Mismatch responses evoked by nociceptive stimuli

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Mismatch responses evoked by nociceptive stimuli"

Transcription

1 Psychophysiology, 50 (2013), Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2012 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: /psyp Mismatch responses evoked by nociceptive stimuli LI HU, a CHEN ZHAO, a HONG LI, b and ELIA VALENTINI c,d a Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (Ministry of Education) and School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China b Research Center of Psychological Development and Education, Liaoning Normal University, Liaoning, China c Psychology Department, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy d Santa Lucia Foundation, Scientific Institute for Research, Hospitalization and Health Care, Rome, Italy Abstract We studied whether nociceptive mismatch negativity (nmmn) could be obtained as result of nociceptive fibers stimulation. The purported nmmn revealed a topography similar to the somatosensory MMN (smmn), which was observed at the bilateral temporal regions of the scalp. Importantly, only early negativities ( ms) located at these regions revealed a selective modulation associated to the processing of deviancy regardless of the attentional focus. The amplitude modulation of the smmn had an earlier onset than the nmmn (110 ms vs. 182 ms) as well as a larger difference of latency between the contralateral and the ipsilateral onset of the activity (52 ms vs. 4 ms). Altogether, these observations provide evidence that (a) a nmmn can be elicited by nociceptive stimuli, and (b) the nmmn is topographically similar to the smmn while differing in latency and possibly in functional organization of their generators. Descriptors: Mismatch negativity, Somatosensory system, Nociceptive system, Attention, Electroencephalography Typing the keyword mismatch negativity (MMN) on PubMed returns more than 1,500 records, thus confirming it as one of the most investigated topics in neurophysiology since its earliest description (Naatanen, Gaillard, & Mantysalo, 1978). Its success in drawing researchers interest is mostly determined by the progressive amount of evidence pinpointing it as an objective index of perceptual processes, due to its functional independence from voluntary attention (Naatanen et al., 2012). Traditionally, MMN is evoked by a deviant or rare (i.e., oddball) event embedded in a stream of repeated or familiar events (i.e., standards), which can be noninvasively recorded using electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG). Consensus is established on the notion that MMN is an index of automatic change detection regulated by a preattentive sensory memory mechanism (Tiitinen, May, Reinikainen, & Naatanen, 1994; Winkler, Reinikainen, & Naatanen, 1993). Such agreement originates from a bulk of research on MMN elicited by auditory stimuli (and stimuli features). When evoked by auditory stimuli, MMN peaks at about ms from change onset and exhibits a low amplitude of no more than 5 mv, with a temporal-frontal topography (e.g., Sams et al., 1985) thought to be at least partially Li Hu was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China ( ), Natural Science Foundation Project of CQ CSTC, and Special Financial Grant from the China Postdoctoral Science Special Foundation (2012T50755). Hong Li was supported by the R&D Special Fund for Health Profession ( ) and the National Natural Science Foundation of China ( ). Address correspondence to: Elia Valentini, PhD, Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Via dei Marsi , Roma, Italy. elia.valentini@uniroma1.it or to Hong Li, Research Center of Psychological Development and Education, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, Liaoning Province, , China. lihong@lnnu.edu.cn generated from the auditory region of the supratemporal cortex (e.g., Giard, Perrin, Pernier, & Bouchet, 1990; Shalgi & Deouell, 2007). Several studies reported the existence of a visual MMN counterpart at about ms, and triggered by changes of stimulus movement, form, and orientation, among other physical features (e.g., Czigler, Balazs, & Pato, 2004; Kimura, Ohira, & Schroger, 2010). Conversely, it is difficult to track down similar evidence concerning mismatch responses elicited by somatosensory stimuli. Indeed, only a few studies of mismatch-related cortical responses following electrical or vibratory input have been published. In a pioneer study, Kekoni et al. (1997) reported evidence of a somatosensory MMN (smmn) occurring between 150 and 250 ms following deviant vibration bursts, which was further confirmed by other researchers using MEG (Akatsuka et al., 2007). Crucially, as a matter of fact, no study to date has reported the existence of a mismatch phenomenon exerted through the selective stimulation of Ad nociceptive fibers. Given the current quest for an objective neuronal marker of pain sensitivity (Schulz, Zherdin, Tiemann, Plant, & Ploner, 2012; Tracey, 2011), it is surprising that an attention-independent index of sensory change has not been investigated yet in the field of pain. In fact, among the cognitive variables affecting pain experience, the brain mechanisms underlying attentional control have probably been the most extensively studied (see Van Damme, Legrain, Vogt, & Crombez, 2010, for a review). Yet, no evidence suggests the existence of a nociceptive attention-independent brain response, even at the earliest latency of neural activation (i.e., the N1 wave of laser-evoked potentials; see Lorenz & Garcia-Larrea, 2003). Since a preattentive electrophysiological index of nociceptiverelated brain activity would benefit in both basic and clinical pain studies, here we aimed (a) to identify a nociceptive MMN (nmmn) as compared to smmn in the context of a roving paradigm (e.g., bs_bs_banner 158

2 Nociceptive system and the mismatch negativity 159 Figure 1. Experimental design. Event-related potentials were recorded in four sessions (two modalities: non-nociceptive somatosensory and nociceptive stimuli; two conditions: active and passive) (top left), and two repeated blocks were included in each session. Block order was counterbalanced across participants. In each block, trains of non-nociceptive somatosensory and nociceptive stimuli were delivered to lateral (L), median (M), or wrist (W) section of the participants right and left hands, respectively (top left and top central). Each block had about 50 trains of stimuli with an intertrain interval of 1,000 ms. Each train consisted of 4 8 repeated identical stimuli delivered to the same section at a constant interstimulus interval of 1,000 ms (bottom). In each modality, participants were required either to focus their attention on the stimuli in two blocks (A: active condition), or to focus their attention on watching a silent video in the remaining two blocks (P: passive condition). The first stimulus in each train was a deviant (d) that became a standard (s: the last stimulus in each train) through repetition. This design provided four conditions (Ad, As, Pd, and Ps; top right). In this paradigm, deviants and standards had exactly the same number of trials and physical properties. Note that in each train the stimuli (gray squares) between deviant and standard were omitted from the data analysis (bottom). Boly et al., 2011; Garrido et al., 2008; Haenschel, Vernon, Dwivedi, Gruzelier, & Baldeweg, 2005), and (b) to discriminate the cortical effects associated to a somatosensory/nociceptive change against the effects of attending such a change in the environment. Participants Method EEG data were collected from 15 healthy, right-handed volunteers (seven males and eight females) aged (mean SD, range = years). All participants gave their written informed consent and were paid for their participation. The local ethics committee approved the procedures, which were in accordance with the standards of the Declaration of Helsinki. Stimulation Non-nociceptive somatosensory and nociceptive stimuli were delivered on the participants right and left hands, respectively. Participants were first familiarized with both non-nociceptive and nociceptive stimuli, then a sensory threshold assessment took place whereby the energy of non-nociceptive and nociceptive stimuli was adjusted individually using the method of limits. Non-nociceptive somatosensory stimuli (i.e., transcutaneous electrical stimuli, TES) consisted of three rapidly succeeding constant-current square-wave pulses (0.5-ms duration) delivered through a pair of surface electrodes (2-cm distance between electrodes). The interpulse interval was 12 ms. The stimulus intensity was twice the individual perceptual threshold, an intensity classically used to activate the Ab fibers in humans (Garcia-Larrea, Lukaszewicz, & Mauguiere, 1995; Hu et al., 2011). These stimuli elicited a vibratory sensation and were never reported as painful. Nociceptive stimuli (i.e., intraepidermal electrical stimuli, IES) consisted of three rapidly succeeding constant-current, square-wave pulses (0.5-ms duration) delivered through three stainless steel concentric bipolar needle electrodes (located according to an equilateral triangle shape). Each of the electrodes consisted of a needle cathode (length: 0.1 mm, Ø: 0.2 mm) surrounded by a cylindrical anode (Ø: 1.4 mm) (Inui, Tran, Hoshiyama, & Kakigi, 2002; Inui, Tsuji, & Kakigi, 2006). The interpulse interval was 12 ms. The stimulus intensity was twice the individual perceptual threshold, an intensity that proved to preferentially activate the Ad nociceptive fibers without simultaneously activating the Ab fibers (Mouraux, Iannetti, & Plaghki, 2010). These stimuli elicited a painful pinprick sensation in all participants. Experimental Design and Procedure A schematic illustration of the experimental design is shown in Figure 1. Participants were seated on a comfortable chair in a silent, temperature-controlled room. Somatosensory and nociceptive evoked potentials (SEPs and NEPs) were evoked by trains of non-nociceptive TES and nociceptive IES delivered to the lateral (L), median (M), or wrist (W) section of the participants left and right hands, respectively (Figure 1, top left; see Legrain, Perchet, & Garcia-Larrea, 2009, for a reference to such spatial partitioning of the hand s stimulation area). All participants had no difficulty in distinguishing the stimuli at the three sites for both non-nociceptive TES and nociceptive IES. SEPs and NEPs were recorded either while the participants were attending to (active condition), or were distracted from

