Appetite 52 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Appetite. journal homepage:

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Appetite 52 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Appetite. journal homepage:"

Transcription

1 Appetite 52 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Appetite journal homepage: Research report Individual differences in food-cue reactivity. The role of BMI and everyday portion-size selections Amanda Tetley a, *, Jeffrey Brunstrom b, Paula Griffiths a a Department of Human Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough, Leicestershire LE11 3TU, UK b Department of Experimental Psychology, 12a Priory Road, Bristol BS8 1TU, UK ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Article history: Received 15 August 2008 Received in revised form 12 January 2009 Accepted 17 February 2009 Keywords: Overweight Everyday portion size Dietary restraint Food-cue reactivity This study sought to explore the association between sensitivity to food cues (sight and smell of food) and (i) body weight (overweight or non-overweight), (ii) typical everyday portion sizes, and (iii) the DEBQ-restraint scale. One hundred and twenty female participants (aged between 18 and 30) were exposed to the sight and smell of pizza for three minutes. Both before and after this period, three measures of food-cue reactivity were taken (ratings of desire to eat, craving, and a measure of desired portion size of the cued food). Separate regression analyses were used to explore the associations between the change in measures of cue reactivity from pre- to post-cue exposure, and (i) an overweight/ non-overweight classification, (ii) a measure of participants everyday portion-size selection, and (iii) scores on the DEBQ-restraint scale. This analysis revealed that scores on the DEBQ-restraint scale were not significantly associated with changes in food-cue reactivity. However, individuals identified as overweight, and participants who reported consuming the largest everyday portion sizes, experienced a significantly greater change in their desired portion size of the cued food. The findings suggest that heightened food-cue reactivity might present an under-explored risk factor for overeating, and becoming overweight, and/or maintaining an overweight body shape. ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Introduction The effects of cue reactivity in drug-taking behaviour have been explored extensively. Numerous studies show that exposure to a cue such as the sight of a cigarette, or other drug paraphernalia, can elicit both a physiological response (e.g., Breteler, Schippers, De Jong, van der Stark, 2000; Rikard-Figuero & Zeichner, 1985), and a specific craving (e.g., Burton & Tiffany, 1997; Field & Duka, 2005; Pomerlau, Fertig, Baker, & Conney, 1983) that can lead to drugtaking behaviour (e.g., Wigmore & Hinson, 1991). By contrast, relatively less is known about reactivity to food cues. In animals, exposure to a familiar food is found to elicit feeding behaviour, and this occurs even in the absence of nutritional need (Weingarten, 1985). Similar reactivity to food cues has also been observed in humans. For example, exposure to food cues has been found to stimulate a physiological preparedness to consume food (see Mattes, 1997), evidenced by increases in salivation, heart rate, heart rate variability, blood pressure, skin conductance, and gastric activity (Nederkoorn, Smulders, Havermans, & Jansen, 2004; Nederkoorn, Smulders, & Jansen, 2000). * Corresponding author. address: Amanda.Tetley@nottshc.nhs.uk (A. Tetley). Likewise, exposure to the sensory characteristics of food has been found to stimulate food intake (Cornell, Rodin, & Weingarten, 1989). Despite basic research investigating food-cue reactivity, relatively few studies have explored individual differences in this dietary phenomenon, and very little is known about the extent to which elevated food-cue reactivity represents a risk factor for behaviours leading to overeating and obesity. In a recent study, Jansen et al. (2003) suggested that sensitivity to food cues might differ in overweight, and non-overweight, children. Yet, to date, this finding has not been confirmed in adults. Rather, previous authors have considered differences in food-cue reactivity across restrained, and unrestrained, eaters. These studies have revealed that restrained eaters consume larger amounts of a cued food than unrestrained eaters (Fedoroff, Polivy, & Herman, 1997; Fedoroff, Herman, & Polivy, 2003; Jansen & van den Hout, 1991; Rogers & Hill, 1989) after food-cue exposure, and experience a greater desire to eat, elevated levels of craving (Fedoroff et al., 1997, 2003), increases in salivation (Brunstrom, Yates, & Witcomb, 2004; Klajner, Herman, Polivy, & Chhabra, 1981) and increased blood pressure (Nederkoorn et al., 2000). There is reason to suspect that heightened food-cue reactivity in adults might be associated with a more general tendency towards being overweight and daily overeating. Put simply, food-cue /$ see front matter ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi: /j.appet

2 A. Tetley et al. / Appetite 52 (2009) reactivity is assumed to represent a form of conditioned response. Thus, after repeated pairing of an external cue with the ingestion of food, mere exposure to the cue is found to initiate appetitive behaviour, even in the absence of prior hunger (Birch, McPhee, Sulivan, & Johnson, 1989; Weingarten, 1984). Given this, one possibility is that individuals who are especially cue reactive may be prone to overeating and being overweight. This is because they are more likely to engage in dietary behaviours that promote a positive energy balance. Here, our primary objective was to examine whether cue reactivity is associated with being overweight and consuming larger everyday portion sizes. Specifically, we hypothesised (i) that overweight individuals will experience greater reactivity to a food cue than non-overweight individuals and (ii) that individuals who report consuming larger everyday portion sizes will experience greater food-cue reactivity than individuals who typically consume smaller everyday portion sizes. A secondary aim was to determine the extent to which food-cue reactivity is associated with a tendency to restrict ones dietary intake. As discussed, previous studies have suggested that restrained eaters experience elevated levels of food-cue reactivity (Fedoroff et al., 1997, 2003). However, these studies have assessed dietary restraint using a measure which identifies restrained eaters who engage in dietary restriction interspersed with bouts of disinhibited eating. Accordingly, in this study we sought to determine the extent to which food-cue reactivity is associated with a tendency to restrict ones dietary intake, independently of a tendency to engage in disinhibited eating by using the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire Restraint Scale (DEBQ-R; van Strien, Frijiter, Begers, & Defares, 1986) as a measure of food restriction. In the present study, we used a version of a paradigm that was introduced by Cornell et al. (1989). Following Weingarten s (1985) notion of cue-induced hunger, Cornell et al. (1989) sought to determine the extent to which food-cue exposure induces a motivation to eat in the absence of physiological need. Accordingly, participants were offered a buffet lunch prior to food-cue exposure. Likewise, in the present study, participants were asked to consume items from a buffet lunch until they felt satiated prior to exposure to a food cue. In previous food-cue reactivity studies (Fedoroff et al., 1997, 2003), food-cue reactivity has been assessed using a measure of intake of a cued food. Typically, in these studies, intake in a control (non-cued) condition is compared with intake in a foodcued condition. However, this design is flawed given that even the presentation of food immediately prior to an eating episode acts as a cue to stimulate intake (Weingarten, 1985). Thus, a non-cued condition effectively becomes a cued condition. Consequently, in the present study, a novel measure of desired portion size of the cued food was employed to minimise undesirable cueing effects in the non-cued condition. Participants were asked to indicate their desired portion of the test food (pizza) before, and after, exposure to this food using a representation of the food, rather than the actual food. This allowed us to detect changes in prospective intake brought about by exposure to the food cue, and most importantly it minimised cueing effects in the pre-exposure phase. Method Participants One hundred and twenty participants were recruited via from the population of female undergraduate students at Loughborough University (UK) (mean age = 20.9, SD = 2.6). Ethics approval was obtained from the local Departmental Ethics Committee. All participants gave written consent to participate in the study and were informed that they could withdraw at any time during the experiment. Measures Cue reactivity Craving and desire to eat were measured using 100-mm visualanalogue rating scales. Respectively, these were headed How strong is your desire to eat pizza right now? and How strong is your craving for pizza right now? Both were anchored with the phrases Not at all and Extremely. Desired portion size was assessed using a cardboard rectangular model of pizza (150 mm 130 mm 32 mm). This was identical in size to the slice presented during cue exposure and provided participants with an indication of the depth of the pizza. Participants were told that this model represented a cheese and tomato pizza. Using this model as a reference, the participants were asked to select the amount of pizza they would want to eat at that moment in time. They indicated this selection using a 420 mm 594 mm sheet of card. The card had a diagonal line running from the bottom left to the top right hand corner, and participants were told that the corner of their selected portion size should intersect this line (examples were provided). The area of the selected portion size was then calculated. Everyday portion size To obtain a measure of everyday portion size, participants were shown sets of eight photographs. Each set depicted a commonly consumed food (Nelson, Atkinson, & Meyer, 1997). The series of pictures contained portion sizes ranging from the 5th to 95th centile on a distribution of portion sizes observed in The Dietary and Nutritional Survey of British Adults (Gregory, Foster, Tyler, & Wiseman, 1990). Participants were asked to use these photographs to indicate the amount of food that they typically consume. They were told that if the amount of a food corresponded exactly with the amount depicted in one of the pictures, then they should put a cross through the corresponding number on a scale. However, if the amount was slightly larger or smaller then they should indicate this by placing a cross to the left or right of the corresponding number. This response was recorded using a 9-point visual analogue scale (80 mm in length) anchored with the numbers 0 and 8. The points on the scale were numbered consecutively at 10-mm intervals. Participants may place their cross between 0 and 1 on the scale if their everyday portion was smaller than the first picture presented. In total 15 foods were presented in this way (pasta, cornflakes, chocolate cake, sponge pudding, potatoes, baked beans, lasagne, spaghetti bolognaise, chips, cheesecake, roast beef, battered fish, carrots, fruit salad, and quiche). Pizza was not included in the assessment of everyday portion size to exclude any cuing effects that may be brought about by exposure to a pictorial cue of this food. Dietary restraint Dietary restraint was assessed using the restraint scale of the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ-R; van Strien et al., 1986). This scale is described as a measure of successful dietary restriction (Gorman & Alison, 1995). 1 Procedure Before arriving to be tested the participants were told that the aim of the experiment was to explore the relationship between 1 Participants were not asked to complete the external, and emotional, scales of the DEBQ to avoid overburdening the participants with further assessments in a study already incorporating a number of assessments. Furthermore, these scales did not address the specified study aims.

