Joint Influence of Visual and Auditory Words in the Stroop Task

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Joint Influence of Visual and Auditory Words in the Stroop Task"

Transcription

1 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: DOI /s Joint Influence of Visual and Auditory Words in the Stroop Task Wendy S. Francis 1 & Colin M. MacLeod 2 & Randolph S. Taylor 1 Published online: 14 October 2016 # The Psychonomic Society, Inc Abstract We conducted four Stroop color word experiments to examine how multiple stimuli influence interference. Experiments 1a and 1b showed that interference was strong when the word and color were integrated, and that visual and auditory words made independent contributions to interference when these words had different meanings. Experiments 2 and 3 confirmed this pattern when the word information and color information were not integrated, and hence when overall interference was substantially less. Auditory and visual interference effects are comparable except when the visual distracter is integrated with the color, in which case interference is substantially enhanced. Overall, these results are interpreted as being most consistent with a joint influence account of interference as opposed to a capture account. Keywords Selective attention. Interference. Stroop effect. Modality There are a great many situations in our experience where irrelevant information disrupts our processing of relevant information the phenomenon known as cognitive interference. None, however, is more familiar than the classic Stroop color word interference effect (Stroop, 1935). Incongruent color words cause robust interference in naming print colors when those words are presented visually. In fact, incongruent color words also interfere with color naming * Wendy S. Francis wfrancis@utep.edu 1 2 Department of Psychology, University of Texas at El Paso, 500 W. University Ave, El Paso, TX 79968, USA University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON N2L 3G1, Canada when the words are presented auditorily (e.g., Cowan, 1989; Elliott, Cowan, & Valle-Inclan, 1998; Elliott et al., 2014; Roelofs, 2005). To say that the visual Stroop color word task has been extensively studied would be a major understatement (see MacLeod, 1991, for an extensive review), but auditory versions have been much less thoroughly explored (see, e.g., Green & Barber, 1981; McClain, 1983), and there has been little work on cross-modal interference, the subject of this article. Thackray and Jones (1971) initially reported that crossmodal interference did not occur between auditory and visual stimuli in the Stroop task. However, their use of a manual response, where participants press buttons for colors, likely limited interference overall (see MacLeod, 1991). Cowan and Barron (1987) instead used the traditional oral response and the classic multiple color word stimuli per card version of the Stroop task. They showed that, relative to auditory controls (hearing letters of the alphabet or the word the presented repeatedly), hearing an auditory recording of random color words interfered with naming print colors. They also suggested that the interfering effects of auditory and visual distracter words were additive. This cross-modal auditory interference effect has since been replicated using trial-by-trial manipulations of word color congruency (e.g., Elliot et al., 1998; Roelofs, 2005), more analogous to the standard visual procedure, but auditory interference was not directly compared to visual interference. Most recently, Elliott et al. (2014) used the trial-by-trial version of the Stroop task with vocal responding. Their controls were noncolor words and a silent condition. Contrary to Cowan and Barron (1987), they observed that combining the two modalities did not increment interference (see also Miles, Madden, & Jones, 1989). Given this empirical conflict, the relative influences of auditory and visual distracters remain unclear. Our first empirical goal, therefore, was to determine how these two modality-specific effects combine.

2 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: Combined Effects of Multiple Sources of Interference Only a few studies have investigated whether presenting two incongruent visual distracters increases interference relative to presenting only one incongruent visual distracter, or have compared the effects of pairs of visual distracters that were consistent or inconsistent with each other. Moreover, these studies have differed in the paradigms used for presenting the second stimulus and whether the distracters were integrated with or separate from the color stimulus. Generally, no differences have been observed (Kahneman & Chajczyk, 1983; MacLeod & Bors, 2002; MacLeod & Hodder, 1998; Yee & Hunt, 1991); that is, interference has not been greater for multiple interfering visual words. Understanding the degree to which multiple incongruent stimuli within modality or across modalities influence interference is our second empirical goal of this research, growing out of our first goal. The apparent lack of increased interference from a second distracter, or from having two inconsistent rather than two consistent distracters, was first explained using a capture account. The idea, developed in the context of visual attention, was that whichever word was attended first captured attention and the other was not processed sufficiently to produce additional interference (Kahneman & Chajczyk, 1983). By mixing incongruent and congruent word stimuli, however, MacLeod and Bors (2002) showed that the second word did have an influence. Specifically, if one word was congruent and the other was incongruent, the interfering effect of the incongruent word was reduced, thus calling the capture account into question. However, there still was no difference in the interference observed for two inconsistent versus two consistent incongruent visual distracters. The alternative explanation offered by MacLeod and Bors was that there is joint influence of the two distracters, that the Bword dimension continues to be monitored during the attempt to identify and produce the name of the color^ (p. 789). More recent work also leads to the conclusion that attention shifting can occur within Stroop stimuli (Cho, Choi, & Proctor, 2012; Cho, Lien, & Proctor, 2006). We set out in this study to determine whether having incongruent color word inputs from both the auditory and visual modalities would lead to greater interference than having incongruent input from only one modality, or whether there would be a pattern of joint influence. One possible reason to expect a pattern of joint influence would be that visual and auditory distracters might influence color naming at different stages. For example, unlike visual distracters, auditory distracters might have their primary effect at the level of retrieving the phonology of the response word. Another possible reason to expect joint influence would be that auditory and visual distracters may tap different pools of modality-specific attentional resources (Wickens, 1984), allowing both modalities to influence responding. Only a couple of previous studies have combined auditory and visual distracters. Cowan and Barron (1987) showedthat playing a recording of random color words interfered with naming print colors of a mixture of congruent and incongruent color words. The magnitude of the visual effect was larger than that of the auditory effect. However, because incongruent and congruent auditory color words were mixed, the interfering effect of the auditory distracters was likely underestimated. Also, because the distracting words were presented in a continuing stream, there could be no separation of trial types particular combinations of auditory and visual color words could not be examined. Nevertheless, these results, along with the lack of interaction between the presence of the auditory recording and the congruency of the printed color words, suggest that auditory and visual inputs together cause more interference than either alone. Elliot et al. (1998) extended these results, reaching a similar conclusion. In contrast, as mentioned, Elliott et al. (2014) recently presented findings using the single trial Stroop method that suggested no enhanced interference when distracter words were presented in both modalities compared to only one modality. Our goals are to resolve this empirical discrepancy at the same time as we tackle the capture vs joint influence theoretical distinction. The Present Study The experiments that we report all used large sample sizes and quite large numbers of trials per condition to optimize power to address the critical questions. Experiments 1a and 1b compared interference effects for auditory distracters, visual distracters, and combinations of auditory and visual distracters that were either congruent or incongruent with the target response and with each other. In both of these experiments, visual distractors were integrated with the target color information, as in the classic Stroop task, and Experiment 1b was a direct replication of Experiment 1a. Experiment 2 was a replication of Experiment 1, but with visual distracters that were spatially separated from the target color stimulus, a change well established to sharply reduce overall interference (see, e.g., Kahneman & Chajczyk, 1983; MacLeod, 1998). Experiment 3 involved only visual distracters and examined whether spatially integrated and spatially separated distracters would produce nonredundant interference. In all cases, the capture account predicts that combined conditions should produce interference comparable to that of one of the distracters (the one that captures): There should be no increment from one to two distracters. In contrast, the joint influence account predicts that two incongruent distracters should produce more interference than one incongruent distracter. Similarly, the

