Adaptive Approaches to Competition: Challenge Appraisals and Positive Emotion

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Adaptive Approaches to Competition: Challenge Appraisals and Positive Emotion"

Transcription

1 JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2004, 26, Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. Adaptive Approaches to Competition: Challenge Appraisals and Positive Emotion Natalie Skinner and Neil Brewer Flinders University The influence of negative emotions such as anxiety on athletes preparation and performance has been studied extensively. The focus of this review is on more adaptive approaches to competition such as the experience of positive emotion and beneficial perceptions of emotion. Evidence on the antecedents and adaptive consequences of positive emotions is reviewed, and implications for research and practice in a sport context are suggested. We focus on the cognitive appraisal of challenge as a significant antecedent of both positive emotion and beneficial perceptions of emotion. A theoretical model of beneficial and harmful perceptions of emotion is presented which incorporates appraisals of challenge, coping expectancies, and valence (positive vs. negative) of emotion. Research that supports the model is reviewed, and implications for research, coaching, and training in the sport context are suggested. Key Words: threat, perception of emotion, confidence, excitement This paper explores the influence of athletes emotions on preparation and performance prior to sport competitions. There is evidence that athletes experience a range of positive and negative emotions prior to competition (Hanin, 2000; Williams & Krane, 1998). Yet much of the research in this area has only focused on the effects of anxiety on preparation and performance (for reviews see Hardy, Jones, & Gould, 1996; Jones, 1991, 1995; Parfitt, Jones, & Hardy, 1990; Raglin & Hanin, 2000). In addition, literature reviews and meta-analyses (Jones, 1995; Kleine, 1990) have indicated that anxiety has a relatively modest influence on performance. Here we highlight the importance of positive emotions. Although mostly overlooked in studies of elite or advanced level athletes, it has been argued that positive emotions can provide supportive and adaptive functions during or prior to stressful events (Fredrickson, 1998; Lazarus, Kanner, & Folkman, 1980). We review evidence indicating that athletes positive emotions are a key component in the relationship between their emotional state and their performance. In particular we focus on the cognitive appraisal of challenge as a significant influence on positive emotions prior to stressful achievement events. We also reexamine directional interpretations of emotion the perceived facilitative or debilitative effect of emotion on performance. Although the term directional interpretations The authors are with the School of Psychology, Flinders University, GPO Box 2100, Stuart Rd., Bedford Park, Adelaide 5001, South Australia. 283

2 284 / Skinner and Brewer has been used in the sport literature, we will use the simpler terms of beneficial and harmful perceptions of emotion. We focus in particular on an issue largely neglected in the literature (Hanin, 2000), the impact of beneficial perceptions of positive emotion prior to sport competitions. We highlight how positive emotions contribute to our understanding of (a) athletes ability to cope with stress and manage their own emotional experiences effectively, (b) coaching techniques to motivate and sustain peak performance, and (c) the impact of athletes experience of emotion on their performance. Finally, we present a model of beneficial and harmful perceptions of emotion that integrates challenge appraisals, valence, and perceptions of emotion. Preliminary examinations of this model in the domain of academic achievement have been supportive and suggest new directions for research in a sport context. Emotions are conceptualized in this model as mental states, and accompanying bodily feelings, that result from the evaluation of progress (or lack thereof) toward an important goal or concern and stimulate a readiness to act (Oatley & Jenkins, 1998, p. 96). Athletic and academic achievement situations present similar demands and pressures on individuals to perform at their peak with the potential for significant rewards or losses to social and self-esteem. In sport psychology, the literature on academic achievement has always provided the foundation for many significant advances in theory and applied research. Such advances include the distinction between cognitive and somatic dimensions of anxiety (Deffenbacher, 1980; Morris, Davis, & Hutchings, 1981), the concept of beneficial and harmful perceptions of anxiety (Alpert & Haber, 1960), and identification of differing types of achievement motivation (Elliot & Dweck, 1988; Nicholls, 1984). We hope this review will stimulate a new wave of research in sport psychology that seeks to adapt existing work on challenge and positive emotion to the context of sport performance. We now turn to an overview of the key concepts in our theoretical model, followed by a review of literature in academic and sport psychology that contributed to the development of this model. A detailed discussion of the model and supporting evidence is provided by Skinner and Brewer (2002). Adaptive Approaches to Competition: A Theoretical Model As Sarason and Sarason (1990) observed, individuals responses to achievement situations may range from virtual immobilization in the face of potential criticism to exhilaration at the prospect of receiving accolades (p. 477). These divergent responses reflect cognitive appraisals of threat (perceived danger to one s well-being or self-esteem and low coping confidence) and challenge (confidence that the demands of a stressful situation can be overcome and positive benefits obtained) (cf. Lazarus, 1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Smith, 1991). Similar to trait-state dimensions of anxiety, cognitive appraisals can be considered in terms of trait tendencies to perceive events in a certain way (Lazarus, 1991) or state appraisals of one event in particular. The relationship between threat appraisals, low coping expectancies, and anxiety is well established (Bandura, 1997; Beck, Emery, & Greenberg 1985; Lazarus 1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Sarason & Sarason, 1990; Skinner & Brewer, 1999). In contrast to threat, challenge appraisals are suggested to facilitate positive emotions such as enjoyment from applying effort to overcome difficulties,

3 Adaptive Approaches to Competition / 285 and eagerness or excitement in anticipation of personal benefit (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lazarus, Kanner, & Folkman, 1980). As Figure 1 shows, our model suggests that event-specific coping expectancies mediate the influence of trait threat and challenge appraisal styles on event-specific appraisals and emotion. Coping expectancies activate the meaning or significance underlying trait threat and challenge appraisals by indicating whether the fears associated with a threat appraisal style (state threat and anxiety arises) or the goals associated with a challenge appraisal style (state challenge and positive emotion develops) will be obtained (Skinner & Brewer, 2002). The model assumes a significant degree of crossover between coping expectancies concerning the management of one s external environment (e.g., the demands of test preparation) and the optimal management of one s internal states (i.e., beneficial vs. harmful perceptions of cognition and emotion). Specifically, high coping expectancies are suggested to facilitate beneficial perceptions of state appraisals and emotion. Valence of emotion is also expected to influence beneficial vs. harmful perceptions of these states. Consistent with previous conceptualizations of high anxiety as distressing and intrusive (Kent & Jambunathan, 1989; Sarason & Sarason, 1990), perceptions of harm are suggested to increase with higher levels of anxiety. In contrast, positive emotions are considered to fulfill important adaptive functions by providing temporary breaks from stress and sustaining coping efforts under prolonged demands (Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000; Lazarus et al., 1980). Therefore, higher positive emotion and cognition (i.e., state challenge) are expected to increase the perceived benefit to preparation and performance. Based on earlier findings (Skinner & Brewer, 2002), the intensity of threat appraisals is not suggested to influence the perceived impact on performance. This prediction is consistent with Lazarus (1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) argument that coping expectancies play a key role in the appraisal of threat. Specifically, fears of negative evaluation and worries about poor performance, the hallmarks of a threat appraisal, will be perceived as having an unfavorable impact on performance only when the expected ability to avoid these harms is low. Athletes Experience of Positive Emotion Research on the Individual Zones of Optimal Functioning (IZOF) model (see Hanin, 2000, for a review) has consistently found that athletes report the experience of both positive and negative emotions prior to competition. Similarly, there is evidence that positive and negative emotions are experienced prior to other kinds of stressful achievement events such as taking an academic exam (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Smith & Ellsworth, 1987). In the sport context, positive emotions such as joy have been identified as a key element of peak performance (Williams & Krane, 1998). The most common positive emotion studied in athletes, however, is enjoyment experienced during performance. Although conceptual definitions vary, enjoyment can be broadly defined as positive affective responses to the sport experience that reflect generalized feelings such as pleasure, liking, and fun (Scanlan & Simons, 1992, p. 201). Much of the research on positive emotion in sport has focused on identifying the antecedents of enjoyment and positive affect, including (a) mastery orientated motivational climate (Kavussanu & Roberts, 1996; Seifriz, Duda, & Chi,

4 286 / Skinner and Brewer Figure 1 Proposed model of threat, challenge, valence, and perception of emotion prior to achievement situations.

