UNIVERSITY OF CINCINNATI

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "UNIVERSITY OF CINCINNATI"

Transcription

1 UNIVERSITY OF CINCINNATI Date: I,, hereby submit this work as part of the requirements for the degree of: in: It is entitled: This work and its defense approved by: Chair:

2 Omission Detection and Inferential Adjustment A dissertation submitted to the Division of Research and Advanced Studies of the University of Cincinnati in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTORATE OF PHILOSOPHY (PH.D.) in the Department of Marketing of the College of Business 2008 by Bruce E. Pfeiffer B.S., University of Colorado at Boulder M.B.A., Rockhurst University M.S., University of Colorado at Boulder Committee Chair: Frank R. Kardes, Ph.D.

3 ABSTRACT Omission Detection and Inferential Adjustment by Bruce E. Pfeiffer Chair: Frank R. Kardes Omission neglect refers to insensitivity to missing or unknown information. Omission detection occurs once an omission reaches the threshold of awareness. Research in this area has demonstrated that consumers typically rely heavily on the information provided, and neglect omitted information, resulting in inappropriately extreme and confidently held judgments. Three experiments have been designed investigating the underlying processes of how consumers make inferences about omitted attributes. Two plausible explanations are investigated: anchoring and adjustment versus discounting. Adjustment refers to revising or updating an opinion, whereas discounting refers to not using information that is perceived to be non-diagnostic. In Experiment 1, the underlying process is investigated using a point vs. range procedure. Salience of the omitted attribute is manipulated to elicit omission detection and participants are asked to provide either a point estimate or range estimates of the cost of the omitted attribute. Comparisons of the point and range estimates support an anchoring and adjustment explanation, but do not eliminate a discounting explanation. Experiment 2 further investigates the processing differences utilizing a cognitive load manipulation. The results suggest an anchoring and adjustment mechanism for people low in need for cognitive closure and a discounting mechanism for people high in need for cognitive closure. ii

4 Experiment 3 combines the two procedures used in Experiments 1 and 2 to more clearly differentiate the underlying process and investigates additional individual differences. The results suggests that people who are low in need for cognitive closure, people who are low self-monitors, and people who are high maximizers are more likely to utilize an anchoring and adjustment process. Conversely, people who are high in need for cognitive closure, people who are high self-monitors, and people who are low maximizers are more likely to be discounting. These experiments suggest that there are two possible underlying mechanisms for forming inferential judgments and that the type of mechanism used depends on individual trait differences and possibly situational variables. This research adds to the growing body of omission neglect research by helping to explain the underlying mechanisms in the formation of inferential judgments of omitted attribute information once an omission is detected. iii

5 Copyright 2008 Bruce E. Pfeiffer ALL RIGHTS RESERVED iv

6 DEDICATION This work is dedicated to my wife, Lisa, for her love, encouragement, support, tolerance, and patience. v

7 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This has been a challenging and rewarding process, and I would like to express my gratitude to all those who have helped me along the way. First, I would like to thank my committee chair, Frank Kardes. I cannot express how fortunate I am to have had the opportunity to learn from such a knowledgeable and accomplished researcher. He is a great mentor and friend. Thank you to James Kellaris for his endless supply of advice, wisdom, and wit. His door has always been open to me in times of need. Thank you to David Lundgren for his insightful comments and suggestions. I feel very privileged to have had him serve on my committee. Thank you to the entire marketing faculty at the University of Cincinnati for giving me the opportunity and support to achieve this goal. Specifically, I would like to thank Ph.D. coordinator, David Curry, and department head, Karen Machleit for their excellent advice and guidance throughout my time in the program. I would like to thank Paul Herr for his support, guidance, and friendship. Thank you to Donald Lichtenstein and Dipankar Chakravarti for their continued encouragement. I would like to thank my fellow doctoral students for their friendship through the good times and the suffering. Lastly, I would like to thank my wife, Lisa. Her unconditional belief in me provided me with the courage and determination to achieve this goal. vi

8 TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER ONE Introduction... 1 CHAPTER TWO Literature Review... 4 Omission Neglect and Detection... 4 Inferential Judgments of Omitted Information... 7 Need for Cognitive Closure CHAPTER THREE Experiment Method Results Discussion CHAPTER FOUR Experiment Method Results Discussion CHAPTER FIVE Experiment Method Results Discussion CHAPTER SIX General Discussion References Tables and Figures Appendix A Manipulations and Measures for Experiment Appendix B Manipulations and Measures for Experiment Appendix C Manipulations and Measures for Experiment Appendix D Individual Difference Measures vii

9 TABLES AND FIGURES TABLE 1 TABLE 2 TABLE 3 TABLE 4 TABLE 5 TABLE 6 Average Point Estimates and Rages Provided by Participants in the Salient and Not Salient Conditions (Experiment 1) Inferential Judgment Regressed on Salience and Overall Evaluation (Experiment 2) Average Point Estimates and Rages Provided by Participants in the Salient and Not Salient Conditions (Experiment 3) Inferential Judgment Regressed on Salience and Overall Evaluation (Experiment 3) Inferential Judgment Regressed on Salience and Overall Evaluation (Experiment 3) Inferential Judgment Regressed on Salience and Overall Evaluation (Experiment 3) FIGURE 1 Effect of Salience and Cognitive Load Manipulations for High and Low Need for Cognitive Closure (Experiment 2) viii

10 CHAPTER ONE Introduction Omission neglect refers to insensitivity to missing or unknown information (e.g., attributes, features, properties, qualities, alternatives, options, cues, stimuli, or possibilities) (Kardes and Sanbonmatsu, 2003). In many consumer decision making situations, consumers need to form judgments based on incomplete information. The information may come from different sources (e.g., advertising, promotions, salespeople, the media, independent, word-of-mouth). Advertisers may focus on specific attributes in which they have an advantage over their competition. For instance, some truck ads focus on towing capacity and fully boxed frames while others may focus on passenger comfort, quiet ride, or even fuel economy. Advertisers choose what to present based on how their distinct advantages match their target segment. Similarly, other information sources have their own agendas. Regardless of the source, the information available rarely includes all the attribute information that may be important in forming a judgment. In these cases, consumers usually use whatever information is available, neglecting or even failing to notice what might be missing. Omission detection occurs once an omission reaches the threshold of awareness. For example, Fibersure advertises its product by asking consumers what is missing from their diet. They show various foods with large holes cut out of them, informing consumers, you d be surprised at how much fiber is missing from your diet new Fibersure, it s just what you ve been missing. In these ads, Fibersure, makes a common 1

11 missing attribute salient, and then provides a solution, strengthening the perceived benefits of the product. Not bringing omissions to consumers awareness is, however, much more common, and research has demonstrated that omission detection is surprisingly difficult and the failure to detect omissions is often the norm. Research in this area has demonstrated that consumers typically rely heavily on the information provided and neglect the omitted information, and this results in inappropriately extreme and confidently held judgments (Sanbonmatsu, Kardes, and Sansone, 1991; Sanbonmatsu, Kardes, and Herr, 1992; Sanbonmatsu, Kardes, Posavac, and Houghton, 1997; Sanbonmatsu, Kardes, Houghton, Ho, and Posavac, 2003). In general, people should only form extreme attitudes when they have a large amount of relevant information (Anderson, 1981). Judgments made with limited information should be more conservative. When information is incomplete or limited, moderate judgments will be more accurate than extreme judgments (Griffin and Tversky, 1992). It has been shown that more moderate judgments are made when people are sensitive to the omission of information (Kardes et al., 2006; Sanbonmatsu et al., 1991, 1992, 1997, 2003). Up to now, the omission neglect literature has focused on sensitivity to omissions and how this sensitivity debiases omission neglect through increased omission detection. We are attempting to separate sensitivity to omissions from the effects of adjustment following omission detection. Specifically, we will be looking at the process by which people adjust their inferences about missing information once the omission is detected. 2

12 Three experiments have been designed investigating the underlying processes of how consumers make inferences about omitted attributes. In this research, two plausible explanations are investigated: anchoring and adjustment and discounting. Adjustment refers to revising or updating an opinion, whereas discounting refers to not using information that is perceived to be non-diagnostic. Two different methods available in the literature are used for investigating these underlying mechanisms. 3

