SCN Controlled Circadian Arousal and the Afternoon "Nap Zone"

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "SCN Controlled Circadian Arousal and the Afternoon "Nap Zone""

Transcription

1 Sleep Research Online 1(4): , Printed in the USA. All rights reserved X 1998 WebSciences SCN Controlled Circadian Arousal and the Afternoon "Nap Zone" Roger J. Broughton Division of Neurology, University of Ottawa and Ottawa Hospital (General Campus), Ottawa, K1H 8L6, Canada This paper outlines a conceptual model for the regulation of the circasemidian sleep propensity process with emphasis on a possible mechanism of the afternoon "nap zone". It is proposed that the afternoon nap zone is due to increasing sleep propensity after morning wakening (Borbély's Process-S) being overwhelmed by a light-sensitive SCN-dependent circadian arousal process of the type discovered by Edgar et al., (1993) and currently being identified in its pathways and neurochemistry by Jouvet and colleagues. It is maintained that this arousal process is reflected in the circadian core body temperature pattern, and that under normal entrained conditions the latter does not resemble a sine-wave or skewed sine-wave. Rather it is very asymmetrical in time and somewhat asymmetrical in amplitude. Cosinor type analyses which enforce symmetry in time and amplitude are therefore ill suited to adequately curve-fit the empirical data. The shape of the circadian arousal system was clarified by meta-analyses of data from three laboratories for three conditions: the normal entrained state, the constant routine, and temporal isolation. Under normal entrained conditions for about one-third of the circadian day core body temperature, and therefore the assumed intensity of the circadian arousal system, is below the mesor with the nadir being at about 0500h; and for about two-thirds of the circadian day it is above the mesor with the acrophase on average being at about 2100h. For modeling purposes, the homeostatic process (Process-S) employed the actual data of the Zurich laboratories for night sleep, but altered the equation for the daytime period to ensure an exponential increase after wake-up. Combining these modified processes indicated that the nap zone could be explained, as predicted, by an increasing homeostatic pressure for sleep across the daytime being reversed by the circadian arousal process. This 2-process combination predicted quite well the shape of the entire circasemidian sleep/wake propensity process and can explain the presence of morning sleep inertia without requiring a third process. It would appear that the circadian arousal process can be modified in phase and in amplitude by a number of normal and pathological conditions. CURRENT CLAIM: Altering the shape of the circadian core temperature curve from a sine-wave or skewed sine-wave to that of data empirically observed in the normal entrained state, equating this curve with the newly identified circadian arousal process, and combining it with a homeostatic sleep function, predicts quite accurately the daily circasemidian (biphasic) sleep/wake propensity curve. The circasemidian wake-sleep pattern When following the common pattern of habitual monophasic night sleep, we typically experience a transitory period of increased daytime sleepiness around the time of the mid-afternoon followed by a period of heightened arousal which then rapidly diminishes shortly before the onset of the next night of sleep. This so-called "afternoon nap zone" can be expressed variously as a temporary period of poorer performance (Blake, 1967); the last nap given up in ontogenetic development (Webb and Dinges, 1989); the return of an afternoon nap after retirement and in the elderly, when social constraints are reduced (Webb and Dinges, 1989); napping in students (Dinges et al., 1980); siesta taking both in cultures which limit night sleep amount (Broughton, 1983) and in so-called primitive tribes studied by combined anthropological and polysomnographic techniques (Quadens, 1990); the period of both maximum sleepiness and (more or less) irresistible sleep attacks in sleep disordered patients with excessive daytime sleepiness (Richardson et al., 1978; Broughton, 1989a); and an increased rate both of accidents attributed to sleepiness and of death from all causes (Mitler et al., 1988). In fact, despite its varying inter-individual and intraindividual variability, the afternoon nap zone represents the most intense predictable fluctuation of level of sleepiness/alertness experienced during the normal waking daytime period. Possible mechanisms Numerous possible mechanisms have been suggested for this secondary transient period of facilitated sleep. The initial proposal (Broughton, 1975) was that the major (typically night-time) sleep period together with the afternoon nap zone reflect an endogenous brain rhythm providing a twice a day increase in sleep propensity, i.e., that sleep/wake regulation shows a circasemidian (circa, about; semi, a half; dias, a day), as well as a circadian, pattern. It was also noted in this paper that the major and minor sleep periods are about 180 apart. An endogenous origin for the 2/day sleep propensity phenomenon was later proven by its presence when subjects slept ad libitum under conditions of temporal isolation (Zulley and Campbell, 1985). It was further suggested (Broughton, 1975) both that the pattern might reflect a 12-hour rhythm for slow wave sleep (SWS) which typefies the first third of night sleep and afternoon naps of sufficient duration to permit its appearance; Correspondence: Roger J. Broughton, M.D., Ph.D., Division of Neurology, Ottawa Hospital (General Campus), 501 Smyth Road, Ottawa, ON, Canada K1H 8L6, Tel: , Fax: , rbroughton@ogh.on.ca.

2 167 BROUGHTON Circadian Arousal Process and Process-S Process-S Circadian Arousal Process Figure 1. Conceptual model of the generation of the afternoon "nap zone" (Broughton, 1994). Process-S decreases exponentially from sleep onset (shown as 2300h), reaches a minimum at morning wake-up (0800h) and then increases exponentially across the daytime. The circadian arousal process (plotted inversely-greatest arousal is downwards) increases during the night and into the day and reaches a maximum at around 2000h and then decreases rapidly towards night sleep onset. At around 1400h circadian arousal is sufficiently intense to override the effects of increasing Process-S from morning wake-up, thereby creating the afternoon nap zone, and continuing to increase into the evening hours. and that the circa-12-hour periodicity might represent a superharmonic of the fundamental 24-hour circadian rhythm linked to the solar day, a suggestion also made by Aschoff and Gerkema (1985). Three later, and not mutually incompatible, specific proposals were that: (1) in phylogenetic evolution there is preferential selection of biological rhythms whose periodicities show simple fixed integer ratios (of 2:1 and 3:1) thereby allowing them to be consistently phase related to each other and to the solar day with consequent energy efficiences (Broughton, 1985) whereas, for example, rhythms at 24, 17, 7 and 5 hours cannot; (2) that the 2/day periodicity is not actually one of sleep but rather one of arousal with sleep being facilitated and/or simply permitted to meet the needed daily quota during the intercalated periods of lowered arousal (Broughton, 1989b); and (3) that the nap zone reflects the combined effects of increasing sleep pressure across the daytime and an opposing circadian system (Broughton, 1994; Webb, 1994). In Broughton (1994) it was further specified that the afternoon nap zone would be created by the increase in sleep propensity due to accumulating wakefulness after morning wake-up being overwhelmed and reversed by an active circadian arousal system of the type shown to exist in primates (squirrel monkeys) by Edgar et al. (1993) rather than by Process-C as defined and tested in respect to the afternoon nap zone by Borbély et al. (1989). A schematic representation of this conceptual model (Broughton, 1994) is provided in Figure 1. Despite individual differences (Lack and Lushington, 1996), the remarkable consistency of the 2-day sleep propensity curve was revealed at the 1991 Zurich sleep/wake modeling workshop in a meta-analysis of experimental results accomplished by superimposing the sleep propensity curves around the 24 hours from four protocols with quite different demands on the sleep regulatory system(s) (Broughton and Mullington, 1992). These four experimental paradigms ranged from: hourly assessments of speed of falling asleep (sleep latency in min) across a habitual entrained night of sleep followed by daytime wakefulness (Richardson et al., 1982); similar sleep latency measures (Carskadon, personal communication) during the so-called "constant routine" introduced by Mills et al. (1978) in which controls are made for the main factors which can alter ("mask") the shape of circadian rhythms including sleep, light/dark cycles, rest/activity levels and large meals; the percentage (%) of subjects asleep across the 24-hours during temporal isolation with ad libitum sleep (Zulley and Campbell, 1985); and sleep amount (min) during attempts to sleep in 7-min periods every 20-min around the clock in Lavie's (1986) so-called 7:13 ultrashort sleep schedule. Therefore, whether sleep is fragmented into brief 7-min maximum naps around the 24 hours, follows its endogenous tendency without time constraints or is measured as sleep latency under very different conditions, the circasemidian sleep/wake propensity curve remains robust and essentially the same, at least across groups of young adult subjects.

