Cannabis intoxication inhibits avoidance action tendencies: a field study in the Amsterdam coffee shops

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Cannabis intoxication inhibits avoidance action tendencies: a field study in the Amsterdam coffee shops"

Transcription

1 Psychopharmacology (2013) 229: DOI /s ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Cannabis intoxication inhibits avoidance action tendencies: a field study in the Amsterdam coffee shops Janna Cousijn & Robin W. M. Snoek & Reinout W. Wiers Received: 25 October 2012 / Accepted: 26 March 2013 / Published online: 18 April 2013 # Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013 Abstract Rationale Experimental laboratory studies suggest that the approach bias (relatively fast approach responses) toward substance-related materials plays an important role in problematic substance use. How this bias is moderated by intention to use versus recent use remains unknown. Moreover, the relationship between approach bias and other motivational processes (satiation and craving) and executive functioning remains unclear. Objectives The aim of this study was to investigate the cannabis approach bias before and after cannabis use in real-life setting (Amsterdam coffee shops) and to assess the relationship between approach bias, craving, satiation, cannabis use, and response inhibition. Methods Cannabis, tobacco, and neutral approach and avoidance action tendencies were measured with the Approach Avoidance Task and compared between 42 heavy cannabis users with the intention to use and 45 heavy cannabis users shortly after cannabis use. The classical Stroop was used to Janna Cousijn and Robin W. M. Snoek contributed equally to this paper. J. Cousijn : R. W. M. Snoek : R. W. Wiers Addiction Development and Psychopathology (ADAPT)-lab, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands J. Cousijn Amsterdam Institute for Addiction Research, Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Centre, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands J. Cousijn Department of Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands J. Cousijn (*) Weesperplein 4, 1018 XA Amsterdam, The Netherlands j.cousijn@gmail.com measure response inhibition. Multiple regression analyses were conducted to investigate relationships between approach bias, satiation, craving, cannabis use, and response inhibition. Results In contrast to the hypotheses, heavy cannabis users with the intention to use did not show a cannabis approach bias, whereas intoxicated cannabis users did show an approach bias regardless of image category. This could be attributed to a general slowing of avoidance action tendencies. Moreover, craving was negatively associated with the approach bias, and no relationships were observed between the cannabis approach bias, satiation, prior cannabis use, and response inhibition. Conclusion Cannabis intoxication in a real-life setting inhibited general avoidance. Expression of the cannabis approach bias appeared not to be modulated by satiation or response inhibition. Keywords Approach Avoidance Task. Approach bias. Cannabis. Cannabis abuse. Craving. Satiation. Response inhibition Introduction Individuals with a substance use disorder are often aware of the potential negative consequences of their substance use, but still continue with these self-destructive behaviors (Stacy and Wiers 2010). According to dual-process theories of addiction, this paradox arises from an imbalance between a substanceoriented motivational system and a compromised reflective system (Stacy and Wiers 2010; Bechara 2005). The motivational system is characterized by fast relatively automatic processes and evaluates stimuli in terms of their affective and motivational significance. After repeated substance use, the motivational system is thought to become sensitized and conditioned toward substance-related cues (Stacy and Wiers

2 168 Psychopharmacology (2013) 229: ). The reflective system is characterized by slower regulatory processes. To stop or override the automatically triggered motivations to engage in substance use, one must have sufficient executive resources, which are often compromised in individuals with a substance use disorder (Bechara 2005; Grenard et al. 2008; Houben and Wiers 2009; Houben et al. 2011; Thush et al. 2008). Cognitive biases are thought to be behavioral manifestations of the underlying motivational processes to engage in substance use. Indeed, substance-related cues tend to grab attention (attentional bias; Bradley et al. 2003; Field and Cox 2008; Jones et al. 2003) and activate approach action tendencies (approach bias; Bradley et al. 2008; Field et al. 2008; Ostafin and Palfai 2006; Palfai and Ostafin 2003; Wiers et al. 2009) in substance-abusing individuals. These biases appear to be common for various substances of abuse including heroin, cocaine, alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis. Most models of addiction predict that the biases are strongly correlated with subjective craving and with quantitative substance use (Field et al. 2009; Stacy and Wiers 2010). Moreover, the relationship between subjective craving and cognitive biases is suggested to be bidirectional. However, studies investigating the relationship between craving and cognitive biases have reported contradictory findings. In a recent meta-analysis on the relation between the attentional bias and subjective craving, only a modest correlation of 0.19 was found (Field et al. 2009). Additional analyses revealed that the relationship was stronger when subjective craving was high. The authors concluded that attentional bias and craving are weakly related phenomena. The strength of the biases may also depend on other motivational mechanisms reflecting the current incentive value of substance use. This predicts moderating effects of substance availability, prior substance use, and satiation (satiation being defined as the satisfaction one feels with respect to a specific substance) on the expression of cognitive biases (Field et al. 2009; Watson et al. in press). Moreover, it has repeatedly been found that the association between motivational processes and measures of substance use is moderated by executive functions (Grenard et al. 2008; Houben and Wiers 2009; Peeters et al. 2012; Thush et al. 2008). The influence of motivational processes on substance use has been found to be stronger in individuals with relatively poor inhibitory control over (inappropriate) behavior (Wiers et al. 2010b). These findings suggest that the biases are strongest when substance (ab)users have the intention and the opportunity to use. In turn, satiety may (temporarily) decrease the bias through a temporary devaluation of the incentive value of substance use. Interestingly, a study investigating the attentional bias in alcohol users in the pub showed that attentional bias decreased with increasing alcohol consumption in participants who had been binge drinking (Schoenmakers and Wiers 2010). These findings support the idea that the attentional bias is strongest before the initiation of substance use, but weakens as satiation increases and motivation to use decreases. The Netherlands provides a unique cultural setting to investigate cannabis use. Cannabis outlets (Dutch coffee shops ) offer a real-life setting in which individuals can buy and use cannabis. In a laboratory study, we recently showed that the cannabis approach bias predicted cannabis use in heavy cannabis users (Cousijn et al. 2011). However, the relationship between approach bias and motivational processes (i.e., intention to use, satiation, and craving) and executive functioning remains unclear. Moreover, how these processes generalize to real-life behavior remains to be tested. The aim of this study therefore was to investigate the cannabis approach bias before and after cannabis use in real-life setting (Amsterdam coffee shops) and to assess the relationship between approach bias, craving, satiation, history of cannabis use, and response inhibition. In various Amsterdam coffee shops, we measured the approach bias, craving, satiation, prior cannabis use, and response inhibition in heavy cannabis users with the intention to use cannabis (pre-cannabis group) and heavy cannabis users after cannabis intake (post-cannabis group). Since most cannabis users also smoked cigarettes, we also measured the smoking approach bias to control potential effects of nicotine use. We hypothesized that satiation, craving, and prior cannabis use would be significant predictors of the cannabis approach bias in both the pre-cannabis group and the post-cannabis group. Moreover, the approach bias would be lower in the postcannabis group since these participants would be more satiated and experience less craving. Finally, response inhibition was expected to act as a moderator between the approach bias and cannabis use: A stronger correlation was expected between the bias and prior cannabis use in cannabis users with low executive functioning. Materials and methods Participants Participants were 82 male and 8 female cannabis users, aged 18 59, who were recruited and tested in five different cannabis outlets (coffee shops) in Amsterdam by a single experimenter. Roughly half of the approached coffee shop costumers declined participation. Potential participants had to use cannabis at least once per week and were excluded if they were under the influence of alcohol (Field et al. 2005) or if they had ever been treated for a psychiatric disorder. This was verified with a short questionnaire. If all inclusion criteria were met, participants were allocated to the precannabis and post-cannabis groups based on when they last used cannabis; participants in the pre-cannabis group had