13 CHAPTER TWO Literature Review Omission Neglect and Detection As stated earlier, omission neglect refers to insensitivity to missing or unknown information (Kardes and Sanbonmatsu, 2003). Conversely, omission detection refers to sensitivity to missing or unknown information and occurs once an omission reaches the threshold of awareness. Research, however, has demonstrated that omission detection is surprisingly difficult, and the failure to detect omissions often results in inappropriately extreme and confidently held judgments (Sanbonmatsu et al., 1991, 1992, 1997, 2003). For example, Sanbonmatsu et al. (1992) presented participants with attribute information about a camera and asked them to form evaluations. Half the participants received positive information on four attributes, and half received positive information on eight attributes. People should form more favorable evaluations of the camera when they are provided with positive information on eight attributes as opposed to only four. This was, however, only the case for highly knowledgeable people (vs. unknowledgeable or moderately knowledgeable). Only the highly knowledgeable people detected the omission. Unknowledgeable or moderately knowledgeable people formed equally favorable evaluations. These people neglected the omission. Thus, their evaluations based on only four positive attributes were overly extreme given the asymmetry in evaluative information. Research has demonstrated that omission neglect influences all the stages of information processing: perception (Baron and Ritov, 2004; Caputo and Dunning, 2005; Masuda and Nisbett, 2006; Treisman and Souther, 1985), learning (Cronley, Posavac, 4

14 Meyer, Kardes, and Kellaris, 2005; Kardes, Cronley, Kellaris, and Posavac, 2004; Newman, Wolff, and Hearst, 1980), evaluation (Kardes and Gurumurthy, 1992; Kardes and Sanbonmatsu, 1993; Sanbonmatsu et al., 1992, 1997, 2003; Zhang, Kardes, & Cronley, 2002), persuasion (Sanbonmatsu et al., 1997; Tormala and Petty, 2007), and decision making (Kardes et al., 2006; Mantel, Kardes, Isen, and Herr, 2008; Pfeiffer et al., 2008; Sanbonmatsu et al., 1991; Silvera, Kardes, Harvey, Cronley, and Houghton, 2005; Simonson and Kivetz, 2000). It has been shown that more moderate judgments are made when people are sensitive to the omitted information and omissions are detected (Kardes et al., 2006; Sanbonmatsu et al., 1991, 1992, 1997, 2003). Sanbonmatsu, Kardes, and their colleagues have demonstrated that omission detection can be accomplished through manipulating motivation and context. For example, more moderate judgments were made when people were warned that given information was incomplete (Sanbonmatsu et al., 1992), when people had high levels of prior product knowledge (Sanbonmatsu et al., 1991, 1992, 2003), when comparisons between objects were described in various levels of detail (Sanbonmatsu et al., 1997), when comparing objects described by different attributes (Sanbonmatsu et al., 2003), and when people were asked to consider the judgment criteria prior to evaluation (Kardes et al., 2006; Sanbonmatsu et al., 2003;). Further, it has been demonstrated that changes in information format from the initial evaluation to the secondary evaluation can influence sensitivity to missing information and thus the degree of evaluative revision (Muthukrishnan and Ramaswami, 1999). In Experiment 1, Muthukrishnan and Ramaswami (1999) presented participants with positive information about a writing pen in either a comparative or noncomparative 5

15 format. In the noncomparative format, six pieces of attribute information were presented about the target pen. In the comparative format, the target pen was compared to another pen on three attributes. All three attributes favored the target pen. Following an initial evaluation of the target pen, participants were provided with additional information. This time the information was negative. Again, the information was presented in a comparative or noncomparative format. They found that the amount of evaluative change from evaluation one to evaluation two depends on change of format. The greatest evaluative correction occurred when the format changed from a noncomparative to comparative format, or vice versa. In the following experiments, they found that the evaluative correction was mediated by perceived insufficiency of information, demonstrating that the effect was driven by sensitivity to omissions. Up to now, the omission neglect literature has focused on sensitivity to omissions and how this sensitivity debiases omission neglect. We are attempting to separate sensitivity to omissions from the effects of adjustment following omission detection. When consumers become aware that attribute information is missing or are asked to evaluate the importance of an attribute that was not presented, they are forced to make an inferential judgment. Inferential judgments are judgments that go beyond the information that is presented. The question then is how do people go about making these inferential judgments about missing information. In this research, we investigate two plausible explanations: anchoring and adjustment versus discounting. Adjustment refers to revising or updating an opinion, whereas discounting refers to not using information that is perceived to be non-diagnostic. 6

16 One strategy for forming inferences about an unknown is to start with what one does know and then adjust to a reasonable estimation. This is known as the anchoring and adjustment heuristic (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974). In the case of omission neglect, people may anchor on the implications of the presented information and adjust based on the implications of the omitted information. When missing information is made salient, people may engage in effortful processing in order to adjust their inference to a plausible estimate based on the implications of the missing information. Their final inferential evaluation should depend on whether the omissions are detected and on their motivation and ability to adjust away from the initial anchor. Conversely, people may perceive the presented information or their initial evaluation based on this information as non-diagnostic to their inferential judgment of the missing attribute information. As a result, they may discount this information and base their inferential judgment solely on the implications of the omitted information. Inferential Judgments of Omitted Information In most omission neglect studies, people are presented with some but not all of the relevant information on a product or service and asked to evaluate the product or service. As discussed above, the omission neglect literature has shown that people form more moderate evaluations when omissions are detected, implying that the overall evaluations are based on both the implications of the presented information and the implications of the omitted information. When consumers are asked about an attribute that was not presented, they are forced to make an inferential judgment. How do people go about making these inferential judgments about missing information? As stated above, 7

17 we investigate two plausible explanations: anchoring and adjustment versus discounting. Adjustment refers to revising or updating an opinion, whereas discounting refers to not using information that is perceived to be non-diagnostic. In their seminal article Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, Tversky and Kahneman (1974) identified three heuristics people use in judgment and decision making: representativeness, availability, and anchoring and adjustment. Anchoring and adjustment refers to a specific cognitive process by which people initially focus on an anchor and then adjust toward a final estimate. For example, one group of people were asked whether the percentage of African countries in the United Nations was more or less than 10% and another group was asked whether the percentage was more or less than 65%. Those in the first group estimated that the actual percentage was 25% and those in the second group estimated that the actual percentage was 45%. The initial inquiry (more or less than 10% vs. more or less than 65%) served as an anchor or starting point from which people adjust to form their final estimate (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974). Similarly, Strack and Mussweiler (1997) found that different initial anchors resulted in different estimates of Gandhi s age when he died. If they were asked if he died before or after the age of 140, they provided an average estimate of 67 years old. If they were asked if he died before or after the age of 9, they provided an average estimate of 50 years old. People focused on the initial anchor (140 or 9) and adjusted to a more plausible estimation. Anchoring effects have proven to be very robust and have been demonstrated in many different domains from evaluation of gambles (Carlson, 1990; Chapman and 8

18 Johnson, 1994; Schkade and Johnson, 1989) to judgments of future performance (Switzer and Sniezek, 1991). Further, the anchoring and adjustment heuristic has been very useful in helping to explain a variety of phenomena (e.g., preference reversals, subadditivity, social comparison, egocentric biases, hindsight bias, probability estimates, and trait inference) (Chapman and Johnson, 2002; Epley and Gilovich, 2002; Epley, 2004). Interestingly, it has even been shown that people use an anchor even when the anchor is overly extreme or even implausible (Strack and Mussweiler, 1997), and even when they are explicitly made aware of the anchoring effect (Quattrone, Lawrence, Finkel, 1981; Wilson, Houston, Etling, and Brekke, 1996). Despite the robustness of the effect, not all information makes effective anchors. Research suggests that the most effective anchors are ones that receive sufficient attention and are relevant to the target judgment (Chapman and Johnson, 2002). Epley and Gilovich (2006) demonstrated that adjustment is effortful, and whether people adjust away from the anchor depends on their ability and motivation to seek more accurate estimates. In three studies, they found that people who scored high in need for cognition, people who were sober, and people who were not under cognitive load provided estimates that were further from self-generated anchors than people who were low in need for cognition, people who were intoxicated, and people who were not under cognitive load (respectively). Another explanation for inferential judgment formation is discounting. People may discount the implications of the presented attribute information and base their inferential judgment primarily on the implications of the omitted attribute information. Research has demonstrated that people discount information that they perceive as 9

19 irrelevant or non-diagnostic (e.g., Shul and Burnstein, 1985; Van Overwalle and Labiouse, 2004). Discounting depends on competition between information. The attribution literature has demonstrated that people tend to ignore alternative causes when one of the causes is perceived as primary or acquires a strong causal weight (e.g., Hansen and Hall, 1985; Kruglanski, Schwartz, Maides, and Hamel, 1978; Van Overwalle and Van Rooy 2001). For example, Van Overwalle and Van Rooy (2001) used a variety of novel stories depicting attribution scenarios to demonstrate that more observations of an alternative cause resulted in discounting of the target cause. Trope and Gaunt (2000) demonstrated that when situational information was salient, accessible, or specific, it elicited a strong discounting effect of dispositional inference. Further, in a product evaluation task, Simmons and Lynch (1991) demonstrated that perceived incompleteness of information resulted in less sensitivity and attention to presented information. They used a competitive information context to increase the salience of undescribed attribute information, attenuating of the effects of the described attribute information on product evaluations. The formation of an inferential judgment of a missing attribute may also depend on competition between information. If the implications of the omitted information is salient and viewed as sufficiently relevant, the presented attribute information may be discounted and not used in the inferential judgment. As discussed above, we are investigating two plausible explanations for the underlying process involved in the formation of inferential judgments of missing attribute information: anchoring and adjustment versus discounting. These two processes may depend on how information is encoded. Schul and Burnstein (1985) demonstrated that 10

