Traits and Metatraits: Their Reliability, Stability, and Shared Genetic Influence

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Traits and Metatraits: Their Reliability, Stability, and Shared Genetic Influence"

Transcription

1 PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Traits and Metatraits: Their Reliability, Stability, and Shared Genetic Influence Scott L. Hershberger and Robert Plomin Pennsylvania State University Nancy L. Pedersen The Karolinska Institute Metatraits measure individual differences in construct relevancy, whereas traits measure individual differences in construct extremity. Twenty-four traits and metatraits were examined in this study using 57 pairs of identical twins reared together, 95 pairs of identical twins reared apart, pairs of fraternal twins reared together, and 8 pairs of fraternal twins reared apart obtained from the Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging (see N. L. Pedersen et al., 99). Reliability and stability analyses of the metatraits revealed generally lower reliability and stability across time compared to traits. Quantitative genetic analyses of the relationship between traits and metatraits indicated that approximately 69% of the metatrait's genetic variance is shared with the trait, with 3% of its genetic variance unique to the metatrait. These results suggest that metatraits provide a useful additional view of personality. The most common measure of individual differences in personality research has undoubtedly been the total scale score obtained on an inventory or questionnaire. This total scale score, formed by the sum of item responses, serves as a measure of an individual's standing on a trait. Although the conclusion has been vigorously contested (e.g., Epstein, 983), some researchers have asserted that the use of total scale scores for the investigation of personality, particularly for the prediction of behavior, has been less than productive (e.g., Mischel, 968). Bern and Allen (974) suggested that the source of this predictive difficulty might rest with the relevancy of the personality trait for the individual's behavior. According to Bern and Allen, if a person is behaviorally consistent across situations, the personality trait responsible for the behavior may be more relevant to this person than to a behaviorally inconsistent person. As one measure of consistency, Bern and Allen calculated an ipsatized variance index, or the variance of item responses on one trait scale divided by the total variance across all items on several trait Scott L. Hershberger and Robert Plomin, Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University; Nancy L. Pedersen, The Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden. The Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging (SATSA) is an ongoing study conducted at the Department of Epidemiology of the Institute for Environmental Medicine at the Karolinska Institute in Stockholm in collaboration with the Research Center for Developmental and Health Genetics at Pennsylvania State University. SATSA is supported in part by grants from the National Institute on Aging (AG-4563 and AG- IO 75), the Swedish Social Research Council, and the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Aging. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Scott L. Hershberger, who is now at the Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, 46 Fraser Hall, Lawrence, Kansas scales. In their study, the ipsatized variance index proved successful in differentiating individuals who were cross-situationally consistent from those who were inconsistent in conscientiousness, but it was not successful for the trait of friendliness. Various efforts to reproduce and extend Bern and Allen's results have met with modest success (e.g., Chaplin & Goldberg, 985). Some researchers have pointed to statistical difficulties with the ipsatized index itself, among which is the confounding of item variance caused by the trait with the interitem variance of all other measured traits (e.g., Paunonen & Jackson, 985). Others have claimed that measures of consistency possess little discriminant validity from total scale scores (e.g., Burke, Kraut, & Dworkin, 984). Yet another problem exists with the correlation between the total scale score and ipsatized variance index, which is frequently not linear but curvilinear because of the restrictions placed on the extremity of the total scale scores of inconsistently responding persons (Paunonen, 988). Nonetheless, recent reviews of attempts to use behavioral consistency as a measure of trait relevancy have found some promise in the approach (Paunonen, 988; Tellegen, 988). Indeed, the work of Baumeister and Tice (988; Baumeister, 99; Tice, 989) has been particularly supportive of the usefulness of behavioral consistency as an index of trait relevancy. Baumeister and Tice (988) proposed the term metatrait to describe that "trait of having versus not having a particular trait." A metatrait is measured by the simple standard deviation of an individual's item responses, thus avoiding the problems attendant on the Bern and Allen ipsatized variance index. Although not without its critics (e.g.,. Paunonen, 988), the interitem standard deviation index has proved useful in many contexts (Tice, 989). Metatraits, as measured by the interitem standard deviation index, have been shown to mediate the relationship between total scale score (locus of control) and a behavioral criterion (persistence on a laboratory task) and, when the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 995, Vol. 69, No. 4, Copyright 995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. -354/95/S3 673

2 674 S. HERSHBERGER, R. PLOMIN, AND N. PEDERSEN metatrait is self-esteem, to predict a behavioral criterion (advice seeking) directly (Baumeister & Tice, 988). Metatraits have also been shown to be stable across time and to be equally reliable for "traited" and "untraited" individuals (Baumeister, 99). However, the reliability estimates were restricted to test-retest correlations. Reliability estimates obtained from test-retest correlations commonly confound the reliability of a trait with its stability across time. Behavioral genetic studies of personality scale scores have revealed a substantial degree of genetic influence on personality, with heritabilities ranging typically between.5 and.5 (Bouchard & McGue, 99; Plomin, Chipuer, & Loehlin, 99). However, the heritabilities of metatraits are unknown. Waller and Reise (99) examined genetic influence on individual differences in scalability, a concept related to but distinct from the concept of metatraits. Scalability refers to the degree to which an individual's item response pattern conforms to a perfect Guttman scale. Although an extreme degree of item response inconsistency is incompatible with a perfect Guttman scale, two individuals with item patterns conforming identically to a Guttman scale may nonetheless demonstrate dramatically different degrees of response variability. One goal of the present study was to examine genetic and environmental influences on metatraits. A second goal was to address the important issues of the reliability and stability of metatraits. Baumeister's (99) study addressed the two latter issues with the correlation between metatrait scores assessed at two time periods several weeks apart. The current study includes three periods of measurement spanning 6 years, thus allowing for a more refined approach to reliability and stability estimation. A third goal was to examine the correlation between trait and metatrait scores. If, as Burke et al. (984) claim, metatraits essentially provide no information in addition to that provided by the level of a trait, the metatrait and trait level should be highly correlated. If this is the case, most of the genetic influence affecting the metatrait should be shared with the trait, with little genetic influence affecting the metatrait uniquely. If metatraits capture an aspect of personality not captured by the trait level, a significant proportion of the genetic variance affecting the metatrait should be unique to the metatrait and not shared with the trait. Participants Method The data for this study were obtained from the Swedish Adoption/ Twin Study of Aging (SATSA; see Pedersen et al., 99), a longitudinal study of the cognitive, personality, and health characteristics of a sample of twins. The twins were identified from the Swedish Twin Registry, which includes nearly 5, pairs of same-sex twins born in Sweden between 886 and 958 (Cederlof & Lorich, 978). The twins reared together were matched to the twins reared apart on the basis of age, gender, and county of birth. For the twins reared apart, the average age of separation was.8 years; 48% were separated before their first birthday and 8% before their fifth. None of the twin pairs were separated after the age of. The first wave of testing was conducted in two parts in 984. Responses to the first questionnaire were received from 74 intact twin pairs: 99 identical twins reared apart (MZA), 66 identical twins reared together (MZT), 38 fraternal twins reared apart (DZA), and fraternal twins reared together (DZT). A second questionnaire was mailed directly after receipt of the first; responses were received from 576 intact twin pairs: 83 MZA, 36 MZT, 8 DZA, and 75 DZT. The second wave of testing was conducted in 987, with a single questionnaire sent to all individuals who had not requested discontinuation of participation. Responses were received from 53 intact twin pairs: 7 MZA, 7 MZT, 78 DZA, and 55 DZT. An identical questionnaire was used for the third wave of testing in 99. Responses were received from 474 intact twin pairs: 6 MZA, 4 MZT, 53 DZA, and 45 DZT. The average age of the twins was 58.6 years at the first wave of testing, ranging from 6 to 87 years. Forty percent of the sample was male and 6% female. Initially, zygosity status was based on the physical similarity reported at the first wave. All twins older than 5 years, both of whom had responded to the first wave's questionnaires, were approached for participation in a later health interview; during this health interview, in which approximately 5% of the first-wave twin pairs participated, all had their zygosity status determined by comparing serum proteins and red cell enzymes from blood assays. Zygosity was changed for 8% of the sample on the basis of the assay results. Subsequent analyses comparing behavioral genetic results before and after the zygosity changes showed little difference in results. Further information concerning SATSA may be found in Pedersen et al. (99). Measures Each of the measures used in this study was translated into Swedish by a professional translator and back-translated into English to ensure that the meaning of the original items was retained. Short form of the Eysenck Personality Inventory. The short form of the Eysenck Personality Inventory (Floderus, 974) was used to measure extraversion and neuroticism. Each scale consists of nine items, with a yes or no response format. Internal consistency as measured by Cronbach's a was.66 for extraversion and.75 for neuroticism (Pedersen, Plomin, McClearn, & Friberg, 988). EAS Temperament Survey. The EAS Temperament Survey for adults (Buss & Plomin, 984) assesses the temperament traits of distress, fear, anger (the three composing an emotionality factor), activity, and sociability. Each trait is assessed with four items answered on a 5- point Likert scale. Buss and Plomin reported average -week test-retest reliability estimates of.8 for emotionality,.8 for activity, and.85 for sociability. Karolinska scales oflmpulsivity, Monotony Avoidance, and Inhibition of Aggression. Three scales were selected from the Karolinska Scales of Personality (Schalling, Edman, & Asberg, 983) impulsivity, monotony avoidance, and inhibition of aggression each measured on a 5-point Likert scale. The impulsivity scale measures the individual's propensity to act on the spur of the moment (e.g., "I tend to speak first and think afterwards"). The monotony avoidance scale measures the individual's desire to seek out novelty and excitement (e.g., "I am drawn to places where exciting things happen"). Cronbach's a is.7 for impulsivity and.76 for monotony avoidance (Pedersen et al., 988). The inhibition of aggression scale is more accurately conceived of as a measure of assertiveness rather than inhibited aggression, with items such as, "If I'm treated badly at a restaurant I don't like to complain" and "I find it difficult to refuse when someone asks me to do something for them even if I don't feel like doing it." Cronbach's a for inhibition of aggression is.69 (Pedersen, Lichtenstein etal., 989). Framingham hard-driving. Five items, responded to on a 5-point Likert scale, were adopted from the Framingham scale of Type A Behavior (Haynes, Levine, Scotch, Feinleib, & Kanner, 978) to assess the trait of hard-driving. The hard-driving items measure the demands that people put on themselves in their interactions with others (e.g., "I can be described as domineering"). Cronbach's a for the hard-driving scale is.7 (Pedersen, Lichtenstein, et al., 989)., life direction, and responsibility. Twenty-three forced-choice items from the Rotter (966) Locus of Control Scale were converted to

