Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1"

Transcription

1 Japanese Psychological Research 1998, Volume 40, No. 2, Short Report Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of rats with lever depressing or releasing as a target response 1 KATSUYA KITAGUCHI and SADAHIKO NAKAJIMA Department of Psychology, Kwansei Gakuin University, Uegahara, Nishinomiya 662, Japan Abstract: The effect of a diffuse auditory signal filling a brief delay between a response and reinforcement depends upon the nature of the trained target response. Rats in a signaled condition responded slower than rats in an unsignaled condition if the target response was lever release. If the target response was lever depression, however, rats in the signaled condition responded faster than rats in the unsignaled condition at the end of training Key words: signaled reinforcement, response topography, fixed-interval schedule, rats. In a typical operant chamber, a lever-press response emitted by a rat consists of two components: depressing and releasing the lever. It is conventional to define lever depression as a response, to count it, and to deliver the outcome for it (Ator, 1991). We accidentally found that if we chose lever release instead of lever depression as a target response, different results were obtained. The following is a report of serendipity (cf. Skinner, 1956) in our research on signaled reinforcement. In a recent paper (Nakajima & Kitaguchi, 1996), we reported the effects of signals of forthcoming reinforcement on lever-press performance maintained on fixed-interval (FI) schedules of briefly delayed reinforcement. Although a localized panel light signal filling the delay attenuated the overall rate of responding (Tarpy, Roberts, Lea, & Midgley, 1984), a diffuse tone signal increased it. We attributed the former effect to the interference of responding by tracking behavior to the localized signal (Iversen, 1981; Reed, 1992; Reed, Schachtman, & Hall, 1988b; Williams, 1982; Williams & Heyneman, 1982) and the latter to the facilitative effect of the diffuse signal on rapid responding before reinforcement (Reed, 1989a, 1989b; Reed & Hall, 1988, 1989; Reed et al., 1988a, 1988b; Reed, Schachtman, & Rawlins, 1992; Schachtman & Reed, 1990). After a series of experiments reported by Nakajima and Kitaguchi (1996), we did another experiment with the switching off of the house light as a signal. Assuming that this signal does not elicit tracking behavior, which interferes with lever-press responses, we expected an increment in the rate of responding with this signal, as with the tone signal. However, a 1 This research was conducted with a grant in aid for scientific research (H5-0808) from the Ministry of Education, Science, Sports, and Culture of Japan to the second author when he was a postdoctoral fellow by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science. He was also supported by JSPS Postdoctoral Fellowships for Research Abroad when the manuscript was being written. We are grateful to Hiroshi Imada for encouragement of the research and to K. Matthew Lattal for helpful comments on a draft and for refinement of English expression. Portions of the data were reported at the 55th annual meeting of the Japanese Society for Animal Psychology, Osaka, August Japanese Psychological Association. Published by Blackwell Publishers Ltd, 108 Cowley Road, Oxford OX4 1JF, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA.