5 Adaptive Approaches to Competition / ), (b) task orientation (Boyd & Yin, 1996; Duda, Fox, Biddle, & Armstrong, 1992; Newton & Duda, 1993), and (c) perceptions of competence, and the social and life opportunities gained through participation (Bakker, de Koning, van Ingen Schenau, & de Groot, 1993; Scanlan, Stein, & Ravizza, 1989). Positive emotion has also been considered in the context of optimal states of flow during performance. Flow is a complex state involving the conjunction of optimal states of motivation, concentration, perceived ability, and perception, of which positive emotion is only one dimension (Csikszentmihalyi, 1988; Jackson, 1996). Since our main purpose is to explore the unique effects of positive emotion, the complex experience of flow will not be discussed in detail (see Jackson, 2000, for a review), although peak flow experiences are considered later in relation to the appraisal of challenge. Given such clear evidence that positive emotions are commonly experienced by athletes during performance, it is surprising that relatively little research has examined the consequences of these emotions for actual performance. One exception is Scanlan and colleagues research (Carpenter, Scanlan, Simons, & Lobel, 1993; Scanlan, Carpenter, Schmidt, Simons, & Keller, 1993; Scanlan & Simons, 1992) which demonstrated that enjoyment is a major factor contributing to sport commitment. Also, in a study of trait levels of positive and negative affect, Jones, Swain, and Harwood (1996) found that trait positive affect was associated with more beneficial perceptions of anxiety, whereas trait negative affect was associated with higher intensity and a less beneficial perception of anxiety. The association between trait affective states and beneficial/harmful perceptions of anxiety, however, was relatively weak, with correlations below.30. It is clear that sport enjoyment has been studied extensively. But our concern is to explore the anticipatory positive emotions that may be experienced prior to competition and their impact on athletes preparation and performance. As for what we mean by positive emotions, they are considered in terms of pleasant affective feelings that reflect a perception of advancement toward a desired end state. A second important characteristic of emotion is the level of activation or arousal involved. Russell and colleagues (Feldman, Barrett, & Russell, 1998; Russell & Carroll, 1999) have identified a range of affect categorized on the basis of valence (positive vs. negative) and activation (low vs. high). In this paper we consider both high-activation positive emotions (e.g., excitement, enthusiasm) reflecting the anticipation of favorable or desired outcomes, and low-activation positive emotions (e.g., calm, satisfaction, happiness) reflecting the achievement of valued goals and the absence of a need for action (Fredrickson, 1998). Consequences of Positive Emotion As already outlined, the consequences of positive emotions have not been researched extensively in a sport context. Our first task therefore is to show that positive emotions are likely to have concrete and important influences on athletes preparation and performance. A common theme underlying many theories of emotion is that emotional states serve adaptive functions for our physical survival or social well-being (Fredrickson, 1998; Frijda, 1986; Lazarus, 1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Smith, 1991). Anxiety, for example, is proposed to alert us to potential danger and stimulate action to counter the threat (Lazarus, 1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Smith, 1991). Similarly, worry has been identified as adaptive

6 288 / Skinner and Brewer in terms of stimulating planning and other activities to avoid or reduce a potential threat (Borkovec, 1994; Power & Dalgleish, 1997). Positive emotions may also have equally important adaptive benefits. Lazarus et al. (1980) suggest that positive emotions may benefit performance by providing breathers from stress and facilitating coping efforts when situations become increasingly demanding. This function may be critical in helping athletes sustain high levels of performance and perhaps reducing the risk of dropout during challenging training schedules over long periods of time. In line with this functional perspective on emotion, it is possible that different types of positive emotion will have different implications for functioning and well-being. Research on the IZOF model indicates that athletes commonly identify positive emotions of high (e.g., energetic, excited), rather than low (e.g., relaxed, at ease), activation as advantageous for performance (Hanin, 2000). From a functional perspective, high-activation emotions are particularly useful in sport competitions in which there is a need to direct energy and resources toward achieving a desired goal (cf. Hanin, 2000; Skinner & Brewer, 2002). Performance during actual competition represents only one context in which positive emotions can affect an athlete s overall success. It is possible that low-activation positive emotions may reduce the risk of burnout whereas high-activation emotions may support long-term intrinsic motivation and commitment (cf. Scanlan & Simons, 1992). Much of the empirical research on the effects of positive emotion has focused on cognitive processes such as information processing and memory (see Aspinwall, 1998, and Isen, 1984, for reviews). Of particular relevance to athletic performance is the effect of positive emotion on attention to and processing of negative information. Aspinwall (1998) has shown that positive emotion enhances careful consideration of negative information that is perceived to be useful, relevant, and important to the self. Athletes typically receive large amounts of performance feedback from coaches and fellow athletes, or from monitoring of their own performance. In this context the ability to consider negative self-relevant information carefully, rather than deny or avoid it, may strengthen the impact of such feedback. It has also been suggested that positive emotions play a key role in enhancing intrinsic motivation to explore, master, and engage in an activity for its own sake (Fredrickson, 1998; Pekrun, 1992). In addition, compared to negative emotion, positive emotion has been associated with increased attractiveness of approach goals, and more positive self-appraisals and expectancies of goal attainment (Kavanagh & Bower, 1985; Schwarz & Bohner, 1996). The task facing an elite competitor is extremely challenging: extended periods of rigorous training and relatively short bursts of competitive performance. Hence the importance of intrinsic motivation the capacity to remain engaged in one s sport and derive enjoyment in the sport for its own sake is very clear. There is also some evidence that positive emotion can facilitate team performance. Positive emotions have been found to increase helping behaviors (Isen, 1984), which suggests that positive emotions among teammates may enhance the ability and willingness to coordinate and cooperate, a key requirement for effective team performance. Earlier we identified a number of possible antecedents of positive emotion explored in previous research, for example mastery-orientated climate. Reviews by Hardy et al. (1996) and Rotella and Lerner (1993) on cognition, emotion, and sports performance have called for further research on adaptive approaches such as optimism and selfconfidence. These approaches are likely to be associated with positive emotions, as

7 Adaptive Approaches to Competition / 289 each involves a confident and optimistic anticipation of success. We argue that the cognitive appraisal of challenge is also likely to be a particularly strong antecedent of positive emotion in stressful achievement situations. Antecedents of Positive Emotion Optimism. Scheier and Carver (1985) defined optimism as a generalized outcome expectancy that good things will happen regardless of whether the cause resides in oneself, the environment, or chance. Optimists have been found to have higher levels of self-esteem, a more internal locus of control, and lower levels of hopelessness, depression, perceived stress, alienation, and social anxiety (Scheier & Carver, 1985). Several studies have also found that optimists experience less distress and are better adjusted than their pessimistic counterparts during stressful events such as major surgery, coping with AIDS, or starting college (Scheier & Carver, 1992). This positive outcome results from the use of an appropriate balance of problem-focused and emotion-focused coping strategies (Scheier & Carver 1985, 1992). In addition, Scheier, Carver, and Bridges (1994) found that optimists were more likely to interpret things in a positive light and to perceive opportunities for personal growth when experiencing stressful events. The research on optimism demonstrates that the general tendency to expect positive outcomes has significant benefits for both emotion and coping in response to stressful events. In a sport context, however, the optimistic anticipation of success regardless of the cause may not be the best approach. The importance of attributing good performance to internal causes is clearly reflected in research on the antecedents of successful sports performance which emphasizes variables such as perceived ability (Hall & Kerr, 1998), perceived readiness (Hanton & Jones, 1995; Jones, Swain, & Cale, 1990), and self-confidence (Hardy et al., 1996; Vealey, 1986). We believe that to understand athletes responses to sport competitions requires an approach more tailored to the demands of achievement situations. Self-Confidence. The concept of self-confidence is based on the perceived ability to succeed in certain skills or activities Bandura s concept of self-efficacy or a more general belief that one can be successful in a sport competition Vealey s concept of sport confidence (Hardy et al., 1996). Self-efficacy in a number of domains from academic tests to social interaction has been shown to improve performance and reduce task related anxiety (Bandura, 1997). Higher self-confidence in sport has been associated with better performance in a variety of sports including swimming (Burton, 1988), gymnastics (Jones, Swain, & Hardy, 1993), golf, and wrestling (Hardy et al., 1996) (see Feltz, 1988, for a review). In a meta-analysis of 45 studies, Moritz, Feltz, Fahrbach, and Mack (2000) found an average correlation of.38 between self-efficacy and sport performance. Self-confidence has also been identified as a key element of peak performance (Williams & Krane, 1998) although, as Hardy et al. (1996) acknowledge, the direction of causality between confidence and athletic success has not been clearly established. High levels of self-confidence have also been associated with lower levels of cognitive and somatic anxiety (Gould, Petlichkoff, & Weinberg, 1984; Hall, Kerr, & Matthews, 1998; Jones & Cale, 1989; Jones et al., 1990), although once again the direction of this relationship has not been clearly established. Self-confidence, seen as either task-specific or generalized self-efficacy, is clearly an important consideration in terms of an athlete s emotional responses

8 290 / Skinner and Brewer and performance. Yet it can be argued that an athlete s appraisal of a competition extends beyond evaluation of Can I do this? Will I succeed? To understand an athlete s reactions to competitive situations we must consider the personal meaning attached to such events. What significance does the competition have for an athlete s self-esteem, his or her sense of well-being, or material assets such as prize money? More specifically, what does success mean to this individual? Is the frustration of losing seen as a learning experience or as a personal failure? Smith, Haynes, Lazarus, and Pope (1993) labeled these appraisals hot cognitions, the types of thoughts that have the greatest impact on emotional responses to events. Challenge. The perception of challenge is a cognitive appraisal involving the assessment of personal meaning of an event relative to one s values, commitments, and well-being (Lazarus, 1991; Smith et al., 1993). Challenge appraisals represent effortful optimism (Smith, 1991, p. 123) focused on opportunities for success, social rewards (e.g., recognition and praise), mastery, learning, and personal growth (Lazarus, 1991, Lazarus et al., 1980; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). In other words the focus is on the potential for mastery such as increasing skills and confidence, and gain such as obtaining rewards, rather than on harm or loss (Lazarus et al., 1980). Challenge does not involve unrealistic optimism; the difficulties and demands of a stressful situation and the possibility of failure are acknowledged. Rather, challenge is based on confidence that, with sufficient effort, such demands can be overcome (Lazarus et al., 1980; Park & Folkman, 1997). This highlights an important distinction between challenge and optimism: appraisals of challenge incorporate the perception that a goal can be attained given the application of enough effort to overcome obstacles (Lazarus, 1991; Smith & Ellsworth, 1987). The notion of self-agency in challenge is similar to conceptualizations of perceived ability and self-confidence in athletes (Hall & Kerr, 1998; Hardy et al., 1996). Challenge appraisals take this concept one step further in considering the personal meaning of such striving, e.g., opportunities for learning and personal growth. The concept of challenge has particular relevance for stressful achievement situations such as sport competitions. Optimal levels of challenge have been identified as key elements of intrinsic motivation (Deci & Ryan, 1985) and peak experiences or flow during performance (Csikszentmihalyi, 1988). Similar to Lazarus (1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), Csikszentmihalyi argued that optimal levels of challenge depend more on one s perception of the match between skills and environmental demands than on the environment itself. The importance of a perceived balance between challenges and skills was also emphasized by Jackson (2000) in a review of empirical studies of athletes flow experiences. Very little empirical research has been conducted on appraisals of challenge in achievement situations. Smith and Ellsworth (1985) found that the experience of challenge prior to events such as sport competitions was described in terms of an interest and desire to attend to the situation, a requirement for a high level of effort, and moderate attributions of self-responsibility or personal control. A series of studies by Blascovich, Tomaka, and colleagues found that compared to threat, challenge was associated with higher coping expectancies, lower subjective stress, and higher perceptions of performance on mental arithmetic tasks (Blascovich & Mendes, 2000; Tomaka, Blascovich, Kelsey, & Leitten, 1993; Tomaka, Blascovich, Kibler, & Ernst, 1997). In addition, challenge elicited a unique physiological response pattern characterized by enhanced cardiac performance, with little change