3 616 A. Tetley et al. / Appetite 52 (2009) Table 1 Within-subject t-tests, means, and standard deviations, for pre-exposure and post-exposure appetite ratings and portion-size selections. n Pre-exposure Post-exposure t p Mean SD Mean SD Desire to eat pizza (mm) <.001 * Craving for pizza (mm) <.001 * Desired portion size of pizza (mm 2 ) * p < appetite and mood. They were also told that they would have to rate their mood throughout the experiment, that they would be asked to offer an opinion on various foods, and that they would receive a buffet-lunch. Participants were tested individually between 11 a.m. and 3 p.m. All were instructed to refrain from eating for three hours prior to the onset of the experiment. On arrival, participants rated their hunger and fullness using 100-mm visual-analogue ratings scales. Hunger and fullness rating scales were headed with How hungry/ full are you right now? Both were anchored with the phrases Not at all hungry/full and Extremely hungry/full. They were then presented with a buffet-lunch and were asked to eat until they felt comfortably full. The buffet lunch comprised three sandwiches (ham, cheese, and chicken), one and half sausage rolls, six small scotch eggs (egg filling covered in sausage meat), three handfuls of original flavoured crisps, two large oranges, six Jaffa Cakes (sponge and orange filling covered in chocolate), and a glass of water. To determine the efficacy of the lunch, participants provided a second set of hunger and fullness ratings after lunch. They were then asked to complete the everyday portion-size assessment. This was followed by the first set of cue reactivity measures. Consistent with the cover story, this initial set of reactivity measures also included a number of 100-mm visual-analogue ratings scales relating to participants current mood. The participants were then exposed to the sight and smell of pizza for three minutes. The pizza was presented in a rectangular slice and weighed 300 g (810 kcal). It was placed on the table directly in front of the participant and the participants were told that the experimenter would return shortly. The duration of the exposure phase was set at three minutes because similar cue exposure periods have been reported to induce changes in motivation to eat (Cornell et al., 1989). After pizza-cue exposure, the participants provided a second set of reactivity measures, after which they rated their liking for pizza and completed the DEBQ-restraint scale. Finally, height was measured using a stadiometer (Bodycare, Warwickshire, UK) and weight was assessed using a set of weighing scales (Soehnle, Germany). BMI was then calculated from this data. Data analysis In this experiment we were interested to assess the effect of foodcue exposure on an individual s motivation to eat. We reasoned that if food-cue exposure is able to motivate food intake, desire to eat, craving for, and desired portion size of the cued food should be elevated after, relative to before, food-cue exposure. In our initial analyses, we sought to compare how the pre- and post-cue exposure ratings of desire to eat and craving, and desired portion size differed by applying a series of within-subjects t-tests. For the remaining analyses, we calculated a change score for each of our three measures of reactivity (desire to eat, craving, desired portion size), derived from the difference between the measure of reactivity before and after cue exposure. To determine the extent to which these changes are associated with overweight/normal weight, average everyday portion size, and the DEBQ-restraint scale, we used separate regression analyses. An average measure of everyday portion size was calculated using the scores for the 15 foods. BMI scores were used to dichotomise individuals into a normal weight (BMI < 25.0), and an overweight (BMI 25.0), group. In each regression model, we controlled statistically for liking for pizza by entering this simultaneously into the regression model as a covariate. For each measure of change in reactivity, we also included the corresponding baseline measure of reactivity by entering this simultaneously into the regression model as a covariate. Where one of our independent variables (overweight/non-overweight classification, everyday portion-size selection and scores on the DEBQ-restraint scale) shared a statistically significant association with a measure of food-cue reactivity, the parameter estimates from the regression equation were used to predict the change in the measure of food-cue reactivity across the values of the independent measure. 2 All analyses in the present study were conducted using SPSS Version 11. The significance level tested was set at p <.05 and all tests were two-tailed to ensure that associations between variables (regression analyses), or changes across variables (within-subject t-tests), were detected irrespective of whether they were in the hypothesised direction. Results Efficacy of the buffet lunch The buffet lunch was designed to ensure that participants were satiated prior to pizza-cue exposure. After the buffet lunch, mean hunger rating was 11.2 mm (SD = 11.8 mm) and mean fullness rating was 73.4 mm (SD = 17.8 mm), suggesting that participants were satiated by this phase in the experiment. These ratings represented a statistically significant change from baseline (p <.05). Post-lunch hunger and fullness ratings did not differ across overweight and nonoverweight individuals, across our measure of everyday portion size, or across DEBQ-restraint scores (all p <.05). Effects of pizza-cue exposure Pizza-cue exposure significantly increased participants craving for pizza, and their desire to eat this food. However, it did not significantly stimulate the selection of a larger portion of pizza (Table 1). Correlations between changes in reactivity measures Change in ratings of desire to eat pizza from pre- to post-cue exposure were not significantly correlated with changes in desired 2 These were calculated using the following formula; y = b 1 x + b 2 z + b 3 w + b 0, where y is the change in the measure of reactivity, x is the independent variable, z is the liking for pizza, and y is the relevant pre-exposure reactivity measure, b 1,b 2,b 3 is the relevant parameter estimate from the regression model (the gradient of the regression line), b 0 is the constant coefficient from the model (i.e., the change in the measure of reactivity when the value of the independent variable is zero). In all predictions, the relevant pre-exposure measure and liking for pizza were held at their average in the sample. Thus, all predictions were made for a participant with an average liking for pizza and average pre-exposure rating for the particular measure of cue reactivity.