3 Psychopharmacology (2013) 229: not used cannabis on the day of the experiment, whereas participants in the post-cannabis group used it less than an hour ago. Additionally, participants in the pre-cannabis group had the intention to use cannabis at least within 2 h after the test session, which was verified with a short questionnaire. The experimenter was consequently not blinded to participants drug state. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Psychology Faculty of The University of Amsterdam, and all participants signed an informed consent before participation. Questionnaires The Cannabis Use Disorder Identification Test Revised (CUDIT-R; Adamson et al. 2010) was used to assess cannabis use and related problems during the past 6 months. The Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test (AUDIT; Saunders et al. 1993) was used to assess alcohol use and related problems during the past 6 months. Nicotine dependence was measured with the Fagerström Test of Nicotine Dependence (FTND; Heatherton et al. 1991). Subjective craving was measured with the Marijuana Craving Questionnaire Short Form (MCQ-SF). A visual analogue scale (VAS) was used to assess satiation. Participants rated the intensity on a 100 unit line from not at all to extremely. There is support for the reliability and validity of a VAS scale for food satiation (Geiselman et al. 1998; Flint et al. 2000). Additionally, participants were asked about their intention to use cannabis within 2 h after the experiment. Furthermore, demographics (age, sex, and level of education) and a detailed history of cannabis and tobacco use were obtained, including variables on daily use, weekly use, duration of use (years), and last cannabis use (hours). Classical Stroop The validated paper version of the classical Stroop task was used. The Stroop is hypothesized to be a good indicator of response inhibition (Krompinger and Simons 2011). The task consists of three subtasks. In the first subtask, participants had to read color words (red, blue, yellow, and green) printed in black ink placed in random order on a gray sheet of paper. In the second subtask, participants were instructed to name the colors of solid color patches. The last subtask consisted of naming the ink color of color words, the ink color being incongruent to the color word. Response time is, on average, longest in the last subtask. The difference between congruent and incongruent conditions is used as a measure of response inhibition. Approach Avoidance Task The AAT (Cousijn et al. 2011; Wiers et al. 2009, 2010a) was used to measure approach bias toward cannabis- and tobaccorelated stimuli. It consisted of one cannabis and one tobacco block, of which the order was counterbalanced over participants. The tobacco block was included to control potential confounding effects of tobacco use as most heavy cannabis users also smoke tobacco and the resemblance between cannabis and tobacco cigarettes may also activate approach actions toward tobacco in tobacco users (Cousijn et al. 2011). In the cannabis block, participants were presented with 12 cannabis-related images (pictures of someone using cannabis and pictures of objects associated with cannabis use) and 12 visually matched neutral images (i.e., someone holding a pen). The tobacco block was similar to the cannabis block, except that tobacco-related images were used instead of cannabisrelated images. In addition, a separate set of visually matched neutral images was used. The images were rotated 3 to the left or right, and participants had to pull (approach) or push (avoid) a joystick in response to the rotation direction, as fast as possible. Half of the participants had to push images rotated to the left and pull images rotated to the right, while the other half of the participants were given opposite instructions. Pulling gradually increased image size, whereas pushing decreased it (zooming feature). Each image was presented twice in pull and twice in push format, resulting in 98 trials per block. Procedure The experiment was conducted inside the coffee shop on weekdays between 11:00 and 20:00 immediately following recruitment. After signing informed consent, participants completed questionnaires on demographic variables, history of cannabis and tobacco use, craving, satiation, and intention to use cannabis within the next 2 h. Participants then performed the classical Stroop test and the AAT. The AAT was performed on a laptop. The tasks were followed by a second assessment of craving, satiation, and intention to use. Participants finally completed questionnaires on substance use and related problems. Total session time was around 30 min, and reimbursement was 5 euro. Data preparation and statistical analysis Regarding the AAT, median scores were used to summarize participants performance on the AAT as medians are less sensitive to outliers than mean scores (Wiers et al. 2009; Palfai 2006). Reaction times below 200 ms, above 2,000 ms, and deviating more than 3 standard deviations from the individual mean were removed per participant. Error trials were also removed. For each participant, a cannabis bias score, a tobacco bias score, and a neutral bias score were calculated by subtracting the median approach RT from the median avoid RT, for the corresponding images (e.g., separately for the cannabis images, tobacco images,

4 170 Psychopharmacology (2013) 229: and all neutral images combined). This way, a positive score indicated faster approach compared to avoidance (approach bias). Reliability of the AAT was investigated by calculating Cronbach s alpha for each bias score, with individual bias scores per image. Internal reliability of the cannabis bias (12 items, Cronbach s α=0.42), tobacco bias (12 items, Cronbach s α=0.55), and neutral bias (24 items, Cronbach s α=0.53) was fairly poor (Cortina 1993). For the classical Stroop task, interference scores were used as an indication of response inhibition. These scores were calculated by subtracting the average time needed to complete the first two congruent subtasks from the time needed to complete the third incongruent subtask (Interference = Stroop III [(Stroop I + Stroop II) / 2] (Van der Elst et al. 2006). Errors in the Stroop tasks were most often self-corrected by participants. This requires extra time, and therefore, the scores on the Stroop tasks were indirectly corrected for errors. Mixed ANOVAs were used to examine differences between and within groups in the AAT biases. Post hoc one-sample t-tests were used to test whether the AAT biases deviated significantly from zero. 1 Regression analyses were performed to examine the influence of satiation and craving on the approach bias, as well as to investigate the potential moderating effects of response inhibition between the approach bias, subjective craving, and prior cannabis use. The speed of push and pull responses with the joystick vary within and between participants. Individual differences in approach and avoidance tendencies are reflected in the neutral bias score (push neutral pull neutral), but affect the cannabis bias and smoking bias as well. To control general biases in approach and avoid actiontendencies, regression analyses were conducted with neutral bias score as the covariate. Partial correlations were also conducted with neutral bias score as covariate based on the same rationale. Results Sample characteristics Three participants in the pre-cannabis group were excluded (all male) since they indicated to have no intention to use cannabis within 2 h after the test session. Of the 87 remaining participants, 77 % also smoked tobacco cigarettes. The pre- and postgroups did not differ significantly in gender, education level, Stroop interference score, cannabis use and problems, tobacco use and problems, and alcohol use and problems (see Table 1). However, participants in the post-cannabis group were significantly older than participants in the pre-cannabis group (t 85 =2.22, P=0.03). 1 Non-parametric tests were used for variables that were not normally distributed (Mann Whitney U test or Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Self-reported satiation and craving A mixed-design ANOVA, comparing the two groups across the two within-participant measures of satiation (start session and end session), was performed to examine differences in satiation (Table 2). There was a main effect of group, F 1,85 =17.94, P<0.001, η 2 =0.17. As expected, satiation was higher in the post-cannabis compared to the precannabis group. No main effect was found for time, F 1,85 = 1.00, P=0.320, indicating that satiation did not change during the 30-min test session. There was also no interaction effect between group and time, F 1,85 =1.60, P= A mixed-design ANOVA, comparing the two groups across the two measures of subjective craving (MCQ) was performed to examine differences in craving (Table 2). There was a main effect of group, F 1,85 =7.63, P=0.007, η 2 =0.08. Subjective craving was higher in the pre-cannabis group than in the post-cannabis group. No main effect was found for time, F 1,85 =0.065, P=0.800, indicating that craving did not change during the test session. There was also no interaction effect between group and time, F 1,85 =0.19, P= Main analyses 2 Group comparison AAT bias scores Three participants made more than 25 % errors on the AAT (all male) and were therefore excluded from analyses. The AAT biases were not fully normally distributed. 3 In response to this issue of non-normality, the variables were transformed into ranks, and to test for differences in the AAT biases between groups across the three biases (e.g., cannabis, tobacco, and neutral), a mixed-design ANOVA was conducted with these ranked data. 4 In contrast to our hypothesis, there was no main effect of bias type, F 2,82 =0.001, P=0.999, indicating that the bias scores did not significantly differ, irrespectively, of group. However, there was a main effect of group, F 1,82 =12.15, 2 All main analyses have been conducted a second time with only male participants, a third time with the three participants who made more than 25 % errors on the AAT included, and a fourth time with age as a covariate. Results and interpretations did not differ when only male participants were included. There was no longer group difference in neutral bias when the three participants who made more than 25 % errors on the AAT were included (t 85 = 1.906, P=0.06) and when age was entered as a covariate (t 81 = 1.83, P=0.071). 3 Cannabis, skewness 0.539, SE 0.263, Z=2.09, kurtosis 0.611, SE 0.520, Z=1.18; tobacco, skewness 0.019, SE 0.263, Z=0.07, kurtosis 0.520, SE 0.520, Z=1.00; neutral skewness 0.911, SE 0.263, Z= 3.46, kurtosis 1.45, SE 0.52, Z= This mixed-design ANOVA was performed a second time with whether participants also smoked tobacco cigarettes as a between-subjects factor and a third time with age as a covariate. Results and interpretations did not differ from the mixed-design ANOVA discussed.

5 Psychopharmacology (2013) 229: Table 1 Sample characteristics Pre-cannabis group Post-cannabis group P-value SD standard deviation, CUDIT-R Cannabis Use Disorder Identification Test Revised, AUDIT Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test, FTND Fagerström Test of Nicotine Dependence *P<0.05; **P<0.01; ***P<0.001 % male (n) 92.9 (42) 88.9 (45) Age, mean (SD) (8.13) (10.18)* Also tobacco cigarettes smoking, n (%) 34 (81.0) 31 (68.9) Level of education (%low) (%average) (%high) 7.1; 66.7; ; 48.9; Daily tobacco use (number of cigarettes), mean (SD) 8.9 (6.43) 8.26 (5.52) Duration of tobacco use (years), mean (SD) 9.41 (8.64) (9.98) Weekly cannabis use (gram), mean (SD) 5.10 (4.83) 4.98 (3.05) Weekly cannabis use (days), mean (SD) 5.35 (1.52) 5.78 (1.85) Duration regular cannabis use (years), mean (SD) 7.54 (6.08) (9.17) Stroop interference, mean (SD) (14.55) (13.81) CUDIT-R score, mean (SD) (5.23) (6.43) AUDIT score, mean (SD) 8.60 (3.77) 7.91 (4.71) FTND score, mean (SD) 3.60 (2.28) 3.15 (2.36) Hours since last cannabis use, mean (SD) 20.5 (5.52) 0.20 (0.24)*** <0.001 P=0.001, η 2 =0.13. Post hoc independent t-tests (Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons) indicated that the groups differed significantly for each of the biases: cannabis, t 82 =2.64, P=0.01, d=0.58; tobacco, t 82 =3.02, P=0.003, d=0.66; neutral, t 82 =2.06, P=0.043, d=0.45. Subsequent one-sample t-tests (not corrected for multiple comparisons) showed that, in contrast to our hypothesis, the cannabis bias, tobacco bias, and neutral bias were significantly greater than zero in the postcannabis group (Fig. 1) (cannabis, mean=29.51, SD=89.21, t 43 =2.19, P=0.034, d=0.67; tobacco, mean=38.86, SD= 79.49, t 43 =3.24, P=0.002, d=0.99; neutral, mean=25.205, SD=65.68, t 43 =2.55, P=0.015, d=0.78). The biases in the pre-cannabis group did not deviate from zero. No interaction effect between group and biases was found, F 2,82 =0.434, P= To examine whether the between-group differences in the biases were due to greater variance in the post-cannabis group [cannabis intoxication may increase RTs (Lenné et al. 2010)], independent t-tests were performed, comparing the mean RTs of cannabis trials, tobacco trials, and neutral trials between groups (Table 3). None of these tests were significant. In addition, Box s test of equality of covariance matrices and Levene s test of equality of error variances contained no significant differences, which provided support for the assumption that the variances in bias scores did not differ between groups. Independent t-tests revealed that the overall approach bias in the post-cannabis group was due to a slower avoidance: Participants in the post-cannabis group had longer RTs for push trials compared to participants in the pre-cannabis group (Mann Whitney U, Z=2.44, P=0.015). Mean RTs for the pull trials did not differ between the post-cannabis and pre-cannabis groups (Mann Whitney U, Z=1.69, P=0.091; see Table 3). Relationship between cannabis bias, satiation, craving, and prior cannabis use To assess the relationship between the cannabis bias score and satiation, subjective craving, and prior cannabis use and Table 2 Satiation, craving, and intention to use cannabis Start session End session SD standard deviation, MCQ Marijuana Craving Questionnaire Satiation, mean (SD) Pre-cannabis group (26.86) (27.17) Post-cannabis group (26.39) (24.27) P-value <0.001 <0.001 Craving (MCQ), mean (SD) Pre-cannabis group (9.67) (12.93) Post-cannabis group (13.98) (14.76) P-value Intention to smoke (%yes) (%no) Pre-cannabis group 95.2; 0.0; ; 0.0; 4.8 (%maybe) Post-cannabis group 80.0; 15.6; ; 13.3; 8.9 P-value

