This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and"

Transcription

1 This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution and sharing with colleagues. Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party websites are prohibited. In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or institutional repository. Authors requiring further information regarding Elsevier s archiving and manuscript policies are encouraged to visit:

2 Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: Grasping for parsimony: Do some motor actions escape dorsal processing? Markus Janczyk a,, Volker H. Franz b, Wilfried Kunde a a Dortmund University of Technology, Department of Psychology, Emil-Figge-Straße 50, Dortmund, Germany b University of Hamburg, Department of Psychology, von Melle Park 5, Hamburg, Germany article info abstract Article history: Received 2 March 2010 Received in revised form 18 June 2010 Accepted 24 June 2010 Available online 3 July 2010 Keywords: Dorsal Ventral PRP Dual-task Action Grasping It is an open question whether the visual transformations guiding human actions are similar to those generating visual perception. The Action Perception model assumes a strict division of labor: the ventral cortical stream generates perception while the dorsal stream guides actions. However, only skilled and natural actions are assumed to be under dorsal control, while awkward and left-handed actions should be under ventral control in the same way as perception. Here, we used a combination of Garner-Interference and the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm to test this notion. We found that all types of grasping (left-handed, awkward, using a tool) behave in a way similar to skilled right-handed grasping: other than perception they show no Garner-Interference, but similar to perception they show a limitation of processing capacities as indicated by the PRP paradigm. This behavior suggests that similar processes guide all these actions Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. In every second the human brain processes a huge amount of visual information. On the one hand we regularly need to identify objects in our surroundings, and on the other hand we also need to visually guide ourselves through space. The Action Perception model attributes these two purposes to two different anatomical pathways (Goodale & Milner, 1992; Milner & Goodale, 2006): the ventral vision for perception system is assumed to generate the visual percept, while the dorsal vision for action system is assumed to guide motor actions. However, not all actions are assumed to be under dorsal control. Only if an action is highly skilled and natural, the dorsal stream can be exploited. If an action is unskilled or performed in an awkward and unskilled way, it is assumed that the more flexible processing of the ventral stream will guide this action. The aim of our study was to shed some light on this notion and our results question that such a switch from dorsal to ventral control takes place. In the next sections, we will first sketch the Action Perception model, then we will discuss behavioral markers for dorsal versus ventral processing, and finally we show how we applied these markers to left-handed, unskilled, and awkward actions that are in the traditional view of the Action Perception model exceptions from dorsal processing. Surprisingly, we found for all these actions a similar pattern of results as for highly skilled, right-handed actions. Corresponding author. Tel.: address: markus.janczyk@tu-dortmund.de (M. Janczyk). These results are inconsistent with the notion that different processing streams control certain classes of actions. 1. The Action Perception model Among the most prominent and extensively investigated topics in visual sciences and cognitive psychology is the distinction into a ventral and a dorsal pathway of visual processing: both pathways process roughly the same visual input and start their projections from the primary visual cortex (V1). Then the dorsal pathway terminates in posterior parietal areas, while the ventral pathway terminates in inferior temporal areas. Following earlier models that ascribed to these pathways different functions in processing spatial vs. identity information (Ungerleider & Mishkin, 1982), the currently most influential model is the Action Perception model by Goodale and Milner (1992) and Milner and Goodale (2006). According to this model the purpose of the ventral pathway is the perceptual identification of stimuli ( vision for perception ), while the dorsal pathway serves to program and control visually guided motor actions ( vision for action ). In other words: both pathways analyze the same visual input but for different purposes. The initial evidence for this model came from the neuropsychological double dissociation of (visual (form) agnosia) and optic ataxia. Visual agnostic patients, suffering from lesions to ventral occipitotemporal areas, are typically not able to recognize or discriminate between objects, but at the same time have no (or only few) problems acting on them. The opposite pattern is found in optic ataxic patients, suffering from lesions in dorsal stream areas of the posterior parietal cortex. These patients have no problems discriminating /$ see front matter 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi: /j.neuropsychologia

3 3406 M. Janczyk et al. / Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) or recognizing objects, but exhibit marked problems when, e.g., grasping them. Other lines of evidence have been proposed subsequent to the initial formulation of the Action Perception model, among them are results from imaging studies and the different effect of visual illusions on perception and action which will be discussed in greater detail below (see Milner & Goodale, 2006, for an exhaustive review of evidence). The Action Perception model soon became influential and extremely fruitful in producing testable hypotheses. How can we decide whether a given task was carried out with the help of the dorsal or the ventral pathway in a behavioral experiment? At present two suggestions exist in the literature: the dissociable effects of visual illusions on dorsal and ventral tasks (e.g., Aglioti, DeSouza, & Goodale, 1995), and the use of Garner- Interference which indicates ventral processing (Ganel & Goodale, 2003). We will discuss these two methods successively. 2. Visual illusions as behavioral markers of ventral vs. dorsal processing The Action Perception model ascribes to the ventral pathway an allocentric and to the dorsal pathway an egocentric coding of spatial coordinates. This allows for an interesting prediction concerning the influence of visual illusions in non-clinical populations and thus to gather support for the Action Perception model from experiments with healthy participants: certain visual (size) illusions should only have an effect on the performance in tasks guided by ventral mechanisms (e.g., judging the size of the inner circle in the Ebbinghaus illusion); while the same illusions should have no effect on tasks where the dorsal pathway carries out the bulk of work (e.g., grasping the inner circle of the Ebbinghaus illusion with a precision grip, i.e., between the thumb and the index finger). A suitable dependent measure in latter tasks is the maximum grip aperture (MGA; i.e., the maximal distance between thumb and index-finger in-flight) which is achieved after about two thirds of a grasping movement, and is linearly related to the size of the target object. Aglioti et al. (1995) were the first to test this prediction and they interpreted their results as supporting the Action Perception model. Subsequently, this line of research attracted many studies and the basic pattern was replicated several times (e.g., Haffenden & Goodale, 1998; Haffenden, Schiff, & Goodale, 2001). However, other researchers came to opposite conclusions. Indeed, in many studies there is a positive effect of visual illusions on grasping (Franz, 2001; Franz & Gegenfurtner, 2008) which is not predicted by the Action Perception model. Importantly, this effect seems not being due to other mechanisms like obstacle-avoidance during the grasp (Franz, Bülthoff, & Fahle, 2003). Moreover, whether or not grasping and perceptual measures are differently affected by illusions seems to depend on two conditions. First, the perceptual measure needs to be calibrated adequately. For example, the perceptual measure manual size estimation responds to any variation of object size be it illusionary or non-illusionary much stronger than grasping does. To take this adequately into account, certain corrections are necessary, and if these corrections are performed, there doesn t seem to be a difference between illusion effects in perception and action (Franz, 2003; Franz & Gegenfurtner, 2008). Secondly, the grasping and perception tasks need to be equal in terms of task demands (e.g., attentional demands). Studies with a good match across both tasks repeatedly found no difference between the illusion effects on grasping and perception (e.g., Franz et al., 2003; Franz, Gegenfurtner, Bülthoff, & Fahle, 2000; Pavani, Boscagli, Benvenuti, Rabuffetti, & Farnè, 1999). But this debate is far from resolved (for recent summaries see Franz & Gegenfurtner, 2008 and Goodale, 2008) such that it seems beneficial to search for other behavioral markers for dorsal and ventral processing. 3. Garner-Interference as a behavioral marker of ventral vs. dorsal processing An alternative behavioral indicator for the dichotomy of dorsal versus ventral processing can be derived from the study by Ganel and Goodale (2003). According to these authors the dorsal pathway is able to process its input in an analytical fashion, while the ventral pathway processes the input holistically. In other words: the dorsal pathway should be able to ignore variations of task-irrelevant stimulus dimensions, while this should not be true for the ventral pathway. As a consequence, this should give rise to interference phenomena when task-irrelevant stimulus dimensions are varied in addition to the task-relevant characteristics. This has been demonstrated by Ganel and Goodale (2003) in an elegant way using a variant of Garner s speeded classification task (Garner, 1978; Garner & Felfoldy, 1970). In their experiments four small wooden blocks resulting from a factorial combination of two widths (30 mm vs mm) and lengths (63 mm vs. 75 mm) were used as stimuli. In the grasping task (presumably a typical dorsal task), participants were to grasp the stimulus blocks across their width with a precision grip, in the perceptual judgment task (presumably a typical ventral task) the participants were to judge the blocks width. Thus, the width was the relevant stimulus dimension for both tasks. Importantly, both tasks were performed under two experimental conditions. (1) In the baseline condition only the two stimulus blocks of the same length were used, i.e., the irrelevant length dimension was constant. (2) In contrast, in the filtering condition all four stimulus blocks were used, and thus the irrelevant dimension also varied. As predicted, this task-irrelevant variation produced interference only in the perceptual judgment task, where response times were higher in the filtering condition than in the baseline condition (i.e., Garner-Interference ). In the grasping task, however, no differences in response times (and in movement trajectories) were found. To the extent that the underlying logic is correct Garner-Interference can serve as an alternative behavioral indicator: if a task relies on ventral processing, Garner-Interference should emerge. 4. The psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm as a marker of resource-limited processing According to the Action Perception model, dorsal processing should be automatic and independent of ventral processing. Therefore, performance in a typical dorsal task (like grasping) should not suffer if performed in parallel to a typical ventral task (like pitch discrimination). This notion was the main focus of two recent studies by Kunde, Landgraf, Paelecke, and Kiesel (2007) and Janczyk and Kunde (2010) who used a classical dual-task situation, the PRP paradigm. 1 The results of these studies show that both grasping and the perceptual judgment task of Ganel and Goodale (2003) operate in a capacity-limited, non-automatic way. Interestingly, Kunde, Landgraf, et al. (2007) and Janczyk and Kunde (2010) combined their PRP paradigm with the Garner-Interference paradigm of Ganel and Goodale (2003). Replicating Ganel and Goodale (2003), they found Garner-Interference only in the perceptual judgment of the stimulus blocks width, but not when grasping these same blocks. Thus, Garner-Interference appears suitable as 1 The present Experiments 1, 2 and 4 were for reasons laid out in the introduction to Experiment 1 settled within the PRP paradigm, too, and assume the existence of a central response selection bottleneck (Pashler, 1994; Pashler & Johnston, 1998; Welford, 1952). However, the focus of this study is not the dual-task behavior of motor actions and we thus omitted a detailed theoretical account and description in the main part of the text. See also Appendix A and further in-depth treatments of these topics (e.g., Pashler, 1994; Pashler & Johnston, 1998).