3 202 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: capture account predicts that two inconsistent incongruent distracters should produce equivalent interference to two consistent incongruent distractors, whereas the joint influence account predicts stronger interference for two inconsistent incongruent distracters, each of which would make a separate contribution to overall interference. Experiment 1 The purpose of Experiment 1 was to compare interference effects for auditory and visual distracters and to determine whether their effects were or were not redundant. Participants performed color naming with incongruent auditory, visual, or both auditory and visual stimuli that were the same or different from each other in meaning. They also named colors with congruent auditory, visual, or both auditory and visual stimuli. In all cases, the visual words were integrated with the to-be-named color information and responding was vocal: Both of these features follow the standard Stroop procedure (see MacLeod, 1991, forareview). Researchers have often debated the best control condition in a Stroop experiment (see MacLeod, 1991; Melara & Mounts, 1993). Should one use meaningless symbols, as Stroop (1935) himself did, or is it more appropriate to use noncolor words, which require reading but without specific competition (see also Klein, 1964, for consideration of multiple possible comparison conditions)? The answer in experimental psychology is always that there is no control condition that is perfect regardless of the question being asked. Here, we have chosen to use an auditory tone, the best analogy we could make in the auditory modality to a meaningless string of symbols in the visual modality. While this control condition might be seen as overestimating interference, it certainly can be argued that it does not itself have the potential to interfere with the color naming response, making it a baseline if not a perfect control. We chose this simple, Bno information^ control to align with the bulk of the Stroop literature. Experiment 1a Method Participants Participants were 50 students (10 men, 40 women) from the University of Texas at El Paso whose primary language was English (i.e., they reported speaking no other languages fluently). Their median age was 19 years. Most were recruited from introductory psychology classes and received credit toward a class research requirement; the rest were unpaid volunteers. Apparatus Visual stimuli were presented on the monitor of a Macintosh computer. Sequencing of stimuli and timing of responses were controlled using PsyScope software (Cohen, MacWhinney, Flatt, & Provost, 1993). Auditory stimuli were presented via headphones. Vocal response times were measured using a high-impedance microphone attached to a PsyScope button box (New Micros, Dallas). Stimuli Visual word stimuli were the color words red, blue, green, and purple; the control visual stimulus was xxxxx. The visual stimuli served as the color carriers (i.e., the color was integrated with the visual stimulus). Auditory stimuli were the same color words recorded by a native English speaker. The control sound was an unramped tone (middle C, 262 Hz) generated by the computer at approximately the same volume as the auditory word stimuli and lasting for 500 ms. Design The eight trial types are illustrated in Table 1. For each trial type, there were 48 trials, with every possible color combination used equally often, resulting in 384 total trials. It is important to note that there were two types of Bincongruent both^: The auditory and visual word could be the same or they could be different. Procedure Participants were tested individually in sessions lasting approximately 30 minutes. They were instructed to name aloud into the microphone the print color of each visual stimulus as quickly and accurately as possible. The visual stimulus on each trial appeared in lowercase letters at the center of the screen in Arial 36-point font. On each trial, the auditory and visual stimuli were presented simultaneously, and the visual stimulus remained on the screen until a vocal response was registered. The intertrial interval was 1,250 ms. After 10 practice trials, the 384 experimental trials were presented in a random sequence, with a rest after every 96 trials. The experimenter recorded unexpected responses and voice relay misfires on a worksheet preprinted with the expected responses so that these trials could be removed prior to analysis. Results Only correct and valid responses were included in the RT analysis. Trials with response errors or voice relay errors were excluded, as were trials with outlier RTs (over 3,000 ms or under 300 ms); 97.5 % of trials were retained for analysis. For each participant, a mean RT was calculated for each condition, and these were the scores that were submitted to analysis. Interference (facilitation) scores were obtained for each participant by subtracting the RT of the control condition from the RT of each of the other conditions. Thus, a positive score indicates interference whereas a negative score indicates

4 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: Table 1 Experiments 1a and 1b: Response Times and Interference Scores as a Function of Condition Condition Stimulus Examples Experiment 1a Experiment 1b Auditory Stimulus Visual Stimulus Print Color RT (ms) Interference (ms) RT (ms) Interference (ms) Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Control <tone> xxxxx Blue 680 (13) 758 (18) Congruent Visual <tone> blue Blue 692 (16) 12 (6) 781 (21) 22 (6) Auditory blue xxxxx Blue 715 (13) 35 (5) 779 (17) 21 (5) Visual + Auditory blue blue Blue 750 (16) 69 (7) 838 (20) 80 (7) Incongruent Visual <tone> red Blue 802 (16) 122 (7) 872 (20) 114 (8) Auditory red xxxxx Blue 739 (16) 58 (7) 804 (17) 46 (5) Vis + Aud Same red red Blue 834 (16) 153 (8) 878 (17) 120 (8) Vis + Aud Diff red green Blue 855 (19) 174 (10) 901 (19) 143 (10) Each cell is based on a maximum of 48 trials (before elimination of error and equipment-failure trials). facilitation. The principal data, the response times and the interference/facilitation scores derived from those response times, are displayed in Table 1. Table 4 displays the error rates and interference/facilitation scores derived from those error rates; their corresponding analyses are presented in the Appendix. Patterns evident in the response times typically were also apparent in the error rates; there was no evidence of speed accuracy tradeoff. Congruent conditions None of the congruent conditions showed facilitation; rather, all actually showed interference despite the word color congruence (ps.05). A 2 (auditory word present or absent) 2 (visual word present or absent) repeated-measures ANOVA showed that, relative to the control condition, interference increased due to the presence of a word whether that word was visual, F(1, 49) = 18.67, MSE = 1,435, p <.001, partial η 2 =.28, or auditory, F(1, 49) = , MSE =894,p <.001, partial η 2 =.71. The interfering effects of congruent visual and auditory words interacted, F(1, 49) = 14.86, MSE =420, p <.001, partial η 2 =.23, such that their combined effect was greater than the sum of their individual effects. The unanticipated interference on the congruent trials was part of the motivation for carrying out a faithful replication of Experiment 1a Experiment 1b reported below. Incongruent conditions Interference was significant in each of the four incongruent conditions relative to the control condition (ps <.001). To test whether the effects of interference from the two modalities interacted or were independent, two 2 (visual word present or absent) 2 (auditory word present or absent) repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted. These analyses differed only in which visual + auditory condition (same or different) was included. For the analysis including the visual + auditory same condition, relative to the control condition, the presence of a word increased interference both when the word was visual, F(1, 49) = , MSE =1,512, p <.001, partial η 2 =.89, and when the word was auditory, F(1, 49) = 84.52, MSE =1,188, p <.001, partial η 2 =.63. In this case, the interaction of the auditory and visual words, F(1, 49) = 11.28, MSE =786, p =.002, partial η 2 =.19, indicated that the combined effect was less than the sum of the individual effects. For the analysis including the visual + auditory different condition, again the presence of a word increased interference whether that word was visual, F(1, 49) = , MSE =1,821,p <.001, partial η 2 =.89,orauditory, F(1, 49) = 92.51, MSE =1,650,p <.001, partial η 2 =.65. In this case, auditory and visual words did not interact, F <1, partial η 2 =.02, indicating that, when visual and auditory words have different meanings, their effects are independent. Experiment 1b Experiment 1b was a direct replication of Experiment 1a conducted with a new participant sample and using new equipment. At issue was the unexpected appearance of interference in the congruent conditions, but we also sought to confirm the entire pattern observed in Experiment 1a. Method Participants Participants were 50 students (14 men, 36 women) from the University of Texas at El Paso who were fluent speakers of English. These individuals were from the same source as Experiment 1a, but in a subsequent academic year. Their median age was 19 years. Most were recruited from