4 A. Tetley et al. / Appetite 52 (2009) Table 2 Between-subject t-tests, means, and standard deviations for age, dietary restraint, and everyday portion-size selection for overweight, and non-overweight, individuals. Table 4 Predicted values from the linear regression model for overweight, and normal weight, individuals for pre-exposure portion-size selection, and the total amounts these individuals would be expected to consume a. Non-overweight Overweight t p Pre-exposure Post-exposure Mean SD Mean SD Age DEBQ-restraint scores Everyday portion-size selection Non-overweight Overweight a Holding liking for pizza, and pre-exposure pizza size, at their mean values for the sample (70.83 mm, mm 2 [ kcal], respectively). portion size of pizza after cue exposure (r =.062, p =.503). Although there was a statistically significant association between change in craving for pizza from pre- to post-exposure and change in desired portion of this food after pizza-cue exposure (r =.233, p =.010), this association was modest. Changes in the ratings of desire to eat and craving after cue exposure were significantly correlated (r =.462, p <.001). Cue reactivity and overweight Mean BMI was 22.9 (SD = 2.6). Twenty-six participants were classified as overweight (BMI > 25.0, BMI range ), and 94 were classified as not overweight (BMI < 25.0, BMI range ). There was no evidence of a statistically significant difference in the age of the overweight, relevant to the non-overweight, group or in the scores they obtained on the DEBQ-restraint scale (both p >.05) (Table 2). However, overweight individuals did report selecting larger everyday portion sizes that non-overweight individuals (p =.014) (Table 2). After cue exposure, overweight participants experienced a greater change in both their desire to eat and their craving for pizza. However, in each case, the difference between overweight and normal weight participants was not statistically significant (desire, p =.122; craving, p =.128) (Table 3). After controlling for each participant s liking for pizza and their pre-exposure desired portion size in a regression analysis, overweight participants were found to experience a greater increase in their desired pizza portion size following exposure to pizza (p =.031) (Table 3) and the model explained over 60% of the variance in change in desired portion size. Using the parameter estimates from the regression model for change in desired portion size of the cued food, we predicted the change in the number of kilocalories an overweight individual with an average liking for pizza (average liking for pizza = 70.8 mm) and average pre-exposure desired portion size ( mm 2 [174.2 kcal]) wanted to eat after cue exposure compared to a non-overweight individual with an average liking for pizza and average pre-exposure portion size. This prediction suggested that their desired portion size increased on average by 46.1 kcal. By contrast, the desired portion-size selection of normalweight participants decreased by 17.2 kcal. We also calculated the total amount of pizza in kilocalories that overweight, and nonoverweight, individuals were likely to consume after cue exposure by using a regression model to predict the amount of pizza (kcal) that would be ingested prior to cue exposure, and then adding onto this the change in intake after cue exposure (Table 4). Cue reactivity and everyday portion size A further aim of the present study was to explore the possibility that high levels of cue-reactivity are associated with a general tendency to consume large portion sizes. The findings from the regression models suggest that everyday-portion size was not significantly associated with either change in craving for pizza (p =.378) or change in desire for pizza (p =.328) (Table 5). However, everyday portion size was associated with a significant change in desired pizza size (p =.036) (Table 5) after controlling statistically for participants liking for pizza and their pre-exposure desired portion size. In fact, this model accounted for over 60% of the variance in change in desired portion size. Using the parameter estimates from the regression model for change in desired portion size of pizza, we predicted the change in desired portion size after pizza-cue exposure in kilocalories for an individual who reported consuming small, medium, and large, everyday portion sizes (calculated as the mean value in each tertile of the data). In this analysis, we held liking for pizza and pre-exposure portion sizes at their mean values for the sample (70.8 mm and mm 2 [174.2 kcal] respectively) (Fig. 1). This suggested that individuals who typically consume the largest everyday portion size experi- Table 3 Parameter estimates from linear regression models of associations between overweight and three measures of food-cue reactivity (n = 120), and the corresponding covariates a. Dependent variable Independent variables and covariates R square B SE p Change in desired portion size.636 Overweight b * Pre-exposure desire-to-eat.7.05 <.001 * Liking for pizza <.001 * Change in craving.114 Overweight b Pre-exposure craving Liking for pizza <.001 * Change in desire-to-eat.109 Overweight b Pre-exposure desire-to-eat * Liking for pizza * a Covariates are liking for pizza and the relevant pre-exposure rating. b Reference category is non-overweight. * p < 0.05.

5 618 A. Tetley et al. / Appetite 52 (2009) Table 5 Parameter estimates from linear regression models of associations between everyday portion-size selection (small, medium, and large) and three measures of food-cue reactivity and corresponding covariates (n = 120) a. Dependent measure Independent variables and covariates R square B SE p Change in desired portion size.636 Average everyday portion size b * Pre-exposure desired portion size <.001 * Change in craving Liking for pizza * Average everyday portion size b Pre-exposure craving Liking for pizza * Change in desire-to-eat.097 Average everyday portion size b Pre-exposure desire-to-eat * Liking for pizza * * p < a Covariates were liking for pizza and the relevant pre-exposure rating. b Comparison across the continuous measure of average everyday portion-size selection enced the greatest change in desired portion size after cue exposure. Again, we also calculated the total number of kilocalories that would be ingested after pizza-cue exposure for an individual reporting consuming small, medium, and large, everyday portion sizes. To do this, parameter estimates from a regression model predicting pre-exposure portion-size selection were added to the predicted change in desired pizza sizes (Table 6). This revealed that individuals who consume larger everyday portion sizes report a desire to eat a portion of a cued food comprising a greater number of kilocalories (approximately 100 kcal) relative to individuals who consume smaller everyday portion sizes. To summarise, in this experiment, both being overweight and selecting larger everyday portion sizes was associated with desiring larger portion sizes of a cued food. To determine the extent to which one of these predictor variables have greater predictive validity in relation to food-cue reactivity, weight status (overweight/normal weight) and average everyday portion size were entered simultaneously into the regression model predicting change in desired portion size of the cued food. In this model, being overweight and selecting larger everyday portion sizes failed to significantly predict change in desired portion size (p >.05). This shows that neither everyday portion-size selection, nor being overweight, has greater predictive validity in relation to food-cue reactivity, and consequently that shared variance between the two variables accounts for changes in desired portion of a cued food observed in this experiment. Cue reactivity and dietary restraint The final aim of this study was to determine the extent to which food-cue reactivity is associated with participants scores on the DEBQ-restraint scale. The results of the regression analyses provided little evidence to suggest that this scale was associated with a change in any of the measures of cue reactivity (all p >.05, results not shown here). Discussion The present study was designed to determine the extent to which exposure to the visual and olfactory characteristics of food would stimulate a greater motivation to eat in (i) overweight, relative to non-overweight, individuals, (ii) individuals who typically consume larger everyday portion sizes, and (iii) individuals who obtain higher scores on the DEBQ-restraint scale. To test these relationships, we compared participants ratings of desirability, craving for, and desired portion size of pizza, both before and after brief exposure to the olfactory and visual characteristics of this food. The results of the study revealed little evidence to suggest that when dietary restraint is assessed independently of disinhibited eating it shares any association with food-cue reactivity. Further, the results suggested that whilst an individual s weight status Table 6 Predicted values from the linear regression model for pre-exposure pizza size, and the total amounts individuals would be expected to consume in kilocalories for small, medium, and large, portion sizes (calculated as the average value in each tertile of the data) a. Fig. 1. Predicted change in pizza size in kilocalories (kcal) for individuals with small (2.8), medium (3.7) and large (4.5) average everyday portion size (calculated as the average score in each tertile of the data) estimated using the parameter estimates from a linear regression model for change in pizza size (B = ). In this model pre-exposure pizza size and liking for pizza are held at their mean values in the sample ( mm 2 [ kcal] and 70.8 mm respectively) and their respective parameter estimates (B = 0.7, B = 64.5, respectively) were used to predict change in desired pizza size in kilocalories. Pre-exposure Everyday portion size Small Medium Large Post-exposure a Holding liking for pizza, and pre-exposure pizza size, at their mean values for the sample (70.83 mm, mm 2 [ kcal], respectively).