4 M. Janczyk et al. / Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) a behavioral indicator for ventral processing-even under dual-task situations. 5. Are some actions under ventral control? From its very outset, the Action Perception model assumed that certain actions are under ventral control. This idea bears on the fact that, for example, the famous patient D.F. performs normal grasping along the width of an object (interpreted as intact visual input to the dorsal stream), but is not able to indicate the width with her fingers (Goodale & Milner, 1992). Indicating the width with the fingers is therefore interpreted as being ventrally controlled. Because the visual input to the ventral stream is assumed to be damaged, the Action Perception model therefore can explain D.F. s deficit. In general, all actions that are not highly skilled and are not performed on easily available visual input are assumed to be controlled by the ventral stream. This includes mimicked actions (Goodale, Jakobson, & Keillor, 1994), actions that are performed after the visual input has been removed from sight (Goodale, Westwood, & Milner, 2004), awkward and unskilled actions (Gonzalez, Ganel, Whitwell, Morrissey, & Goodale, 2008), and left-handed 2 actions (Gonzalez, Ganel, & Goodale, 2006). If this is true, then it is an important question to determine exactly which visually guided actions are under dorsal control, and which are not not only for psychological theorizing, but for applied purposes as well. Consider, for example, that in modern working environments many motor tasks produce effects in the environment that are transformations of the natural movements of the hand or the fingers. This is the case with physical tools, such as levers or grippers, as well as with virtual tools such as a computermouse or other pointing devices (e.g., Kunde, Müsseler, & Heuer, 2007). Without knowing whether such artificial actions are constrained by the same factors as natural actions, it is impossible to evaluate their ergonomic value or to identify conditions that render them error-prone. Therefore, we used a sample of relevant actions that might be under ventral control and tested them using the combined PRP and Garner-Interference design as developed by Kunde, Landgraf, et al. (2007). In Experiment 1 we tested left-handed grasping in right-handers, in Experiments 2 and 3 we tested awkward grasping, and in Experiment 4 unskilled grasping using a tool. This subset of actions is of particular interest since they all aim at directly interacting with a target stimulus and can be visually guided with available input. As such they are good candidates for dorsal processing, but still have been suggested to be guided by the ventral stream (Gonzalez, Ganel, et al., 2006; Gonzalez et al., 2008). If any of these actions is indeed under ventral control, we would expect Garner-Interference. 6. Experiment 1 In this experiment, we tested whether left-handed (precision) grasping is under ventral control, as suggested by Gonzalez, Ganel, et al. (2006). Evidence for this proposal came from an effect of visual illusions on grasping with the left but not with the right 2 The reader might be tempted to add: in right-handers, but this would not correctly reflect the theory. Gonzalez, Ganel, et al. (2006) and Gonzalez, Goodale, et al. (2006) did indeed claim that even in left-handers only right handed actions are controlled by the dorsal stream, thereby implying that left handers cannot exploit the evolutionary advantages of the dorsal stream as long as they are using their dominant, left hand. Of course, this is a very strong claim that has been criticized heavily (Derakhshan, 2006) and Gonzalez, Goodale, et al. (2006) admitted in a reply that this was only a tentative idea. Because we don t want to build up a straw-man, we will, for the purposes of this paper, only consider the more cautious assumption that left-handed actions in right-handers are controlled by the ventral stream. hand. We have argued that the (non-) existence of effects of visual illusions on grasping is still quite controversial (Franz & Gegenfurtner, 2008; Goodale, 2008). This in turn prompts the search for converging evidence using a different indicator, such as Garner-Interference introduced above. If left-handed grasping indeed relies more on perceptual information provided by the ventral pathway (as opposed to the dorsal system, which applies for right-handed precision grasping) this should result in Garner- Interference. Experiment 1 (and also the following Experiments 2 and 4) is settled within the PRP paradigm for mainly two reasons. First, we have shown elsewhere that a (dorsal) right-handed precision grasp is subject to the PRP effect, i.e., relies on central resources (Janczyk & Kunde, 2010; Kunde, Landgraf, et al., 2007). For better comparability with these studies and to generalize this finding to other classes of grasping movements, we applied the PRP paradigm here, too. Second, in previous work the Garner-Interference observed in the perceptual judgment tasks combined additively with the dualtask factor stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA; Janczyk & Kunde, 2010, Experiment 1; Kunde, Landgraf, et al., 2007). According to the PRP logic and the central bottleneck model (see Appendix A; Pashler, 1994; Pashler & Johnston, 1998) this suggests an implication of central attention to resolve Garner-Interference. At first glance, this appears somewhat counterintuitive since Garner-Interference can be seen as a perceptual effect (which should result in an underadditive combination with SOA; see Pashler, 1994, or Pashler & Johnston, 1998). The use of the PRP paradigm thus enables us to assess whether this finding also applies to the classes of movements under investigation here, insofar as they produce Garner-Interference Method Participants Sixteen undergraduates from Dortmund University of Technology (2 male, mean age = 24;0 years) participated in return of course credit. All participants were right-handed (by self-report) and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision Design, apparatus, and stimuli Each participant was tested in a single-session PRP experiment of about min. Task 1 was a binary tone classification task and was responded to with a right hand key press. Stimuli (S1) were two 50 ms tones presented via headphones (low tone: 300 Hz; high tone: 900 Hz). Task 2 was a grasping task, where participants grasped a stimulus across its width using a left-handed precision grip (i.e., between thumb and index-finger of the left hand). Stimuli in this task (S2) were four small wooden blocks constructed according to a factorial combination of two widths (30 mm and 35.7 mm) and two lengths (63 mm and 75 mm). S2 visibility was controlled by using computer controlled PLATO shutter glasses (Translucent Technologies) and the stimuli were presented on a small custom-made table where they depressed a hidden micro switch. To avoid misunderstandings here: participants did not preview the to-be-grasped stimuli and started the grasping movement upon a go-signal. Rather, the shutter glasses opened and provided view of the stimuli after the SOA, and participants were then to start their grasping movement. As such, the grasping movement was not an instance of delayed movements, which have been described to rely on perceptual information provided by the ventral stream (e.g., Hu & Goodale, 2000). The grasping movements were performed closed-loop. This was done to make the experiments comparable to the studies by Ganel and Goodale (2003), Gonzalez, Ganel, et al. (2006) and Gonzalez et al. (2008). Note that in supplementary material, Ganel and Goodale (2003) reported a control experiment where grasping was performed open-loop, with similar results as