5 204 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: introductory psychology classes and received credit toward a class research requirement; the rest were unpaid volunteers. Apparatus Visual stimuli were presented on the monitor of an imac desktop computer. Sequencing of stimuli and timing of responses were controlled using PsyScope X software (Cohen et al., 1993). Auditory stimuli were presented on headphones. Vocal response times were measured using a high-impedance microphone attached to an iolabs button box. Design, materials, and procedure These all were identical to those of Experiment 1a. Results Only correct and valid responses were included in the RT analysis. Trials with response errors or voice relay errors were excluded, as were trials with outlier RTs (over 3,000 ms or under 300 ms); 98.5 % of trials were retained for analysis. The principal data, the response times and the interference/facilitation scores derived from those response times, are displayed in Table 1. Error rates and interference/facilitation scores derived from those error rates are displayed in Table 4, with the corresponding analyses presented in the Appendix. Patterns evident in the response times were typically also apparent in the error rates; there was no evidence of speed accuracy tradeoff. Response times were about 70 ms slower overall in Experiment 1b than in Experiment 1b, a change that we attribute to two factors: (1) there was no language restriction in Experiment 1b, so participants who were bilingual were included, and (2) participants seemed to place a somewhat higher value on accuracy in Experiment 1b than in Experiment 1a. This overall slower responding did not, however, result in any substantial change in the pattern of response times across the eight conditions with respect to Experiment 1a,as Table 1 shows. Congruent conditions None of the congruent conditions showed facilitation; rather, all actually showed interference (ps <.001). A 2 (auditory word present or absent) 2 (visual word present or absent) repeated-measures ANOVA showed that, relative to the control condition, the presence of a visual word increased interference, F(1, 49) = 59.94, MSE =1,366, p <.001, partial η 2 =.55, as did the presence of an auditory word, F(1, 49) = 56.04, MSE = 1,377, p <.001, partial η 2 =.53. The interfering effects of congruent visual and auditory words interacted, F(1, 49) = 29.84, MSE =544,p <.001, partial η 2 =.38, such that the combined effect was greater than the sum of the individual effects. Essentially, the pattern that was unexpected in Experiment 1a was replicated in Experiment 1b. Incongruent conditions Interference was significant in each of the four incongruent conditions (ps <.001). To test whether the effects of interference from the two modalities interacted or were independent, the same 2 (visual word present or absent) 2 (auditory word present or absent) repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted as in Experiment 1a. For the analysis including the visual + auditory same condition, the presence of a word increased interference relative to the control condition both when the word was visual, F(1, 49) = , MSE =2,484,p <.001, partial η 2 =.78, and when it was auditory, F(1, 49) = 36.54, MSE =949, p <.001, partial η 2 =.43. In this case, the interaction of the auditory and visual words, F(1, 49) = 38.91, MSE =511, p <.001, partial η 2 =.44, indicated that the combined effect was less than the sum of the individual effects. For the analysis including the visual + auditory different condition, the presence of a word increased interference both when the word was visual, F(1, 49) = , MSE =2,988, p <.001, partial η 2 =.79, and when it was auditory, F(1, 49) = 62.67, MSE = 1,153, p <.001, partial η 2 =.56. The interaction of the auditory and visual words, F(1, 49) = 8.27, MSE = 413, p =.006, partial η 2 =.14, indicated that the combined effect was less than the sum of the individual effects. Again, the critical patterns observed in Experiment 1a replicated here. Discussion The results of Experiment 1a and Experiment 1b correspond very well. We will shortly consider the principal results those from the incongruent condition. But the results from the congruent condition, although not the focus of this study, seem puzzling: Rather than facilitation, there was consistent interference resulting from words congruent with the display color both across conditions within each experiment and across the two replications. Certainly, the average interference in the congruent conditions (overall mean of 40 ms) is substantially lower than the average interference in the incongruent conditions (overall mean of 116 ms), but this unanticipated interference from congruent words still appears to be reliable and thus warrants an explanation, although any such explanation is necessarily post hoc. Our suspicion is that the apparent interference in the congruent condition has to do not with the congruent condition being slow but with the control condition being fast. Notice that for the incongruent conditions, the lowest interference occurs in the auditory-only condition where the to-beignored visual stimulus is xxxxx (mean interference across the replications = 54 ms), and the next lowest interference occurs in the visual-only condition where the to-be-ignored auditory stimulus is the tone (mean interference across the replications = 118 ms), contrasting with the cases where there is a word in both modalities: visual + auditory same (mean interference across the replications = 137 ms) and visual + auditory different (mean interference across the replications = 158 ms).

6 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: We note that this same pattern is evident for the congruent conditions: Interference is lowest in the visual-only and auditory-only congruent conditions. The Bplaceholders^ in each modality tone and xxxxx are quite distinctive both in physical form and in frequency of occurrence relative to the words appearing on all of the other trials. In particular, their combination in the control condition may have signaled to participants that the color was all that mattered on such trials, alerting them and making it easier for them to focus on the color, thereby speeding responding. This would, of course, lead to apparent interference in the congruent condition. We note that this apparent interference in the congruent condition disappears in Experiments 2 and 3 where the distinctiveness of xxxxx in particular, but also of the tone, is reduced. Our principal results of interest, though, are those from the incongruent conditions. Here, auditory words produced less interference than visual words, as suggested by previous research but not previously tested within a single experiment nor using the individual item method to permit examination of specific relations. Because we used the SOA previously showntomaximizeauditory interference (Roelofs, 2005), we are confident that this result is not merely an artifact of the serial nature of auditory stimulus presentation. It is likely the case, however, that the separation of the auditory word and the visual color as opposed to their integration when both are visual contributes to this difference. Two incongruent words led to greater interference than did a single incongruent word, as previously reported in a visualonly study by MacLeod and Bors (2002). Even two distracters with the same meaning (e.g., seeing and hearing the word green when the response should be Bred^ to the print color) produced stronger interference than did a visual or auditory word alone. When the two distracters had the same meaning, their effects were underadditive, indicating that they were not independent. When the two distracters differed in meaning, interference was greater: In fact, interference in the combined condition was approximated by the sum of the interference from the visual and auditory words alone, suggesting that the effects of the two different words were independent. These results are inconsistent with the capture account because only one word should Bcapture^ attention, so that a second word should not influence responding. These results are, however, consistent with the joint influence account, where multiple distracters can each influence responding. Indeed, these findings even demonstrate that independent contributions can be made by words in different modalities, where one might expect the visual word the color carrier to uniquely capture distracter processing. Our results also demonstrate that, in accord with intuition, experiencing the same distracter twice has less impact than does experiencing two different distracters. Experiment 2 Although somewhat counterintuitive, in that integrated stimuli would seem to encourage capture more than separated stimuli, it is possible that results consistent with the joint influence account like those found in Experiments 1a and 1b could be limited to the situation where one distracter is perceptually integrated with the target stimulus and the other is not. For this reason, we carried out a conceptual replication of Experiments 1a and 1b in which the visual distracter stimuli were spatially separated from the color stimulus, a method that has been used frequently in the Stroop literature (see MacLeod, 1998, for a brief review). Interference was expected to be smaller, as is typically the case with separated as opposed to integrated Stroop stimuli (cf. MacLeod, 1998; Roberts & Besner, 2005), but the question was whether the interference pattern would still be the same. Method Participants Participants were 48 students (19 men, 29 women) from the University of Texas at El Paso whose primary or only language was English. Their median age was 20 years. All were undergraduate students, most of whom were recruited from introductory psychology classes and received credit toward a class research requirement; the rest were unpaid volunteers. Materials, design, and procedure The design and procedure were identical to those of Experiment 1a with one exception rather than being integrated with the color information, the visual words were presented in black as flankers to the colored target stimulus, which was always xxxxx. The colored target stimulus appeared in the center of the screen, with the flanker words presented immediately above and immediately below the target. The experimental conditions are listed in Table 2. Results RT data were processed in the same manner as in Experiments 1a and 1b. RTs and their derived interference scores are given in Table 2. AsinExperiments1a and 1b, the error analyses appear in the Appendix, with Table 5 displaying the error data for Experiment 2. Congruent conditions No reliable facilitation or interference effects were observed in the congruent condition (ps >.10). Incongruent conditions Interference was reliable for all incongruent conditions (ps <.001). As in Experiments 1a and 1b, we carried out two 2 2 ANOVAs. In the analysis