3 SCN CONTROLLED AROUSAL AND THE NAP ZONE The Borbély-Daan-Beersma 2-process model The Borbély-Daan-Beersma 2-process model of sleep/wake regulation combines a homeostasis sleep process (Process-S) with a circadian process (Process-C) (Borbély, 1982; Daan and Beersma, 1983; Daan et al., 1984). Sleep homeostasis is said to be reflected in the quantified EEG (Q-EEG) during sleep measured as spectral power in the lower frequency bands (slow wave activity, SWA) which was initially proposed as comprising Hz (Borbély et al., 1981) and later modified to Hz (Brunner et al., 1990). Process-S is said to increase exponentially with accumulating wakefulness across the daytime and decrease exponentially across the night sleep period, and also to be increased by sleep deprivation and, in homeostatic fashion, be dissipated by recovery sleep. Increased power in the selected SWA frequency band is considered a measure of "sleep intensity" (Borbély, 1982). The model, as proposed, does not specify predictions about Q-EEG changes in the daytime waking EEG when Process-S is accumulating in exponential fashion; but one could reasonably expect that a similar SWA increase might occur. Process-C in this model is believed to derive from a single light-sensitive SCN oscillator and be manifested in the daily pattern of the core body temperature with increased sleep propensity occurring at times of lowered body temperature. The shape of the circadian process is considered to be either a pure sine-wave (Borbély, 1982) or a somewhat skewed sinewave (Daan et al., 1984). Curve-fitting of this circadian process has usually been done either by single cosinor analysis for a 24-hour periodicity or a two-cosinor analysis for combined 24-hour and 12-hour periodicities. Some weaknesses of the current 2-process model As currently defined, this model unfortunately does not explain a number of empirical experimental findings. Concerning Process-S, Q-EEG analyses repeated across the waking daytime have not uniformly shown a progressive increase in delta power. Both the studies of Cacot et al. (1995) and of Sterman (personal communication) show a temporary afternoon period of increased spectral power in most or all frequency bands followed by a reduction in the late afternoonearly evening period with again an increase prior to evening sleep onset. Moreover, our laboratory has not been able to confirm the expectation that sleep deprivation leads mainly or exclusively to an increase in the spectral power in the delta range of the waking EEG. In fact we have found little, if any, change in this band but rather a strong increase in both absolute and relative power in the theta-1 (4-6 Hz) and theta-2 (6-8 Hz) frequency bands (Yan et al., 1998). As the model specifies SWA changes in the sleep EEG, it is not unexpected that the large majority of the published Q-EEG studies related to the 2- process model have exclusively examined the nocturnal and diurnal sleep EEG (e.g., Knowles et al., 1986; Brunner et al., 1990; Dijk et al., 1991) rather than the awake EEG, although such studies are beginning to appear (Cajochen et al., 1998). Concerning Process-C, the assumption that an increase in sleep propensity necessarily correlates closely with periods of lowered core body temperature is also open to serious challenge. Both the afternoon nap zone under entrained 168 conditions (Dinges et al., 1980) and the equivalent minor sleep period in the temporal isolation studies of Campbell and Zulley (1989) occur somewhat before or near the time of maximum, rather than minimum, core body temperature; and they are very distant in circadian time from the daily temperature nadir which is, of course, associated with the major sleep period. Similarly, under the free-running conditions of temporal isolation in which napping is not permitted, sleep and circadian temperature may become dissociated in the phenomenon called "internal desynchronisation" (Aschoff et al., 1967; Wever, 1979). Therefore the periods of heightened sleep propensity and the actual timing of both naps and the major sleep period are not always in close association with periods of low core body temperature. The model also does not predict the occasional appearance of so-called bicircadian days consisting of a 48-hour wake/sleep rhythm with about 32 hours of wakefulness followed by 16 hours of sleep under conditions of more or less prolonged temporal isolation (Chouvet et al., 1974; Honma and Honma, 1988). The phenomenon has also been reported under the conditions of constant low illumination and the unique performance demands experienced in polar treks, for example, by the two members of the unassisted (no dogs, no food drop offs) Weber-Malakov North Pole expedition (Broughton et al., 1994). The small amount of core body temperature data which exists during the bicircadian pattern suggests that it lengthens in period (Colin et al., 1968). Another striking feature of the laboratory data in the normal entrained state, whether masked or unmasked, is the regular finding that the daily core body temperature fluctuation is far from sinusoidal in shape. This is a characteristic remarked on by Vokac and Vokac (1987) and Broughton et al. (1993). Under normal entrained conditions the lower portion of the curve (i.e., that below the mesor or daily average level), which consists of decreasing temperature to the nadir and back up to the mesor average level, is some 40-50% shorter than the period of higher core body temperature above the mesor (see also Figure 2). It has been shown (Broughton et al., 1993) that cosinor analysis and other derived single sine-wave approaches will distort this asymmetrical shape in predictable ways (including a false delay of the fitted nadir). On the other hand, curve-fitting approaches which do not assume a specific shape provide a more accurate description of the real data. This improvement includes a more precise definition of the time of the temperature nadir as an index of circadian phase and a simultaneous statistical explanation of a substantially higher percent of the variance of the data (Broughton et al., 1993; Lack and Lushington, 1996). A polynomial least squares regression curve fit is one approach which in general gives a more accurate description of the actual data than that derived by single or double cosinorbased analyses and without the wave-shape assumptions of the latter (Broughton et al., 1993; Lack and Lushington, 1996). When applied using 10 regressions to circadian temperature data of 10 randomly chosen subjects participating in another project (Broughton et al., 1993), polynomial regression explained a range of the variance across subjects of 69-95% (mean 88.1%) compared to only 20-83% (mean 57.9%) for

4 169 BROUGHTON cosinor analysis. When used to fit the meta-analysed circasemidian sleep/wake distribution pattern reported by Broughton and Mullington (1992), a ninth-order polynomial regression explained 99.5% of the variance. The equation of this curve is provided elsewhere (Broughton, 1994). The software applied (TableCurve, Jandel Scientific, Corte Madre, CA) fits 221 equations including ones with a very large variety of approaches with varying or no assumptions, and included cosinor analysis. Polynomial regression gave the best curve fit of all 221 algorithms. The 2-process model, as currently defined, also does not accurately predict the now well documented circasemidian pattern of sleep/wake distribution around the 24-hours. There is mention in early studies of appearance of ultradian components (Daan et al., 1984) but these are not fully defined and their shape does not approximate that of the circasemidian sleep propensity curve. An explicit attempt by Borbély et al. (1989) to predict the timing of the afternoon nap zone using the model's most commonly employed skewed-sinusoidal function for Process-C was sufficiently inaccurate that a repeat effort was made further altering the shape of the circadian process. Even when this was done, comparison of the predicted sleep/wake propensity curve with real laboratory data indicated that the model had become acceptably accurate only for the timing of the afternoon nap zone but remained highly inaccurate for sleep/wake status at all other times of day (Broughton and Mullington, 1992; Fig. 1; also Fig. 7). The main reason for this poor predictive power appeared to stem from the insistence that in the normal entrained state the circadian process is either a pure sine-wave or a somewhat skewed sine-wave. In an attempt to define modeling parameters which would better predict the empirical laboratory results for the entrained state, the assumption can be made that the associated circadian core body temperature curve directly reflects the circadian arousal process identified by Edgar et al. (1993). This system, which has its primary site with SCN afferents in the anterior hypothalamus (Lin et al., 1996), appears to be activated by the new stimulant modafinil (Bastugi and Jouvet, 1988; Billiard et al., 1994; Broughton et al., 1997). The SCN/hypothalamic/lower brainstem/cortical GABA-ergic connections are currently being identified by Michel Jouvet and his collaborators. This recently discovered arousal system appears to be the biological basis for the daily periodicity of wakefulness and of activity peaks across the animal kingdom and is therefore of major importance. To better clarify the shape(s) of Process-C it is instructive to make comparisons of typical circadian core body temperature patterns under normally entrained, constant routine, and freerunning conditions using the meta-analysis approach previously employed to define the pattern of the circasemidian sleep/wake distribution (Broughton and Mullington, 1992). METHODS In order to better define the most characteristic shape of the circadian temperature fluctuation, typical results under the most common experimental conditions expressed as group means for similar age groups were chosen for meta-analysis. As the analysis was to be performed in sidereal (clock) time, the results were selected and expressed in clock time, as opposed to circadian phase. Published graphic results were digitally scanned and a file created for manipulation. Inevitably, in published studies, the time axis is not constant either for hour of onset or for page spacing. The data were therefore considered to repeat themselves across subsequent 24-hour periods, as is normally done in double and triple plotting; and then were adjusted ("wrapped") within the file around the 24-hours to begin at the same hour. Similarly, the ordinate is regularly scaled differently in different publications. Consequently, it was necessary to rescale it for direct superimposition of results across the selected studies. No significant distortions arose from such data re-alignments. This horizontal and vertical scaling process was repeated for three published representative studies of 24-hour core body temperature variations from each of the normal entrained state, constant routine, and temporal isolation (with napping not permitted and permitted). For the normal entrained state, data came from Aschoff (1981), Minors and Waterhouse (1984) and Czeisler et al. (1986), the latter consisting of the baseline data before a constant routine was applied. Core body temperature results under the condition of a constant routine used the data of Minors and Waterhouse (1984), Monk et al. (1992) and Dijk et al. (1992). This technique removes the masking effects of sleep (by involving one night of total sleep deprivation); variations in ambient light and temperature (by using constant levels of low illumination and of environmental temperature); activity levels (by constant bed rest); and large meals (by eating small snacks every hour around the 24 hrs). The effects of temporal isolation ("free-running") with napping both not permitted (Czeisler et al., 1980; Strogatz et al., 1987) and permitted (Campbell and Zulley, 1989) were similarly analysed. For each condition, to better characterize the circadian temperature patterns, first, the mean average of the data shown for the individual conditions was calculated and then curve-fit by appropriate algorithms similar to the approach performed in earlier studies for sleep/wake data (Broughton and Mullington, 1992). This curve-fit employed the polynomial regression software inherent in Excel (Microsoft Systems, Seattle, WA) with the criteria both of using at least the 5 regressions needed to fit a bimodal distribution (Lack and Lushington, 1996) and the lowest number sufficient to explain at least 98% of the variance in the data. The curve-fit formula for the 3-experiment averaged data under each condition is provided at the top of each figure. In order to test the main hypothesis, i.e., that in the normal entrained state the biphasic 24-hour sleep/wake propensity curve is due to a Process-S type function being reversed by a circadian arousal process (Broughton, 1994), the newly characterized circadian temperature process was combined with a modified Process-S to attempt to predict the empirical laboratory sleep/wake data. To use the data describing the assumed circadian arousal process (based on body temperature data) in an attempt to predict sleep/wake propensity status in sidereal time, it was assumed that the pattern selected should be that derived under