5 3408 M. Janczyk et al. / Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) those from the closed-loop condition. The response time in Task 1 (RT1) was measured from tone presentation until the right hand key press. Response time in Task 2 (RT2) was measured from the shutter glasses opening until the left index-finger left a home button. The time from this event until when the target S2 was lifted was measured as the movement time (MT) Procedure The whole experiment comprised one short practice block (24 trials) followed by four experimental blocks of 72 trials each. Of the experimental blocks, two were baseline conditions in which only the two S2 stimulus blocks of the same length were used, the remaining blocks were filtering conditions in which all four possible S2 stimulus blocks were used. Prior to every block, participants were shown those S2 stimuli used in the upcoming block, and were asked to thoroughly inspect them. Four orders of experimental blocks were applied, resulting from counterbalancing the two baseline and the two filtering conditions with the order of the two possible baseline conditions also counterbalanced. Each trial began with a short warning click, and 1000ms later S1 was played. Following a varying SOA of 50, 500, or 1000 ms the shutter glasses opened and provided view of S2. Task 1 errors were detected automatically, Task 2 grasping accuracy was judged by the experimenter (only a precision-grip was judged correct). The experimenter gave feedback after each trial. After the participant placed the left index-finger back on the home-button, the shutter glasses became opaque, and the experimenter prepared the next trial. Participants received written instructions prior to the experiments proper which emphasized speed and accuracy. Task 1 performance was given priority over Task 2. The order of experimental blocks (4) and the stimulus-responsemapping of Task 1 (2) were counterbalanced across participants. Each baseline condition block comprised 2 (Task 1 stimuli) 2 (Task 2 stimuli) 3 (SOA) 6 (repetitions) trials presented in a random order; filtering condition blocks comprised 2 (Task 1 stimuli) 4 (Task 2 stimuli) 3 (SOA) 3 (repetitions) trials presented in a random order Data treatment and analyses Analyses were mainly done by means of analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the factors condition (baseline vs. filtering) and SOA (50 ms vs. 500 ms vs ms) as repeated measures (if necessary, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied and the corresponding ε is reported). It is a significant effect of condition that would signal the presence of Garner-Interference in all here reported experiments. For all statistical analyses a significance level of =.05 was used throughout this paper and sample effect sizes are reported as partial 2. (We ran all analyses again after applying the logtransformation to RT/MT data. Overall, this gave the same results as the analyses using raw RT/MT data for the only exception see the Results section of Experiment 4.) Trials with general errors (e.g., too slow responses, leaving the home button prior to the glasses opening, etc.) were excluded from all analyses. The reported error analyses are based on Task 1 and Task 2 accuracy of the remaining trials. For RT analyses only those trials were included where both tasks were responded to correct. Furthermore, RTs less than 150 ms (anticipations) or exceeding the individual s mean by more than 2.5 individual standard deviations (calculated separately for each participant and analyzed condition; outliers) were excluded (Task 1: 2.9; Task 2: 4.2%) Results Of primary importance for the present research are the results from the grasping task (Task 2). We thus present Fig. 1. Mean response times (RTs) and movement times (MTs) in milliseconds for the left-handed grasping task (Experiment 1) as a function of SOA (stimulus onset asynchrony) and condition. these first, followed by a brief presentation of the Task 1 results RT and MT analyses Mean RTs and MTs in Task 2 are visualized in Fig. 1. As can been seen, RT2 showed a large increase with a decreasing SOA from about 250 ms at the longest SOA to 800 ms at the shortest SOA (the PRP effect). However, there is not much of an RT difference between baseline and filtering conditions. The corresponding ANOVA confirmed only the effect of SOA as significant; F(2,30) = , p <.01, partial 2 =.98, ε =.71. Neither the effect of condition; F(1,15) = 0.02, p =.89, partial 2 <.01; nor the interaction SOA condition; F(2,30) = 0.28, p =.69, partial 2 =.02, ε =.71; were significant. MTs showed only a small variation between 642 and 666 ms, thus they were roughly at one level independent of SOA and condition. The ANOVA on MTs revealed no effect as significant; SOA: F(2,30) = 3.16, p =.08, partial 2 =.17, ε =.64; condition: F(1,15) = 1.03, p =.33, partial 2 =.06; interaction SOA condition: F(2,30) = 0.05, p =.95, partial 2 <.01. Mean RTs in Task 1 (see Table 1) showed a slight and significant increase with an increasing SOA; F(2,30) = 9.28, p <.01, Table 1 Response times to Task 1 in milliseconds from Experiments 1, 2 and 4 as a function of SOA (stimulus onset asynchrony) and condition. Condition SOA Experiment 1 (left-handed grasping) Baseline Filtering Experiment 2 (awkward grasping) Baseline Filtering Experiment 4 (perceptual judgment) Baseline Filtering Experiment 4 (tool grasping) Baseline Filtering

6 M. Janczyk et al. / Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) partial 2 =.38, ε =.55. No other effect was significant; condition: F(1,15) = 0.22, p =.64, partial 2 =.02; interaction SOA condition: F(2,30) = 1.39, p =.26, partial 2 =.09, ε = Error analyses Mean error percentages in both tasks are presented in Table 2, and detailed statistics are summarized in Table 3. In general, Task 2 errors were rare and showed no systematic variation with either factor or the respective interaction. In contrast, Task 1 errors were overall higher and showed a significant decrease with an increasing SOA Discussion Experiment 1 was run to assess whether left-handed grasping (in right-handers) relies on ventral instead of on dorsal processing, as suggested by Gonzalez, Ganel, et al. (2006). If left-handed grasping is indeed under ventral control, Garner-Interference (i.e., an RT difference between baseline and filtering conditions) should emerge. In addition to Garner-Interference, we tested for a PRP effect with a binary tone classification as Task 1. As Task 2 participants used a left-handed precision grip to grasp a stimulus block across its width. First, the data show the typical pattern observed in PRP experiments. Whereas RT1 was largely unaffected by any manipulation (the small increase with an increasing SOA may even be due to a speed-accuracy trade-off), RT2 decreased about 500 ms with an increasing SOA a finding well known as the PRP effect. Previous reports of non-automatic dorsal processing of grasping (Janczyk & Kunde, 2010; Kunde, Landgraf, et al., 2007) thus generalize to left-handed grasping. Second, and of more importance, RTs did not differ between baseline and filtering conditions. In other words: we found no Garner-Interference for left-handed grasping, indicating that the involved planning mechanisms were able to efficiently ignore the task-irrelevant variation of the stimuli length. Crucially, this ability has been ascribed to the dorsal pathway (Ganel & Goodale, 2003), and thus using Garner-Interference as the indicator leads to a conclusion contrasting the one by Gonzalez, Ganel, et al. (2006): we need to conclude that there is no difference between left- and right-handed precision grasping in right-handers. Fig. 2. Illustration of the awkward grasping task used in Experiments 2 and 3. of course credit. All participants were right-handed (by self-report) and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision Design, apparatus, stimuli, procedure, data treatment, and analyses This experiment was very similar to Experiment 1, and we therefore only introduce the major changes. In this experiment participants were asked to grasp the stimulus blocks across their width using an awkward grip of the right-hand (i.e., between their thumbs and ring-finger; Gonzalez et al., 2008; see Fig. 2). As a consequence, the home button was now depressed with the index-finger of the right hand, and the responses to Task 1 were given with the left hand. 3.3% and 3.3% of the trials were excluded as outliers in Tasks 1 and 2, respectively Results RT and MT analyses Mean RTs and MTs in Task 2 are visualized in Fig. 3. Compared with the results of Experiment 1 (see Fig. 1), there are striking sim- 7. Experiment 2 In Experiment 1 we used Garner-Interference instead of the absence or presence of visual illusory effects to assess whether lefthanded grasping is indeed under ventral instead of dorsal control (Gonzalez, Ganel, et al., 2006; Gonzalez, Goodale, et al., 2006). The results were not supportive; rather they point to a recruitment of processes similar to those that have been observed for right-handed grasping (Ganel & Goodale, 2003; Janczyk & Kunde, 2010; Kunde, Landgraf, et al., 2007). Recently, a second candidate action relying on ventral instead of dorsal processing has been identified, namely unskilled actions (Gonzalez et al., 2008). Experiment 2 resembles Experiment 1, but the participants used an awkward grip (they grasped the target stimulus using their thumb and ring-finger) with their right hand. Again, we tested whether Garner-Interference might indicate a ventral mode of processing for this kind of action Method Participants Sixteen new undergraduates from Dortmund University of Technology (3 male, mean age = 22;4 years) participated in return Fig. 3. Mean response times (RTs) and movement times (MTs) in milliseconds for the awkward grasping task (Experiment 2) as a function of SOA (stimulus onset asynchrony) and condition.

