Mismatch negativity (MMN): an objective measure of sensory memory and long-lasting memories during sleep

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Mismatch negativity (MMN): an objective measure of sensory memory and long-lasting memories during sleep"

Transcription

1 International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) Mismatch negativity (MMN): an objective measure of sensory memory and long-lasting memories during sleep Mercedes Atienza*, Jose L. Cantero, Elena Dominguez-Marin Laboratory of Sleep and Cognition, Seville, Spain Received 7 July 2002; received in revised form13 August 2002; accepted 3 September 2002 Abstract Sleep, unlike wakefulness, facilitates the internal stimulus generation and hinders the processing of external stimulation. Nevertheless, evidence yielded by physiological studies in animals and event-related potential (ERP) studies in humans suggest that basic functions of the central auditory system are still preserved during sleep. This review is focused on the automatic change-detection function of the auditory system as revealed by a negative ERP component called mismatch negativity (MMN). MMN mainly originates in the auditory cortex, although it also receives an important contribution from subcortical areas (especially at thalamic level), as well as frontal areas. We discuss recent experiments supporting the use of MMN as an objective measure of sensory memory and long-lasting memories not only during wakefulness, but also during sleep. The outcome of the activation of MMN generating systemduring sleep highly differs fromthat in waking, especially when there is no previous information about the stimulus sequence in the neuronal network as a result of learning. We discuss these differences in MMN generation in terms of a dynamicist view of the brain that emphasizes the importance of the integration between bottom-up and top-down influences on sensory processing, independently of the processing level in the auditory hierarchy Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Mismatch negativity (MMN); Brain generators; Sleep; Sensory memory; Learning; Bottom-up mechanisms; Top-down mechanisms 1. Introduction Automatic detection of any discernible change in the surrounding auditory environment is an adaptive function of the auditory nervous system that persists to some degree under different states *Corresponding author. Division de Neurociencias, Laboratorio Andaluz de Biologıa, Universidad Pablo de Olavides. Carretera de Utrera Km. 1, Sevilla (Spain). Tel.yfax: q address: mercedes_atienza@hms.harvard.edu (M. Atienza). of consciousness. Because of the change-detection mechanisms, any input can potentially reach awareness in the absence of or during low states of consciousness, for example during sleep. Automatic change-detection processes have been extensively studied during wakefulness using the mismatch negativity (MMN) as electrophysiological index. The MMN is a component of the auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) typically elicited when a repetitive sound (standard) is substituted by another low-probability sound /02/$ - see front matter 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. PII: S Ž

2 216 M. Atienza et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) Fig. 1. (a) Frontal (Fz) grand average waveforms to frequent tones (80%, thick line) of 1000 Hz and infrequent tones (20%, thin line) of different frequencies (as indicated on the left side). (b) Difference waveforms resulting from subtracting ERPs elicited by the infrequent tone fromthose elicited by the frequent tone. Adapted, with permission, from Sams et al. (1985). (deviant) that differs in some physical feature (e.g. frequency, loudness, duration, location, see Fig. 1). MMN elicitation is claimed to not require attention and is thought to be the result of a preperceptual process able to detect changes in a repetitive sound sequence based on a regularity detection process (Winkler et al., 1996) and an automatic comparison process (Naatanen, 1990). The presentation of a sound sequence activates neural analyzers located at different levels of the ascending auditory pathway in order to extract information about physical, temporal, and spatial features of single sounds and regularities of the sound sequence, such as stimulus repetition or stimulus order. This information is maintained in sensory memory for a short time period extending up to several seconds. Each time that a new input reaches the auditory cortex, the result of its analysis is compared with the contents of that sensory representation. If the sound matches the representation of the foregoing sound sequence, it may be incorporated into the memory representation; on the contrary, neural mechanisms responsible for the automatic change detection are activated, as revealed by MMN elicitation. MMN generator mechanisms have been further hypothesized to be involved in the involuntary switching of attention to sound changes (Naatanen, 1990). Accordingly, once the change-detection mechanism is activated, it can trigger a chain of neural events whereby attention is shifted toward the deviant stimulus. This automatic attention shift may result in conscious awareness of the stimulus. MMN reflects, therefore, a pre-attentive process, i.e. it is elicited by the deviant sound even when subjects attention is directed elsewhere, for example when individuals are engaged in a different task. Its elicitation in the absence of attention has motivated the use of MMN as a valid tool in studying automatic change-detection processes during brain states different fromwakefulness. Physiological studies in animals have demonstrated that auditory function is preserved during sleep to some extent (see Coenen and Drinkenburg, in press), supporting the hypothesis that basic preattentive processes such as auditory change detection could also be preserved during sleep. The main goal of the present review is to determine to what extent the auditory discrimination process is feasible during human sleep as revealed by the MMN component. Initially, we review the main brain areas involved in MMN generation. The knowledge of MMN generators may help to understand better the effects of sleep on its elicitation. The relationship between MMN and different forms of memory are addressed in order to determine the influence of sleep on some aspects of memory function. Finally, differences in MMN generation between wakefulness and sleep are discussed on the basis of bottom-up and top-down mechanisms that might be affected during sleep. 2. MMN generation relies on the activation of a temporofrontal neural network MMN elicitation requires, as mentioned above, several parallel andyor sequential processes: (1) the pre-attentive analysis of sound features; (2) the extraction of regularities fromthe auditory sequence on the basis of the organization of sound; (3) the maintenance in memory of a neural rep-

3 M. Atienza et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) resentation of the sound sequence; and finally (4) a comparison process between the current sensory input and the memory traces of the foregoing sound sequence. All these processes are thought to be confined to areas located at or near the temporal auditory cortex (Naatanen, 1992). According to results fromintracranial ERP recordings in humans, different areas within the temporal cortex appear to have distinctive roles in sound discrimination. Specifically, Kropotov et al. (2000) found that responses fromarea 41 are especially sensitive to changes in the sound frequency, suggesting that the neuronal circuits in the primary auditory cortex are mainly responsible for the analysis of stimulus features. ERPs recorded from secondary auditory cortex (area 42) were significantly affected by stimulus rate, probably reflecting the formation of memory traces on the basis of information provided by the analysis of the repetitive sound in the primary auditory cortex. Finally, specific responses to the deviant sound were recorded fromthe auditory association cortex (area 22). Evidence for the temporal component of MMN further comes from scalp current-density analysis of electroencephalographic (EEG) (Deouell et al., 1998) and electromagnetic (MEG) recordings (Alho et al., 1998); fromhigh-spatial-resolution techniques, such as positron emission tomography (PET) (Dittmann-Balcar et al., 2001), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) (Celsis et al., 1999), event-related optical signal (EROS) recordings (Rinne et al., 1999); and fromhuman lesion studies (Alain et al., 1998). However, the exact location of the MMN generators within the auditory cortex and within each hemisphere appears to depend on which specific single-sound feature is changed (Levanen et al., 1996), on whether a single feature or a conjunction of two or more features are changed (Schairer et al., 2001), and on the complexity of the sound (Alho et al., 1996). Results fromanimal studies also support the involvement of the temporal cortex in the changedetection mechanism. Kraus and colleagues demonstrated that an MMN in response to tonal contrasts (Kraus et al., 1994a) and synthesized speech syllables (Kraus et al., 1994b) can be recorded in anesthetized guinea pigs fromthe surface midline and caudiomedial division of the medial geniculate body (MGcm), both cerebral locations associated with the non-primary auditory pathway. These results further indicate distinct contributing sources for duration and spectral speech contrasts in the MGcm, but not at the surface midline (Kraus et al., 1994b), supporting the existence of different generators to detect changes in different features, even at the subcortical level. In addition, it was found that the midline evoked response to changes in the interaural phase differed, depending on the context in which the deviant stimulus was presented (King et al., 1995). In this study, responses to stimuli presented in-phase and out-of-phase to both ears were very similar at the midline surface and significantly different fromthe temporal response, suggesting that differences in the phase are represented in the primary auditory cortex. In contrast, the midline response to stimuli out-of-phase was different when this stimulus was presented in a sequence of standard stimuli or in a series alone, supporting the non-primary pathway origin for the MMN. King and colleagues draw the conclusion that both primary and non-primary pathways are necessary for MMN elicitation, but neither is sufficient alone. In addition to auditory cortex and thalamus, MMN has also been recorded in inferior colliculus and hippocampus (for a review Csepe, 1995). These findings together make it clear that independent processing of features and the change detection process reflected by MMN can occur at lower levels of the auditory system. The MMN may also play a role in triggering involuntary attention switches toward the sound changes (Naatanen, 1990). Current evidence suggests that MMN recorded fromthe scalp in humans receives an important contribution from the frontal cortex, especially fromthe right frontal lobe (e.g. Levanen et al., 1996; but see also Deouell et al., 1998 for bilateral frontal contribution). Patients with dorsolateral prefrontal lesions showed a marked reduction of the MMN, especially over the damaged hemisphere (Alain et al., 1998). However, the only direct evidence of a frontal MMN generator, located in the opercular part of the right inferior frontal gyrus in adults, has recently been reported by Opitz et al. (2002) using ERPs and