6 A. Tetley et al. / Appetite 52 (2009) and their everyday portion-size selections were not significantly associated with changes in ratings of appetite (desire to eat and craving) following food-cue exposure, they were associated with a relatively greater change in desired portion size of the cued food. This latter finding is unsurprising given that changes in desire to eat and craving for pizza were poorly correlated with changes in desired pizza portion size. One possibility is that that the rating scales for desire to eat and craving lacked sufficient sensitivity to reveal weight-related differences in food-cue reactivity. A further finding from this study was that neither everyday portion-size selection, nor being overweight, significantly predicted change in desired portion size when entered simultaneously into a regression model. This is likely to result from the fact that overweight individuals were more likely to report selecting larger everyday portion sizes than their normal-weight counterparts. This suggests that everyday portion size and BMI were acting as proxy measures for each other. We also note several limitations to this study. The most important of these is that we did not include a control condition in which participants are exposed to three minutes of non-foodexposure. In the absence of this condition, it is difficult to conclude that differences in pre- and post-exposure desired portion sizes resulted exclusively from pizza-cue exposure. For example, they may result from differences in food digestion in overweight, relative to non-overweight, individuals following the buffet lunch, or differences in intake at the buffet lunch between these two groups. Both differences could have caused overweight individuals to desire larger portion sizes of the cued food. The second limitation of this study was that the assessment of participants everyday portion sizes relied on a self-report measure. This may not provide a valid measure of everyday portion size as individuals may attempt to portray themselves in particular ways. Final limitations were that the study sample comprised a non-random sample of the same gender (female) from a specific age range (18 30), of a particular educational level (university students), and most likely from a particular social class. Further to this, the study was only able to test the effects of exposure to one food cue (pizza) and did not account for any effects of individual differences in sense of olfaction on sensitivity to an olfactory, and visual, cue. If the findings of this study are replicated in future studies addressing the limitations of this experiment, one possibility is that selecting larger everyday portion sizes may be a precursor to elevated food-cue reactivity (Jansen, 1998). Specifically, individuals who overeat may come to habitually overeat in the presence of food cues. Given this, one possibility might be that an overweight individual s tendency to overeat causes them to desire relatively larger portions of a cued food. There could be other potential explanations for an overweight individual s tendency to desire larger portions of a cued food. One possibility is that they are more cue reactive than their nonoverweight counterparts because they have a tendency to be more impulsive (Nederkoorn, Smulders, Havermans, Roefs, & Jansen, 2006), and to have a heightened sensitivity to reward (Franken & Muris, 2005). This possibility has not been considered empirically and would be an interesting avenue for further research. Another possibility, identified by Jansen et al. (2003), is that overweight individuals experience delayed satiety signals when consuming a cued food. According to Jansen et al. (2003), this might be because the decline in the pleasantness of food, which occurs during intake and leads to the termination of an eating episode (sensory specific satiety), takes longer in overweight individuals. The authors suggest that this decline in the pleasantness of a food s sensory characteristic may occur during exposure to a food cue for nonoverweight individuals and consequently causes these individuals to consume smaller amounts of a cued food. However, the exact length of cueing required to initiate these effects in overweight, and non-overweight, individuals is unclear from Jansen et al. s (2003) account. One implication of observing greater sensitivity to food cues in overweight individuals is that levels of food-cue reactivity could be manipulated and reduced in these individuals. Given that food-cue reactivity is assumed to result from learned associations between the sensory characteristics of a food (visual and olfactory) and food ingestion (Wardle, 1990; Weingarten, 1985), it may be possible that this can be achieved by extinguishing these learned associations. Previously, authors have reported utilising a technique referred to as response prevention to reduce cue reactivity in bulimics (Jansen, 1998) and alcoholics (Drummond & Glautier, 1994; Monti et al., 1993). This involves presenting individuals with a cue associated with food or alcohol and preventing them from eating, or from drinking alcohol, in the presence of this stimulus. Given this, future studies may wish to explore the prospect that levels of food-cue reactivity can be manipulated in overweight and obese individuals. References Birch, L. L., McPhee, L., Sulivan, S., & Johnson, S. (1989). Conditioned meal initiation in young children. Appetite, 13(2), Breteler, J. A., Schippers, C. M., De Jong, C. A. J., & van der Stark, C. P. F. (2000). No effect of negative mood on the alcohol cue reactivity of in-patient alcoholics. Addictive Behaviors, 25(4), Brunstrom, J. M., Yates, H. M., & Witcomb, G. L. (2004). Dietary restraint and heightened reactivity to food. Physiology & Behaviour, 81(1), Burton, S. M., & Tiffany, S. T. (1997). The effect of alcohol consumption on craving to smoke. Addiction, 92(1), Cornell, C. E., Rodin, J., & Weingarten, H. P. (1989). Stimulus-induced eating when satiated. Physiology & Behaviour, 45, Drummond, D. C., & Glautier, S. T. (1994). A controlled trial of cue exposure treatment in alcohol dependence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, Fedoroff, I., Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1997). The effects of pre-exposure to food cues on the eating behaviour of restrained and unrestrained eaters. Appetite, 28, Fedoroff, I., Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2003). The specificity of restrained versus unrestrained eaters responses to food cues: general desire to eat, or craving for the cued food? Appetite, 41, Field, M., & Duka, T. (2005). Cue reactivity in smokers: the effects of perceived availability and gender. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behaviour, 78(3), Franken, I. H. A., & Muris, P. (2005). Individual differences in reward sensitivity are related to food craving and relative body weight in healthy women. Appetite, 45, Gorman, B. S., & Alison, D. B. (1995). Measures of restrained eating. In D. B. Allison (Ed.), Handbook of assessment methods for eating behaviors and weight-related problems (pp ). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Gregory, J., Foster, K., Tyler, H., & Wiseman, M. (1990). The dietary and nutritional survey of British adults. London: HMSO. Jansen, A. (1998). A learning model of binge eating: cue reactivity and cue exposure. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(3), Jansen, A., Theunissen, N., Slechten, K., Nederkoorn, C., Boon, B., Mulkens, S., et al. (2003). Overweight children overeat after exposure to food cues. Eating Behaviours, 4, Jansen, A., & van den Hout, M. (1991). On being led into temptation counterregulation of dieters after smelling a preload. Addictive Behaviors, 16, Klajner, F., Herman, C. P., Polivy, J., & Chhabra, R. (1981). Human obesity, dieting, and anticipatory salivation to food. Physiology & Behaviour, 27(2), Mattes, R. D. (1997). Physiologic responses to sensory stimulation by food nutritional implications. Journal of American Dietetic Association, 97, Monti, P. M., Rohsenow, D. J., Rubonis, A. V., Niaura, R. S., Sirota, A. D., Colby, S. M., et al. (1993). Cue exposure with coping skills treatment for male alcoholics. A preliminary investigation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F., Havermans, R., & Jansen, A. (2004). Exposure to bingefood in bulimia nervosa: finger pulse amplitude as a potential measure of urge to eat and predictor of food intake. Appetite, 42(2), Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F., Havermans, R., Roefs, A., & Jansen, A. (2006). Impulsivity in obese women. Appetite, 47, Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F. T. Y., & Jansen, A. (2000). Cephalic phase responses, craving and food intake in normal subjects. Appetite, 35(1), Nelson, M., Atkinson, M., & Meyer, J. (1997). A photographic atlas of food portion sizes. London: MAFF Publications. Pomerlau, O. F., Fertig, J., Baker, L., & Conney, N. (1983). Reactivity to alcohol cues in alcoholics and non-alcoholics: implications for a stimulus control analysis of drinking. Addictive Behaviors, 8, 1 10.

7 620 A. Tetley et al. / Appetite 52 (2009) Rikard-Figuero, K., & Zeichner, A. (1985). Assessment of smoking urge and its concomitants under an environmental smoking cue manipulation. Addictive Behaviors, 10, Rogers, P. J., & Hill, A. (1989). Breakdown of dietary restraint following mere exposure to food stimuli: interrelationships between restraint, hunger, salivation and food intake. Addictive Behaviors, 14, van Strien, T., Frijiters, J. E., Bergers, G. P. A., & Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ-R) for assessment of restrained, emotional and external eating behaviour. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, Wardle, J. (1990). Conditioning processes and cue exposure in the modification of excessive eating. Addictive Behaviors, 15(4), Weingarten, H. P. (1984). Meal initiation controlled by learned cues: basic Behavioural Properties. Appetite, 5, Weingarten, H. P. (1985). Stimulus control of eating: implications for a two-factor theory of hunger. Appetite, 6, Wigmore, S. W., & Hinson, R. E. (1991). The influence of setting on consumption in the balanced placebo design. British Journal of Addiction, 86(2),

Individual differences and food-cue reactivity: Predictors of BMI, portion size, and everyday dietary behaviour.

Individual differences and food-cue reactivity: Predictors of BMI, portion size, and everyday dietary behaviour. REFERENCE No. RES-000-22-1745 Page 15 Individual differences and food-cue reactivity: Predictors of BMI, portion size, and everyday dietary behaviour. Investigator: Dr J M Brunstrom Institution: Department

More information

The Proof of the Pudding is in the Eating: Is the DEBQ - External Eating Scale a Valid Measure of External Eating?