3 METATRAITS Likert-type items; of these, items (4 for each) were selected to measure the traits of, Life Direction, and. These three scales previously were identified by Klockars and Varnum (975) in a factor analysis of Rotter's scale. The items assess the individual's belief in the importance of luck in determining life events (e.g., "Most people don't realize to what extent their lives are ruled by coincidences"). The Life Direction items assess the individual's belief in personal control over life events (e.g., "When I make plans I'm almost certain that I can follow them through"). The items are concerned with personal responsibility for not having a successful career or friendships ("People who are disliked don't know how to get on with others"). Cronbach's as for, Life Direction, and are.55,.49, and.66, respectively (Pedersen, Gatz, Plomin, Nesselroade, &McClearn,989). NEO openness. A shortened version of the to Experience scale from the NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 985) was used. Of the original 48 items, 6 were retained. The validity of the shortened to Experience scale was assessed by its correlation with an adjective checklist, a method employed by McCrae and Costa (985) for the original to Experience scale. The correlations for the short and long versions of the scale did not differ significantly. Cronbach's a is.77 for the to Experience scale (Bergeman et al., 993). OARS depression, somatic symptom, and alienation. The Short Psychiatric Evaluation Schedule from the OARS (Duke University, 978) comprises three scales depression, somatic symptom, and alienation each assessed with five yes or no response-type items. The depression items are concerned with mood and sleep patterns; the somatic-symptom items, with perceived healthfulness; and the alienation items, with difficulty in interpersonal relations. Gatz, Pedersen, and Harris (987) cite internal consistency (Cronbach's a) estimates of.57 for depression,.64 for somatic symptom, and.7 for alienation. Cook-Medley hostility. The original Cook-Medley Scale (Cook & Medley, 954) contains 5 items; however, items were retained on the basis of a factor analysis in which 5 items loaded highly on a Paranoid factor, and 5 items loaded on a Cynicism factor. These items were combined to create a hostility scale, each with a 5- point Likert-scale format. Examples include, for Paranoid, "I have a feeling that I've often been punished unjustly," and for Cynicism, "I think most people would lie to get ahead." Cronbach's a for the hostility scale is (Pedersen, Lichtenstein, et al., 989).. was measured by 3 items, scored on a 5-point Likert-type scale, taken from Wood, Wylie, and Sheafor (969). One example is, "Most of my expectations have been filled." Cronbach's a for this scale is.8. State anxiety. Three scales, adapted from Spielberger (979), were used to measure state anxiety: a 5-item state anxiety scale referring to positive affective states (e.g., "I feel relaxed"); a 5-item state anxiety scale referring to negative affective states (e.g., "I feel nervous"); and a total state anxiety scale that sums the positive and negative scales. Responses were obtained on a 5-point Likert-type scale. Metatrait scales. The metatrait scales corresponding to each of the 3 traits described above were constructed by calculating the standard deviation of item responses for each individual. In addition, a metatrait factor was created by conducting a principal components analysis of of the metatrait scales (leaving out state anxiety positive and negative because of their dependency on the state anxiety total score) and retaining the first component. This first principal component accounted for % of the variance at the first occasion, 5% of the variance at the second occasion, and 6% of the variance at the third occasion. The metatrait factor represents the general behavioral consistency of the individual. However, given the great heterogeneity in the personality scales used in this study, a comparable component for the trait scale has no meaning and was not created. Figure. Reliability and stability model across three occasions of measurement for metatraits and trait levels, where X r = the true score; X o = the observed score; r» x = the square root of the reliability coefficient; j8 = the stability coefficient; and e - random error. Model Fitting Two models were evaluated in the present study: The first model assesses the reliability and stability of trait and metatrait scores; the second was a bivariate behavioral genetic model that indicates the degree to which the observed correlation between traits and metatraits is due to shared genetic and environmental influences. Reliability and stability model. The model used to estimate the reliability (r xx ) of the trait and metatrait scores is shown in Figure. In this model, adapted from Heise (969), at each of the three occasions, the observed score (X o ) is a linear function of the true score (X T ) and random measurement error (e). The relationship between the true scores across time is represented by ft the coefficient of stability. This model assumes, in accordance with the classical test theory model (e.g., Allen & Yen, 979), that random measurement error is uncorrelated with true scores and uncorrelated with the random measurement error from other occasions. Although /3, and fe may be unequal, the reliability of the scores is assumed constant across time. In addition, the disturbances from the second and third occasions are also assumed to be equal. The model may then be tested with one (fe # /8 3 ) or two (/3 = /3 3 ) degrees of freedom. The model of Figure was evaluated by the model-fitting program LISREL 8 (Joreskog & Sorbom, 993), which provides maximum-likelihood estimates of the parameters. Bivariate behavioral genetic model. The linear model underlying quantitative genetic theory posits that the variance in a phenotype may be partitioned into genetic and environmental sources (Plomin, De- Fries, & McClearn, 99). When two or more phenotypes are analyzed simultaneously, the degree of shared genetic and environmental sources among the phenotypes can be determined as well. In this study, a bivariate model-fitting analysis was conducted on the covariance between trait and metatrait scores to determine (a) the proportion of variance attributable uniquely to genetic and environmental sources for metatraits, and (b) the extent of shared genetic and environmental sources of variance between trait and metatrait scores. To conduct a quantitative genetic analysis, the correlations between relatives of varying genetic relatedness are required. When the relatives have been exposed to differing environments, further information of the effect of the environment is obtained. With MZ and DZ twins reared apart and together, the SATS A study provides a powerful design to assess genetic and environmental parameters. The first parameter, additive genetic effects (A), represents the additive (or summative) influence of the multiple genes affecting the phenotype. Identical twins share all their genetic effects and have a unit correlation for additive genetic effects; fraternal twins share, on average, 5% of their genes and are correlated.5 for additive genetic effects. The second parameter, nonadditive genetic effects, or dominance

4 676 S. HERSHBERGER, R. PLOMIN, AND N. PEDERSEN MZ= DZ=.5 Trait Level Score Metatrait Score Metatrait Score Trait Level Score MZ= DZ=.5 Figure. Bivariate path model showing common and unique factors for genetic and environmental sources of variance and covariance for trait levels and metatraits. Latent variables Ens, A, D, and Es refer to nonshared environmental, additive genetic, nonadditive genetic, and shared rearing environmental effects, respectively. All primed (') symbols refer to the co-twin. (D), refers to the nonlinear interaction between alleles at the same locus. Identical twins are again correlated unity for nonadditive dominance effects, whereas fraternal twins are, on average, correlated.5 for nonadditive dominance effects. In general, significant genetic effects on a phenotype are indicated when the identical twin correlation significantly exceeds the fraternal twin correlation. When the concern is with the genetic mediation between two phenotypes, a greater identical twin cross-correlation (the correlation between one twin's metatrait score and the co-twin's trait scale score) is indicative of significant shared genetic effects. The third parameter, shared-rearing environmental effects (Es), refers to environmental factors that produce similarities in siblings reared together. By definition, twins reared together, MZT and DZT, are correlated unity for shared-rearing environmental effects; twins reared apart, MZA and DZA, are correlated zero. Thus, if the shared-rearing environment is significant, the average of the MZT and DZT correlations should exceed the average of the MZA and DZA correlations. Analogously, if the average of the MZT and DZT cross-correlations exceeds the average of the MZA and DZA cross-correlations, the correlation between the two phenotypes is mediated by significant shared-rearing environmental effects. The fourth parameter, nonshared environmental effects (Ens), refers to environmental effects that contribute to a lack of twin resemblance. The correlation for each of the twin types attributable to nonshared environmental effects is therefore zero. Unsystematic measurement error (unreliability), itself an effect that cannot contribute to the correlation between the twins, must by necessity inflate the estimate of the nonshared environment when no information is included in the analysis concerning reliability of measurement. Figure presents the bivariate model of trait and metatrait scores. The bivariate (or Cholesky) model is commonly used in quantitative genetic analyses (Neale & Cardon, 99) and allows for the decomposition of the correlation between two phenotypes into components of variance shared between the phenotypes and unique to one of the phenotypes. It should be noted that only sources of variance unique to one phenotype are identifiable in the Cholesky model with the four twin types; the metatraits were selected for the identification of unique effects because of the emphasis of this study o"n the properties of metatraits. Each of the four parameters discussed previously is included in the model. Path coefficients subscripted with represent effects unique to the metatrait scores. Path coefficients subscripted with or represent effects emanating from factors shared by the two phenotypes. Thus, the model of Figure implies the following variances of the traits and metatraits, as well as their covariance: Var, Var Ttai t = n u + a u + dn + s n. () = «l + fl i + ^ + * + «+ <Z + ^ +.S - () CV T + S,,S I. (3) The model of Figure also implies the following twin covariance traits (Equations 4 through 7) and twin covariances for the metatraits (Equations 8 through ): Cov M a,, + d n Cov DZTTniii =.5a n d n (4) (5)