7 206 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: Table 2 Experiment 2: Response Times and Interference Scores as a Function of Condition Stimulus Examples RT (ms) Interference (ms) Condition Auditory Flanker Target Color Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Control <tone> xxxxx xxxxx Blue 722 (22) Congruent Visual <tone> blue xxxxx Blue 722 (22) 0 (4) Auditory blue xxxxx xxxxx Blue 732 (23) 10 (6) Visual + Auditory blue blue xxxxx Blue 725 (23) 2 (6) Incongruent Visual <tone> red xxxxx Blue 758 (21) 36 (5) Auditory red xxxxx xxxxx Blue 768 (22) 46 (5) Vis + Aud Same red red xxxxx Blue 811 (22) 88 (7) Vis + Aud Diff red green xxxxx Blue 808 (22) 86 (6) involving the visual + auditory same condition, the presence of a word increased interference, relative to the control condition, both when that word was visual, F(1, 47) = 96.92, MSE =756,p <.001, partial η 2 =.67, and when it was auditory, F(1, 47) = , MSE =1,047,p <.001, partial η 2 =.70. The auditory and visual words did not interact, F(1, 47) = 2.01, MSE = 291, p =.163, partial η 2 =.04. In the analysis including the visual + auditory different condition, again the presence of a word increased interference both for visual words, F(1, 47) = , MSE = 569, p <.001, partial η 2 =.72, and for auditory words, F(1, 47) = , MSE = 1,049, p <.001, partial η 2 =.69. And again, the auditory and visual words did not interact, F <1,partialη 2 =.02. Discussion Experiment 2 showed that the interference magnitudes of auditory words and of spatially separated visual words were comparable; if anything, the spatially separated visual words appeared to have a smaller basic interference effect. Auditory color word distracters and spatially separated visual color word distracters produced independent interference effects in color naming, whether they were the same word or two different words. Thus, although effect sizes were smaller in Experiment 2 because of the word and color being separated in the visual display, the critical interference effects seen in Experiments 1a and 1b were replicated, again more consistent with the joint influence account than with the capture account. Note that the use of xxxxx as the color carrier on all trials made that visual stimulus more frequent and less distinctive than it was in Experiments 1a and 1b, helping to explain why the apparent interference observed for congruent trials in Experiments 1a and 1b was no longer evident in Experiment 2. Experiment 3 Finally, we considered the possibility that using the auditory modality was somehow special in producing nonredundant effects. Auditory and visual words could have independent effects because, as separate modalities, they might tap different pools of attentional resources (e.g., Wickens, 1984). If so, then auditory and visual words might exert joint influence, but multiple visual words might not. To address this issue, in Experiment 3, only visual distracters were included. A visual distracter word was presented either integrated with the target color, separated from the target color (the word above or below the colored stimulus), or both. In the case of both, paralleling the preceding experiments, the two words could be the same or different. The critical change was that there were no auditory stimuli in Experiment 3. Would the pattern for two visual stimuli one integrated and one separated be analogous to that for the case of an auditory stimulus and a visual stimulus? How large a role has using two modalities played in the results observed thus far? Method Participants Participants were 52 students (19 men, 33 women) from the University of Texas at El Paso whose primary or only language was English. Their median age was 19 years. All were undergraduate students, most of whom were recruited from introductory psychology classes and received credit toward a class research requirement; the remainder were unpaid volunteers. Stimuli As previously, visual word stimuli were the color words red, blue, green, and purple, and the control stimulus was xxxxx. On each trial, three visual stimuli were presented. The target stimulus was presented in color and two distracter

8 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: stimuli were presented in black, one immediately above and one immediately below the target, thus flanking the target. The flanker stimuli were always identical to each other. The center target stimulus was used for presenting integrated distracters, and the black flanking stimuli were used to present the spatially separated distracters. Design The eight included trial types are illustrated in Table 3. In the integrated-only condition, the target stimulus was the name of a color word, and the flankers were both xxxxx. In the separated-only condition, the target stimulus was xxxxx, and the flanker stimuli were both the same color word. In the integrated + separated same condition, the target and flanker stimuli were the same color word. In the integrated + separated different condition, the target word and flanker words were different color words. For each trial type, every possible color combination was used equally often, and there were 48 trials of each type. Procedure Participants were given instructions and were tested as in Experiment 1. On each trial, the target and flanker stimuli appeared simultaneously in the center of the computer monitor and remained until a vocal response was registered. Results RT data were processed in the same manner as in Experiments 1a, 1b, and 2. RTs and interference scores derived from the RTs are given in Table 3. As in the prior experiments, the error analyses appear in the Appendix, with Table 6 displaying the error data for Experiment 3. Congruent conditions None of the congruent condition RTs differed from that of the control condition, ps >.20. Incongruent conditions Interference was reliable in all incongruent conditions, ps <.001. Two 2 (integrated word present or absent) 2 (separated word present or absent) repeatedmeasures ANOVAs were conducted. The first analysis was conducted using the integrated + separated same condition. The presence of an integrated word increased interference, F(1, 51) = , MSE = 5,188, p <.001, partial η 2 =.79; the presence of a separated word marginally increased interference, F(1, 51) = 4.05, MSE =1,032, p =.050, partial η 2 =.07. The effects of the integrated and separated words interacted, F(1, 51) = 22.65, MSE = 1,508, p <.001, partial η 2 =.31, such that their combined effect was less than the sum of their individual effects. The second analysis was conducted using the integrated + separated different condition. The presence of a word increased interference both for integrated stimuli, F(1, 51) = , MSE =5,808, p <.001, partial η 2 =.82, and for separated stimuli, F(1, 51) = 32.43, MSE = 1,345, p <.001, partial η 2 =.39. In this analysis, the effects of integrated and separated words did not interact, F(1, 51) = 1.28, MSE =1,282,p =.262, partial η 2 =.03. Discussion As in previous research (e.g., MacLeod, 1998; Roberts & Besner, 2005), integrated color word distracters produced greater interference than did spatially separated color word distracters, t(51) = 11.13, p <.001, partial η 2 =.71. The effects of these two distracter types were nonredundant when the distracter words were different from each other: Both contributed to total interference. The observed pattern of interference shows that it is not necessary to have different modalities of distracter presentation to produce non-redundant interference effects in color naming. The data pattern in Experiment 3 is consistent with those of Experiments 1a, 1b, and 2, again Table 3 Experiment 3: Response Times and Interference Scores as a Function of Condition Condition Stimulus Examples RT (ms) Interference (ms) Flanker Stimulus Target Stimulus Target Color Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Control xxxxx xxxxx Blue 813 (13) Congruent Integrated xxxxx blue Blue 815 (14) 2 (7) Separated blue xxxxx Blue 816 (13) 3 (6) Integ + Sep blue blue Blue 807 (14) -6 (7) Incongruent Integrated xxxxx red Blue 977 (21) 164 (13) Separated red xxxxx Blue 848 (14) 35 (6) Integ + Sep Same red red Blue 961 (17) 148 (10) Integ + Sep Diff green red Blue 1001 (18) 188 (12)