7 3410 M. Janczyk et al. / Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Table 2 Mean error percentages in Tasks 1 and 2 from Experiments 1, 2 and 4 as a function of SOA (stimulus onset asynchrony) and condition. Condition Task 1 Task 2 SOA SOA Experiment 1 (left-handed grasping) Baseline Filtering Experiment 2 (awkward grasping) Baseline Filtering Experiment 4 (perceptual judgment) Baseline Filtering Experiment 4 (tool grasping) Baseline Filtering Table 3 Detailed statistics on mean error percentages in Tasks 1 and 2 for Experiments 1, 2 and 4. Degrees of freedom (dfs) are given as numerator s, denominator s dfs, sample effect sizes are given as partial 2, and significant F-ratios are marked with an asterisk (*). Task 1 Task 2 F dfs part. 2 F dfs part. 2 Experiment 1 SOA 5.66 * Condition SOA Condition Experiment 2 SOA # Condition SOA Condition < Experiment 4 SOA 5.82 * Condition * Task * SOA Condition SOA Task Condition Task * SOA Condition Task # p =.07. ilarities. Again, RT2 dropped several hundred milliseconds when the SOA increased, thus showing the typical PRP effect. Moreover, there was again no difference between baseline and filtering conditions, i.e., no Garner-Interference. The ANOVA confirmed the effect of SOA as significant; F(2,30) = , p <.01, partial 2 =.91, ε =.61; but neither was the effect of condition significant; F(1,15) = 0.02, p =.89, partial 2 <.01; nor was the interaction SOA condition; F(2,30) = 0.06, p =.94, partial 2 <.01. MTs showed only little variation between 605 ms and 617 ms, and no effect reached significance in the respective ANOVA; SOA: F(2,30) = 1.31, p =.27, partial 2 =.08, ε =.57; condition: F(1,15) = 0.36, p =.56, partial 2 =.02; interaction SOA condition: F(2,30) = 0.84, p =.41, partial 2 =.05, ε =.69. Mean RTs in Task 1 (see Table 1) significantly increased with an increasing SOA; F(2,30) = 10.11, p <.01, partial 2 =.40, ε =.70. No other effect was significant; condition: F(1,15) = 3.65, p =.08, partial 2 =.20; interaction SOA condition: F(2,30) = 2.28, p =.12, partial 2 = Error analyses Mean error percentages are presented in Table 2 and detailed statistics are summarized in Table 3. In general, error rates were low in both tasks, but extremely low in Task 2. Mean error percentages in Task 1 showed no reliable variation with either factor. For Task 2, however, there was a small, but nonetheless, significant increase in error rates at the highest SOA of 1000 ms Discussion In Experiment 2 we investigated awkward grasping (using thumb and ring-finger; see Fig. 2). This grip type was previously used as an example of an unskilled action by Gonzalez et al. (2008). On the basis of a visual illusory effect on this kind of grasping, these authors concluded that unskilled actions are likely to be controlled by the ventral pathway instead of the dorsal pathway. In our experiment we instead used Garner-Interference as the indicator of the processing mode (Ganel & Goodale, 2003). The results are almost identical to what we observed in Experiment 1. All aspects concerning the dual-task behavior were replicated, and again no Garner-Interference was observed. This suggests that awkward (or more general: unskilled) grasping is also not controlled by a qualitatively different processing stream than normal grasping is. Again, this interpretation contrasts the one advanced by Gonzalez et al. (2008). 8. Experiment 3 Experiments 1 and 2 were PRP experiments, where the particular grasping task was implemented as Task 2. The reason for this was

8 M. Janczyk et al. / Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) that we expected Garner-Interference, as the used grasping types were suggested relying on ventral information (Gonzalez, Ganel et al., 2006; Gonzalez, Goodale, et al., 2006; Gonzalez et al., 2008). This would have allowed assessing whether the additive interaction of Garner-Interference and SOA, as observed for perceptual judgment tasks, would replicate in action contexts. Yet, no Garner- Interference was observed so far, and this prompts the question if perhaps the dual-task PRP situation may have blurred this effect in grasping tasks. To exclude this possibility, we ran Experiment 3 as a single-task experiment where participants used again the awkward grip Method Participants Sixteen new undergraduates from Dortmund University of Technology (1 male, mean age = 23;9 years) participated in return of course credit. All participants were right-handed (by self-report) Design, apparatus, stimuli, procedure, and data treatment and analyses In this experiment, participants worked only on a grasping task and used the awkward grip of Experiment 2 to grasp the stimulus blocks (see Figure 2). Every trial began with a short warning click, and after 800 ms or 1200 ms (randomly varied) the shutter glasses opened and provided view of the stimulus block. Data analysis was done by means of ANOVA with condition (baseline vs. filtering) as a repeated measure. 3.1% of the trials were excluded as outliers Results and discussion Mean RTs were 316 ms and 311 ms and mean MTs were 634 ms and 639 ms for baseline and filtering conditions, respectively. Neither on RTs; F(1,15) = 0.35, p =.56, partial 2 =.02; nor on MTs; F(1,15) = 0.24, p =.63, partial 2 =.02; had condition a significant effect. Mean error percentages were 1.56 and 1.48 for baseline and filtering blocks. This difference was not significant; F(1, 15) = 0.78, p =.79, partial 2 =.01. In sum, these results suggest that also under single-task conditions no Garner-Interference is observed for awkward grasping, again pointing to a dorsal mode of processing (Ganel & Goodale, 2003). As such, the claims made from Experiments 1 and 2 are further strengthened. 9. Experiment 4 So far we are left with two rather disappointing results. Neither left-handed grasping (Experiment 1) nor awkward grasping (Experiments 2 and 3) showed any signs of Garner-Interference. This outcome would actually be predicted assuming that the involved planning mechanisms and processes were able to ignore the task-irrelevant variation of the stimuli length. Of importance, however, this ability has been ascribed to dorsal processing (Ganel & Goodale, 2003) and we thus must conclude that both left-handed and awkward grasping are controlled by this very mode. Taking this interpretation for serious, our results (where we used the Garner- Interference as the indicator) show exactly the opposite than those reported by Gonzalez, Ganel, et al. (2006) and Gonzalez et al. (2008), using the effect of visual illusions on grasping as the indicator. Before we discuss the implications of these results we wish to point out three issues. First, our interpretations so far are based on three null-findings, what immediately raises the question of whether we had employed enough statistical power. We suspect this an unlikely problem to our study (actually, the mean RT2 differences between baseline and filtering conditions were very small), but concur that a higher-powered experiment would strengthen our interpretation. A second and related issue is that we have thus far not included any control condition to test whether Garner- Interference can be observed in a typical ventral task. If the same participants would produce Garner-Interference in such a task, but at the same time not in a grasping movement under investigation here, this would be compelling evidence for our conclusions. Thirdly, even though having shown the desired effect in an earlier study (Gonzalez et al., 2008) the awkward grip may not have been unskilled enough and has thus escaped ventral control. Hence the use of a different unskilled grasping task appears desirable. In Experiment 4 we addressed all of these three points: to enlarge the power of the experiment we used 32 (instead of 16) participants, all participants took part in a grasping task and in a ventral perceptual judgment (control) task, and they grasped the target stimuli using a realistic tool (pliers). In light of the results from Experiments 1 to 3 we (1) expected to replicate the dual-task characteristics, and (2) more importantly, we expected the occurrence of Garner-Interference in the perceptual task, but not in the tool grasping task Method Participants Thirty-two new undergraduates from Dortmund University of Technology (6 male, mean age = 24;6 years) participated in return of course credit. All participants were right-handed (by self-report) and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision Design, apparatus, and stimuli Each participant was tested in two sessions of a PRP experiment lasting between 60 and 90 min. Task 2 was a tool grasping task in one session, in the other session Task 2 was a perceptual judgment task. Task 1 was always the binary tone classification task as described in Experiment 1, and the responses to this task were given with the left hand. With tool grasping as Task 2, participants used pliers with their right hand. The home-button was depressed with the tip of the pliers (we mounted a small plate of 3cm 3 cm on the home-button to facilitate this), and the participants were asked to precisely grasp the stimulus blocks with these pliers (see Fig. 4). The perceptual judgment task was modeled after Ganel and Goodale (2003). Here, participants were asked to judge the stimulus blocks width and to respond with a right-hand key press accordingly. This task has been shown to reliable produce Garner-Interference in single-task settings (Ganel & Goodale, 2003), but also in dual-task settings (Janczyk & Kunde, 2010, Experiment 1; Kunde, Landgraf, et al., 2007). RT2 in this task was measured from the glasses opening until the right-hand key press was given. Fig. 4. Illustration of the tool grasping task used in Experiment 4.