5 SCN CONTROLLED AROUSAL AND THE NAP ZONE 170 Normal Entrained y = -6E-10x + 2E-07x - 2E-05x x th x x R = , (6 order) Temperature (C ) Aschoff (1981) Minors and Waterhouse (1984) Czeisler et al. (1986) average Mesor of the average curve Figure 2. Normal entrained state. The circadian core body temperature curves, and therefore the assumed circadian arousal process, are shown from three representative studies in young adults (Aschoff, 1981; Minors and Waterhouse, 1984; Czeisler et al., 1986) as is the average of these three studies (dark line). The average core body temperature curve was curve-fit by polynomial regression (see Figure 5). A sixth order polynomial regression explained 98.7% of the variance. The horizontal line is the average level (mesor) of the 3-experiment averaged curve. the conditions of the prediction. For example, to make predictions under the usual real-life conditions which include more or less regular hours of a major sleep period, regular awakening either by environmental Zeitgebers or by an alarm clock, a typical light/dark cycle, usual activity levels and normal meals, the circadian arousal process to be employed should be that which is present under such normal entrained conditions and which uses sidereal time for which such predictions would be desired. Concerning Process-S during night sleep, the shape defined by Borbély and Achermann (1992) was employed using their formula values. However, their values for the daytime wake period, when plotted, indicated that the portion of rapid exponential increase occurred soon after daytime wake onset rather than in the evening. Moreover, their assessment of Process-S in the daytime involves quantification of delta power during day sleep whereas, as noted above, there is evidence that theta, but not delta power increases in the waking EEG with increasing levels of sleepiness. A different pattern was therefore created with an exponentially increasing value across the day. Process-C (derived from experimental data under the entrained condition) and Process-S (derived from experimental data only for the night period and with an assumed pattern in the daytime) were then superimposed in sidereal time. As a first approximation, it was assumed that their effects on sleep propensity are opposing (Edgar et al., 1993) and without any complex interactions. Finally, the predicted curve was superimposed upon the meta-analysis of the empirical laboratory sleep/wake data (from Broughton and Mullington, 1992; Fig. 1) and the prediction of Borbély et al. (1989). RESULTS The circadian core body temperature curves from the three studies of the normal entrained state in young adults are superimposed in Figure 2. The figure confirms that under normal entrained conditions the circadian distribution of core body temperature is quite asymmetrical across time as the portion below the mesor (group-average level across the 24-hour period) consistently lasts about one-third of the sidereal day and the period of increased temperature above the mesor lasts about two-thirds of the day. There is also a lesser degree of amplitude asymmetry, with the portion below the mesor being of somewhat greater amplitude than the portion above. Results under the condition of a constant routine are shown in Figure 3. This technique removes the masking effects of sleep (by involving one night of total sleep deprivation); variations in ambient light and temperature (by using constant levels of low illumination and of environmental temperature); activity levels (by constant bed rest); and large meals (by eating small snacks every hour around the 24 hrs). Again the curves from different studies are very similar and the 3-

6 171 BROUGHTON Constant Routine y = 2E-08x - 4E-06x x x 2 th x R = , (5 order) Temperature (C ) Minors and Waterhouse (1984) Monk et al. (1992) Dijk et al. (1992) 36.4 Average Figure 3. Constant routine. Circadian core body temperature curves and therefore the assumed circadian arousal process are shown from Minors and Waterhouse (1984), Monk et al. (1992) and Dijk et al. (1992), as is the three-study average curve (dark line). Note that the process has become rather more sinusoidal than in the normal entrained state. A fifth order polynomial regression of the average curve explained 98.5% of the variance. Temporal Isolation y = 7E-11x - 6E-09x - 8E-07x x x 2 th x R = , (6 order) 37.4 Temperature (C ) Czeisler et al. (1980) Strogatz et al. (1987) Campbell and Zulley (1989) Average Figure 4. Temporal isolation. Circadian core body temperature curves, and therefore the assumed circadian arousal process, are from Czeisler et al. (1980), Strogatz et al. (1987) and Campbell and Zulley (1989). In the Campbell and Zulley study, but not in the other two, subjects were allowed to nap ad libitum. The three curves are very similar, although in the study permitting napping, daytime sleep occurred frequently at around 1400h and in Campbell and Zulley (1989) was associated with a transient decrease in temperature believed to be nap-sleep induced. The average of the three studies is also shown. A sixth order polynomial regression explained 99.3% of the variance. Note that the pattern has become even more sinusoidal.

7 SCN CONTROLLED AROUSAL AND THE NAP ZONE Curve Fits of Meta-Analyzed Core Body Temperature Data 37.4 Temperature (C ) Normal Entrained Constant Routine Temporal Isolation Figure 5. Curve-fits by polynomial regression for the three meta-analyses of studies from the normal entrained state, the constant routine and temporal isolation. The pattern in the normal entrained state is quite asymetrical with the period of low circadian arousal (core body temperature) being about one-third the duration of the nycthemeron. The proportion of decreased circadian arousal progressively increases to about 50% in temporal isolation, when the process then becomes essentially sinusoidal. experiment mean and its curve-fit formula are provided. Under the condition of a constant routine the circadian core body temperature can be seen to become somewhat more symmetrical both in time (across the 24-hour day) and in amplitude. The effects of temporal isolation ("free-running") with napping both not permitted (Czeisler et al., 1980; Strogatz et al., 1987) and permitted (Campbell and Zulley, 1989) are similarly shown in Figure 4. Under these conditions, core body temperature is essentially sinusoidal in sidereal time with the experimental sleep data (not shown) indicating that the onset of the major sleep period occurs most frequently around the time of the temperature nadir. The naps, however, peak around the time of temperature maximum with a superimposed, and apparently nap-sleep evoked, small decrease. This can be seen as the transient decrease in temperature at around 1600h in the data of Campbell and Zulley (1989). Again, data from different studies are almost superimposable. Forced desychrony involves an attempt to impose a sleep/wake schedule ("circadian day") outside of the normal entrainment range of the human circadian system in order to distinguish the patterns of the circadian temperature and the homeostatic sleep processes (Dijk et al., 1992; Dijk and Czeisler, 1995). When plotted in sidereal time (Dijk et al., 1992; Fig. 2), the shape of the circadian process is asymmetrical and quite similar to that of the normal entrained state whereas, when plotted by circadian phase, it is quite symmetrical and sinusoidal. For the normal entrained condition a sixth order polynomial regression was sufficient to explain 98.7% of the variance (r 2 = ) in the 3-experiment averaged data. The specific equation is shown in Figure 2. This fitted curve of the circadian core body temperature variation under entrained conditions confirmed the asymmetry in time of approximately 1:2 (8 hrs below the mesor and 16 hrs above) already evident in the raw data and the lesser amplitude asymmetry (greater for the portion below the mesor). For operational reasons it was decided to consider this function as representing a direct efferential expression of the status of the circadian arousal system under conditions of normal entrainment and with normal levels of masking. For the constant routine a fifth-order polynomial regression explained 98.5% of the variance. The formula is given in Figure 3. It was assumed that this curve reflects the circadian arousal process under conditions involving unmasking from the effects of sleep, activity levels, the light/dark cycle, and large meals combined with the superimposed effects of one night of total sleep deprivation. The curve-fit, like the mean group data, indicated that during a constant routine the circadian arousal process had become somewhat more sinusoidal and symmetrical than in the normal entrained state. Data from the temporal isolation conditions which either did not permit or permitted napping had 99.3% of the data explained by sixth-order polynomial regression. It is evident that under free-running conditions the core body temperature's fluctuation in sidereal time became much more sinusoidal. Again it was assumed that the resultant curve reflects the circadian arousal process under these highly particular living conditions. Figure 5 shows the three curve-fits superimposed for direct comparison.