3 160 L. Hu et al. (passive condition), the sensory stimulation (Figure 1, top left). SEPs and NEPs were collected during eight experimental blocks. Two blocks were repeated for each modality and each experimental condition, thus providing four blocks per modality and four blocks per condition in total. The order of all blocks was counterbalanced across participants. In active blocks, participants were instructed to focus on the stimuli and required to report the total number of the stimuli at the end of each block. If the participants could not report the total number of stimuli or reported a wrong value, the corresponding active block was discarded and restarted. This rule was followed until an accurate value was provided by the participants. In addition, at the end of each active block, participants were required to rate verbally the average intensity of the sensation associated to non-nociceptive somatosensory and nociceptive stimuli, using a numerical rating scale ranging from 0 to 10, where 0 was no sensation/no pain and 10 was unbearable sensation/ unbearable pain. In passive blocks, participants were instructed to watch a silent video with subtitles, and required to answer general and specific questions in a structured interview taking place at the end of each passive block. Each block contained about 50 trains of stimuli with an intertrain interval (ITI) of 1,000 ms. The total number of trains for each condition (active or passive) in each modality (somatosensory or nociceptive) was 100. In each train, 4 to 8 repeated identical stimuli (the number of stimuli in each train was pseudorandomly distributed across trains) were delivered to the same hand s section with an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 1,000 ms. Each hand section had the same number of trains across subjects (n = for each condition and each modality). Note that in each block the number of trains was kept constant across the different types of trains (n = 10; train length ranging from 4 to 8 stimuli). The sensory events were administered according to a standard roving paradigm (see Garrido et al., 2008, for a detailed discussion). The first stimulus in each train was a deviant (d) that became a standard (s: the last stimulus in each train) through repetition. In other words, each train of stimulation started with a deviant stimulus delivered on one of the three different hand s sections (Figure 1, bottom). This paradigm ensured that deviants and standards had exactly the same number of trials and physical properties. EEG Recording The EEG data were recorded using a Brain Products system (bandpass: Hz, sampling rate: 500 Hz), connected to a standard EEG cap with 60 scalp Ag-AgCl electrodes placed according to the International system. The left mastoid was used as reference channel, and all channel impedances were kept below 5 kw. To monitor ocular movements and eye blinks, electrooculographic (EOG) signals were simultaneously recorded from four surface electrodes placed on the upper and lower eyelid, and next by the outer canthus of the left and right eye. Data Analysis Psychophysics. In the active condition, the average ratings of the sensation elicited by non-nociceptive somatosensory and nociceptive stimuli were compared across different stimulus sites using one-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with 3 within-subject levels (L, M, and W sections). Statistical significance was set at p <.01. Mauchly s test was applied to assess the possible violations of sphericity. If the assumption of sphericity was violated (p <.01), the degrees of freedom were adjusted (e < 0.75: Greenhouse-Geisser correction, e > 0.75: Huynh and Feldt correction). When the main effect of the ANOVA was significant, Tukey pairwise post hoc comparisons were performed. EEG data preprocessing. EEG data were processed using EEGLAB (Delorme & Makeig, 2004), an open source toolbox running in the MATLAB environment. Continuous EEG data were low-pass filtered at 30 Hz. EEG epochs were extracted using a time window of 700 ms (200 ms prestimulus and 500 ms poststimulus) and baseline corrected using the prestimulus interval. Trials contaminated by eyeblinks and movements were corrected using an Independent Component Analysis (ICA) algorithm (Delorme & Makeig, 2004; Jung et al., 2001). In all datasets, these independent components had a large EOG channel contribution and a frontal scalp distribution. After ICA, epochs were baseline corrected once more using the prestimulus interval, and rereferenced to a common average reference. In each participant, epochs belonging to the same experimental condition were averaged together (regardless of the stimulation site) and were time-locked to the onset of the stimulus. This procedure yielded eight average waveforms in each participant (four waveforms for both SEPs and NEPs: active-deviant [Ad], activestandard [As], passive-deviant [Pd], and passive-standard [Ps]). Each participant s average waveforms were subsequently averaged to obtain group-level average waveforms. Group-level scalp topographies were computed by spline interpolation. Scalp topographies of SEPs in the four experimental conditions were plotted, in steps of 20 ms, from 80 ms to 300 ms. Similarly, scalp topographies of NEPs in the four experimental conditions were plotted, in steps of 20 ms, from 140 ms to 360 ms. Statistical approach to somatosensory and nociceptive brain evoked potentials Definition of spatial regions of interest (srois). A point-bypoint, two-way repeated measures ANOVA was used to assess the effects of mismatch (two levels: deviant vs. standard) and attention (two levels: active vs. passive) on SEPs and NEPs, respectively, to define their significant srois. This yielded three time-course waveforms of F values for each modality and channel representing (1) the main effect of mismatch, (2) the main effect of attention, and (3) the interaction between the two factors. Significant srois were defined based on the criteria that (a) they had to be composed of at least two nearby significant channels, and (b) they had to be composed of more than 10 consecutive significant time points (20 ms) for the factor mismatch and attention, respectively (F > 8.9, p <.01). Definition of temporal regions of interest (trois) within each sroi. For each sroi that was significantly modulated by mismatch, we computed the average stimulus-evoked brain responses across all channels within the sroi for each participant and experimental condition (Ad, As, Pd, and Ps). The resulting average brain responses were submitted to a point-by-point, two-way repeated measures ANOVA, combined with nonparametric permutation testing (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007), to assess the time course of the effects of mismatch and attention on SEPs and NEPs, respectively. This was performed according to the following five steps. First, each time point of the stimulus-evoked brain responses averaged across channels in each spatial region was compared using a twoway repeated measures ANOVA, with mismatch (two levels: deviant vs. standard) and attention (two levels: active vs. passive) as factors. This yielded three time course waveforms of F values,