The Proof of the Pudding is in the Eating: Is the DEBQ - External Eating Scale a Valid Measure of External Eating? REGULAR ARTICLE The Proof of the Pudding is in the Eating: Is the DEBQ - External Eating Scale a Valid Measure of External Eating? Anita Jansen, PhD 1 * Chantal Nederkoorn, PhD 1 Anne Roefs, PhD 1 Peggy

More information

(This is a sample cover image for this issue. The actual cover is not yet available at this time.)

(This is a sample cover image for this issue. The actual cover is not yet available at this time.) (This is a sample cover image for this issue. The actual cover is not yet available at this time.) This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author

More information

Appetite 56 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Appetite. journal homepage:

Appetite 56 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Appetite. journal homepage: Appetite 56 (2011) 345 349 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Appetite journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/appet Research report Training inhibitory control. A recipe for resisting sweet temptations

More information

Restraint, inhibition and extinction of appetite. Anita Jansen Professor of Psychology Maastricht

Restraint, inhibition and extinction of appetite. Anita Jansen Professor of Psychology Maastricht Restraint, inhibition and extinction of appetite Anita Jansen Professor of Psychology Maastricht University @professorjansen www.eetonderzoek.nl Eating disorders eating restraint restraint BED BN Binge

More information

Perceived parental control of food intake is related to external, restrained and emotional eating in 7 12-year-old boys and girls

Perceived parental control of food intake is related to external, restrained and emotional eating in 7 12-year-old boys and girls Appetite 49 (2007) 618 625 Research Report Perceived parental control of food intake is related to external, restrained and emotional eating in 7 12-year-old boys and girls Tatjana van Strien, Francien

More information

Appetite 51 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Appetite. journal homepage:

Appetite 51 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Appetite. journal homepage: Appetite 51 (2008) 296 301 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Appetite journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/appet Research report Estimating everyday portion size using a method of constant

More information

The dual pathway model of overeating Ouwens, Machteld; van Strien, T.; van Leeuwe, J. F. J.; van der Staak, C. P. F.

The dual pathway model of overeating Ouwens, Machteld; van Strien, T.; van Leeuwe, J. F. J.; van der Staak, C. P. F. Tilburg University The dual pathway model of overeating Ouwens, Machteld; van Strien, T.; van Leeuwe, J. F. J.; van der Staak, C. P. F. Published in: Appetite Publication date: 2009 Link to publication

More information

Perspective. Biological perspective entering psychology. Understanding Eating from a psychological perspective

Perspective. Biological perspective entering psychology. Understanding Eating from a psychological perspective Understanding Eating from a psychological perspective The 1st Scandinavian Critical Dietetics Conference Uppsala, August 25, 2017 Ata Ghaderi, Ph.D. Professor, Clin. Psychologist, Lic. Psychotherapist

More information

City, University of London Institutional Repository

City, University of London Institutional Repository City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Brunstrom, J. M., Rogers, P. J., Pothos, E. M., Calitri, R. & Tapper, K. (2008). Estimating everyday portion size using

More information

Chapter 92 Cued Overeating

Chapter 92 Cued Overeating Chapter 92 Cued Overeating Anita Jansen, Remco C. Havermans, and Chantal Nederkoorn Abbreviations WHO CS US UR CR CBT World Health Organisation Conditioned stimulus (= cues) Unconditioned stimulus Unconditioned

More information

The relationship between impulsivity, weight concern and the yoyo-effect in healthy women

The relationship between impulsivity, weight concern and the yoyo-effect in healthy women ASTRID MEESTERS The relationship between impulsivity, weight and the yoyo-effect in healthy women Weight cycling, or the so-called yoyo-effect, is an unhealthy aspect of eating behaviour. It is hypothesized

More information

Exposure to binge food in bulimia nervosa: finger pulse amplitude as a potential measure of urge to eat and predictor of food intake

Exposure to binge food in bulimia nervosa: finger pulse amplitude as a potential measure of urge to eat and predictor of food intake Appetite 42 (2004) 125 130 Research Review Exposure to binge food in bulimia nervosa: finger pulse amplitude as a potential measure of urge to eat and predictor of food intake Chantal Nederkoorn*, Fren

More information

Ogden, J., Whyman, C. (1997) The effects of repeated weighing on psychological state. European Eating Disorders Review 5,

Ogden, J., Whyman, C. (1997) The effects of repeated weighing on psychological state. European Eating Disorders Review 5, 1 Ogden, J., Whyman, C. (1997) The effects of repeated weighing on psychological state. European Eating Disorders Review 5, 121-130. The effect of repeated weighing on psychological state Jane Ogden and

More information

External cues & appetite control

External cues & appetite control External cues & appetite control Dr. Ellen van Kleef, Wageningen University ILSI Europe 2018 Annual Symposium - Food for the Future, March 27 Today 1. Many people struggle with body weight 2. External

More information

Swiss Food Panel. -A longitudinal study about eating behaviour in Switzerland- ENGLISH. Short versions of selected publications. Zuerich,

Swiss Food Panel. -A longitudinal study about eating behaviour in Switzerland- ENGLISH. Short versions of selected publications. Zuerich, Vertrag 10.008123 ENGLISH Swiss Food Panel -A longitudinal study about eating behaviour in Switzerland- Short versions of selected publications Zuerich, 16.10. 2013 Address for Correspondence ETH Zurich

More information

INVESTIGATING SATIATING PROPERTIES OF FOODS

INVESTIGATING SATIATING PROPERTIES OF FOODS The Pennsylvania State University The Graduate School College of Health and Human Development INVESTIGATING SATIATING PROPERTIES OF FOODS A Dissertation in Nutritional Sciences by Rachel A. Williams 2013

More information

Eat it or beat it: The differential effects of food temptations on overweight and normal-weight. restrained eaters. Netherlands.

Eat it or beat it: The differential effects of food temptations on overweight and normal-weight. restrained eaters. Netherlands. 1 Running Head: BMI, RESTRAINED EATING, AND WANTING Eat it or beat it: The differential effects of food temptations on overweight and normal-weight restrained eaters Carolijn Ouwehand 1, and Esther K.

More information

WEIGHT GAIN. This module provides information about weight gain for people with schizophrenia. SERIES: HEALTH MATTERS

WEIGHT GAIN. This module provides information about weight gain for people with schizophrenia. SERIES: HEALTH MATTERS SERIES: HEALTH MATTERS WEIGHT GAIN This module provides information about weight gain for people with schizophrenia. IT IS VERY EASY TO GAIN WEIGHT if you eat more than your body needs, if you have a medical

More information

Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: a meta-analytic review

Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: a meta-analytic review doi: 10.1111/obr.12354 Etiology and Pathophysiology Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: a meta-analytic review Rebecca G. Boswell 1 and Hedy Kober 1,2 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

Inhibitory Control Training as a potential behavioural intervention for overweight and obesity. Andrew Jones

Inhibitory Control Training as a potential behavioural intervention for overweight and obesity. Andrew Jones Inhibitory Control Training as a potential behavioural intervention for overweight and obesity. Andrew Jones AIMS Background into Inhibitory Control and related cognitive processes, and how they might

More information

Recording of Swallowing Events Using Electromyography as a Non-invasive Measurement of Salivation

Recording of Swallowing Events Using Electromyography as a Non-invasive Measurement of Salivation Appetite, 1999, 33, 361 369 Article No. appe.1999.6, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Recording of Swallowing Events Using Electromyography as a Non-invasive Measurement of Salivation

More information

Veenstra, E.M., De Jong, P.J., Koster, E.H.W., & Roefs, A. (2010). Attentional avoidance of

Veenstra, E.M., De Jong, P.J., Koster, E.H.W., & Roefs, A. (2010). Attentional avoidance of Attentional Avoidance 1 Running head: ATTENTIONAL AVOIDANCE OF HIGH-FAT FOOD Veenstra, E.M., De Jong, P.J., Koster, E.H.W., & Roefs, A. (2010). Attentional avoidance of high-fat food in disinhibited eaters.