6 172 Psychopharmacology (2013) 229: variable entered in step 1. Preliminary analyses indicated no violation of the assumption of normality, linearity, multicollinearity, and homoscedasticity (maximum Cook s distance=0.20, maximum standardized residual=2.68). Total variance explained by the final model was 37 % (F 6,75 =7.27, P<0.001; see Table 4 model b). The tobacco bias did not significantly predict the cannabis bias. However, craving was no longer a significant predictor of the cannabis bias (Table 4). 5 Moderating effect of executive functioning on relationship between cannabis bias and cannabis use Fig. 1 AAT bias scores with standard error bars for cannabis, tobacco, and neutral images in the pre-cannabis group and the post-cannabis group. A positive score reflected an approach bias (faster RT on pull trials than push trials). Participants in the post-cannabis group have an approach bias toward all image types, and these scores were significantly higher than those in the pre-cannabis group related problems (indexed with the CUDIT-R), hierarchical multiple regression was conducted. All variables were zerocentered. For the satiation and craving measure, the average of each measure was used (start-session and end-session correlation satiation, r=0.826, P<0.001; craving, r=0.821, P<0.001; correlation satiation craving, r= 0.23, P=0.034). In order to control general approach and avoidance tendencies and direct effects of cannabis, group and the neutral bias were entered in the first step. Additional variables in the second step were satiation, craving, and CUDIT-R scores. Preliminary analyses indicated no violation of the assumption of normality, linearity, multicollinearity, and homoscedasticity (maximum Cook s distance=0.17, maximum standardized residual=2.69). Total variance explained by the final model was 36 % (F 5,76 =8.70, P<0.001; see Table 4 model a). The control variables in step 1 explained 29 % of the variance in cannabis bias (F change 2,79 =16.25, P<0.001), with neutral bias being a significant predictor (P<0.001). In step 2, satiation, craving, and CUDIT-R explained an additional 7.3 % of the variance (F change 3,76 =2.89, P=0.041), with craving being a significant predictor (P=0.048). Participants with higher levels of craving had a weaker cannabis approach bias. Influence of tobacco bias on relationship between cannabis bias, satiation, craving, and prior cannabis use Most cannabis users also smoked tobacco. Cannabis-related images may have activated action tendencies toward tobacco in smoking cannabis users. To control the possible influences of tobacco on the cannabis bias, the same regression analysis was performed, with tobacco bias included as an extra control To test the hypothesis that executive functioning acted as a moderator between approach bias and cannabis use and related problems, we performed multiple regression analyses on CUDIT-R scores. After correction for the neutral bias, we entered the cannabis bias, Stroop interference (an indication of executive functioning), and the interaction between the Stroop score and cannabis bias. These regression analyses were performed a second time with weekly cannabis use (gram) as the dependent variable. None of the predictors were significant in these analyses. In contrast to the hypotheses, executive functioning did not act as a moderator between approach bias and cannabis use and related problems. Exploratory correlations within groups For each group, the association between the cannabis bias, smoking bias, craving, satiation, and measures of tobacco use, cannabis use, and alcohol use and problems were explored separately (partial correlations corrected for neutral bias). In the pre-cannabis group, the cannabis bias correlated significantly with the AUDIT (R=0.39, P=0.011). In the post-cannabis group, the cannabis bias correlated significantly (negatively) with craving (R= 0.41, P=0.007). Discussion This study investigated the influence of satiation, subjective craving, and cannabis use on cannabis approach and avoidance action tendencies in heavy cannabis users inside Amsterdam coffee shops. Heavy cannabis users with the intention to use cannabis (pre-cannabis group) were compared to heavy cannabis users under the influence of cannabis (post-cannabis 5 The regression analyses as shown in Table 4 have been performed a second time with age and years of regular cannabis use additionally entered in the first step. The MCQ-SF now also significantly predicted the cannabis bias when the smoking bias was included in the model (beta= 0.227, p=0.034, R 2 change step 2b=0.074, p=0.041). Neither age nor duration of regular use predicted the cannabis bias.

7 Psychopharmacology (2013) 229: Table 3 AAT reaction time per group for each image type and response type Mean reaction time with standard error *P<0.05; **P<0.001 push pull comparison; # P<0.05 group comparison Push trials Pull trials Overall AAT cannabis Pre-cannabis group (27.26) (27.88) (26.98) Post-cannabis group (31.25) (28.59)* (29.31) AAT tobacco Pre-cannabis group (21.29) (22.35) (21.22) Post-cannabis group (29.82) (26.79)** (27.69) AAT neutral Pre-cannabis group (21.99) (24.88) (23.02) Post-cannabis group (27.40) (25.06)** (25.89) AAT overall Pre-cannabis group (22.74) (24.01) (22.99) Post-cannabis group (22.80)*,# (26.10) (27.19) group). While in accordance with our hypotheses, the postcannabis group reported higher satiation than the pre-cannabis group; unexpectedly, the post-cannabis group showed a stronger instead of a weaker approach bias for cannabis as well as for the other image categories (smoking and neutral) compared with the pre-cannabis group. Moreover, the cannabis approach bias was negatively associated with craving. We did not observe a significant relationship between the cannabis approach bias and satiation, prior cannabis use and related problems, and response inhibition. Experimental laboratory studies support an important role for the approach bias in the development and maintenance of addictive behaviors (Stacy and Wiers 2010; Watson et al. in press). However, the motivational mechanisms underlying the expression of approach bias are unclear. If the approach bias is a goal-directed response, its expression is expected to be modulated by the incentive value of cannabis use (Watson et al. in press). The incentive value of cannabis is thought to (gradually) increase over the course of cannabis use, whereas satiety may temporarily devalue the rewarding effects of cannabis use. We did not observe a relationship between the cannabis bias and satiation, and prior cannabis use and related problems. Moreover, stronger craving was related to a lower cannabis approach bias. Cannabis intoxication appears to be associated with a general approach bias. These findings suggest that strong intentions and motivations to use do not increase the approach bias, contradicting the goal-directed account of the approach bias. The post-cannabis group was slower on overall avoidance tendencies, while response inhibition (classical Stroop task) did not differ between the pre-cannabis and postcannabis groups. Moreover, average RTs and variance in RTs did not differ between groups. These results suggest that cannabis intoxication inhibits avoidance responses in general and that the general approach bias in the postcannabis groups is unlikely due to a general effect of cannabis on cognition. The literature on acute effects of cannabis in heavy cannabis users is sparse, reporting mixed findings about how it affects response inhibition and psychomotor responses (for review, see Crean et al. 2011). Discrepancies between studies may be caused by differences in intoxication level, with a higher THC dose leading to stronger impairments. Also, tolerance may decrease acute effects of cannabis intoxication on cognitive functioning in general. Interestingly, cannabis intoxication in heavy cannabis users potentially selectively improves sustained attention (Hart et al. 2001) and divided attention (Haney et al. 1999). It may therefore be that cannabis intoxication specifically Table 4 Hierarchical multiple regression analyses for variables predicting AAT cannabis bias Final model a R 2 =0.36***, adjusted R 2 =0.32. Final model b R 2 =0.37***, adjusted R 2 =0.32. SE standard error, MCQ-SF Marijuana Craving Questionnaire Short Form, CUDIT-R Cannabis Use Disorder Identification Test Revised *P<0.05; **P<0.01; ***P<0.001 Model a Model b B SE B β B SE B β Step 1a change R 2 : 0.291*** Step 1b change R 2 : 0.308*** Pre-/post-cannabis group AAT neutral bias *** *** AAT tobacco bias Step 2a change R 2 : 0.073* Step 2b change R 2 : 0.06 Pre-/post-cannabis group AAT neutral bias *** *** AAT tobacco bias Satiation MCQ-SF * CUDIT-R