9 3412 M. Janczyk et al. / Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Fig. 5. Mean response times (RTs) in milliseconds for the perceptual judgment task of Experiment 4 (left panel) and mean RTs and movement times (MTs) in milliseconds for the tool grasping task of Experiment 4 (right panel) as a function of SOA (stimulus onset asynchrony) and condition. In all other aspects the experiment was similar to Experiments 1 and Procedure Procedural details were for the most parts as described in Experiment 1. In both sessions participants were given the appropriate written instructions and performed a practice block before the four experimental blocks were administered. The order of the tool grasping and the perceptual judgment task as Task 2 (2), of experimental blocks (4), and the stimulus-responsemapping of Task 1 (2) were counterbalanced across participants Data treatment and analyses Analyses were done by means of analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the factors type of Task 2 (tool grasping vs. perceptual judgment), condition (baseline vs. filtering), and SOA (50 ms vs. 500 ms vs ms) as repeated measures (Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied where necessary). With tool grasping as Task 2, 3.7% and 4.0% of the trials in Tasks 1 and 2 were excluded as outliers; the corresponding values for perceptual judgment as Task 2 are 3.3% and 3.0% 9.2. Results RT and MT analyses Mean RTs and MTs in Task 2 are visualized in Fig. 5, separately for the perceptual judgment task (left panel) and the tool grasping task (right panel). In general, RT2 in the perceptual judgment task were significantly longer than in the tool grasping task; F(1,31) = , p <.01, partial 2 =.82; and showed a decrease with an increasing SOA the PRP effect; F(2,62) = , p <.01, partial 2 =.91, ε =.61. This decrease was roughly the same for both possible Tasks 2, yielding a non-significant interaction of SOA and Task 2 type 3 ; F(2,62) = 2.03, p =.16, partial 2 =.06, ε =.61; and it was also the same for baseline and filtering conditions, resulting in 3 When this analysis was applied to log-transformed RTs, this interaction effect reached significance, indicating differences in the PRP effect between both tasks. As differences in PRP effects are difficult to interpret, and this interaction is not of theoretical interest for the present purposes we refrain from going into detail here. a non-significant interaction of SOA and condition; F(2,62) = 1.11, p =.34, partial 2 =.03. Of most importance for the present purposes was the significant main effect of condition with longer RTs in filtering conditions than in baseline conditions, indicating the occurrence of Garner- Interference; F(1,31) = 6.82, p =.01, partial 2 =.18. However, this main effect was further modulated by the type of Task 2: the size of Garner-Interference was about 100 ms in the perceptual judgment task, at the same time Garner-Interference was nearly absent in the tool grasping task. Accordingly, the interaction of condition and type of Task 2 was significant; F(1,31) = 7.94, p <.01, partial 2 =.20. The three-way interaction of SOA condition Task 2 type did not reach significance; F(2,62) = 1.29, p =.28, partial 2 =.04. MTs were higher than those observed in Experiments 1 3 and they varied between 1007 ms and 1028 ms. No effect reached significance in the respective ANOVA; SOA: F(2,62) < 0.01, p >.99, partial 2 <.01; condition: F(1,31) = 2.20, p =.15, partial 2 =.07; interaction SOA condition: F(2,62) = 0.02, p =.98, partial 2 <.01. Mean RTs in Task 1 (see Table 1) were higher with the perceptual judgment task as Task 2 compared to tool grasping as Task 2; F(1,31) = 27.44, p <.01, partial 2 =.47. RT1 were numerically slightly smaller in the baseline conditions than in the filtering conditions, but the corresponding effect did only approach significance; F(1,31) = 3.77, p =.06, partial 2 =.11. There was a reliable effect of SOA on RT1; F(2,62) = 4.97, p =.03, partial 2 =.14, ε =.57; but this effect was modulated by a significant interaction of SOA and type of Task 2; F(2,62) = 10.46, p <.01, partial 2 =.25, ε =.61: whereas RT1 again increased with an increasing SOA with tool grasping as Task 2, RT1 remained almost constant with the perceptual judgment task as Task 2. No other effect was significant Error analyses Mean error percentages are presented in Table 2 and the detailed statistics are summarized in Table 3. Task 1 error rates were comparable to those in Experiments 1 and 2, and they decreased significantly with an increasing SOA. Task 2 error rates were in general higher than in the previous Experiments 1 and 2. Also, they were significantly higher with the perceptual judgment task as Task 2. Of importance, they were also significantly higher in filtering conditions than in baseline conditions, but this was only true when perceptual judgment was Task 2 (resulting in a significant interaction of condition and type of Task 2).

10 M. Janczyk et al. / Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Discussion Experiments 1 3 have consistently shown that left-handed grasping and awkward grasping (as an instance of an unskilled movement) are not susceptible to Garner-Interference. According to the logic of Ganel and Goodale (2003) this suggests a dorsal processing mode, and at the same time contradicts recent proposals that these actions escape dorsal processing and are under ventral control (Gonzalez, Ganel, et al., 2006; Gonzalez, Goodale, et al., 2006; Gonzalez et al., 2008). In Experiment 4 we investigated another variant of a likely unskilled movement, and the participants were asked to grasp the stimuli with a realistic tool, namely pliers. This tool grasping task was implemented as Task 2 in a PRP experiment, and in addition the participants were tested in a second session where Task 2 was a perceptual judgment task. With this, presumably typical ventral, task Garner-Interference has repeatedly been shown (Ganel & Goodale, 2003; Janczyk & Kunde, 2010; Kunde, Landgraf, et al., 2007) and it thus served as a control condition in Experiment 4. The results are quickly summarized, as they are very similar to those reported for Experiments 1 and 2. First, for both tasks (the tool grasping and the perceptual judgment task) we observed the typical dual-task behavior and both tasks appear to recruit central resources (to the same degree). Second, and more importantly, large Garner-Interference was found for the perceptual judgment task, and Garner-Interference combined additively with SOA as has been reported previously (Janczyk & Kunde, 2010; Kunde, Landgraf, et al., 2007). Third, and most important, despite doubling the sample size and successfully finding Garner-Interference in the perceptual judgment task with the same participants, no Garner- Interference was found in the tool grasping task. This result nicely corroborates the results of our previous Experiments 1 3 and reinforces the claim that neither left-handed nor unskilled grasping movements are controlled by a different processing stream than normal, right-handed grasping is. 10. General discussion The present study was conducted to test whether left-handed and/or unskilled grasping relies on perceptual information from the ventral pathway instead of being controlled by the dorsal pathway (Gonzalez, Ganel, et al., 2006; Gonzalez, Goodale, et al., 2006; Gonzalez et al., 2008). Contrary to these earlier studies we used Garner-Interference as the behavioral indicator of the underlying processing mode (Ganel & Goodale, 2003). We ran three dual-task PRP experiments and one single-task experiment where different grasping tasks were administered (as Task 2 in the PRP experiments): in Experiment 1 the participants used a left-handed precision grip, in Experiments 2 and 3 they used an awkward grip with their right hand, and in Experiment 4 they used pliers (handled by their right hand) to grasp small wooden blocks. The results can be summarized quickly since they all converge in the same conclusion: never did we find Garner- Interference across these three grasping tasks, and following the reasoning of Ganel and Goodale (2003) we need to conclude that they all were mediated by the dorsal pathway instead of relying on perceptual information provided by the ventral pathway. Obviously this contrasts with the proposals by Gonzalez, Ganel, et al. (2006) and Gonzalez et al. (2008) and highlights the importance of independent evidence and the use of varying methods. In light of the controversial debate around the illusory effects on action and perception, we consider our results clear-cut and suggest that there is no need for distinguishing between two classes of actions: one guided by a different processing stream than the other. In terms of theory building this would also be good news as it means a step (back) towards parsimony Illusory effects revisited As mentioned above several studies have reported comparable effects of visual illusions on perception and action (e.g., Franz et al., 2000, 2003; Pavani et al., 1999). Proponents of the Action Perception model tried to reconcile these findings with their model by arguing that these studies have used intrusive devices to measure grip aperture, such that participants performed an awkward and unskilled movement. In consequence, grasping would have been guided by the ventral stream and therefore would unsurprisingly yield the same results as a typical perceptual task (e.g., Goodale, 2008). However, this argument relies on the one study by Gonzalez et al. (2008) which did not test this notion directly. In a direct test, contrasting the supposedly intrusive devices with non-intrusive devices, Franz, Hesse, and Kollath (2009) found equal illusion effects with both methods, thereby showing that the counterargument of Goodale (2008) is not valid. This result is corroborated and generalized by the present study using Garner-Interference as the behavioral marker The dual task behavior of left-hand and unskilled grasping movements In addition our results allow concluding that unskilled and left-handed grasping is subject to massive dual-task interference, by demonstrating a PRP effect across all three PRP experiments. This is less surprising since we have shown elsewhere that even right-handed precision grasping is subject to dual-task interference (Janczyk & Kunde, 2010; Kunde, Landgraf, et al., 2007). Still, our results may be informative regarding the cognitive costs of tool use. An ANOVA comparing the PRP effects (calculated as the difference of RT2 between the SOAs of 50 ms and 1000 ms) across the three PRP experiments yielded a significant main effect of experiment; (F(2,61) = , p <.01, partial 2 =.33). Post-hoc Scheffé tests, however, localized the source for this effect in the fact that the PRP effect in Experiment 1 (531 ms) was significantly larger than in Experiment 2 (317 ms) and Experiment 4 (367 ms). The latter two PRP effects were not significantly different. Thus, in terms of cognitive costs, grasping with a tool seems to not differ from other unskilled actions. It may even be that the higher PRP effect for lefthanded grasping (Experiment 1) was inflated, since RT1 was the highest in this experiment, too. Note that we need to be cautious when comparing PRP effects across experiments (or experimental conditions). In general, PRP effects are difficult to interpret, since the size of PRP effects is, for example, a function of RT1 (see, e.g., Lien, McCann, Ruthruff, & Proctor, 2005). Clearly, further and more sophisticated research using a within-subject manipulation of the grasping conditions and including a right-hand precision grip control condition is necessary to more thoroughly assess the dual-task costs of different grasping actions. Interestingly there seems to be a consistent (but non-significant) trend of condition on RT1. Such a (crosstalk-) effect of Task 2 on Task 1 would not be compatible with strictly serial bottleneck models like the one advanced by Pashler (1994). To accommodate such findings Hommel (1998) and Lien and Proctor (2002) suggested to divide the central stage into two sub-stages: (1) a first stage of stimulus-response translation that can proceed in parallel to other stages and when two such stages overlap is the source of mutual interference (or facilitation) between tasks, and (2) a second bottleneck stage of final response selection. We also replicated Garner-Interference in Experiment 4 when a perceptual judgment was used as Task 2. Comparable to previous reports (Janczyk & Kunde, 2010; Kunde, Landgraf, et al., 2007)