9 208 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: fitting the joint influence account better than the capture account. General Discussion Consistent with previous comparisons done across different studies, Experiment 1 showed that the interference caused by an auditory color word was weaker than the standard visual effect when presentation of the visual word is integrated with the color. In fact, though, Experiment 2 demonstrated that the interference from auditory distracters was comparable to the interference observed for visual distracters when the visual word is presented in a spatially separate location. Taken together, these findings imply that it is not the visual modality itself but the fact that the visual stimuli are ordinarily integrated that accounts for the differential modality effect. Across the present series of experiments, two distracters elicited larger interference effects than one distracter, whether the two distracters were the same word or two different words. This was true for an auditory distracter and an integrated visual distracter (Experiment 1a) and for an auditory distracter and a spatially separated visual distracter (Experiment 2). When auditory and visual distracters were different words, they made independent contributions to color naming interference, whether the visual distracter was spatially integrated with (Experiment 1a) or spatially separated from (Experiment 2) the target color stimulus. Similarly, when both distracters were visual, spatially integrated and spatially separated distracters had independent effects on color naming (Experiment 3). Our argument has been that these results conflict with predictions of a capture account but are consistent with a joint influence account. Under a capture account (see, e.g., Kahneman & Chajczyk, 1983; MacLeod& Hodder, 1998), while trying to attend to the color, a simultaneously available nominally irrelevant word can intrude on attention, causing interference. But ordinarily only one word would do this on a given trial, the basis for using the word capture: The first word that is attended gates out other words that are also present. In contrast, under a joint influence account (see, e.g., MacLeod & Bors, 2002), multiple nominally irrelevant words can attract and share attention, with each word potentially influencing color naming and contributing to overall total interference. How do our results align with the predictions of these two accounts? First, we have created three situations where multiple nominally irrelevant words are present: one visual and integrated plus one auditory (Experiment 1), one visual and separated plus one auditory (Experiment 2), and one visual and integrated plus one visual and separated (Experiment 3). The patterns of results across these three situations correspond well, as we have just summarized. Interference from an auditory word (necessarily separated) or from a separated visual word is relatively small and consistent, as seen in Experiments 1 and 2. Interference grows substantially when the irrelevant word is integrated with the color information the classic Stroop procedure. Having the same irrelevant word appear in two places (either auditory + visual or visual integrated + visual separated) does little to alter the interference. But having two different irrelevant words occur on a single trial does further increment interference. This last result implies that all words that are present on a trial are processed and that, when these include more than one incongruent word, interference is heightened. Only the joint influence account correctly anticipates this outcome. Is it nevertheless possible for a version of the capture account to handle this pattern? Certainly, attention can be engaged and then disengaged (the Bcatch^ and Brelease^ idea), as the work of Posner and his colleagues (e.g., Posner, Walker, Friedrich, & Rafal, 1984) and others has clearly demonstrated. So following an initial capture by one dimension of a stimulus event, attention could disengage and then subsequently be captured by another dimension of that same stimulus event. In this way, each dimension of a stimulus could influence performance, thereby mimicking joint influence. It may really be a question, then, of whether the two elements are exerting their influence in parallel or sequentially. Although we see our results as more in keeping with a joint influence explanation, particularly given other results in the literature (e.g., La Heij, van der Heijden, & Plooij, 2001; MacLeod& Bors, 2002), we acknowledge that the successive capture account is difficult to discriminate from the joint influence account, and that no single study will be able to do so. This study has provided an approach to understanding interference that converges on the one used by MacLeod and Bors (2002), who reached the same conclusion favoring joint influence. We suspect that prior studies that failed to observe more interference for two distracters than for one, or more interference for two different distracters than for the same distracter presented twice (e.g., Kahneman & Chajczyk, 1983; MacLeod & Hodder, 1998; Yee & Hunt, 1991), did not have sufficient power to observe these effects, due to substantially smaller sample sizes (and perhaps also number of trials per condition) than we used in this series of experiments: Our relatively small standard errors are testament to this. We first established the joint influence using auditory and visual distracters, but it is also clear that joint influence occurs even within a single modality, given the pattern for visual-only stimuli in Experiment 3. Taking this analysis a step farther, the data from Experiments 1 to 3 display an interesting regularity. Whenthereisnointegrationofcolorandword,asin Experiment 2, interference is relatively small about 40 ms as seen in the incongruent visual (separated) condition. But whenever a Stroop stimulus is integrated the

10 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: color is carried by the words itself (e.g., RED in green) this results in a considerably larger amount of interference in our experiments, at least twice as large. The incongruent visual condition of Experiment 1 is the best illustration, with interference incremented by 80 ms from the 40 ms in Experiment 2 to the 120 ms in Experiment 1 as a consequence of integration. Adding a second, necessarily nonintegrated distracter, raises interference by about 40 ms. This can best be seen in Experiment 1 in moving from the single (integrated) visual distracter to the auditory + visual dual distracters, and in Experiment 2 in moving from the single (separated) visual or auditory distracter to one of each. The Stroop task continues to serve as a fertile testing ground for developing our understanding of interference. As Melara and Algom (2003) pointed out, Stroop chose just the right combination of stimulus and response conditions to maximize interference. Indeed, our experiments reconfirm that his choice of integrating color and word is crucial to observing large interference. It is the case, however, that nonintegrated stimuli can exert interference, and that they can do so even in the presence of integrated stimuli by augmenting the amount of interference. Moreover, this is true even for stimuli in a different modality the focus of the present study. We appear routinely to process multiple stimuli in Stroop settings, as indicated by their apparent joint influence on interference. Author Note We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Jacqueline Carbajal, Ana Flores-Chan, and J me Jennifer García Phelps in data collection and Arlene Luévano in data processing. Experiment 2 was conducted as a senior honors thesis by the third author. The findings of Experiment 1a were presented at the 50th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society. Address correspondence to Wendy S. Francis, Department of Psychology, University of Texas at El Paso, El Paso, TX 79968, wfrancis@utep.edu Appendix: Error Rate Analyses Experiment 1a Error rates and error interference scores for Experiment 1a are given in Table 4. Congruent conditions None of the congruent conditions showed significant facilitation or interference, ps >.10. Incongruent conditions Interference was significant in the visual word condition and in both of the visual + auditory conditions, ps <.001. However, interference was not significant in the auditory word condition, t <1. To test whether the effects of interference from the two modalities interacted or were independent, two 2 (visual) 2 (auditory) repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted on the control, visual-only, auditory-only, and visual + auditory condition error rates. The first analysis used the visual + auditory same condition. Presence of the visual word increased interference, F(1, 49) = 28.01, MSE =.00206, p <.001, partial η 2 =.36, whereas presence of the auditory word did not, F <1,partialη 2 =.02. Effects of the auditory and visual words did not interact, F <1, partial η 2 =.01. The second analysis used the visual + auditory different condition. Presence of the visual word increased interference reliably, F(1, 49) = 39.43, MSE =.00231, p <.001, partial η 2 =.45, whereas presence of the auditory word did not, F(1, 49) = 3.22, MSE =.00057, p =.079, partial η 2 =.06. Effects of the auditory and visual words did not interact, F(1, 49) = 2.09, MSE =.00076, p =.155, partial η 2 =.04. Table 4 Experiments 1a and 1b: Error Rates and Interference Scores Experiment 1a Experiment 1b Error Rate (%) Interference (%) Error Rate (%) Interference (%) Condition Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Control 0.8 (.23) 0.5 (.18) Congruent Visual 0.5 (.18) 0.3 (.26) 0.0 (.04) 0.5 (.19) Auditory 0.8 (.19) 0.0 (.21) 0.4 (.17) 0.1 (.26) Visual + Auditory Incongruent 0.4 (.12) 0.4 (.24) 0.2 (.10) 0.3 (.19) Visual 4.5 (.74) 3.7 (.68) 2.0 (.37) 1.5 (.36) Auditory 0.8 (.22) 0.0 (.28) 0.3 (.16) 0.2 (.26) Vis + Aud Same 3.9 (.82) 3.1 (.72) 1.2 (.26) 0.7 (.31) Vis + Aud Diff 5.7 (.90) 4.9 (.81) 2.2 (.47) 1.7 (.52)