4 Nociceptive system and the mismatch negativity 161 representing (1) the main effect of mismatch, (2) the main effect of attention, and (3) the interaction between the two factors. Time points with a p value <.01 (F > 8.9) were selected for subsequent analyses. Second, to account for the multiple comparison problem in the point-by-point statistical analysis (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007), significant time points (p <.01) were categorized in clusters based on their adjacency in the time course (cluster-level statistical analysis). Only clusters composed of more than 10 adjacent significant time points were considered (20 ms), to account for the problem of multiple comparisons. The sum of the F values of all time points composing a cluster defined its cluster-level statistics (SF). Third, for every participant, we randomly permutated 5,000 times the time course waveforms of the four conditions (Ad, As, Pd, and Ps). In each permutation, the same two-way repeated measures ANOVA was performed at every time point of the clusters identified in step two, thus yielding a cluster-level statistics S F(m) at the mth permutation. Permutation distributions D(SF) of the cluster-level F statistics were obtained from S F(m). Fourth, for each cluster identified in the second step, its two-tailed p value pf was obtained by locating the observed SF under the permutation distribution D(SF) estimated from permutated S F(m). Fifth, clusters were used to define trois for the subsequent quantitative analysis based on the criterion that the cluster had a p value smaller than a defined threshold (pf <.01) (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007). For each indentified sroi that was significantly modulated by attention, identical statistical analysis (point-by-point, two-way repeated measures ANOVA and nonparametric permutation testing) was performed to define the significant trois for SEPs and NEPs, respectively. Statistical analysis within each troi. In each mismatch and attention modulated troi, the mean value of all time points within the troi was computed for each participant and each experimental condition (Ad, As, Pd, and Ps). The obtained mean values were then compared using a two-way repeated measures ANOVA, with mismatch (two levels: deviant vs. standard) and attention (two levels: active vs. passive) as factors. Mauchly s test was applied to assess the possible violations of sphericity. If the assumption of sphericity was violated (p <.01), the degrees of freedom were adjusted (e < 0.75: Greenhouse-Geisser correction, e > 0.75: Huynh and Feldt correction). In mismatch modulated srois, when the interaction was significant, post hoc Tukey s HSD test was performed to compare the responses obtained in the four experimental conditions (Ad, As, Pd, and Ps) in each modality. Psychophysics Results Response accuracy in the passive condition was 90.81% 6.73% and did not differ between non-nociceptive TES and nociceptive IES trials (p >.05; 93.12% 4.51% vs % 6.94%). The sensations elicited by the non-nociceptive somatosensory stimuli and nociceptive stimuli were reported as vibratory and pricking, respectively. In the active condition, the average ratings ( SD) ofthe sensation elicited by non-nociceptive somatosensory stimuli were as follows: lateral section: ; median section: ; wrist section: One-way repeated measures ANOVA showed that the ratings were significantly different at diverse stimulation sites, F(2,42) = 6.57; p <.01. Post hoc analysis revealed that the intensity ratings were significantly lower when the stimuli were delivered on the wrist section than on the lateral (p <.01) and median section (p <.01), whereas they were not significantly different when delivered on the lateral versus the median section (p >.05). To investigate whether this perceptual difference could be associated to any difference in the electrophysiological patterns, an additional statistical comparison was performed to compare SEP amplitudes obtained from all the hand sections versus amplitudes recorded following only the stimulation of lateral and median sections and omitting the wrist section (no significant difference was found. See online supplementary material for details). The average ratings ( SD) of the sensation elicited by nociceptive stimuli were as follows: lateral section: ; median section: ; wrist section: One-way repeated measures ANOVA showed that the intensity ratings were not significantly different among stimulation sites, F(2,42) = 0.35; p >.05. Somatosensory Evoked Potentials Figure 2 (top panel) shows the grand average scalp topographies of SEPs ranging from 80 to 300 ms at four different conditions (Ad, As, Pd, and Ps). All conditions were characterized by a positivity over central-parietal electrodes contralateral to the stimulated body part (i.e., right hand) peaking at around 80 ms, and a negativity on the contralateral central-temporal electrodes peaking at around 120 ms. In addition, a large persisting positivity maximal at frontalcentral electrodes was observed between 140 and 300 ms. Effects of mismatch and attention in SEPs Spatial regions of interest in SEPs. The factor mismatch significantly modulated SEPs in three distinct srois (Figure 2, bottom panel): contralateral temporal region (i.e., FT7, T7, TP7, C5, CP5); ipsilateral temporal region (i.e., T8, TP8); central region (i.e., FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2, P1, Pz, P2). The factor attention significantly modulated SEPs in three distinct srois: contralateral temporal region (i.e., T7, TP7, C5, CP5, CP3); ipsilateral temporal region (i.e., C6, TP8); frontal-central region (i.e., F1, Fz, F2, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2). Temporal regions of interest within each sroi in SEPs. The time course of neural modulations exerted by the factors mismatch and attention within each modulated sroi is represented in Figure 3 (left panel) and Figure 4 (left panel), respectively (cf. Table 1 for details). Main effect of mismatch. The factor mismatch had a strong modulatory impact on the significant SEP srois by exerting a negative amplitude modulation at both contralateral temporal region (SEP-M-tROI 1 a: ms; SEP-M-tROI b: ms; SEP-M-tROI c: ms) and ipsilateral temporal region (SEP-M-tROI d: ms), as well as a persisting positive amplitude modulation at central region (SEP-M-tROI e: ms). For each troi, the modulatory effect of mismatch on scalp topography was represented as the difference between response amplitudes obtained in the deviant and standard conditions during both active and passive conditions, according to the formula ([Ad - As + Pd - Ps]/2) (Figure 3, right panel). Main effect of attention. The factor attention had a similar modulatory impact on the significant SEP srois by exerting a 1. SEP-M-tROI: SEP troi that was significantly modulated by the factor mismatch.

5 162 L. Hu et al. Figure 2. Scalp topographies of SEPs and identification of srois. Top panel: Series of 12 scalp topographies of group average SEPs ( ms) at four different conditions (Ad: active-deviant, As: active-standard, Pd: passive-deviant, and Ps: passive-standard) are displayed with a 20-ms interval. Color scale represents amplitude (mv). Note that the scalp topography of the early part of the response displays a contralaterally distributed positivity around 80 ms and a contralaterally distributed negativity around 120 ms (right hand stimulation), whereas the scalp topography of the late part of the response displays a centrally distributed positivity from 140 to 300 ms. Bottom panel: Two-way repeated measures ANOVA to assess the effect of mismatch (deviant vs. standard) and attention (active vs. passive) on SEPs to define significant srois. Series of 12 scalp topographies showing each of the factors and their interaction on the SEPs ( ms) are displayed with a 20-ms interval. Color scale corresponds to the F value. The factor mismatch significantly modulated SEPs in three distinct srois (contralateral temporal region: FT7, T7, TP7, C5, CP5; ipsilateral temporal region: T8, TP8; central region: FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2, P1, Pz, P2). The factor attention significantly modulated SEPs in three distinct srois (contralateral temporal region: T7, TP7, C5, CP5, CP3; ipsilateral temporal region: C6, TP8; frontal-central region: F1, Fz, F2, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2). negative amplitude modulation at both contralateral temporal region (SEP-A-tROI 2 a: ms; SEP-A-tROI b: ms) and ipsilateral temporal region (SEP-A-tROI c: ms), as well as a positive amplitude modulation at frontal-central region (SEP-A-tROI d: ms; SEP-A-tROI e: ms; SEP- A-tROI f: ms). For each troi, the modulatory effect of attention on scalp topography was represented as the difference of response amplitudes between active and passive conditions during deviant and standard trials, according to the formula ([Ad + As - Pd - Ps]/2) (Figure 4, right panel). Interaction of mismatch and attention in mismatch modulated srois. Post hoc analysis (Table 2) revealed that SEP amplitudes were significantly larger in Ad than in As (p <.01) and Pd 2. SEP-A-tROI: SEP troi that was significantly modulated by the factor attention. (p <.001) conditions at the SEP-M-tROI b ( ms). Similarly, SEP amplitudes were significantly larger in Ad than in As (p <.001), Ps (p <.001), and Pd (p <.01) conditions at the SEP- M-tROI e ( ms). In sum, the two trois showed higher amplitudes during the processing of deviant stimuli and when the focus of attention was directed towards the stimulation (Figure 3, top left and right panels). Nociceptive Evoked Potentials Figure 5 (top panel) shows the grand average scalp topographies of NEPs ranging from 140 to 360 ms at four different conditions (Ad, As, Pd, and Ps). All conditions were characterized by a negativity on the central-temporal electrodes ranging from 140 to 180 ms and contralateral to the stimulated body part (i.e., left hand), as well as by a large persisting positivity maximal at frontal-central electrodes and ranging from 180 to 360 ms.

6 Nociceptive system and the mismatch negativity 163 Figure 3. SEPs at the mismatch modulated srois. Left panel: In each mismatch modulated sroi, point-by-point, two-way repeated measures ANOVA with the factors mismatch and attention, combined with nonparametric permutation testing, was used to define the mismatch modulated trois. x axis, latency (ms); y axis, amplitude (mv). The gray scale represents the F value for the factor mismatch. At the contralateral temporal region, the factor mismatch significantly modulated SEPs in three trois (a: ms; b: ms; c: ms). At the ipsilateral temporal region, the factor mismatch significantly modulated SEPs in one troi (d: ms). At the central region, the factor mismatch significantly modulated SEPs in one troi (e: ms). Right panel: In each defined mismatch modulated troi (a, b, c, d, and e), the amplitudes (mean SEM) across all participants are displayed using bar plots in the left part of this panel, and the corresponding scalp topographies showing the modulatory effect of mismatch ([Ad - As + Pd - Ps]/2) are displayed in the right part of this panel.

7 164 L. Hu et al. Figure 4. SEPs at the attention modulated srois. Left panel: Within each attention modulated sroi, point-by-point, two-way repeated measures ANOVA with the factors mismatch and attention, combined with nonparametric permutation testing, was used to define the attention modulated trois. x axis, latency (ms); y axis, amplitude (mv). The gray scale represents the F value for the factor attention. At the contralateral temporal region, the factor attention significantly modulated SEPs in two trois (a: ms; b: ms). At the ipsilateral temporal region, the factor attention significantly modulated SEPs in one troi (c: ms). At the frontal-central region, the factor attention significantly modulated SEPs in three trois (d: ms; e: ms; f: ms). Right panel: Within each defined attention modulated troi (a, b, c, d, e, and f), the amplitudes (mean SEM) across all participants are displayed using bar plots in the left part of this panel, and the corresponding scalp topographies showing the modulatory effect of attention ([Ad + As - Pd - Ps]/2) are displayed in the right part of this panel.