11 3414 M. Janczyk et al. / Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Garner-Interference combined additively with the factor SOA suggesting according to the central bottleneck model and the PRP logic (Pashler, 1994; Pashler & Johnston, 1998; Appendix A) an implication of central mechanisms, although intuitively Garner- Interference comes across as a perceptual phenomenon Conclusions The present study tested two pieces of results related to the Action Perception model (Milner & Goodale, 2006) for their mutual consistency: left-handed and unskilled grasping should rely on perceptual information from the ventral stream (Gonzalez, Ganel, et al., 2006; Gonzalez, Goodale, et al., 2006; Gonzalez et al., 2008), and the ventral stream should be susceptible to Garner-Interference (Ganel & Goodale, 2003). Across our four experiments never did we observe Garner-Interference for the grasping movements in question. As such, our study revealed an inconsistency in theorizing related to the Action Perception model, and we suggest that left-handed and unskilled grasps are controlled by similar mechanisms as natural (right-handed) grasping is. Thus there is no need to distinguish several classes of visually guided (grasping) actions, which are controlled by different processing streams. It might be helpful to think about a less strong interpretation of the Action Perception model, as this model is basically a neuroanatomical and neuropsychological theory. Both the visual illusion and the Garner-Interference arguments are derived from characteristics attributed to both visual pathways. Of course, our results do not question such interpretation: the core assertion of the Action Perception model (the different purposes of both streams) may still be valid, even if such assumed characteristics turn out being wrong. Our interpretation of the results, however, has also implications for applied research concerning the question of what characteristics of natural actions generalize to transformed and other artificial movements (Kunde, Müsseler, et al., 2007). In particular, the characteristics of movements using virtual tools like a computer-mouse or other input devices might be quite similar to natural actions, a topic that should be investigated in future research. Appendix A. The central bottleneck model and the PRP paradigm The psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm is a dualtask paradigm allowing for rigorous control of the temporal succession of two stimuli. Because usually two different tasks are to be performed in response to these stimuli the amount of task overlap can be manipulated by the experimenter. In a typical PRP experiment two tasks (Task 1 and Task 2) have to be performed in each trial and in rapid succession. Each task requires its own response (R1 and R2) to the corresponding imperative stimulus (S1 and S2). S1 and S2 are separated by a time interval referred to as the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). The SOA is experimentally manipulated and typically short and long intervals are used within an experiment, e.g., 50 ms and 1000 ms. The common finding is that RT2 (the response time in Task 2) decreases with an increasing SOA (the PRP effect), while RT1 (the response time in Task 1) is largely unaffected by the SOA manipulation. This pattern is quite robust and has been found with a variety of tasks and even if different stimulus und response modalities are employed (see Pashler, 1994; Pashler & Johnston, 1998, for overviews). Several accounts for the PRP effect were advanced, but the most widely accepted is the central (or response selection) bottleneck model first proposed by Welford (1952) and later formalized by Pashler (1994; Pashler & Johnston, 1998). Fig. A.1. Illustration of a PRP experiment assuming a structural central bottleneck: (a) processing of Tasks 1 and 2 without experimental manipulations in addition to SOA, and (b) processing of Task 1 and 2 with a manipulation implemented in the central stage of Task 2. (A = pre-central stage, B = central stage, C = post-central stage; for further explanations please see Appendix A). According to this model the processing of each task can be subdivided into at least three stages (for the following, please see Fig. A.1a in this Appendix): a pre-central perceptual stage (A), a central response selection stage (B), and a post-central motor stage (C). The crucial assumption is that stages A and C can be processed in parallel to other stages, while at any time only one stage B can be processed, thus constituting a processing bottleneck. With a short SOA the perceptual stage of Task 2 (A2) finishes before the central bottleneck has been released from the central stage of Task 1 (B1). Thus, Task 2 processing must be deferred yielding a longer RT2. In contrast, with a long enough SOA, stage A2 likely finishes only after the bottleneck has been released from Task 1, and thus Task 2 processing can continue without any deferment. A further prediction from this model is that any manipulation implemented in Task 2 that affects a central (or also post-central) stage adds the same amount of time to RT2 across all SOA levels. In this case the respective manipulation and the factor SOA combine additively. This situation is illustrated in Fig. A.1b. References Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. F. X., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, Derakhshan, I. (2006). Laterality of the command center in relation to handedness and simple reaction time: A clinical perspective. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96, Franz, V. H. (2001). Action does not resist visual illusions. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, Franz, V. H. (2003). Manual size estimation: A neuropsychological measure of perception? Experimental Brain Research, 151, Franz, V. H., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2003). Grasp effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion: Obstacle-avoidance is not the explanation. Experimental Brain Research, 149, Franz, V. H., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2008). Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11, Franz, V. H., Hesse, C., & Kollath, S. (2009). Visual illusions, delayed grasping, and memory: No shift from dorsal to ventral control. Neuropsychologia, 47, Ganel, T., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape. Nature, 426, Garner, W. R. (1978). Selective attention to attributes and to stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, Garner, W. R., & Felfoldy, G. L. (1970). Integrality of stimulus dimensions in various types of information processing. Cognitive Psychology, 1,

Dorsal and Ventral Processing Under Dual-Task Conditions Wilfried Kunde, 1 Franziska Landgraf, 1 Marko Paelecke, 1 and Andrea Kiesel 2

Dorsal and Ventral Processing Under Dual-Task Conditions Wilfried Kunde, 1 Franziska Landgraf, 1 Marko Paelecke, 1 and Andrea Kiesel 2 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report Dorsal and Ventral Processing Under Dual-Task Conditions Wilfried Kunde, 1 Franziska Landgraf, 1 Marko Paelecke, 1 and Andrea Kiesel 2 1 Martin-Luther-University,

More information

Does dorsal processing require central capacity? More evidence from the PRP paradigm

Does dorsal processing require central capacity? More evidence from the PRP paradigm DOI 10.1007/s00221-010-2211-9 RESEARCH ARTICLE Does dorsal processing require central capacity? More evidence from the PRP paradigm Markus Janczyk Wilfried Kunde Received: 21 August 2009 / Accepted: 28

More information

and the surrounding annuli. Effects on grasp scaling be-

and the surrounding annuli. Effects on grasp scaling be- Brief Communication 177 The dissociation between perception and action in the Ebbinghaus illusion: Nonillusory effects of pictorial cues on grasp Angela M. Haffenden, Karen C. Schiff and Melvyn A. Goodale