2 Signaled reinforcement effects in rats 105 mistake occurred in electric wiring after an early session, and we, unaware of the mistake, continued the experiment under this condition, in which lever release, instead of lever depression, now triggered an input to the computer interface. The results obtained in this experiment surprised us: The signaled group responded more slowly than the unsignaled group, though the difference failed to reach statistical significance. We then conducted another experiment, still unaware of the mistake in wiring. Although this experiment used a tone signal, as in our previous report (Nakajima & Kitaguchi, 1996), rats in the signaled condition responded significantly more slowly than in the unsignaled condition. Fortunately, we had videotaped rats behavior on several sessions in this and the preceding experiments. The video prompted us to examine the equipment and we found the wiring mistake. We were uncertain whether the wiring mistake was the cause of the inconsistencies in the results. We therefore conducted another experiment, and report its results here. In this experiment, rats in the tone-signaled condition were compared with rats in the unsignaled condition. Some rats were trained to depress the lever; others were trained to release the lever. We call the former release rats and the latter depress rats for convenience. Our question here was whether the effect of the signal depends on the nature of target response. Method Subjects Subjects were 24 experimentally naive male Wistar rats housed individually in wire-mesh cages with free access to water. Throughout the experiment, they were maintained at 85% of their free-feeding weights (range g) by supplementary feeding after each session. When the experiment started, they were approximately 90 days old. Apparatus Each of two chambers 30.5 cm long, 24.7 cm wide, and 27 cm high were housed in an individual ventilated sound-attenuating shell with an observation window. Each chamber was made of stainless-steel front and back panels, two acrylic side walls, an acrylic ceiling, and a grid floor consisting of stainless-steel rods (0.5 cm in diameter) spaced 1.3 cm apart. A stainless-steel lever 3 cm wide by 1 cm thick protruded 2.6 cm from the front panel. The edge of the lever was 2.4 cm from the left wall and 4 cm above the floor. The conventional lever action from the up position to the down position required 0.10 N to operate. A food cup 1 cm wide and 2 cm above the floor protruded 1.8 cm from the center of the front panel. A pellet dispenser delivered a 45-mg pellet (Bio-Serve, DPP F-200) into the food cup with a click (92 db, Scale C). A bulb (24 V d.c.,.11 A) on the top of the front panel of each chamber was used as a house light during the session. This bulb had a white jeweled cover and was 25 cm above the floor. An electric buzzer (National EB-2114, Japan) on the front panel, 21.5 cm above the floor, was used to present a tone signal (2300 Hz, 100 db). In addition to ventilation fan s noise, a speaker in the room provided a masking noise. The background noise level in each chamber was 75 db. Wiring of the lever microswitch of each chamber and the computer program made it possible to accept an input from either lever depression or lever release. Procedure Experimental sessions were conducted 7 days a week. Acclimation to the chamber was conducted on the first day. After the food cup was filled with six pellets, rats were exposed to the chamber for 20 min during which the house light was on and the lever was inoperative. One rat was eliminated from the experiment because it did not eat the pellets during the acclimation. The experiment was conducted with the remaining 23 rats. During the next two days, 12 rats were trained to depress the lever and the remaining 11 were trained to release the lever. For the latter rats, a release response was registered when the lever moved back from the down position to the up position. The pellet was

3 106 K. Kitaguchi and S. Nakajima delivered automatically by the dispenser every 60 s. In addition, every response immediately produced one pellet and reset the 60-s timer for the free pellets. Before these sessions, the food cup was filled with pellets and the lever was baited with crushed pellets to facilitate establishment of the target response. The sessions began with the switching on of the house light and terminated with its switching off 1 s after the 30th pellet. Establishment of responding by hand shaping was conducted just after the second training session for those rats that failed to respond reliably. Each group was then divided into signaled (Sig) and unsignaled (Unsig)/lever-depression (D) and lever-release (R) subgroups: six rats for each of Groups D-Sig, D-Unsig, and R-Sig, and 5 rats for Group R-Unsig. The groups were matched as far as possible for performance during the pretraining, the order of running on daily sessions, the assigned chamber, and body weights. Groups D-Sig and D-Unsig received lever-depress training under an FI schedule with a 500-ms delay of reinforcement: The first responses after the fixed interval from the last reinforcer (or from the onset of the session for the first interval) initiated the delay and then produced the reinforcer. Groups R-Sig and R-Unsig received the same training except that the target response was lever release rather than lever depression. The delay was filled with a 500-ms tone signal for Groups D-Sig and R-Sig, but it was unsignaled by any stimuli for Groups D-Unsig and R-Unsig. Again, the sessions began with the switching on of the house light and they were terminated with its switching off 1 s after the 30th reinforcer. On the first day, the interval value was 15 s. For the remaining 15 sessions, the interval value was 30 s. Measures Each FI 30-s session consisted of 30 intervals and reinforcement delays, and the cumulative waste time. Each period of waste time was the time from the end of the interval to the response which initiated the delay and then the reinforcer. Each interval was split into 10 3-s consecutive time segments, and the number of responses during each segment was accumulated for the whole session. In addition, the number of responses during the delay was accumulated. Thus, the total number of responses in a given session was the sum of the interval responding, the delay responding, and the 30 responses which initiated the delay. The data accumulated over the entire session were used to calculate the following measures. Response rate. The sum of the total number of the interval responses and the responses which initiated the delay was divided by the sum of the total interval duration and the cumulative waste time. This is the rate except during the delay period, and we will call this measure the response rate as in the previous study (Nakajima & Kitaguchi, 1996). Delay rate. The cumulative number of delay responses was divided by the total delay duration. Waste time per interval. The cumulative waste time was divided by the number of intervals (i.e., 30). Index of curvature (IC). Calculation of this response-pattern measure was based on the method of Fry, Kelleher, and Cook (1960) for the accumulated data. Constant responding gives 0, and convergence of responding on the later segments gives a high value. With the 10-segment interval used here, the maximum value is.9, when all responses are located in the final segment. The IC was directly used for parametric analysis, but the rate measures and the waste time were subjected to a square-root transformation before the analyses because the variances in the latter measures tended to increase with the mean. All statistical tests were evaluated with respect to an alpha level of.05 unless otherwise noted. Results Each panel of Figure 1 shows group averages of each measure for 15 FI 30-s sessions. A splitplot factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) with response (D vs. R), signal (Sig vs. Unsig), and session (repeated) as factors was used for each measure.