4 218 M. Atienza et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) fmri measures. This frontal activation occurs several milliseconds later than the temporal activation (Rinne et al., 2000), which is in agreement with the hypothesis that the change detection (temporal areas) may lead to subsequent initiation of an involuntary attention switch toward the sound eliciting MMN (frontal areas). Together, these studies suggest that MMN reflects the activation of a temporofrontal neural network mainly involved in stimulus-driven change-detection processes that may otherwise facilitate involuntary switches of attention. 3. MMN as an objective index of sensory memory in wakefulness and sleep Pre-attentive change detection reflected by MMN requires that a neural representation of the auditory context is maintained in auditory sensory memory for a period of time long enough to compare it with the incoming stimulus. This neural representation is based, in turn, on the information provided by feature-extraction processes along the afferent auditory pathway (Naatanen and Winkler, 1999). According to this view, MMN elicitation depends on whether or not auditory sensory memory contains a strong (or activated) representation of the immediately previous sound sequence at the moment that the deviant stimulus is delivered. Thus, the stronger this representation, the larger is the MMN amplitude. The probability of the repetitive sound is presumed to be a key factor of the strength of the neural representation in sensory memory (Naatanen, 1992). In a recent study, Sato et al. (2000) found that the frontal MMN component increased as the probability of the deviant stimulus was decreased. In contrast, the temporal generators were equally activated, independently of the deviant probability of occurrence. On the basis of these results, the authors draw the conclusion that a weak representation of the standard stimulus is enough to activate the automatic change-detection process reflected by MMN, but that activation of the frontal component and subsequent initiation of the attention switch to the auditory change depend on the strength of such a neural representation. These findings, coupled with the intrinsic pre-attentive nature of the changedetection process reflected by MMN, make its elicitation during sleep feasible. Sleep is not a homogeneous brain state. It is characterized by tonic and phasic fluctuations of the background electroencephalographic (EEG) activity. The sleep state consists of the well-known stages 1, 2, 3, 4 and REM (where the four first stages are often grouped as non-rem wnremx sleep and stages 3 and 4 are known as slow-wave sleep wswsx). These stages are, in turn, characterized by different transient phasic events. In addition to changes in the EEG activity, neuromodulation and cognition also vary dramatically fromwakefulness to sleep and across stages within sleep itself (for an extensive review see Hobson et al., 2000). All these changes are thought to affect information processing of external stimuli. Nevertheless, in agreement with the notion that auditory function is preserved to some extent during sleep, MMN, as for other auditory waking- ERP components, has been demonstrated to be elicited during sleep, especially during REM sleep (for a review see Atienza et al., 2001). Csepe et al. (1987) were the first to observe an attenuated MMN during SWS in cats. However, Paavilainen et al. (1987) failed to record MMN in any stage of NREM sleep in humans (subjects did not reach REM sleep), probably as a result of the small frequency deviance (1000 vs Hz) employed in their study. Campbell et al. (1992) demonstrated that a large frequency deviation (1000 vs Hz) may elicit a small MMN in late stage 2 and REM sleep. In the years following the publication of these results, other human studies using the oddball paradigmduring stages 2, 3, or 4 of NREM sleep also failed to record MMN (Nielsen-Bohlman et al., 1991; Niiyama et al., 1994; Sallinen et al., 1994; Winter et al., 1995; Loewy et al., 1996; Nordby et al., 1996; Nashida et al., 2000). Sallinen et al. (1994) did, however, record an MMN-like wave during stage 2 that was only evident when the deviant stimulus was followed by a K-complex. Nevertheless, this result demands caution because it could not be replicated in a subsequent study by the same authors using similar experimental conditions (Sallinen et al., 1996). It is possible that this MMN was over-

5 M. Atienza et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) Fig. 2. Grand average waveforms recorded from frontal (Fz) and right mastoid (RM) sites to frequent tones (90%, thick line) of 1000 Hz and infrequent tones (10%, thin line) of 2000 Hz. Stimuli were presented in wakefulness and all stages of sleep with stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 500 ms to the left ear at an intensity of 60 db SPL. Adapted, with permission, fromnashida et al. (2000). lapped by other negative waves unique to sleep, such as the N350 and N550. It would therefore appear that in general the MMN cannot easily be elicited in NREM sleep when simple oddball paradigms are employed. In contrast, several studies have replicated the result first reported by Campbell et al. (1992) during REM sleep under similar conditions (Loewy et al., 1996; Atienza et al., 1997, 2000; Nashida et al., 2000). Fig. 2 shows ERPs to single tones differing in frequency recorded fromfz and right mastoid in wakefulness and all stages of sleep (Nashida et al., 2000). In this study, MMN was elicited by the deviant tone in wakefulness, stage 1 and REM sleep. Interestingly in these studies, the interstimulus interval (ISI) varied between 450 and 600 ms. When slower stimulus rates were used (ISI of s), no MMN was recorded to similar frequency changes (one octave) during REM sleep (Niiyama et al., 1994; Nordby et al., 1996). Stimulus rate is another factor thought to determine the strength of stimulus representation in sensory memory. The longer the interstimulus interval, the weaker is the neural representation as a consequence of its progressive decay in sensory memory, which in turn may lead to decreases in the MMN amplitude (see Muller-Gass and Campbell, in press). However, against this assumption, manipulations of stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) in wakefulness did not lead to significant differences in the MMN amplitude (e.g. Schroger and Winkler, 1995). On the basis of results obtained by varying SOA, it has been concluded that auditory sensory memory duration is no longer than 4 10 s, with MMN disappearing abruptly when this time period is exceeded. The absence of a progressive decrease in MMN amplitude as the rate is slowed could be due to the fact that variations in the time between standard stimuli also run parallel to variations in the temporal probability of the deviant, which is known to be inversely related with MMN amplitude (e.g. Sabri and Campbell, 2001). Results fromstudies using alternative strategies in which stimuli are presented at a constant SOA within trains separated by a longer silent interval do not support the hypothesized progressive deterioration of the stimulus representation in sensory memory with the passage of time. For instance, Winkler et al. (2001) found MMNs for durationdeviant tones in all their subjects when the stimuli were presented at regular intervals of 7 s, but not when the same stimuli were presented at fast rate (SOAs0.5 s) within trains and the silent interval preceding the deviant stimulus was 7 s. These results cannot be explained by a rapid decay in the stimulus representation in sensory memory. An alternative explanation is provided by the modeladjustment hypothesis proposed to explain MMN elicitation (Winkler et al., 1996). According to this hypothesis, the auditory systemcontinuously updates the model of the auditory environment,