5 METATRAITS 677 COV DZAM, = an + d x s l + a d + s.5a i + s l +.5fl + -5d + s = a l + d, + a + d.5a.5d t.5a.5d +d u d l +s n s i. Cov DZTltailJiltuli>it =.5a, ifl i + -5 d,, d x i +d u d l.5d u d l. (7) (8) (9) () () Specific to bivariate models are the cross-twin covariances between the two phenotypes: () (3) (4) (5) The model of Figure was also evaluated with LISREL 8. Covariances between the twin pairs, rather than correlations, were used in the model-fitting analyses. Comprehensive summaries of the univariate analyses of the personality trait scores may be found in Pedersen et al. (99) and Plomin and McClearn (99). Results Reliability and Stability of Metatraits To conduct the reliability and stability analyses of the metatraits, the covariances among the metatrait scores across the three occasions of measurement were required. The variances of the metatrait scores at each occasion, as well as the correlations among the occasions, are shown in Table. Comparable information is given for the traits in Table. The variances and covariances are based on the entire SATSA sample, regardless of whether the twin had a co-twin responding to the personality inventory. In each case, the average of the across-occasion trait correlations exceeds the average of the across-occasion metatrait correlations. However, on average, the metatrait stability correlations are at least moderate, with the metatrait factor exhibiting the highest level of stability. Using LISREL 8, tests were conducted to assess the equality of the variances and covariances of the traits and metatraits across the three occasions. As shown in Table, for of the metatraits, the variance of one occasion differed significantly from that of the other two occasions. In most cases, the largest variance occurred at the first occasion. For the trait variances in Table, all of the 4 traits had one occasion variance that differed significantly from the other two occasion variances, but again, the largest variance appeared most frequently at the first occasion. Not unexpectedly, as shown in Tables and, a majority of the covariances between the first and third occasions were significantly smaller than the two covariances between adjacent occasions. In a few instances, one of the covariances between adjacent occasions significantly exceeded the other two covariances; it is important to note, however, that in no instance was the covariance between the first and third occasions significantly greater than the other occasion covariances. Thus, the data conform to a simplex structure, which stipulates that the correlation between occasions closer together in time must be greater than between occasions further apart in time. Table 3 presents the results of the reliability and stability Table Variances and Intercorrelations Among Metatrait Scores Across Three Occasions Measure N o, Variances o o 3 O, Correlations O 3 o 3 Metatrait factor *.3**.55**.7*.76** **.9** 9 5** ** * **.5..3** ** ** ** * ** ** *.4.56**.533*.374**.4.4**.456**.8.373** **.56**.64** Note. A correlation noted as significant differed significantly from adjacent correlations. *p<. **p<.l.

6 678 S. HERSHBERGER, R. PLOMIN, AND N. PEDERSEN Table Variances and Intercorrelations Among Trait Scores Across Three Occasions Measure N o, Variances o 5.49* * * 5..83*'" 8.784*'*.73.6*"*.6.634*'* *" * *" ' " * *' 3.84* * * ** * " o ** ** ** * ** ** Note. A correlation noted as significant differed significantly from adjacent correlations. *p<. **p<.l. Correlations o **.673*.738**.58*.6*.64**.77** O,,.65**.685**.586**.65**.65** ** *.679** **.45*.479 Table 3 Reliability and Stability of Trait and Metatrait Scores Across Time Metatrait scores Trait scores Measure. ' XX X df r XX /3 X c V Metatrait factor I : >

7 METATRAITS 679 analyses of the trait and metatrait scores. The reliability of a variable is a function of the intercorrelations across the three occasions and is given by ( r ^ ^ / r u, for example, in the case of the neuroticism metatrait, ( X.53)/.44 =.57. The reliabilities of the metatraits vary widely, ranging from.8 for responsibility to for the metatrait factor. The trait reliabilities vary less widely, from.58 for luck to for hostility. For each variable, the reliability of the trait scores exceeded the reliability of the metatrait scores. The stability of the variables is, as in the case of reliability, a function of the correlations between occasions. For example, the stability of the neuroticism metatrait between the first two occasions (/8 i) is given by r 3 /r 3 and between the second and third occasions ( f e ) by r, 3 /r,, or.44/.53 = and.44/ =.9, respectively. The average of these two stability coefficients is equal to.88, the average of the maximum-likelihood estimates of the stability coefficients obtained from the model fitting. For a majority of the variables, the stabilities of the metatrait scores were within points of the stabilities of the trait scores, the trait stabilities generally exceeding the metatrait stabilities. Notably, the metatrait stabilities of life satisfaction, hard-driving, openness, state anxiety positive, and state anxiety negative were more than points lower than the comparable trait stabilities. All of the models fit from Table 3 had nonsignificant chi-squares, indicating that the reliability-stability model fit the data acceptably for all variables. In conclusion, although some of the test-retest reliabilities of the metatraits were barely acceptable, others were close to the generally more reliable trait scale scores. On the other hand, traits and metatraits differ less in stability, with the stability of metatraits on the whole only slightly less than that of traits. Correlation Between Traits and Metatraits One goal of the present analysis was to determine to what extent the correlation between traits and metatraits is mediated genetically. But a question that arises is this: Does the (linear) correlation between traits and metatraits capture adequately the relationship between the two? Past research suggested a strong nonlinear component to the correlation, resulting from the improbability of receiving a high metatrait score at the extremes of the trait level. To the degree that this is so, the linear correlation may underestimate the true relationship, possibly leading to the underestimation of shared genetic influence. Thus, to determine if a significant nonlinear component underlies the relationship between traits and metatraits, hierarchical multiple regressions were conducted, with the metatrait score first regressed onto the trait score alone and, in a second step, regressed onto both the trait score and the square of the trait score to detect the presence of a significant quadratic relationship. The cube of the trait score was added in the equation in a third step to detect a significant cubic relationship. Results from the regression analyses are presented in correlational form in Table 4. The correlations presented in the table are the semipartial correlations between the traits and metatraits from the step at which a term entered the equation. It is important to note that the linear relationship between the trait and metatrait scores has been partialed from the quadratic correlation and that the linear and quadratic have both been partialed from the cubic Table 4 Semipartial Correlations Between Trait and Metatrait Scores Measure 'Quadratic tubic N.54.Ola a a.8a a a.a.4a.a.6a -.a a.4a a.8a -a.7a.7a a Note, p < for all correlations unless indicated otherwise, a = not significant. correlation. This is important because, with the exception of luck, which did not have a significant linear, quadratic, or cubic correlation, each of the quadratic correlations and several of the cubic correlations were significant. Indeed, in most cases, the quadratic term not only added significantly to the regression equation, it also was significantly greater than the value of the linear term. Furthermore, three scales extraversion, anger, and responsibility had a significant quadratic but not a significant linear correlation. For these three variables, the relationship between traits and metatraits resembled an inverted U- shaped function, illustrated most dramatically by extraversion. Figure 3 shows the plot of extraversion metatrait scores against trait scores. The general shape of the function for variables with an additional, significant cubic term did not differ from those with a significant quadratic term, illustrated in Figure 4 by depression. Given the existence of significant nonlinear correlations for nearly all the scales, the question remained whether the magnitude of the shared genetic influence would be underestimated if the linear correlation were used for the analysis. To examine this, modeling analyses were conducted on both the linear and quadratic correlations for extraversion, a trait for which the linear correlation did not differ from zero and the quadratic correlation was near unity, and for life direction, for which the correlations were approximately equal. Behavioral Genetic Model-Fitting Analyses Model-fitting analyses of the model depicted in Figure were conducted with the covariances between twins, computed from a double-entry data file from the first wave of data collection, shown in Table 5. All twin data were corrected for the effects of

8 68 S. HERSHBERGER, R. PLOMIN, AND N. PEDERSEN.6 H I Trait Level i Figure 3. The relationship between extroversion metatrait scores and trait level scores. age, gender, and the Age X Gender interaction within each twin group separately. The standardized maximum-likelihood path coefficients from the modeling analyses are presented in Table 6. The chisquare goodness-of-fit test indicated a good fit of the model for 7 of the 4 variables. Note that the metatrait factor was analyzed under a univariate model. To calculate the proportion of genetic and environmental variance attributable uniquely to the metatrait scores, each of the paths leading to the metatrait were squared and summed. For example, for neuroticism, the proportion of variance.6- caused by additive genetic effects is a i + # ( =.3); the proportion of variance caused by nonadditive effects is cfei + fi? ( =.3); the proportion of variance caused by the shared-rearing environment is 5 i + s (.3 + ) = ; and the proportion of variance caused by nonshared environmental effects is «i + «( =.6). The results of these univariate calculations for the 4 metatraits are presented in Table 7. On average, 5% of the variance in the metatrait scores was attributable to genetic influence (6% to additive effects and 9% to nonadditive effects); 5% to shared-rearing environmental in Trait Level Figure 4. The relationship between depression metatrait scores and trait level scores.