4 Signaled reinforcement effects in rats 107 R-Unsig R-Sig D-Unsig D-Sig Index of curvature Response rate Waste time (s) Delay rate Sessions Figure 1. Group mean measures during FI 30-s training: (a) the number of responses per minute, excluding the delay period; (b) the responses per minute during the delay period; (c) the index of curvature; (d) the waste time per interval. Groups R-Unsig and D-Unsig were trained with unsignaled delay of reinforcement, and Groups R-Sig and D-Sig received the tone filling the delay. The reinforced target response was releasing the lever for Groups R-Unsig and R-Sig, and depressing it for Groups D-Unsig and D-Sig. In the response rate (Figure 1a), there was a significant session effect, F(14, 266) = Although the other main effects and all two-way interactions were nonsignificant, the response signal session interaction was significant, F(14, 266) = This interaction seems to reflect the fact that Group R-Sig responded slower than Group R-Unsig throughout the training and that Group D-Sig responded slower on the early sessions but faster on the later sessions than Group D-Unsig. Further analyses supported this impression. For R-Sig and R-Unsig, the effects of signal, F(1, 9) = 5.93, and session, F(14, 126) = 2.04, were significant, but the signal session interaction was not. On the other hand, for D-Sig and D-Unsig, the session effect, F(14, 140) = 8.09, and the signal session interaction, F(14, 140) = 2.81, were significant, but the signal effect was not. Subsequent least-squares difference tests for D-Sig and D-Unsig revealed significant group differences on sessions 3, 12, 14, and 15. In the delay rate (Figure 1b), the three main effects were significant: response, F(1, 19) = 6.77; signal, F(1, 19) = 13.54; and session, F(14, 266) = That is, the rats trained to release the lever responded faster than those trained to depress it, the unsignaled groups responded faster than signaled groups, and all groups showed gradual increment in the response rate throughout the training. No interaction was significant. In the IC (Figure 1c), there were significant signal, F(1, 19) = 11.61, and session, F(14, 266) = 88.46, effects. Though Figure 1c suggests the signal effect was greater in R-Sig and R-Unsig than in D-Sig and D-Unsig, the response signal interaction failed to reach significance,

5 108 K. Kitaguchi and S. Nakajima F(1, 19) = 4.21, p =.054. The response effect and the other interactions were far from significance. Figure 1d shows the waste time. Only the response, F(1, 19) = 4.51, and the session, F(14, 266) = 30.70, effects were significant. Discussion These results replicated those of our previous experiments. For the rats releasing the lever, the signal attenuated the response rate and the delay rate. For the rats depressing the lever, the signal attenuated the delay rate. On the response rate measure of the rats depressing the lever, the signal initially had an attenuating effect, but it enhanced responding in the later sessions. The initial decrement was also observed in Nakajima and Kitaguchi (1996) and it was probably caused by unconditioned aversive properties of the auditory signal (cf. Reed, Collison, & Nokes, 1995). This experiment, thus, clearly demonstrated that the effect of the signal depends on the nature of the target response, depressing or releasing the lever. The signal attenuated the lever-releasing response, but enhanced the lever-depressing response at the end of training. One plausible account of this finding is that the lever release was not the operant to be affected by the consequences. In order to release the lever, rats have to depress the lever, so that the lever depression may have been the operant even in the rats which received the arranged consequences on lever releasing. The signal could have become a secondary reinforcer because of its temporal contiguity with the pellet delivery. The rats were trained under briefly delayed reinforcement schedules, thus, the delay-filling signal may have modified the situation into immediate reinforcement. Although this immediate reinforcement contingency may have increased responding of the rats depressing the lever in the later sessions, the same contingency may have had a different effect on responding of the rats releasing it. If lever depression was the operant for the latter, then the signal immediately following lever release must have been a briefly delayed reinforcement of lever depression. This is because there was the inevitable delay between the lever release and the preceding lever depression. In the absence of the signal, on the other hand, the rats may have emitted an additional lever depression during the delay which had been set between the lever release and the pellet delivery, and the lever depression may have received immediate reinforcement. This superstitious contingency (cf. Herrnstein, 1966) may have been a cause of the higher response rate of the rats in the unsignaled condition compared with those in the signaled condition. If the reasoning described above is correct, we expect that without the delay and the signal, rats trained to depress the lever should respond faster than those trained to release the lever. The former rats should receive immediate reinforcement of lever-depression but the latter should receive delayed reinforcement of that operant which preceded lever release. We examined this prediction in another experiment, but there was no significant group difference and, indeed, the group average suggested the opposite pattern. Thus, another explanation of the strange effect of the signal on the briefly delayed reinforcement for depressing or releasing the lever is required. For the rats depressing the lever, a diffuse signal can enhance responding by facilitation of rapid responding prior to reinforcement (Reed, 1989a, 1989b; Reed & Hall, 1988, 1989; Reed et al., 1988a, 1988b, 1992; Schachtman & Reed, 1990). The underlying mechanism of this facilitating function of the signal is unknown, but secondary reinforcement, delay bridging (Rescorla, 1982) and marking (Lieberman, McIntosh, & Thomas, 1979) are potential candidates (cf. Reed, 1989b). On the other hand, the attenuating effect of the tone signal on FI performance of the rats releasing the lever does not fit with this account. Instead, these results match the hypothesis that the signal stimulus attenuates the response rate under FI schedules by facilitation of temporal discrimination (Tarpy, Lea, & Midgley, 1983; Tarpy et al., 1984). The rats trained to release the lever without the signal showed poorer temporal discrimination than their signaled counterparts (see Figure 1c). If