8 Nociceptive system and the mismatch negativity 165 Table 1. Amplitudes of SEPs and NEPs at Different Experimental Conditions (Ad, As, Pd, and Ps) and ANOVA Results srois Channels Label trois (ms) Ad (mv) As (mv) Pd (mv) Ps (mv) Main effect of mismatch Main effect of attention Interaction Mean SE Mean SE Mean SE Mean SE F p F p F p Mismatch modulation areas of SEPs (Figure 4) Attention modulation areas of SEPs (Figure 5) Mismatch modulation areas of NEPs (Figure 6) Attention modulation areas of NEPs (Figure 7) FT7, T7, TP7, C5, CP5 a < > >.01 b < > <.01 c < > >.01 T8, TP8 d < > >.01 FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2, P1, Pz, P2 e < < <.01 T7, TP7, C5, CP5, CP3 a > < >.01 b > < >.01 C6, TP8 c > < >.01 F1, Fz, F2, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2 d > < >.01 e < < >.01 f < < >.01 T8, TP8 a < > >.01 b < > >.01 CP5, P7 c < > >.01 d < > >.01 FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, e < < >.01 CP1, CPz, CP2 f < > <.01 CP6, P6, PO8, P4, PO4 a > < >.01 b > < >.01 T7, TP7, P7 c > < >.01 F1, Fz, F2, FC1, FCz, FC2, d > < >.01 C1, Cz, C2 e < < >.01

9 166 L. Hu et al. Table 2. Post Hoc Analysis Using Tukey s HSD Test troi Ad vs. As Pd vs. Ps Ad vs. Pd As vs. Ps sroi p p p p Mismatch modulation areas of SEPs (Figure 4) b <.01 >.01 <.001 >.01 e <.001 <.001 <.01 >.01 Mismatch modulation area of NEPs (Figure 6) f <.001 >.01 <.001 >.01 Effect of mismatch and attention in NEPs Spatial regions of interest in NEPs. The factor mismatch significantly modulated NEPs in three distinct srois (Figure 5, bottom panel): contralateral temporal region (i.e., T8, TP8); ipsilateral temporal region (i.e., CP5, P7); central region (i.e., FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2). The factor attention significantly modulated NEPs in three distinct srois: contralateral temporal-parietal region (i.e., C6, P6, PO8, P4, PO4); ipsilateral temporal-parietal region (i.e., T7, TP7, P7); frontal-central region (i.e., F1, Fz, F2, FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2). Temporal regions of interest in NEPs. The time course of neural modulations exerted by the factors mismatch and attention Figure 5. Scalp topographies of NEPs and identification of srois. Top panel: Series of 12 scalp topographies of group average NEPs ( ms) at four different conditions (Ad: active-deviant, As: active-standard, Pd: passive-deviant, and Ps: passive-standard) are displayed with a 20-ms interval. Color scale corresponds to amplitude (mv). Note that the scalp topography of the early part of the response displays a contralaterally distributed negativity from 140 to 180 ms (left hand stimulation), whereas the scalp topography of the late part of the response displays a centrally distributed positivity from 180 to 360 ms. Bottom panel: Two-way repeated measures ANOVA to assess the effect of mismatch (deviant vs. standard) and attention (active vs. passive) on NEPs to define significant spatial regions. Series of 12 scalp topographies showing each of the factors and their interaction on NEPs ( ms) are displayed with a 20-ms interval. Color scale corresponds to the F value. The factor mismatch significantly modulated NEPs in three distinct srois (contralateral temporal region: T8, TP8; ipsilateral temporal region: CP5, P7; central region: FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2). The factor attention significantly modulated NEPs in three distinct srois (contralateral temporal-parietal region: C6, P6, PO8, P4, PO4; ipsilateral temporal-parietal region: T7, TP7, P7; frontal-central region: F1, Fz, F2, FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2).

10 Nociceptive system and the mismatch negativity 167 Figure 6. NEPs at the mismatch modulated srois. Left panel: Within each mismatch modulated sroi, point-by-point, two-way repeated measures ANOVA with the factors mismatch and attention, combined with nonparametric permutation testing, was used to define the mismatch modulated trois. x axis, latency (ms); y axis, amplitude (mv). The gray scale represents the F value for the factor mismatch. At the contralateral temporal region, the factor mismatch significantly modulated NEPs in two trois (a: ms; b: ms). At the ipsilateral temporal region, the factor mismatch significantly modulated NEPs in two trois (c: ms; d: ms). At the central region, the factor mismatch significantly modulated NEPs in two trois (e: ms; f: ms). Right panel: Within each defined mismatch modulated troi (a, b, c, d, e, and f), the amplitudes (mean SEM) across all participants are displayed using bar plots in the left part of this panel, and the corresponding scalp topographies showing the modulatory effect of attention ([Ad - As + Pd - Ps]/2) are displayed in the right part of this panel.

11 168 L. Hu et al. Figure 7. NEPs at the attention modulated srois. Left panel: Within each attention modulated sroi, point-by-point, two-way repeated measures ANOVA with the factors mismatch and attention, combined with nonparametric permutation testing, was used to define the attention modulated trois. x axis, latency (ms); y axis, amplitude (mv). The gray scale represents the F value for the factor attention. At the contralateral temporal-parietal region, the factor attention significantly modulated NEPs in two trois (a: ms; b: ms). At the ipsilateral temporal-parietal region, the factor attention significantly modulated NEPs in one troi (c: ms). At the frontal-central region, the factor attention significantly modulated NEPs in two trois (d: ms; e: ms). Right panel: Within each defined attention modulated troi (a, b, c, d, and e), the amplitudes (mean SEM) across all participants are displayed using bar plots in the left part of this panel, and the corresponding scalp topographies showing the modulatory effect of attention ([Ad + As - Pd - Ps]/2) are displayed in the right part of this panel.

12 Nociceptive system and the mismatch negativity 169 within each modulated sroi is represented in Figure 6 (left panel) and Figure 7 (left panel), respectively (cf. Table 1 for details). Main effect of mismatch. The factor mismatch had a strong modulatory impact on the significant NEP srois by exerting a negative amplitude modulation at both contralateral temporal region (NEP-M-tROI 3 a: ms; NEP-M-tROI b: ms) and ipsilateral temporal region (NEP-M-tROI c: ms; NEP-M-tROI d: ms), as well as a positive amplitude modulation at central region (NEP-M-tROI e: ms; NEP-M-tROI f: ms). For each troi, the modulatory effect of mismatch on scalp topography was represented as the difference between response amplitudes obtained in the deviant and standard conditions during both active and passive conditions, according to the formula ([Ad - As + Pd - Ps]/2) (Figure 6, right panel). Main effect of attention. The factor attention had a similar modulatory impact on the significant NEP srois by exerting a negative amplitude modulation at both contralateral temporalparietal region (NEP-A-tROI 4 a: ms; NEP-A-tROI b: ms) and ipsilateral temporal-parietal region (NEP-A-tROI c: ms), as well as a positive amplitude modulation at frontal-central region (NEP-A-tROI d: ms; NEP-A-tROI e: ms). For each troi, the modulatory effect of attention 3. NEP-M-tROI: NEP troi that was significantly modulated by the factor mismatch. 4. NEP-A-tROI: NEP troi that was significantly modulated by the factor attention. on scalp topography was represented as the difference of response amplitudes between active and passive conditions during deviant and standard trials, according to the formula ([Ad + As - Pd - Ps]/2) (Figure 7, right panel). Interaction of mismatch and attention. Post hoc analysis (Table 2) revealed that NEP amplitudes were significantly larger in Ad than in As (p <.001) and in Pd (p <.001) conditions at the NEP-M-tROI f ( ms). Similar to SEPs, the interaction was explained by higher amplitudes during the processing of deviant stimuli and when the focus of attention was directed towards the stimulation (Figure 6, bottom left and right panels). The Identification of MMN Activity The difference waves obtained during the active (Ad-As), rather than passive conditions (Pd-Ps), showed a general increase in signal amplitude for both SEPs and NEPs (Figure 8), especially during the late latencies at central locations (Figure 8, right panel). However, for the purpose of MMN activity identification the pattern of significant trois that was statistically affected only by the factor mismatch while being unaffected by the factor attention has been considered. In particular, Figure 8 shows the modulations included in a biologically plausible MMN latency range ( ms) for both SEPs (top panel) and NEPs (bottom panel). As SEP-M-tROIs a ( ms) and d ( ms), as well as NEP-M-tROIs a ( ms), c ( ms), and d ( ms), were located within such latency range, they were identified as candidates of pure automatic preattentional mismatch detection responses. Figure 8. The effect of mismatch as represented by the comparison between active and passive difference waveforms. SEPs elicited by non-nociceptive TES (top) and NEPs elicited by nociceptive IES (bottom) associated to both srois and trois that were significantly modulated only by the factor mismatch (displayed in Figure 3 and Figure 6, left column), and included in a plausible MMN latency range ( ms, in light green). Importantly, SEP-M-tROIs a and d as well as NEP-M-tROIs a, c, and d were statistically unaffected by attention, and thus can be considered as candidates of pure automatic preattentional mismatch detection. Note how the amplitude of ERP waveforms was increased by directed attention (the comparison between Ad-As and Pd-Ps) to non-nociceptive somatosensory and nociceptive stimuli, particularly for the positivity at late latencies in central region (right panel).