9 METATRAITS 68 Table 5 Twin Intraclass Correlations for the Metatraits Metatrait Metatrait factor MZT (9-57) DZT (9-) MZA (6-95) DZA (3-8) Note The sample sizes refer to twin pairs. Also, the sample sizes for the metatrait factor were somewhat lower in comparison to the other variables: MZT (58); DZT (8); MZA (35); DZA (68). Table 6 Standardized Path Coefficients From Bivariate Analysis of Trait and Metatrait Scores Common factors Unique factors Measure «n On dn S\\ " «4 i S ««dn * X Metatrait factor * 53.44** * * ** 77.7*** * Note. All df= except for the Metatrait factor, for which df= 4. */><. **p<.. ***p<.

10 68 S. HERSHBERGER, R. PLOMIN, AND N. PEDERSEN Table 7 Proportions of Variance Attributable to Genetic and Environmental Sources for Metatraits Metatrait Metatrait factor A D Es 5 Is) Ens Note. A = additive genetic variance; D = dominance variance; E s = shared environmental variance; E m = nonshared environmental variance. fluence; and 8% to nonshared environmental influence. Although the value of 5% for the average degree of variance explained by genetic sources may appear to be on the low side, genetic influence varied dramatically among the metatraits, ranging from a total of 36% for life satisfaction to % for distress, sociability, and openness to experience. Thus, whereas genetic influence may be relatively unimportant for some metatraits (as is the case for some trait scale scores), it is quite important for others. Shared-rearing environment was on the average low, similar to its magnitude for the trait scale score, although the value of % for the sociability metatrait is nearly twice the value found for the sociability scale score (cf. Plomin, Pedersen, McClearn, Nesselroade, & Bergeman, 988). Not unexpectedly, the proportion of variance caused by nonshared environmental effects is high throughout, in part caused by unreliability of measurement. To calculate the proportion of genetic influence that is common to traits and metatraits, and the proportion of genetic variance that is unique to the metatrait, the genetic path coefficients that lead to the metatrait (e.g., a i, a, d l, d ) were squared and summed. This again represents the total amount of genetic variance affecting the metatrait. The square of fhe path coefficients a and d, paths that are unique to the metatrait, is an estimate of the metatrait's genetic variance that is not shared with the trait. Therefore, (a + d )/(a + a i + d + d ) is an estimate of the proportion of genetic variance that is unique to the metatrait. The remainder of the genetic variance is due to genetic influence shared with the trait. For example, the sum of the four squared genetic path coefficients leading to impulsivity (a + a + d, + d or OO ) is.3 (the heritability of the impulsivity metatrait). The square of the genetic paths unique to impulsivity {a + d or ) is.4. Then.4/.3, or 48% of the variance affecting the impulsivity metatrait is unique to the metatrait. The residual, 5%, is the proportion of genetic variance the metatrait shares with the trait. The results of these calculations for the common and unique genetic proportions of variance are shown in Table 8. According to the results of Table 8, an average of approximately 3% of the genetic variance is unique to the metatrait scores; 69% of the genetic variance is held in common with the trait scale scores. The genetic variance unique to the metatrait scores appears to be less than half the magnitude of the genetic variance shared with the trait scores. However, a number of the proportions in Table 8, particularly several % proportions under the column for common variance, are not significant on the basis of the results of chi-square difference tests removing the relevant parameters from the model. For example, although % of the genetic variance is held in common for activity and openness to experience, the total amount of genetic variance is extremely small, 3% and nearly %, respectively. If only the variables with significant common genetic variance are averaged, the average rises from 69% to 88%. On the other hand, if a comparable average is calculated only for variables with significant unique genetic variance, the average rises from 39% to 84%. Notably, only one variable, impulsivity, had both significant common and significant unique genetic variance. Separate modeling analyses were conducted with the covariance between extraversion and life direction metatrait scores and squared trait scale scores (with the linear component par- Table 8 Proportion of Genetic Influence Common to Trait and Metatrait Scores and Unique to Metatrait Scores Measure Common * * * * 66* 7* * * * * 4 86* 78* Note. Significant proportions based on Xdartest, df=. *p<. Unique * 4 8* 6 48* * 86* 9* 4

11 METATRAITS 683 tialed from each). The common path coefficients from the analysis of extraversion were n i = -.8, a i = -, d x = -.49, and S =.4; and the unique coefficients were «= -.8, =, d =., and s =. The proportion of genetic variance unique to the extraversion metatrait is now %, and the proportion of genetic variance held in common with the extraversion scale score is now %, a complete reversal of the results found from analyzing the linear covariance. For life direction, the common path coefficients were n l = -.7, a i = -., d x = -, and s i = -.8; and the unique coefficients were n =.88, a =, d =, and s =.4. Here, % of the genetic variance held is shared between the life direction trait and metatrait, identical to the results obtained from performing the model-fitting analyses with the linear covariances. Thus, using the linear covariance when there is a significant quadratic component does not necessarily lead to the underestimation of the magnitude of the genetic variance common to the trait and metatrait. An extreme example (extraversion) in which the linear correlation did not differ from zero was deliberately selected to emphasize the caution that must be exercised in interpreting the results of linear correlations between traits and metatraits. If both the linear and quadratic components contributing to the association between the trait and metatrait level are of equivalent magnitude, no bias toward underestimating the genetic variance held in common is introduced. It could also be argued that the failure of seven of the bivariate models reported in Table 6 to fit the data could be due to the presence of a significant nonlinear component (either a quadratic or cubic or both) to the correlation between the trait and metatrait. When model-fitting analyses were performed on the quadratic covariances for these seven variables, only two models, neuroticism {\ =.8, ns) and extraversion (X =, ns), now fit the data well. In no case among the seven variables did modeling the cubic covariances result in a goodfitting model. Therefore, the failure of the seven bivariate models to fit the data using the linear covariances cannot be largely attributed to the presence of nonlinearity. Discussion Metatraits have received increasing attention from personality researchers since publication of the seminal paper by Bern and Allen (974). Yet a number of aspects of metatraits remain relatively unexamined. This is certainly not the case for traits, the total scale score on an inventory; studies of traits are endemic to the field of personality psychology, in which their measurement characteristics have been well described. Within behavioral genetic research also, the focus has almost exclusively been on the genetic and environmental sources of individual differences in traits. This study sought to add to the growing information on metatraits by exploring their reliability and stability over time and by exploring their genetic and environmental structure, independent of traits. The test-retest reliabilities of the 4 metatraits examined in this study were decidedly modest, with the exception of a metatrait factor, which had a reliability of. The test-retest reliabilities of the traits were uniformly higher than their metatrait counterparts, a result consistent with that of Baumeister (99): The average reliability of the traits was.7; the average reliability of the metatraits was.49. However, Baumeister's observation that the metatrait reliabilities were as close to the trait reliabilities as the trait reliabilities were to was not borne out. Indeed, Baumeister assumes from this relationship between the reliabilities of the traits and metatraits that the metatrait scores are as reliable as the trait scores, an assumption that is based on the unproven claim that the reliability of the trait serves as a "ceiling" for the reliability of the metatrait. Although in some cases this relationship between the reliabilities was found in this study, in other cases it was not. For example, the reliability of the extraversion trait was.75 (a.5 difference from ), and the reliability of the extraversion metatrait was.45 (a.3 difference from.75). Thus, the relative similarity mentioned by Baumeister obtains in this case. However, the reliability of the state anxiety total trait was.68 (a.3 difference from ), and the reliability of the state anxiety total metatrait was (a.3 difference from.78). We cannot conclude, as Baumeister did, that metatrait scores are as reliable as trait scores, but for several metatraits, the reliabilities are reasonable. On the whole, the stabilities of the metatraits fared better than their reliabilities, with an average stability of.85; the average trait stability was.9. Nonetheless, the metatrait stabilities covered a broad range of values, from.44 (for life satisfaction) to (depression). Some metatraits are extremely stable, whereas others are much less so. On the other hand, more uniformity existed among the values of the trait stabilities, which ranged from (for alienation) to. (for hard-driving). It can be inferred, if only for metatraits showing high stability, that individual differences in metatraits remain relatively constant over time. Low metatrait (inconsistent) people tend to remain low metatrait people across time, and high metatrait (consistent) people tend to remain high metatrait people across time. Thus, there is evidence for the predictability of inconsistency as well as consistency from one occasion to the next. Quantitative genetic analyses of the metatraits showed, on the average, 5% of the phenotypic variance to be attributable to genetic influence, 5% to shared-rearing environmental influence, and 8% to nonshared environmental influences. Although the magnitude of the shared-rearing environment is consistent with past quantitative genetic analyses of trait scale scores, the magnitude of the genetic effects is smaller, and the magnitude of the nonshared environment is larger. One possible source of this discrepancy might lie with the lower reliabilities of the metatraits. Unreliability of measurement contributes to the estimate of the nonshared environment, to the proportional decrement of the genetic effects. Bivariate quantitative genetic analyses were conducted on the trait and metatrait scores with the primary purpose of assessing how much of the metatrait's genetic influence could be uniquely attributed to the metatrait and how much was held in common with the trait. One difficulty with this analysis stemmed from the nonlinearity of the correlations between the traits and metatraits. Linear correlations cannot capture the complete magnitude of the association between two nonlinearly related variables. One possible result of using a linear correlation is the underestimation of the amount of genetic influence common to both variables. Using linear correlations, it was found that on the average, 3% of the genetic variance affecting the metatrait was unique to the metatrait, with the remaining 69% shared with the trait. Recognizing only variables with significant common and unique genetic variance yielded values of 88% and