8 174 Psychopharmacology (2013) 229: decreases the ability to disengage attention and thereby avoidance, causing the general slowing of avoidance in the post-cannabis group without affecting response inhibition. Importantly, these inferences are speculative, and we cannot exclude potential confounding (sub)acute effects of cannabis on cognitive functioning with a between-subject design. In line with our expectations, satiation was higher and craving was lower in the post-cannabis group, whereas the approach bias was stronger in the post-group regardless of satiation. These findings contradict earlier experimental laboratory studies investigating the effect of alcohol consumption on the alcohol approach bias in heavy drinkers (Christiansen et al. 2012). Findings from these studies suggest that anticipation of alcohol increases an alcohol approach bias but that the pharmacological effects may not contribute to this. Therefore, inhibition of avoidance in the post-cannabis group may likely be a specific pharmacological effect of cannabis, rather than a phenomenon general to heavy substance use. Alternatively, it could be that the level of cannabis intoxication in the current sample was relatively low, acting as a prime to use even more (average satiation in the post-cannabis group was 67 %, and 79 % indicated to still have the intention to use more cannabis after the test session). This view is in line with attentional bias studies in heavy alcohol users, which showed an increase of the bias at low doses but a decrease at moderate and high doses (Schoenmakers et al. 2008; Schoenmakers and Wiers 2010; Dukaand Townshend2004). However, given the observed increase in satiation and decrease in craving in the post-cannabis group, we consider this alternative hypothesis unlikely. Since experimental laboratory studies support an important role of cognitive biases in the development and maintenance of addictive behaviors, it is important to know if these processes generalize to a real-life setting in which individuals have the opportunity to use. A real-life setting may also contain contextual factors (social elements and environmental cues) that could increase the biases and craving (McKay and Schare 1999). In contrast to our laboratory AAT study in heavy cannabis users (Cousijn et al. 2011), we did not observe a cannabis approach bias in the pre-cannabis group in the coffee shop. Thereby, this study cannot validate the role of the approach bias in real life. However, the lack of an approach bias for cannabis in the pre-cannabis group may be explained by the differences in sample characteristics between studies. The heavy cannabis users included in the present study were long-term users with slightly higher levels of use-related problems. It could be that the approach bias only plays a role in earlier stages of cannabis use, predicting onset and repeated cannabis use rather than chronic cannabis use and dependence. This appears to disagree with the incentive sensitization theory of addiction (Robinson and Berridge 1993) and seems more in line with theories where incentive sensitization is mainly important during early escalation of drug use and less with subsequent compulsive drug use (Di Chiara 2000; Everitt and Robbins 2005). Alternatively, the lack of an approach bias may be explained by the relatively poor reliability of the AAT and the high level of distraction inside the coffee shop. To test this hypothesis, associations between approach bias and cannabis use need to be assessed in dependent, heavy, and sporadic cannabis users compared to non-using controls inside the coffee shop. Subjective craving is also thought to be an important motivational process contributing to continued substance use and relapse (Franken 2003). A bidirectional positive relationship could be expected between craving and other cognitive biases: Craving may direct attention toward substances of abuse and activate approach action tendencies, which, in turn, may lead to a further increase of craving (Field et al. 2009). In contrast to our hypothesis but in line with our previous study (Cousijn et al. 2011), the approach bias was negatively associated with craving (MCQ-SF; Heisman et al. 2009). These findings are difficult to explain; however, a potential explanation may be that the MCQ craving scores (at least in part) reflect the inability to control the urge to use cannabis rather than the urge itself, as is suggested by our recent neuroimaging study (Cousijn et al. 2012). Important to note, the relationship between cannabis approach bias and craving was not significant when we controlled the tobacco approach bias. This suggests that craving as measured with the MCQ may be related to a general smoking approach bias rather than a cannabis-specific approach bias. This relationship between cannabis intoxication and a general inhibition of avoidance may have clinical implications. Individuals who are trying to abstain from substance use other than cannabis or tobacco could be advised to give up cannabis as well since the general approach tendencies resulting (or lack of avoidance) from cannabis intoxication may increase chances to relapse. This may, in part, be caused by cross sensitization. For example, it has been shown that an alcohol prime increases the attention bias for cocaine (Montgomery et al. 2010). These inferences are speculative, and further research is needed to verify if cannabis intoxication increases the approach bias for other substances of abuse than cannabis and tobacco. Finally, some limitations must be considered. First, we applied a between- rather than a within-subjects design which may have confounded the findings. This quasiexperimental study should be repeated in a more controlled fashion with a counterbalanced within-subject design; however, such an approach is more difficult to implement as it requires a two-time participation of the heavy cannabis users. Second, this study was conducted at a single location (the coffee shop). To make inferences about the influence of a real-life setting on approach bias and response inhibition, one should compare the coffee shop setting to a laboratory

9 Psychopharmacology (2013) 229: setting. Third, we did not include an objective THC measure, which makes it impossible to control (sub)acute effects of THC intoxication. Moreover, we only included a self-report of satiation, not a self-report of prior cannabis use. Unfortunately, prior cannabis use may have affected the results. The reliability of self-reports is questionable, especially in individuals with a substance use disorder (Goldstein et al. 2009). In addition, THC levels in cannabis cigarettes greatly vary between participants depending on type and amount of cannabis inside the cigarette. Including an absolute measure of cannabis intoxication and/or controlling the THC intake is therefore recommended in future studies. Fourth, the age difference between the pre-cannabis and post-cannabis groups could be confounded. Including age as a covariate did not change the overall results; however, age can be expected to correlate positively with duration of heavy use and cumulative lifetime cannabis use. Unfortunately, we did not assess cumulative lifetime cannabis use, which may have differed between groups given the age difference between groups. Fifth, we did not measure history of any other drug than alcohol, cannabis, and tobacco, which may have confounded our findings. Finally, only the CUDIT-R was used as a measure of cannabisuse-related problems. Cognitive biases like the approach bias may be higher in dependent versus non-dependent cannabis users. There was no significant relationship between the CUDIT-R and the cannabis approach bias, but a clinical diagnosis of cannabis dependence was not assessed. Including a clinical assessment of cannabis use disorder is therefore recommended in future studies. In conclusion, cannabis intoxication in a real-life setting inhibited general avoidance. Expression of the cannabis approach bias appeared not to be modulated by satiation or response inhibition. Thereby, this field study increased our understanding and, at the same time, raised new issues regarding the motivational mechanisms underlying an approach bias for cannabis and other psychoactive substances. Acknowledgments This research was supported by the National Science Foundation (NWO) Vici grant awarded to R.W. Wiers. The authors report no conflict of interest. References Adamson SJ, Kay-Lambkin FJ, Baker AL, Lewin TJ, Thornton L, Kelly BJ et al (2010) An improved brief measure of cannabis misuse: the Cannabis Use Disorders Identification Test Revised (CUDIT-R). Drug Alcohol Depend 110: Bechara A (2005) Decision making, impulsive control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci 8: Bradley BP, Mogg K, Wright T, Field M (2003) Attentional bias in drug dependence: vigilance for cigarette-related cues in smokers. Psychol Addict Behav 17:66 72 Bradley BP, Field M, Healy H, Mogg K (2008) Do the affective properties of smoking-related cues influence attentional and approach biases in cigarette smokers? J Psychoparmacol 22: Christiansen P, Rose AK, Cole JC, Field M (2012) A comparison of the anticipated and pharmacological effects of alcohol on cognitive bias executive function, craving, and ad-lib drinking. J Psychopharmacol. doi: / Cortina JM (1993) What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. J Appl Psychol 78: Cousijn J, Goudriaan AE, Wiers RW (2011) Reaching out towards cannabis: approach-bias in heavy cannabis users predicts changes in cannabis use. Addiction 106: Cousijn J, Goudriaan AE, Ridderinkhof KR, Veltman DJ, Van den Brink W, Wiers RW (2012) Approach-bias predicts the development of cannabis problem severity: results from a prospective fmri study. PLoS One 7:e42394 Crean RD, Crane NA, Mason BJ (2011) An evidence based review of acute and long-term effects of cannabis use on executive cognitive functions. J Addict Med 5:1 8 Di Chiara G (2000) Role of dopamine in the behavioural actions of nicotine related to addiction. Eur J Pharmacol 393: Duka T, Townshend JM (2004) The priming effect of alcohol pre-load on attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli. Psychopharmacol 176: Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (2005) Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci 8: Field M, Cox WM (2008) Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend 97:1 20 Field M, Mogg K, Bradley BP (2005) Alcohol increases cognitive biases for smoking cues in smokers. Psychopharmacol 180:63 72 Field M, Kiernan A, Eastwood B, Child R (2008) Rapid approach responses to alcohol cues in heavy drinkers. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 39: Field M, Munafo MR, Franken IHA (2009) A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychol Bull 135: Flint A, Raben A, Blundell JE, Astrup A (2000) Reproducibility, power and validity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensations in single test meal studies. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 24:38 48 Franken IHA (2003) Drug craving and addiction: integrating psychological and neuropsychopharmacological approaches. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 27: Geiselman PJ, Anderson AM, Dowdy ML, West DB, Redmann SM, Smith SR (1998) Reliability and validity of a macronutrient selfselection paradigm and a food preference questionnaire. Physiol Behav 63: Goldstein RZ, Craig AD, Bechara A, Garavan H, Childress AR, Paulus MP, Volkow ND (2009) The neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction. Trends Cogn Sci 13: Grenard JL, Ames SL, Wiers RW, Thush C, Sussman S, Stacy AW (2008) Working memory capacity moderates the predictive effects of drug-related associations on substance use. Psychol Addict Behav 22: Haney M, Ward AS, Comer SD, Foltin RW, Fischman MW (1999) Abstinence symptoms following smoked marijuana in humans. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 141: Hart CL, van Gorp W, Haney M, Foltin RW, Fischman MW (2001) Effects of acute smoked marijuana on complex cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology 25: Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerstrom KO (1991) The Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict 86:

10 176 Psychopharmacology (2013) 229: Heisman SJ, Evans RJ, Singleton EG, Levin KH, Copersion ML, Gorelick DA (2009) Reliability and validity of a short form of the Marijuana Craving Questionnaire. Drug Alcohol Depend 102:35 50 Houben K, Wiers RW (2009) Response inhibition moderates the relationship between implicit associations and drinking behavior. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 33: Houben K, Wiers RW, Jansen A (2011) Getting a grip on drinking behavior: training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychol Sci 22:1 8 Jones BT, Jones BC, Smith H, Copley N (2003) A flicker paradigm for inducing change blindness reveals alcohol and cannabis information processing biases in social users. Addiction 98: Krompinger JW, Simons RF (2011) Cognitive inefficiency in depressive undergraduates: Stroop processing and ERP s. Biol Psychol 86: Lenné MG, Dietze PM, Triggs TJ, Walsmley S, Murphy B, Redman JR (2010) The effects of cannabis and alcohol on simulated arterial driving: influences of driving experience and task demand. Accid Anal Prev 42: McKay D, Schare ML (1999) The effects of alcohol and alcohol expectancies on subjective reports and physiological reactivity: a meta-analysis. Addict Behav 24: Montgomery C, Field M, Atkinson AM, Cole JC, Goudie AJ, Sumnall HR (2010) Effects of alcohol preload on attentional bias towards cocaine-related cues. Psychopharmacol 210: Ostafin BS, Palfai TP (2006) Compelled to consume: the Implicit Association Test and automatic alcohol motivation. Psychol Addict Behav 20: Palfai TP (2006) Activating action tendencies: the influence of action priming on alcohol consumption among male hazardous drinkers. J Stud Alcohol 67: Palfai TP, Ostafin BD (2003) Alcohol-related motivational tendencies in hazardous drinkers: assessing implicit response tendencies using the modified IAT. Behav Res Ther 41: Peeters M, Wiers RW, Monshouwer K, Janssen T, Vollebergh WAM (2012) Automatic processes in at-risk adolescent: the role of alcohol-approach tendencies and response inhibition in drinking behavior. Addiction 107: Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev 18: Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M (1993) Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption II. Addiction 88: Schoenmakers TM, Wiers RW (2010) Craving and attentional bias respond differently to alcohol priming: a field study in the pub. Eur Addict Res 16:9 16 Schoenmakers TM, Wiers RW, Field M (2008) Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers. Psychopharmacol 197: Stacy AW, Wiers RW (2010) Implicit cognition and addiction: a tool for explaining paradoxal behaviour. Ann Rev Clin Psychol 6: Thush C, Wiers RW, Ames SL, Grenard JL, Sussman S, Stacy AW (2008) Interactions between implicit and explicit cognition and working memory capacity in the prediction of alcohol use in at-risk adolescents. Drug Alcohol Depend 94: Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MPJ, Van Breukelen GJP, Jolles J (2006) The Stroop color word test: influence of age, sex and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment 13:62 79 Watson P, De Wit S, Hommel B, Wiers RW (in press) Motivational mechanisms and outcome expectancies underlying the approach bias towards addictive substances. Front Cogn Wiers RW, Rinck M, Dictus M, van den Wildenberg E (2009) Relatively strong automatic appetitive action-tendencies in male carriers of the OPRM1G-allele. Genes Brain Behav 8: Wiers RW, Rinck M, Kordts R, Houben K, Strack F (2010a) Retraining automatic action-tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. Addiction 105: Wiers RW, Ames SL, Hofmann W, Krank M, Stacy AW (2010b) Impulsivity, impulsive and reflective processes and the development of alcohol use and misuse in adolescence and young adults. Front Psychol 2010:1 12

Attentional bias for cannabis and nicotine related words in cannabis users

Attentional bias for cannabis and nicotine related words in cannabis users Attentional bias for cannabis and nicotine related words in cannabis users Word count abstract: 263 Word count text: 8893 Tables and Figures: 10 Pages: 27 Björn van Vlokhoven b.j.h.a.vanvlokhoven@uu.students.nl

More information

Prevention in Addiction: Using Serious Games to (re)train Cognition in Dutch Adolescents

Prevention in Addiction: Using Serious Games to (re)train Cognition in Dutch Adolescents Prevention in Addiction: Using Serious Games to (re)train Cognition in Dutch Adolescents Wouter J. Boendermaker ( ), Pier J.M. Prins, and Reinout W. Wiers Developmental Psychology, University of Amsterdam,

More information

Pump up the volume! Can transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) enhance Cognitive Bias Modification in Addiction?

Pump up the volume! Can transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) enhance Cognitive Bias Modification in Addiction? Pump up the volume! Can transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) enhance Cognitive Bias Modification in Addiction? Reinout W. Wiers Thomas E. Gladwin Tess den Uijl Addiction Development and Psychopathology

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Implicit associations and explicit expectancies toward cannabis in heavy cannabis users and controls Beraha Menahem, E.M.; Cousijn, J.; Hermanides, E.; Goudriaan,

More information

Evaluating the interactive effects of responsible drinking messages and attentional bias on actual drinking behaviours

Evaluating the interactive effects of responsible drinking messages and attentional bias on actual drinking behaviours Evaluating the interactive effects of responsible drinking messages and attentional bias on actual drinking behaviours February 2017 Key findings Responsible drinking posters do not attract significant

More information

Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers

Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers DOI 10.1007/s00213-007-1023-5 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers Tim Schoenmakers & Reinout W. Wiers & Matt Field Received: 17 August

More information

Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up

Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 3060 3064 Short communication Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up Michael Ussher a,, Robert West

More information

Cannabis Use Disorders: Using Evidenced Based Interventions to Engage Students in Reducing Harmful Cannabis Use or Enter Recovery

Cannabis Use Disorders: Using Evidenced Based Interventions to Engage Students in Reducing Harmful Cannabis Use or Enter Recovery Cannabis Use Disorders: Using Evidenced Based Interventions to Engage Students in Reducing Harmful Cannabis Use or Enter Recovery Lisa Laitman MSEd, LCADC Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey National

More information

Retraining Automatic Action Tendencies Changes Alcoholic Patients Approach Bias for. Alcohol and Improves Treatment Outcome

Retraining Automatic Action Tendencies Changes Alcoholic Patients Approach Bias for. Alcohol and Improves Treatment Outcome Research Article Running Head: Retraining Alcoholic Patients Approach Bias Retraining Automatic Action Tendencies Changes Alcoholic Patients Approach Bias for Alcohol and Improves Treatment Outcome Reinout

More information

Katrijn Houben, Remco C. Havermans, Chantal Nederkoorn & Anita Jansen. Alcohol, go/no-go task, implicit attitudes, response inhibition.

Katrijn Houben, Remco C. Havermans, Chantal Nederkoorn & Anita Jansen. Alcohol, go/no-go task, implicit attitudes, response inhibition. bs_bs_banner RESEARCH REPORT doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03827.x Beer à no-go: learning to stop responding to alcohol cues reduces alcohol intake via reduced affective associations rather than increased

More information

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers.

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers. Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers. 1 This work is a result of the combined efforts of myself and my research advisor, Dr.

More information

On the Development of Implicit and Control Processes in Relation to Substance Use in Adolescence

On the Development of Implicit and Control Processes in Relation to Substance Use in Adolescence Curr Addict Rep (2015) 2:141 155 DOI 10.1007/s40429-015-0053-z ADOLESCENT SUBSTANCE ABUSE (TA CHUNG, SECTION EDITOR) On the Development of Implicit and Control Processes in Relation to Substance Use in

More information

Cognitive Biases in Alcohol and Marijuana Users

Cognitive Biases in Alcohol and Marijuana Users Central Washington University ScholarWorks@CWU All Master's Theses Master's Theses Summer 2017 Cognitive Biases in Alcohol and Marijuana Users Wyatt G. Frahm Central Washington University, frahmw@cwu.edu

More information

Are Alcohol Expectancies Associations? Comment on Moss and Albery (2009)

Are Alcohol Expectancies Associations? Comment on Moss and Albery (2009) Psychological Bulletin 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 136, No. 1, 12 16 0033-2909/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0017769 Are Alcohol Expectancies Associations? Comment on Moss and Albery (2009)

More information

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way Mortality and morbidity attributable to use of addictive substances in the United States. The Association of American Physicians from 1999 60 million tobacco smokers in the U.S. 14 million dependent on

More information

ALCOHOLISM: CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH Vol. 30, No. 2 February 2006

ALCOHOLISM: CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH Vol. 30, No. 2 February 2006 ALCOHOLISM: CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH Vol. 30, No. 2 February 2006 The Search for New Ways to Change Implicit Alcohol-Related Cognitions in Heavy Drinkers Reinout W. Wiers, W. Miles Cox, Matt

More information

Impulsivity, impulsive and reflective processes and the development of alcohol use and misuse in adolescents and young adults

Impulsivity, impulsive and reflective processes and the development of alcohol use and misuse in adolescents and young adults Hypothesis and Theory Article published: 30 September 2010 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00144 Impulsivity, impulsive and reflective processes and the development of alcohol use and misuse in adolescents and

More information

Implicit and explicit determinants of cannabis consumption The role of intoxication, attitude and implicit processes.