More information

A Double Dissociation Between Action and Perception in the Context of Visual Illusions

A Double Dissociation Between Action and Perception in the Context of Visual Illusions PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report A Double Dissociation Between Action and Perception in the Context of Visual Illusions Opposite Effects of Real and Illusory Size Tzvi Ganel, 1 Michal Tanzer, 1 and

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article Research Article GRASPING VISUAL ILLUSIONS: No Evidence for a Dissociation Between Perception and Action V.H. Franz, 1 K.R. Gegenfurtner, 1 H.H. Bülthoff, 1 and M. Fahle 2 1 Max-Planck-Institut für Biologische

More information

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1994, 47A (1) 201-205 Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing Harold Pashler University of

More information

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning

To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Activation of effect codes in response planning - 1 - To appear in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning Michael Ziessler

More information

Vision Research 71 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 71 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 71 (2012) 37 43 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Attention is needed for action control: Further evidence

More information

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 396 409 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.396

More information

7 Grip aperture and target shape

7 Grip aperture and target shape 7 Grip aperture and target shape Based on: Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ. The influence of target object shape on maximum grip aperture in human grasping movements. Exp Brain Res, In revision 103 Introduction

More information

Perception and Action Are Inseparable

Perception and Action Are Inseparable ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 13(2), 163 166 Copyright 2001, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Perception and Action Are Inseparable Jeroen B. J. Smeets and Eli Brenner Vakgroep Fysiologie Erasmus Universiteit

More information

Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation

Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2008, 25 (7 8), 920 950 Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation Volker H. Franz and Karl R. Gegenfurtner University of Giessen, Giessen, Germany The finding

More information

Max Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik. Spemannstraße Tübingen Germany

Max Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik. Spemannstraße Tübingen Germany MP Max Planck Institut für biologische Kybernetik Spemannstraße 38 72076 Tübingen Germany Technical Report No. 067 visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Volker

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Corresponding author: Karl K. Kopiske a, f

Corresponding author: Karl K. Kopiske a, f The functional subdivision of the visual brain: Is there a real illusion effect on action? A multi-lab replication study Karl K. Kopiske a, f, Nicola Bruno b, Constanze Hesse c, Thomas Schenk d, Volker

More information

The Locus of Tool-Transformation Costs

The Locus of Tool-Transformation Costs Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 3, 703 714 2011 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0026315 The Locus of Tool-Transformation

More information

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Chapter 3: 2 visual systems Overview Explain the significance of the turn to the brain in cognitive science Explain Mishkin and Ungerleider s hypothesis that there are two distinct visual systems Outline

More information

Behavioural Brain Research

Behavioural Brain Research Behavioural Brain Research 284 (2015) 167 178 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report How coordinate and categorical

More information

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION Jordan Schoenherr, Craig Leth-Steensen, and William M. Petrusic psychophysics.lab@gmail.com, craig_leth_steensen@carleton.ca, bpetrusi@carleton.ca Carleton

More information

Is the psychological refractory period effect for ideomotor compatible tasks eliminated by speed-stress instructions?

Is the psychological refractory period effect for ideomotor compatible tasks eliminated by speed-stress instructions? Psychological Research (2007) 71: 553 567 DOI 10.1007/s00426-006-0066-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Yun Kyoung Shin Æ Yang Seok Cho Æ Mei-Ching Lien Robert W. Proctor Is the psychological refractory period effect

More information

Biomechanical factors may explain why grasping violates Weber s law

Biomechanical factors may explain why grasping violates Weber s law Biomechanical factors may explain why grasping violates Weber s law Kathrin S. Utz a, Constanze Hesse b, Nina Aschenneller a, Thomas Schenk a * a Department of Neurology, Friedrich-Alexander University

More information

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications Bruce N. Walker and Addie Ehrenstein Psychology Department Rice University 6100 Main Street Houston, TX 77005-1892 USA +1 (713) 527-8101

More information

Delayed action does not always require the ventral stream: A study on a patient with visual form agnosia

Delayed action does not always require the ventral stream: A study on a patient with visual form agnosia Delayed action does not always require the ventral stream: A study on a patient with visual form agnosia Constanze Hesse 1 & Thomas Schenk 2 16/01/2014 keywords: perception action model, visual form agnosia,

More information

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task?

Bachelor s Thesis. Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a. sequential movement task? Bachelor s Thesis Can the Dual Processor Model account for task integration with a sequential movement task? Nils Hatger Prof. Dr. Ing. Willem Verwey Jonathan Barnhoorn, PhD student BMS Faculty/Cognitive

More information

An Investigation of Methodological. Concerns Regarding Grasping and Its. Comparison with Perception

An Investigation of Methodological. Concerns Regarding Grasping and Its. Comparison with Perception An Investigation of Methodological Concerns Regarding Grasping and Its Comparison with Perception Rachel M. Foster Kumulative Dissertation zur Erlangung der Würde eines Doktors der Naturwissenschaften

More information

A model of parallel time estimation

A model of parallel time estimation A model of parallel time estimation Hedderik van Rijn 1 and Niels Taatgen 1,2 1 Department of Artificial Intelligence, University of Groningen Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen 2 Department of Psychology,

More information

The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology?

The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology? ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 13(2), 117 122 Copyright 2001, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Relation Between Perception and Action: What Should Neuroscience Learn From Psychology? Patrick R. Green Department

More information

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan

University of Alberta. The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm. Shawn Tan University of Alberta The SNARC effect as a tool to Examine Crosstalk during Numerical Processing in a PRP paradigm by Shawn Tan A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies and Research in partial

More information

Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm

Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm VISUAL COGNITION, 2002, 9 (4/5), 421 457 Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm Peter Wühr and Jochen Müsseler Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research,

More information

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition Ewald Neumann (ewald.neumann@canterbury.ac.nz) Department of Psychology, University of Canterbury Christchurch, New Zealand Stephen J.

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction

More information

Planning movements well in advance

Planning movements well in advance COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2008, 25 (7 8), 985 995 Planning movements well in advance Constanze Hesse University of Giessen, Giessen, Germany Denise D. J. de Grave Human Movement Science, Vrije Universiteit

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? Keyed. THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 55B (1), 1 25 Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations? J.H. Wearden, A. Parry, and L. Stamp University of

More information

Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck

Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (4), 729-734 Repetition blindness is immune to the central bottleneck PAUL E. DUX AND RENÉ MAROIS Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee The attentional blink

More information

Representational momentum in perception and grasping: Translating versus transforming objects

Representational momentum in perception and grasping: Translating versus transforming objects Journal of Vision (2004) 4, 575-584 http://journalofvision.org/4/7/5/ 575 Representational momentum in perception and grasping: Translating versus transforming objects Anne-Marie Brouwer Volker H. Franz

More information

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2001, 61: 175-179 Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions El bieta Szel¹g 1, Krystyna Rymarczyk 1 and Ernst Pöppel 2 1 Department

More information

Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation

Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (7), 1791-1802 doi:10.3758/app.72.7.1791 Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation SANDRA A J. THOMSON,

More information

Do we have distinct systems for immediate and delayed actions? A selective review on the role of visual memory in action

Do we have distinct systems for immediate and delayed actions? A selective review on the role of visual memory in action Do we have distinct systems for immediate and delayed actions? A selective review on the role of visual memory in action Thomas Schenk a * & Constanze Hesse b a Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Separating Limits on Preparation Versus Online Processing in Multitasking Paradigms: Evidence for Resource Models

Separating Limits on Preparation Versus Online Processing in Multitasking Paradigms: Evidence for Resource Models Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2017, Vol. 43, No. 1, 89 102 2016 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000277 Separating

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Planning to Reach for an Object Changes How the Reacher Perceives It Peter M. Vishton, Nicolette J. Stephens, Lauren A. Nelson, Sarah E. Morra, Kaitlin L. Brunick,

More information

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SESORY COSTRAIS? TOWARD A UIFIED THEORY OF GEERAL TASK DIFFICULTY Hanna Benoni and Yehoshua Tsal Department of Psychology, Tel-Aviv University hannaben@post.tau.ac.il

More information

Kimron Shapiro (Ed.), pp Oxford University Press

Kimron Shapiro (Ed.), pp Oxford University Press Temporal Constraints on Human Information Processing Kimron Shapiro (Ed.), pp. 100-123 Oxford University Press Attentional Blink and the PRP effect Perceptual and Central Interference in Dual-Task Performance