12 684 S. HERSHBERGER, R. PLOMIN, AND N. PEDERSEN 84%, respectively. Bivariate analyses of two traits using quadratic correlations with the linear component removed showed that if the quadratic component contributing to the relationship between the variables was about equal to the linear component, the quantitative genetic results did not differ substantially. However, if the quadratic component was substantially larger, a reduction in the estimated amount of shared genetic influence between the traits and metatraits occurred, with an equivalent enhancement in the magnitude of the metatrait's unique genetic influence. In this study, the quadratic correlation between the traits and metatraits was substantially larger than the linear correlation in a minority of the cases. Thus, the analysis of the linear correlation provides an accurate description of the amount of genetic influence held in common between trait and metatraits. Two important implications follow from the results of this study. One is that the reliability and stability of metatraits can be respectable, and depending on the phenotype, researchers can use them with as much confidence as trait scale scores. The second implication is that the concept of a metatrait provides a viable, additional view of personality. This is supported not only by the high stability of some of the metatraits across time, but also by the unique genetic influence, independent of the trait levels, found for the metatraits. The common finding of a significant association between traits and metatraits has led some researchers (Burke et al., 984; Tellegen, 988) to dismiss the interitem standard deviation as an appropriate method of measuring metatraits. Although better methods may be possible, the existence of a significant correlation between traits and metatraits does not negate the unique information brought to the investigation of personality metatraits. As long as the correlation falls below unity, metatraits have unique, reliable variance to contribute to the prediction of individual differences in personality, beyond the variance contributed by the level of behavior. A useful future direction for metatrait research to take concerns identifying sources of influence common to two or more metatraits. Multivariate behavioral genetic models could partition the observed correlation between metatraits into shared genetic and environmental sources of variance, as the present study did for the observed correlation between a trait and metatrait. Undoubtedly, substantial correlations exist among metatraits, as evidenced by the metatrait factor used in this study, which accounted for an average of 4% of the variance. Multivariate behavioral genetic modeling has successfully uncovered the genetic and environmental correlations underlying the association among many traits; there is no reason to assume the same could not be accomplished for metatraits. References Allen, M. J., & Yen, W. M. (979). Introduction to measurement theory. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Baumeister, R. E. (99). On the stability of variability: Retest reliability of metatraits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, Baumeister, R. E., & Tice, D. M. (988). Metatraits. Journal ofpersonality, 56, Bern, D. J., & Allen, A. (974). On predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior. Psychology Review, 8, Bergeman, C. S., Chipuer, H. M., Plomin, R., Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G. E., Nesselroade, J. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (993). Genetic and environmental effects on openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness: An adoption/twin study. Journal of Personality, 6, Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (99). Genetic and rearing environmental influence on adult personality: An analysis of adopted twins reared apart. Journal of Personality, 58, Burke, P. A., Kraut, R. E., & Dworkin, R. H. (984). Traits, consistency, and self-schemata: What do our methods measure? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cederlof, R., & Lorich, U. (978). The Swedish twin registry. In W. E. Nance, G. Allen, & P. Parisi (Eds.), Twin research: Part C. Biology and epidemiology (pp ). New York: Alan R. Liss. Chaplin, W. E, & Goldberg, L. R. (985). A failure to replicate the Bern and Allen study of individual differences in cross-situational consistency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, Cook, W. W., & Medley, D. M. (954). Proposed hostility and pharisaic-virtue scales for the MMPI. Journal of Applied Psychology, 38, Costa, P. T, Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (985). The NEO Personality Inventory Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Duke University Center for the Study of Aging and Human Development. (978). Multidimensional functional assessment: The OARS methodology. Durham, NC: Duke University Medical Center. Epstein, S. (983). Aggregation and beyond: Some basic issues in the prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality, 5, Floderus, B. (974). Psycho-social factors in relation to coronary heart disease and associated risk factors. Nordisk Hygienisk Tidskrift [Monograph, Suppl. 6], -48. Gatz, M., Pedersen, N. L., & Harris, J. (987). Measurement characteristics of the mental health scale from the OARS. Journal of Gerontology, 4, Haynes, G. S., Levine, S., Scotch, N., Feinleib, M., & Kanner, W. B. (978). The relationship of psychosocial factors to coronary heart disease in the Framingham study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 7, Heise, D. R. (969). Separating reliability and stability in test-retest correlation. American Journal of Sociology, 75, 93-. Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (993). LISREL 8 user's reference. Chicago: Scientific Software. Klockars, A. J., & Varnum, S. W. (975). A test of the dimensionality assumptions of Rotter's Internal-External scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 39, McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (985). to experience. In R. Hogan & W. H. Jones (Eds.), Perspectives in personality (Vol., pp. 45-7). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Neale, M. C, & Cardon, L. R. (99). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrocht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. Mischel, W. (968). Personality and assessment. London: Wiley. Paunonen, S. V. (988). Trait relevance and the differential predictability of behavior. Journal of Personality, 56, Paunonen, S. V., & Jackson, D. N. (985). Idiographic measurement strategies for personality and prediction: Some unredeemed promissory notes. Psychological Review, 9, Pedersen, N. L., Gatz, M., Plomin, R., Nesselroade, J. R., & McClearn, G. E. (989). Individual differences in locus of control during the second half of the lifespan for identical and fraternal twins reared apart and reared together. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 44, -5. Pedersen, N. L., Lichtenstein, P., Plomin, R., DeFaire, U., McClearn, G. E., & Matthews, K. A. (989). Genetic and environmental influ-

13 METATRAITS 685 ences for Type A-like and related traits: A study of twins reared together and twins reared apart. Psychosomatic Medicine, 5, Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G. E., Plomin, R., Nesselroade, J. R., Berg, S., & DeFaire, U. (99). The Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging: An update. Ada Geneticae Medicae et Gemmellologiae, 4, 7-. Pedersen, N. L., Plomin, R., McClearn, G. E., & Friberg, L. (988)., extraversion, and related traits in adult twins reared apart and reared together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, Plomin, R., Chipuer, H. M., & Loehlin, J. C. (99). Behavioral genetics and personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 5-43). New York: Guilford Press. Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C, & McClearn, G. E. (99). Behavior genetics: A primer. New York: Freeman. Plomin, R., & McClearn, G. E. (99). Human behavioral genetics of aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp ). New York: Academic Press. Plomin, R., Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G. E., Nesselroade, J. R., & Bergeman, C. S. (988). EAS temperaments during the last half of the life span: Twins reared apart and twins reared together. Psychology and Aging, 3, Rotter, J. B. (966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 8 (, Whole No. 69). Schalling, D., Edman, G., & Asberg, M. (983). Impulsive cognitive style and inability to tolerate boredom: Psychological studies of temperamental vulnerability. In M. Zuckerman (Ed.), Biological bases of sensation seeking, impulsivity and anxiety. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Spielberger, C. D. (979). Preliminary manual for the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI). Tampa: University of Florida. Tellegen, A. (988). The analysis of consistency in personality assessment. Journal of Personality, 56, Tice, D. M. (989). Metatraits: Interitem variance as personality assessment. In D. M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), Personality psychology: Recent trends and emerging directions (pp. 94-). New York: Springer-Verlag. Waller, N. G., & Reise, S. P. (99). Genetic and environmental influences on item response pattern scalability. Behavior Genetics,, Wood, V., Wylie, M. L., & Sheafor, B. (969). An analysis of a short selfreport measure of life satisfaction: Correlation with rater judgment. Journal of Gerontology, 4, Received January 7,993 Revision received February 8,995 Accepted February 4, APA Convention Call for Programs The Call for Programs for the 996 APA annual convention appears in the September issue of the APA Monitor. The 996 convention will be held in Toronto, Ontario, Canada, from August 9 through August 3. The deadline for receipt of program and presentation proposals is December, 995. Additional copies of the Call are available from the APA Convention Office, effective in September. As a reminder, agreement to participate in the APA convention is now presumed to convey permission for the presentation to be audiotaped if selected for taping. Any speaker or participant who does not wish his or her presentation to be audiotaped must notify the person submitting the program either at the time the invitation is extended or before the December deadline for proposal receipt.

Can Personality Explain Genetic Influences on Life Events?

Can Personality Explain Genetic Influences on Life Events? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1997, Vol. 72, No, 1, 196-26 Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 22-3514/97/J3. Can Personality Explain Genetic Influences on Life

More information

Variability and Stability in Cognitive Abilities Are Largely Genetic Later in Life

Variability and Stability in Cognitive Abilities Are Largely Genetic Later in Life Behavior Genetics, VoL 24, No. 3, 1994 Variability and Stability in Cognitive Abilities Are Largely Genetic Later in Life R. Plomin, a N. L. Pedersen, a,2 P. Lichtenstein, 2 and G. E. McClearn I Received

More information

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability

Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability Chapter 2 Interactions Between Socioeconomic Status and Components of Variation in Cognitive Ability Eric Turkheimer and Erin E. Horn In 3, our lab published a paper demonstrating that the heritability

More information

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences University of Lethbridge Research Repository OPUS Faculty Research and Publications http://opus.uleth.ca Lalumière, Martin 2005 Behavioral genetics: The study of differences Lalumière, Martin L. Department

More information

Personality Stability in Late Adulthood: A Behavioral Genetic Analysis

Personality Stability in Late Adulthood: A Behavioral Genetic Analysis Personality Stability in Late Adulthood: A Behavioral Genetic Analysis Wendy Johnson, Matt McGue, and Robert F. Krueger University of Minnesota Twin Cities ABSTRACT A sample of 833 twins from the Minnesota

More information

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81.