13 170 L. Hu et al. Summary of Findings Discussion Here, we aimed to dissect the selective contribution of (a) the effect of a deviant sensory event in a stream of standard events, and (b) the effect of being distracted from such stream of events, in determining the appearance of change detection responses during selective nociceptive stimulation (as compared to somatosensory stimulation), in the context of a roving paradigm (Figure 1). We observed three main findings. First, the factor mismatch (standard vs. deviant) significantly modulated three distinct srois for both SEPs and NEPs (bilateral temporal and central regions). Similarly, the factor attention (active vs. passive) significantly modulated three distinct srois (bilateral temporal-parietal and frontal-central regions) for both SEPs and NEPs (Figures 2 and 5, bottom panels). Second, we disclosed the time course of the amplitude modulation exerted by the factors mismatch and attention on each significant sroi. This analysis revealed that both mismatch and attention significantly modulated early and late poststimulus activity elicited by both non-nociceptive TES (Figures 3 and 4) and nociceptive IES (Figures 6 and 7), thus suggesting a high sensitivity of both SEPs and NEPs to the break of regularities, despite the manipulation of the attentional focus. Third, within mismatch modulated srois, some trois in the expected MMN latency range were modulated by the factor mismatch without being concurrently affected by the factor attention (Figure 8). Altogether, these findings brought about evidence that (a) mismatch brain responses can be elicited by nociceptive stimuli as much as those elicited by non-nociceptive somatosensory stimuli, provided that the effect of attention is accounted for; and (b) these responses are topographically similar but crucially different in their contralateral and ipsilateral onset of activation. The Effect of Mismatch on SEPs and NEPs The trois identified as smmn and nmmn were distributed at bilateral temporal regions (Figure 8). Recent studies on somatosensory mismatch brain responses localized the generation of mismatch responses over the contralateral primary and secondary somatosensory cortex (SI and SII) using MEG (Akatsuka et al., 2007). In this regard, it is important to note that one of the main potential generators of the putative smmn and nmmn, namely SII, is considered to play a role in tactile objective recognition and memory (Seitz, Roland, Bohm, Greitz, & Stone-Elander, 1991). Importantly, two spatiotemporal features could be appreciated when comparing the significant modulation of smmn and nmmn (see Figures 3 and 6, left panel, first two graphs from top): (1) The amplitude modulation of the smmn had an earlier onset than the nmmn both at the contralateral temporal region (SEP-M-tROI a: 110 ms vs. NEP-M-tROI a: 182 ms) and ipsilateral temporal region (SEP-M-tROI d: 162 ms vs. NEP-M-tROI c: 186 ms) a finding that is fully compatible with the conduction velocity of somatosensory Ab and nociceptive Ad fibers (Ploner, Schmitz, Freund, & Schnitzler, 1999, 2000), respectively. (2) The difference of latency between the contralateral and the ipsilateral onset of activity was larger for the smmn than for the nmmn (52 ms vs. 4 ms), thus supporting the notion that nociceptive processing is faster than tactile processing in the human brain; that is, somatosensory inputs are processed in a serial fashion (e.g., Hu, Zhang, & Hu, 2012) whereas the nociceptive inputs are processed in a parallel manner (Liang, Mouraux, & Iannetti, 2011; Ploner, Gross, Timmermann, & Schnitzler, 2006; Ploner, Schoffelen, Schnitzler, & Gross, 2009). Crucially, as the topographies of these responses largely overlapped, latency discrepancies are the main salient differences between smmn and nmmn. The Effect of Attention on SEPs and NEPs The impact of attention on SEPs and NEPs had no less importance than the factor mismatch in determining amplitude modulations. As observed in previous studies, active attention enhanced eventrelated potential (ERP) amplitudes related to passive shifts of attention to somatosensory stimuli (Kida, Nishihira, Wasaka, Nakata, & Sakamoto, 2004a, 2004b; Kida, Wasaka, Nakata, Akatsuka, & Kakigi, 2006). Noteworthy is that, while part of the negative activity at bilateral temporal regions resembled the activity associated to the mismatch-related modulation, its vertex positive counterpart was distributed at frontal-central electrodes (maximal at FCz, Figures 4 and 7, right panels) rather than at central sites (maximal at Cz, Figures 3 and 6, right panels), as result of the effect of attention. The allocation of attention may trigger the activation of medial structures, such as different portions of the anterior and mid cingulate cortex, which is widely acknowledged as one of the most important areas responsible for the allocation of voluntary and involuntary attention, regardless of the sensory modality used in the experimental task (e.g., see Legrain, Iannetti, Plaghki, & Mouraux, 2011, for a review). It is worth noting that a prefrontal activation was reported by the original study of somatosensory mismatch responses (Kekoni et al., 1997). This generator was observed in auditory mismatch studies as well, and interpreted as the source of a top-down modulation of the deviance detection system in the temporal cortices (e.g., Doeller et al., 2003). However, contrary to the literature on auditory mismatch responses, it is still unclear which central and frontal structures may be involved in somatosensory change detection responses. The present findings hint of a potential difference in the amount of structures recruited during SEPs versus NEPs. In particular, SEPs were associated to larger mismatch modulated srois than NEPs, whereas NEPs were coupled to larger attention modulated srois than SEPs (Figures 2 7). This observation may suggest an important functional difference between nociceptive and somatosensory change detection responses, associated to voluntary and involuntary attentional gains. This notion is consistent with the general consensus on the ability of pain to grab attention per se (Seminowicz & Davis, 2007; Wiech, Ploner, & Tracey, 2008). Methodological Considerations Acrucial methodological aspect of this study rests on the application of intraepidermal electrodes to preferentially stimulate skin nociceptors (Inui et al., 2002, 2006). The IES allowed us to implement a roving paradigm, whereby deviants and standards had exactly the same number of trials and physical properties. This procedure ensured that standards were really perceived as standards, and that the only difference between deviants and standards was associated to their position in the train (i.e., deviant first, standard last). Conversely, radiant heat stimulators such as Nd: YAP (neodymiumdoped yttrium aluminum perovskite) and CO 2 laser stimulators, although widely used to selectively activate nociceptive Ad fibers, are affected by a few important limitations, which prevented their application in the present context. First, radiant heat stimulators require the delivery of each single thermal pulse either at long ISIs (at least 5 to 10 s) and/or at different spatial locations after each trial,

14 Nociceptive system and the mismatch negativity 171 to avoid skin overheating, nociceptors fatigue, and/or sensitization (Plaghki & Mouraux, 2003). This technical aspect is clearly incompatible with the requirements of a roving paradigm (or similar oddball paradigm used to elicit a MMN). In fact, both long ISIs and the displacement of each single sensory event would have hampered the creation of real standard events, due to their inner disparity in time and space. Second, currently available laser stimulators do not allow online skin temperature control, which changes independently from the physical properties of the impending stimuli (Plaghki & Mouraux, 2003). Altogether, these reasons concurred to prove that IES was the most effective way to determine the identification of a possible nmmn. A second methodological aspect of the present study concerns the experimental design. An important characteristic of the MMN generation in classic auditory oddball paradigms rests in the possibility to detect this ERP component when the participant is not paying attention to the sensory event, as compared with a condition of active attention. However, many studies simply did not include a condition whereby the participant is required to attend to the sensory event, thus assuming that the sole condition of inattention would suffice showing the MMN phenomenon (Sussman, 2007). Unfortunately, this methodological flaw applies to most previous somatosensory change detection studies as well (e.g., Kekoni et al., 1997; Shinozaki, Yabe, Sutoh, Hiruma, & Kaneko, 1998). It is worth noting that the majority of studies investigating the MMN phenomenon assumed that watching a silent video with subtitles while ignoring the sensory test stimulation is the best means to obtain a mismatch response. However, other authors have put forth that the MMN elicited under these conditions may not be an attention-independent measure because these tasks do not require highly focused attention (Haroush, Hochstein, & Deouell, 2010). Conversely, asking the subject to disregard the sensory test stimulation (e.g., auditory) while focusing on a concurrent demanding task (e.g., detection of visual stimuli) should provide a better characterization of the MMN response. In the present experiment, participants were asked to report details and plots related to the movie watched during the passive condition, thus requiring them to deploy attention. Nevertheless, an active distraction associated to a high attentional load exerted by a visual search task could be implemented by future studies on nmmn, to further detail the methodological constraints of this phenomenon. A last methodological issue requires the reader s attention. Somatosensory and nociceptive afferents have different conduction pathways (Ploner et al., 1999, 2000). This feature makes difficult any direct quantitative comparison between smmn and nmmn, as both latencies and amplitudes would be intrinsically different regardless of the experimental manipulation, and such difference would in turn affect the partitioning of variance and its attribution to a given experimental condition (this would apply to a direct quantitative comparison of auditory and visual MMN as well). Thus, our experimental investigation implied a qualitative comparison of the smmn and nmmn latency profiles. Theoretical Considerations On the basis of the current results, we speculate that the identified modulations may represent an index of automatic detection of nociceptive change. Yet, the present experiment does not allow us to answer whether the purported mismatch responses may rely only on sensory memory mechanisms (Naatanen, Paavilainen, Rinne, & Alho, 2007) or whether they are contributed by neural refractoriness and lateral inhibition mechanisms (May & Tiitinen, 2010). Interestingly, previous research in the domain of cortical responses elicited by repeated nociceptive stimuli of identical energy (Iannetti, Hughes, Lee, & Mouraux, 2008; Wang, Mouraux, Liang, & Iannetti, 2010) seems to support the notion that the suppression of nociceptive-related ERPs are strongly determined by the novelty (as a function of saliency) of the eliciting stimulus (Valentini, Torta, Mouraux, & Iannetti, 2011; Wang et al., 2010), rather than by neural refractoriness per se (Truini, Galeotti, Cruccu, & Garcia- Larrea, 2007; Truini et al., 2004). However, contemporary research in the field of auditory MMN increasingly put forth the idea that neural adaptation and sensory memory may be reconciled within the predictive coding framework (Garrido, Kilner, Stephan, & Friston, 2009). According to this account, the MMN represents a failure to suppress prediction error when novel and rare stimuli are presented once a generative model of the standards was established. It is claimed that the predictive coding theory may account both for the adjustment of a mnestic model of the ongoing stimulus trains (i.e., the model-adjustment hypothesis; Naatanen & Winkler, 1999) and for the local changes in postsynaptic sensitivity (i.e., adaptation hypothesis; Jaaskelainen et al., 2011), likely contributing to perceptual learning. Interestingly, a recent study proposed a neuronal model of MMN generation in the auditory cortex based on the predictive coding features, without any additional assumption associated to synaptic habituation (Wacongne, Changeux, & Dehaene, 2012). This model was able to account for several empirical properties of MMN such as frequency-dependent response to rare deviants and response to unexpected repetitions in alternating sensory events. We agree with the authors of this study that these findings provide strong evidence in support of the notion that the MMN phenomenon can be interpreted as an index of novelty detection while arguing against a strong contribution of neural refractoriness, and thus strengthening the notion of a purely predictive account of MMN regardless of neural refractoriness. In conclusion, the MMN has been advocated as a tool for assessing abnormal brain function in a large number of clinical conditions, except that of chronic pain conditions (Naatanen et al., 2011). Further research effort is therefore mandatory to disclose the extent of this phenomenon in nociceptive-related ERPs. References Akatsuka, K., Wasaka, T., Nakata, H., Kida, T., Hoshiyama, M., Tamura, Y., & Kakigi, R. (2007). Objective examination for two-point stimulation using a somatosensory oddball paradigm: An MEG study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, doi: /j.clinph Boly, M., Garrido, M. I., Gosseries, O., Bruno, M. A., Boveroux, P., Schnakers, C.,... Friston, K. (2011). Preserved feedforward but impaired top-down processes in the vegetative state. Science, 332, doi: /science Czigler, I., Balazs, L., & Pato, L. G. (2004). Visual change detection: Event-related potentials are dependent on stimulus location in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 364, doi: /j.neulet Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134, doi: /j.jneumeth Doeller, C. F., Opitz, B., Mecklinger, A., Krick, C., Reith, W., & Schroger, E. (2003). Prefrontal cortex involvement in preattentive

