes Induction Observation/Association/Using Correlation Trying to assess, through observation of a large group/sample, what is associated with what? Examples:

More information

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo Please note the page numbers listed for the Lind book may vary by a page or two depending on which version of the textbook you have. Readings: Lind 1 11 (with emphasis on chapters 10, 11) Please note chapter

More information

Steven A. Cholewiak ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

Steven A. Cholewiak ALL RIGHTS RESERVED 2 Steven A. Cholewiak ALL RIGHTS RESERVED PERCEPTUAL ESTIMATION OF VARIANCE IN ORIENTATION AND ITS DEPENDENCE ON SAMPLE SIZE by Steven A. Cholewiak A thesis submitted to the Graduate School-New Brunswick

More information

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity Perception & Psychophysics 1980,28 (3), 249-253 Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity STEPHEN HANDEL and JAMES W. RHODES University oftennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee 37916 Subjects

More information

CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA

CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA Data Analysis: Describing Data CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA In the analysis process, the researcher tries to evaluate the data collected both from written documents and from other sources such

More information

1 Introduction. st0020. The Stata Journal (2002) 2, Number 3, pp

1 Introduction. st0020. The Stata Journal (2002) 2, Number 3, pp The Stata Journal (22) 2, Number 3, pp. 28 289 Comparative assessment of three common algorithms for estimating the variance of the area under the nonparametric receiver operating characteristic curve

More information

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities David P. McGovern, Andrew T. Astle, Sarah L. Clavin and Fiona N. Newell Figure S1: Group-averaged learning

More information

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints Jean-Pierre Thibaut (jean-pierre.thibaut@univ-poitiers.fr) University of Poitiers, CeRCA, CNRS UMR 634, 99 avenue du recteur Pineau

More information

THE SYMBOLIC DISTANCE EFFECT FOR ALPHABETIC ORDER JUDGEMENTS : A SUBJECTIVE REPORT AND REACTION TIME ANALYSIS

THE SYMBOLIC DISTANCE EFFECT FOR ALPHABETIC ORDER JUDGEMENTS : A SUBJECTIVE REPORT AND REACTION TIME ANALYSIS Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (1978) 30, 33-43. THE SYMBOLIC DISTANCE EFFECT FOR ALPHABETIC ORDER JUDGEMENTS : A SUBJECTIVE REPORT AND REACTION TIME ANALYSIS J. M. E. HAMILTON AND A. J.

More information

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge Kimery R. Levering (kleveri1@binghamton.edu) Kenneth J. Kurtz (kkurtz@binghamton.edu) Department of Psychology, Binghamton

More information

1 Version SP.A Investigate patterns of association in bivariate data

1 Version SP.A Investigate patterns of association in bivariate data Claim 1: Concepts and Procedures Students can explain and apply mathematical concepts and carry out mathematical procedures with precision and fluency. Content Domain: Statistics and Probability Target

More information

Perception Lecture 1

Perception Lecture 1 Page 1 Perception Lecture 1 Sensation vs Perception Sensation is detection Perception is interpretation The distal and proximal stimulus Distal stimulus: the object out there in the world (distal=distant).

More information

Modeling Human Perception

Modeling Human Perception Modeling Human Perception Could Stevens Power Law be an Emergent Feature? Matthew L. Bolton Systems and Information Engineering University of Virginia Charlottesville, United States of America Mlb4b@Virginia.edu

More information

The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories

The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories Wai Keen Vong (waikeen.vong@adelaide.edu.au) Andrew T. Hendrickson (drew.hendrickson@adelaide.edu.au) Amy Perfors (amy.perfors@adelaide.edu.au)

More information

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH Marie Rivenez 1,2, Chris Darwin 1, Anne Guillaume 2 1 Department of Psychology University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK 2 Département Sciences Cognitives Institut

More information