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81. MSP RESEARCH NOTE B5PQ Reliability and Validity This research note describes the reliability and validity of the B5PQ. Evidence for the reliability and validity of is presented against some of the key

More information

Personality and Coping: A Study of Twins Reared Apart and Twins Reared Together

Personality and Coping: A Study of Twins Reared Apart and Twins Reared Together Behavior Genetics, Vol. 35, No. 2, March 2005 (Ó 2005) Personality and Coping: A Study of Twins Reared Apart and Twins Reared Together Kenji Kato 1,*, and Nancy L. Pedersen 1,2 Received 20 Nov. 2003 Final

More information

Preliminary Conclusion

Preliminary Conclusion 1 Exploring the Genetic Component of Political Participation Brad Verhulst Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics Virginia Commonwealth University Theories of political participation,

More information

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness: An Adoption/Tvrin Study

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness: An Adoption/Tvrin Study Genetic and Environmental Effects on Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness: An Adoption/Tvrin Study C. S. Bargeman,' Heather M. Chlpuer,' Robert Plomin,' Nancy L. Pedersen,'^ G.

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Scienc es 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 V Congress of Russian Psychological Society Genetic and Environmental Influences on

More information

Discussion. were best fit by models of determination that did not include genetic effects.

Discussion. were best fit by models of determination that did not include genetic effects. A Behavior Genetic Investigation of the Relationship Between Leadership and Personality Andrew M. Johnson 1, Philip A. Vernon 2, Julie Aitken Harris 3, and Kerry L. Jang 4 1 Faculty of Health Sciences,The

More information

Genetic and Environmental In uences on Personality in Adult Russian Twins

Genetic and Environmental In uences on Personality in Adult Russian Twins INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 1999, 23 (2), 375 389 Genetic and Environmental In uences on Personality in Adult Russian Twins Kimberly J. Saudino and Jeffrey R. Gagne Boston University,

More information

BroadcastMed Bipolar, Borderline, Both? Diagnostic/Formulation Issues in Mood and Personality Disorders

BroadcastMed Bipolar, Borderline, Both? Diagnostic/Formulation Issues in Mood and Personality Disorders BroadcastMed Bipolar, Borderline, Both? Diagnostic/Formulation Issues in Mood and Personality Disorders BRIAN PALMER: Hi. My name is Brian Palmer. I'm a psychiatrist here at Mayo Clinic. Today, we'd like

More information

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES Correlational Research Correlational Designs Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are

More information

Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration

Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration J Fam Viol (2007) 22:121 129 DOI 10.1007/s10896-006-9059-6 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Etiological Similarities Between Psychological and Physical Aggression in Intimate Relationships: A Behavioral Genetic Exploration

More information

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas 1 Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas Reference: Barchard, K.A. (2002, May). Three subfactors of the empathic personality. Poster presented

More information

PERSONALITY TRAITS, TRAITEDNESS, AND DISORDERS: TOWARDS AN ENHANCED UNDERSTANDING OF TRAIT-DISORDER RELATIONSHIPS. A Dissertation MEGAN BETH WARNER

PERSONALITY TRAITS, TRAITEDNESS, AND DISORDERS: TOWARDS AN ENHANCED UNDERSTANDING OF TRAIT-DISORDER RELATIONSHIPS. A Dissertation MEGAN BETH WARNER PERSONALITY TRAITS, TRAITEDNESS, AND DISORDERS: TOWARDS AN ENHANCED UNDERSTANDING OF TRAIT-DISORDER RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation by MEGAN BETH WARNER Submitted to the Office of Graduate Studies of Texas

More information

Paul Irwing, Manchester Business School

Paul Irwing, Manchester Business School Paul Irwing, Manchester Business School Factor analysis has been the prime statistical technique for the development of structural theories in social science, such as the hierarchical factor model of human

More information

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins

Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins Behavior Genetics, Vol. 33, No. 5, September 2003 ( 2003) Causes of Stability of Aggression from Early Childhood to Adolescence: A Longitudinal Genetic Analysis in Dutch Twins C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt,

More information

Nonshared Environmental Influences and Personality Differences in Adult Twins

Nonshared Environmental Influences and Personality Differences in Adult Twins Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1990, Vol. 58, No. 1,103-110 Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/90/S00.75 Nonshared Environmental Influences and Personality

More information

A Behavior Genetic Study of the Connection Between Social Values and Personality

A Behavior Genetic Study of the Connection Between Social Values and Personality ARTICLE AVAILABLE ONLINE Twin Research and Human Genetics Volume 14 Number 3 pp. 233 239 A Behavior Genetic Study of the Connection Between Social Values and Personality Julie Aitken Schermer, 1 Philip

More information

Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease

Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease (2007) 15, 872 877 & 2007 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 1018-4813/07 $30.00 ARTICLE www.nature.com/ejhg Genetic influences on angina pectoris and its impact on coronary heart disease Slobodan

More information

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study

Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study Diabetologia (2013) 56:1031 1035 DOI 10.1007/s00125-013-2859-3 SHORT COMMUNICATION Shared genetic influence of BMI, physical activity and type 2 diabetes: a twin study S. Carlsson & A. Ahlbom & P. Lichtenstein

More information

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder DOI 10.1007/s10519-014-9670-x ORIGINAL RESEARCH Contrast Effects and Sex Influence Maternal and Self-Report Dimensional Measures of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder J. L. Ebejer S. E. Medland J.

More information

Introduction. TL Nelson 1 *, GP Vogler 1, NL Pedersen 2,3 and TP Miles 4

Introduction. TL Nelson 1 *, GP Vogler 1, NL Pedersen 2,3 and TP Miles 4 International Journal of Obesity (1999) 23, 449±455 ß 1999 Stockton Press All rights reserved 0307±0565/99 $12.00 http://www.stockton-press.co.uk/ijo Genetic and environmental in uences on waistto-hip

More information

Chapter X Personality

Chapter X Personality 2000, Gregory Carey (4/7/00) Personality - 1 Chapter X Personality Personality and its Measurement The word personality has various meanings that range from the qualities of an individual person to evaluative

More information

A Comparison of Several Goodness-of-Fit Statistics

A Comparison of Several Goodness-of-Fit Statistics A Comparison of Several Goodness-of-Fit Statistics Robert L. McKinley The University of Toledo Craig N. Mills Educational Testing Service A study was conducted to evaluate four goodnessof-fit procedures

More information

CHAPTER 6. Conclusions and Perspectives

CHAPTER 6. Conclusions and Perspectives CHAPTER 6 Conclusions and Perspectives In Chapter 2 of this thesis, similarities and differences among members of (mainly MZ) twin families in their blood plasma lipidomics profiles were investigated.

More information

Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions

Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions Psychological Medicine, 2000, 30, 1315 1325. 2000 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom Genetic and environmental influences on juvenile antisocial behaviour assessed on two occasions

More information

Genetic and Shared Environmental Contributions to the Relationship between the Home Environment and Child and Adolescent Achievement

Genetic and Shared Environmental Contributions to the Relationship between the Home Environment and Child and Adolescent Achievement Genetic and Shared Environmental Contributions to the Relationship between the Home Environment and Child and Adolescent Achievement Hobart H. Cleveland The University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC,

More information

Personal Style Inventory Item Revision: Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Personal Style Inventory Item Revision: Confirmatory Factor Analysis Personal Style Inventory Item Revision: Confirmatory Factor Analysis This research was a team effort of Enzo Valenzi and myself. I m deeply grateful to Enzo for his years of statistical contributions to

More information

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship Correlational research is used to describe the relationship between two or more naturally occurring variables. Is age related to political conservativism? Are highly extraverted people less afraid of rejection

More information

Control and Turnout. Christopher Dawes. University of California, San Diego. March 18, 2010

Control and Turnout. Christopher Dawes. University of California, San Diego. March 18, 2010 Control and Turnout Christopher Dawes University of California, San Diego March 18, 2010 Abstract Political scientists have recently explored the genetic basis of political participation. Fowler, Baker

More information

Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality: A Longitudinal Multiple-Rater Twin Study

Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality: A Longitudinal Multiple-Rater Twin Study Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 98, No. 6, 995 1008 0022-3514/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019558 Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality:

More information

Environmental Predictors of Personality Differences: A Twin and Sibling Study

Environmental Predictors of Personality Differences: A Twin and Sibling Study PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Environmental Predictors of Personality Differences: A Twin and Siling Study Philip A. Vernon University of Western Ontario Kerry L. Jang University of

More information

BIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY NATURE VS. NURTURE

BIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY NATURE VS. NURTURE BIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY NATURE VS. NURTURE Founder of the Trait Theory Predispositions to respond, in a same or similar manner, to different stimuli After meeting Freud, Allport felt there was too much emphasis

More information

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating

Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Obesity and Binge Eating Cynthia M. Bulik,* Patrick F. Sullivan, and Kenneth S. Kendler Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics of Virginia

More information

Parenting as Phenotype: A Behavioral Genetic Approach to Understanding Parenting

Parenting as Phenotype: A Behavioral Genetic Approach to Understanding Parenting The University of San Francisco USF Scholarship: a digital repository @ Gleeson Library Geschke Center Psychology College of Arts and Sciences 2012 Parenting as Phenotype: A Behavioral Genetic Approach

More information

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance

Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of family resemblance Twin Research (1999) 2, 99 107 1999 Stockton Press All rights reserved 1369 0523/99 $12.00 http://www.stockton-press.co.uk/tr Frequency of church attendance in Australia and the United States: models of

More information

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H

More information

Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures

Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures Journal of Substance Abuse, 5, 227-246 (7993) Genetic Contribution to Risk of Smoking Initiation: Comparisons Across Birth Cohorts and Across Cultures Andrew C. Heath Departments of Psychiatry, Psychology

More information

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION. Are Genetic Influences on Peptic Ulcer Dependent or Independent of Genetic Influences for Helicobacter pylori Infection?