6 220 M. Atienza et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) generating as many MMNs as regularities (e.g. stimulus repetition, stimulus-feature repetition, alternating presentation of two stimuli) are violated. To date, only one study has evaluated MMN elicitation during sleep using the stimulus train paradigm (Atienza et al., 2000). In this study, subjects were tested during wakefulness and REM sleep. They were presented with single tones occurring at relatively fast rate (SOAs650 ms) within trains separated by a silent interval of 3, 6 or 9 s. The deviant tone could occur in position 1, 2, 4 or 6 within each train. As in previous waking studies, MMN elicitation did not occur when the silent interval preceding the deviant stimulus was 9 s. However, MMN was elicited to deviant tones presented in any other position within the train, independently of the silent interval. These results support the predictions of the model-adjustment hypothesis. Stimuli presented within trains are grouped as belonging to a specific context. Thus, when the deviant stimulus occurs in position 1 after a long silent interval, no reference is available for the comparison process. However, if the same deviant stimulus occurs in position 2, the first stimulus works as a reference capable of anticipating the subsequent acoustic event. The MMNgenerating systemis activated only if the second stimulus violates this prediction. During REM sleep, the MMN was elicited to the deviant tones occurring in all positions within the train only when the silent interval between trains was 3 s. This MMN was, however, attenuated relative to the waking state. Unexpectedly, the MMN-generating systemwas not activated to any deviant tone within the train when the silent interval was longer than 3 s, suggesting that sensory memory during REM sleep is much shorter than in wakefulness. The results of studies examining the MMN during sleep indicate that it occurs much more often during REM compared with NREM sleep. Parameters such as the rate presentation and the stimulus features are further determinant factors of MMN elicitation during sleep. For instance, while the MMN does occur in response to frequency changes, large changes in intensity elicited no MMN during stage 2 or REM sleep (Loewy et al., 2000), and interstimulus intervals longer than 1.5 s also prevented MMN elicitation during REM sleep (Nielsen-Bohlman et al., 1991; Nordby et al., 1996). However, the duration of sensory memory during REM sleep can be extended up to 3 s, provided that stimuli are rapidly presented within trains (Atienza et al., 2000). 4. MMN as an objective index of long-lasting memories in wakefulness and sleep The MMN-generating systemdoes not only access information stored in sensory memory, but also information contained in other, more permanent forms of memory. Naatanen et al. (1993b) presented subjects with two complex patterns of stimuli. If they could not discriminate between, themno MMN was generated. However, after they learned the discrimination, the deviant pattern now elicited the MMN. This MMN response progressively increased as performance became more accurate. No changes in amplitude were observed in those subjects who were initially very good in the discrimination task. MEG recordings have shown that this training-dependent MMN is generated in the auditory cortex (Alho et al., 1996; Tervaniemi et al., 2001). Subsequent studies have demonstrated changes in MMN not only immediately after training, but also several days later (Kraus et al., 1995; Tremblay et al., 1998; Menning et al., 2000; Atienza et al., 2002), suggesting a training-dependent long-termeffect on pre-attentive sensory processing. The ability of the brain to generate a permanent neural representation of a complex auditory pattern as a consequence of learning is also determined by the previous experience. For instance, musicians have demonstrated to have a special capability to extract information out of musically relevant stimuli as revealed by MMN (Tervaniemi et al., 2001). Interestingly, these authors showed that none of the non-musicians in their study learned to discriminate changes in a melodic pattern presented at several frequency levels, and that the previous experience of those musicians who showed accurate performance was more related to auditory than visual information. These effects of long-termexperience on pre-attentive auditory processing have also been reported for language-

7 M. Atienza et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) specific phonemes in infancy (Cheour et al., 1998) and in adulthood (Naatanen et al., 1997). Evidence fromanimal studies indicate that information learned during wakefulness can be recovered during REM sleep when the same stimulation is delivered (Hennevin et al., 1998). Likewise, a recent study conducted in adult humans has demonstrated that MMN can be recorded during REM sleep to small changes in a complex auditory pattern if subjects previously learned to behaviorally discriminate the two patterns (Atienza and Cantero, 2001). Fig. 3 shows frontal (Fz) ERPs to the standard and deviant patterns and the corresponding difference waveforms during the preand post-training phase, as well as during REM sleep. None of the subjects in that study showed MMN to the deviant pattern during wakefulness before training in the discrimination task. After learning, however, the MMN emerged during wakefulness and REM sleep with similar amplitudes. These results suggest that the neural changes initiated during wakefulness, probably during the training session, appeared to be accessible during REM sleep 2 days after training. On the basis of these findings, MMN might be used as a valid electrophysiological index to evaluate whether information acquired during training is stabilized in more permanent forms of memory during diurnal naps (Mednick et al., 2002) or nocturnal sleep (for a review see Maquet, 2001; Stickgold et al., 2001). Interestingly, Cheour et al. (2002) have recently demonstrated that newborns are able to learn to discriminate speech sounds during sleep. However, this result cannot be extrapolated to the adult human population. The main reason is that sleep in newborns is very different fromthat in adults, and it could subserve different functions. In fact, MMN in newborns can be recorded during wakefulness, quiet (equivalent to SWS in adults) and active (equivalent to REM sleep in adults) sleep, and its amplitude does not vary across these states (Cheour et al., 2000). In contrast, when MMN is recorded during sleep in adult humans, its amplitude is very small, even in REM sleep, compared with that obtained during wakefulness, suggesting that the MMN-generating systemis affected during sleep in a different way in newborns and adult humans. 5. Differences in sensory processing between wakefulness and sleep as revealed by MMN elicitation: bottom-up and top-down mechanisms 5.1. Bottom-up mechanisms The change-detection process underlying MMN elicitation mainly stems from the activity of ascending connections in the auditory hierarchy. Accordingly, MMN is elicited even when the subjects do not pay attention to the eliciting stimuli (Naatanen et al., 1993a), or control the occurrence of the infrequent deviant sounds (Rinne et al., 2001). Fromthis perspective, the much-reduced MMN during sleep can be explained by an inhibition of sensory input prior to entry in the MMN generators located in the temporal lobe. Indeed, single-unit studies in animals have reported a decrease in responsiveness to single tones during SWS for a high percentage of cells in different subthalamic and thalamic nuclei (see Coenen and Drinkenburg, in press). However, changes in the auditory cortex only partially reflect those occurring at subcortical level (Edeline et al., 2001), suggesting that cortical circuits might process auditory information relatively independently. Therefore, although the subcortical response is clearly diminished for a high percentage of cells, a high number of cortical cells show similar responses in both wakefulness and sleep. In agreement with the overall responsiveness level showed by the cortical cells in sleep and wakefulness, PET studies have shown no significant decreases in regional blood flow or glucose metabolism in temporal areas when wakefulness was compared with SWS and REM sleep (Braun et al., 1997). These results, coupled with the fact that MMN has been recorded at thalamic and subthalamic level (see Section 2 on MMN brain generators), support the notion that changes in bottom-up mechanisms may partially explain the reduced MMN observed during sleep Top-down mechanisms Classical theories of sensory processing assume that perception is the result of a serial bottom-up processing that requires a full and detailed analysis

8 222 M. Atienza et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) Fig. 3. Frontal (Fz) grand average waveforms to frequent (87%, thick line) and infrequent (13%, thin line) auditory patterns during wakefulness before training (pre-training), just after training (post-training) and during REM sleep. A schematic illustration of auditory patterns is shown below the vertical dotted line, which indicates the moment at which the frequency change was introduced within the pattern (sixth segment within the pattern, black bar). Adapted fromatienza and Cantero (2001). of the different units within the hierarchy. As a result of this ascending processing, the complexity of neural representations depends on the processing level within the hierarchy. In contrast to these classical theories, current approaches emphasize the functional importance of integration between bottom-up and top-down influences on sensory processing (for a review see Engel et al., 2001). According to dynamicist models, the idea of topdown is broader than the notion of feedback signal flow and does not necessarily require a processing hierarchy. The neural representation of a sensory event within a neuronal assembly could be modulated or modified by a dynamic process of neuronal recruiting into a larger assembly. A core idea in the top-down dynamicist models is that the recruiting process could occur between areas at the same processing level or within one area (Varela et al., 2001). For instance, incoming afferent signals induce a specific stimulus-driven pattern of synchronized activity in cortical areas. As a result of the spreading of this synchronized activity pattern through lateral connections, other neuronal populations in the same area andyor in other areas become part of the same overall assembly. These synchronizing (or desynchronizing) influences carry predictions about the sensory events. The specific pattern of synchronized activity that better expresses a successful match of the input to the predicted event will be a salient signal to other neuronal populations, recruiting of which will support the stabilization of a temporal coherence pattern (Engel et al., 2001). Fromthis perspective, the processing of an incoming stimulus depends on the previous experience. Accordingly, the pre-attentive detection of an auditory deviant event would depend on the representation of the previous stimulus sequence. In fact, MMN is only elicited when a stimulus is sorted as an infrequent event that happens among other frequent events, but not when it is presented in a series alone. The representation of the sound sequence could be modified on the basis of changes in the synchronicity of activity within a specific neuronal assembly, which in turn might be determined by the general state of the brain. A feasible hypothesis is that the background activity during sleep does not facilitate the spreading of synchronization within the auditory cortex with stimulus repetition, which in turn may affect the changedetection process underlying MMN elicitation. Fromthis viewpoint, differences in the self-generated synchronization within the auditory cortex