Title change detection system in the visu

Title change detection system in the visu Title Attention switching function of mem change detection system in the visu Author(s) Kimura, Motohiro; Katayama, Jun'ich Citation International Journal of Psychophys Issue Date 2008-02 DOI Doc URLhttp://hdl.handle.net/2115/33891

More information

Determinants of Laser-Evoked EEG Responses: Pain Perception or Stimulus Saliency?

Determinants of Laser-Evoked EEG Responses: Pain Perception or Stimulus Saliency? J Neurophysiol 100: 815 828, 2008. First published June 4, 2008; doi:10.1152/jn.00097.2008. Determinants of Laser-Evoked EEG Responses: Pain Perception or Stimulus Saliency? G. D. Iannetti, 1 N. P. Hughes,

More information

An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials

An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials Psychophysiology, 36 ~1999!, 22 34. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1999 Society for Psychophysiological Research An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related

More information

Novelty is not enough: laser-evoked potentials are determined by stimulus saliency, not absolute novelty

Novelty is not enough: laser-evoked potentials are determined by stimulus saliency, not absolute novelty J Neurophysiol 109: 692 701, 2013. First published November 7, 2012; doi:10.1152/jn.00464.2012. Novelty is not enough: laser-evoked potentials are determined by stimulus saliency, not absolute novelty

More information

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change Elena Yago, MarõÂa Jose Corral and Carles Escera CA Neurodynamics Laboratory, Department

More information

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 827 831. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research BRIEF REPT Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the

More information

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics Scott Makeig Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla CA sccn.ucsd.edu Talk given at the EEG/MEG course

More information

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task Psychophysiology, 44 (2007), 913 917. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2007 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00561.x BRIEF REPORT Reward prediction

More information

What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3

What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 * Manuscript-title pg, abst, fig... Debener et al. Independent components of the auditory novelty oddball What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related

More information

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Biological Psychology 66 (2004) 1 20 The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes Bettina Forster, Martin Eimer School of

More information

Supporting Information

Supporting Information Supporting Information ten Oever and Sack 10.1073/pnas.1517519112 SI Materials and Methods Experiment 1. Participants. A total of 20 participants (9 male; age range 18 32 y; mean age 25 y) participated

More information

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction Proceedings of the First International Conference on Complex Medical Engineering (CME2005) May 15-18, 2005, Takamatsu, Japan (Organized Session No. 20). Paper No. 150, pp. 715-719. Event-Related Potentials

More information

The Journal of Physiology

The Journal of Physiology J Physiol 596.6 (28) pp 3655 3673 3655 High-precision voluntary movements are largely independent of preceding vertex potentials elicited by sudden sensory events M. Kilintari,R.J.Bufacchi,G.Novembre,Y.Guo,P.Haggard

More information

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY OF UNIMODAL AND AUDIOVISUAL SPEECH PERCEPTION

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY OF UNIMODAL AND AUDIOVISUAL SPEECH PERCEPTION AVSP 2 International Conference on Auditory-Visual Speech Processing ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY OF UNIMODAL AND AUDIOVISUAL SPEECH PERCEPTION Lynne E. Bernstein, Curtis W. Ponton 2, Edward T. Auer, Jr. House Ear

More information

International Journal of Neurology Research

International Journal of Neurology Research International Journal of Neurology Research Online Submissions: http://www.ghrnet.org/index./ijnr/ doi:1.1755/j.issn.313-511.1..5 Int. J. of Neurology Res. 1 March (1): 1-55 ISSN 313-511 ORIGINAL ARTICLE

More information

Activation of the auditory pre-attentive change detection system by tone repetitions with fast stimulation rate

Activation of the auditory pre-attentive change detection system by tone repetitions with fast stimulation rate Cognitive Brain Research 10 (2001) 323 327 www.elsevier.com/ locate/ bres Short communication Activation of the auditory pre-attentive change detection system by tone repetitions with fast stimulation

More information

Temporal integration: intentional sound discrimination does not modulate stimulus-driven processes in auditory event synthesis

Temporal integration: intentional sound discrimination does not modulate stimulus-driven processes in auditory event synthesis Clinical Neurophysiology 113 (2002) 1909 1920 www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Temporal integration: intentional sound discrimination does not modulate stimulus-driven processes in auditory event synthesis

More information

Theta Oscillation Related to the Auditory Discrimination Process in Mismatch Negativity: Oddball versus Control Paradigm

Theta Oscillation Related to the Auditory Discrimination Process in Mismatch Negativity: Oddball versus Control Paradigm ORIGINAL ARTICLE J Clin Neurol 2012;8:35-42 Print ISSN 1738-6586 / On-line ISSN 2005-5013 http://dx.doi.org/10.3988/jcn.2012.8.1.35 Open Access Theta Oscillation Related to the Auditory Discrimination

More information

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm

The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm Research paper Acta Neurobiol Exp 2008, 68: 362 372 The auditory P3 from passive and active three-stimulus oddball paradigm Eligiusz Wronka 1,2 *, Jan Kaiser 1, and Anton M.L. Coenen 2 1 Institute of Psychology,

More information

Stefan Debener a,b, *, Scott Makeig c, Arnaud Delorme c, Andreas K. Engel a,b. Research report

Stefan Debener a,b, *, Scott Makeig c, Arnaud Delorme c, Andreas K. Engel a,b. Research report Cognitive Brain Research 22 (2005) 309 321 Research report What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component

More information

Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory

Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory Elyse S. Sussman CA and Valentina Gumenyuk Department of Neuroscience, Albert Einstein College of Medicine,141

More information

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing Cognitive Neuroscience 0 0 0 0 0 p Website publication November NeuroReport, () ONE of the basic properties of the auditory system is the ability to analyse complex temporal patterns. Here, we investigated

More information

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study Armita Faghani Jadidi, Raheleh Davoodi, Mohammad Hassan Moradi Department of Biomedical Engineering Amirkabir University