20 discounting depends on discrete versus integrated encoding. The type of encoding determines the number of associations formed among different pieces of information. Discounting is more likely in cases where the encoding is discrete and less likely when encoding is integrative. Discrete encoding takes place when information can be understood individually. Integrative encoding takes place when the information is combined and considered as a whole. Once information is integrated, all the information is likely to be seen as diagnostic. As a result, an inference about one piece of information is likely to be based on all the information available. If the information is not integrated, however, it may not be as strongly connected. As a result, when asked about one specific attribute, people may be less likely to view the other attribute information as diagnostic. This enables them to discount the other information and based their inference only on the implications of the omitted information. We believe that anchoring and adjustment requires more of an integrated focus requiring a more effortful adjustment. Once the information has been integrated, people are more likely to consider all the information available when forming their inferential judgment about the omitted attribute. This would include all the presented attribute information and their overall evaluation. They anchor on the implications of the presented attribute information and then adjust based on the implications of the omitted attribute information. We believe that discounting is a less involved process. If the information is not integrated, people can focus on any of the pieces of information individually. If the information in encoded discretely, people are able to focus only on the most diagnostic of 11

21 the information. If this is the implications of the omitted attribute information, then they may discount the implication of the presented attribute information and any initial evaluation based on this information, not using it in the formation of their inferential judgment. We use two methods available in the literature for investigating these underlying mechanisms. Epley, Keysar, Van Boven, and Gilovich (2004) use a point versus range procedure to look at the role of anchoring and adjustment in egocentric biases. They found that people estimate the perspective of others by first anchoring on their own perspective and then adjusting. They used ambiguous messages that could either be interpreted as sincere or sarcastic. Participants first read clarifying information about past events making it clear that the message was either sincere or sarcastic. Then participants read the ambiguous message. After the participants had read all the information, they asked half the participants to estimate the number of their peers who would interpret the message (without the clarifying information) as sincere (vs. sarcastic). The other half of the participants were asked to provide a plausible range of this estimate by estimating upper and lower bounds of sincere (vs. sarcastic). For example, all participants were asked to imagine that 100 people were asked to read the (ambiguous) message. The participants in the point condition were then asked to estimate the percentage of these 100 people who thought the message was sincere (vs. sarcastic). The participants in the range condition were asked to provide two estimates, an upper and lower bound of the percentage of people who thought the message was sincere (vs. sarcastic). This betweenparticipant measure was used to ensure that the point generation would not influence the ranges estimations or vice versa. They found that participants point estimates were 12

22 skewed in the direction of the upper or the lower bounds provided by the participants in the range condition. Sarcastic event clarification information resulted in point estimates that were skewed toward the sarcastic end of the range estimates and sincere event clarification information resulted in point estimates that were skewed toward the sincere end of the range estimates. Another method for investigating the underlying process is to place participants under cognitive load (e.g., Chun and Kruglanski, 2006; Epley and Gilovich, 2006). As discussed previously, adjustment is effortful and depends on people s ability and motivation to seek more accurate estimates. Placing people under cognitive load reduces peoples ability to perform effortful cognitive tasks. To manipulate cognitive load, Epley and Gilovich (2006) randomly divided participants into load or no load conditions. Participants in the load condition where asked to memorize an eight-letter string prior to answering questions. Participants in the no load condition did not perform the memorization task. They found that people who were not under cognitive load provided estimates that were further from self-generated anchors on various topics than people who were not under cognitive load, demonstrating that cognitive load interfered with participant ability to adjust away from an anchor. Anchoring and adjustment is a two-step process requiring more cognitive effort than discounting. Since anchoring and adjustment has been shown to depend on ability and motivation, we expect that a cognitive load manipulation should interfere more with the anchoring and adjustment process than the discounting process. The specific use of the point versus range method and the cognitive load method will be described in more detail later in the method sections. 13

23 The Need for Cognitive Closure To aid in investigating the distinction between the two inferential judgment processes, we will also be measuring the need for cognitive closure (Kruglanski and Webster, 1996). The need for cognitive closure refers to a desire to form a definite opinion ( seize ) and to maintain the opinion once it is formed ( freeze ). The need for cognitive closure is conceptualized as a continuum anchored at one end with a high need for closure and at the other with a high need to avoid closure. As need for cognitive closure increases, people s desire to form an opinion increases, and they tend to focus on information that will facilitate closure. As a result, people may form a snap decision or leap to judgment based on incomplete information. Once an opinion is formed, the opinion becomes rigid and people tend to safeguard the opinion and show resistance to change. Conversely, as need for cognitive closure decreases, people s desire to avoid closure increases, and they become more reluctant to commit to a definite opinion. The robustness of the cognitive closure construct has been shown as both a situational variable and as individual difference variable (Webster and Kruglanski, 1994). For example, need for cognitive closure has been shown to increase with time pressure (Cronley, Posavac, Meyer, Kardes, and Kellaris, 2005; Kruglanski and Freund, 1983; Kruglanski and Webster, 1996; Sanbonmatsu and Fazio, 1990; Suri and Monroe, 2003) and fatigue (Kruglanski, 1989, Kruglanski, 1990; Webster, Richter, and Kruglanski, 1996) and to decrease with accountability (Kruglanski, 1989; Kruglanski and Freund, 1983; Tetlock, 1985; Webster, Richter, and Kruglanski, 1996) and enjoyableness of the decision task (Webster, 1993). 14

24 To investigate need for cognitive closure as an individual trait difference, Webster and Kruglanski (1994) developed the Need for Cognitive Closure Scale. The original version is a 42-items scale that has five facets: order, predictability, decisiveness, ambiguity, and close-mindedness. More recent research has divided the scale into two orthogonal subscales: need for structure and decisiveness (Hirt, Kardes, and Markman, 2004; Neuberg, Judice, and West, 1997) that relate more directly to the seizing and freezing process described above. Need for cognitive closure and has been shown to moderate a large variety of judgment phenomena. For example, research has demonstrated that people who are high in need for cognitive closure exhibit exaggerated correspondence bias (Webster, 1993), are more susceptible to the disrupt-then-reframe influence technique (Kardes, Fennis, Hirt, Tormala, and Bullington, 2007), and are influenced more by price when forming inferences about quality (Cronley et al., 2005). Based on the findings of Kardes et al. (2006), people high in need for cognitive closure may be less sensitive to missing information and more sensitive to debiasing procedures that increase the salience of the missing attribute information. Whereas, people low in need for cognitive closure may be more sensitive to missing information and less sensitive to the debiasing technique. Since people who are high in need for cognitive closure have a desire to form quick judgments ( seize ) and to maintain the judgments as long as possible ( freeze ), they are more likely to draw snap judgments that may involve more heuristic processing and may be more likely to ignore difficult-toprocess information (e.g., missing information). In contrast, people who are low in need for cognitive closure are more concerned with accuracy and less likely to form snap 15

25 decisions, resulting in more systematic processing. As a result, they may be more sensitive to information that is difficult to process (e.g., missing information). Further, Kruglanski and Webster (1996) suggest that selective information processing increases as need for cognitive closure increases. People are more likely to focus on early cues and consider less evidence before forming a judgment. This early cue utilization may be similar to an anchoring effect representing a form of seizing. Kruglanski and Freund (1983) use time pressure to manipulate need for cognitive closure. They demonstrate that anchoring is more pronounced when participants are under high (vs. low) time pressure. Therefore, people who are high in need for cognitive closure may be more likely to anchor on initial important information and be less likely to adjust once the anchor is activated. In omission neglect studies, there are two possible anchors on which people high in need for cognitive closure might seize and freeze when forming their inferential judgments: the implications of the presented information or the implications of the omitted information. If they anchor on the implications of the presented information and do not adjust then their inferential judgment of the omitted attribute should be the same regardless of whether the omission is detected or not. If they anchor on the implications of the omitted attribute and do not adjust, then their inferential judgment of the omitted attribute should be more moderate. 16