9 M. Atienza et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) could explain differences in MMN elicitation between wakefulness and sleep. As mentioned in previous sections, MMN receives an important contribution from the frontal cortex, supporting its role in triggering involuntary attention switches toward the infrequent auditory event. The outcome of this process might, through long-range interactions, modulate the synchronized activity pattern that arises fromlocal computations (sensory areas) operating on the input, affecting the neural representation of the auditory event. The deactivation of prefrontal areas during sleep as revealed by PET (Maquet et al., 1996; Braun et al., 1997) and fmri studies (Portas et al., 2000) could prevent the frontal MMN component elicitation contributing, at least partially, to the sleep- MMN attenuation. Indeed, a reduction of the later portion of MMN was reported during REM sleep by Loewy et al. (1996), which was interpreted by the authors as a result fromdeactivation of the frontal MMN generators. Similarly, changes in alertness seem to mainly affect the temporal component of MMN (Sallinen and Lyytinen, 1997), reflected by the reversal polarity in mastoids when the nose is used as a reference. Together, these results support the notion that sleep-mmn attenuation might partially stem from the lack of topdown influences originating in the prefrontal cortex. Experience-dependent learning constitutes another source of top-down processing. As in wakefulness, improved sensory processing, revealed by enhanced MMN, was reported to occur during REM sleep 2 days after subjects were trained to behaviorally discriminate two complex auditory patterns (Atienza and Cantero, 2001). This result is strong evidence that learning-induced changes affect the stimulus-driven change-detection process reflected in the MMN component. Furthermore, the effects of learning on MMN during sleep strongly support the notion drawn in the present study, i.e. the lack of dynamic topdown influences on the sensory processing mainly affects the formation and maintenance of the neural representation, which in turn could affect the MMN amplitude. Obviously, these top-down influences do not include attention or expectancy factors, because evidence reviewed earlier demonstrated that these factors may modulate the MMN generating process but do not eliminate it (Naatanen et al., 1993a; Rinne et al., 2001; Muller- Gass and Campbell, in press). Instead, these topdown influences involve a wide variety of brain signals that convey information related to immediately previous experience and learning. 6. Conclusion MMN studies conducted in animals and humans during different brain states support the notion that the automatic detection of a change in a sound sequence is a basic function of the central auditory systemthat is also preserved during sleep. In adult humans, this function is especially evident during REM sleep, a brain state in which the sensory input and motor output gates are hardly open and internal stimulus generation is enhanced (Hobson et al., 2000). Despite these adverse conditions for sensory processing, the auditory cortex maintains the ability to organize information supplied by the ascending auditory systeminto a neural representation. The maintenance in sensory memory of such a representation activates the MMN generating system, provided that the information extracted from the incoming input does not match the information related to immediate experience. The outcome of this process highly differs from that in wakefulness, which we have hypothesized to be the result not only of weakened bottom-up influences, but also of altered top-down processing during sleep. Only under a brain model including the integration of self-generated temporal dynamics in neuronal networks at different processing levels with the view of a continuously adaptive brain is it possible to understand the reduced sleep- MMN to changes in a sequence of single tones and the enhanced sleep-mmn to changes in complex auditory patterns as a result of previous learning. References Alain, C., Woods, D.L., Knight, R.T., A distributed cortical network for auditory sensory memory in humans. Brain Res. 812, Alho, K., Tervaniemi, M., Huotilainen, M., et al., Processing of complex sounds in the human auditory cortex

10 224 M. Atienza et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) as revealed by magnetic brain responses. Psychophysiology 33, Alho, K., Winkler, I., Escera, C., et al., Processing of novel sounds and frequency changes in the human auditory cortex: magnetoencephalographic recordings. Psychophysiology 35, Atienza, M., Cantero, J.L., Complex sound processing during human REM sleep by recovering information from long-term memory as revealed by the mismatch negativity (MMN). Brain Res. 901, Atienza, M., Cantero, J.L., Gomez, C., The mismatch negativity component reveals the sensory memory during REM sleep in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 237, Atienza, M., Cantero, J.L., Gomez, C., Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness and REM sleep. Psychophysiology 37, Atienza, M., Cantero, J.L., Escera, C., Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by eventrelated brain potentials. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, Atienza, M., Cantero, J.L., Dominguez-Marin, E., The time course of neural changes underlying auditory perceptual learning. Learn. Mem. 9, Braun, A.R., Balkin, T.J., Wesensten, N.J., et al., Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep wake cycle. Brain 120, Campbell, K., Bell, I., Bastien, C., Evoked potential measures of information processing during natural sleep. In: Broughton, R.J., Ogilvie, R.D. (Eds.), Sleep, Arousal, and Performance. Birkhauser, Boston, MA, pp Celsis, P., Boulanouar, K., Doyon, B., et al., Differential fmri responses in the left posterior superior temporal gyrus and left supermarginal gyrus to habituation and change detection in syllables and tones. Neuroimage 9, Cheour, M., Ceponiene, R., Lehtokoski, A., et al., Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain. Nat. Neurosci. 1, Cheour, M., Leppanen, P.H., Kraus, N., Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool for investigating auditory discrimination and sensory memory in infants and children. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, Cheour, M., Martynova, O., Naatanen, R., et al., Speech sounds learned by sleeping newborns. Nature 415, Coenen, A.M.L., Drinkenburg, W.H.I.M., in press. Animal models for information processing during sleep. Int. J. Psychophysiol. Csepe, V., On the origin and development of the mismatch negativity. Ear Hearing 16, Csepe, V., Karmos, G., Molnar, M., Evoked potential correlates of stimulus deviance during wakefulness and sleep in cat animal model of mismatch negativity. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 66, Deouell, L.Y., Bentin, S., Giard, M.H., Mismatch negativity in dichotic listening: evidence for interhemispheric differences and multiple generators. Psychophysiology 35, Dittmann-Balcar, A., Juptner, M., Jentzen, W., Schall, U., Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation during automatic auditory duration-mismatch processing in humans: a positron emission tomography study. Neurosci. Lett. 308, Edeline, J.M., Dutrieux, G., Manunta, Y., Hennevin, E., Diversity of receptive field changes in auditory cortex during natural sleep. Eur. J. Neurosci. 14, Engel, A.K., Fries, P., Singer, W., Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, Hennevin, E., Maho, C., Hars, B., Neuronal plasticity induced by fear conditioning is expressed during paradoxical sleep: evidence fromsimultaneous recordings in the lateral amygdala and the medial geniculate in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 112, Hobson, J.A., Pace-Schott, E.F., Stickgold, R., Dreaming and the brain: toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, Discussion pp King, C., McGee, T., Rubel, E.W., Nicol, T., Kraus, N., Acoustic features and acoustic changes are represented by different central pathways. Hearing Res. 85, Kraus, N., McGee, T., Carrell, T., King, C., Littman, T., Nicol, T., Discrimination of speech-like signals in auditory thalamus and cortex. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 96, Kraus, N., McGee, T., Littman, T., Nicol, T., King, C., Encoding of acoustic change involves non-primary auditory thalamus. J. Neurophysiol. 72, Kraus, N., McGee, T., Carrell, T.D., King, C., Tremblay, K., Nicol, T., Central auditory systemplasticity associated with speech discrimination training. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 7, Kropotov, J.D., Alho, K., Naatanen, R., et al., Human auditory-cortex mechanisms of preattentive sound discrimination. Neurosci. Lett. 280, Levanen, S., Ahonen, A., Hari, R., McEvoy, L., Sams, M., Deviant auditory stimuli activate human left and right auditory cortex differently. Cereb. Cortex 6, Loewy, D.H., Campbell, K.B., Bastien, C., The mismatch negativity to frequency deviant stimuli during natural sleep. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 98, Loewy, D.H., Campbell, K.B., Lugt, D.R., Elton, M., Kok, A., The mismatch negativity during natural sleep: intensity deviants. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, Maquet, P., The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science 294, Maquet, P., Peters, J.M., Aerts, J., et al., Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature 383, Mednick, S.C., Nakayama, K., Cantero, J.L., et al., The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration. Nat. Neurosci. 5, Menning, H., Roberts, L.E., Pantev, C., Plastic changes in the auditory cortex induced by intensive frequency discrimination training. Neuroreport 11,