11 210 Atten Percept Psychophys (2017) 79: Experiment 1b Error rates and error interference scores for Experiment 1b are given in Table 4. Congruent conditions None of the congruent conditions showed significant facilitation or interference. Incongruent conditions Interference was significant in the visual word condition, t(49) = 4.21, p <.001 and in the visual + auditory different condition, t(49) = 3.30, p =.002. However, interference was not significant in the auditory word condition, t < 1, or the visual + auditory same condition, t(49) = 2.27, p =.028. To test whether the effects of interference from the two modalities interacted or were independent, two 2 (visual) 2 (auditory) repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted on the control, visual-only, auditory-only, and visual + auditory condition error rates. The first analysis used the visual + auditory same condition. Presence of the visual word increased interference, F(1, 49) = 22.76, MSE =.00031, p <.001, partial η 2 =.32, as did presence of the auditory word, F(1, 49) = 4.70, MSE =.00025, p =.035, partial η 2 =.09. Effects of the auditory and visual words did not interact, F(1, 49) = 1.83, MSE =.00027, p =.183, partial η 2 =.04. The second analysis used the visual + auditory different condition. Presence of the visual word increased interference reliably, F(1, 49) = 22.17, MSE =.00065, p <.001, partial η 2 =.31, whereas presence of the auditory word did not, F <1,partialη 2 =.00. Effects of the auditory and visual words did not interact, F(1, 49) = 1.08, MSE =.00016, p =.304, partial η 2 =.02. Experiment 2 Error rates and error interference scores for Experiment 2 are given in Table 5. Facilitation did not reach significance in any of the congruent conditions, ps >.04, nor did interference reach significance in any of the incongruent conditions, ps >.025. Table 5 Experiment 2: Error Rates and Interference Scores Condition Error Rate (%) Interference (%) Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Control 0.9 (.21) Congruent Visual 0.8 (.21) -0.1 (.28) Auditory 0.5 (.17) -0.4 (.19) Visual + Auditory 0.4 (.18) -0.5 (.23) Incongruent Visual 1.1 (.22) 0.2 (.26) Auditory 1.2 (.27) 0.3 (.29) Vis + Aud Same 1.6 (.28) 0.7 (.32) Vis + Aud Diff 1.6 (.31) 0.7 (.37) ps <.001. However interference was not significant in the separated word condition, t < 1. To test whether the effects of interference from the integrated and separated words interacted or were independent, two 2 (integrated) 2 (separated) repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted on the control, integrated-only, separated-only, and integrated + separated condition error rates. The first analysis used the integrated + separated same condition. The presence of the integrated word increased interference, F(1, 51) = 42.49, MSE =.00238, p <.001, partial η 2 =.45. However, the presence of the separated word did not, F <1,partialη 2 =.01,nor did the effects of the integrated and separated words interact, F <1,partialη 2 < 01. The second analysis used the integrated + separated different condition. The presence of the integrated word increased interference, F(1, 51) = 42.69, MSE =.00241, p <.001, partial η 2 =.46, but the presence of the separated word did not, F <1,partialη 2 =.01. The effects of the integrated and separated words did not interact, F <1, partial η 2 <.01. Table 6 Experiment 3: Error Rates and Interference Scores Condition Error Rate (%) Interference (%) Mean (SE) Mean (SE) Experiment 3 Error rates and error interference scores for Experiment 3 are given in Table 6. Congruent conditions There was no significant facilitation in any of the congruent conditions, ps >.20. Incongruent conditions Interference was significant in the integrated word condition, the integrated + separated same condition, and the integrated + separated different condition, Control 1.1 (.26) Congruent Integrated 1.0 (.27) -0.1 (.25) Separated 0.9 (.24) -0.2 (.21) Integ + Sep 1.2 (.38) 0.0 (.29) Incongruent Integrated 5.7 (.84) 4.6 (.78) Separated 1.0 (.28) -0.1 (.29) Integ + Sep Same 5.2 (.83) 4.1 (.76) Integ + Sep Diff 5.3 (.78) 4.2 (.76)

Stroop Dilution Depends on the Nature of the Color Carrier but Not on Its Location

Stroop Dilution Depends on the Nature of the Color Carrier but Not on Its Location Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 4, 826 839 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.826

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

The relative attractiveness of distractors and targets affects the coming and going of item-specific control: Evidence from flanker tasks

The relative attractiveness of distractors and targets affects the coming and going of item-specific control: Evidence from flanker tasks Atten Percept Psychophys (2015) 77:373 389 DOI 10.3758/s13414-014-0752-x The relative attractiveness of distractors and targets affects the coming and going of item-specific control: Evidence from flanker

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals Nart Bedin Atalay (natalay@selcuk.edu.tr) Selcuk University, Konya, TURKEY Mine Misirlisoy

More information

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task Neath, I., & Brown, G. D. A. (2005). Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task. Memory Lab Technical Report 2005-01, Purdue University. Scale Invariance and Primacy

More information

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2009, 16 (1), 57-61 doi:10.3758/pbr.16.1.57 The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words DANIEL SMILEK, GRAYDEN J. F. SOLMAN, PETER MURAWSKI, AND

More information

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. Semantic Component of a Cross-Modal Stroop-like Task Author(s): David M. Stuart and Marisa Carrasco Source: The American Journal of Psychology, Vol. 106, No. 3 (Autumn, 1993), pp. 383-405 Published by:

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

When the visual format of the color carrier word does and does not modulate the Stroop effect

When the visual format of the color carrier word does and does not modulate the Stroop effect Memory & Cognition 2005, 33 (8), 1337-1344 When the visual format of the color carrier word does and does not modulate the Stroop effect CHRIS BLAIS and DEREK BESNER University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario,

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Attention and Facilitation: Converging Information Versus Inadvertent Reading in Stroop Task Performance

Attention and Facilitation: Converging Information Versus Inadvertent Reading in Stroop Task Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2010, Vol. 36, No. 2, 411 422 2010 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0018523 Attention and Facilitation:

More information

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2001, 8 (2), 315-323 Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return ANA B. VIVAS City Liberal Studies: Affiliated Institution of the University of Sheffield, Thessaloniki,

More information

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002 Autumn 2007 674107 Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund Stroop Effect Dual processing causing selective attention. 674107 November 26, 2007 Abstract This document

More information

Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return

Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (6), 935-944 doi:10.3758/mc.37.6.935 Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return JONG MOON CHOI

More information

Hypnotic control of attention in the Stroop task: A historical footnote

Hypnotic control of attention in the Stroop task: A historical footnote Consciousness and Cognition 12 (2003) 347 353 Consciousness and Cognition www.elsevier.com/locate/concog Commentary Hypnotic control of attention in the Stroop task: A historical footnote Colin M. MacLeod

More information

Role of Gestalt grouping in selective attention: Evidence from the Stroop task

Role of Gestalt grouping in selective attention: Evidence from the Stroop task Perception & Psychophysics 2007, 69 (8), 1305-1314 Role of Gestalt grouping in selective attention: Evidence from the Stroop task Martijn J. M. Lamers and Ardi Roelofs Radboud University, Nijmegen, The

More information

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1994, 47A (1) 201-205 Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing Harold Pashler University of

More information

Stroop Process Dissociations: The Relationship Between Facilitation and Interference

Stroop Process Dissociations: The Relationship Between Facilitation and Interference Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1994, Vol. 20, No. 2, 219-234 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/94/$3.00 Stroop Process Dissociations:

More information

Automaticity of Number Perception

Automaticity of Number Perception Automaticity of Number Perception Jessica M. Choplin (jessica.choplin@vanderbilt.edu) Gordon D. Logan (gordon.logan@vanderbilt.edu) Vanderbilt University Psychology Department 111 21 st Avenue South Nashville,

More information

Selective attention and response set in the Stroop task

Selective attention and response set in the Stroop task Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (7), 893-904 doi:10.3758/mc.38.7.893 Selective attention and response set in the Stroop task MARTIJN J. M. LAMERS, ARDI ROELOFS, AND INGE M. RABELING-KEUS Radboud University

More information

The effects of age and task context on Stroop task performance

The effects of age and task context on Stroop task performance Memory & Cognition 2005, 33 (3), 514-530 The effects of age and task context on Stroop task performance SHARON A. MUTTER, JENNIFER C. NAYLOR, and EMILY R. PATTERSON Western Kentucky University, Bowling

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Why It Is too Early to Lose Control in Accounts of Item-Specific Proportion Congruency Effects Julie M. Bugg, Larry L. Jacoby, and Swati

More information

Perceptron Example: Computational model for the Stroop Task. Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio and Brain Engineering, KAIST

Perceptron Example: Computational model for the Stroop Task. Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio and Brain Engineering, KAIST Perceptron Example: Computational model for the Stroop Task Jaeseung Jeong, Ph.D Department of Bio and Brain Engineering, KAIST GREEN YELLOW BLUE BLUE YELLOW GREEN BLUE RED The Stroop Task Can Teach Us

More information

The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure?