26 CHAPTER THREE Experiment 1 In Experiment 1, the underlying process of inferential judgment formation of omitted attribute information is investigated using the point versus range procedure (e.g., Epley et al., 2004). In this study, we manipulate the salience of the omitted attribute, measure either point or range estimates of the cost of the omitted attribute and measure need for cognitive closure. Salience is manipulated to elicit awareness of the omitted attribute, resulting in omission detection. The point vs. range procedure requires that the participants be randomly assigned to either a point estimate condition or a range estimate condition. Our main prediction is that omission detection will result in greater adjustment away from the anchor. Using this methodology, the point estimate is compared to the lower range estimate (the anchor) to see if it differs significantly. Nonsignificance reflects no adjustment whereas significance reflects adjustment. Further, different levels of cognitive closure may result in different processing in the formation of the inferential judgment of the omitted attribute. People low in need for cognitive closure are expected to follow our main prediction for the point versus range procedure described above. They should anchor on the implications of the presented information and adjust based on the implications of the omitted information once it is detected. People high in need for cognitive closure, however, are more likely to anchor ( seize ), but not adjust ( freeze ) using more of a discounting process. If they anchor on the implications of the presented information but do not adjust, they will provide point estimates of the omitted attribute that are not significantly different than the lower range estimate regardless of omission detection. If, instead, they anchor on the implications of 17

27 the omitted information but do not adjust, they will provide point estimates that differ significantly from the lower range estimates when the omission is detected. If the later occurs, their point versus range estimates should not differ from those of the people low in need for cognitive closure. If this is the case, then the point versus range estimate might not be sensitive enough to distinguish between anchoring and adjustment versus discounting. Method A 2 (Salience of Omitted Attribute: Salient or Not) x 2 (Need for Cognitive Closure: High or Low) between subjects design was used. Participants were asked to imagine that they were shopping for a new cell phone plan and provided with a brief description. The primary depended variables of interest were the point versus range estimates and the overall evaluation of the cell phone plan. Manipulations and measures for Experiment 1 can be found in Appendix A. Salience of the Omitted Attribute. The salience of the omitted attribute, roaming fee, was manipulated by either not listing the attribute in the description or listing the attribute information as unknown. By listing the attribute as unknown, it was made salient to the participants that information was omitted from the description, increasing the sensitivity to the omitted attribute information, eliciting omission detection. Need for Cognitive Closure. Need for Cognitive Closure (NFCC) was measured using a 42-item scale (Webster and Kruglanski, 1994). The complete scale can be found in 18

28 Appendix D. After participants completed all the dependent measures and the 42-item Need for Cognitive Closure Scale, a need for cognitive closure index was created by summing the scores (α = 0.83). A median split was performed blocking the participants into either high or low need for cognitive closure conditions. Point vs. Range Estimates. People in the point estimate condition were asked to provide a cost estimate of what they believed the omitted attribute, roaming fee, to be. They were asked to indicate their estimate of the roaming fee on an 11-point scale ($0.00 to $2.00 at $0.20 increments). People in the range estimate condition were asked to provide an estimated upper and lower range for the omitted attribute, roaming fee. What do you assume is the lowest possible roaming fee? What do you assume is the highest possible roaming fee? They were provided with an 11-point scale ($0.00 to $2.00 at $0.20 increments) for each question. Overall Evaluation. Participants indicated their evaluations of the cell phone plan on a 7- point scale ranging from 1 (extremely bad) to 7 (extremely good). Ninety-two undergraduate business students participated for course credit. The participants were randomly assigned to the experimental conditions. Participants were seated at a computer and were informed that the purpose of the study was to investigate consumers attitudes and perceptions. MediaLab software was used to administer the experiment. Participants were asked to imagine that they were shopping for a new cell phone plan. They read a brief description of a new cell phone plan that contained positive 19

29 information on four attributes (anytime minutes, service coverage area, monthly fee, nights and weekends); however, no information was provided about the omitted attribute, roaming fee. They were asked to read the description and evaluate the plan. Once they had completed this task they were not allowed to return to the information page. The participants were then asked to provide an either a point estimate or upper and lower range estimates of the cost of the roaming fee. This was followed by the measure of overall evaluation of the cell phone plan, and the measure of need for cognitive closure. Results A 2 (Salience of Omitted Attribute) x 2 (NFCC) ANOVA was performed on the overall evaluation of the cell phone plan yielding a main effect for Salience (F(1, 88) = 5.28, p <.05). Participants overall evaluations for the plan were greater when the omitted attribute was not salient than when it was salient (Ms = 5.07 vs. 4.58). This finding supports prior debiasing omission neglect findings and confirms that our salience manipulation was effective. No other main effects or interactions were expected or found. Our main prediction for Experiment 1 was that participants in the salience condition would provide point estimates of the roaming fee that are significantly higher than the low range estimates provided by participants in the range condition. The salience of the missing attribute information would result in greater adjustment away from the lower end of the estimated range. This is precisely what we found. The results are presented in Table 1. When the omitted attribute was not salient, the point estimates of the cost was not significantly different than the lowest cost estimate (Ms = $0.57 vs. $0.31 respectively; t(45) = 1.87, ns.). Participants did not adjust away from the lowest 20

30 cost estimate. When the omitted attribute was salient, however, the point estimates of cost differed significantly from the lowest cost estimate (Ms = $1.00 vs. $0.49 respectively; t(43)= 3.18, p <.01). They adjusted their estimated cost upward to a more moderate level. This result did not depend on need for cognitive closure. These results are consistent with an anchoring and adjustment explanation for people who are low in need for cognitive closure, but is inconclusive for people who are high in need for cognitive closure, they could either be anchoring and adjusting or discounting. Discussion In experiment 1, we replicated previous findings by demonstrating that making people aware of omissions results in more moderate overall evaluations. Further, the point versus range measure demonstrated an anchoring and adjustment mechanism. When the omitted attribute information was not salient, we found no significant difference between the lowest cost estimate and the point estimate. People appear to be using the positive attribute information to infer a positive (low cost) estimate of the omitted attribute. When the omitted attribute is salient, however, people provide a significantly higher cost estimate of the omitted attribute. This suggests that people are using the presented attribute information as a cue or reference point. They are anchoring on the implications of the presented attribute information and are adjusting to a more moderate estimate based on the implications of the omitted attribute. Since we did not find an effect of need for cognitive closure in Experiment 1, it is quite possible that the point vs. range procedure may not be sensitive enough to distinguish between anchoring and adjustment and discounting. In the point vs. range 21

31 procedure, either mechanism could result in the higher cost estimate for the omitted attribute when it is salient. People who are low in need for cognitive closure may be anchoring on the implications of the presented information and adjusting based on the implications of the omitted attribute information resulting in a higher cost estimate. People who are high in need for cognitive closure may be discounting their overall evaluations in the inferential judgment, resulting in a similar higher cost estimate. Both may be giving a higher cost estimate but using a different mechanism. As a result, it is necessary to investigate further the differences between anchoring and adjustment and discounting using a different procedure. We do this in Experiment 2, utilizing a cognitive load manipulation. 22

32 CHAPTER FOUR Experiment 2 In Experiment 2, the underlying process of inferential judgment formation of omitted attribute information is investigated using a cognitive load manipulation. In this study, we manipulate the salience of the omitted attribute, manipulate cognitive load, and measure need for cognitive closure. As in Experiment 1, salience is manipulated to elicit awareness of the omitted attribute, resulting in omission detection. It is expected that people will form more positive inferential judgments of the omitted attribute when the omission is neglected than when it is detected. Anchoring and adjustment is thought to be a more effortful than discounting since it is a two-step process. As a result, the cognitive load manipulation should interfere with the anchoring and adjustment process but not the discounting process. People who are under cognitive load should not be able to adjust away from their initial anchor. This should result in more positive inferential judgments of the omitted attribute when people are under cognitive load than when they are not. If people are discounting, however, the cognitive load manipulation should have no effect on the inferential judgment. Although the point versus range procedure in Experiment 1 may not have been sensitive enough to distinguish between anchoring and adjustment and discounting, it did suggest that if people are discounting they are cuing on the implications of the omitted attribute not the implications of the presented information. Therefore, regardless of the load manipulation, people should provide more moderate inferential judgments. Further, different levels of need for cognitive closure may result in different processing in the formation of the inferential judgment of the omitted attribute. People 23

33 who are high in need for cognitive closure have a desire to form quick judgments ( seize ) and to maintain the judgments as long as possible ( freeze ). They are more likely to draw snap judgments that may involve more heuristic processing. People who are low in need for cognitive closure are more concerned with accuracy and less likely to form snap decisions resulting in more systematic processing. It is expected that people low in need for cognitive closure should be more inclined to use all of the information that is available to them in an effort to maximize accuracy of the inferential judgment. Therefore, they should anchor on the implications of the presented information. Once the omission is detected, they should adjust based on the implications of the omitted information, resulting in a more moderate final inferential judgment. People high in need for cognitive closure, however, should have a desire to form a quick judgment, using only the information that is necessary for inferential judgment formation. As a result, they should be more likely to anchor ( seize ) on what they perceive to be most diagnostic, but not adjust ( freeze ), using more of a discounting process. Method A 2 (Salience of Omitted Attribute: Salient or Not) x 2 (Cognitive Load: Load or Not) x 2 (Need for Cognitive Closure: High or Low) between subjects design was used. As in Experiment 1, participants were asked to imagine that they were shopping for a new cell phone plan and provided with a brief description. The primary depended variables of interest were the inferential judgment of the omitted attribute and overall 24