8 173 BROUGHTON Circadian Arousal Process (C ) Superimposition of the Circadian Arousal Process and Process-S Major Sleep Period Circadian arousal process Process-S Figure 6. Superimposition of the circadian arousal process (empirical average core body temperature curve-plotted inversely) and the homeostatic sleep process. As Process-S increases across the daytime it is crossed by the circadian arousal process at around 1400h. It is assumed that at this point the circadian arousal process overwhelms sleep pressure from Process-S. Daytime arousal then increases until the evening at around 2000h then rapidly decreases before sleep onset at which time Process-S is rapidly increasing. The two processes act synergistically, exceed a threshold and if behavioral factors permit, lead to evening sleep onset. Circasemidian Sleep/Wake Propensity Process 1 0 Process-S 1.0 Empirical Date (from Broughton and Mullington 1994) Predicted by Borbely ' et al. (1989) Predicted by current model Sleep Propensity (%) Figure 7. Superimposition of the circasemidian sleep/wake propensity curve from the empirical laboratory data meta-analysed by Broughton and Mullington (1992), the shape of the sleep propensity curve approximated by the current conceptual model and the prediction of Borbély et al., (1989). The prediction by the current conceptual model assumes that the shape of the circadian arousal process in the normal entrained state is that of the core body temperature under these same conditions.

9 SCN CONTROLLED AROUSAL AND THE NAP ZONE The two processes are presented in Figure 6 with increasing sleep propensity plotted upwards. Therefore the circadian arousal process (fitted curve for empirical core body temperature data in Fig. 5) is plotted inversely to the normal procedure, whereas Process-S is plotted in the usual fashion. The superimposition clearly shows that during the normal night-sleep-period arousal (Process-C) is low with a minimum at around 0500h, arousal starts to increase at around 0500h, before the usual time of morning wake-up which averages at around 0700h and then increases across the daytime with a maximum around 2100h. Process-S, on the other hand, decreases exponentially during the night, reaches its minimum at around 0700h, and increases exponentially across the daytime. It can be seen, as predicted (Broughton, 1994), that the increasing daytime Process-S is crossed by the increasing arousal process at the usual time of the afternoon nap zone of around h. Later in the day a combination of high Process-S and rapidly decreasing arousal precedes sleep onset as an apparent "sleep gate" (Lavie, 1986) and leads to sleep initiation. The predicted curve was then superimposed upon the metaanalysis of the empirical laboratory sleep/wake data (from Broughton and Mullington, 1992; Fig. 1) and the prediction of Borbély et al. (1989). It can be seen (Figure 7) that the first two curves are quite similar. By comparison, the prediction of Borbély et al. (1989) using the usual skewed-sinusoidal circadian process is a considerably weaker prediction of the empirical laboratory data. DISCUSSION The Borbély-Daan-Beersma 2-process model has made a major theoretical contribution to our understanding of sleep/wake regulation. It clearly identifies and distinguishes the two main processes involved; and the model's postulate of a single circadian oscillator (the paired SCN nuclei) appears much closer to the evidence, at least for human data, than are models which propose two, three, or more, independent or coupled oscillators. The circadian component has been characterized over the past 15 years by many workers, but especially in the elegant studies of the Munich, Birmingham, Groningen and Boston groups. Yet, when applied to the consensus 24-hour distribution of wake/sleep patterns (Broughton and Mullington, 1992), the model does not accurately predict empirical laboratory data characterizing the circasemidian sleep/wake propensity curve. Employing the fitted shape of empirical circadian temperature data, and abandoning the belief that the circadian process in the normal entrained living conditions must be a sine-wave or skewed sine-wave, produces a much closer fit to the 24-hour sleep/wake propensity pattern defined by laboratory data. This prediction is arguably as close as one can reasonably expect for data arising from different laboratories in different countries using differing technologies. The predicted 24-hour sleep/wake propensity process by this approach clearly demarcates both the nocturnal major sleep period and the afternoon nap zone, as well as the morning and especially the much stronger late afternoon/early evening periods of low 174 sleep propensity that Lavie (1986) has called "forbidden zones for sleep" and Strogatz (1986) refers to as "wake maintenance zones". As in Broughton and Mullington (1992), the magnitude of the curve-fitted afternoon period of increased sleep propensity ("nap zone") is about 25% of that of the major sleep period. This afternoon "nap zone" represents the greatest predictable variation in daytime sleepiness/alertness levels. Perhaps more impressive than the "nap zone" is the immediately subsequent wake maintenance zone which is the most robust component of the daytime data in the metaanalysis of Broughton and Mullington (1992) and which for most people represents the period of maximum daily sustained alertness. Even after sleep deprivation, sleep only rarely occurs during this period of heightened alertness. In short, the daily 24-hour sleep/wake propensity curve truly consists of two periods of heightened alertness with low sleep probability and two intercalated periods of reduced arousal with increased sleep propensity. There is recent strong evidence for a circadian arousal process which is controlled by the SCN. Edgar et al. (1993) showed in the squirrel monkey that SCN destruction not only leads to loss of the circadian periodicity of activity/inactivity, and therefore of wake/sleep patterns, but also to an increase in sleep amount, thus necessitating involvement of an active arousal process. Similarly, it has also been shown that bright light stimulation in man not only phase sets the circadian pacemaker, but also has an alerting effect (Dawson and Campbell, 1991). While performing c-fos studies of the effects of the new stimulant modafinil, Lin et al. (1996) noted that the substance's major effect was mainly circumscribed to an area of the anterior hypothalamus which has inputs from the SCN. This discovery has led to identification of a new GABA-ergic wake maintaining system whose connections appear to include SCN-anterior hypothalamus, periaquaductal grey matter in the pons and medulla, and rostral projections to the forebrain (Jouvet, personal communication). This system would represent the mechanism of the daily awakening from sleep and the succeeding wake period during which alertness is further modulated by a group of other arousal systems (including the cholinergic reticular, serotonergic raphé, dopaminergic extrapyramidal, noradrenergic locus coeruleus and histaminergic posterior hypothalamic projection systems) in a hierarchy yet to be determined. The arousal generated by this light-sensitive system has superimposed upon it a number of lower amplitude circa-3-4 hour and min ultradian rhythms of alertness level which are beyond the scope of the current modeling effort, as is explanation of the phenomenon of bicircadian days. The results reported here indicate that under normal entrained conditions the circadian arousal system expressed in sidereal hours is neither sinusoidal or quasi-sinusoidal. Rather it is very asymmetrical in time and somewhat so in amplitude. This lack of a sinusoidal shape for core body temperature was remarked on by Vokac and Vokac (1987) and Broughton et al. (1993). The data confirm the limitations of employing single or double cosinor fits in situations where the pattern of the process does not at all approximate a sine-wave function (Broughton et al., 1993). The three meta-analyses also show

10 175 BROUGHTON Circadian Arousal Process and Process-S: Creation of the Afternoon Nap Zone 1.0 Circadian Arousal Process Process-S Combined Effect Sleep Propensity 0.0 Major Sleep Period S.I. M-WMZ Nap-Zone E-WMZ Major Sleep Figure 8. Modified conceptual model of the physiological mechanism generating the afternoon nap zone and other features of the circasemidian sleep/wake process. Sleep onset is plotted at 2300h, morning wake-up at 0700h and maximum daytime sleep propensity (nap zone) at around 1500h. The homeostatic sleep process (Borbély's Process-S) increases exponentially across the daytime and decreases exponentially during night sleep. The circadian arousal process is plotted inversely to the shape of the circadian core body temperature curve in the entrained state. The nap zone is shown to be created by the increasing homeostatic sleep pressure being overwhelmed by the circadian arousal process which later becomes maximum later at around 2000h. The combined processes generate the major sleep period, a post-sleep period of sleep inertia (S.I.), the morning wake maintenance zone (M-WMZ), the afternoon nap zone, the late afternoon/early evening wake maintenance zone (E-WMZ) and evening sleep onset. the problems inherent in making the assumption that the endogenous circadian process must always have the same shape under all conditions, as for sidereal time do the forced desynchronization results of Dijk and colleagues (Dijk et al., 1992; Dijk and Czeisler, 1995). It seems self-evident that one should not use circadian functions derived under one condition to predict sleep/wake status under another, and that, if one wishes to predict in clock time, one should use clock time as opposed to circadian phase. The use of the true efferential shape of the circadian arousal process as Process-C rather than a sine-wave or skewed sinewave, in combination with an altered Process-S for the daytime period, has been found to predict a much closer approximation to the shape of empirical laboratory data of the circasemidian sleep/wake propensity curve as meta-analysed by Broughton and Mullington (1992) than does the normally used skewed sine-wave approach (Borbély et al. 1989). If one follows the two processes across the nychthemeron for the normal entrained state (Figure 6), a number of features become clear. The night sleep period is characterized by high but decreasing Process-S and low circadian arousal. Circadian arousal begins to increase before morning wake-up. Morning sleep inertia may be explained as the period when Process-S has not yet been reduced to its minimum by (sufficient) sleep duration and circadian arousal is still relatively low. A similar conclusion was made by Webb (1994). Such a mechanism is more parsimonious adding a third process to unaltered descriptions of Process-S and Process-C, as proposed by Folkard and Åkerstedt (1992). Across the morning period arousal levels increase and Process-S remains low giving rise to the morning wake maintenance zone of Strogatz (1986). The afternoon nap zone can then be explained as an over-riding of increasing Process- S by the increasing and more powerful circadian arousal process. Experimental support for this mechanism is provided in the study of Krupa et al. (1998) in which phase advance and phase delay of the circadian temperature process by bright light therapy led to a parallel advance and delay of the afternoon peak of worst performance on a complex choice reaction time task. After the nap zone period, circadian arousal continues to increase strongly creating the late afternoon-early evening wake maintenance zone. This is followed in turn by increasing homeostatic pressure for sleep and declining arousal levels leading to a "sleep gate" (Lavie, 1986) for sleep onset. Sleep would then become highly probable, when a threshold of the combined effects of these two processes is reached. These findings have led to a modified conceptual model shown in Figure 8.