11 M. Atienza et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 46 (2002) Muller-Gass, A., Campbell, K.B., in press. Event-related potential measures of the inhibition of information processing. I. Selective attention in the waking state. Int. J. Psychophysiol. Naatanen, R., The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function. Behav. Brain Sci. 13, Naatanen, R., Attention and Brain Function. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ. Naatanen, R., Winkler, I., The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience. Psychol. Bull. 125, Naatanen, R., Paavilainen, P., Tiitinen, H., Jiang, D., Alho, K., Attention and mismatch negativity. Psychophysiology 30, Naatanen, R., Schroger, E., Karakas, S., Tervaniemi, M., Paavilainen, P., Development of a memory trace for a complex sound in the human brain. Neuroreport 4, Naatanen, R., Lehtokoski, A., Lennes, M., et al., Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses. Nature 385, Nashida, T., Yabe, H., Sato, Y., et al., Automatic auditory information processing in sleep. Sleep 23, Nielsen-Bohlman, L., Knight, R.T., Woods, D.L., Woodward, K., Differential auditory processing continues during sleep. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 79, Niiyama, Y., Fujiwara, R., Satoh, N., Hishikawa, Y., Endogenous components of event-related potential appearing during NREM stage 1 and REM sleep in man. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 17, Nordby, H., Hugdahl, K., Stickgold, R., Bronnick, K.S., Hobson, J.A., Event-related potentials (ERPs) to deviant auditory stimuli during sleep and waking. Neuroreport 7, Opitz, B., Rinne, T., Mecklinger, A., von Cramon, D.Y., Schroger, E., Differential contribution of frontal and temporal cortices to auditory change detection: fmri and ERP results. Neuroimage 15, Paavilainen, P., Cammann, R., Alho, K., Reinikainen, K., Sams, M., Naatanen, R., Event-related potentials to pitch change in an auditory stimulus sequence during sleep. In: Johnson, R., Rohrbaugh, J.W., Parasuraman, R. (Eds.), Current Trends in Event-Related Potential Research, EEG Suppl. 40. Elsevier Science Publishers, Amsterdam, pp Portas, C.M., Krakow, K., Allen, P., Josephs, O., Armony, J.L., Frith, C.D., Auditory processing across the sleep wake cycle: simultaneous EEG and fmri monitoring in humans. Neuron 28, Rinne, T., Alho, K., Alku, P., et al., Analysis of speech sounds is left-hemisphere predominant at ms after sound onset. Neuroreport 10, Rinne, T., Alho, K., Ilmoniemi, R.J., Virtanen, J., Naatanen, R., Separate time behaviors of the temporal and frontal mismatch negativity sources. Neuroimage 12, Rinne, T., Antila, S., Winkler, I., Mismatch negativity is unaffected by top-down predictive information. Neuroreport 12, Sabri, M., Campbell, K.B., Effects of sequential and temporal probability of deviant occurrence on mismatch negativity. Cognit. Brain Res. 12, Sallinen, M., Lyytinen, H., Mismatch negativity during objective and subjective sleepiness. Psychophysiology 34, Sallinen, M., Kaartinen, J., Lyytinen, H., Is the appearance of mismatch negativity during stage 2 sleep related to the elicitation of K-complex? Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 91, Sallinen, M., Kaartinen, J., Lyytinen, H., Processing of auditory stimuli during tonic and phasic periods of REM sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials. J. Sleep Res. 5, Sams, M., Paavilainen, P., Alho, K., Naatanen, R., Auditory frequency discrimination and event-related potentials. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 62, Sato, Y., Yabe, H., Hiruma, T., et al., The effect of deviant stimulus probability on the human mismatch process. Neuroreport 11, Schairer, K.S., Gould, H.J., Pousson, M.A., Source generators of mismatch negativity to multiple deviant stimulus types. Brain Topogr. 14, Schroger, E., Winkler, I., Presentation rate and magnitude of stimulus deviance effects on human pre-attentive change detection. Neurosci. Lett. 193, Stickgold, R., Hobson, J.A., Fosse, R., Fosse, M., Sleep, learning, and dreams: off-line memory reprocessing. Science 294, Tervaniemi, M., Rytkonen, M., Schroger, E., Ilmoniemi, R.J., Naatanen, R., Superior formation of cortical memory traces for melodic patterns in musicians. Learn. Mem. 8, Tremblay, K., Kraus, N., McGee, T., The time course of auditory perceptual learning: neurophysiological changes during speech sound training. Neuroreport 9, Varela, F., Lachaux, J.P., Rodriguez, E., Martinerie, J., The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, Winkler, I., Karmos, G., Naatanen, R., Adaptive modeling of the unattended acoustic environment reflected in the mismatch negativity event-related potential. Brain Res. 742, Winkler, I., Schroger, E., Cowan, N., The role of largescale memory organization in the mismatch negativity eventrelated brain potential. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 13, Winter, O., Kok, A., Kenemans, J.L., Elton, M., Auditory event-related potentials to deviant stimuli during drowsiness and stage 2 sleep. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 96,

Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials

Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials Clinical Neurophysiology 112 (2001) 2031 2045 www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials Mercedes Atienza a, *, José

More information

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing Cognitive Neuroscience 0 0 0 0 0 p Website publication November NeuroReport, () ONE of the basic properties of the auditory system is the ability to analyse complex temporal patterns. Here, we investigated

More information

Primitive intelligence in the auditory cortex

Primitive intelligence in the auditory cortex Review Primitive intelligence in the auditory cortex Risto Näätänen, Mari Tervaniemi, Elyse Sussman, Petri Paavilainen and István Winkler 283 The everyday auditory environment consists of multiple simultaneously

More information

Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness and REM sleep

Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness and REM sleep Psychophysiology, 37 ~2000!, 485 493. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2000 Society for Psychophysiological Research Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness

More information

Activation of the auditory pre-attentive change detection system by tone repetitions with fast stimulation rate

Activation of the auditory pre-attentive change detection system by tone repetitions with fast stimulation rate Cognitive Brain Research 10 (2001) 323 327 www.elsevier.com/ locate/ bres Short communication Activation of the auditory pre-attentive change detection system by tone repetitions with fast stimulation

More information

An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials

An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials Psychophysiology, 36 ~1999!, 22 34. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1999 Society for Psychophysiological Research An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related

More information

Early Occurrence Of Auditory Change Detection In The Human Brain

Early Occurrence Of Auditory Change Detection In The Human Brain Early Occurrence Of Auditory Change Detection In The Human Brain SABINE GRIMM University of Leipzig, Germany (formerly University of Barcelona, Spain) ICON, Brisbane, July 28 th, 2014 Auditory deviance

More information

Separate memory-related processing for auditory frequency and patterns

Separate memory-related processing for auditory frequency and patterns Psychophysiology, 36 ~1999!, 737 744. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1999 Society for Psychophysiological Research Separate memory-related processing for auditory frequency and

More information

RAPID COMMUNICATION Scalp-Recorded Optical Signals Make Sound Processing in the Auditory Cortex Visible

RAPID COMMUNICATION Scalp-Recorded Optical Signals Make Sound Processing in the Auditory Cortex Visible NeuroImage 10, 620 624 (1999) Article ID nimg.1999.0495, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on RAPID COMMUNICATION Scalp-Recorded Optical Signals Make Sound Processing in the Auditory Cortex