The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure? The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure? Contributed by Bruno Rossion, May 2013 Comparing the matching of same top face halves when they are aligned vs. misaligned with different bottom

More information

Yuka Kotozaki Cognitive Psychology, Graduate School of Information Sciences Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan

Yuka Kotozaki Cognitive Psychology, Graduate School of Information Sciences Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan Working Memory and Persistent Inhibitory Set: An Individual Differences Investigation Syoichi Iwasaki (siwasaki@cog.is.tohoku.ac.jp) Cognitive Psychology, Graduate School of Information Sciences Tohoku

More information

Larger IOR effects following forget than following remember instructions depend on exogenous attentional withdrawal and target localization

Larger IOR effects following forget than following remember instructions depend on exogenous attentional withdrawal and target localization Atten Percept Psychophys (2011) 73:1790 1814 DOI 10.3758/s13414-011-0146-2 Larger IOR effects following forget than following remember instructions depend on exogenous attentional withdrawal and target

More information

Auditory Dominance: Overshadowing or Response Competition?

Auditory Dominance: Overshadowing or Response Competition? Auditory Dominance: Overshadowing or Response Competition? Christopher W. Robinson (robinson.777@osu.edu) Center for Cognitive Science The Ohio State University 208F Ohio Stadium East, 1961 Tuttle Park

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load

Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load Psychonomic Bulletin & Review In press Attentional Capture Under High Perceptual Load JOSHUA D. COSMAN AND SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Attentional capture by abrupt onsets can be

More information

Contrast and the justification of effort

Contrast and the justification of effort Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2005, 12 (2), 335-339 Contrast and the justification of effort EMILY D. KLEIN, RAMESH S. BHATT, and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky When humans

More information

Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing

Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing Nooraini Yusoff 1, André Grüning 1 and Antony Browne 1 1 Department of Computing, Faculty of Engineering and Physical

More information

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E. In D. Algom, D. Zakay, E. Chajut, S. Shaki, Y. Mama, & V. Shakuf (Eds.). (2011). Fechner Day 2011: Proceedings of the 27 th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 89-94). Raanana,

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

PSY 3393 Experimental Projects Spring 2008

PSY 3393 Experimental Projects Spring 2008 PSY 3393 Experimental Projects Spring 2008 Dr. Peter Assmann Assignment: journal article report Find an article on a topic of special interest to you from any peer-reviewed journal in Psychology, Neuroscience

More information

Aging, Emotion, Attention, and Binding in the Taboo Stroop Task: Data and Theories

Aging, Emotion, Attention, and Binding in the Taboo Stroop Task: Data and Theories Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 12803-12833; doi:10.3390/ijerph121012803 OPEN ACCESS Article International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health ISSN 1660-4601 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph

More information

Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations

Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations R461B GTG DG Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2003,?? (?),?? Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations LARRY L. JACOBY Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri D. STEPHEN

More information

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set In press, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set Timothy J. Wright 1, Walter

More information

Switching Between Tasks of Unequal Familiarity: The Role of Stimulus-Attribute and Response-Set Selection

Switching Between Tasks of Unequal Familiarity: The Role of Stimulus-Attribute and Response-Set Selection Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2003, Vol. 29, No. 2, 455 469 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.455

More information

The Stroop Effect The Effect of Interfering Colour Stimuli Upon Reading Names of Colours Serially ABSTRACT

The Stroop Effect The Effect of Interfering Colour Stimuli Upon Reading Names of Colours Serially ABSTRACT The Stroop Effect The Effect of Interfering Colour Stimuli Upon Reading Names of Colours Serially ABSTRACT This experiment, a partial duplication of the work of Stroop (l935) l, aimed to demonstrate the

More information

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen The following full text is a preprint version which may differ from the publisher's version. For additional information about this

More information

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 396 409 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.396

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Percent-produced effects in recognition 1. The production effect in recognition memory: Weakening strength can strengthen distinctiveness

Percent-produced effects in recognition 1. The production effect in recognition memory: Weakening strength can strengthen distinctiveness Percent-produced effects in recognition 1 Running head: Percent-produced effects in recognition The production effect in recognition memory: Weakening strength can strengthen distinctiveness Glen E. Bodner

More information

Cross-Modal Stimulus Conflict: The Behavioral Effects of Stimulus Input Timing in a Visual-Auditory Stroop Task

Cross-Modal Stimulus Conflict: The Behavioral Effects of Stimulus Input Timing in a Visual-Auditory Stroop Task : The Behavioral Effects of Stimulus Input Timing in a Visual-Auditory Stroop Task Sarah E. Donohue 1,2,3,4 *, Lawrence G. Appelbaum 1,6, Christina J. Park 1,5, Kenneth C. Roberts 1, Marty G. Woldorff

More information

Adjustment of control in the numerical Stroop task

Adjustment of control in the numerical Stroop task Mem Cogn (2017) 45:891 902 DOI 10.3758/s13421-017-0703-6 Adjustment of control in the numerical Stroop task Gal Dadon 1 & Avishai Henik 1 Published online: 23 March 2017 # The Author(s) 2017. This article

More information

Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance

Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance Target-to-distractor similarity can help visual search performance Vencislav Popov (vencislav.popov@gmail.com) Lynne Reder (reder@cmu.edu) Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh,

More information

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level Jennifer A. Kaminski (kaminski.16@osu.edu) Center for Cognitive Science, Ohio State University 10A

More information

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the 1 Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 835-54. doi:10.1037/a0012726

More information

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY Thomas Lachmann *, Gunjan Khera * and Cees van Leeuwen # * Psychology II, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany # Laboratory

More information

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS BRUC W. HAMILL VISUAL PRCPTION OF STRUCTURD SYMBOLS A set of psychological experiments was conducted to explore the effects of stimulus structure on visual search processes. Results of the experiments,

More information

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH Marie Rivenez 1,2, Chris Darwin 1, Anne Guillaume 2 1 Department of Psychology University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK 2 Département Sciences Cognitives Institut

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task? Bachelor s Thesis Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a sequential movement task? Nils Hatger Prof. Dr. Ing. Willem Verwey Jonathan Barnhoorn, PhD student BMS Faculty/Cognitive

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

Image generation in a letter-classification task

Image generation in a letter-classification task Perception & Psychophysics 1976, Vol. 20 (3),215-219 Image generation in a letter-classification task THOMAS R. HERZOG Grand Valley State Colleges, Allandale, Michigan 49401 Subjects classified briefly

More information

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES Carryl L. Baldwin and Joseph T. Coyne Department of Psychology Old Dominion University

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

APPENDIX A TASK DEVELOPMENT AND NORMATIVE DATA

APPENDIX A TASK DEVELOPMENT AND NORMATIVE DATA APPENDIX A TASK DEVELOPMENT AND NORMATIVE DATA The normative sample included 641 HIV-1 seronegative gay men drawn from the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study (MACS). Subjects received a test battery consisting