11 SCN CONTROLLED AROUSAL AND THE NAP ZONE This conceptual model has been developed, as have all others, using absolute levels only. Yet many biological processes are sensitive to rate of change rather than to absolute levels. There is some evidence that rate of change may well play a role in probability of sleep/wake status. For example, Campbell and Broughton (1994) found that self selected sleep onset of the major sleep period followed soon after the time of the most rapid rate of change of decreasing core body temperature (i.e., the peak in the first derivative of the data). It is beyond the breadth of this article to describe quantitative, as opposed to descriptive, processes, as was the case for the original (Borbély, 1982) paper for which quantified descriptors of Process-C and time constants for Process-S were added later (Daan et al., 1984). Such quantification and predictions of sleep/wake status following a change from the normal entrained state to other conditions will be considered in subsequent papers. The analyses performed here have further clarified the shape of the circasemidian sleep propensity curve and a possible mechanism of the afternoon nap zone. It is of interest to consider why this phenomenon is not always expressed as napping behavior. Although its overall magnitude in the entrained condition, documented earlier in Broughton and Mullington (1992) and further analysed here, is some 25-30% of that of the major sleep period, the nap zone often passes relatively unnoticed other than as a tendency towards increased yawning, poorer concentration and further effects other than overt sleep. In fact, unless one is quite sleep deprived, which increases its intensity and may in part explain its varying intensity (Lack and Lushington, 1996), the increased sleep propensity during the nap zone generally causes little inconvenience. Despite this regular period of transitory increase in sleep propensity, one has the option not to nap; and indeed the social and other conditions for a period of afternoon sleep are often not available. Moreover, daily experience teaches one that this temporary period of increased sleepiness will soon be replaced by one of sustained arousal, typically the daily period of greatest alertness. The functional significance of the nap zone also remains uncertain. Some have proposed that its evolutionary role has been to get humans out of the mid-day sun (Webb and Dinges, 1989). Others have suggested that it increases flexibility of the timing of sleep giving more opportunities to meet the daily sleep needs (Broughton, 1989b). It is evident that the data presented here, which are derived from studies mainly of normal young adults most of whom were males, may not be representative of other ages or of females in whom factors relating to the menstrual cycle and menopause also play a role. Moreover, it has not escaped attention that the characteristics of the circadian arousal system may vary producing different sleep/wake styles. For instance, across ontogeny the circadian arousal system appears to strengthen from the neonatal period to adolescence reducing sleep need while producing an initial polyphasic pattern, then in preschoolers a circasemidian one, in adolescence often a purely circadian distribution. This is followed by apparent weakening of the system and a reversion to a circasemidian pattern in adulthood. The relative intensity of this process 176 between individuals may in large part be genetically determined leading to variations in sleep need. It would appear to be phase advanced in morning persons ("larks") and phase delayed in evening persons ("owls"), respectively. Its intensity may be further modulated by CNS active substances including stimulants, especially for those affecting GABA-ergic neurons. Similarly, its intensity may be reduced in neurological disorders involving increased amounts of sleep or excessive daytime sleepiness such as occur in idiopathic CNS hypersomnia, narcolepsy and certain organic hypersomnias resulting from brainstem lesions. Recent studies of patients with narcolepsy/cataplexy syndrome (Broughton et al., 1998) indicate that the appearance and timing of daytime sleep episodes are explicable by reduced intensity of the circadian arousal system which is reversed by the direct action of the new stimulant modafinil. Conversely, increased intensity of the system could explain a number of types of insomnia. It remains uncertain whether, as has been assumed here, the nap zone reflects the effects of a single arousal process which begins before wake-up and reverses the effects of Process-S. This reversal appears to sculpt the shape of the nap zone. Such a single generator appears to be the most parsimonious explanation. The pattern could, however, also be generated by dual (coupled) arousal processes, the first one of which is expressed as the REM circadian distribution pattern shown in extended sleep studies to have its acrophase around the time of morning wake-up and to continue across the morning (Broughton et al., 1990); and the second to the circadian arousal process. In any event, the increased sleep propensity in the afternoon certainly does not reflect a nadir of core body temperature. Obvious requirements to further improve this approach to modeling are better charactization of Process-S, the creation of a quantitative mathematical model, and the application of the model to the prediction of the effects of acute changes from the normal entrained condition to other conditions such as night shift work, jet lag and ultrashort sleep schedules. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author thanks the Medical Research Council of Canada for a research grant and Career Investigator Award ( ) covering the period of this research. Janet Mullington read an early version of the paper and suggested the evening sleep threshold. Francesca Cañellas read the manuscript at several stages and made many helpful suggestions. The assistance of Brigitte Boucher for graphics and curve-fitting was invaluable. REFERENCES 1. Aschoff J. Circadian rhythms: interference with and dependence on work-rest schedules. In: Johnson LC, Tepas DL, Colquhoun WP, Colligan MJ, eds. Biological Rhythms, Sleep and Shift Work. Jamaica NY: SP Medical and Scientific Books, 1981, pp Aschoff J. Naps as integral parts of the wake time within the human sleep-wake cycle. J Biol Rhythms 1994; 9: Aschoff J, Gerecke U, Wever R. Desynchronization of human circadian rhythms. Jn J Physiol 1967; 17:

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle Sleep 10(1):62-68, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components

More information

Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes

Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY Copyright 1989 by The Society for Psychophysiological Research, Inc. Vol. 26, No. 5 Printed in U.S.A. Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes

More information

SLEEP-WAKE AS A BIOLOGICAL RHYTHM

SLEEP-WAKE AS A BIOLOGICAL RHYTHM Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2001. 52:277 303 Copyright c 2001 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved SLEEP-WAKE AS A BIOLOGICAL RHYTHM P. Lavie Sleep Laboratory, Faculty of Medicine, Technion-Israel Institute

More information

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms People typically sleep about 8 hours per day, and spend 16 hours awake. Most people sleep over 175,000 hours in their lifetime. The vast amount of time spent sleeping

More information

Managing Sleep and Fatigue in Today s Healthcare Environment Tricks of the Trade

Managing Sleep and Fatigue in Today s Healthcare Environment Tricks of the Trade Managing Sleep and Fatigue in Today s Healthcare Environment Tricks of the Trade 92 nd Annual Meeting of the American Association of Thoracic Surgery Scott Shappell, Ph.D. Clemson University How tired

More information

Why Do We Sleep At Night?

Why Do We Sleep At Night? 4 Why Do We Sleep At Night? Now that you know what actually happens across the sleep period, let s look at what determines when we sleep. There are two biological determiners of sleep, sleep pressure and

More information

Sleep and Dreaming. Sleep Deprivation Trivia

Sleep and Dreaming. Sleep Deprivation Trivia Sleep and Dreaming Sleep Deprivation Trivia Peter Tripp stayed awake for 201 hours in 1959. Guinness Book of Records record is 18 days, 21 hours, 40 minutes. Sleep deprivation implicated in Three Mile

More information

Effects of light exposure and sleep displacement on dim light melatonin onset Gordijn, Margaretha; Beersma, DGM; Korte, HJ; Van den Hoofdakker, RH

Effects of light exposure and sleep displacement on dim light melatonin onset Gordijn, Margaretha; Beersma, DGM; Korte, HJ; Van den Hoofdakker, RH University of Groningen Effects of light exposure and sleep displacement on dim light melatonin onset Gordijn, Margaretha; Beersma, DGM; Korte, HJ; Van den Hoofdakker, RH Published in: Journal of Sleep

More information

Circadian rhythm and Sleep. Radwan Banimustafa MD

Circadian rhythm and Sleep. Radwan Banimustafa MD Circadian rhythm and Sleep Radwan Banimustafa MD Homeostasis Maintenance of equilibrium by active regulation of internal states: Cardiovascular function (blood pressure, heart rate) Body temperature Food

More information

Bio-Rhythms. Biorhythms. Presented by: Dr. Magdy Akladios 1. What is a Biorhythm. Biorhythms Theory. SENG/ INDH 5334: Human Factors Engineering

Bio-Rhythms. Biorhythms. Presented by: Dr. Magdy Akladios 1. What is a Biorhythm. Biorhythms Theory. SENG/ INDH 5334: Human Factors Engineering SENG/ INDH 5334: Human Factors Engineering Bio-Rhythms By: Magdy Akladios, PhD, PE, CSP, CPE, CSHM 1 What is a Biorhythm A biorhythm is a hypothetical cyclic pattern of alterations in physiology, emotions,

More information

HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS. Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann Acacia Aguirre Martin Moore-Ede

HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS. Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann Acacia Aguirre Martin Moore-Ede Proceedings of the 23 Winter Simulation Conference S. Chick, P. J. Sánchez, D. Ferrin, and D. J. Morrice, eds. HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann

More information

SLEEP APNEA IN THE ELDERLY SLEEP THAT KNITS UP THE RAVELED SLEEVE OF CARE

SLEEP APNEA IN THE ELDERLY SLEEP THAT KNITS UP THE RAVELED SLEEVE OF CARE SLEEP APNEA IN THE ELDERLY SLEEP THAT KNITS UP THE RAVELED SLEEVE OF CARE OBJECTIVES 1. TO DESCRIBE THE NORMAL AGE RELATED CHANGES TO SLEEP 2. TO DESCRIBE THE SPECTRUM OF SLEEP-DISORDERED BREATHING. 3.