More information

Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory

Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory Elyse S. Sussman CA and Valentina Gumenyuk Department of Neuroscience, Albert Einstein College of Medicine,141

More information

Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fmri and ERP Results

Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fmri and ERP Results NeuroImage 15, 167 174 (2002) doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0970, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fmri

More information

Temporal integration: intentional sound discrimination does not modulate stimulus-driven processes in auditory event synthesis

Temporal integration: intentional sound discrimination does not modulate stimulus-driven processes in auditory event synthesis Clinical Neurophysiology 113 (2002) 1909 1920 www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Temporal integration: intentional sound discrimination does not modulate stimulus-driven processes in auditory event synthesis

More information

Human cortical functions in auditory change detection evaluated with multiple brain research methods

Human cortical functions in auditory change detection evaluated with multiple brain research methods Human cortical functions in auditory change detection evaluated with multiple brain research methods Teemu Rinne Cognitive Brain Research Unit Department of Psychology University of Helsinki Finland Academic

More information

Title change detection system in the visu

Title change detection system in the visu Title Attention switching function of mem change detection system in the visu Author(s) Kimura, Motohiro; Katayama, Jun'ich Citation International Journal of Psychophys Issue Date 2008-02 DOI Doc URLhttp://hdl.handle.net/2115/33891

More information

fmri (functional MRI)

fmri (functional MRI) Lesion fmri (functional MRI) Electroencephalogram (EEG) Brainstem CT (computed tomography) Scan Medulla PET (positron emission tomography) Scan Reticular Formation MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) Thalamus

More information

Development of infant mismatch responses to auditory pattern changes between 2 and 4 months old

Development of infant mismatch responses to auditory pattern changes between 2 and 4 months old European Journal of Neuroscience European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 29, pp. 861 867, 2009 doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06625.x COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE Development of infant mismatch responses to auditory

More information

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia In general, sensory processing as well as selective attention impairments are common amongst people with schizophrenia. It is important to note that experts have in

More information

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction Alexandra Muller-Gass, Erich

More information

The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential

The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential IstvaÂn Winkler 1,2, Erich SchroÈger 3, and Nelson Cowan 4 Abstract & The mismatch negativity (MMN)

More information

Myers Psychology for AP*

Myers Psychology for AP* Myers Psychology for AP* David G. Myers PowerPoint Presentation Slides by Kent Korek Germantown High School Worth Publishers, 2010 *AP is a trademark registered and/or owned by the College Board, which

More information

ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage

ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage INO056 10/18/04 6:34 PM Page 339 CHAPTER 56 ERP Measures of Multiple Attention Deficits Following Prefrontal Damage Leon Y. Deouell and Robert T. Knight ABSTRACT Maintaining a goal-directed behavior requires

More information

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY OF UNIMODAL AND AUDIOVISUAL SPEECH PERCEPTION

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY OF UNIMODAL AND AUDIOVISUAL SPEECH PERCEPTION AVSP 2 International Conference on Auditory-Visual Speech Processing ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY OF UNIMODAL AND AUDIOVISUAL SPEECH PERCEPTION Lynne E. Bernstein, Curtis W. Ponton 2, Edward T. Auer, Jr. House Ear

More information

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function: A Comparison of Electrophysiological and Other Neuroimaging Approaches Leun J. Otten Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & Department of Psychology University

More information

Atypical processing of prosodic changes in natural speech stimuli in school-age children with Asperger syndrome

Atypical processing of prosodic changes in natural speech stimuli in school-age children with Asperger syndrome Atypical processing of prosodic changes in natural speech stimuli in school-age children with Asperger syndrome Riikka Lindström, PhD student Cognitive Brain Research Unit University of Helsinki 31.8.2012

More information

FINE-TUNING THE AUDITORY SUBCORTEX Measuring processing dynamics along the auditory hierarchy. Christopher Slugocki (Widex ORCA) WAS 5.3.

FINE-TUNING THE AUDITORY SUBCORTEX Measuring processing dynamics along the auditory hierarchy. Christopher Slugocki (Widex ORCA) WAS 5.3. FINE-TUNING THE AUDITORY SUBCORTEX Measuring processing dynamics along the auditory hierarchy. Christopher Slugocki (Widex ORCA) WAS 5.3.2017 AUDITORY DISCRIMINATION AUDITORY DISCRIMINATION /pi//k/ /pi//t/

More information

Functional neuroanatomy of auditory mismatch processing: an event-related fmri study of duration-deviant oddballs

Functional neuroanatomy of auditory mismatch processing: an event-related fmri study of duration-deviant oddballs NeuroImage 20 (2003) 729 736 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Functional neuroanatomy of auditory mismatch processing: an event-related fmri study of duration-deviant oddballs Ulrich Schall, a, * Patrick

More information

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials Chinese Science Bulletin 2007 Science in China Press Springer-Verlag Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials SONG Yan 1, PENG

More information

Implicit, Intuitive, and Explicit Knowledge of Abstract Regularities in a Sound Sequence: An Event-related Brain Potential Study

Implicit, Intuitive, and Explicit Knowledge of Abstract Regularities in a Sound Sequence: An Event-related Brain Potential Study Implicit, Intuitive, and Explicit Knowledge of Abstract Regularities in a Sound Sequence: An Event-related Brain Potential Study Titia L. van Zuijen, Veerle L. Simoens, Petri Paavilainen, Risto Näätänen,

More information

Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST November Jennifer Melcher

Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST November Jennifer Melcher Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST 722 - November 27 Jennifer Melcher Forward suppression of unit activity in auditory cortex Brosch and Schreiner (1997) J Neurophysiol 77: 923-943. Forward

More information

Auditory sensory memory in 2-year-old children: an event-related potential study

Auditory sensory memory in 2-year-old children: an event-related potential study LEARNING AND MEMORY Auditory sensory memory in -year-old children: an event-related potential study Elisabeth Glass, te achse and Waldemar von uchodoletz Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

More information

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change Elena Yago, MarõÂa Jose Corral and Carles Escera CA Neurodynamics Laboratory, Department

More information

The Nervous System. Neuron 01/12/2011. The Synapse: The Processor

The Nervous System. Neuron 01/12/2011. The Synapse: The Processor The Nervous System Neuron Nucleus Cell body Dendrites they are part of the cell body of a neuron that collect chemical and electrical signals from other neurons at synapses and convert them into electrical

More information

The Central Nervous System

The Central Nervous System The Central Nervous System Cellular Basis. Neural Communication. Major Structures. Principles & Methods. Principles of Neural Organization Big Question #1: Representation. How is the external world coded

More information

Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements

Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements Supplementary Material Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements Xiaomo Chen, Katherine Wilson Scangos 2 and Veit Stuphorn,2 Department of Psychological and Brain

More information

Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, and Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Canada

Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, and Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Canada [Frontiers in Bioscience 5, d202-212, January 1, 2000] SELECTIVELY ATTENDING TO AUDITORY OBJECTS Claude Alain and Stephen R Arnott Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, and Department

More information

Takwa Adly Gabr Assistant lecturer of Audiology

Takwa Adly Gabr Assistant lecturer of Audiology Mismatch Negativity as an Objective Tool for the Assessment of Cognitive Function in Subjects with Unilateral Severe to Profound Sensorineural Hearing Loss Takwa Adly Gabr Assistant lecturer of Audiology

More information

Neural representation of consciously imperceptible speech sound differences

Neural representation of consciously imperceptible speech sound differences Perception & Psychophysics 2000, 62 (7), 1383-1393 Neural representation of consciously imperceptible speech sound differences JEFFREY ALLEN, NINA KRAUS, and ANN BRADLOW Northwestern University, Evanston,

More information

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM STRUCTURAL ORGANIZATION OF THE BRAIN The central nervous system (CNS), consisting of the brain and spinal cord, receives input from sensory neurons and directs

More information

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131 ! Chapter 5 Summary and Conclusions! 131 Chapter 5!!!! Summary of the main findings The present thesis investigated the sensory representation of natural sounds in the human auditory cortex. Specifically,

More information

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1 Hearing-aids Induce Plasticity in the Auditory System: Perspectives From Three Research Designs and Personal Speculations About the