Implicit and explicit determinants of cannabis consumption The role of intoxication, attitude and implicit processes. UNIVERSITY OF TWENTE Implicit and explicit determinants of cannabis consumption The role of intoxication, attitude and implicit processes. Bachelorthese Hannah Holländer (s1188305) 1ste begeleider: Dr.

More information

COGNITIVE EFFECTS Avoidance of Alcohol-Related Stimuli Increases During the Early Stage of Abstinence in Alcohol-Dependent Patients

COGNITIVE EFFECTS Avoidance of Alcohol-Related Stimuli Increases During the Early Stage of Abstinence in Alcohol-Dependent Patients Alcohol & Alcoholism Vol. 44, No. 5, pp. 458 463, 2009 doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agp056 COGNITIVE EFFECTS Avoidance of Alcohol-Related Stimuli Increases During the Early Stage of Abstinence in Alcohol-Dependent

More information

Response Inhibition Moderates the Relationship Between Implicit Associations and Drinking Behavior

Response Inhibition Moderates the Relationship Between Implicit Associations and Drinking Behavior ism: Clinical and Experimental Research Vol. 33, No. 4 April 2009 Response Inhibition Moderates the Relationship Between Implicit Associations and Drinking Behavior Katrijn Houben and Reinout W. Wiers

More information

Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision

Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision Andy McEwen, PhD University College London & National Centre for Smoking Cessation and Training (NCSCT) Tuesday 29th January 2013 Cardiff Conflict

More information

Psychology Research Process

Psychology Research Process Psychology Research Process Logical Processes Induction Observation/Association/Using Correlation Trying to assess, through observation of a large group/sample, what is associated with what? Examples:

More information

The effectiveness of attentional bias modification for substance use disorder symptoms in adults: a systematic review

The effectiveness of attentional bias modification for substance use disorder symptoms in adults: a systematic review Heitmann et al. Systematic Reviews (2018) 7:160 https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-018-0822-6 RESEARCH Open Access The effectiveness of attentional bias modification for substance use disorder symptoms in

More information

Management of Tobacco Dependence. Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur

Management of Tobacco Dependence. Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur Management of Tobacco Dependence Dr. Lokesh Kumar Singh Associate Professor Department of Psychiatry AIIMS, Raipur Difficult to identify any other condition that presents such a mix of lethality, prevalence,

More information

The Effect of Self-Control on Attentional Bias for Alcohol Cues in Male Heavy Drinkers 1

The Effect of Self-Control on Attentional Bias for Alcohol Cues in Male Heavy Drinkers 1 The Effect of Self-Control on Attentional Bias for Alcohol Cues in Male Heavy Drinkers 1 HANNEKE A. TEUNISSEN 2 AND RENSKE SPIJKERMAN Behavioural Science Institute Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands

More information

Can a new form of inhibitory training reduce heavy drinking?

Can a new form of inhibitory training reduce heavy drinking? Can a new form of inhibitory training reduce heavy drinking? Janette L Smith Inhibitory deficits in addiction Inhibition: the ability to interrupt, delay, or withhold performance of an inappropriate response

More information

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen The following full text is a publisher's version. For additional information about this publication click this link. http://hdl.handle.net/2066/126061

More information

Author: Marvin D. Krank, Ph.D. Author s Affiliation: UBC Okangan. Second Author: Aarin P. Frigon Second Author s Affiliation: UBC Okanagan

Author: Marvin D. Krank, Ph.D. Author s Affiliation: UBC Okangan. Second Author: Aarin P. Frigon Second Author s Affiliation: UBC Okanagan Individual Presentation Title: Preventing Substance Use Transitions in Adolescents Using Feedback from Implicit Cognition Session Title: Youth Treatment Initiatives Thread & Session Number: Treatment Approaches

More information

Drug and Alcohol Dependence

Drug and Alcohol Dependence Drug and Alcohol Dependence 106 (2010) 204 211 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Drug and Alcohol Dependence journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/drugalcdep Seeing the forest through the trees:

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Attentional Bias and Disinhibition Toward Gaming Cues Are Related to Problem Gaming in Male Adolescents

Attentional Bias and Disinhibition Toward Gaming Cues Are Related to Problem Gaming in Male Adolescents Journal of Adolescent Health 50 (2012) 541 546 www.jahonline.org Original article Attentional Bias and Disinhibition Toward Gaming Cues Are Related to Problem Gaming in Male Adolescents Ruth J. van Holst,

More information

Negative affect words prime beer consumption in young drinkers

Negative affect words prime beer consumption in young drinkers Addictive Behaviors 31 (2006) 169 173 Short communication Negative affect words prime beer consumption in young drinkers Martin Zack a, *, Constantine X. Poulos a, Fofo Fragopoulos a, Tracy M. Woodford

More information

HIV and alcohol use: why is risk reduction in alcohol use important in HIV care?

HIV and alcohol use: why is risk reduction in alcohol use important in HIV care? HIV and alcohol use: why is risk reduction in alcohol use important in HIV care? Susanne Astrab Fogger, DNP, PMHNP-BC, CARN-AP, FAANP sfogger@uab.edu Objectives for today s session Define alcohol use disorder

More information

EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTIONS TO HELP PATIENTS QUIT TOBACCO

EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTIONS TO HELP PATIENTS QUIT TOBACCO EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTIONS TO HELP PATIENTS QUIT TOBACCO Lena Matthias Gray, MSA, CTTS-M University of Michigan MHealthy Tobacco Consultation Service Overview of Tobacco Use The World Health Organization

More information

Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions

Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions Pathological Gambling: Neurobiological research and its relevance for treatment and relapse prevention: Recent research and future questions Anneke. Goudriaan 1,2, Ph.D. Ruth van Holst 1,2, M.Sc. Dick

More information

Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings. Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005

Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings. Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005 Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005 Outline What are cravings? The role of cravings in addiction What

More information

Treatment Outcomes from the TDC: A Look at Smoking Cessation Among Patients with Co- Occurring Disorders

Treatment Outcomes from the TDC: A Look at Smoking Cessation Among Patients with Co- Occurring Disorders University of Kentucky From the SelectedWorks of Chizimuzo T.C. Okoli December, 2010 Treatment Outcomes from the TDC: A Look at Smoking Cessation Among Patients with Co- Occurring Disorders Chizimuzo T.C.

More information

Marijuana: Potency, Policy and Public Impact May 3, 2017

Marijuana: Potency, Policy and Public Impact May 3, 2017 Marijuana: Potency, Policy and Public Impact May 3, 2017 Marijuana and the Adolescent Brain Bonnie Nolan, PhD How Marijuana Works Cannabis THC - tetrahydrocannabinol one of the active ingredients in marijuana

More information

Understanding Addiction

Understanding Addiction Understanding Addiction How Addiction Hijacks the Brain Addiction involves craving for something intensely, loss of control over its use, and continuing involvement with it despite adverse consequences.

More information

Drug Addiction NROD66H3. (Friday 10:00-12:00 pm; AA 204) COURSE DESCRIPTION

Drug Addiction NROD66H3. (Friday 10:00-12:00 pm; AA 204) COURSE DESCRIPTION Drug Addiction NROD66H3 (Friday 10:00-12:00 pm; AA 204) Instructor: Suzanne Erb Office: Temporary location (Sept 2010 until further notice), SW-414B; Permanent location, SW-531 Office hours: Monday 2:30-4:30

More information

Data Analysis. Richard W. Foltin, Ph.D. Columbia University Department of Psychiatry New York State Psychiatric Institute

Data Analysis. Richard W. Foltin, Ph.D. Columbia University Department of Psychiatry New York State Psychiatric Institute Data Analysis Richard W. Foltin, Ph.D. Columbia University Department of Psychiatry New York State Psychiatric Institute Missing Data Missing data can be imputed if missingness is random Of course this

More information

Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence

Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence Blackwell Science, LtdOxford, UKADDAddiction1360-0443 2003 Society for the Study of Addiction to Alcohol and Other Drugs98Original ArticleBiases in eye movements in smokerskarin Mogg et al. RESEARCH REPORT

More information

Dual process models point towards an interaction between topdown control and bottom-up influences.