More information

Introduction to Physiological Psychology

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Introduction to Physiological Psychology Psych 260 Kim Sweeney ksweeney@cogsci.ucsd.edu cogsci.ucsd.edu/~ksweeney/psy260.html What could possibly go wrong? n Causes of Narcolepsy Uncertain, but appears

More information

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm Sleep, 2(3):299-307 1980 Raven Press, New York The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm L. C. Johnson Naval Health Research Center, San Diego, California Summary: Two studies, using data from fragmented

More information

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch Dr Alex Bartle Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch 11:00-11:55 WS #113: Circadian Sleep Disorders 12:05-13:00 WS #125: Circadian Sleep Disorders (Repeated) Overview The Structure of Sleep

More information

The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia

The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia Chapter e39 The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia Leon Lack and Helen Wright Department of Psychology, Flinders University, Adelaide, South Australia PROTOCOL NAME The use of bright light

More information

Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms

Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms Keywords: 8853 Journal of Physiology (1999), 516.2, pp. 611 627 611 Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms

More information

41 st Annual AAGBI Linkman Conference Birmingham. Dr Kathleen Ferguson Honorary Treasurer AAGBI

41 st Annual AAGBI Linkman Conference Birmingham. Dr Kathleen Ferguson Honorary Treasurer AAGBI 41 st Annual AAGBI Linkman Conference Birmingham Dr Kathleen Ferguson Honorary Treasurer AAGBI Objectives and CoI Chaired the 2014 review Member of SALG Published 2004 & 2014 EWTD & New Deal Guidance document

More information

EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122

EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122 EEG Sleep Circadian rhythms Learning Objectives: 121, 122 Zoltán Lelkes Electroencenphalography Hans Berger pen time amplifier electrodes 1 The waves of the EEG gamma > 30 Hz beta: 13-30 Hz Mental activity:

More information

7/23/2018. Nobel Prize Outline. Results of Insufficient or Poorly Timed Sleep

7/23/2018. Nobel Prize Outline. Results of Insufficient or Poorly Timed Sleep Nobel Prize 2017 Sleep, Circadian Rhythm and the Impact of Insufficient and Poorly Timed Sleep Awarded to Jeffrey C. Hall, Michael Rosbash and Michael W. Young for their discoveries of molecular mechanisms

More information

Piecewise smooth maps for the circadian modulation of sleep-wake dynamics

Piecewise smooth maps for the circadian modulation of sleep-wake dynamics Piecewise smooth maps for the circadian modulation of sleep-wake dynamics Victoria Booth, Depts of Mathematics and Anesthesiology University of Michigan Cecilia Diniz Behn, Dept of Applied Mathematics

More information

Light treatment for sleep disorders: consensus report. IV. Sleep phase and duration disturbances.

Light treatment for sleep disorders: consensus report. IV. Sleep phase and duration disturbances. J Biol Rhythms 1995 Jun;10(2):135-47 Related Articles, Books, LinkOut Light treatment for sleep disorders: consensus report. IV. Sleep phase and duration disturbances. Terman M, Lewy AJ, Dijk DJ, Boulos

More information

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11:

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11: Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11: 876-881. CLINICAL PEARL Managing the Effects of Shift Work in Medicine Holger Link, MD, and Robert Sack,

More information

Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People

Sleep and Body Temperature in Morning and Evening People Sleep. 8(4):311-318 1985 Raven Press. New York Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People Jean Foret, *Nathalie Touron, *Odile Benoit, and *Ginette Bouard Laboratoire de Physiologie Neurosensorielle

More information

Make sure you remember the Key Concepts

Make sure you remember the Key Concepts A2 Psychology Term 1 Module 4 Physiological Psychology Biological Rhythms, Sleep and Dreaming Area of Study: Biological Rhythms. Lesson 7 Getting you Thinking pg 403 Make sure you remember the Key Concepts

More information

NEURAL MECHANISMS OF SLEEP (p.1) (Rev. 3/21/07)

NEURAL MECHANISMS OF SLEEP (p.1) (Rev. 3/21/07) NEURAL MECHANISMS OF SLEEP (p.1) (Rev. 3/21/07) 1. Revisitation of Bremer s 1936 Isolated Brain Studies Transected the brain: a. Cut between the medulla and the spinal cord ( encephale isole ) Note: recall

More information

Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming. Elaine M. Hull

Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming. Elaine M. Hull Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming Elaine M. Hull Rhythms of Waking and Sleeping Animals generate 24 hour cycles of wakefulness and sleep. Some animals generate endogenous circannual rhythms (yearly

More information

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days Sleep Research Online 5(1): 13-17, 2003 http://www.sro.org/2003/hayashi/13/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 96-214X 2003 WebSciences The Effects of Short Daytime s for Five Consecutive Mitsuo

More information

6.4 Interaction Between the Sleep-Wake Cycle and the Rhythm of Rectal Temperature

6.4 Interaction Between the Sleep-Wake Cycle and the Rhythm of Rectal Temperature 6.4 Interaction Between the Sleep-Wake Cycle and the Rhythm of Rectal Temperature J. Zulley and R.A. Wever 1 1 Introduction The human circadian system has been considered as controlled by two or even more

More information

Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation

Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation Sleep. 18( I): 1--6 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation Validation of the Sand C Components of the Three-Process Model of Alertness Regulation

More information

The Forty-Eight Hour Day

The Forty-Eight Hour Day Sleep, 1(2):191197 1978 Raven Press, New York The FortyEight Hour Day Wilse B. Webb Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida Summary: Four normal young adult male subjects

More information

Symptoms of Narcolepsy

Symptoms of Narcolepsy Symptoms of Narcolepsy v Sleep attacks Brief episodes of sleep that occur many times a day May occur without warning or be preceded by drowsiness Patient usually feels refreshed afterwards Refractory period

More information

Overview. Surviving shift work. What is the circadian rhythm? Components of a Generic Biological Timing System 31/10/2017

Overview. Surviving shift work. What is the circadian rhythm? Components of a Generic Biological Timing System 31/10/2017 Overview Surviving shift work Dr Claire M. Ellender Respiratory and Sleep Physician Princess Alexandra Hospital Conflicts nil relevant Circadian rhythm Impacts of shift work on health Case example Circadian

More information

Lecture 8. Arousal & Sleep. Cogs17 * UCSD

Lecture 8. Arousal & Sleep. Cogs17 * UCSD Lecture 8 Arousal & Sleep Cogs17 * UCSD Arousal in the Brain Stimulated by sensory input Initiated, maintained endogenously Basal Forebrain Delivers ACh throughout cortex Arousal in the Brain Lateral Hypothalamus

More information

Sleep Disorders. Sleep. Circadian Rhythms

Sleep Disorders. Sleep. Circadian Rhythms Sleep Disorders Sleep The Sleep Wakefulness Cycle: Circadian Rhythms Internally generated patterns of bodily functions that vary over a ~24-hour period Function even in the absence of normal cues 2 Circadian

More information

Circadian variation of EEG power spectra in NREM and REM sleep in humans: Dissociation from body temperature

Circadian variation of EEG power spectra in NREM and REM sleep in humans: Dissociation from body temperature J. Sleep Res. (1999) 8, 189 195 Circadian variation of EEG power spectra in NREM and REM sleep in humans: Dissociation from body temperature DERK-JAN DIJK Circadian, Neuroendocrine and Sleep Disorders

More information

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep Sleep, 6(4):319-325 1983 Raven Press, New York Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep T. Roehrs, F. Zorick, J. Sicklesteel, R. Wittig, and T. Roth Sleep Disorders and Research

More information

Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services

Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services Thomas W. O Reilly, MS, PCC in cooperation with Lakeshore Educational and Counseling Services www.lakeshoresupport.com Humans have biological rhythms, known as Circadian Rhythms (CR) CR refers to cyclical

More information

Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison With Young Adults

Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison With Young Adults Sleep 20(4):294-300 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep In Normal Subjects Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison

More information

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include:

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include: INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS Introduction Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include: Intrinsic sleep disorders (e.g. narcolepsy, obstructive sleep apnoea/hypopnea

More information

Modelling the relation of body temperature and sleep: importance of the circadian rhythm in skin temperature

Modelling the relation of body temperature and sleep: importance of the circadian rhythm in skin temperature Modelling the relation of body temperature and sleep: importance of the circadian rhythm in skin temperature EUS J.W. VAN SOMEREN NETHERLANDS INSTITUTE FOR BRAIN RESEARCH, AMSTERDAM A close relation between

More information

LESSON 4.5 WORKBOOK How do circuits regulate their output?