More information

Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (3) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (3) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang 580.626 Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (3) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang Laboratory of Auditory Neurophysiology Department of Biomedical Engineering Johns

More information

Prefrontal cortex involvement in preattentive auditory deviance detection: neuroimaging and electrophysiological evidence

Prefrontal cortex involvement in preattentive auditory deviance detection: neuroimaging and electrophysiological evidence NeuroImage 20 (2003) 1270 1282 www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Prefrontal cortex involvement in preattentive auditory deviance detection: neuroimaging and electrophysiological evidence Christian F. Doeller,

More information

Timing & Schizophrenia. Deana Davalos Colorado State University

Timing & Schizophrenia. Deana Davalos Colorado State University Timing & Schizophrenia Deana Davalos Colorado State University What is Temporal Processing? (Thank you to all for background) In everyday terms Can I cross the street without being hit by a car? Do I have

More information

Thomas D. Carrell Department of Communication Disorders, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, Nebraska 68588

Thomas D. Carrell Department of Communication Disorders, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, Nebraska 68588 Central auditory system plasticity: Generalization to novel stimuli following listening training Kelly Tremblay a) Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Auditory Neuroscience Laboratory,

More information

Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm

Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm Ž. Cognitive Brain Research 7 1998 Research report Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm Erich Schroger a,), Christian Wolff b a Institut

More information

Lateralized Automatic Auditory Processing of Phonetic Versus Musical Information: A PET Study

Lateralized Automatic Auditory Processing of Phonetic Versus Musical Information: A PET Study Human Brain Mapping 10:74 79(2000) Lateralized Automatic Auditory Processing of Phonetic Versus Musical Information: A PET Study M. Tervaniemi, 1 * S. V. Medvedev, 2 K. Alho, 1,3 S. V. Pakhomov, 2 M. S.

More information

Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang Neurobiology of Hearing (Salamanca, 2012) Auditory Cortex (2) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang Laboratory of Auditory Neurophysiology Department of Biomedical Engineering Johns Hopkins University web1.johnshopkins.edu/xwang

More information

Newborns discriminate novel from harmonic sounds: A study using magnetoencephalography

Newborns discriminate novel from harmonic sounds: A study using magnetoencephalography Clinical Neurophysiology 117 (2006) 496 503 www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Newborns discriminate novel from harmonic sounds: A study using magnetoencephalography Anke Sambeth a,b,c, *, Minna Huotilainen

More information

Birkbeck eprints: an open access repository of the research output of Birkbeck College

Birkbeck eprints: an open access repository of the research output of Birkbeck College Birkbeck eprints: an open access repository of the research output of Birkbeck College http://eprints.bbk.ac.uk Sambeth, Anke; Huotilainen, Minna; Kushnerenko, Elena; Fellman, Vineta and Pihko, Elina (2006).

More information

Brain and Cognition. Cognitive Neuroscience. If the brain were simple enough to understand, we would be too stupid to understand it

Brain and Cognition. Cognitive Neuroscience. If the brain were simple enough to understand, we would be too stupid to understand it Brain and Cognition Cognitive Neuroscience If the brain were simple enough to understand, we would be too stupid to understand it 1 The Chemical Synapse 2 Chemical Neurotransmission At rest, the synapse

More information

Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 218 229 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Maturation of cortical mismatch responses to occasional

More information

This article was originally published in the Encyclopedia of Neuroscience published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's

More information

The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex

The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex Nelli H. Salminen and Hannu Tiitinen Department of Biomedical Engineering and Computational Science, Helsinki University of Technology,

More information

Graded Visual Attention Modulates Brain Responses Evoked by Task-irrelevant Auditory Pitch Changes

Graded Visual Attention Modulates Brain Responses Evoked by Task-irrelevant Auditory Pitch Changes Graded Visual Attention Modulates Brain Responses Evoked by Task-irrelevant Auditory Pitch Changes Gunes Yucel 1, Christopher Petty 1, Gregory McCarthy 1,2, and Aysenil Belger 1,3 Abstract & Previous studies

More information

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization 1 7.1 Overview This chapter aims to provide a framework for modeling cognitive phenomena based

More information

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT (1) Foreword (optional) (2) Table of Contents (if report is more than 10 pages) (3) List of Appendixes, Illustrations and Tables (if applicable) (4) Statement of the problem studied FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR In Physiology Today What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may

More information

The Mismatch Negativity (MMN) and the McGurk Effect

The Mismatch Negativity (MMN) and the McGurk Effect The Mismatch Negativity (MMN) and the McGurk Effect Colin, C. 1,2, Radeau, M. 1,3 and Deltenre, P. 1,2. 1 Research Unit in Cognitive Neurosciences, Université Libre de Bruxelles 2 Evoked Potentials Laboratory,

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

Event-related brain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries

Event-related brain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries Neurophysiology 10, 2125±2129 (1999) EVENT-RELATED brain potentials (ERPs) to auditory stimuli were recorded from 11 closed head injured (CHI) and 10 age-matched healthy adults. Auditory stimuli consisted

More information

Subdural recordings of the mismatch negativity (MMN) in patients with focal epilepsy

Subdural recordings of the mismatch negativity (MMN) in patients with focal epilepsy doi:10.1093/brain/awh442 Brain (2005), 128, 819 828 Subdural recordings of the mismatch negativity (MMN) in patients with focal epilepsy Timm Rosburg, 1 Peter Trautner, 1 Thomas Dietl, 1 Oleg A. Korzyukov,

More information

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention

Chapter 6. Attention. Attention Chapter 6 Attention Attention William James, in 1890, wrote Everyone knows what attention is. Attention is the taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously

More information

BRAIN AND ITS VITAL FUNCTIONS 1 Brain and Its Vital Functions Student s Name Institution Name Professor s Name Course Title BRAIN AND ITS VITAL FUNCTIONS 2 The brain is the integral organism and all its

More information

Functional magnetic resonance imaging measure of automatic and controlled auditory processing

Functional magnetic resonance imaging measure of automatic and controlled auditory processing AUDITORYAND VESTIBULAR SYSTEMS Functional magnetic resonance imaging measure of automatic and controlled auditory processing Teresa V. Mitchell, 3,w Rajendra A. Morey, 2,4,w,CA Seniha Inan 1,2 and Aysenil

More information

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS Franziska Kopp* #, Erich Schröger* and Sigrid Lipka # *University of Leipzig, Institute of General Psychology # University of Leipzig,

More information

What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010

What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010 What do you notice? Woodman, Atten. Percept. Psychophys., 2010 You are trying to determine if a small amplitude signal is a consistent marker of a neural process. How might you design an experiment to

More information

Title Page. Altering the primacy bias How does a prior task affect mismatch negativity (MMN)?

Title Page. Altering the primacy bias How does a prior task affect mismatch negativity (MMN)? Title Page Title Altering the primacy bias How does a prior task affect mismatch negativity (MMN)? Author names and affiliations: Daniel MULLENS 1-2, Jessica WOODLEY 1, Lisa WHITSON 1-2, Alexander PROVOST

More information

states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY

states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY states of brain activity sleep, brain waves DR. S. GOLABI PH.D. IN MEDICAL PHYSIOLOGY introduction all of us are aware of the many different states of brain activity, including sleep, wakefulness, extreme

More information

Chapter 5 The Research Methods of Biopsychology

Chapter 5 The Research Methods of Biopsychology Chapter 5 The Research Methods of Biopsychology Understanding What Biopsychologists Do This multimedia product and its contents are protected under copyright law. The following are prohibited by law: any

More information

Expectations Modulate the Magnitude of Attentional Capture by Auditory Events

Expectations Modulate the Magnitude of Attentional Capture by Auditory Events Expectations Modulate the Magnitude of Attentional Capture by Auditory Events Anatole Nöstl 1 *, John E. Marsh 1,2, Patrik Sörqvist 1,3 1 Department of Building, Energy and Environmental Engineering, University

More information

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION INTRODUCTION Research on the Deaf Brain is beginning to provide a new evidence base for policy and practice in relation to intervention with deaf children. This talk outlines the multi-channel nature of