9 Adaptive Approaches to Competition / 291 in blood pressure argued to represent the efficient mobilization of energy for coping (Blascovich & Mendes, 2000, p. 67). These studies suggest that challenge appraisals are likely to be associated with important benefits for athletes in terms of (a) cognitive strategies conducive to enhanced performance, e.g., high motivation, attentional focus, and confident coping expectancies, and (b) adaptive physiological response patterns likely to benefit performance. The importance of coping expectancies in producing a physiological state beneficial to sport performance has also been recognized in the sport psychology literature. In Dienstbier s (1989) physiological toughness model, the appraisal of challenge as a capacity to cope with the stressor under conditions of stress is argued to lead to an optimal tough physiological pattern of arousal, which in turn facilitates the experience of positive emotion. A tough physiological pattern of arousal entails resistance to central catecholamine depletion, peripheral catecholamine responsivity, increased beta-receptor receptivity, and cortisol suppression (Dienstbier, 1989, p. 92). Perceived threat on the other hand, as in doubts about one s capacity to cope, leads to suboptimal patterns of physiological arousal and to negative emotion. From an extensive review of the literature, Dienstbier (1989) demonstrated that optimal tough levels of arousal are associated with higher performance on physical and cognitive tasks. Consistent with Lazarus (1991), the perceived capacity to control stressors plays a pivotal role in Dienstbier s model as the primary difference between threatening and challenging situations. Similarly, Neiss (1988, 1989) has demonstrated that performance and outcome expectancies play a key role in physiological responses to motor performance situations. Neiss (1989) argued that expectancies discriminate between being psyched up in meeting a challenge from being anxious in the face of threat (p. 273). An important perspective on the role of positive emotions in preparation and performance concerns the perspective of the individual actually experiencing the emotion. Individuals are not just passive victims of their emotions, but rather, anxiety or excitement can be used to enhance preparation and performance. This notion of harmful vs. beneficial perceptions of emotion first arose in the literature on test anxiety (Alpert & Haber, 1960) and has been studied extensively in sport psychology. Beneficial and Harmful Perceptions of Emotion Perceptions of emotion refer to the perceived beneficial (encourage effort, motivate) or harmful (interfere with concentration, encourage procrastination) effects of emotion on one s efforts to prepare and perform (Alpert & Haber, 1960). They represent a type of metacognition directed toward the perceived effects of emotional states on preparation and performance (cf. Frijda, 1986; Jones, 1995). Swain and Jones (1996) found that athletes perceptions of their anxiety accounted for 18 to 23% of performance variance. They also found that the perceived beneficial or harmful effect of anxiety on performance, rather than intensity of anxiety as measured by the CSAI-2 (Martens, Burton, Vealey, Bump, & Smith, 1990), was the best predictor of performance. A key question then becomes, what are the major antecedents of beneficial perceptions of emotion? The following discussion focuses on two possibilities: coping expectancies (Carver, 1996; Jones, 1995) and valence of emotion (Skinner & Brewer, 2002).

10 292 / Skinner and Brewer Antecedents of Beneficial and Harmful Perceptions of Emotion A number of studies have explored the antecedents of athletes perceptions of their anxiety. Stronger beneficial perceptions have been linked with (a) the higher status of elite vs. non-elite athletes (Jones, Hanton, & Swain, 1994; Jones & Swain, 1995; Perry & Williams, 1998), (b) higher levels of self-confidence (Jones et al., 1993; Ntoumanis & Biddle, 1998), (c) greater confidence of goal attainment (Jones & Hanton, 1996), and (d) personality traits such as internal locus of control (Ntoumanis & Jones, 1998), trait competitiveness (Jones & Swain, 1992), and positive affect (Jones et al., 1996). Although these findings are significant in their own right, what is needed is an overall theoretical framework by which to understand the mechanisms underlying the perceptions of emotion. This would enable us to design intervention strategies to train athletes to use their emotions for maximum effectiveness because, in order to achieve long-term and stable changes, we must understand the causal mechanisms for beneficial perceptions. Jones (1995) has provided one such theoretical framework which emphasizes the importance of coping expectancies in determining beneficial vs. harmful perceptions of anxiety. Based on the work of Carver and Scheier (1986, 1988), Jones (1995) argued that confident coping expectancies enable the cognitive (e.g., worry, fears of failure) and somatic (e.g., physical tension) symptoms of anxiety to be perceived as beneficial or advantageous to performance, whereas low coping expectancies result in the perception of harm. This argument is consistent with the view of perceptions of emotion as a type of coping response applied specifically to the experience of emotion (Raffety, Smith, & Ptacek, 1997). There is some support for this perspective. Jones and Hanton (1996) found that athletes with positive expectancies of goal attainment perceived moderate levels of cognitive and somatic anxiety as mildly beneficial. In contrast, athletes with negative goal attainment expectancies perceived cognitive anxiety as neutral or mildly harmful, and perceived somatic anxiety as having a smaller beneficial effect. Consistent with the previous study, higher levels of self-confidence in athletes, defined in terms of both goal attainment and the capacity to perform well under pressure (Martens et al., 1990), have also been associated with increasingly beneficial perceptions (Jones et al., 1993). It is important to note in the interpretation of these findings that athletes only perceived moderate levels of anxiety to be mildly beneficial for preparation and performance. Indeed, Carver (1996) argued that ephemeral doubts associated with negative emotion can be overridden by a more firmly established sense of confidence about the ultimate outcome (p. 25). It is not surprising then that mild or weak levels of anxiety are not perceived as a significant source of motivation to stimulate effort, nor as a source of distraction that is difficult to control or overcome. In other words, mild or moderate levels of anxiety are only associated with mild or weak perceptions of harm or benefit. We suggest that the valence of emotion is also likely to exert a strong influence on perceptions of emotion. It has been well established that rising levels of anxiety are associated with an increase of negative thoughts (Kendall & Hollon, 1989; Kent & Jambunathan, 1989; Sarason & Sarason, 1990), in addition to problems dismissing and controlling negative thoughts (Kent & Jambunathan, 1989). This suggests that a combination of strong coping expectancies and high anxiety is fairly unlikely. Therefore we would expect that as the intensity of anxiety rises, becoming increasingly unpleasant and difficult to control, perceptions of harm would

11 Adaptive Approaches to Competition / 293 increase. Since low levels of anxiety are more easily controlled, the potential for a negative impact on performance is reduced or neutralized, with anxiety perceived as relatively neutral. There is some evidence supporting the relationship between the intensity and perception of anxiety. Higher levels of cognitive and somatic anxiety have been found to be associated with less favorable (more harmful/less beneficial) perceptions of emotion (Hale & Whitehouse, 1998; Jones & Hanton, 1996; Jones et al., 1993; 1996; Wiggins, 1998), although the strength of this relationship varies considerably in that the correlations range from.35 to.19 for cognitive anxiety and.57 to.21 for somatic anxiety. This inconsistency may be due to the failure to distinguish between positive and negative emotions prior to measuring perceptions of emotion. Burton and Naylor (1997) have argued that many items in a commonly used measure of competitive anxiety (CSAI-2, Martens et al., 1990) could be interpreted in either a positive or negative manner (e.g., I am concerned about this competition ). To our knowledge, the relationship between valence (positive vs. negative) and perception of emotion has not been explored in previous research. Positive emotions represent a pleasant approach orientation to a situation that has been linked to adaptive outcomes such as increased creativity, intrinsic motivation, and coping capacities (Fredrickson, 1998; Lazarus et al., 1980). Therefore we would expect that high levels of positive emotions, excitement for example, would be associated with beneficial perceptions. Two of our studies which show that perceptions of emotion are strongly influenced by both coping expectancies and the valence of emotion are discussed below. Our research explored perceptions of cognition prior to stressful achievement events and focused on the appraisal of threat and challenge. Preliminary tests of this model in academic achievement situations have provided consistent support (Skinner & Brewer, 2002). Sport psychology has a long history of adapting theoretical models of emotion and performance from clinical and educational psychology to the context of sports performance. Furthermore, it is increasingly recognized across various fields in psychology that positive emotions play a key role in well-being and effective functioning (cf. Seligman & Csikzentmihalyi, 2000). The focus of our model on challenge and positive emotions provides researchers with a useful starting point for exploring the role of positive emotion and cognition in sports performance. We will indicate potential applications in a sport context of the model and explore directions for future research. Putting it All Together The research described above suggests that challenge, positive emotion, and perceptions of emotion are likely to exert a significant influence on athletes preparation and performance. We have integrated these three adaptive approaches into a model of perceptions of emotion (Skinner & Brewer, 2002) as shown in Figures 2 and 3. We based the model on two studies (Skinner & Brewer, 2002) involving (a) hypothetical scenarios of common stressful events for academics (conference presentation) and students (exam), and (b) tracking 118 first-year psychology students cognitive appraisals and emotions in response to an upcoming exam over three time points: 1 week, 1 night, and 10 minutes prior to the exam. The results described here are from the second study, in which the model was expanded to include state cognitive appraisals and the prediction of performance on exams.