34 evaluation of the cell phone plan. Manipulations and measures for Experiment 2 can be found in Appendix B. Salience of the Omitted Attribute. The salience of the omitted attribute, roaming fee, was manipulated by either not listing the attribute in the description or listing the attribute information as unknown. By listing the attribute as unknown, it was made salient to the participants that information was omitted from the description, increasing the sensitivity to the omitted attribute information, eliciting omission detection. Cognitive Load. Cognitive load was manipulated by having the participants silently rehearse a 9-digit number from the beginning of the study through the end of the evaluative and rating task (e.g., Chun and Kruglanski, 2006). Participants were told that an additional objective of this study was to investigate how well people can perform two different tasks simultaneously. They were asked to silently rehearse a nine-digit number while they are completing the survey, and that they would be asked to reproduce this number later in the survey. Need for Cognitive Closure. Need for Cognitive Closure (NFCC) was measured using a 42-item scale (Webster and Kruglanski, 1994). The complete scale can be found in Appendix D. After participants completed all the dependent measures and the 42-item Need for Cognitive Closure Scale, a need for cognitive closure index was created by summing the scores (α = 0.85). A median split was performed blocking the participants into either high or low need for cognitive closure conditions. 25

35 Inferential Judgment. Participants were asked to rate the cell phone plan on the omitted attribute using a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (extremely bad) to 7 (extremely good). This measure was taken to provide a rating based measure of the inference of the omitted attribute, roaming fee. Overall Evaluation. Participants indicated their evaluations of the cell phone plan on a 7- point scale ranging from 1 (extremely bad) to 7 (extremely good). One hundred and nineteen undergraduate business students participated for course credit. The participants were randomly assigned to the experimental conditions. Participants were seated at a computer and were informed that the purpose of the study was to investigate consumers attitudes and perceptions. MediaLab software was used to administer the experiment. Participants were asked to imagine that they were shopping for a new cell phone plan. They read a brief description of a new cell phone plan that contained positive information on four attributes (anytime minutes, service coverage area, monthly fee, nights and weekends); however, no information was provided about the omitted attribute, roaming fee. They were asked to read the description and evaluate the plan. Once they had completed this task they were not allowed to return to the information page. The participants were then asked to provide an overall evaluation of the cell phone plan. This was followed by the inferential judgment measure and the need for cognitive closure scale. 26

36 Results As a manipulation check for the cognitive load manipulation, participants were asked, How difficult was it for you to concentrate carefully on the product evaluation? (7-point scale, 1 = not at all difficult; 7 = extremely difficult). A 2 (Salience of Omitted Attribute) x 2 (Cognitive Load) x 2 (NFCC) ANOVA revealed a main effect for Cognitive Load (F(1, 111) = 6.84, p <.01). Participants found it more difficult to concentrate when they were under cognitive load than not (M = 3.52, M = 2.70 respectively). The cognitive load manipulation had the desired effect. No other main effects or interactions were expected or found. A 2 (Salience of Omitted Attribute) x 2 (Cognitive Load) x 2 (NFCC) ANOVA was performed on the overall evaluation of the cell phone plan yielding a main effect for Salience (F(1, 111) = 4.20, p <.05). Participants overall evaluations for the plan were greater when the omitted attribute was not salient than when it was salient (M = 5.25, M = 4.88 respectively). This finding supports prior debiasing omission neglect findings and confirms that our salience manipulation was effective. No other main effects or interactions were expected or found. A 2 (Salience of Omitted Attribute) x 2 (Cognitive Load) x 2 (NFCC) ANOVA performed on the inferential judgment revealed a main effect for Salience (F(1, 111) = 24.15, p <.001). Participants rated the omitted attribute, roaming fee, as more positive when it was not salient than when it was (Ms = 3.86 vs. 2.74, respectively). The increased salience of the omitted attribute information made participants sensitive to the fact that it was missing, resulting in a more moderate inferential judgment. Further, although it is possible that cognitive load may also impact omission detection or people s 27

Debiasing Omission Neglect

Debiasing Omission Neglect Marquette University e-publications@marquette Marketing Faculty Research and Publications Business Administration, College of 6-1-2006 Debiasing Omission Neglect Frank Kardes University of Cincinnati Steven

More information

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal Seoul Journal of Business Volume 11, Number 1 (June 2005) Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

More information

Contextual Influences on Omission Neglect in the Fault Tree Paradigm

Contextual Influences on Omission Neglect in the Fault Tree Paradigm OMISSION SILVERA, KARDES, NEGLECT HARVEY, IN THE FAULT CRONLEY, TREES HOUGHTON JOURNAL OF CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY, 15(2), 117 126 Copyright 2005, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Contextual Influences on

More information

Activating a mental simulation mind-set through generation of alternatives: Implications for debiasing in related and unrelated domains

Activating a mental simulation mind-set through generation of alternatives: Implications for debiasing in related and unrelated domains Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) 374 383 Journal of Experimental Social Psychology www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Activating a mental simulation mind-set through generation of alternatives:

More information

The Anchoring-and-Adjustment Heuristic

The Anchoring-and-Adjustment Heuristic PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Anchoring-and-Adjustment Heuristic Why the Adjustments Are Insufficient Nicholas Epley 1 and Thomas Gilovich 2 1 University of Chicago and 2 Cornell University

More information

The Effect of Accuracy Motivation on Anchoring and Adjustment: Do People Adjust from Provided Anchors?

The Effect of Accuracy Motivation on Anchoring and Adjustment: Do People Adjust from Provided Anchors? University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Marketing Papers Wharton Faculty Research 12-2010 The Effect of Accuracy Motivation on Anchoring and Adjustment: Do People Adjust from Provided Anchors? Joseph

More information

An Investigation of Factors Influencing Causal Attributions in Managerial Decision Making

An Investigation of Factors Influencing Causal Attributions in Managerial Decision Making Marketing Letters 9:3 (1998): 301 312 1998 Kluwer Academic Publishers, Manufactured in The Netherlands An Investigation of Factors Influencing Causal Attributions in Managerial Decision Making SUNDER NARAYANAN

More information

Exploring the relationship between knowledge and anchoring effects: is the type of knowledge important?

Exploring the relationship between knowledge and anchoring effects: is the type of knowledge important? University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Theses and Dissertations Summer 2011 Exploring the relationship between knowledge and anchoring effects: is the type of knowledge important? Andrew Robert Smith

More information

ABSTRACT. Professor Dr. Rebecca Hamilton and Professor Dr. Joydeep Srivastava, Department of Marketing

ABSTRACT. Professor Dr. Rebecca Hamilton and Professor Dr. Joydeep Srivastava, Department of Marketing ABSTRACT Title of Document: THE INFLUENCE OF CONSUMER MOTIVATIONS ON CONSUMPTION INTENTIONS AND BEHAVIOR Francine da Silveira Espinoza, Doctor of Philosophy, 2009 Co-Directed By: Professor Dr. Rebecca

More information

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 118 128 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/obhdp

More information

Missing Information and Discounting: Exploration and Extension. Sandra J. Burke, Macquarie Graduate School of Management. Abstract

Missing Information and Discounting: Exploration and Extension. Sandra J. Burke, Macquarie Graduate School of Management. Abstract Missing Information and Discounting: Exploration and Extension Sandra J. Burke, Macquarie Graduate School of Management Abstract Missing information (MI), if detected, often leads to alternative discounting.

More information

Susceptibility to anchoring effects: How openness-toexperience influences responses to anchoring cues

Susceptibility to anchoring effects: How openness-toexperience influences responses to anchoring cues McElroy, T. & Dowd, K. (2007). Susceptibility to anchoring effects: How openness-to-experience influences responses to anchoring cues. Judgment and Decision Making, 2(1): 48-53. (Feb 2007) Published by

More information

Precision of the Anchor Influences the Amount of Adjustment Chris Janiszewski and Dan Uy

Precision of the Anchor Influences the Amount of Adjustment Chris Janiszewski and Dan Uy PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Precision of the Anchor Influences the Amount of Adjustment Chris Janiszewski and Dan Uy University of Florida ABSTRACT The ing-and-adjustment heuristic has been

More information

I love it or I hate it? The positivity effect in stated preferences for agent evaluation

I love it or I hate it? The positivity effect in stated preferences for agent evaluation Market Lett (2006) 17: 103 117 DOI 10.1007/s11002-006-4594-8 I love it or I hate it? The positivity effect in stated preferences for agent evaluation Andrew D. Gershoff Ashesh Mukherjee Anirban Mukhopadhyay

More information

Incorporating the Irrelevant: Anchors in Judgments of Belief and Value. Gretchen Chapman. Department of Psychology, Rutgers University

Incorporating the Irrelevant: Anchors in Judgments of Belief and Value. Gretchen Chapman. Department of Psychology, Rutgers University Incorporating the Irrelevant: Anchors in Judgments of Belief and Value Gretchen B. Chapman Department of Psychology, Rutgers University Eric J. Johnson Columbia University Graduate School of Business Address

More information

THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT SAN ANTONIO, COLLEGE OF BUSINESS Working Paper SERIES

THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT SAN ANTONIO, COLLEGE OF BUSINESS Working Paper SERIES ONE UTSA CIRCLE SAN ANTONIO, TEXAS 78249-0631 210 458-4317 BUSINESS.UTSA.EDU THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT SAN ANTONIO, COLLEGE OF BUSINESS Working Paper SERIES Date April 9, 2013 WP # 0020MKT-562-2013 Getting

More information

How do People Really Think about Climate Change?