6 Signaled reinforcement effects in rats 109 the signal enhanced the salience of the FI offset and afforded better temporal discrimination, it led to attenuation of the response rate by inhibiting futile responding during the interval (see Figure 1a). However, if this had been the case, the signal should have had the same effect in the rats depressing the lever. The signal had a small facilitative effect on the temporal discrimination of these rats (see Figure 1c) and it enhanced, rather than attenuated, the response rate of the former rats (see Figure 1a). Although the experiment reported here indicates that the signaled reinforcement effect depends on the nature of the target response, we have no satisfactory explanation of this fact. Investigation with other responses, manipulanda, species, length of delay and reinforcement schedules is necessary. The limitation of our apparatus disallowed us more finely grained recordings of events, such as each interresponse time and duration of each response. Future exploration would require such recordings to resolve the enigma underlying this phenomenon. References Ator, N. A. (1991). Subjects and instrumentation. In I. H. Iversen & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), Experimental analysis of behavior: Part 1 (pp. 1 62). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Fry, W., Kelleher, R. T., & Cook, L. (1960). A mathematical index of performance on fixedinterval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3, Herrnstein, R. J. (1966). Superstition: A corollary of the principles of operant conditioning. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp ). New York: Appleton. Iversen, I. H. (1981). Response interactions with signaled delay of reinforcement. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 1, 3 9. Lieberman, D. A., McIntosh, D. C., & Thomas, G. V. (1979). Learning when reward is delayed: A marking hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, Nakajima, S., & Kitaguchi, K. (1996). Signaled reinforcement effects on fixed-interval performance of the rat. Animal Learning and Behavior, 24, Reed, P. (1989a). Influence of interresponse time reinforcement on signalled-reward effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, Reed, P. (1989b). Marking effects in instrumental performance on DRH schedules. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41B, Reed, P. (1992). Signalled delay of reward: Overshadowing versus sign-tracking explanations. Learning and Motivation, 23, Reed, P., & Hall, G. (1988). The schedule dependency of the signaled reinforcement effect. Learning and Motivation, 19, Reed, P., & Hall, G. (1989). The quasi-reinforcement effect: The influence of brief stimuli uncorrelated with reinforcement on variable ratio schedules. Learning and Motivation, 20, Reed, P., Collison, T., & Nokes, T. (1995). Aversive properties of auditory stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 26, Reed, P., Schachtman, T. R., & Hall, G. (1988a). Overshadowing and potentiation of instrumental responding in rats as a function of the schedules of reinforcement. Learning and Motivation, 19, Reed, P., Schachtman, T. R., & Hall, G. (1988b). Potentiation of responding on a VR schedule by a stimulus correlated with reinforcement: Effects of diffuse and localized signals. Animal Learning and Behavior, 16, Reed, P., Schachtman, T. R., & Rawlins, J. N. P. (1992). The effect of signalled reinforcement on a synthetic VI schedule. Learning and Motivation, 23, Rescorla, R. A. (1982). Effects of a stimulus intervening between CS and US in autoshaping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, Schachtman, T. R., & Reed, P. (1990). The role of response-reinforcer correlation in signaled reinforcement effects. Animal Learning and Behavior, 18, Skinner, B. F. (1956). A case history in scientific method. American Psychologist, 11, Tarpy, R. M., Lea, S. E. G., & Midgley, M. (1983). The role of response-reward correlation in stimulusresponse overshadowing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35B, Tarpy, R. M., Roberts, J. E., Lea, S. E. G., & Midgley, M. (1984). The stimulus-response overshadowing phenomenon with VI versus FI schedules of reinforcement. Animal Learning and Behavior, 12,