12 294 / Skinner and Brewer Figure 2 Model of perceptions of state challenge and emotion, and exam performance averaged across the three time points (± SD). The model is based on the results of Skinner and Brewer (2002).

13 Adaptive Approaches to Competition / 295 Figure 3 Model of perceptions of state threat and emotion, and exam performance averaged across the three time points (± SD). The model is based on the results of Skinner and Brewer (2002).

14 296 / Skinner and Brewer Structural equation modeling analyses (EQS 3.0; Bentler & Wu, 1995) on the model shown in Figure 1 indicated that the strength of the predicted relationships were generally consistent across the three time points. Consequently, Figure 2 (state challenge) and Figure 3 (state threat) show the average path coefficients, with standard deviations provided as an index of the degree of variability over time points. Path coefficients significant at one time point are not shown (see Skinner & Brewer, 2002, for more detail). Note that the equivalence of relationships across time may not occur in the context of sport performance. The strength of relationships between certain variables (e.g., threat appraisal and coping expectancies) might possibly change with closer temporal proximity to a sport competition. This clearly merits further research. As Figures 2 and 3 show, we considered two types of cognitive appraisals: threat and challenge. Cognitive appraisals were considered in terms of (a) consistent trait dispositions to perceive events in a certain way (Lazarus, 1991), and (b) state appraisals specific to a particular event. Questionnaire measures of threat appraisals tapped the perception of potential danger to one s sense of well-being or self-esteem (Lazarus 1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Sarason & Sarason, 1990; Smith, 1991), and low confidence in one s ability to cope with or overcome the potential threat (Bandura, 1997; Lazarus, 1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Smith, 1991). The challenge appraisal measures contained items reflecting the approach of effortful optimism (Smith, 1991, p. 123) and the extent to which individuals focused on opportunities for benefit and gain rather than on harm and loss (Lazarus et al., 1980). Eventspecific measures of threat and challenge were an additive combination of the intensity and frequency of cognition. Coping expectancies were assessed in relation to the anticipated ability to cope with the demands of the situation, avoid failure, and achieve a desired grade. Emotion, measured as affective states, was assessed on a bipolar scale following Russell and colleagues (Feldman Barrett & Russell, 1998; Russell & Carroll, 1999) and others (Green, Goldman, & Salovey, 1993). Two scales measured (a) positive activated (e.g., excitement) vs. negative deactivated (e.g., dejected) feelings representing the anticipation of favorable outcomes vs. the absence of this expectation, and (b) negative activated (e.g., anxiety) vs. positive deactivated (e.g., calm) emotions representing the anticipation of unfavorable outcomes vs. the absence of this expectation. The scales were labeled excitement and anxiety, respectively. The final scale used bipolar items to tap perceptions that state appraisals would have beneficial or harmful effects on preparation and performance. Threat Appraisals and Anxiety Our findings (Skinner & Brewer, 2002) on the dynamics of threat appraisals were consistent with previous research in the sport literature. Threat appraisal style was associated with lower coping expectancies, which in turn were associated with high levels of anxiety. Anxiety also increased with stronger state (eventspecific) appraisals of threat. Implications for Sport Psychology. Coping expectancies and trait and state threat appraisals all contain elements of self-efficacy, the capacity to achieve a particular goal or overcome a danger. Low self-confidence, also representing a deficit in self-efficacy, has also been associated with higher levels of anxiety in athletes (Gould et al., 1984; Hall et al., 1998; Jones & Cale, 1989; Jones, Swain, & Cale,

15 Adaptive Approaches to Competition / ). Our findings were consistent with previous research demonstrating that fears of negative evaluation, a key component of threat appraisals, are associated with higher levels of anxiety prior to competition (Leary, 1992; Wilson & Eklund, 1998). Overall, students reported mild levels of emotion and moderate levels of threat. Consistent with previous theory and research in the sport literature (Jones, 1995; Jones et al., 1993; Jones & Hanton, 1996), perceptions of mild levels of anxiety and threat were increasingly favorable with stronger coping expectancies. The finding that perceptions of emotion were less favorable as the valence of emotion moved from positive to negative supports our argument that high anxiety is most likely to be perceived as harmful, regardless of the level of coping confidence. The influence of coping expectancies on perceptions of threat appraisal was particularly strong. This indicates that fears of negative evaluation and worries about performance were only perceived to decrease performance when the perceived ability to avoid these harms was low. In sum, anxiety is not always a detrement. Precompetition nerves and performance worries in an athlete may motivate increased effort to ensure that failure is avoided as long as the athlete is reasonably confident in his or her skills and abilities. Challenge Appraisals and Positive Emotion Skinner and Brewer s (2002) findings on the dynamics of challenge and positive emotion indicate interesting areas for research in a sport context. A positive association was found between coping expectancies and positive emotions, reflecting both activation (e.g., excitement) and deactivation (e.g., calm). Consistent with the optimistic nature of challenge, event-specific appraisals of challenge were associated with higher levels of positive activated emotions such as excitement. This finding was consistent with Blascovich s research indicating that challenge appraisals result in a physiological state representing optimal coping readiness (Blascovich & Mendes, 2000). For the students themselves, positive emotions and state challenge facilitated their efforts to prepare and do well on the exam, an evaluation that was strengthened with higher intensities of positive emotion/challenge and more confident coping expectancies. In addition, higher levels of state challenge were also associated with more beneficial perceptions of mild levels of anxiety. In terms of objective performance, perceptions of challenge emerged as the most consistent predictor, accounting for 5 to 11% of the variance at each time point, whereas perceptions of threat only accounted for 4 to 7% of performance variance. Implications for Sport Psychology. These findings endorse the importance of challenge appraisals and positive emotion in stressful achievement situations. In addition, state challenge also enhanced the perception that mild levels of anxiety were beneficial. If athletes also perceive positive emotion as adaptive and beneficial, as evidence from studies on the IZOF model suggests (Hanin, 2000), this has implications for coaching and therapy techniques aimed at the management of emotion. Rather than just focusing on reducing anxiety, it may also be advantageous to focus on enhancing athletes experience of positive emotion. For example, a coach may promote a culture of acceptance and communication of positive emotions by encouraging athletes to express their excitement, hope, and enthusiasm. Also, the athlete should be encouraged to anticipate the rewards of success and the joy of masterful performance in order to build up high activation positive emotions and a state of optimistic readiness for action (cf. Frijda, 1986).

16 298 / Skinner and Brewer The adaptive benefits of challenge were further supported by the finding that beneficial perceptions of challenge, rather than threat, had the greatest positive impact on performance. In a sport context, Swain and Jones (1996) found that basketball players beneficial perceptions of cognitive anxiety, a concept similar to threat appraisals, accounted for 23% of the variance in their performance. Whether athletes perception of their challenge appraisals have a similar or stronger impact on performance is clearly a question for future research. Our findings (Skinner & Brewer, 2002) also have implications for studies on the relationship between intensity and perception of anxiety. Jones (1995) and Burton and Naylor (1997) have suggested that beneficial perceptions of anxiety may in fact represent states of high activation positive emotions. Using bipolar measurement of emotion, we found that high-activation positive emotions are perceived as beneficial for performance. This result does not directly support Jones (1995) argument. It does suggest, however, that beneficial perceptions of anxiety (a high-activation negative emotion) as measured on a unipolar scale may contain some degree of contamination or noise from high-activation positive emotions. Our findings suggest that a clear picture of how anxiety and positive emotions are perceived is best obtained using bipolar measures that differentiate between the valence and level of activation of emotion. Questions still remain concerning the dynamics of challenge and positive emotion for athletes. The types of positive emotion perceived as beneficial may vary depending on the type of sport or the temporal proximity to competition. Just as cognitive and somatic anxiety have different effects on performance depending on the demands of a particular sport (Jones, 1995; Parfitt et al., 1990), high activation positive emotions may be more likely to facilitate performance in sports requiring high levels of energy and arousal whereas low-activation positive emotions may be more likely to facilitate performance in sports requiring fine-tuned technical responses such as gymnastics. In relation to time, high-activation positive emotions may benefit training and preparation by facilitating motivation and effort, whereas low-activation positive emotions may be most beneficial immediately prior to performance by facilitating effective concentration on the task at hand. Implicit in the discussion so far is that athletes are likely to be characterized by either a threat or challenge appraisal style. We have good evidence that athletes experience a range of positive and negative emotions prior to competition (cf. Hanin, 2000; Williams & Krane, 1998), suggesting the presence of both threat and challenge appraisals. At the level of trait styles of cognitive appraisal, an athlete might also display a dual threat/challenge appraisal style in which he or she is high on cognitive anxiety, a construct similar to threat, but can nevertheless see sport competitions as challenging opportunities for success and other personal benefits. This possibility has implications for sport research. In terms of research on the effects of threat appraisal styles (e.g., high trait cognitive anxiety), it may be necessary to distinguish two subgroups: athletes with pure trait threat (i.e., low challenge or other positive trait) vs. athletes with a dual appraisal style (i.e., high threat and high challenge). Treating both groups as identical would be conceptually inappropriate. A dual threat/challenge appraisal style increases the stakes associated with competition, and hence may enhance motivation over and above a threat or challenge appraisal alone. Research in clinical psychology has identified an optimal ratio of positive to negative cognition (1.6:1.0) that defines a healthy state of mind (Kendall, Howard, & Hays, 1989). Similarly, there may be an optimal ratio of threat:

17 Adaptive Approaches to Competition / 299 challenge appraisals associated with the greatest benefit to performance. Research addressing these questions could provide valuable insight into the relationship between athletes cognitive appraisal, emotion, and sport performance. Temporal Patterns In our work (Skinner & Brewer, 2002) we also explored the temporal patterns of state appraisals, emotion, and perceptions of these states. It has been suggested that as an event draws closer, cognitive appraisal becomes more intense (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) and the threatening aspects of the event become more salient (Beck et al., 1985; Lazarus, 1991; Lewin, 1951). Consistent with this perspective, Gilovich, Kerr, and Medvec (1993, p. 556) describe the time immediately before performance as the moment of truth in which individuals may feel more accountable for their estimates of success and failure given the close proximity to objective confirmation of their skills and abilities, i.e., actual performance. In general, responses to the increasing temporal proximity of a sport competition have supported these predictions. From 1 to 2 days prior to important competitions, for example college championships, the expectancy of success (Jones, Swain, & Cale, 1991) and self-confidence (Jones & Cale, 1989; Jones et al., 1991; Swain & Jones, 1992) tends to decline whereas cognitive and somatic anxiety tend to increase (Jones & Cale, 1989; Jones et al., 1991; Swain & Jones, 1992, 1993). However, there is some disagreement as to the precise temporal changes that occur and the strength of gender differences in these patterns. We are only aware of two studies that have explored the temporal patterns of perceptions of emotion. Wiggins (1998) found that coping expectancies, intensity, and perceptions of anxiety remained relatively stable from 24 hours to 1 hour prior to three high school matches in a variety of sports. Swain and Jones (1990) found that perceptions of anxiety remained stable for 49 track & field athletes over four time points prior to competition. In our research (Skinner & Brewer, 2002) we found that, similar to research reported with athletes, students state challenge and coping expectancies decreased over time. Declines were also observed in beneficial perceptions of anxiety, state threat, and challenge. These findings suggest, contrary to previous research, that the perceived ability to utilize cognition and emotion to facilitate preparation and performance is likely to decline as a stressful achievement event draws closer. Implications for Sport Psychology. Our findings (Skinner & Brewer, 2002) are consistent with the well-established pattern of rising anxiety and faltering selfconfidence as the moment of truth in which one s skills are put to the test draws closer. In contrast, research on the temporal patterns of perceptions of emotion and appraisal is at an early stage. There is preliminary evidence that athletes may be able to maintain stable perceptions as a competition approaches. Our study with students suggests another possibility: that athletes may have a reduced capacity to utilize their emotions to the best advantage, whether this be alleviating the negative effects of anxiety or drawing upon the benefits of positive emotions. This finding is particularly significant given the well-established relationship between beneficial perceptions of emotion and enhanced performance. It is not possible to draw firm conclusions regarding the temporal patterns of perceptions of emotion on the basis of so few studies. Whether athletes perceived ability to effectively manage their emotions declines prior to competition

Interpretation of competitive trait anxiety symptoms as a function of locus of control beliefs. Nikos Ntoumanis and Graham Jones

Interpretation of competitive trait anxiety symptoms as a function of locus of control beliefs. Nikos Ntoumanis and Graham Jones 1 Interpretation of competitive trait anxiety symptoms as a function of locus of control beliefs. Nikos Ntoumanis and Graham Jones Nikos Ntoumanis, Research Support Unit, School of Education, Exeter University.

More information

Self-Efficacy And Psychological Skills During The Amputee Soccer World Cup

Self-Efficacy And Psychological Skills During The Amputee Soccer World Cup August, 2002 Volume 4, Issue 2 Self-Efficacy And Psychological Skills During The Amputee Soccer World Cup James Lowther Wimbledon Football Club Selhurst Park Stadium and Andrew Lane & Helen Lane School

More information

Emotion- & Action-focused Coping in Sport

Emotion- & Action-focused Coping in Sport Emotion- & Action-focused Coping in Sport Yuri L. Hanin Research Institute for Olympic Sports, Jyväskylä, Finland Presidential address at the ICAP, July 13, 2010 Melbourne, Australia 3/19/2013 1 Traditional

More information

CHAPTER 7: Achievement motivation, attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence. Practice questions - text book pages

CHAPTER 7: Achievement motivation, attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence. Practice questions - text book pages QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS CHAPTER 7: Achievement motivation, attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence Practice questions - text book pages 111-112 1) Which one of the following best explains achievement

More information

Competitive Anxiety and Coping Strategies in Young Martial Arts and Track and Field Athletes

Competitive Anxiety and Coping Strategies in Young Martial Arts and Track and Field Athletes Journal of Human Kinetics volume 27/2011, 180-189 DOI: 10.2478/v10078-011-0014-0 180 Section III Sport, Physical Education & Recreation Competitive Anxiety and Coping Strategies in Young Martial Arts and

More information

Examining the Relationship between Team Cohesion, Comparative Anxiety and Self-Confidence among Ethiopian Basketball Teams

Examining the Relationship between Team Cohesion, Comparative Anxiety and Self-Confidence among Ethiopian Basketball Teams Examining the Relationship between Team Cohesion, Comparative Anxiety and Self-Confidence among Ethiopian Basketball Teams Tesfay Asgedom Haddera Assistant Professor in Department of Sports Science, Addis

More information

The relationship between anxiety and performance

The relationship between anxiety and performance 2018; 3(1): 10-14 ISSN: 2456-0057 IJPNPE 2018; 3(1): 10-14 2018 IJPNPE www.journalofsports.com Received: 04-11-2017 Accepted: 05-12-2017 Research Scholar, University College of Physical Education Bangalore

More information

CHAPTER 7: Attribution theory, confidence and self-efficacy. Practice questions at - text book pages 124 to 125 QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS.

CHAPTER 7: Attribution theory, confidence and self-efficacy. Practice questions at - text book pages 124 to 125 QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS. QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS CHAPTER 7: Attribution theory, confidence and self-efficacy Practice questions at - text book pages 124 to 125 1) Weiner s model of attribution refers to: a. stable factors, such

More information

Mood and Anxiety Scores Predict Winning and Losing Performances in Tennis

Mood and Anxiety Scores Predict Winning and Losing Performances in Tennis Mood and Anxiety Scores Predict Winning and Losing Performances in Tennis Peter C. Terry (terryp@usq.edu.au) Department of Psychology University of Southern Queensland, Toowoomba QLD 43 Australia Angus

More information

Top-50 Mental Gym Workouts

Top-50 Mental Gym Workouts Top-50 Mental Gym Workouts Workout Name Overview Description Power Posing Developing A Growth Mindset Champions Time: On Time = Early Your Morning Ritual - Make Your Bed! Changing Your Story to Succeed

More information

Precompetition Anxiety and Self-Confidence in Wheelchair Sport Participants

Precompetition Anxiety and Self-Confidence in Wheelchair Sport Participants ADAPTED PHYSICAL ACTIVITY QUARTERLY, 1997,14,95-107 O 1997 Human Kinetics Publishers. Inc. Precompetition Anxiety and Self-Confidence in Wheelchair Sport Participants Elizabeth Campbell and Graham Jones

More information

The Deteriorate Function of Cognitive Anxiety on Rowing Athletes

The Deteriorate Function of Cognitive Anxiety on Rowing Athletes The International Journal of Indian Psychology ISSN 2349-3429 (e) ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) Volume 2, Special Issue, Paper ID: IJIPS20150209 http://www.ijip.in February 2015 The Deteriorate Function of Cognitive

More information

The Deteriorate Function of Cognitive Anxiety on Sepak Takraw Athletes

The Deteriorate Function of Cognitive Anxiety on Sepak Takraw Athletes The International Journal of Indian Psychology ISSN 2349-3429 (e) ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) Volume 2, Special Issue, Paper ID: IJIPS20150205 http://www.ijip.in February 2015 ABSTRACT: The Deteriorate Function

More information

WHY? A QUESTION BEHIND EVERY OUTCOME FINALLY ANSWERED

WHY? A QUESTION BEHIND EVERY OUTCOME FINALLY ANSWERED WHY? A QUESTION BEHIND EVERY OUTCOME FINALLY ANSWERED Rebeka Prosoli, Renata Barić, Martina Jurković University of Zagreb, Faculty of Kinesiology, Croatia ABSTRACT Purpose: Attribution theories investigate

More information

c) Redraw the model and place on it relevant attributions for each of the four boxes.

c) Redraw the model and place on it relevant attributions for each of the four boxes. CHAPTER 6: Attribution theory, self-efficacy and confidence, and leadership Practice questions - text book pages 107-108 1) a) Figure 6.21 partly illustrates Weiner s model of attribution. Explain the