How do People Really Think about Climate Change? How do People Really Think about Climate Change? #LSESunstein Professor Cass Sunstein Robert Walmsley University Professor at Harvard Law School. Former Administrator of the White House Office of Information

More information

Integrating information from multiple sources: A metacognitive account of self-generated and externally provided anchors

Integrating information from multiple sources: A metacognitive account of self-generated and externally provided anchors Thinking & Reasoning, 2014 Vol. 20, No. 3, 315 332, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2013.811442 Integrating information from multiple sources: A metacognitive account of self-generated and externally

More information

The Impact of Implausible Anchors. Thesis. Steven Timothy Bengal, B.A. Graduate Program in Social Psychology. The Ohio State University

The Impact of Implausible Anchors. Thesis. Steven Timothy Bengal, B.A. Graduate Program in Social Psychology. The Ohio State University The Impact of Implausible Anchors Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in the Graduate School of the Ohio State University By Steven Timothy Bengal,

More information

The Fragile Basic Anchoring Effect

The Fragile Basic Anchoring Effect Journal of Behavioral Decision Making J. Behav. Dec. Making, 15: 65±77 2002) DOI: 10.1002/bdm.403 The Fragile Basic Anchoring Effect NOEL T. BREWER* and GRETCHEN B. CHAPMAN Rutgers University, USA ABSTRACT

More information

The Influence of Framing Effects and Regret on Health Decision-Making

The Influence of Framing Effects and Regret on Health Decision-Making Colby College Digital Commons @ Colby Honors Theses Student Research 2012 The Influence of Framing Effects and Regret on Health Decision-Making Sarah Falkof Colby College Follow this and additional works

More information

CHAPTER TEN SINGLE-SUBJECTS (QUASI-EXPERIMENTAL) RESEARCH

CHAPTER TEN SINGLE-SUBJECTS (QUASI-EXPERIMENTAL) RESEARCH CHAPTER TEN SINGLE-SUBJECTS (QUASI-EXPERIMENTAL) RESEARCH Chapter Objectives: Understand the purpose of single subjects research and why it is quasiexperimental Understand sampling, data collection, and

More information

References. Christos A. Ioannou 2/37

References. Christos A. Ioannou 2/37 Prospect Theory References Tversky, A., and D. Kahneman: Judgement under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, Science, 185 (1974), 1124-1131. Tversky, A., and D. Kahneman: Prospect Theory: An Analysis of

More information

Nudges: A new instrument for public policy?

Nudges: A new instrument for public policy? Nudges: A new instrument for public policy? M.C. Villeval (CNRS, GATE) - Origin: Behavioral Economics BE blends experimental evidence and psychology in a mathematical theory of strategic behavior (Camerer,

More information

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration Dr. Chan-Chien Chiu, Associate Professor of Taiwan Hospitality & Tourism College, Taiwan ABSTRACT This

More information

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance 1 Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior Erica Behrens Winona State University Abstract This study examines the effects of self-consciousness

More information

Defining Social Psychology. informal definition: the study of how people think about, influence, and relate to other people

Defining Social Psychology. informal definition: the study of how people think about, influence, and relate to other people Social Psychology Defining Social Psychology informal definition: the study of how people think about, influence, and relate to other people formal definition: the study of how a person s thoughts, feelings,

More information

PSYCHOLOGY : JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING

PSYCHOLOGY : JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING PSYCHOLOGY 4136-100: JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING http://psych.colorado.edu/~vanboven/teaching/psyc4136/psyc4136.html Monday, 1:00-3:30, Muenzinger D156B Instructor Assistant Dr. Leaf Van Boven Jordan

More information

Gender specific attitudes towards risk and ambiguity an experimental investigation

Gender specific attitudes towards risk and ambiguity an experimental investigation Research Collection Working Paper Gender specific attitudes towards risk and ambiguity an experimental investigation Author(s): Schubert, Renate; Gysler, Matthias; Brown, Martin; Brachinger, Hans Wolfgang

More information

2015 NADTA Conference Pre-Education Committee Book Club Everyday Bias, Howard J. Ross, Suggested Group Discussion Questions

2015 NADTA Conference Pre-Education Committee Book Club Everyday Bias, Howard J. Ross, Suggested Group Discussion Questions 2015 NADTA Conference Pre-Education Committee Book Club Everyday Bias, Howard J. Ross, Suggested Group Discussion Questions 1. After reading this book, which research examples stand out the most in your

More information

INTRODUCTION. NICHOLAS EPLEY 1 * and THOMAS GILOVICH 2 1 University of Chicago, USA 2 Cornell University, USA ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION. NICHOLAS EPLEY 1 * and THOMAS GILOVICH 2 1 University of Chicago, USA 2 Cornell University, USA ABSTRACT Journal of Behavioral Decision Making J. Behav. Dec. Making, 18: 199 212 (2005) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/bdm.495 When Effortful Thinking Influences

More information

PSY The Psychology Major: Academic and Professional Issues. Module 8: Critical Thinking. Study Guide Notes

PSY The Psychology Major: Academic and Professional Issues. Module 8: Critical Thinking. Study Guide Notes PSY 201 - The Psychology Major: Academic and Professional Issues Module 8: Critical Thinking Study Guide Notes Module 8 Objectives: Kuther Chapter 10: Section on Personal Statements You will learn: How

More information

Comparative Ignorance and the Ellsberg Paradox

Comparative Ignorance and the Ellsberg Paradox The Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 22:2; 129 139, 2001 2001 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Manufactured in The Netherlands. Comparative Ignorance and the Ellsberg Paradox CLARE CHUA CHOW National University

More information

Available online at Journal of Consumer Psychology 20 (2010) Research Dialogue. Anchoring unbound

Available online at   Journal of Consumer Psychology 20 (2010) Research Dialogue. Anchoring unbound Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY Journal of Consumer Psychology 20 (2010) 20 24 Research Dialogue Anchoring unbound Nicholas Epley a,, Thomas Gilovich b a University

More information

A Selective Hypothesis Testing Perspective on Price-Quality Inference and Inference-Based Choice

A Selective Hypothesis Testing Perspective on Price-Quality Inference and Inference-Based Choice PRICE QUALITY CRONLEY ET AL. INFERENCE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY, 15(2), 159 169 Copyright 2005, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. A Selective Hypothesis Testing Perspective on Price-Quality Inference

More information

Intuitive Confidence Reflects Speed of Initial Responses in Point Spread Predictions. Alexander C. Walker. A thesis

Intuitive Confidence Reflects Speed of Initial Responses in Point Spread Predictions. Alexander C. Walker. A thesis Intuitive Confidence Reflects Speed of Initial Responses in Point Spread Predictions by Alexander C. Walker A thesis presented to the University of Waterloo in fulfilment of the thesis requirement for

More information

Different Scales for Different Frames: The Role of Subjective Scales and Experience in Explaining Attribute Framing Effects

Different Scales for Different Frames: The Role of Subjective Scales and Experience in Explaining Attribute Framing Effects 1 Different Scales for Different Frames: The Role of Subjective Scales and Experience in Explaining Attribute Framing Effects CHRIS JANISZEWSKI* TIM SILK ALAN D. J. COOKE September, 2002 Forthcoming Journal

More information

Survey Methods in Relationship Research

Survey Methods in Relationship Research Purdue University Purdue e-pubs Department of Psychological Sciences Faculty Publications Department of Psychological Sciences 1-1-2009 Survey Methods in Relationship Research Christopher Agnew Purdue

More information

Emotional Quotient. Andrew Doe. Test Job Acme Acme Test Slogan Acme Company N. Pacesetter Way

Emotional Quotient. Andrew Doe. Test Job Acme Acme Test Slogan Acme Company N. Pacesetter Way Emotional Quotient Test Job Acme 2-16-2018 Acme Test Slogan test@reportengine.com Introduction The Emotional Quotient report looks at a person's emotional intelligence, which is the ability to sense, understand

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Conflict and Bias in Heuristic Judgment Sudeep Bhatia Online First Publication, September 29, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000307

More information

Chapter 13. Social Psychology

Chapter 13. Social Psychology Social Psychology Psychology, Fifth Edition, James S. Nairne What s It For? Social Psychology Interpreting the Behavior of Others Behaving in the Presence of Others Establishing Relations With Others Social

More information

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior Copyright 2015 McGraw-Hill Education. All rights reserved. No reproduction or distribution without the prior written consent of McGraw-Hill Education. Learning

More information

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect. David K. Sherman

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect. David K. Sherman 1 Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect David K. Sherman University of California, Santa Barbara Traci Mann University of California,

More information

The wicked learning environment of regression toward the mean

The wicked learning environment of regression toward the mean The wicked learning environment of regression toward the mean Working paper December 2016 Robin M. Hogarth 1 & Emre Soyer 2 1 Department of Economics and Business, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona 2

More information

Perspective Taking as Egocentric Anchoring and Adjustment

Perspective Taking as Egocentric Anchoring and Adjustment Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 87, No. 3, 327 339 0022-3514/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.327 Perspective Taking

More information

The study of behavioral economics includes how market decisions are made and the mechanisms that drive public choice.