7 110 K. Kitaguchi and S. Nakajima Williams, B. A. (1982). Blocking the responsereinforcer association. In M. L. Commons, R. J. Herrnstein, & A. R. Wagner (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior, Vol. 3: Acquisition (pp ). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger. Williams, B. A., & Heyneman, N. (1982). Multiple determinants of blocking effects on operant behavior. Animal Learning and Behavior, 10, (Received Nov. 27, 1995; accepted Nov. 6, 1996)

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Phil Reed. Learn Behav (2011) 39:27 35 DOI /s Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

Phil Reed. Learn Behav (2011) 39:27 35 DOI /s Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010 Learn Behav (211) 39:27 35 DOI 1.17/s1342-1-3-5 Effects of response-independent stimuli on fixed-interval and fixed-ratio performance of rats: a model for stressful disruption of cyclical eating patterns

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning.

Magazine approach during a signal for food depends on Pavlovian, not instrumental, conditioning. In Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/xan/index.aspx 2013, vol. 39 (2), pp 107 116 2013 American Psychological Association DOI: 10.1037/a0031315

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1971, 15, 243-247 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) POSITIVE CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION: EFFECTS OF CS DURATION' KLAUS A. MICZEK AND SEBASTIAN P. GROSSMAN UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO

More information

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 11, 79-91 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2010) 79 Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats Rogelio Escobar National Autonomous University of Mexico

More information

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning

Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (3), 230-245 doi:10.3758/lb.37.3.230 Representations of single and compound stimuli in negative and positive patterning JUSTIN A. HARRIS, SABA A GHARA EI, AND CLINTON A. MOORE

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Potentiation of responding on a VR schedule by a stimulus correlated with reinforcement: Effects of diffuse and localized signals

Potentiation of responding on a VR schedule by a stimulus correlated with reinforcement: Effects of diffuse and localized signals AnimalLearning & Behavior 1988, 16 (1), 75-82 Potentiation of responding on a VR schedule by a stimulus correlated with reinforcement: Effects of diffuse and localized signals PHIL REED, TODD R. SCHACHTMAN,

More information

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 261-268 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) CONCURRENT SCHEULES OF PRIMARY AN CONITIONE REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' ONAL W. ZIMMERMAN CARLETON UNIVERSITY Rats responded

More information

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors

Contextual Control of Chained Instrumental Behaviors Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 42, No. 4, 401 414 2329-8456/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000112 Contextual

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

Operant Conditioning B.F. SKINNER

Operant Conditioning B.F. SKINNER Operant Conditioning B.F. SKINNER Reinforcement in Operant Conditioning Behavior Consequence Patronize Elmo s Diner It s all a matter of consequences. Rewarding Stimulus Presented Tendency to tell jokes

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

Learning and Motivation

Learning and Motivation Learning and Motivation 4 (29) 178 18 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Learning and Motivation journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Context blocking in rat autoshaping: Sign-tracking

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting

Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting Animal Learning & Behavior 1989, 17 (3), 269-279 Feature extinction enhances transfer of occasion setting PETER C. HOLLAND Duke University, Durham, North Carolina In three experiments, transfer effects

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

The influence ofbrief stimuli uncorrelated with reinforcement on choice between variable-ratio schedules

The influence ofbrief stimuli uncorrelated with reinforcement on choice between variable-ratio schedules Animal Learning & Behavior /993. 2/ (2). /59-/67 The influence ofbrief stimuli uncorrelated with reinforcement on choice between variable-ratio schedules PHIL REED and VAL SZZUDLO University ollege London

More information

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules

Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules Animal Learning& Behavior 1981,9 (3),406-410 Operant response topographies of rats receiving food or water reinforcers on FR or FI reinforcement schedules JOHN H. HULL, TIMOTHY J. BARTLETT, and ROBERT