More information

Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials

Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials Clinical Neurophysiology 112 (2001) 2031 2045 www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials Mercedes Atienza a, *, José

More information

Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm

Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm Ž. Cognitive Brain Research 7 1998 Research report Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm Erich Schroger a,), Christian Wolff b a Institut

More information

Alpha and Gamma Oscillation Amplitudes Synergistically Predict the Perception of Forthcoming Nociceptive Stimuli

Alpha and Gamma Oscillation Amplitudes Synergistically Predict the Perception of Forthcoming Nociceptive Stimuli r Human Brain Mapping 37:501 514 (2016) r Alpha and Gamma Oscillation Amplitudes Synergistically Predict the Perception of Forthcoming Nociceptive Stimuli Yiheng Tu, 1,2 Zhiguo Zhang, 2,3,4 * Ao Tan, 2

More information

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Sleep Deprivation and Selective Attention An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention Logan T. Trujillo, PhD 1 ; Steve Kornguth,

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential Title of Thesis Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential 2014 September Yang Weiping The Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology (Doctor s Course)

More information

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure American Orthotic and Prosthetic Association (AOPA) Center for Orthotics and Prosthetics Leraning and Outcomes/Evidence-Based Practice (COPL) Final Summary 2-28-14 Project Title: Cognitive Workload During

More information

The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential

The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential IstvaÂn Winkler 1,2, Erich SchroÈger 3, and Nelson Cowan 4 Abstract & The mismatch negativity (MMN)

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Teaching brain-machine interfaces as an alternative paradigm to neuroprosthetics control

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Teaching brain-machine interfaces as an alternative paradigm to neuroprosthetics control SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Teaching brain-machine interfaces as an alternative paradigm to neuroprosthetics control Authors: Iñaki Iturrate 1,2, Ricardo Chavarriaga 2, Luis Montesano 1, Javier Minguez 1,

More information

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln 1 DATA MANAGEMENT Backups Storage Identification Analyses 2 Data Analysis Pre-processing Statistical Analysis

More information

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14 CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines Assignment 5: Neural mechanisms of object-based attention Due: Friday, April 14 This Assignment is a guided reading of the 2014 paper, Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based

More information

Separate memory-related processing for auditory frequency and patterns

Separate memory-related processing for auditory frequency and patterns Psychophysiology, 36 ~1999!, 737 744. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1999 Society for Psychophysiological Research Separate memory-related processing for auditory frequency and

More information

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:296 301 DOI 10.3758/s13423-012-0353-4 BRIEF REPORT Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention Risa Sawaki & Steven J. Luck Published online: 20 December 2012 #

More information

Research Article Mindfulness Trait Predicts Neurophysiological Reactivity Associated with Negativity Bias: An ERP Study

Research Article Mindfulness Trait Predicts Neurophysiological Reactivity Associated with Negativity Bias: An ERP Study Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine Volume 5, Article ID 368, 5 pages http://dx.doi.org/.55/5/368 Research Article Mindfulness Trait Predicts Neurophysiological Reactivity Associated

More information

Differentiation of conversive sensory loss and malingering by P300 in a modified oddball task

Differentiation of conversive sensory loss and malingering by P300 in a modified oddball task Pain 0 0 0 0 0 p Website publication January NeuroReport, () WE applied the methodology of evoked potentials (EP) to reveal the functional level of abnormality in a patient with circumscribed complete

More information

The spatio temporal dynamics of deviance and target detection in the passive and active auditory oddball paradigm: a sloreta study

The spatio temporal dynamics of deviance and target detection in the passive and active auditory oddball paradigm: a sloreta study https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-018-0422-3 BMC Neuroscience RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access The spatio temporal dynamics of deviance and target detection in the passive and active auditory oddball paradigm:

More information

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture Psychophysiology, 53 (2016), 650 662. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright VC 2016 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12615 The role of top-down spatial attention

More information

High-precision voluntary movements are largely independent from preceding vertex potentials elicited by sudden sensory events

High-precision voluntary movements are largely independent from preceding vertex potentials elicited by sudden sensory events DOI: 10.1113/JP275715 High-precision voluntary movements are largely independent from preceding vertex potentials elicited by sudden sensory events M Kilintari 1, RJ Bufacchi 1, G Novembre 1, Y Guo 1,

More information

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Cognitive Brain Research 18 (2004) 255 272 Research report Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures Audrey Duarte a, *, Charan Ranganath

More information

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials Chinese Science Bulletin 2007 Science in China Press Springer-Verlag Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials SONG Yan 1, PENG

More information

Effects of discrepancy between imagined and perceived sounds on the N2 component of the event-related potential

Effects of discrepancy between imagined and perceived sounds on the N2 component of the event-related potential Psychophysiology, 47 (2010), 289 298. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2009 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00936.x Effects of discrepancy

More information

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction Alexandra Muller-Gass, Erich

More information

AUDITORY ERPS IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL COORDINATION DISORDER

AUDITORY ERPS IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL COORDINATION DISORDER ... ANS: Journal for Neurocognitive Research Journal Homepage: www.activitas.org SHORT COMMUNICATION... AUDITORY ERPS IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL COORDINATION DISORDER Irena Holeckova, 1 Ladislav Cepicka,

More information

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures

Event-related potentials as an index of similarity between words and pictures Psychophysiology, 42 (25), 361 368. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 25 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 1.1111/j.1469-8986.25.295.x BRIEF REPORT Event-related potentials

More information

Supplementary material

Supplementary material Supplementary material S1. Event-related potentials Event-related potentials (ERPs) were calculated for stimuli for each visual field (mean of low, medium and high spatial frequency stimuli). For each

More information

An EEG-based Approach for Evaluating Audio Notifications under Ambient Sounds

An EEG-based Approach for Evaluating Audio Notifications under Ambient Sounds An EEG-based Approach for Evaluating Audio Notifications under Ambient Sounds Yi-Chieh Lee 1, Wen-Chieh Lin 1, Jung-Tai King 2, Li-Wei Ko 2,3, Yu-Ting Huang 1, Fu-Yin Cherng 1 1 Dept. of Computer Science,

More information

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals Takako Mitsudo *1, Yoshitaka Nakajima 2, Gerard B. Remijn 3, Hiroshige Takeichi 4, Yoshinobu Goto

More information

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source

More information

Thoughts of Death Modulate Psychophysical and Cortical Responses to Threatening Stimuli

Thoughts of Death Modulate Psychophysical and Cortical Responses to Threatening Stimuli Thoughts of Death Modulate Psychophysical and Cortical Responses to Threatening Stimuli Elia Valentini 1,2 *, Katharina Koch 1,2, Salvatore Maria Aglioti 1,2 1 Sapienza Università di Roma, Dipartimento

More information

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2004 Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude Tim Skinner University of South Florida Follow this and

More information

ANALYZING EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS

ANALYZING EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS Adavanced Lifespan Neurocognitive Development: EEG signal processing for lifespan research Dr. Manosusos Klados Liesa Ilg ANALYZING EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS Chair for Lifespan Developmental Neuroscience

More information

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention Tineke Grent- t-jong 1,2, Marty G. Woldorff 1,3* PLoS BIOLOGY 1 Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University,

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage

ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage INO056 10/18/04 6:34 PM Page 339 CHAPTER 56 ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage Leon Y. Deouell and Robert T. Knight ABSTRACT Maintaining a goal-directed behavior requires

More information

Changes in the somatosensory N250 and P300 by the variation of reaction time

Changes in the somatosensory N250 and P300 by the variation of reaction time Eur J Appl Physiol (2003) 89: 326 330 DOI 10.1007/s00421-003-0801-y ORIGINAL ARTICLE Tetsuo Kida Æ Yoshiaki Nishihira Æ Arihiro Hatta Toshiaki Wasaka Æ Hiroki Nakata Masanori Sakamoto Æ Tsuyoshi Nakajima

More information

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA) Comments Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA) Is phase locking to transposed stimuli as good as phase locking to low-frequency

More information

This article was originally published in the Encyclopedia of Neuroscience published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's

More information

Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene

Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene Marissa L. Gamble and Marty G. Woldorff Abstract To make sense of our dynamic and complex auditory environment, we must be able

More information

This presentation is the intellectual property of the author. Contact them for permission to reprint and/or distribute.

This presentation is the intellectual property of the author. Contact them for permission to reprint and/or distribute. Modified Combinatorial Nomenclature Montage, Review, and Analysis of High Density EEG Terrence D. Lagerlund, M.D., Ph.D. CP1208045-16 Disclosure Relevant financial relationships None Off-label/investigational

More information

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention.