LESSON 4.5 WORKBOOK How do circuits regulate their output? DEFINITIONS OF TERMS Homeostasis tendency to relatively stable equilibrium. Feed-forward inhibition control mechanism whereby the output of one pathway inhibits the activity of another pathway. Negative

More information

כשהשעון הביולוגי מזייף יעקב סיון

כשהשעון הביולוגי מזייף יעקב סיון כשהשעון הביולוגי מזייף יעקב סיון מכון ריאות, בי"ח "ספרא" לילדים, המרכז הרפואי שיבא חיפ"פ, גליליון, 3.2018 Adjustment insomnia Paradoxical insomnia Insomnia due to medical cond. Insomnia due to drugs Childhood

More information

Sleep. No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness,

Sleep. No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness, Sleep Neil B. Kavey, MD Columbia Presbyterian Medical Center No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness, It is an active physiologic

More information

Sleep. No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness,

Sleep. No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness, Sleep Neil B. Kavey, MD Columbia Presbyterian Medical Center No longer think of sleep as an isolated block of time at the end of the day. Sleep is not just the absence of wakefulness, It is an active physiologic

More information

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants Sleep, 5:S128-S134 1982 Raven Press, New York Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants T. Roth, T. Roehrs, and F. Zorick Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, Michigan

More information

Concepts and models of sleep regulation: an overview

Concepts and models of sleep regulation: an overview J. Sleep Res. (1992) 1, 63-19 Concepts and models of sleep regulation: an overview ALEXANDER A.BORBELY and PETER ACHERMANN Institute of Pharmacology. University of Zurich, CH-8006 Zurich, Switzerland Accepted

More information

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children SLEEP DISORDERS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children Distinctive Features of Pediatric Sleep Daytime sleepiness uncommon

More information

Dag Stenberg Institute of Biomedicine/Physiology, University of Helsinki, Finland

Dag Stenberg Institute of Biomedicine/Physiology, University of Helsinki, Finland SLEEP Dag Stenberg Institute of Biomedicine/Physiology, University of Helsinki, Finland Keywords: Adenosine, insomnia, learning and memory, sleep center, sleep stages, REM sleep, transmitters, wakefulness

More information

A Novel Approach to Eliminating Jetlag Using Natural Ingredients

A Novel Approach to Eliminating Jetlag Using Natural Ingredients A Novel Approach to Eliminating Jetlag Using Natural Ingredients Overview One of the unwanted consequences of our busy lifestyles is travelling over different time zones, and the need to adapt our bodies

More information

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults Sleep 13(5):385-394, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1990 Association of Professional Sleep Societies The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults M.

More information

IN ITS ORIGINAL FORM, the Sleep/Wake Predictor

IN ITS ORIGINAL FORM, the Sleep/Wake Predictor Commentary on the Three-Process of Alertness and Broader ing Issues Jaques Reifman and Philippa Gander REIFMAN J, GANDER P. Commentary on the three-process model of alertness and broader modeling issues.

More information

Waiting Time Distributions of Actigraphy Measured Sleep

Waiting Time Distributions of Actigraphy Measured Sleep The Open Sleep Journal, 2008, 1, 1-5 1 Waiting Time Distributions of Actigraphy Measured Sleep J.E. Slaven*,1, A. Mnatsakanova 1, S. Li 1,2, J.M. Violanti 3, C.M. Burchfiel 1, B.J. Vila 4 and M.E. Andrew

More information

Chapter Eleven. Sleep and Waking

Chapter Eleven. Sleep and Waking Chapter Eleven Sleep and Waking Sleep Are we getting enough. How z it work? Sleep Deprivation contributed to the Exxon Valdez, Challenger Explosion, and 3 Mile Island Deprivation is VERY common, and quite

More information

Circadian Rhythm Temperature Change

Circadian Rhythm Temperature Change Circadian Rhythm Temperature Change Objective: To study body temperature fluctuations and yawning to gain a better understanding of circadian rhythms. Grade Level: 5-8 Subject(s): Biology, Mathematics

More information

Homeostatic and Circadian Regulation of the Sleep-Wake Cycle

Homeostatic and Circadian Regulation of the Sleep-Wake Cycle Homeostatic and Circadian Regulation of the Sleep-Wake Cycle Derk-Jan Dijk, PhD, FRSB Professor of Sleep and Physiology Presentation for International Sleep Medicine Course Cardiff 6-9 June 216 Monday,

More information

Circadian photoreception in humans: More than meets the eye

Circadian photoreception in humans: More than meets the eye DAYLIGHTING (4.430) MIT Architecture Circadian photoreception in humans: More than meets the eye Steven W. Lockley, Ph.D. Division of Sleep Medicine, Brigham and Women s Hospital, Boston, MA Division of

More information

P08 Reversible loss of consciousness. E365 Aviation Human Factors

P08 Reversible loss of consciousness. E365 Aviation Human Factors P08 Reversible loss of consciousness E365 Aviation Human Factors Need to sleep Sleep is a natural state of rest for the body and mind that involves the reversible loss of consciousness. You sleep to not

More information

Homeostatic Regulation of REM Sleep in Humans During Extended Sleep

Homeostatic Regulation of REM Sleep in Humans During Extended Sleep Homeostatic Regulation of REM Sleep in Humans During Extended Sleep Giuseppe Barbato and Thomas A. Wehr Clinical Psychobiology Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda Md Summary: Benington

More information

Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours

Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours Halszka OGINSKA Department of Ergonomics, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland Abstract:

More information

Chronobiology and Sleep. Prolonged Interval From Body Temperature Nadir to Sleep Offset in Patients With Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome

Chronobiology and Sleep. Prolonged Interval From Body Temperature Nadir to Sleep Offset in Patients With Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome Sleep, 19(1):36-40 1996 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Chronobiology and Sleep Prolonged Interval From Body Temperature Nadir to Sleep Offset in Patients With Delayed Sleep

More information

Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance

Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance Clin Sports Med 24 (2005) 237 249 Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance Hans P.A. Van Dongen, PhD*, David F. Dinges, PhD Division of Sleep and Chronobiology, Department of Psychiatry, University

More information

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. A Actigraphy, 475, 485, 496 Adolescents, sleep disorders in, 576 578 Adults, sleep disorders in, 578 580 Advanced sleep phase disorder, 482 Age,

More information

Chapter Five. Sleep McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved.

Chapter Five. Sleep McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. Chapter Five Sleep 2011 McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. Endocrine System Made up of ductless glands that produce hormones Hormones control various body functions/processes Hormones are

More information

Definition 1: A fixed point iteration scheme to approximate the fixed point, p, of a function g, = for all n 1 given a starting approximation, p.

Definition 1: A fixed point iteration scheme to approximate the fixed point, p, of a function g, = for all n 1 given a starting approximation, p. Supplemental Material: A. Proof of Convergence In this Appendix, we provide a computational proof that the circadian adjustment method (CAM) belongs to the class of fixed-point iteration schemes (FPIS)

More information

Sleep Across the Life Cycle

Sleep Across the Life Cycle SECTION II Anatomy and Physiology CHAPTER 3 Sleep Across the Life Cycle IOURI KREININ L E A R N I N G O B J E C T I V E S On completion of this chapter, the reader should be able to 1. Describe the elements

More information

COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY

COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY Jeklin, A., Aguirre, A., Guttkuhn, R., Davis, W. Circadian Technologies Inc., Boston, United States Introduction Petrochemical

More information

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children QUESTION #1: ARE SLEEP ISSUES IN CHILDREN THE SAME AS IN ADULTS? Distinctive Features

More information

Circadian Rhythm Disturbances: What Happens When Your Biological Clock Is In The Wrong Time Zone

Circadian Rhythm Disturbances: What Happens When Your Biological Clock Is In The Wrong Time Zone Circadian Rhythm Disturbances: What Happens When Your Biological Clock Is In The Wrong Time Zone Steven A. Thau MD Chief, Pulmonary, Sleep Department. Phelps Hospital, Northwell Health Internal Clock Examples

More information

Chronobiology Biological rhythm Ultradian Infradian Circadian Circatidal Circalunar Circannual Endogenous Free-running Pacemaker Target tissue

Chronobiology Biological rhythm Ultradian Infradian Circadian Circatidal Circalunar Circannual Endogenous Free-running Pacemaker Target tissue Chronobiology Biological rhythm Ultradian Infradian Circadian Circatidal Circalunar Circannual Endogenous Free-running Pacemaker Target tissue Overt rhythm Nocturnal Diurnal LL DD LD 12:12 Study of biological

More information

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects ORIGINAL ARTICLES Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects Maria Josè Esposito, Ms.Sc.,Vincenzo Natale, M.D., Ph.D., Miranda Occhionero, M.D., Ph.D., and PierCarla Cicogna, Ph.D.