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION. Are Genetic Influences on Peptic Ulcer Dependent or Independent of Genetic Influences for Helicobacter pylori Infection? ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Are Genetic Influences on Peptic Ulcer Dependent or Independent of Genetic Influences for Helicobacter pylori Infection? Hoda M. Malaty, MD, PhD; David Y. Graham, MD; Inger Isaksson,

More information

The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items

The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items Kenn Konstabel Department of Psychology, Estonian Centre of Behavioural and Health Sciences University

More information

Examining the Origins of Gender Differences in Marital Quality: A Behavior Genetic Analysis

Examining the Origins of Gender Differences in Marital Quality: A Behavior Genetic Analysis Journal of Family Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 20, No. 4, 605 613 0893-3200/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.4.605 Examining the Origins of Gender Differences

More information

Basic concepts and principles of classical test theory

Basic concepts and principles of classical test theory Basic concepts and principles of classical test theory Jan-Eric Gustafsson What is measurement? Assignment of numbers to aspects of individuals according to some rule. The aspect which is measured must

More information

A Caution on the Use of the MMPI K-Correction in Research on Psychosomatic Medicine

A Caution on the Use of the MMPI K-Correction in Research on Psychosomatic Medicine A Caution on the Use of the MMPI K-Correction in Research on Psychosomatic Medicine ROBERT R. MCCRAE, PHD, PAUL T. COSTA, JR., PHD, W. GRANT DAHLSTROM, PHD, JOHN C. BAREFOOT, PHD, ILENE C. SIEGLER, PHD,

More information

Test Partnership TPAQ Series Psychometric Properties

Test Partnership TPAQ Series Psychometric Properties Test Partnership TPAQ Series Psychometric Properties 2018 1 Construct Validity The IPIP-NEO-120 (Johnson, 2014) is a validated measure of the Big-5 model of personality, specifically the OCEAN model (Costa

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) PSYSOC 2013

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) PSYSOC 2013 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) 506 510 PSYSOC 2013 Personality Traits and Different Career Stages A Study on Indian School

More information

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H PSYC H O PAT H O LO G Y : PSYC H I AT R I C P R O B LEMS A N D T H E A S SO C I

More information

Analysis of single gene effects 1. Quantitative analysis of single gene effects. Gregory Carey, Barbara J. Bowers, Jeanne M.

Analysis of single gene effects 1. Quantitative analysis of single gene effects. Gregory Carey, Barbara J. Bowers, Jeanne M. Analysis of single gene effects 1 Quantitative analysis of single gene effects Gregory Carey, Barbara J. Bowers, Jeanne M. Wehner From the Department of Psychology (GC, JMW) and Institute for Behavioral

More information

Journal of Research in Personality

Journal of Research in Personality Journal of Research in Personality 43 (2009) 737 746 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Research in Personality journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp Temperament in middle childhood:

More information

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION

GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION Chapter 8 GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON ANXIOUS/DEPRESSION A Longitudinal Study in 3- to 12-Year-Old Children Dorret I. Boomsma, Ph.D. C.E.M. van Beijsterveldt, Ph.D. Meike Bartels, Ph.D. James

More information

Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students

Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students Gary Felsten Stress Reactivity and Depressed Mood 1 Department of Psychology, Indiana University Purdue University Columbus 4601

More information

Evidence for Genetic Influences on Personality From Self-Reports and Informant Ratings

Evidence for Genetic Influences on Personality From Self-Reports and Informant Ratings Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1992, Vol. 63. No. 1,85-96 Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. OO22-3514/92/S3.OO Evidence for Genetic Influences on Personality

More information

Factor Analytic Theories. Chapter 11

Factor Analytic Theories. Chapter 11 Factor Analytic Theories Chapter 11 Raymond Cattell 1905- Born Staffordshire, England Happy childhood Lived through WW I Admitted he was heavily influenced by the war Chemistry & Physics background before

More information

Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN

Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN Vs. 2 Background 3 There are different types of research methods to study behaviour: Descriptive: observations,

More information

Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries

Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries Donald Macnab Psychometrics Canada Copyright Psychometrics Canada 2011. All rights reserved. The Work Personality Index is a trademark of

More information

Personality: Definitions

Personality: Definitions Personality: Definitions Anastasi "Personality tests are instruments for the measurement of emotional, motivational, interpersonal and attitudinal characteristics, as distinct from abilities. Kaplan &

More information

Trait Negative Affect Relates to Prior-Week Symptoms, But Not to Reports of Illness Episodes, Illness Symptoms, and Care Seeking Among Older Persons

Trait Negative Affect Relates to Prior-Week Symptoms, But Not to Reports of Illness Episodes, Illness Symptoms, and Care Seeking Among Older Persons Trait Negative Affect Relates to Prior-Week Symptoms, But Not to Reports of Illness Episodes, Illness Symptoms, and Care Seeking Among Older Persons PABLO A. MORA, MS, CHANTAL ROBITAILLE, PHD, HOWARD LEVENTHAL,

More information

Extraversion and Neuroticism in Contact Athletes, No Contact Athletes and Non-athletes: A Research Note

Extraversion and Neuroticism in Contact Athletes, No Contact Athletes and Non-athletes: A Research Note September, 2003 Volume 5, Issue 3 Extraversion and Neuroticism in Contact Athletes, No Contact Athletes and Non-athletes: A Research Note Stuart J. McKelvie, Patrice Lemieux, and Dale Stout Department

More information

Estimating Individual Rater Reliabilities John E. Overall and Kevin N. Magee University of Texas Medical School

Estimating Individual Rater Reliabilities John E. Overall and Kevin N. Magee University of Texas Medical School Estimating Individual Rater Reliabilities John E. Overall and Kevin N. Magee University of Texas Medical School Rating scales have no inherent reliability that is independent of the observers who use them.

More information

Liability Threshold Models

Liability Threshold Models Liability Threshold Models Frühling Rijsdijk & Kate Morley Twin Workshop, Boulder Tuesday March 4 th 2008 Aims Introduce model fitting to categorical data Define liability and describe assumptions of the

More information

Manifestation Of Differences In Item-Level Characteristics In Scale-Level Measurement Invariance Tests Of Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analyses

Manifestation Of Differences In Item-Level Characteristics In Scale-Level Measurement Invariance Tests Of Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analyses Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods Copyright 2005 JMASM, Inc. May, 2005, Vol. 4, No.1, 275-282 1538 9472/05/$95.00 Manifestation Of Differences In Item-Level Characteristics In Scale-Level Measurement

More information

CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT

CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT Informal Tes3ng Personality Tes3ng Assessment Procedures Ability Tes3ng The Clinical Interview 3 Defining Clinical Assessment The process of assessing the client through

More information

Relational tendencies associated with broad personality dimensions

Relational tendencies associated with broad personality dimensions 1 British Journal of Medical Psychology (2004), 00, 1 6 q 2004 The British Psychological Society www.bps.org.uk Brief report Relational tendencies associated with broad personality dimensions Dionyssios

More information

Saville Consulting Wave Professional Styles Handbook

Saville Consulting Wave Professional Styles Handbook Saville Consulting Wave Professional Styles Handbook PART 4: TECHNICAL Chapter 19: Reliability This manual has been generated electronically. Saville Consulting do not guarantee that it has not been changed

More information

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT RESEARCH AND REVIEW

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT RESEARCH AND REVIEW INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT RESEARCH AND REVIEW BIG FIVE INVENTORY (BFI) OF PERSONALITY TRAITS OF ENTREPRENEURS S.Subramanian* 1, Dr.V.Gopalakrishnan 2, Dr.I.Prem Rose Thayammal 3 1 Assistant Professor,

More information

Bias in Correlations from Selected Samples of Relatives: The Effects of Soft Selection

Bias in Correlations from Selected Samples of Relatives: The Effects of Soft Selection Behavior Genetics, Vol. 19, No. 2, 1989 Bias in Correlations from Selected Samples of Relatives: The Effects of Soft Selection M. C. Neale, ~ L. J. Eaves, 1 K. S. Kendler, 2 and J. K. Hewitt 1 Received

More information

Notes for an Undergraduate Course on Behavioral Genetics and Evolution

Notes for an Undergraduate Course on Behavioral Genetics and Evolution Notes for an Undergraduate Course on Behavioral Genetics and Evolution 2008 M. Frank Norman (norman@psych.upenn.edu) University of Pennsylvania 8/10/2008 (revised 10/2/2008) My course at Penn covered,

More information

Issues That Should Not Be Overlooked in the Dominance Versus Ideal Point Controversy