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention. Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention. All behaviors unfold over time; therefore, our ability to perceive and adapt our behavior according to the temporal constraints

More information

PROCESSING OF STIMULUS REPETITION AND CHANGE IN THE SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEM: RECORDINGS OF ELECTRICAL AND MAGNETIC BRAIN RESPONSES

PROCESSING OF STIMULUS REPETITION AND CHANGE IN THE SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEM: RECORDINGS OF ELECTRICAL AND MAGNETIC BRAIN RESPONSES PROCESSING OF STIMULUS REPETITION AND CHANGE IN THE SOMATOSENSORY SYSTEM: RECORDINGS OF ELECTRICAL AND MAGNETIC BRAIN RESPONSES Doctoral Dissertation JOUNI KEKONI Department of Psychology University of

More information

Early Occurrence Of Auditory Change Detection In The Human Brain

Early Occurrence Of Auditory Change Detection In The Human Brain Early Occurrence Of Auditory Change Detection In The Human Brain SABINE GRIMM University of Leipzig, Germany (formerly University of Barcelona, Spain) ICON, Brisbane, July 28 th, 2014 Auditory deviance

More information

Reward positivity is elicited by monetary reward in the absence of response choice Sergio Varona-Moya a,b, Joaquín Morís b,c and David Luque b,d

Reward positivity is elicited by monetary reward in the absence of response choice Sergio Varona-Moya a,b, Joaquín Morís b,c and David Luque b,d 152 Clinical neuroscience Reward positivity is elicited by monetary reward in the absence of response choice Sergio Varona-Moya a,b, Joaquín Morís b,c and David Luque b,d The neural response to positive

More information

BRAIN RESEARCH 1104 (2006) available at

BRAIN RESEARCH 1104 (2006) available at available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Comparative analysis of event-related potentials during Go/NoGo and CPT: Decomposition of electrophysiological markers

More information

Effects of multiple congruent cues on concurrent sound segregation during passive and active listening: An event-related potential (ERP) study

Effects of multiple congruent cues on concurrent sound segregation during passive and active listening: An event-related potential (ERP) study *Manuscript Click here to view linked References 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 Effects of multiple congruent cues on concurrent sound segregation during passive and active listening: An event-related potential

More information

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology Rui Cao (caorui.beilia@gmail.com) Thomas A. Busey (busey@indiana.edu) Robert M. Nosofsky (nosofsky@indiana.edu) Richard

More information

Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action

Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action Clare Press 1,2, Elena Gherri 1, Cecilia Heyes 3, and Martin Eimer 1 Abstract Several theories of the mechanisms linking perception and action

More information

Research Paper. 1. Introduction

Research Paper. 1. Introduction Research Paper Touch inhibits subcortical and cortical nociceptive responses Flavia Mancini a,b, *, Anne-Lise Beaumont a,lihu c, Patrick Haggard b, Gian Domenico D. Iannetti a Abstract The neural mechanisms

More information

The mind s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control in internal attention shifting

The mind s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control in internal attention shifting Psychophysiology, 40 (2003), 572 585. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2003 Society for Psychophysiological Research The mind s eye, looking inward? In search of executive control

More information

D. Debatisse, E. Fornari, E. Pralong, P. Maeder, H Foroglou, M.H Tetreault, J.G Villemure. NCH-UNN and Neuroradiology Dpt. CHUV Lausanne Switzerland

D. Debatisse, E. Fornari, E. Pralong, P. Maeder, H Foroglou, M.H Tetreault, J.G Villemure. NCH-UNN and Neuroradiology Dpt. CHUV Lausanne Switzerland Vegetative comatose and auditory oddball paradigm with Cognitive evoked potentials (CEPs) and fmri: Implications for the consciousness model of Damasio and Guerit D. Debatisse, E. Fornari, E. Pralong,

More information

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014)

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014) Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014) Christian Graulty, Orestis Papaioannou, Phoebe Bauer, Michael Pitts & Enriqueta Canseco-Gonzalez, Reed College. Funded by the Murdoch

More information

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search Clayton Hickey 1,2, Vincent Di Lollo 2, and John J. McDonald 2 Abstract & Attentional selection of a target presented among

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing Categorical Speech Representation in the Human Superior Temporal Gyrus Edward F. Chang, Jochem W. Rieger, Keith D. Johnson, Mitchel S. Berger, Nicholas M. Barbaro, Robert T. Knight SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

More information

Clinical Neurophysiology

Clinical Neurophysiology Clinical Neurophysiology 120 (2009) 453 463 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Clinical Neurophysiology journal homepage: www. elsevier. com/ locate/ clinph Invited review The mismatch negativity:

More information

Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness and REM sleep

Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness and REM sleep Psychophysiology, 37 ~2000!, 485 493. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2000 Society for Psychophysiological Research Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (4), 951-962 doi:10.3758/app.72.4.951 Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

More information

Montages are logical and orderly arrangements of channels

Montages are logical and orderly arrangements of channels GUIDELINE American Clinical Neurophysiology Society Guideline 3: A Proposal for Standard Montages to Be Used in Clinical EEG Jayant N. Acharya,* Abeer J. Hani, Partha D. Thirumala, and Tammy N. Tsuchida

More information

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 50 (2010) 329 339 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Switching associations between facial identity and emotional expression:

More information

Atypical processing of prosodic changes in natural speech stimuli in school-age children with Asperger syndrome

Atypical processing of prosodic changes in natural speech stimuli in school-age children with Asperger syndrome Atypical processing of prosodic changes in natural speech stimuli in school-age children with Asperger syndrome Riikka Lindström, PhD student Cognitive Brain Research Unit University of Helsinki 31.8.2012

More information

Electrophysiological Measures of Time Processing in Infant and Adult Brains: Weber s Law Holds

Electrophysiological Measures of Time Processing in Infant and Adult Brains: Weber s Law Holds Electrophysiological Measures of Time Processing in Infant and Adult Brains: Weber s Law Holds Elizabeth M. Brannon, Melissa E. Libertus, Warren H. Meck, and Marty G. Woldorff Abstract & Behavioral studies

More information

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System Christopher J. Rozell, Don. H. Johnson and Raymon M. Glantz Department of Electrical & Computer Engineering Department of

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI-0-0.R Manuscript Type: Short report Date Submitted by the Author:

More information

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL for Cardoso et al. 22 The Neuroimaging Signal is a Linear Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components. : Appendix: Homogeneous Linear ( Null ) and Modified Linear

More information

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition RESEARCH ARTICLE A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition Sameer Sethi 1 *, Dr. Simon Rigoulot 2, Dr. Marc D. Pell 3 1 Faculty of Science, McGill University,

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Submitted to Current Biology Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Anna Plodowski, Rachel Swainson, Georgina M. Jackson, Chris Rorden and Stephen R. Jackson School of Psychology

More information

Chapter 14: The Cutaneous Senses

Chapter 14: The Cutaneous Senses Chapter 14: The Cutaneous Senses Somatosensory System There are three parts Cutaneous senses - perception of touch and pain from stimulation of the skin Proprioception - ability to sense position of the

More information

ABSTRACT 1. INTRODUCTION 2. ARTIFACT REJECTION ON RAW DATA

ABSTRACT 1. INTRODUCTION 2. ARTIFACT REJECTION ON RAW DATA AUTOMATIC ARTIFACT REJECTION FOR EEG DATA USING HIGH-ORDER STATISTICS AND INDEPENDENT COMPONENT ANALYSIS A. Delorme, S. Makeig, T. Sejnowski CNL, Salk Institute 11 N. Torrey Pines Road La Jolla, CA 917,

More information

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings J. Besle, P. Lakatos, C.A. Schevon, R.R. Goodman, G.M. McKhann, A. Mehta, R.G. Emerson, C.E.

More information

Intracortical modulation, and not spinal inhibition, mediates placebo analgesia.

Intracortical modulation, and not spinal inhibition, mediates placebo analgesia. Intracortical modulation, and not spinal inhibition, mediates placebo analgesia. M. Martini *(1,2,3), M.C.H. Lee * (4), E. Valentini (1,2,3) & G.D. Iannetti (3) (1) Department of Psychology, Sapienza University

More information

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Cerebral Cortex March 2006;16:415-424 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi121 Advance Access publication June 15, 2005 Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study Mika

More information

International Journal of Psychophysiology

International Journal of Psychophysiology International Journal of Psychophysiology 75 (2010) 339 348 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect International Journal of Psychophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho Flanker

More information

Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fmri and ERP Results

Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fmri and ERP Results NeuroImage 15, 167 174 (2002) doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0970, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fmri

More information

Auditory sensory memory in 2-year-old children: an event-related potential study

Auditory sensory memory in 2-year-old children: an event-related potential study LEARNING AND MEMORY Auditory sensory memory in -year-old children: an event-related potential study Elisabeth Glass, te achse and Waldemar von uchodoletz Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

More information

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory? Nick Berggren and Martin Eimer Abstract During the retention of

More information

Towards natural human computer interaction in BCI

Towards natural human computer interaction in BCI Towards natural human computer interaction in BCI Ian Daly 1 (Student) and Slawomir J Nasuto 1 and Kevin Warwick 1 Abstract. BCI systems require correct classification of signals interpreted from the brain

More information