Dual process models point towards an interaction between topdown control and bottom-up influences. Dual process models point towards an interaction between topdown control and bottom-up influences. Research suggests obesity is linked to low self-control combined with high reward sensitivity. Childhood

More information

Implicit priming of conflicting motivational orientations in heavy drinkers

Implicit priming of conflicting motivational orientations in heavy drinkers Baker et al. BMC Psychology 2014, 2:28 RESEARCH ARTICLE Implicit priming of conflicting motivational orientations in heavy drinkers Samantha Baker 1,2, Joanne M Dickson 1 and Matt Field 1,3* Open Access

More information

Attitudes and Beliefs of Adolescent Experimental Smokers: A Smoking Prevention Perspective

Attitudes and Beliefs of Adolescent Experimental Smokers: A Smoking Prevention Perspective Attitudes and Beliefs of Adolescent Experimental Smokers: A Smoking Prevention Perspective By: Min Qi Wang, Eugene C. Fitzhugh, James M. Eddy, R. Carl Westerfield Wang, M.Q., Fitzhugh, E.C.*, Eddy, J.M.,

More information

by popular demand: Addiction II

by popular demand: Addiction II by popular demand: Addiction II PSY/NEU338: Animal learning and decision making: Psychological, computational and neural perspectives drug addiction huge and diverse field of research (many different drugs)

More information

Factors Influencing Smoking Behavior Among Adolescents

Factors Influencing Smoking Behavior Among Adolescents RESEARCH COMMUNICATION Factors Influencing Smoking Behavior Among Adolescents Urmi Sen 1, Arindam Basu 2 Abstract Objective To study the impact of tobacco advertisements and other social factors on the

More information

Snowball sampling by mail: application to a survey of smokers in the general population

Snowball sampling by mail: application to a survey of smokers in the general population International Epidemiological Association 2000 Printed in Great Britain International Journal of Epidemiology 2000;29:43 48 Snowball sampling by mail: application to a survey of smokers in the general

More information

Understanding Addiction and Its Impact on the Brain. SDSMA Webinar Matthew Stanley, DO

Understanding Addiction and Its Impact on the Brain. SDSMA Webinar Matthew Stanley, DO Understanding Addiction and Its Impact on the Brain SDSMA Webinar Matthew Stanley, DO Estimated Economic Cost to Society Due to Substance Abuse and Addiction: Illegal drugs: Alcohol: Tobacco: $181 billion/year

More information

Bertha K Madras, PhD Professor of Psychobiology Department of Psychiatry Harvard Medical School McLean Hospital

Bertha K Madras, PhD Professor of Psychobiology Department of Psychiatry Harvard Medical School McLean Hospital Bertha K Madras, PhD Professor of Psychobiology Department of Psychiatry Harvard Medical School McLean Hospital bmadras@partners.org Users The Brain Adolescent Marijuana Safety Marijuana and Opioids Protecting

More information

Web-based brief personalized feedback intervention in a Non- Treatment Seeking Population of Heavy Drinkers a Randomized Controlled Trial

Web-based brief personalized feedback intervention in a Non- Treatment Seeking Population of Heavy Drinkers a Randomized Controlled Trial Web-based brief personalized feedback intervention in a Non- Treatment Seeking Population of Heavy Drinkers a Randomized Controlled Trial 7th Conference of INEBRIA Friday 10th September 2010 Ph.d.-student

More information

Designing Psychology Experiments: Data Analysis and Presentation

Designing Psychology Experiments: Data Analysis and Presentation Data Analysis and Presentation Review of Chapter 4: Designing Experiments Develop Hypothesis (or Hypotheses) from Theory Independent Variable(s) and Dependent Variable(s) Operational Definitions of each

More information

The effect of acute negative affect on approach biases to alcohol cues in coping-motivated drinkers

The effect of acute negative affect on approach biases to alcohol cues in coping-motivated drinkers Syracuse University SURFACE Theses - ALL January 2017 The effect of acute negative affect on approach biases to alcohol cues in coping-motivated drinkers Katherine Anne Buckheit Syracuse University Follow

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Self-control conflict in the eating domain Becker, D. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Self-control conflict in the eating domain Becker, D. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Self-control conflict in the eating domain Becker, D. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Becker, D. (2017). Self-control conflict in the eating

More information

Understanding Addiction: Why Can t Those Affected Just Say No?

Understanding Addiction: Why Can t Those Affected Just Say No? Understanding Addiction: Why Can t Those Affected Just Say No? 1 The Stigma of Addiction There continues to be a stigma surrounding addiction even among health care workers. Consider the negative opinions

More information

The association between harmful alcohol use and Internet addiction among college students: Comparison of personality

The association between harmful alcohol use and Internet addiction among college students: Comparison of personality Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 2009; 63: 218 224 doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01943.x Regular Article The association between harmful alcohol use and Internet addiction among college students: Comparison

More information

Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed

Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed Manard et al. BMC Neuroscience 2014, 15:7 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Age-related decline in cognitive control: the role of fluid intelligence and processing speed Marine Manard 1,2, Delphine Carabin

More information

EDRS. trends. bulletin. Alcohol use disorders amongst a group of regular ecstasy users. Key findings. july Introduction.

EDRS. trends. bulletin. Alcohol use disorders amongst a group of regular ecstasy users. Key findings. july Introduction. ecstasy and related drug trends july 2011 bulletin Authors: Sheena Arora and Lucy Burns, National Drug and Alcohol Research Centre, University of New South Wales Suggested citation: Arora, S. & Burns,

More information

Drug Addiction NROD66H3. (Friday 10:00-12:00 pm; AA206) COURSE DESCRIPTION

Drug Addiction NROD66H3. (Friday 10:00-12:00 pm; AA206) COURSE DESCRIPTION Drug Addiction NROD66H3 (Friday 10:00-12:00 pm; AA206) Instructor: Suzanne Erb Office: SW-627A Office hours: Tues 1-3 pm E-mail: erb@utsc.utoronto.ca COURSE DESCRIPTION This course is designed to provide

More information

PERSPECTIVES ON DRUGS Characteristics of frequent and high-risk cannabis users

PERSPECTIVES ON DRUGS Characteristics of frequent and high-risk cannabis users European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction UPDATED 28. 5. 2013 PERSPECTIVES ON DRUGS Characteristics of frequent and high-risk cannabis users Cannabis is Europe s most commonly used illicit

More information

Poor impulse control and heightened attraction to alcohol-related imagery in repeat DUI offenders

Poor impulse control and heightened attraction to alcohol-related imagery in repeat DUI offenders Poor impulse control and heightened attraction to alcohol-related imagery in repeat DUI offenders Abstract Melissa A. Miller, M.S. and Mark T. Fillmore, Ph.D. Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky

More information

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT Why delaying the onset of alcohol and other drug use during adolescence is so important Aaron White, PhD Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research National Institute

More information

Chapter 7. Screening and Assessment

Chapter 7. Screening and Assessment Chapter 7 Screening and Assessment Screening And Assessment Starting the dialogue and begin relationship Each are sizing each other up Information gathering Listening to their story Asking the questions

More information

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC) Neural Computation Overview of Lecture Models of prefrontal cortex function Dr. Sam Gilbert Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience University College London E-mail: sam.gilbert@ucl.ac.uk Prefrontal cortex

More information

Is cannabis addictive? CANNABIS EVIDENCE BRIEF

Is cannabis addictive? CANNABIS EVIDENCE BRIEF Is cannabis addictive? CANNABIS EVIDENCE BRIEF BRIEFS AVAILABLE IN THIS SERIES: Is cannabis safe to use? Facts for youth aged 13 17 years. Is cannabis safe to use? Facts for young adults aged 18 25 years.

More information

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 170 191; doi:10.3390/bs3010170 Article OPEN ACCESS behavioral sciences ISSN 2076-328X www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living:

More information

Short-term Satiety of High Protein Formula on Obese Subjects: A Pilot Study

Short-term Satiety of High Protein Formula on Obese Subjects: A Pilot Study 2012 International Conference on Nutrition and Food Sciences IPCBEE vol. 39 (2012) (2012) IACSIT Press, Singapore Short-term Satiety of High Protein Formula on Obese Subjects: A Pilot Study Kamalita Pertiwi

More information

Cannabis for Medical Use: Body and Mind

Cannabis for Medical Use: Body and Mind Cannabis for Medical Use: Body and Mind Shaul Schreiber, MD Division of Psychiatry, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center; Tel Aviv University Sackler Faculty of Medicine; Sagol School of Neuroscience, Israel

More information

The National Methamphetamine Symposium

The National Methamphetamine Symposium The National Methamphetamine Symposium Making Research Work in Practice 12 May 2015 Arts Centre, Melbourne Neural and cognitive effects of methamphetamine and implications for treatment Rob Hester National

More information

Understanding Addiction and Dugs Of Abuse

Understanding Addiction and Dugs Of Abuse Understanding Addiction and Dugs Of Abuse Wilkie A. Wilson, Ph.D. DukeLEARN wawilson@duke.edu There is a lot of epidemiological evidence that addiction begins before brain maturity, and lately some biological

More information

VOLUME B. Elements of Psychological Treatment

VOLUME B. Elements of Psychological Treatment VOLUME B Elements of Psychological Treatment VOLUME B MODULE 1 Drug dependence and basic counselling skills Biology of drug dependence Principles of drug dependence treatment Basic counselling skills for

More information

Using the Balanced-Placebo Design for Parsing Nicotine Administration Effects

Using the Balanced-Placebo Design for Parsing Nicotine Administration Effects Using the Balanced-Placebo Design for Parsing Nicotine Administration Effects Thomas H. Brandon, Ph.D. & Amanda M. Palmer, M.A. Moffitt Cancer Center University of South Florida Tampa, Florida, USA Global

More information

PSY 3393 Experimental Projects Spring 2008

PSY 3393 Experimental Projects Spring 2008 PSY 3393 Experimental Projects Spring 2008 Dr. Peter Assmann Assignment: journal article report Find an article on a topic of special interest to you from any peer-reviewed journal in Psychology, Neuroscience

More information

The Neurobiology of Addiction

The Neurobiology of Addiction The Neurobiology of Addiction Jodi Gilman, Ph.D. Center for Addiction Medicine Massachusetts General Hospital Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry Harvard Medical School What is Addiction? commonly

More information

Disclosure 8/27/2014. Smoking and Substance Abuse in Individuals. ADHD and Drug Abuse

Disclosure 8/27/2014. Smoking and Substance Abuse in Individuals. ADHD and Drug Abuse Smoking and Substance Abuse in Individuals with Brooke Molina, Ph.D. University of Pittsburgh Jessica Rhodes, Ph.D. University of Pittsburgh Disclosure Brooke Molina and Jessica Rhodes have no actual or

More information

Behavioral Therapies for Methamphetamine Use

Behavioral Therapies for Methamphetamine Use Behavioral Therapies for Methamphetamine Use Will M. Aklin, PhD National Institute on Drug Abuse Division of Therapeutics and Medical Consequences June 27, 2017 1 Behavioral Therapy Development Program

More information

PSYC W2460: Drugs and Behavior Professor Carl Hart. Classes and Examinations. Percentage of grade. Grading Scale

PSYC W2460: Drugs and Behavior Professor Carl Hart. Classes and Examinations. Percentage of grade. Grading Scale PSYC W2460: Drugs and Behavior Professor Carl Hart Spring 2016 Syllabus This course examines the use of psychoactive drugs, using this topic to explore human behavior: from the interactions between chemicals

More information

The impact of asking about interest in free nicotine patches on smoker s stated. intent to change: Real effect or artefact of question ordering?