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 90 (2012) 311 322 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journa l h omepa g e: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc US specificity of occasion setting:

More information

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations

Transfer of Control in Ambiguous Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 3, 231-248 Copyright 1991 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/53.00 Transfer of Control in Ambiguous

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1998, 70, 165 183 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES

More information

Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli. Centre for Computational and Animal Learning Research

Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli. Centre for Computational and Animal Learning Research Overshadowing by fixed and variable duration stimuli Charlotte Bonardi 1, Esther Mondragón 2, Ben Brilot 3 & Dómhnall J. Jennings 3 1 School of Psychology, University of Nottingham 2 Centre for Computational

More information

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER

OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS A. BRUNER JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2009, 92, 215 231 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) OBSERVING RESPONSES AND SERIAL STIMULI: SEARCHING FOR THE REINFORCING PROPERTIES OF THE S2 ROGELIO ESCOBAR AND CARLOS

More information

Two Kinds of Attention in Pavlovian Conditioning: Evidence for a Hybrid Model of Learning

Two Kinds of Attention in Pavlovian Conditioning: Evidence for a Hybrid Model of Learning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2010, Vol. 36, No. 4, 456 470 2010 American Psychological Association 0097-7403/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0018528 Two Kinds of Attention in Pavlovian

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule*

A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2 (3),168-172. A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule* JOHN HARSH and PETRO BADA Bowling Green State University,

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999 TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL A Senior Honors Thesis Presented By Vanessa E. Castagna June 999 999 by Vanessa E. Castagna ABSTRACT TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: A TEST OF

More information

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior.

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR TECHNIQUES FOR ESTABLISHING SCHEDULES WITH WHEEL RUNNING AS REINFORCEMENT IN RATS IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA 1993, 60, 219-238 NUMBER 1

More information

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association

Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Learning and Motivation 32, 349 366 (2001) doi:10.1006/lmot.2001.1089, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Occasion Setters: Specificity to the US and the CS US Association Charlotte Bonardi

More information

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is

VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY. in the two conditions. If this were possible, well understood where the criterion response is JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR LICK-SHOCK CONTINGENCIES IN THE RATT1 VERNON L. QUINSEY DALHOUSIE UNIVERSITY 1972, 17, 119-125 NUMBER I (JANUARY) Hungry rats were allowed to lick an 8%

More information

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology Title Renewal of Formerly Conditioned Fear in Rats after Extensive Extinction Training Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7d41p8fj Journal International

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Overshadowing by fixed- and variable-duration stimuli

Overshadowing by fixed- and variable-duration stimuli THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2015 Vol. 68, No. 3, 523 542, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.960875 Overshadowing by fixed- and variable-duration stimuli Charlotte Bonardi 1,

More information

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT'

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT' D. ALAN STUBBS AND PHILIP J. SILVERMAN UNIVERSITY OF MAINE, ORONO AND WORCESTER

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1965, Vol. 59, No. 1, 13-17 AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 GORDON BOWER, RONALD STARR, AND LEAH LAZAROVITZ Stanford

More information

The highlight for November is by Sadahiko Nakajima who is at the Department of Psychological Science, Kwansei Gakuin University in Nishinomiya,

The highlight for November is by Sadahiko Nakajima who is at the Department of Psychological Science, Kwansei Gakuin University in Nishinomiya, The highlight for November is by Sadahiko Nakajima who is at the Department of Psychological Science, Kwansei Gakuin University in Nishinomiya, Japan. Dr. Nakajima s interest and efforts in taste aversion

More information

Associative Learning

Associative Learning Learning Learning Associative Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Biological Components of Learning Cognitive Components of Learning Behavioral Therapies Associative

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF OTHER BEHAVIOR AND RESPONSE SUPPRESSION:

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF OTHER BEHAVIOR AND RESPONSE SUPPRESSION: psychological Record, 1993, 43, 271-288 DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF OTHER BEHAVIOR AND RESPONSE SUPPRESSION: THE EFFECTS OF THE RESPONSE-REINFORCEMENT INTERVAL THOMAS S. RIEG, NELSON F. SMITH, and STUART

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 84 (2010) 511 515 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Changes in stimulus control during guided skill

More information

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Chapter 6/9: Learning Chapter 6/9: Learning Learning A relatively durable change in behavior or knowledge that is due to experience. The acquisition of knowledge, skills, and behavior through reinforcement, modeling and natural