More information

Advice on taking enzyme replacement capsules

Advice on taking enzyme replacement capsules Advice on taking enzyme replacement capsules Leicestershire Nutrition and Dietetic Services Hepatobiliary Services i Information for patients Introduction What is the pancreas? The pancreas is part of

More information

Behaviour Research and Therapy

Behaviour Research and Therapy Behaviour Research and Therapy xxx (2011) 1e10 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behaviour Research and Therapy journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat Using stop signals to inhibit

More information

Accepted Manuscript. peer , version 1-10 Aug The allure of forbidden food: On the role of attention in self-regulation

Accepted Manuscript. peer , version 1-10 Aug The allure of forbidden food: On the role of attention in self-regulation Author manuscript, published in "Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44, 5 (2008) 1283" DOI : 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.008 Accepted Manuscript The allure of forbidden food: On the role of attention

More information

Validity and Reliability of an Estimated Daily Intake Scale for Fat

Validity and Reliability of an Estimated Daily Intake Scale for Fat Validity and Reliability of an Estimated Daily Intake Scale for Fat Gregory J. Privitera (Corresponding author) Department of Psychology, Saint Bonaventure University 3261 West State Street, St. Bonaventure,

More information

Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings. Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005

Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings. Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005 Practical ways of reducing cigarette cravings Robert West Cancer Research UK and UCL UK National Smoking Cessation Conference June 2005 Outline What are cravings? The role of cravings in addiction What

More information

Where a licence is displayed above, please note the terms and conditions of the licence govern your use of this document.

Where a licence is displayed above, please note the terms and conditions of the licence govern your use of this document. Personal and social norms for food portion sizes in lean and obese adults Lewis, H B; Forwood, S E; Ahern, A L; Verlaers, K; Robinson, Eric; Higgs, Suzanne; Jebb, S A DOI: 10.1038/ijo.2015.47 License:

More information

Short-term Satiety of High Protein Formula on Obese Subjects: A Pilot Study

Short-term Satiety of High Protein Formula on Obese Subjects: A Pilot Study 2012 International Conference on Nutrition and Food Sciences IPCBEE vol. 39 (2012) (2012) IACSIT Press, Singapore Short-term Satiety of High Protein Formula on Obese Subjects: A Pilot Study Kamalita Pertiwi

More information

Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 41 (2010) 282e288 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbtep Attentional

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen The following full text is a publisher's version. For additional information about this publication click this link. http://hdl.handle.net/2066/28695

More information

The Effects of Social Interactions on Consumption: A Test of Social Facilitation

The Effects of Social Interactions on Consumption: A Test of Social Facilitation The Huron University College Journal of Learning and Motivation Volume 51 Issue 1 Article 1 2013 The Effects of Social Interactions on Consumption: A Test of Social Facilitation Sierra Anderson Follow

More information

CAN PORTION SIZE DECISIONS SUPPORT REDUCTION OF FOOD WASTE

CAN PORTION SIZE DECISIONS SUPPORT REDUCTION OF FOOD WASTE CAN PORTION SIZE DECISIONS SUPPORT REDUCTION OF FOOD WASTE Liisa Lähteenmäki Copenhagen, MAPP Conference 23 November 2017 BACKGROUND Portion size decisions have been studied widely during the last fifteen

More information

Changing memory of food enjoyment to increase food liking, choice and intake. Robinson, Eric; Blissett, Jacqueline; Higgs, Suzanne

Changing memory of food enjoyment to increase food liking, choice and intake. Robinson, Eric; Blissett, Jacqueline; Higgs, Suzanne Changing memory of food enjoyment to increase food liking, choice and intake. Robinson, Eric; Blissett, Jacqueline; Higgs, Suzanne DOI: 10.1017/S0007114511007021 License: None: All rights reserved Document

More information

The addiction model is not appropriate for use with food. Expertise and research funding. Peter Rogers. Potatoes give you drug fix

The addiction model is not appropriate for use with food. Expertise and research funding. Peter Rogers. Potatoes give you drug fix The addiction model is not appropriate for use with food Peter Rogers School of Experimental Psychology Bristol, UK Potatoes give you drug fix You might not have to shoot it up to get a fix. But food is

More information

How to Lose a. Have you tried losing weight the easy way? Take a look inside to find out how.

How to Lose a. Have you tried losing weight the easy way? Take a look inside to find out how. How to Lose a Pound-a-week Have you tried losing weight the easy way? Take a look inside to find out how. Our aim is to help you reach a healthy weight, through healthy eating and increased physical activity.

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Cue-Elicited Anxiety and Craving For Food Using Virtual Reality Scenarios

Cue-Elicited Anxiety and Craving For Food Using Virtual Reality Scenarios 105 Annual Review of Cybertherapy and Telemedicine 2013 B.K. Wiederhold and G. Riva (Eds.) 2013 Interactive Media Institute Cue-Elicited Anxiety and Craving For Food Using Virtual Reality Scenarios Marta

More information

Eating and drinking well. Supporting people living with dementia

Eating and drinking well. Supporting people living with dementia Eating and drinking well Supporting people living with dementia Why are those with dementia at risk of undernutrition and dehydration? Losing weight is common in people living with dementia, who may find

More information

Wanting for chocolate: The influence of body weight and food temptations on the motivation to obtain a favourite snack

Wanting for chocolate: The influence of body weight and food temptations on the motivation to obtain a favourite snack Wanting for chocolate: The influence of body weight and food temptations on the motivation to obtain a favourite snack Marijke Dijkstra Studentnumber: 3064352 Teacher: dr. Carolijn Ouwehand Date: 2 februari,

More information

Binge Eating and Dietary Restraint: A Cross-Cultural Analysis

Binge Eating and Dietary Restraint: A Cross-Cultural Analysis Binge Eating and Dietary Restraint: A Cross-Cultural Analysis Bridget Dolan, Ph.D., C.Psychol. Kathryn Ford, Ph.D. (Accepted 5 March 1990) Investigation of binge eating and dietary restraint has previously

More information

Nutrition and Dietetics Patient Information Leaflet

Nutrition and Dietetics Patient Information Leaflet Dietary advice for people with diabetes Underweight/weight loss Nutrition and Dietetics Patient Information Leaflet How can I make the most of my food? If you want to gain weight, prevent weight loss or

More information

Weigh To Go. September 1, 2010

Weigh To Go. September 1, 2010 Weigh To Go September 1, 2010 Mindless Eating Why We Eat More Than We Think Brian Wansink, PhD Overeating Why do we overeat? Not because of hunger, but because of family/friends, packages, names, numbers,

More information

Is Food Addictive? Nicole M. Avena, Ph.D.

Is Food Addictive? Nicole M. Avena, Ph.D. Is Food Addictive? Nicole M. Avena, Ph.D. Obesity in the US ~69% of adults in the U.S. are overweight, of which ~36% are obese. Being obese or overweight is associated with multiple comorbid health concerns

More information

Food Cues and the Desire to Eat in Dieters and Non-Dieters

Food Cues and the Desire to Eat in Dieters and Non-Dieters The Huron University College Journal of Learning and Motivation Volume 47 Issue 1 Article 7 2009 Food Cues and the Desire to Eat in Dieters and Non-Dieters Jennifer White Follow this and additional works

More information

Reducing Chocolate Craving

Reducing Chocolate Craving Reducing Chocolate Craving Van Gucht, D. et al. (2007). Molly Briley, Ally Guba, Cailin Holt, Nathan Liu, Addie Petterson Introduction Food craving is defined as an intense desire or longing to eat a particular

More information

Ogden, J, Karim, L. Choudry, A., and Brown, K. (2007). Understanding successful

Ogden, J, Karim, L. Choudry, A., and Brown, K. (2007). Understanding successful 1 Ogden, J, Karim, L. Choudry, A., and Brown, K. (2007). Understanding successful behaviour change: the role of intentions, attitudes to the target and motivations and the example of diet. Health Education

More information

21 st November

21 st November 21 st November 2013 Structure What do consumers eat? Prawn nutrition Dietary reference values (HP diets) Appetite control Future research sustainability and health UK Protein intake top 10 (from NDNS survey)

More information

Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up

Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 3060 3064 Short communication Randomized controlled trial of physical activity counseling as an aid to smoking cessation: 12 month follow-up Michael Ussher a,, Robert West

More information

HEDONIC HUNGER AND SELF-CONTROL: THE IMPACT OF PALATABILITY, POWER OF FOOD AND DIETARY RESTRAINT ON SELF-CONTROL DEPLETION. Kathleen M.