The study of behavioral economics includes how market decisions are made and the mechanisms that drive public choice. Behavioral economics: (Wikipedia, 2014 01 22) Behavioral economics and the related field, behavioral finance, study the effects of social, cognitive, and emotional factors on the economic decisions of

More information

Imagine a pharmaceutical company is preparing a television. Motivation to Think and Order Effects in Persuasion: The Moderating Role of Chunking

Imagine a pharmaceutical company is preparing a television. Motivation to Think and Order Effects in Persuasion: The Moderating Role of Chunking PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN Petty et al. / ORDER EFFECTS IN PERSUASION Motivation to Think and Order Effects in Persuasion: The Moderating Role of Chunking Richard E. Petty Zakary L. Tormala

More information

Heuristics & Biases:

Heuristics & Biases: Heuristics & Biases: The Availability Heuristic and The Representativeness Heuristic Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 05/29/2018: Lecture 10-2 Note: This Powerpoint presentation

More information

Consequences of Heterogeneity Across Service Providers (or Vive la Difference) Valerie Folkes University of Southern California

Consequences of Heterogeneity Across Service Providers (or Vive la Difference) Valerie Folkes University of Southern California Consequences of Heterogeneity Across Service Providers (or Vive la Difference) Valerie Folkes University of Southern California Heterogeneity or Variation in the Service Consumers Receive Across different

More information

Reactions of College Students to the Sexuality of Older People

Reactions of College Students to the Sexuality of Older People Reactions of College Students to the Sexuality of Older People Emily Waterman a The reactions of 153 college students to incidents of sexuality were measured using a series of scenarios. Age of target

More information

An Understanding of Role of Heuristic on Investment Decisions

An Understanding of Role of Heuristic on Investment Decisions International Review of Business and Finance ISSN 0976-5891 Volume 9, Number 1 (2017), pp. 57-61 Research India Publications http://www.ripublication.com An Understanding of Role of Heuristic on Investment

More information

Occurrent belief and dispositional belief. Jie Gao University of Edinburgh

Occurrent belief and dispositional belief. Jie Gao University of Edinburgh Occurrent belief and dispositional belief Jie Gao University of Edinburgh EENM, Paris, July 4 th, 2016 Outline 1. Distinctions between dispositional belief and occurrent belief 2. Psychological effects

More information

Attitudes have been viewed by many (e.g., Allport, Resistance Can Be Good or Bad: How Theories of Resistance and Dissonance Affect Attitude Certainty

Attitudes have been viewed by many (e.g., Allport, Resistance Can Be Good or Bad: How Theories of Resistance and Dissonance Affect Attitude Certainty 10.1177/0146167205286110 PERSONALITY Rydell et al. / RESISTANCE AND SOCIAL CAN PSYCHOLOGY BE GOOD OR BULLETIN BAD Resistance Can Be Good or Bad: How Theories of Resistance and Dissonance Affect Attitude

More information

Do Causal Beliefs Influence the Hot-Hand and the Gambler s Fallacy?

Do Causal Beliefs Influence the Hot-Hand and the Gambler s Fallacy? Do Causal Beliefs Influence the Hot-Hand and the Gambler s Fallacy? Giorgio Gronchi (giorgio.gronchi@gmail.com) Università di Firenze, Dipartimento di Psicologia, via della Cittadella 7 50144 Firenze,

More information

Broadening the Conditions for Illusory Correlation Formation: Implications for Judging Minority Groups

Broadening the Conditions for Illusory Correlation Formation: Implications for Judging Minority Groups BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY, 21(4), 263 279 Copyright 1999, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Broadening the Conditions for Illusory Correlation Formation: Implications for Judging Minority Groups

More information

TTI Success Insights Emotional Quotient Version

TTI Success Insights Emotional Quotient Version TTI Success Insights Emotional Quotient Version 2-2-2011 Scottsdale, Arizona INTRODUCTION The Emotional Quotient report looks at a person's emotional intelligence, which is the ability to sense, understand

More information

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect

Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect Motiv Emot (2006) 30:165 169 DOI 10.1007/s11031-006-9001-5 ORIGINAL PAPER Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect David K. Sherman Traci

More information

Priming Effects by Visual Image Information in On-Line Shopping Malls

Priming Effects by Visual Image Information in On-Line Shopping Malls Priming Effects by Visual Image Information in On-Line Shopping Malls Eun-young Kim*, Si-cheon You**, Jin-ryeol Lee*** *Chosun University Division of Design 375 Seosukdong Dong-gu Gwangju Korea, key1018@hanmail.net

More information

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing This document contains 4 examples of writing and feedback comments from Level 2A lab reports, and 4 steps to help you apply

More information

G646: BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING. Graduate School of Business Stanford University Fall 2007

G646: BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING. Graduate School of Business Stanford University Fall 2007 G646: BEHAVIORAL DECISION MAKING Graduate School of Business Stanford University Fall 2007 Professor: Itamar Simonson Littlefield 378; 725-8981 itamars@stanford.edu Office Hours: By appointment Assistant:

More information

Belief in the Immutability of Attitudes Both Increases and Decreases Advocacy. Omair Akhtar. Apple, Inc. S. Christian Wheeler. Stanford Univeristy

Belief in the Immutability of Attitudes Both Increases and Decreases Advocacy. Omair Akhtar. Apple, Inc. S. Christian Wheeler. Stanford Univeristy Running Head: IMPLICIT THEORIES AFFECT ADVOCACY Belief in the Immutability of Attitudes Both Increases and Decreases Advocacy Omair Akhtar Apple, Inc. S. Christian Wheeler Stanford Univeristy IN PRESS:

More information

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions Readings: OpenStax Textbook - Chapters 1 5 (online) Appendix D & E (online) Plous - Chapters 1, 5, 6, 13 (online) Introductory comments Describe how familiarity with statistical methods can - be associated

More information

Giving and Receiving Feedback for Performance Improvement

Giving and Receiving Feedback for Performance Improvement Giving and Receiving Feedback for Performance Improvement Presented by Mark Beese, MBA CM10 5/3/2018 11:15 AM The handout(s) and presentation(s) attached are copyright and trademark protected and provided

More information

Effects of Aggregate Rating on ewom Acceptance: An Attribution Theory Perspective

Effects of Aggregate Rating on ewom Acceptance: An Attribution Theory Perspective Association for Information Systems AIS Electronic Library (AISeL) PACIS 2010 Proceedings Pacific Asia Conference on Information Systems (PACIS) 2010 Effects of Aggregate Rating on ewom Acceptance: An

More information

Resistance to Persuasion and Attitude Certainty: The Moderating Role of Elaboration

Resistance to Persuasion and Attitude Certainty: The Moderating Role of Elaboration 10.1177/0146167204264251 PERSONALITY Tormala, Petty / AND RESISTANCE SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY AND ATTITUDE BULLETIN CERTAINTY Resistance to Persuasion and Attitude Certainty: The Moderating Role of Elaboration

More information

City, University of London Institutional Repository

City, University of London Institutional Repository City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Scopelliti, I., Morewedge, C. K., Min, L., McCormick, E. & Kassam, K. (2016). Measurement, Consequences, and Debiasing

More information

Repositioning Validity

Repositioning Validity Repositioning Validity Karen E. Kirkhart Syracuse University Inaugural Conference April 21-23, 2013 Chicago, Illinois, USA Culture and Validity Culture and Validity Multiplicity Considering Culture Simultaneous

More information

Measuring and Assessing Study Quality

Measuring and Assessing Study Quality Measuring and Assessing Study Quality Jeff Valentine, PhD Co-Chair, Campbell Collaboration Training Group & Associate Professor, College of Education and Human Development, University of Louisville Why

More information

Recent theorizing in dual process models has focused

Recent theorizing in dual process models has focused Motivated Reasoning in Outcome-Bias Effects NIDHI AGRAWAL DURAIRAJ MAHESWARAN* In this research, we examine the conditions under which s bias judgments, and we provide insights on the processes underlying

More information

Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams

Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams Estimated Distribution of Items for the Exams The current plan is that there are 5 exams with 50 multiple choice items that will cover two chapters. Each chapter is planned to have 25 multiple choice items.