More information

Chapter 6 Section 1. The Nervous System: The Basic Structure

Chapter 6 Section 1. The Nervous System: The Basic Structure Chapter 6 Section 1 The Nervous System: The Basic Structure Essential Question: How does studying the biology of the brain give us an understanding of our behavior? Draw or type 2 things you already know

More information

Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming. Elaine M. Hull

Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming. Elaine M. Hull Biological Rhythms, Sleep, and Dreaming Elaine M. Hull Rhythms of Waking and Sleeping Animals generate 24 hour cycles of wakefulness and sleep. Some animals generate endogenous circannual rhythms (yearly

More information

Cortical Auditory Evoked Potentials in Children with Developmental Dysphasia

Cortical Auditory Evoked Potentials in Children with Developmental Dysphasia Prague Medical Report / Vol. 19 (28) No. 4, p. 35 314 35) Cortical Auditory Evoked Potentials in Children with Developmental Dysphasia Charles University in Prague, First Faculty of Medicine and General

More information

III. Studying The Brain and Other Structures

III. Studying The Brain and Other Structures III. Studying The Brain and Other Structures 1. Accidents (case study) In 1848, a railroad worker named Phineas Gage was involved in an accident that damaged the front part of his brain. Gage s doctor

More information

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception

Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception Chapter 11: Sound, The Auditory System, and Pitch Perception Overview of Questions What is it that makes sounds high pitched or low pitched? How do sound vibrations inside the ear lead to the perception

More information

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition

The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition The role of amplitude, phase, and rhythmicity of neural oscillations in top-down control of cognition Chair: Jason Samaha, University of Wisconsin-Madison Co-Chair: Ali Mazaheri, University of Birmingham

More information

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry International Journal of Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities Volume 5 Article 3 June 2013 Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry Whitney R. Arthur Central Washington

More information

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück The Binding Problem This lecture is based on following articles: Adina L. Roskies: The Binding Problem; Neuron 1999 24: 7 Charles M. Gray: The Temporal Correlation Hypothesis of Visual Feature Integration:

More information

Neuroimaging Correlates of Human Auditory Behavior

Neuroimaging Correlates of Human Auditory Behavior Harvard-MIT Division of Health Sciences and Technology HST.722J: Brain Mechanisms for Hearing and Speech Course Instructor: Jennifer R. Melcher HST 722 Brain Mechanisms for Hearing and Speech October 27,

More information

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14 CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines Assignment 5: Neural mechanisms of object-based attention Due: Friday, April 14 This Assignment is a guided reading of the 2014 paper, Neural Mechanisms of Object-Based

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention Cognitive Neuroscience Attention There are many aspects to attention. It can be controlled. It can be focused on a particular sensory modality or item. It can be divided. It can set a perceptual system.

More information

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus Central Visual Pathways V1/2 NEUR 3001 dvanced Visual Neuroscience The Lateral Geniculate Nucleus () is more than a relay station LP SC Professor Tom Salt UCL Institute of Ophthalmology Retina t.salt@ucl.ac.uk

More information

Auditory ERPs to Stimulus Deviance in an Awake Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): Towards Hominid Cognitive Neurosciences

Auditory ERPs to Stimulus Deviance in an Awake Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): Towards Hominid Cognitive Neurosciences Auditory ERPs to Stimulus Deviance in an Awake Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): Towards Hominid Cognitive Neurosciences Ari Ueno 1 *, Satoshi Hirata 2, Kohki Fuwa 2, Keiko Sugama 2, Kiyo Kusunoki 2, Goh Matsuda

More information

Theta Oscillation Related to the Auditory Discrimination Process in Mismatch Negativity: Oddball versus Control Paradigm

Theta Oscillation Related to the Auditory Discrimination Process in Mismatch Negativity: Oddball versus Control Paradigm ORIGINAL ARTICLE J Clin Neurol 2012;8:35-42 Print ISSN 1738-6586 / On-line ISSN 2005-5013 http://dx.doi.org/10.3988/jcn.2012.8.1.35 Open Access Theta Oscillation Related to the Auditory Discrimination

More information

D. Debatisse, E. Fornari, E. Pralong, P. Maeder, H Foroglou, M.H Tetreault, J.G Villemure. NCH-UNN and Neuroradiology Dpt. CHUV Lausanne Switzerland

D. Debatisse, E. Fornari, E. Pralong, P. Maeder, H Foroglou, M.H Tetreault, J.G Villemure. NCH-UNN and Neuroradiology Dpt. CHUV Lausanne Switzerland Vegetative comatose and auditory oddball paradigm with Cognitive evoked potentials (CEPs) and fmri: Implications for the consciousness model of Damasio and Guerit D. Debatisse, E. Fornari, E. Pralong,

More information

Acetylcholine (ACh) Action potential. Agonists. Drugs that enhance the actions of neurotransmitters.

Acetylcholine (ACh) Action potential. Agonists. Drugs that enhance the actions of neurotransmitters. Acetylcholine (ACh) The neurotransmitter responsible for motor control at the junction between nerves and muscles; also involved in mental processes such as learning, memory, sleeping, and dreaming. (See

More information

Neuroscience of Consciousness I

Neuroscience of Consciousness I 1 C83MAB: Mind and Brain Neuroscience of Consciousness I Tobias Bast, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 What is consciousness? 3 Consciousness State of consciousness - Being awake/alert/attentive/responsive

More information

Processing Interaural Cues in Sound Segregation by Young and Middle-Aged Brains DOI: /jaaa

Processing Interaural Cues in Sound Segregation by Young and Middle-Aged Brains DOI: /jaaa J Am Acad Audiol 20:453 458 (2009) Processing Interaural Cues in Sound Segregation by Young and Middle-Aged Brains DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.20.7.6 Ilse J.A. Wambacq * Janet Koehnke * Joan Besing * Laurie L. Romei

More information

Mismatch Responses to Pitch Changes in Early Infancy

Mismatch Responses to Pitch Changes in Early Infancy Mismatch Responses to Pitch Changes in Early Infancy Chao He, Lisa Hotson, and Laurel J. Trainor Abstract & We investigated the emergence of discriminative responses to pitch by recording 2-, 3-, and 4-month-old

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J. The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem By Andrew Laguna, S.J. Outline I. Introduction II. The Visual System III. What is the Binding Problem? IV. Possible Theoretical Solutions

More information

Title of Thesis. Studies on Mechanisms of Visual and Auditory Attention in Temporal or Spatial Cueing Paradigm

Title of Thesis. Studies on Mechanisms of Visual and Auditory Attention in Temporal or Spatial Cueing Paradigm Title of Thesis Studies on Mechanisms of Visual and Auditory Attention in Temporal or Spatial Cueing Paradigm 2015 September Xiaoyu Tang The Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology (Doctor s

More information

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg

Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg Lecture 35 Association Cortices and Hemispheric Asymmetries -- M. Goldberg The concept that different parts of the brain did different things started with Spurzheim and Gall, whose phrenology became quite

More information

Neurophysiology & EEG

Neurophysiology & EEG Neurophysiology & EEG PG4 Core Curriculum Ian A. Cook, M.D. Associate Director, Laboratory of Brain, Behavior, & Pharmacology UCLA Department of Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences Semel Institute for

More information

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development Jessica R. Newton and Mriganka Sur Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Picower Center for Learning & Memory Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR What the Brain Does The nervous system determines states of consciousness and produces complex behaviors Any given neuron may have as many as 200,000

More information

Title Page. Altering the primacy bias How does a prior task affect mismatch negativity (MMN)?

Title Page. Altering the primacy bias How does a prior task affect mismatch negativity (MMN)? Page of 0 0 0 0 0 0 Title Page Title Altering the primacy bias How does a prior task affect mismatch negativity (MMN)? Author names and affiliations: Daniel MULLENS -, Jessica WOODLEY, Lisa WHITSON -,

More information

Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli

Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli Neuroscience Letters 424 (2007) 55 60 Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli Udo Will, Eric Berg School of Music, Cognitive Ethnomusicology, Ohio State University, 110

More information

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications An Overview of BMIs Luca Rossini Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications European Space Research and Technology Centre, European Space Agency Noordvijk, 30 th November 2009 Definition

More information