HEDONIC HUNGER AND SELF-CONTROL: THE IMPACT OF PALATABILITY, POWER OF FOOD AND DIETARY RESTRAINT ON SELF-CONTROL DEPLETION. Kathleen M. HEDONIC HUNGER AND SELF-CONTROL: THE IMPACT OF PALATABILITY, POWER OF FOOD AND DIETARY RESTRAINT ON SELF-CONTROL DEPLETION Kathleen M. Young A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling

More information

Multiple Daily Injection (MDI) & Carbohydrate (CHO) Counting Assessment Tool

Multiple Daily Injection (MDI) & Carbohydrate (CHO) Counting Assessment Tool Multiple Daily Injection (MDI) & Carbohydrate (CHO) Counting Assessment Tool (for patients using analogue insulin) The overall aim of this questionnaire is to ensure that you have the knowledge required

More information

Small. c h a n g e s big. benefits

Small. c h a n g e s big. benefits Small c h a n g e s big benefits Did you know that 3 in 5 adults in Northern Ireland weigh too much? Being overweight increases the risk of health problems, including heart disease, some cancers, diabetes

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

SLENDESTA POTATO EXTRACT PROMOTES SATIETY IN HEALTHY HUMAN SUBJECTS: IOWA STATE UNIVERSITY STUDY Sheila Dana, Michael Louie, Ph.D. and Jiang Hu, Ph.D.

SLENDESTA POTATO EXTRACT PROMOTES SATIETY IN HEALTHY HUMAN SUBJECTS: IOWA STATE UNIVERSITY STUDY Sheila Dana, Michael Louie, Ph.D. and Jiang Hu, Ph.D. SLENDESTA POTATO EXTRACT PROMOTES SATIETY IN HEALTHY HUMAN SUBJECTS: IOWA STATE UNIVERSITY STUDY Sheila Dana, Michael Louie, Ph.D. and Jiang Hu, Ph.D. INTRODUCTION KEY CONCLUSIONS Excessive calorie intake

More information

From stomachs to minds: Why cognition is key to understanding food intake

From stomachs to minds: Why cognition is key to understanding food intake From stomachs to minds: Why cognition is key to understanding food intake Jeff Brunstrom ILSI Annual Meeting 2016 Disclosure Appetite and energy balancing 65 kg woman Energy stored in fat tissue = 55

More information

Is This a Meal or Snack? Environmental Cues or Timing that Drive Food Intake. Brian Wansink. Mitsuru Shimizu. Collin R. Payne

Is This a Meal or Snack? Environmental Cues or Timing that Drive Food Intake. Brian Wansink. Mitsuru Shimizu. Collin R. Payne MEAL OR SNACK? 1 Running head: FOOD PERCEPTION AND HUNGER Is This a Meal or Snack? Environmental Cues or Timing that Drive Food Intake Brian Wansink Mitsuru Shimizu Collin R. Payne Word count: 2,987 Corresponding

More information

An Investigation into Carbohydrate Cravings and BMI in Women with Polycystic Ovary Syndrome

An Investigation into Carbohydrate Cravings and BMI in Women with Polycystic Ovary Syndrome Cronicon OPEN ACCESS NUTRITION Research Article Agnes J Morrell* An Investigation into Carbohydrate Cravings and BMI in Women with Polycystic Ovary Syndrome Department of Life Sciences, University of Roehampton,

More information

Fitness. Nutritional Support for your Training Program.

Fitness. Nutritional Support for your Training Program. Fitness Nutritional Support for your Training Program www.inovacure.com Fitness You should not have to diet constantly to maintain your weight. In fact, the best way to maintain your weight over the long

More information

Advice on taking enzyme replacement capsules

Advice on taking enzyme replacement capsules Advice on taking enzyme replacement capsules Hepatobiliary Services Information for Patients i University Hospitals of Leicester NHS Trust Introduction What is the pancreas? The pancreas is part of your

More information

Appetite 58 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Appetite. journal homepage:

Appetite 58 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Appetite. journal homepage: Appetite 58 (2012) 6 10 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Appetite journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/appet Research report Impulsivity in the supermarket. Responses to calorie

More information

CONTENTS Importance of sports nutrition The basics of sports nutrition Breakfast Lunch Dinner Snacks Fluids Eating before exercise

CONTENTS Importance of sports nutrition The basics of sports nutrition Breakfast Lunch Dinner Snacks Fluids Eating before exercise NUTRITION CONTENTS 1. Importance of sports nutrition 2. The basics of sports nutrition 3. Breakfast 4. Lunch 5. Dinner 6. Snacks 7. Fluids 8. Eating before exercise 9. Preparation for competition/training

More information

Through the Eyes of Dieters: Biased Size Perception of Food Following Tempting Food Primes

Through the Eyes of Dieters: Biased Size Perception of Food Following Tempting Food Primes Accepted Manuscript Through the Eyes of Dieters: Biased Size Perception of Food Following Tempting Food Primes Guido M. van Koningsbruggen, Wolfgang Stroebe, Henk Aarts PII: S0022-1031(10)00229-5 DOI:

More information

My Weight Loss Contract 2009

My Weight Loss Contract 2009 My Weight Loss Contract 2009 Personal Commitment Statement I, confirm my commitment to the goals and weight loss plan below, and agree to make the necessary changes to my lifestyle to improve my health

More information

University of Groningen. Has food lost its attraction in anorexia nervosa? Neimeijer, Renate Antonia Maria

University of Groningen. Has food lost its attraction in anorexia nervosa? Neimeijer, Renate Antonia Maria University of Groningen Has food lost its attraction in anorexia nervosa? Neimeijer, Renate Antonia Maria IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish

More information

What Should I Eat to Help my Pressure Sore or Wound Heal?

What Should I Eat to Help my Pressure Sore or Wound Heal? What Should I Eat to Help my Pressure Sore or Wound Heal? Information for Patients i UHL Nutrition and Dietetic Service UHL Tissue Viability Team Introduction If you have a pressure sore or a large wound

More information

The eatwell plate is based on the Government s Eight Guidelines for a Healthy Diet, which are:

The eatwell plate is based on the Government s Eight Guidelines for a Healthy Diet, which are: The eatwell plate The eatwell plate is a pictorial food guide showing the proportion and types of foods that are needed to make up a healthy balanced diet. The plate has been produced by the Food Standards

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

You do need to be aware of Hidden fats in your diet that will increase the calories you are eating, for example

You do need to be aware of Hidden fats in your diet that will increase the calories you are eating, for example The following are some general guidelines that were developed for football players, but as referees at the highest level often cover the same distances and running intensities in games as players these

More information

Nutrition Assessment 62 y/o BF; 155 lb (70.5 kg); 5 1 (152.4 cm); BMI 29 (overweight); IBW 105 lb, %IBW 148 (obese)

Nutrition Assessment 62 y/o BF; 155 lb (70.5 kg); 5 1 (152.4 cm); BMI 29 (overweight); IBW 105 lb, %IBW 148 (obese) Nutrition Assessment 62 y/o BF; 155 lb (70.5 kg); 5 1 (152.4 cm); BMI 29 (overweight); IBW 105 lb, %IBW 148 (obese) Requirements Energy: 1187 (REE-MSJ) x 1.2 (AF) = 1424 kcals = 1400-1500 kcals/day Protein:

More information

Eaters In The Dark: The Primacy of Cognitive Factors For Food Consumption And Satiety

Eaters In The Dark: The Primacy of Cognitive Factors For Food Consumption And Satiety Eaters In The Dark: The Primacy of Cognitive Factors For Food Consumption And Satiety Benjamin Scheibehenne (bscheibe@indiana.edu) Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Lentzeallee 94 14195 Berlin,

More information

How to improve your food and drink intake if you have a poor appetite

How to improve your food and drink intake if you have a poor appetite 5800 - How to increase your food and drink intake Leaflet June 2018.qxp_Layout 1 07/06/2018 11:07 Page 2 How to improve your food and drink intake if you have a poor appetite BREAD - RICE - POTATOES -

More information

Motivating operations in appetite research

Motivating operations in appetite research Appetite 45 (2005) 95 107 www.elsevier.com/locate/appet Research Review Motivating operations in appetite research Katy Tapper Cardiff Institute of Society, Health and Ethics, Cardiff University, 53 Park