More information

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep Sleep. 19(7):570-575 1996 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep Mats Gillberg, Garan Kecklund, John Axelsson and

More information

EEG Electrode Placement

EEG Electrode Placement EEG Electrode Placement Classifying EEG brain waves Frequency: the number of oscillations/waves per second, measured in Hertz (Hz) reflects the firing rate of neurons alpha, beta, theta, delta Amplitude:

More information

COMPARISON BETWEEN AUTORHYTHMOMETRIC METHODS AND A BASELINE MEASUREMENT OF CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS IN NIGHT-WORKERS

COMPARISON BETWEEN AUTORHYTHMOMETRIC METHODS AND A BASELINE MEASUREMENT OF CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS IN NIGHT-WORKERS J. Human Ergol.,11, Suppl.: 385-391(1982) COMPARISON BETWEEN AUTORHYTHMOMETRIC METHODS AND A BASELINE MEASUREMENT OF CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS IN NIGHT-WORKERS R. MOOD and G. HILDEBRANDT Institut fur Arbeitsphysiologie

More information

Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance. Gregory Belenky, M.D. Sleep and Performance Research Center

Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance. Gregory Belenky, M.D. Sleep and Performance Research Center Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance Gregory Belenky, M.D. The Earth at Night: The Problem of 24/7 Operations The 24-Hour Sleep/Wake Cycle Waking 0000 Slow Wave 1800 0600 REM 1200 Sleep-Related Factors Affecting

More information

This brief animation illustrates the EEG patterns of the different stages of sleep, including NREM and REM sleep.

This brief animation illustrates the EEG patterns of the different stages of sleep, including NREM and REM sleep. Brain wave frequency and amplitude This brief animation illustrates the EEG patterns of the different stages of sleep, including NREM and REM sleep. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=u WYwMnMMEoU&feature=related

More information

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 9: Sleep and Biological Rhythms

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 9: Sleep and Biological Rhythms Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 9: Sleep and Biological Rhythms This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited by law: any public

More information

ASLEF. More than. just a union. Rostering Best Practice THE TRAIN DRIVERS UNION

ASLEF. More than. just a union. Rostering Best Practice THE TRAIN DRIVERS UNION ASLEF THE TRAIN DRIVERS UNION just a union Rostering Best Practice ASLEF THE TRAIn DRIVERS union THE TRAIn DRIVERS union Rostering Best Practice This leaflet is a brief guide to Representatives on best

More information

Stage REM. Stage 3/4. Stage 2. Sleep 101. NREM vs. REM. Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Is Needed To: 9/24/2013

Stage REM. Stage 3/4. Stage 2. Sleep 101. NREM vs. REM. Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Is Needed To: 9/24/2013 The Power of Sleep: Supporting Healthy Sleep in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders REM Stage 1 TERRY KATZ, PHD UNIVERSITY OF COLORADO SCHOOL OF MEDICINE JFK PARTNERS CHILD DEVELOPMENT UNIT, CHILDREN

More information

Consciousness, Stages of Sleep, & Dreams. Defined:

Consciousness, Stages of Sleep, & Dreams. Defined: Consciousness, Stages of Sleep, & Dreams I. Consciousness Conscious is: Waking Consciousness Defined: Altered State of Consciousness Defined: Most of waking life Fuzzy, organized, bizarre thoughts Examples:

More information

Consciousness. Mind-body Problem. Cartesian Substance Dualism 2/2/11. Fundamental issue addressed by psychologists Dualism. Monism

Consciousness. Mind-body Problem. Cartesian Substance Dualism 2/2/11. Fundamental issue addressed by psychologists Dualism. Monism Consciousness Mind-body Problem Fundamental issue addressed by psychologists Dualism Mind is immaterial Mind can exist separate from the body Monism Mind and body are different aspects of the same thing

More information

Healthy Sleep Tips Along the Way!

Healthy Sleep Tips Along the Way! Women and Sleep What You Will Learn The Benefits and Importance of Sleep States and Stages of the Sleep Cycle Unique Physiology of Women s Sleep Common Disorders in Women that Affect Sleep Women s Role

More information

Sleep Onset. Nighttime Drop in Body Temperature: A Physiological Trigger for Sleep Onset?

Sleep Onset. Nighttime Drop in Body Temperature: A Physiological Trigger for Sleep Onset? Sleep, 20(7):505-511 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep Onset Nighttime Drop in Body Temperature: A Physiological Trigger for Sleep Onset? Patricia J. Murphy and

More information

Commercial Vehicle Drivers Hours of Service Module 1 Overview

Commercial Vehicle Drivers Hours of Service Module 1 Overview Module 1 Overview June 23, 2008 Things to think about What if there were no rules limiting how many hours a driver could drive a commercial vehicle? What would happen to the commercial vehicle driver?

More information

Understanding Sleep Regulatory Processes to Improve Waking Performance

Understanding Sleep Regulatory Processes to Improve Waking Performance Understanding Sleep Regulatory Processes to Improve Waking Performance 1 st :envihab Symposium Cologne, Germany May 23, 2011 Daniel Aeschbach Division of Sleep Medicine Brigham and Women s Hospital Harvard

More information

Shift Work: An Occupational Health and Safety Hazard. Sandra Buxton, BA (Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Master of Philosophy

Shift Work: An Occupational Health and Safety Hazard. Sandra Buxton, BA (Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Master of Philosophy Shift Work: An Occupational Health and Safety Hazard Sandra Buxton, BA (Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Master of Philosophy of Murdoch University 2003 ii I declare that this thesis is

More information

States of Consciousness

States of Consciousness States of Consciousness Sleep, Dreams, and Body Rhythms Introduction Consciousness Awareness of oneself and one s environment Body Rhythms Biological Rhythms Periodic physiological fluctuations Can affect

More information

Artificial organisms that sleep

Artificial organisms that sleep Artificial organisms that sleep Marco Mirolli 1,2, Domenico Parisi 1 1 Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, National Research Council Viale Marx 15, 137, Rome, Italy parisi@ip.rm.cnr.it 2

More information

th Ave NE Suite F Bellevue, WA Phone: (425) Fax: (425) Excessive Daytime Sleepiness

th Ave NE Suite F Bellevue, WA Phone: (425) Fax: (425) Excessive Daytime Sleepiness 1414 116 th Ave NE Suite F Bellevue, WA 98004 Phone: (425) 451-8417 Fax: (425) 455-4089 Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Nearly everyone has days when they feel sleepy. But for some people, excessive sleepiness

More information

Physiology of Sleep. Dr Nervana

Physiology of Sleep. Dr Nervana Physiology of Sleep Dr Nervana Objectives: 1. Explain the difference between sleep and coma. 2. Define NREM (non-rapid eye movement, SWS) and REM (rapid eye movement) sleep. 3. Describe how NREM and REM

More information

Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness

Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness Sleep 20(3):215-223 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness Goran Kecklund Torbjom Akerstedt and Arne Lowden National

More information

Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia

Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia Rocky Garrison, PhD, CBSM Damon Michael Williams, RN, PMHNP-BC In House Counseling Laughing Heart LLC 10201 SE Main St. 12 SE 14 th Ave. Suite 10

More information

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association Sleep in patients with a number of sleep disorders and

More information

9.20 M.I.T Lecture #8

9.20 M.I.T Lecture #8 9.20 M.I.T. 2013 Lecture #8 More about innate behavior & motivation Key concepts from Lorenz on fundamentals of ethology: The fixed action pattern 1 Why do animals sleep? A question with multiple levels

More information

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type. A Accidents, at work, effect of shift work disorder on, 263 264 Acetylcholine, in circadian rhythms, 100 105 Acrophase, definition of, 301 Actigraphy,

More information

ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ

ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ ATHLETES & PRESCRIBING PHYSICIANS PLEASE READ USADA can grant a Therapeutic Use Exemption (TUE) in compliance with the World Anti- Doping Agency International Standard for TUEs. The TUE application process

More information

Sleep Wake Cycle in Depression

Sleep Wake Cycle in Depression Sleep Wake Cycle in Depression Constantin R. Soldatos Professor of Psychiatry & Founder of Sleep Study Center Eginition Hospital University of Athens Lecture in Suzdal School, 20/04/2013 SLEEP WAKE CYCLE

More information

EFFECTS OF NIGHTTIME NAPS ON BODY TEMPERATURE CHANGES, SLEEP PATTERNS, AND SELF-EVALUATION

EFFECTS OF NIGHTTIME NAPS ON BODY TEMPERATURE CHANGES, SLEEP PATTERNS, AND SELF-EVALUATION J. Human Ergol., 10:173-184, 1981 EFFECTS OF NIGHTTIME NAPS ON BODY TEMPERATURE CHANGES, SLEEP PATTERNS, AND SELF-EVALUATION OF SLEEP Kazuya MATSUMOTO Department of Hygiene, Kyorin University School of

More information

Chapter 5. Variations in Consciousness 8 th Edition

Chapter 5. Variations in Consciousness 8 th Edition Chapter 5 Variations in Consciousness 8 th Edition Consciousness: Personal Awareness Awareness of Internal and External Stimuli Levels of awareness James stream of consciousness Freud unconscious Sleep/dreaming

More information