The impact of asking about interest in free nicotine patches on smoker s stated. intent to change: Real effect or artefact of question ordering? Impact of asking about interest in free nicotine patches 1 The impact of asking about interest in free nicotine patches on smoker s stated intent to change: Real effect or artefact of question ordering?

More information

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, 2017 The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain David Reyher, MSW, CAADC Behavioral Health Program Director Alvernia University Defining Addiction

More information

Cannabis and Psychosis: Is there an aspect of psychotic Illness that promotes cannabis use?

Cannabis and Psychosis: Is there an aspect of psychotic Illness that promotes cannabis use? Cannabis and Psychosis: Is there an aspect of psychotic Illness that promotes cannabis use? Clifford Cassidy, Mathieu Brodeur, Martin Lepage, Ashok Malla Comorbidity of substance use and mental illness

More information

Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging

Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging Understanding the Impact of Cigarette Smoking Using Brain Imaging Angelica Morales, PhD Postdoctoral Fellow Developmental Brain Imaging Lab Oregon Health & Science University No Financial Disclosures The

More information

We acknowledge the traditional custodians of the land on which we meet today and pay respect to Elders past, present and emerging.

We acknowledge the traditional custodians of the land on which we meet today and pay respect to Elders past, present and emerging. We acknowledge the traditional custodians of the land on which we meet today and pay respect to Elders past, present and emerging. We also extend that respect to other Aboriginal and/or Torres Strait Islanders

More information

STA 3024 Spring 2013 EXAM 3 Test Form Code A UF ID #

STA 3024 Spring 2013 EXAM 3 Test Form Code A UF ID # STA 3024 Spring 2013 Name EXAM 3 Test Form Code A UF ID # Instructions: This exam contains 34 Multiple Choice questions. Each question is worth 3 points, for a total of 102 points (there are TWO bonus

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. The Attraction of Sugar: An Association between Body Mass Index and Impaired Avoidance of Sweet Snacks

Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. The Attraction of Sugar: An Association between Body Mass Index and Impaired Avoidance of Sweet Snacks Journal of Experimental Psychopathology JEP Volume 8 (2017), Issue 1, 40-54 ISSN 2043-8087 / DOI:10.5127/jep.052415 The Attraction of Sugar: An Association between Body Mass Index and Impaired Avoidance

More information

TRIALS. Marilisa Boffo 1,2,3*, Thomas Pronk 2, Reinout W Wiers 2 and Stefania Mannarini 1,4

TRIALS. Marilisa Boffo 1,2,3*, Thomas Pronk 2, Reinout W Wiers 2 and Stefania Mannarini 1,4 Boffo et al. Trials (2015) 16:63 DOI 10.1186/s13063-015-0576-6 TRIALS STUDY PROTOCOL Open Access Combining cognitive bias modification training with motivational support in alcohol dependent outpatients:

More information

Cognitive conflict following appetitive versus negative cues and smoking cessation failure

Cognitive conflict following appetitive versus negative cues and smoking cessation failure Psychopharmacology (2011) 214:603 616 DOI 10.1007/s00213-010-2063-9 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Cognitive conflict following appetitive versus negative cues and smoking cessation failure Tanya R. Schlam & Sandra

More information

Addiction and Change 11/8/2017. Roots of Substance Use Disorders and Paths to Recovery: A Consumer-Centered, Collaborative & Comprehensive Care Model

Addiction and Change 11/8/2017. Roots of Substance Use Disorders and Paths to Recovery: A Consumer-Centered, Collaborative & Comprehensive Care Model Roots of Substance Use Disorders and Paths to Recovery: A Consumer-Centered, Collaborative & Comprehensive Care Model Carlo C. DiClemente, Ph.D. ABPP University of Maryland Baltimore County The work presented

More information

Learning to dislike alcohol: conditioning negative implicit attitudes toward alcohol and its effect on drinking behavior

Learning to dislike alcohol: conditioning negative implicit attitudes toward alcohol and its effect on drinking behavior Psychopharmacology (2010) 211:79 86 DOI 10.1007/s00213-010-1872-1 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Learning to dislike alcohol: conditioning negative implicit attitudes toward alcohol and its effect on drinking

More information

Cannabis Use Disorder: What Nurses Need to Know. Bari K Platter, MS, RN, PMHCNS-BC Clinical Nurse Specialist

Cannabis Use Disorder: What Nurses Need to Know. Bari K Platter, MS, RN, PMHCNS-BC Clinical Nurse Specialist Cannabis Use Disorder: What Nurses Need to Know Bari K Platter, MS, RN, PMHCNS-BC Clinical Nurse Specialist MARIJUANA POTENCY Average THC and CBD Levels in the U.S. 1960-2011 14 12 10 8 6 4 THC: Psychoactive

More information

ATTENTIONAL BIAS AND ALCOHOL ABUSE

ATTENTIONAL BIAS AND ALCOHOL ABUSE University of Kentucky UKnowledge Theses and Dissertations--Psychology Psychology 2012 ATTENTIONAL BIAS AND ALCOHOL ABUSE Jessica Jane Weafer University of Kentucky, jessicaweafer@gmail.com Click here

More information

Archived at the Flinders Academic Commons:

Archived at the Flinders Academic Commons: Archived at the Flinders Academic Commons: http://dspace.flinders.edu.au/dspace/ This is the authors version of an article published in Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. The original publication

More information

A comparison of the abuse liability and dependence potential of nicotine patch, gum, spray and inhaler

A comparison of the abuse liability and dependence potential of nicotine patch, gum, spray and inhaler Psychopharmacology (2000) 149:198 202 Springer-Verlag 2000 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Robert West Peter Hajek Jonathan Foulds Fredrik Nilsson Sylvia May Anna Meadows A comparison of the abuse liability and

More information

Understanding Alcohol And Other Drugs Of Abuse

Understanding Alcohol And Other Drugs Of Abuse Understanding Alcohol And Other Drugs Of Abuse Wilkie A. Wilson, Ph.D. DukeLEARN www.dukelearn.com Duke University Medical Center 1 We teach people to respect their hearts... Exercise Eat good food Reduce

More information

Dr. Kelly Bradley Final Exam Summer {2 points} Name

Dr. Kelly Bradley Final Exam Summer {2 points} Name {2 points} Name You MUST work alone no tutors; no help from classmates. Email me or see me with questions. You will receive a score of 0 if this rule is violated. This exam is being scored out of 00 points.

More information

PRIME Theory of Motivation and its application to addiction. Robert West University College London SSA Annual Conference, York November 2006

PRIME Theory of Motivation and its application to addiction. Robert West University College London SSA Annual Conference, York November 2006 PRIME Theory of Motivation and its application to addiction Robert West University College London SSA Annual Conference, York November 2006 1 Outline 1. Theories of motivation 2. Constructing a coherent

More information

Evaluating Smokers Reactions to Advertising for New Lower Nicotine Quest Cigarettes

Evaluating Smokers Reactions to Advertising for New Lower Nicotine Quest Cigarettes Psychology of Addictive Behaviors Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 20, No. 1, 80 84 0893-164X/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0893-164X.20.1.80 Evaluating Smokers Reactions to

More information

DOES COGNITIVE BIAS MODIFICATION DURING ALCOHOL WITHDRAWAL REDUCE CRAVING?

DOES COGNITIVE BIAS MODIFICATION DURING ALCOHOL WITHDRAWAL REDUCE CRAVING? DOES COGNITIVE BIAS MODIFICATION DURING ALCOHOL WITHDRAWAL REDUCE CRAVING? Dr Victoria Manning APSAD Conference Oct 30 th - 2 nd Nov 2016 Background Most of AOD-dependent clients relapse after treatment

More information

The Effect of Regular Naltrexone Dosing on Disordered Gamblers: An Examination of Neural Activation, Gambling Urges, and Gambling Behaviour

The Effect of Regular Naltrexone Dosing on Disordered Gamblers: An Examination of Neural Activation, Gambling Urges, and Gambling Behaviour The Effect of Regular Naltrexone Dosing on Disordered Gamblers: An Examination of Neural Activation, Gambling Urges, and Gambling Behaviour Darren R. Christensen, PhD Chair in Gambling, Alberta Gambling

More information

ADHD & Addictions -What We Know

ADHD & Addictions -What We Know ADHD & Addictions -What We Know Dr. David Teplin, Psy.D., C.Psych. 4th CADDAC Annual Conference, Toronto October 14, 2012 1 Introduction Adult ADHD commonly co-exists with several other disorders, including

More information