More information

Can(not) Take my Eyes off it: Attention Bias for Food in Overweight Participants

Can(not) Take my Eyes off it: Attention Bias for Food in Overweight Participants Health Psychology Can(not) Take my Eyes off it: Attention Bias for Food in Overweight Participants Jessica Werthmann, Anne Roefs, Chantal Nederkoorn, Karin Mogg, Brendan P. Bradley, and Anita Jansen Online

More information

WHOLE: Wellbeing and Healthy Choices for Older Adults and their Carers

WHOLE: Wellbeing and Healthy Choices for Older Adults and their Carers WHOLE: Wellbeing and Healthy Choices for Older Adults and their Carers Malnutrition in Older Age This dietary information fact sheet is intended for informational purposes only. It is not a substitute

More information

University of Groningen

University of Groningen University of Groningen Validity of self-reported weight and height and predictors of weight bias in female college students Larsen, Junilla K.; Ouwens, Machteld; Engels, Rutger C.M.E.; Eisinga, Rob; Strien,

More information

An easy guide for finding the right balance for you

An easy guide for finding the right balance for you An easy guide for finding the right balance for you Getting portion size right for you We re all individuals with different needs. But, for healthy adults the types of different food and drinks we need

More information

Mindfulness Action-Based Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for Concurrent Binge Eating Disorder and Substance Use Disorders

Mindfulness Action-Based Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for Concurrent Binge Eating Disorder and Substance Use Disorders Mindfulness Action-Based Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for Concurrent Binge Eating Disorder and Substance Use Disorders Leah Shapira, M.A. Christine M. Courbasson, Ph.D., C.Psych Yasunori Nishikawa, Hon.

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL FLEXIBILITY MEDIATES CHANGES IN INTUITIVE EATING IN ACT- INTERVENTIOS

PSYCHOLOGICAL FLEXIBILITY MEDIATES CHANGES IN INTUITIVE EATING IN ACT- INTERVENTIOS PSYCHOLOGICAL FLEXIBILITY MEDIATES CHANGES IN INTUITIVE EATING IN ACT- INTERVENTIOS Essi Sairanen Department of psychology University of Jyväskylä Finland What works in weight management Strong control

More information

Portion Size: What We Know and What We Need to Know

Portion Size: What We Know and What We Need to Know Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition ISSN: 1040-8398 (Print) 1549-7852 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/bfsn20 Portion Size: What We Know and What We Need to Know David

More information

Sport and Exercise Science

Sport and Exercise Science Pearson BTEC Level 3 National in Sport and Exercise Science Unit 13: Nutrition for Sport and Exercise Performance Sample Assessment Materials (SAMs) For use with Extended Diploma in Sport and Exercise

More information

Meal Menu Approximate Amount Eaten

Meal Menu Approximate Amount Eaten Meal Menu Approximate Amount Eaten Myself 16 Year Old Active Male Teen 3 Year Old Female Child ¼ cup 1 slice 70 Year Old Female Breakfast Special K Cereal(red berries) 1 cup 1 Banana (slices) ¼ cup ¾ cup

More information

Available from Deakin Research Online:

Available from Deakin Research Online: This is the published version: Richardson, Ben and Fuller Tyszkiewicz, Matthew 2014, The application of non linear multilevel models to experience sampling data, European health psychologist, vol. 16,

More information

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers.

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers. Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers. 1 This work is a result of the combined efforts of myself and my research advisor, Dr.

More information

Outline. Types of Bariatric Surgery. Adjustable Gastric Band (LAP-BAND) Bariatric surgery

Outline. Types of Bariatric Surgery. Adjustable Gastric Band (LAP-BAND) Bariatric surgery Bariatric surgery Rona Osborne, Specialist Obesity Dietitian Glasgow and Clyde Weight Management Service November 2012- Weight Management Training Outline Types of Bariatric surgery Evidence Clinical Guidelines

More information

Dual process models point towards an interaction between topdown control and bottom-up influences.

Dual process models point towards an interaction between topdown control and bottom-up influences. Dual process models point towards an interaction between topdown control and bottom-up influences. Research suggests obesity is linked to low self-control combined with high reward sensitivity. Childhood

More information

The revised short-form of the Eating Beliefs Questionnaire: Measuring positive, negative, and permissive beliefs about binge eating

The revised short-form of the Eating Beliefs Questionnaire: Measuring positive, negative, and permissive beliefs about binge eating Burton and Abbott Journal of Eating Disorders (2018) 6:37 https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-018-0224-0 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access The revised short-form of the Eating Beliefs Questionnaire: Measuring positive,

More information

Published in final edited form in Eating Behaviors, 12(2), pp , doi: /j.eatbeh

Published in final edited form in Eating Behaviors, 12(2), pp , doi: /j.eatbeh Accepted Manuscript Enhanced behavioral inhibition in restrained eaters Adrian Meule, Steve Lukito, Claus Vögele, Andrea Kübler PII: S1471-0153(11)00007-9 DOI: doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.01.006 Reference:

More information

DIABETES AND CORONARY HEART DISEASE RISK MANAGEMENT

DIABETES AND CORONARY HEART DISEASE RISK MANAGEMENT Lesley Eccott, Community Learning Disability Nurse. November, 2003 DIABETES AND CORONARY HEART DISEASE RISK MANAGEMENT Name D.O.B Address Hospital Number CLDN Diabetic Nurse Consultant Care Manager C.S.W.

More information

Body mass index and age affect Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire scores in male subjects

Body mass index and age affect Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire scores in male subjects Body mass index and age affect Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire scores in male subjects HARDEN, Charlotte, CORFE, B. A., RICHARDSON, J. C., DETTMAR, P. W. and PAXMAN, J. R.

More information

The interactive effect of hunger and impulsivity on food intake and purchase in a virtual supermarket

The interactive effect of hunger and impulsivity on food intake and purchase in a virtual supermarket (2009) 33, 905 912 & 2009 Macmillan Publishers Limited All rights reserved 0307-0565/09 $32.00 www.nature.com/ijo ORIGINAL ARTICLE The interactive effect of hunger and impulsivity on food intake and purchase

More information

OxSalt. Lowering Salt for Health. A guide to reducing salt in our diet

OxSalt. Lowering Salt for Health. A guide to reducing salt in our diet OxSalt Lowering Salt for Health A guide to reducing salt in our diet Version 1.3 1st June 2012 How to Reduce Salt in our Diet What is salt? Salt is a chemical made up of sodium and chloride. Food labels

More information

Metabolic Typing Test

Metabolic Typing Test Metabolic Typing Test Adapted from The Metabolic Typing Diet (Wolcott and Fahey 2000, 135), this simple test is the most basic way to determine your metabolic type. Circle the letter of the phrase that

More information

Appetite 51 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Appetite. journal homepage:

Appetite 51 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Appetite. journal homepage: Appetite 51 (2008) 570 575 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Appetite journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/appet Research report From the Garden of Eden to the land of plenty Restriction of

More information

1 What Are the Different Food Groups?

1 What Are the Different Food Groups? 1 What Are the Different Food Groups? 1. Match the food items below to where they are found in the supermarket. The first one has been done for you. Food item Section Oil Meat Fruits and vegetables Grains

More information

MULTIPLE CHOICE. Choose the one alternative that best completes the statement or answers the question.

MULTIPLE CHOICE. Choose the one alternative that best completes the statement or answers the question. MULTIPLE CHOICE. Choose the one alternative that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1) The four characteristics of a healthful diet are adequacy, balance, moderation, and A) value. B)

More information

Childhood Obesity. Geoffrey Axiak REGISTERED NUTRITIONIST

Childhood Obesity. Geoffrey Axiak REGISTERED NUTRITIONIST Childhood Obesity Geoffrey Axiak M.Sc. Nursing (Manchester), B.Sc. Nursing, P.G. Dip. Nutrition & Dietetics, Dip. Public Management, Cert. Clinical Nutrition (Leeds) REGISTERED NUTRITIONIST What is obesity?

More information

P105 STUDY REPORT A Double Blind Placebo Controlled Trial of the Effect of AppTrim

P105 STUDY REPORT A Double Blind Placebo Controlled Trial of the Effect of AppTrim P105 STUDY REPORT A Double Blind Placebo Controlled Trial of the Effect of AppTrim on Appetite Suppression and Obesity Management Targeted Medical Pharma Inc. 1 6/9/2011 12:41 PM Executive Summary Study

More information