More information

Sports Achievement Motivation and Sports Competition Anxiety: A Relationship Study

Sports Achievement Motivation and Sports Competition Anxiety: A Relationship Study Sports Achievement Motivation and Sports Competition Anxiety: A Relationship Study Zamirullah Khan E-mail: zamirullahkhan@gmail.com Zeeshan Haider (Corresponding author) E-mail: zeeshu.haider@gmail.com

More information

The Influence of Somatic Anxiety on Sport Performance among Taekwondo Athletes

The Influence of Somatic Anxiety on Sport Performance among Taekwondo Athletes EUROPEAN ACADEMIC RESEARCH Vol. II, Issue 10/ January 2015 ISSN 2286-4822 www.euacademic.org Impact Factor: 3.1 (UIF) DRJI Value: 5.9 (B+) The Influence of Somatic Anxiety on Sport Performance among Taekwondo

More information

Motivation CHAPTER FIFTEEN INTRODUCTION DETAILED LECTURE OUTLINE

Motivation CHAPTER FIFTEEN INTRODUCTION DETAILED LECTURE OUTLINE CHAPTER FIFTEEN Motivation INTRODUCTION Many of us have unrealized abilities. Some of us could run marathons, others could write novels, and still others could get straight A s in management classes. But

More information

The effect of gender differences on the level of competitive state. anxiety and sport performance among Rowing Athletes

The effect of gender differences on the level of competitive state. anxiety and sport performance among Rowing Athletes The International Journal of Indian Psychology ISSN 2348-5396 (e) ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) Volume 2, Special Issue, Paper ID: IJIPS20150507 http://www.ijip.in April 2015 The effect of gender differences on

More information

The Relationship Between Task Cohesion and Competitive State Anxiety

The Relationship Between Task Cohesion and Competitive State Anxiety Wilfrid Laurier University Scholars Commons @ Laurier Kinesiology and Physical Education Faculty Publications Kinesiology and Physical Education 2003 The Relationship Between Task Cohesion and Competitive

More information

THE RELATION GOAL ORIENTATION WITH COMPETITIVE STATE ANXIETY IN INDIVIDUAL AND TEAM ATHLETES OF CHAMPIONSHIP SPORT CENTERS OF TEHRAN CITY

THE RELATION GOAL ORIENTATION WITH COMPETITIVE STATE ANXIETY IN INDIVIDUAL AND TEAM ATHLETES OF CHAMPIONSHIP SPORT CENTERS OF TEHRAN CITY THE RELATION GOAL ORIENTATION WITH COMPETITIVE STATE ANXIETY IN INDIVIDUAL AND TEAM ATHLETES OF CHAMPIONSHIP SPORT CENTERS OF TEHRAN CITY *Tooraj Mohammadzamani 1 and Sedighe Azizi 2 Department of Physical

More information

Pre-competitive levels of arousal and self-confidence among elite and non-elite equestrian riders

Pre-competitive levels of arousal and self-confidence among elite and non-elite equestrian riders Comparative Exercise Physiology 5(3-4); 153 159 doi:10.1017/s1478061509356133 Pre-competitive levels of arousal and self-confidence among elite and non-elite equestrian riders Inga A Wolframm 1,2,* and

More information

Mindset For Optimal Performance: Essential Mental Skills DR. RICK MCGUIRE DIRECTOR OF SPORT PSYCHOLOGY ANNE SHADLE M.ED.

Mindset For Optimal Performance: Essential Mental Skills DR. RICK MCGUIRE DIRECTOR OF SPORT PSYCHOLOGY ANNE SHADLE M.ED. Mindset For Optimal Performance: Essential Mental Skills DR. RICK MCGUIRE DIRECTOR OF SPORT PSYCHOLOGY ANNE SHADLE M.ED. THINKING RIGHT IN SPORT It s all about FOCUS! Outline For Today: 1. Thinking Right

More information

Follow this and additional works at: Part of the Exercise Science Commons

Follow this and additional works at:   Part of the Exercise Science Commons The College at Brockport: State University of New York Digital Commons @Brockport Senior Honors Theses Master's Theses and Honors Projects 12-8-2016 Who Matters? The Perceived Motivational Climates Created

More information

The Differences between the Winning and Losing Team Anxiety Level and Self - Confidence in the Malaysia Final FA Cup 2016.

The Differences between the Winning and Losing Team Anxiety Level and Self - Confidence in the Malaysia Final FA Cup 2016. The Differences between the Winning and Losing Team Anxiety Level and Self - Confidence in the Malaysia Final FA Cup 2016. Mazlan Ismail 1 & Sharina Salmi Azmi 2 1 Faculty of Sports Science and Recreation,

More information

Dispositional Flow State among Open Skill Athletes: A Predictor and Quantification of Sport Performance

Dispositional Flow State among Open Skill Athletes: A Predictor and Quantification of Sport Performance International Journal of Science Culture and Sport March 2018 : 6(1) ISSN : 2148-1148 Doi : 10.14486/IntJSCS713 Dispositional Flow State among Open Skill Athletes: A Predictor and Quantification of Sport

More information

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping Amanda Palmer, Ken Sinclair and Michael Bailey University of Sydney Paper prepared for presentation

More information

Jonathan Haverkampf SELF-CONFIDENCE SELF-CONFIDENCE. Christian Jonathan Haverkampf, M.D.

Jonathan Haverkampf SELF-CONFIDENCE SELF-CONFIDENCE. Christian Jonathan Haverkampf, M.D. Christian Jonathan Haverkampf, M.D. Abstract Many people suffering from depression or other mental health conditions feel that low selfconfidence is at the root of many of their problems. Self-confidence

More information

1. Stress: any circumstances that threaten or are perceived to threaten one's well-being and thereby tax one's coping abilities

1. Stress: any circumstances that threaten or are perceived to threaten one's well-being and thereby tax one's coping abilities Chapter Three Outline: Stress and Its Effects I. The nature of stress A. Stress is an everyday event 1. Stress: any circumstances that threaten or are perceived to threaten one's well-being and thereby

More information

C H A P T E R. Leadership. Chapter 9: Leadership

C H A P T E R. Leadership. Chapter 9: Leadership C H A P T E R 9 Leadership Chapter 9: Leadership A boss creates fear, a leader confidence. A boss fixes blame, a leader corrects mistakes. A boss knows all, a leader asks questions. A boss makes work

More information

Predicting swimming performance using state anxiety

Predicting swimming performance using state anxiety Mabweazara, S. Z. et al. (2017). Predicting swimming performance using state anxiety. South African Journal of Psychology, 47(1): 110-120 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0081246316645060 Predicting swimming

More information

on motor performance Yannis Theodorakis Democritos University of Thrace F I N A L Manuscript submitted: June, 10, 1995

on motor performance Yannis Theodorakis Democritos University of Thrace F I N A L Manuscript submitted: June, 10, 1995 Theodorakis, Y. (1996) The influence of goals, commitment and self-efficacy on motor performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 8, 171-182. The influence of goals, commitment, self-efficacy and

More information

Andreas Bund University of Darmstadt. Self-Confidence and Sports Performance

Andreas Bund University of Darmstadt. Self-Confidence and Sports Performance University of Darmstadt Self-Confidence and Sports Performance Self-Confidence and Sports Performance 2. Theoretical and Motor Performance (Moritz et al., 2000) and Motor Learning (Bund, 2001) Statements

More information

Achievement goal orientations and the use of coping strategies among Winter Olympians

Achievement goal orientations and the use of coping strategies among Winter Olympians Psychology of Sport and Exercise 4 (2003) 101 116 www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport Achievement goal orientations and the use of coping strategies among Winter Olympians Anne Marte Pensgaard a,*, Glyn

More information

The Relationship between Competitive State Anxiety and Sport. Performance on Football Players

The Relationship between Competitive State Anxiety and Sport. Performance on Football Players The International Journal of Indian Psychology ISSN 2349-3429 (e) ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) Volume 2, Special Issue, Paper ID: IJIPS20150401 http://www.ijip.in March 2015 The Relationship between Competitive

More information

Psychology of Athletic Preparation and Performance

Psychology of Athletic Preparation and Performance Psychology of Athletic Preparation and Performance NOTE: This presentation relates primarily to the CSCS textbook however all Interns are responsible for the content covered in the lecture. chapter Psychology

More information

THE ANXIETY LEVEL OF HOCKEY ATHLETES AS THE COMPETITION PROGRESSED. Mary Magdalene Teh. Abstract

THE ANXIETY LEVEL OF HOCKEY ATHLETES AS THE COMPETITION PROGRESSED. Mary Magdalene Teh. Abstract THE ANXIETY LEVEL OF HOCKEY ATHLETES AS THE COMPETITION PROGRESSED By Mary Magdalene Teh Abstract This study aimed to examine if there was any significant difference in the level of cognitive anxiety,

More information

Reflect on the Types of Organizational Structures. Hierarch of Needs Abraham Maslow (1970) Hierarchy of Needs

Reflect on the Types of Organizational Structures. Hierarch of Needs Abraham Maslow (1970) Hierarchy of Needs Reflect on the Types of Organizational Structures 1 Hierarch of Needs Abraham Maslow (1970) Self- Actualization or Self- Fulfillment Esteem Belonging, Love, and Social Activities Safety and Security Psychological

More information

CHAPTER 11: Personality, attitudes and motivation

CHAPTER 11: Personality, attitudes and motivation QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS CHAPTER 11: Personality, attitudes and motivation Practice questions - text book page 147-148 1) Which one of the following is not included in the definition of personality psychology?