More information

Psych 1Chapter 2 Overview

Psych 1Chapter 2 Overview Psych 1Chapter 2 Overview After studying this chapter, you should be able to answer the following questions: 1) What are five characteristics of an ideal scientist? 2) What are the defining elements of

More information

Participants. 213 undergraduate students made up the total participants (including the reporter): gender. ethnicity. single/dating/married.

Participants. 213 undergraduate students made up the total participants (including the reporter): gender. ethnicity. single/dating/married. Rape Blame Rape Defined Interpretations of rape o being motivated by a need for power, control or dominance and sexual fulfillment on the part of the perpetrator (Muehlenhard and Linton, 1987) Date rape

More information

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution 1 Robert D. Mather University of Central Oklahoma Charles M. Mather University of Science

More information

Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins

Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins Columbia University ABSTRACT Where does value come from? I propose a new answer to this classic question. People experience

More information

THE FLEXIBLE CORRECTION MODEL: THE ROLE OF NAIVE THEORIES OF BIAS IN BIAS CORRECTION

THE FLEXIBLE CORRECTION MODEL: THE ROLE OF NAIVE THEORIES OF BIAS IN BIAS CORRECTION THE FLEXIBLE CORRECTION MODEL: THE ROLE OF NAIVE THEORIES OF BIAS IN BIAS CORRECTION Duane T. Wegener Richard E. Petty Life often presents us with situations in which it is important to assess the true

More information

CURRICULUM VITAE. David Mitsuo Sanbonmatsu

CURRICULUM VITAE. David Mitsuo Sanbonmatsu CURRICULUM VITAE David Mitsuo Sanbonmatsu July 1, 2016 Department of Psychology (801) 581-8505 (office) 380 South 1530 East Rm. 502 (801) 581-5841 (fax) The University of Utah sanbonmatsu@psych.utah.edu

More information

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information on Consumer Elaboration and Brand Attitude

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information on Consumer Elaboration and Brand Attitude The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information on Consumer Elaboration and Brand Attitude Chan-Chien Chiu, Associate Professor, Department of Food and Beverage Management, Taiwan Hospitality & Tourism

More information

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect Abstract This article reports the effects of hedonic versus utilitarian consumption goals on consumers choices between

More information

CONFIDENCE VIA CORRECTION. ESMT Working Paper THE EFFECT OF JUDGMENT CORRECTION ON CONSUMER CONFIDENCE

CONFIDENCE VIA CORRECTION. ESMT Working Paper THE EFFECT OF JUDGMENT CORRECTION ON CONSUMER CONFIDENCE 13 06 July 16, 2013 ESMT Working Paper CONFIDENCE VIA CORRECTION THE EFFECT OF JUDGMENT CORRECTION ON CONSUMER CONFIDENCE FRANCINE ESPINOZA PETERSEN, ESMT REBECCA W. HAMILTON, UNIVERSITY OF MARYLAND ISSN

More information

School of Psychology. Professor Richard Kemp. Cognitive Bias: Psychology and Forensic Science

School of Psychology. Professor Richard Kemp. Cognitive Bias: Psychology and Forensic Science School of Psychology Professor Richard Kemp Cognitive Bias: Psychology and Forensic Science Professor Bryan Found Cognitive Bias: Psychology and Forensic Science What is Cognitive Bias and why does it

More information

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz

Introduction to Psychology Social Psychology Quiz MULTIPLE CHOICE: 1. We usually adapt our behavior to the demands of the social situation, and in ambiguous situations: A) We take our cues from the behavior of others. B) We will do the same thing that

More information

Psychological. Influences on Personal Probability. Chapter 17. Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc.

Psychological. Influences on Personal Probability. Chapter 17. Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc. Psychological Chapter 17 Influences on Personal Probability Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc. 17.2 Equivalent Probabilities, Different Decisions Certainty Effect: people

More information

Chapter 11 Decision Making. Syllogism. The Logic

Chapter 11 Decision Making. Syllogism. The Logic Chapter 11 Decision Making Syllogism All men are mortal. (major premise) Socrates is a man. (minor premise) (therefore) Socrates is mortal. (conclusion) The Logic Mortal Socrates Men 1 An Abstract Syllogism

More information

ROLE OF HEURISTICS IN RISK MANAGEMENT

ROLE OF HEURISTICS IN RISK MANAGEMENT ROLE OF HEURISTICS IN RISK MANAGEMENT Kuwait Enterprise Risk Management Conference 4 th Edition, 2017 Abhishek Upadhayay CFPS, MIRM its about time We use heuristics to simplify choices in relation to risk.

More information

Chapter 6: Attribution Processes

Chapter 6: Attribution Processes Chapter 6: Attribution Processes 1. Which of the following is an example of an attributional process in social cognition? a. The act of planning the actions to take given a social situation b. The act

More information

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength? Uri Hasson (uhasson@princeton.edu) Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 USA Geoffrey P. Goodwin (ggoodwin@princeton.edu)

More information

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies Emma ter Mors 1,2, Mieneke Weenig 1, Naomi Ellemers 1, Dancker Daamen 1 1 Leiden University,

More information

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE Previous chapter namely Review of the Literature was concerned with the review of the research studies conducted in the field of teacher education, with special reference

More information

The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the Rescorla-Wagner Predictions: Relative Validity

The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the Rescorla-Wagner Predictions: Relative Validity Methods of Psychological Research Online 4, Vol. 9, No. Internet: http://www.mpr-online.de Fachbereich Psychologie 4 Universität Koblenz-Landau The Influence of the Initial Associative Strength on the

More information

DIFFERENCES IN THE ECONOMIC DECISIONS OF MEN AND WOMEN: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE*

DIFFERENCES IN THE ECONOMIC DECISIONS OF MEN AND WOMEN: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE* DIFFERENCES IN THE ECONOMIC DECISIONS OF MEN AND WOMEN: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE* Catherine C. Eckel Department of Economics Virginia Tech Blacksburg, VA 24061-0316 Philip J. Grossman Department of Economics

More information

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective

Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective Academic Leadership Journal in Student Research Volume 4 Spring 2016 Article 4 2016 Risky Choice Decisions from a Tri-Reference Point Perspective Kevin L. Kenney Fort Hays State University Follow this

More information

FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER

FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER 1 ST SEMESTER 2017 ASSIGNMENT 2 ORGANISATIONAL BEHAVIOUR OSB611S 1 Page1 OSB611S - FEEDBACK TUTORIAL LETTER FOR ASSIGNMENT 2-2016 Dear student The purpose of this tutorial letter

More information

CHAPTER 6 CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT

CHAPTER 6 CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT CHAPTER 6 CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT The aim of this research is to extend the discussion on celebrity endorsement by exploring the impact of brand personality-celebrity personality congruence on consumer

More information

Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and

Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and private study only. The thesis may not be reproduced elsewhere

More information

PSYC 210 Social Psychology

PSYC 210 Social Psychology South Central College PSYC 210 Social Psychology Course Information Description Total Credits 4.00 Pre/Corequisites PSYC100 or consent of instructor. Course Competencies Social Psychology introduces the

More information

Hypothesis-Consistent Testing and Semantic Priming in the Anchoring Paradigm: A Selective Accessibility Model

Hypothesis-Consistent Testing and Semantic Priming in the Anchoring Paradigm: A Selective Accessibility Model Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 35, 136 164 (1999) Article ID jesp.1998.1364, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Hypothesis-Consistent Testing and Semantic Priming in the Anchoring

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

Need for Cognition: Does It Influence Professional Judgment?

Need for Cognition: Does It Influence Professional Judgment? Review of Integrative Business and Economics Research, Vol. 6, no. 1, pp.240-248, January 2017 240 Need for Cognition: Does It Influence Professional Judgment? Wimpie Yustino Setiawan Universitas Atma

More information

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing? Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC How are we doing? Prepared by Maura McCool, M.S. Office of Research, Evaluation and Assessment Metropolitan Community Colleges Fall 2003 1 General Education Assessment

More information

Numeracy, frequency, and Bayesian reasoning

Numeracy, frequency, and Bayesian reasoning Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 4, No. 1, February 2009, pp. 34 40 Numeracy, frequency, and Bayesian reasoning Gretchen B. Chapman Department of Psychology Rutgers University Jingjing Liu Department

More information