Issues That Should Not Be Overlooked in the Dominance Versus Ideal Point Controversy Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 3 (2010), 489 493. Copyright 2010 Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. 1754-9426/10 Issues That Should Not Be Overlooked in the Dominance Versus

More information

Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder:

Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder: Major Depression and Generalised Anxiety Disorder: Same Genes, (Partly) Kenneth S. Kendler, M.D. Different Environments Revisited Abstract: In both clinical and epidemiological samples, major depression

More information

Social desirability, defensiveness and self-report psychiatric inventory scores

Social desirability, defensiveness and self-report psychiatric inventory scores Psychological Medicine, 1980,10, 735-742 Printed in Great Britain Social desirability, defensiveness and self-report psychiatric inventory scores KATHARINE R. PARKES 1 From the Department of Experimental

More information

Impulsivity is Important

Impulsivity is Important Impulsivity is Important Involved in every major system of personality Vital role in the understanding & diagnosis of psychopathology: - DSM IV impulse control disorders - Criteria for BPD, ASPD, ADHD

More information

Simultaneous Equation and Instrumental Variable Models for Sexiness and Power/Status

Simultaneous Equation and Instrumental Variable Models for Sexiness and Power/Status Simultaneous Equation and Instrumental Variable Models for Seiness and Power/Status We would like ideally to determine whether power is indeed sey, or whether seiness is powerful. We here describe the

More information

Technical Specifications

Technical Specifications Technical Specifications In order to provide summary information across a set of exercises, all tests must employ some form of scoring models. The most familiar of these scoring models is the one typically

More information

Genetic contribution to the relationship between personality and depressive symptoms among older women

Genetic contribution to the relationship between personality and depressive symptoms among older women Psychological Medicine (2010), 40, 1357 1366. f Cambridge University Press 2009 doi:10.1017/s0033291709991401 Genetic contribution to the relationship between personality and depressive symptoms among

More information

Claim 1. Genetic factors play such a strong role in human development that genes alone can determine certain human behavioral characteristics.

Claim 1. Genetic factors play such a strong role in human development that genes alone can determine certain human behavioral characteristics. Claim 1 Genetic factors play such a strong role in human development that genes alone can determine certain human behavioral characteristics. Susan likes coffee a lot and often has some when out with friends.

More information

Genetic Influences on Midlife Functioning

Genetic Influences on Midlife Functioning 03-Willis-4658.qxd 3/24/2005 2:12 PM Page 65 Three Genetic Influences on Midlife Functioning Nancy L. Pedersen, Erica Spotts, and Kenji Kato Regardless of the definition of midlife, most of the research

More information

Psych 3102 Introduction to Behavior Genetics

Psych 3102 Introduction to Behavior Genetics Psych 3102 Introduction to Behavior Genetics Lecture 12 Quantitative analysis Covariance between relatives Sources of covariance between relatives covariance a measure of shared variance (how similar the

More information

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement

Twins and the Study of Rater (Dis)agreement Psychological Methods 2007, Vol. 12, No. 4, 451 466 CORRECTED APRIL 10, 2008; SEE LAST PAGE Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 1082-989X/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.4.451

More information

Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality

Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality Graduate Faculty Psychology Bulletin Volume 3, No. 1, 2005 Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality Brian Norensberg, M.A. 1 & Peter Zachar Ph.D. 2 Abstract ~ This study investigates the

More information

Critical assumptions of classical quantitative genetics and twin studies that warrant more attention

Critical assumptions of classical quantitative genetics and twin studies that warrant more attention Critical assumptions of classical quantitative genetics and twin studies that warrant more attention Peter J. Taylor Programs in Science, Technology & Values and Critical & Creative Thinking University

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Educ Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 September 14.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Educ Psychol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 September 14. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Educ Psychol. 2007 February 1; 99(1): 128 139. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.128. The Origins of Diverse Domains of Mathematics: Generalist

More information

Multiple Act criterion:

Multiple Act criterion: Common Features of Trait Theories Generality and Stability of Traits: Trait theorists all use consistencies in an individual s behavior and explain why persons respond in different ways to the same stimulus

More information

Running head: NESTED FACTOR ANALYTIC MODEL COMPARISON 1. John M. Clark III. Pearson. Author Note

Running head: NESTED FACTOR ANALYTIC MODEL COMPARISON 1. John M. Clark III. Pearson. Author Note Running head: NESTED FACTOR ANALYTIC MODEL COMPARISON 1 Nested Factor Analytic Model Comparison as a Means to Detect Aberrant Response Patterns John M. Clark III Pearson Author Note John M. Clark III,

More information

In Australian twins participating in three different

In Australian twins participating in three different Heritability and Stability of Resting Blood Pressure in Australian Twins Jouke-Jan Hottenga, 1 John B. Whitfield, 2 Eco J. C. de Geus, 1 Dorret I. Boomsma, 1 and Nicholas G. Martin 2 1 Department of Biological

More information

On the Performance of Maximum Likelihood Versus Means and Variance Adjusted Weighted Least Squares Estimation in CFA

On the Performance of Maximum Likelihood Versus Means and Variance Adjusted Weighted Least Squares Estimation in CFA STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELING, 13(2), 186 203 Copyright 2006, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. On the Performance of Maximum Likelihood Versus Means and Variance Adjusted Weighted Least Squares Estimation

More information

Today s Topics. Cracking the Genetic Code. The Process of Genetic Transmission. The Process of Genetic Transmission. Genes

Today s Topics. Cracking the Genetic Code. The Process of Genetic Transmission. The Process of Genetic Transmission. Genes Today s Topics Mechanisms of Heredity Biology of Heredity Genetic Disorders Research Methods in Behavioral Genetics Gene x Environment Interactions The Process of Genetic Transmission Genes: segments of

More information

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children

Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations between Reaction Times and Intelligence in Young Twin Children J. Intell. 2015, 3, 160-167; doi:10.3390/jintelligence3040160 Brief Report OPEN ACCESS Journal of Intelligence ISSN 2079-3200 www.mdpi.com/journal/jintelligence Phenotypic, Genetic, and Environmental Correlations

More information

Chapter 1: Explaining Behavior

Chapter 1: Explaining Behavior Chapter 1: Explaining Behavior GOAL OF SCIENCE is to generate explanations for various puzzling natural phenomenon. - Generate general laws of behavior (psychology) RESEARCH: principle method for acquiring

More information

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 6.1 Research Design Research is an organized, systematic, data based, critical, objective, scientific inquiry or investigation into a specific problem, undertaken with the

More information

THE CONSISTENCY OF RECALLED AGE AT FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE

THE CONSISTENCY OF RECALLED AGE AT FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE Revise 1st proof 6.11.96 J. biosoc. Sci. (1997) 29,1 7 1997 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United Kingdom THE CONSISTENCY OF RECALLED AGE AT FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE MICHAEL P. DUNNE*, NICHOLAS

More information

additive genetic component [d] = rded

additive genetic component [d] = rded Heredity (1976), 36 (1), 31-40 EFFECT OF GENE DISPERSION ON ESTIMATES OF COMPONENTS OF GENERATION MEANS AND VARIANCES N. E. M. JAYASEKARA* and J. L. JINKS Department of Genetics, University of Birmingham,

More information

Sociodemographic Effects on the Test-Retest Reliability of the Big Five Inventory. Timo Gnambs. Osnabrück University. Author Note

Sociodemographic Effects on the Test-Retest Reliability of the Big Five Inventory. Timo Gnambs. Osnabrück University. Author Note Running head: TEST-RETEST RELIABILITY Sociodemographic Effects on the Test-Retest Reliability of the Big Five Inventory Timo Gnambs Osnabrück University Author Note Timo Gnambs, Institute of Psychology,

More information

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale Simon B. Sherry, 1 Paul L. Hewitt, 1 * Avi Besser, 2 Brandy J. McGee, 1 and Gordon L. Flett 3

More information

Assessing Measurement Invariance in the Attitude to Marriage Scale across East Asian Societies. Xiaowen Zhu. Xi an Jiaotong University.

Assessing Measurement Invariance in the Attitude to Marriage Scale across East Asian Societies. Xiaowen Zhu. Xi an Jiaotong University. Running head: ASSESS MEASUREMENT INVARIANCE Assessing Measurement Invariance in the Attitude to Marriage Scale across East Asian Societies Xiaowen Zhu Xi an Jiaotong University Yanjie Bian Xi an Jiaotong

More information

IJPSS Volume 2, Issue 7 ISSN:

IJPSS Volume 2, Issue 7 ISSN: Teachers` as a Leader and their Traits: Evidence from secondary level Dr.Tahseen Mehmood Aslam* Zulfiqar Ali** Ijaz Ahmad Tatlah*** Dr Muhammad Iqbal**** _ Abstract Teacher must act as a leader because

More information

The present study investigated the location of trait

The present study investigated the location of trait Phenotypic and Genetic Relations Between the HEXACO Dimensions and Trait Emotional Intelligence Livia Veselka, 1 K. V. Petrides, 2 Julie Aitken Schermer, 3 Lynn F. Cherkas, 4 Tim D. Spector 4 and Philip

More information

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Daily Life Stressors: More Evidence for Greater Variation in Later Life

Genetic and Environmental Effects on Daily Life Stressors: More Evidence for Greater Variation in Later Life Psychology and Aging Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 22, No. 2, 331 340 0882-7974/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.2.331 Genetic and Environmental Effects on Daily

More information