More information

ENGAGE: Level of awareness activity

ENGAGE: Level of awareness activity ENGAGE: Level of awareness activity ENGAGE: Meditation instructions Step 1: Explain how people meditate for religious and/or relaxation reasons. Step 2: Describe three major characteristics of an altered

More information

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan University of Alberta The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm by Shawn Tan A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial

More information

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Masters of Science in Psychology By Quan Ying Gao University of Canterbury

More information

SPATIAL STROOP INTERFERENCE AS A FUNCTION OF THE PROTOTYPICALITY OF SPATIAL POSITIONS. Brandi A. Klein. A Thesis

SPATIAL STROOP INTERFERENCE AS A FUNCTION OF THE PROTOTYPICALITY OF SPATIAL POSITIONS. Brandi A. Klein. A Thesis SPATIAL STROOP INTERFERENCE AS A FUNCTION OF THE PROTOTYPICALITY OF SPATIAL POSITIONS Brandi A. Klein A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling Green State University in partial fulfillment

More information

Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect

Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect Q. Gao, Z. Chen, P. Russell Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect Quanying Gao, Zhe Chen, & Paul Russell University of Canterbury Although the effect of working memory (WM) load on the

More information

Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture

Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture Atten Percept Psychophys (2012) 74:1183 1198 DOI 10.3758/s13414-012-0325-9 Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: Evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture

More information

Parallels between Perception without Attention and Perception without Awareness

Parallels between Perception without Attention and Perception without Awareness CONSCIOUSNESS AND COGNITION 6, 219 236 (1997) ARTICLE NO. CC970310 Parallels between Perception without Attention and Perception without Awareness Philip M. Merikle 1 and Steve Joordens 2 University of

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

Cross-modal interaction between vision and touch: the role of synesthetic correspondence

Cross-modal interaction between vision and touch: the role of synesthetic correspondence Perception, 2000, volume 29, pages 745 ^ 754 DOI:10.1068/p2984 Cross-modal interaction between vision and touch: the role of synesthetic correspondence Gail Martinoô, Lawrence E Marks# The John B Pierce

More information

ON THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN RESPONSE INTERFERENCE ' JOHN F. STINS, SJOERD VOSSE, DORRET I. BOOMSMA, ECO J. C. DE GEUS

ON THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN RESPONSE INTERFERENCE ' JOHN F. STINS, SJOERD VOSSE, DORRET I. BOOMSMA, ECO J. C. DE GEUS Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2004,99,947-958. O Perceptual and Motor Skills 2004 ON THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN RESPONSE INTERFERENCE ' JOHN F. STINS, SJOERD VOSSE, DORRET I. BOOMSMA, ECO J. C. DE GEUS

More information

Interactions Between Auditory Elevation, Auditory Pitch and Visual Elevation during Multisensory Perception

Interactions Between Auditory Elevation, Auditory Pitch and Visual Elevation during Multisensory Perception Multisensory Research Interactions Between Auditory Elevation, Auditory Pitch and Visual Elevation during Multisensory Perception Yaseen Jamal 1, Simon Lacey 1, Lynne Nygaard 2 and K. Sathian 1,2,3,4 1

More information

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1040-1050 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1040 Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching ERINA SAEKI AND SATORU

More information

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (3), 504-513 Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks Christian Frings Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany and Peter Wühr Friedrich

More information

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 1 The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning Anila D Mello Georgetown University SOCIAL REWARD AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING 2 Abstract Learning and changing

More information

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 916-920 Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion JOHN PREDEBON University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia Two experiments

More information

Spontaneous Versus Directed Recognition: The Relativity of Automaticity

Spontaneous Versus Directed Recognition: The Relativity of Automaticity Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1993, Vol. 19, No. 4, 777-788 CoDvrieht 1993 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 6 " 0278-7393/93/S3.00 Spontaneous Versus

More information

Precueing Spatial S-R Correspondence: Is There Regulation of Expected Response Conflict?

Precueing Spatial S-R Correspondence: Is There Regulation of Expected Response Conflict? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2008, Vol. 34, No. 4, 872 883 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.872

More information

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search

Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search VISUAL COGNITION, 0000, 00 (0), 111 Irrelevant features at fixation modulate saccadic latency and direction in visual search Walter R. Boot Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee,

More information

Is the psychological refractory period effect for ideomotor compatible tasks eliminated by speed-stress instructions?

Is the psychological refractory period effect for ideomotor compatible tasks eliminated by speed-stress instructions? Psychological Research (2007) 71: 553 567 DOI 10.1007/s00426-006-0066-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Yun Kyoung Shin Æ Yang Seok Cho Æ Mei-Ching Lien Robert W. Proctor Is the psychological refractory period effect

More information

Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention

Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:1064 1070 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0172-z BRIEF REPORT Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention Sophie Forster & Nilli Lavie Published online: 12 October

More information

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity Perception & Psychophysics 1980,28 (3), 249-253 Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity STEPHEN HANDEL and JAMES W. RHODES University oftennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee 37916 Subjects

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance VOL. I I, NO. 6 DECEMBER 1985 Separability and Integrality of Global and Local Levels of Hierarchical Patterns Ruth Kimchi University

More information

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE Åke Hellström Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, S-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden Email: hellst@psychology.su.se Abstract Participants compared successive

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-Signal Paradigm

Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-Signal Paradigm Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2011, Vol. 37, No. 2, 392 404 2010 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021800 Balancing Cognitive Demands:

More information

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2002, Vol. 28, No. 3, 445 457 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.445

More information

Influence of Distraction on Task. Performance in Children and Adults

Influence of Distraction on Task. Performance in Children and Adults 1 Influence of Distraction on Task Performance in Children and Adults Liza Timmermans S767534 Master thesis Augustus 2010 Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences Department of Developmental Psychology

More information

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect VISUAL COGNITION, 2007, 15 (2), 133148 Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect Jay Pratt University of Toronto,

More information

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching Ayla Barutchu 1 *, Stefanie I. Becker 2, Olivia Carter 3, Robert Hester 3, Neil L. Levy 1,4 1 Florey Institute

More information

Parallel scanning ofauditory and visual information

Parallel scanning ofauditory and visual information Memory & Cognition 1975, Vol. 3 (4),416-420 Parallel scanning ofauditory and visual information DAVID BURROWS and BARRY A. SOLOMON State University ofnew York, College at Brockport, Brockport, New York

More information

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER Memory & Cognition 2008, 36 (7), 1236-1247 doi: 10.3758/MC/36.7.1236 An action sequence held in memory can interfere with response selection of a target stimulus, but does not interfere with response activation

More information

Working memory for musical and verbal material under conditions of irrelevant sound

Working memory for musical and verbal material under conditions of irrelevant sound Honors Theses Psychology Spring 2014 Working memory for musical and verbal material under conditions of irrelevant sound Kristi M. Von Handorf Penrose Library, Whitman College Permanent URL: http://hdl.handle.net/10349/1267

More information

PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES?

PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES? PROCEEDINGS of the HUMAN FACTORS AND ERGONOMICS SOCIETY 8th ANNUAL MEETING 88 PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES? J. E. Thropp,

More information

Feature Integration Theory Revisited: Dissociating Feature Detection and Attentional Guidance in Visual Search

Feature Integration Theory Revisited: Dissociating Feature Detection and Attentional Guidance in Visual Search Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2009, Vol. 35, No. 1, 119 132 2009 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.119 Feature Integration

More information

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

The Meaning of the Mask Matters PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Meaning of the Mask Matters Evidence of Conceptual Interference in the Attentional Blink Paul E. Dux and Veronika Coltheart Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science,

More information

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model

Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent Inhibition Model Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 4, 354-373 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association Inc 0096-3445/94/S3.00 Working Memory and Retrieval: A Resource-Dependent

More information

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion Perception & Psychophysics 1980.28 (5). 465-470 Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion GREGORY M. CORSO Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia 30332 A pair of pure-tone sine

More information