More information

AVOIDANCE LEARNING IN SHUTTLING AND NONSHUTTLING SITUATIONS, WITH AND WITHOUT A BARRIER. Kobe Juvenile Detention and Classification Home

AVOIDANCE LEARNING IN SHUTTLING AND NONSHUTTLING SITUATIONS, WITH AND WITHOUT A BARRIER. Kobe Juvenile Detention and Classification Home Japanese Psychological Research 1964, Vol.6, No.3, 129-135 AVOIDANCE LEARNING IN SHUTTLING AND NONSHUTTLING SITUATIONS, WITH AND WITHOUT A BARRIER EIJI KUNITOMI, Kobe Juvenile Detention and Classification

More information

The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance

The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1972 The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance Deborah Ann Cory Western

More information

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition

The Role of Temporal Relationships in the Transfer of Conditioned Inhibition Denniston, James C., Robert P. Cole, and Ralph R. Miller. (1998). "The role of temporal relationships in the transfer of conditioned inhibition." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

More information

CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY

CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY CONTINGENCY VALUES OF VARYING STRENGTH AND COMPLEXITY By ANDREW LAWRENCE SAMAHA A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training

Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 17, 243-268 (1986) Transfer after Serial Feature Positive Discrimination Training PETER C. HOLLAND University of Pittsburgh Three experiments with rat subjects examined the transfer

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour

PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010: Brain and Behaviour PSYC2010 Notes Textbook used Week 1-3: Bouton, M.E. (2016). Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis. 2nd Ed. Sinauer Week 4-6: Rieger, E. (Ed.) (2014) Abnormal Psychology:

More information

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 00, 79, 6 NUMBER (MARCH) RESPONDING FOR SUCROSE AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: EFFECTS OF SUCROSE CONCENTRATION AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCER DURATION

More information

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 6, NUMBER 2 APRIL, 1963 FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3 Responses were maintained by a variable-interval schedule

More information

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern

Effects of Repeated Acquisitions and Extinctions on Response Rate and Pattern Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 322 328 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.3.322

More information

Occasional reinforced trials during extinction can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition

Occasional reinforced trials during extinction can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 371 390 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Occasional reinforced trials during extinction can slow the rate of rapid reacquisition Mark E. Bouton, Amanda M. Woods, and Oskar

More information

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery L132 NT MJA/cla Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 60-73 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.60 Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery Gonzalo P. Urcelay University of Cambridge, Cambridge,

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 6 Appetitive Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 6 Appetitive Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 6 Appetitive Conditioning Midterm Results Animals Search and rescue dogs -- http://www.sardogsus.org/ Dog Tricks Appetitive Conditioning Appetitive something desirable

More information

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE BROWN UNIVERSITY 1967, 10,

More information

Decremental Carryover Effects of Sucrose Ingestion in the Negative Anticipatory Contrast Procedure in Rats

Decremental Carryover Effects of Sucrose Ingestion in the Negative Anticipatory Contrast Procedure in Rats Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1995, Vol. 21, No. 4, 304-317 Copyright 3995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/95/S3.00 Decremental Carryover Effects

More information

Excitation and Inhibition in Unblocking

Excitation and Inhibition in Unblocking Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1988, Vol. 14, No. 3, 261-279 Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/88/S00.75 Excitation and Inhibition

More information

ACQUISITION OF DISCRIMINATED OPERANT RESPONDING: THE EFFECTS OF BRIEF REINFORCEMENT DELAYS. MATTHEW E. ANDRZEJEWSKI University of Wisconsin-Madison

ACQUISITION OF DISCRIMINATED OPERANT RESPONDING: THE EFFECTS OF BRIEF REINFORCEMENT DELAYS. MATTHEW E. ANDRZEJEWSKI University of Wisconsin-Madison The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 603-620 ACQUISITION OF DISCRIMINATED OPERANT RESPONDING: THE EFFECTS OF BRIEF REINFORCEMENT DELAYS MATTHEW E. ANDRZEJEWSKI University of Wisconsin-Madison SHELLY TERRY-CAIN

More information

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation

Relations Between Pavlovian-Instrumental Transfer and Reinforcer Devaluation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2004, Vol. 30, No. 2, 104 117 Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.2.104

More information

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1992. 20 (3). 233-239 The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats JAMES S. MILLER and JOYCE A.

More information

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit Denniston, J. C., Savastano, H. I., Blaisdell, A. P., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Cue competition as a retrieval deficit. Learning and Motivation, 34(1): 1-31. (Feb 2003) Published by Elsevier (ISSN: 1095-9122).