More information

Multiple Choice Questions

Multiple Choice Questions Multiple Choice Questions Which one of these represents intrinsic motivation? (A) Trophies (B) Medals (C) Enjoyment of the activity (D) Money Which one of these represents extrinsic motivation? (A) High

More information

Archery: Emotion Intensity Regulation to Stay in the Zone during Olympic Competition

Archery: Emotion Intensity Regulation to Stay in the Zone during Olympic Competition International Journal of Psychological Studies; Vol. 7, No. 4; 2015 ISSN 1918-7211 E-ISSN 1918-722X Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education Archery: Emotion Intensity Regulation to Stay in

More information

AROUSAL AND ANXIETY SECTION B CHAPTER 11

AROUSAL AND ANXIETY SECTION B CHAPTER 11 SECTION B CHAPTER 11 AROUSAL AND ANXIETY Peak flow theory Csikzentmihalyi derived a theory which asserts that flow is an optimal experience which is intrinsically rewarding. Figure 11.4 shows the relationship

More information

Study of Meditational Role of Self-Esteem in the Relationship Between Perfectionism and Competitive Anxiety Elite Athletes

Study of Meditational Role of Self-Esteem in the Relationship Between Perfectionism and Competitive Anxiety Elite Athletes American Journal of Psychology and Cognitive Science Vol. 4, No. 3, 2018, pp. 26-30 http://www.aiscience.org/journal/ajpcs ISSN: 2381-7453 (Print); ISSN: 2381-747X (Online) Study of Meditational Role of

More information

Future-Mindedness Glossary

Future-Mindedness Glossary Future-Mindedness Glossary At-Risk Population: segments of students/youth who are less likely to transition successfully into adulthood and achieve economic self-sufficiency, with characteristics that

More information

Psychology and performance in sport. Dr. Jane Walsh

Psychology and performance in sport. Dr. Jane Walsh 1 Psychology and performance in sport Dr. Jane Walsh Who am I? 2 Dr. Jane Walsh - Lecturer in Psychology, NUIG Sports fanatic National medal winner Track and Field (2012/13) Winner of Galway Prom Swim

More information

University of Groningen. Mastering (with) a handicap Kunnen, Elske

University of Groningen. Mastering (with) a handicap Kunnen, Elske University of Groningen Mastering (with) a handicap Kunnen, Elske IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

Psychology of Sport and Exercise

Psychology of Sport and Exercise Psychology of Sport and Exercise 13 (01) 491e499 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Psychology of Sport and Exercise journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport Goal orientations,

More information

Personnel Selection in Athletic Programs

Personnel Selection in Athletic Programs May, 2000 Volume 2, Issue 2 Personnel Selection in Athletic Programs Miguel Humara Fordham University New York, New York ABSTRACT Whether it be at a collegiate or professional level, organizations have

More information

SPORT PSYCHOLOGY PAST PAPER QUESTIONS

SPORT PSYCHOLOGY PAST PAPER QUESTIONS SPORT PSYCHOLOGY PAST PAPER QUESTIONS 3.2.3 Sport Psychology Aspects of personality Understanding of the nature vs nurture debate in the development of personality 1. Explain, in terms of personality,

More information

Original Article. Relationship between sport participation behavior and the two types of sport commitment of Japanese student athletes

Original Article. Relationship between sport participation behavior and the two types of sport commitment of Japanese student athletes Journal of Physical Education and Sport (JPES), 17(4), Art 267, pp. 2412-2416, 2017 online ISSN: 2247-806X; p-issn: 2247 8051; ISSN - L = 2247-8051 JPES Original Article Relationship between sport participation

More information

Young table tennis players involvement in the practice: a test of Eysenck and Calvo processing efficiency theory

Young table tennis players involvement in the practice: a test of Eysenck and Calvo processing efficiency theory Young table tennis players involvement in the practice: a test of Eysenck and Calvo processing efficiency theory Chia-Jung Lin 1, Yuh-Yuh Li 2, Chia-Yi Lin 3, Chiu-Ju Lu 4 1 Tainan University of Technology,

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH Volume 4, Number 1 Submitted: August 20, 2008 Revisions: October 16, 2008 Accepted: October 17, 2008 Publication Date: October 20, 2008 Start Today or the Very

More information

CHAPTER 8 SPORT AND EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY. Robin S. Vealey

CHAPTER 8 SPORT AND EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY. Robin S. Vealey CHAPTER 8 SPORT AND EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY Robin S. Vealey Chapter Objectives Discuss what scholars and professionals do in sport and exercise psychology. Describe how sport and exercise psychology evolved

More information

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 Motivation & Emotion Psychological needs & implicit motives Dr James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2016 Image source 1 Psychological needs Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6 3 Psychological

More information

Creating an Optimal Climate for Athlete Achievement & Enjoyment

Creating an Optimal Climate for Athlete Achievement & Enjoyment Creating an Optimal Climate for Athlete Achievement & Enjoyment Dr James Matthews, C. Psychol., Ps.S.I UCD School of Public Health, Physiotherapy and Sports Science IOC consensus statement on youth athletic

More information

How Mental Toughness and Hardiness Interact to Provide the Necessary Will and Skill in the Development of Elite Athletes.

How Mental Toughness and Hardiness Interact to Provide the Necessary Will and Skill in the Development of Elite Athletes. Leroy Pumphrey How Mental Toughness and Hardiness Interact to Provide the Necessary Will and Skill in the Development of Elite Athletes. EPE 556 Elite athletes are often thought to possess super human

More information

Adjustment to Retirement: The Moderating Role of Attachment. Dikla Segel, Peter Bamberger

Adjustment to Retirement: The Moderating Role of Attachment. Dikla Segel, Peter Bamberger Adjustment to Retirement: The Moderating Role of Attachment Dikla Segel, Peter Bamberger Introduction: Adjustment to Retirement Retirement has become a long and meaningful phase in the older adult s life

More information

ANALYSIS OF SOURCES OF SPORT CONFIDENCE AMONG KERALA FOOTBALL PLAYERS

ANALYSIS OF SOURCES OF SPORT CONFIDENCE AMONG KERALA FOOTBALL PLAYERS ANALYSIS OF SOURCES OF SPORT CONFIDENCE AMONG KERALA FOOTBALL PLAYERS 1 Stalin Raphel 2 Dr Anil Ramachandran ¹ Assistant Professor in Physical Education Department of Physical Education, St.Joseph s College,

More information

The Relationships Among Competitiveness, Age and Ability In Distance Runners

The Relationships Among Competitiveness, Age and Ability In Distance Runners Wayne State University Kinesiology, Health and Sport Studies College of Education 12-1-1994 The Relationships Among Competitiveness, Age and Ability In Distance Runners Robert C. Eklund University of Western

More information

CHAPTER 10 Educational Psychology: Motivating Students to Learn

CHAPTER 10 Educational Psychology: Motivating Students to Learn BEFORE YOU READ... The material included in Chapter 10 focuses on motivation: the internal processes that activate, guide, and maintain behavior. Some of the theoretical concepts associated with motivation

More information

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science Ghali Hassan Faculty of Education, Queensland University of Technology, Australia Abstract The purpose of this exploratory study was

More information

Emotions of Living Creatures

Emotions of Living Creatures Robot Emotions Emotions of Living Creatures motivation system for complex organisms determine the behavioral reaction to environmental (often social) and internal events of major significance for the needs

More information

Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers

Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers Marium Din 1, Samra Afzal 2 1,2 National University of Modern Languages, Islamabad, Pakistan dr.mariumdin@gmail.com, afzalsamra@yahoo.com

More information

Imagery and observational learning use and their relationship to sport confidence

Imagery and observational learning use and their relationship to sport confidence Journal of Sports Sciences ISSN: 0264-0414 (Print) 1466-447X (Online) Journal homepage: https://tandfonline.com/loi/rjsp20 Imagery and observational learning use and their relationship to sport confidence

More information

LEADERSHIP. Mental Preparation For Peak Performance. The USA Hockey Coaching Education Program is presented by REVISED 6/15

LEADERSHIP. Mental Preparation For Peak Performance. The USA Hockey Coaching Education Program is presented by REVISED 6/15 LEADERSHIP Mental Preparation For Peak Performance The USA Hockey Coaching Education Program is presented by REVISED 6/15 OBJECTIVES To identify a player s peak performance To identify skills and attributes

More information

THE MENTAL SIDE OF SPORTS INJURY REHABILITATION

THE MENTAL SIDE OF SPORTS INJURY REHABILITATION Welcome to an EXCITING first quarter for me! I am very pleased to share with all of you that we have re-designed our website. Dr. Alex Diaz Consulting is now a more integrated webpage aimed at learning

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 180 ( 2015 )

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 180 ( 2015 ) Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 180 ( 2015 ) 332 337 The 6th International Conference Edu World 2014 Education Facing Contemporary World

More information

Building Resilience The role of challenge and threat appraisals

Building Resilience The role of challenge and threat appraisals Building Resilience The role of challenge and threat appraisals Mark Wilson Exeter Sports Medicine Conference, Sandy Park, May 10th, 2018 Perform at your best when it matters most Choker Growth mindset

More information

A Study of Life Satisfaction and Optimism in Relation to Psychological Well-Being Among Working and Non Working Women

A Study of Life Satisfaction and Optimism in Relation to Psychological Well-Being Among Working and Non Working Women A Study of Life Satisfaction and Optimism in Relation to Psychological Well-Being Among Working and Non Working Women [1] Dr. Fareeda Shaheen Abstract: Purpose of the present investigation was to study