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning Operant Conditioning The nature of reinforcement: Premack s probability differential theory Response deprivation

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

I. Classical Conditioning

I. Classical Conditioning Learning Chapter 8 Learning A relatively permanent change in an organism that occur because of prior experience Psychologists must study overt behavior or physical changes to study learning Learning I.

More information

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1989, Vol. 15, No. 3, 232-241 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-740389$00.75 Contextual Effects in Conditioning,

More information

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping

Predictive Accuracy and the Effects of Partial Reinforcement on Serial Autoshaping Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1985 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/85/$00.75 1985, VOl. 11, No. 4, 548-564 Predictive Accuracy and the

More information

CONTINGENCY RELATIONS: THE ROLE OF

CONTINGENCY RELATIONS: THE ROLE OF JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR COLLEGE STUDENTS' RESPONDING TO AND RATING OF CONTINGENCY RELATIONS: THE ROLE OF TEMPORAL CONTIGUITY EDWARD A. WASSERMAN AND DANNY J. NEUNABER THE UNIVERSITY

More information

Observational learning of instrumental discriminations in the rat: The role of demonstrator type

Observational learning of instrumental discriminations in the rat: The role of demonstrator type THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (11), 1909 1920 Observational learning of instrumental discriminations in the rat: The role of demonstrator type A. L. Saggerson and R. C. Honey

More information

Perceptual learning transfer in an appetitive Pavlovian task

Perceptual learning transfer in an appetitive Pavlovian task Learn Behav (2017) 45:115 123 DOI 10.3758/s13420-016-0245-y Perceptual learning transfer in an appetitive Pavlovian task Antonio A. Artigas 1 & Jose Prados 2 Published online: 5 August 2016 # Psychonomic

More information

Unit 6 Learning.

Unit 6 Learning. Unit 6 Learning https://www.apstudynotes.org/psychology/outlines/chapter-6-learning/ 1. Overview 1. Learning 1. A long lasting change in behavior resulting from experience 2. Classical Conditioning 1.

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION. William Travis Suits TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC EXTINCTION OF PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONED INHIBITION Except where reference is made to the works of others, the work described in this dissertation is my own or was done in collaboration

More information

Acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia by rats in proximity to upcoming food delivery

Acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia by rats in proximity to upcoming food delivery Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 491 499 Acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia by rats in proximity to upcoming food delivery GINESA LÓPEZ-CRESPO and MIGUEL RODRÍGUEZ Universidad de Almería, Almería,

More information

Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data

Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 2004, 36 (4), 661 669 Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data PAULO GUILHARDI and RUSSELL M.

More information

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0039-1 Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Cody W. Polack & Mario A. Laborda & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 July 2011 # Psychonomic

More information

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 479-490 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine

More information

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's JOURNAt, OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1968, 1 1, 1-8 NUMBER I (JANUARY) A UTO-SHAPING OF THE PIGEON'S KEY-PECK' PAUL L. BROWN AND HERBERT M. JENKINS MCMASTER UNIVERSITY Reliable acquistion

More information

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2015, 103, 269 287 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG

More information

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Illinois Wesleyan University Digital Commons @ IWU Honors Projects Psychology 2000 Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Sara J. Estle '00 Illinois Wesleyan University Recommended Citation

More information

EVALUATIONS OF DELAYED REINFORCEMENT IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES

EVALUATIONS OF DELAYED REINFORCEMENT IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES EVALUATIONS OF DELAYED REINFORCEMENT IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES By JOLENE RACHEL SY A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION

TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES SN CONDITIONED SUPPRESSION Tnesis for the Degree of M. A. MiCHiGAN STATE UNWERSITY RODNEY CHARLES HOWARD 1977 f L13 1-;- R Y Michiga: State gr? Umw 1y ABSTRACT TIME DEPENDENT CHANGES

More information

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Psicológica (2004), 25, 45-65. Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Matías López*, Raúl Cantora*, and Luis Aguado** 1 * Universidad de

More information

Overshadowing and Blocking as Acquisition De cits: No Recovery After Extinction of Overshadowing or Blocking Cues

Overshadowing and Blocking as Acquisition De cits: No Recovery After Extinction of Overshadowing or Blocking Cues q THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1999, 52B (4), 307±333 Overshadowing and Blocking as Acquisition De cits: No Recovery After Extinction of Overshadowing or Blocking Cues Peter C. Holland

More information