More information

Best Practices for Coaching the Ego-Oriented Athlete

Best Practices for Coaching the Ego-Oriented Athlete Best Practices for Coaching the Ego-Oriented Athlete Charlotte R. Stith, M.S. Health and Human Performance Oklahoma State University Timothy Baghurst, Ph.D. Health and Human Performance Oklahoma State

More information

Human Motivation and Emotion

Human Motivation and Emotion Human Motivation and Emotion 46-332-01 Dr. Fuschia Sirois Lecture 7 Sept. 28, 2006 Lecture 8 Oct. 3, 2006 Types of Motivation INTRINSIC strive inwardly to be competent and self-determining in their quest

More information

Mental Skills Training Triathletes. Establishing a Sport Psychology Process

Mental Skills Training Triathletes. Establishing a Sport Psychology Process Mental Skills Training Triathletes Establishing a Sport Psychology Process Top 10 Mental Skills Guiding Principles 1. A strong mind does not replace fitness & physical skills 2. Physical fitness & skills

More information

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question

Chapter 9 Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Motivation. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Need-Motive-Value Theories. Trivia Question Trivia Question Where did win one for the gipper come from? Chapter 9 What are the 3 components of motivation? 3 major categories of motivation. Major theories of motivation. How the theories are applied

More information

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE TEST-R

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE TEST-R We thank you for taking the test and for your support and participation. Your report is presented in multiple sections as given below: Menu Indicators Indicators specific to the test Personalized analysis

More information

Human Motivation and Emotion

Human Motivation and Emotion Human Motivation and Emotion 46-332-01 Dr. Fuschia Sirois Lecture 12 Oct. 19, 2006 Lecture 13 Oct. 24, 2006 Personal Control Expectancies about Can I do it? Will it work? If both are high then behaviour

More information

Nature of emotion: Five perennial questions. Motivation & Emotion Nature of emotion. Five questions. Outline Nature of emotion. Remaining chapters

Nature of emotion: Five perennial questions. Motivation & Emotion Nature of emotion. Five questions. Outline Nature of emotion. Remaining chapters Motivation & Emotion Nature of emotion 1. What is an emotion? Five questions 2. What causes an emotion? 3. How many emotions are there? 4. What good are the emotions? Dr James Neill Centre for Applied

More information

Motivation Motivation

Motivation Motivation This should be easy win What am I doing here! Motivation Motivation What Is Motivation? Motivation is the direction and intensity of effort. Direction of effort: Whether an individual seeks out, approaches,

More information

Gill, D.L. (1986). Competitiveness among females and males in physical activity classes. Sex Roles, 15,

Gill, D.L. (1986). Competitiveness among females and males in physical activity classes. Sex Roles, 15, Competitiveness Among Females and Males in Physical Activity Classes By: Diane L. Gill Gill, D.L. (1986). Competitiveness among females and males in physical activity classes. Sex Roles, 15, 233-257. Made

More information

Stress, Health, & Coping. Radwan Banimustafa MD

Stress, Health, & Coping. Radwan Banimustafa MD Stress, Health, & Coping Radwan Banimustafa MD Introduction: What Is Stress? Stress a negative emotional state occurring in response to events that are perceived as taxing or exceeding a person s resources

More information

Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The. Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp ). Blackwell Publishing.

Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The. Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp ). Blackwell Publishing. Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp. 864-867). Blackwell Publishing. Self-Compassion Self-compassion is an open-hearted way

More information

Motivation & Emotion. Extrinsic motivation. Outline Extrinsic motivation. James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2017

Motivation & Emotion. Extrinsic motivation. Outline Extrinsic motivation. James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2017 Motivation & Emotion Extrinsic motivation James Neill Centre for Applied Psychology University of Canberra 2017 Image source 1 Outline Extrinsic motivation Quasi-needs IM vs. EM Expected and tangible rewards

More information

The Influence of Type of Sport and Time of Season on Athletes' Use of Imagery

The Influence of Type of Sport and Time of Season on Athletes' Use of Imagery The Sport Psychologist, 1998,12,440-449 O 1998 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. The Influence of Type of Sport and Time of Season on Athletes' Use of Imagery Krista Munroe, Craig Hall, and Sharon Simms

More information

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use ACM, forthcoming. This is the author s version of the work. It is posted here by permission of ACM for your personal use. Not for redistribution. The definitive version will be published soon. Citation:

More information

Behavior Change Theories

Behavior Change Theories Behavior Change Theories Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD, FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry Behavioral change theories These theories explain the reasons behind alterations in individuals' behavioral patterns.

More information

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE Personal Report JOHN SMITH 2017 MySkillsProfile. All rights reserved. Introduction The EIQ16 measures aspects of your emotional intelligence by asking you questions

More information

Models for Involvement in Physical Activity

Models for Involvement in Physical Activity Models for Involvement in Physical Activity Why do people embrace or do not embrace a physical active lifestyle? 1 Typical Undergrad Complaints What s the point? Aren t these all the same? What does this

More information

Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution Theodosius Dobzhansky Descent with modification Darwin

Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution Theodosius Dobzhansky Descent with modification Darwin Evolutionary Psychology: Emotion, Cognition and Intelligence Bill Meacham, Ph.D. APDG, 11 May 2015 www.bmeacham.com Evolution Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution Theodosius

More information

CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK

CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK CHAPTER II CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK 2.0.0 INTRODUCTION The details about introduction, rationale of the present study, statement of the problem objectives of the study, hypotheses of the study, delimitation

More information

THE EFFECT OF COPING STRATEGIES TECHNIQUES ON SWIMMERS

THE EFFECT OF COPING STRATEGIES TECHNIQUES ON SWIMMERS Abstract THE EFFECT OF COPING STRATEGIES TECHNIQUES ON SWIMMERS Vincent A. Parnabas Faculty of Sport Science and Recreation, Universiti Teknologi MARA (UiTM), Shah Alam, 40450 Selangor, Malaysia Anxiety

More information

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155

Motivation: Internalized Motivation in the Classroom 155 24 Motivation Internalized Motivation in the Classroom Kennon M. Sheldon The motivation that students bring to a classroom setting is critical in determining how much, and how well, they learn. This activity

More information

Running head: ACADEMIC REGRETS, MOTIVATION, AND EMOTIONS. A mixed method investigation of academic regrets, motivation, and emotions

Running head: ACADEMIC REGRETS, MOTIVATION, AND EMOTIONS. A mixed method investigation of academic regrets, motivation, and emotions Running head: ACADEMIC REGRETS, MOTIVATION, AND EMOTIONS A mixed method investigation of academic regrets, motivation, and emotions Problem Regrets are a negative emotion initiated by a counterfactual

More information

Sources of Competence Information and Perceived Motivational Climate Among Adolescent Female Gymnasts Varying in Skill Level

Sources of Competence Information and Perceived Motivational Climate Among Adolescent Female Gymnasts Varying in Skill Level 396 JOURNAL / Halliburton OF SPORT & and EXERCISE Weiss PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 24, 396-419 2002 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. Sources of Competence Information and Perceived Motivational Climate Among Adolescent

More information

Psychology of Sports Injuries. Leonie Lightfoot England Athletics Coach Development Manager BASES Probationary Sport Scientist

Psychology of Sports Injuries. Leonie Lightfoot England Athletics Coach Development Manager BASES Probationary Sport Scientist Psychology of Sports Injuries Leonie Lightfoot England Athletics Coach Development Manager BASES Probationary Sport Scientist This afternoon s session Explain the importance of dealing with injuries using

More information

Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation

Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation Hsiang Chen, Rolf T. Wigand and Michael Nilan School of Information Studies, Syracuse University Syracuse, NY 13244 Email: [ hchen04, rwigand, mnilan]@mailbox.syr.edu

More information

D.O.I: Young Researchers Club, Science and Research Branch, Islamic Azad University, Kurdistan, Iran 2

D.O.I:  Young Researchers Club, Science and Research Branch, Islamic Azad University, Kurdistan, Iran 2 The Relationship between Self-Confidence and Sport Commitment with the Sport Identity of the Gymnasts... D.O.I: http:dx.doi.org/10.4127/ch.2013.0074 Bahareh Nazari 1, Sardar Mohammadi 2, Mozafar Yektayar

More information

Achievement goal and discrete emotions in sport

Achievement goal and discrete emotions in sport Psychology and Behavioral Sciences 2014; 3(5): 151-157 Published online October 30, 2014 (http://www.sciencepublishinggroup.com/j/pbs) doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20140305.12 ISSN: 2328-7837 (Print); ISSN: 2328-7845

More information

Precompetition Self-Confidence: The Role of the Self

Precompetition Self-Confidence: The Role of the Self JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 2004, 26, 427-441 2004 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. Precompetition Self-Confidence: The Role of the Self Stuart Beattie, Lew Hardy, and Tim Woodman University

More information

EVALUATION OF CHALLENGE STRESSORS: EVIDENCE FROM ISLAMIC AZAD UNIVERSITY

EVALUATION OF CHALLENGE STRESSORS: EVIDENCE FROM ISLAMIC AZAD UNIVERSITY EVALUATIO OF CHALLEGE STRESSORS: EVIDECE FROM ISLAMIC AZAD UIVERSITY DaryoushQorbani Department of Management, Germi Branch, Islamic Azad Unversity, Germi, Iran Mohammad Feizi 12 Department of Management,

More information