More information

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition Carmelo M. Vicario¹ ¹ Dipartimento di Psicologia, Università Roma la Sapienza, via dei Marsi 78, Roma, Italy Key words: number- time- spatial

More information

Dual-Task Interference with Equal Task Emphasis: Graded Capacity-Sharing or Central Postponement? and Eliot Hazeltine. NASA - Ames Research Center

Dual-Task Interference with Equal Task Emphasis: Graded Capacity-Sharing or Central Postponement? and Eliot Hazeltine. NASA - Ames Research Center Perception & Psychophysics (in press) Dual-Task Interference Dual-Task Interference with Equal Task Emphasis: Graded Capacity-Sharing or Central Postponement? Eric Ruthruff Harold E. Pashler NASA - Ames

More information

Cross-modal attentional deficits in processing tactile stimulation

Cross-modal attentional deficits in processing tactile stimulation Perception & Psychophysics 2001, 63 (5), 777-789 Cross-modal attentional deficits in processing tactile stimulation ROBERTO DELL ACQUA and MASSIMO TURATTO University of Padua, Padua, Italy and PIERRE JOLICŒUR

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction

It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction Imitation and anticipation 1 It takes two to imitate: Anticipation and imitation in social interaction Roland Pfister 1, David Dignath 1, Bernhard Hommel 2, & Wilfried Kunde 1 1 Department of Psychology,

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab Laboratory Equipment & Test Procedures. Motor learning and control historically has built its discipline through laboratory research. This has led to the

More information

Dynamic Illusion Effects in a Reaching Task: Evidence for Separate Visual Representations in the Planning and Control of Reaching

Dynamic Illusion Effects in a Reaching Task: Evidence for Separate Visual Representations in the Planning and Control of Reaching Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2001, Vol. 27, No. 3, 560-572 Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0O96-1523/O1/J5.0O DOI: 10.1037//OO96-1523.27.3.56O

More information

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? University of Iowa Honors Theses University of Iowa Honors Program Spring 2017 Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? Ann Walsh University of Iowa Follow this and additional works at:

More information

Level 2 Perspective Taking Entails Two Processes: Evidence From PRP Experiments

Level 2 Perspective Taking Entails Two Processes: Evidence From PRP Experiments Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2013, Vol. 39, No. 6, 1878 1887 2013 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0033336 Level 2 Perspective

More information

Enhanced visual perception near the hands

Enhanced visual perception near the hands Enhanced visual perception near the hands Bachelor thesis Marina Meinert (s0163430) Supervisors: 1 st supervisor: Prof. Dr. Ing. W. B. Verwey 2 nd supervisor: Dr. M. L. Noordzij External supervisor: Dr.

More information

Spatially Diffuse Inhibition Affects Multiple Locations: A Reply to Tipper, Weaver, and Watson (1996)

Spatially Diffuse Inhibition Affects Multiple Locations: A Reply to Tipper, Weaver, and Watson (1996) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1996, Vol. 22, No. 5, 1294-1298 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/%/$3.00 Spatially Diffuse Inhibition

More information

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY Thomas Lachmann *, Gunjan Khera * and Cees van Leeuwen # * Psychology II, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany # Laboratory

More information

Does Mental Rotation Require Central Mechanisms?

Does Mental Rotation Require Central Mechanisms? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1995, Vol. 21, No. 3, 552-570 Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc 0096-1523/95/S3.00 Does Mental Rotation

More information

CONTEXT OF BACKWARD COMPATIBILITY EFFECTS

CONTEXT OF BACKWARD COMPATIBILITY EFFECTS CONTEXT OF BACKWARD COMPATIBILITY EFFECTS THE CONTEXTUAL SPECIFICITY OF BACKWARD COMPATIBILITY EFFECTS By KYUNG-HYUN RUTH KIM, B.H.Sc. (Hons), B.A. (Hons) A Thesis Submitted to the School of Graduate Studies

More information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing Zihui Lu (zihui.lu@utoronto.ca) Meredyth Daneman (daneman@psych.utoronto.ca) Eyal M. Reingold (reingold@psych.utoronto.ca)

More information

Study on perceptually-based fitting line-segments

Study on perceptually-based fitting line-segments Regeo. Geometric Reconstruction Group www.regeo.uji.es Technical Reports. Ref. 08/2014 Study on perceptually-based fitting line-segments Raquel Plumed, Pedro Company, Peter A.C. Varley Department of Mechanical

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article Research Article PERCEPTION-ACTION DISSOCIATIONS OF A WALKABLE MÜLLER-LYER CONFIGURATION University of Virginia Abstract These studies examined the role of spatial encoding in inducing perception-action

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks

Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (7), 1685-1699 Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks Rico Fischer Humboldt University, Berlin, Germany Jeff Miller University of Otago, Dunedin, New

More information

Detecting and identifying response-compatible stimuli

Detecting and identifying response-compatible stimuli Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 125-129 Detecting and identifying response-compatible stimuli JOCHEN MÜSSELER and BERNHARD HOMMEL Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, Germany

More information

Chapter 3 What do Psychological Refractory Period and Attentional Blink have in Common?

Chapter 3 What do Psychological Refractory Period and Attentional Blink have in Common? What do Psychological Refractory Period and Attentional Blink have in Common? Merel M. Pannebakker, Lorenza S. Colzato, Guido P. H. Band, & Bernhard Hommel Manuscript submitted for publication 63 Abstract

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis

The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis The Attentional Blink is Modulated by First Target Contrast: Implications of an Attention Capture Hypothesis Simon Nielsen * (sini@imm.dtu.dk) Tobias S. Andersen (ta@imm.dtu.dk) Cognitive Systems Section,

More information

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage Sensory to Motor Transformation Sensory information (visual,

More information

Left hand, but not right hand, reaching is sensitive to visual context

Left hand, but not right hand, reaching is sensitive to visual context Exp Brain Res (010) 03:7 3 DOI 10.1007/s001-010-14-6 RESEARCH NOTE Left hand, but not right hand, reaching is sensitive to visual context Jos J. Adam Rick Müskens Susan Hoonhorst Jay Pratt Martin H. Fischer

More information

Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration. John Wearden Keele University U.K.

Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration. John Wearden Keele University U.K. Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration John Wearden Keele University U.K. In this talk I want to illustrate the use of the techniques of production, reproduction, and verbal estimation

More information

Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill

Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 1979, Vol. S, No. 2, 179-187 Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill John B. Shea and

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks Memory & Cognition 2005, 33 (6), 1085-1095 Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks IRING KOCH and WOLFGANG PRINZ Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Munich, Germany

More information

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks*

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks* The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks* ARNOLD D. WELLt University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 Multidimensional stimuli, which could vary on one, two, or all three

More information

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (6), 1111-1116 doi:10.3758/pbr.15.6.1111 Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm Mei-Ching Lien Oregon

More information

The Neuroscience of Vision II

The Neuroscience of Vision II Striate Cortex (V1) is not Sufficient for Seeing The Neuroscience of Vision II Hubel and Wiesel conclude their 1968 paper by Where else is Visual Processing Performed? Beyond V1 An important step in identifying

More information

Natural prehension in trials without haptic feedback but only when calibration is allowed

Natural prehension in trials without haptic feedback but only when calibration is allowed Neuropsychologia 45 (2007) 288 294 Natural prehension in trials without haptic feedback but only when calibration is allowed Geoffrey Bingham a, Rachel Coats b, Mark Mon-Williams b, a Department of Psychology,

More information

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab This report details my summer research project for the REU Theoretical and Computational Neuroscience program as

More information

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task Neath, I., & Brown, G. D. A. (2005). Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task. Memory Lab Technical Report 2005-01, Purdue University. Scale Invariance and Primacy

More information

Cognitive Neuroscience History of Neural Networks in Artificial Intelligence The concept of neural network in artificial intelligence

Cognitive Neuroscience History of Neural Networks in Artificial Intelligence The concept of neural network in artificial intelligence Cognitive Neuroscience History of Neural Networks in Artificial Intelligence The concept of neural network in artificial intelligence To understand the network paradigm also requires examining the history

More information

Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning

Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (3), 449 456 Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning Natacha Deroost and Eric Soetens Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels,

More information

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 (2004) 703 708 Test review Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., 2002 1. Test description The Trail Making Test

More information

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays Sung Park and Richard Catrambone 2 School of Psychology & Graphics, Visualization, and Usability Center (GVU) Georgia Institute of

More information

Learning at any rate: action effect learning for stimulus-based actions

Learning at any rate: action effect learning for stimulus-based actions Psychological Research (2011) 75:61 65 DOI 10.1007/s00426-010-0288-1 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Learning at any rate: action effect learning for stimulus-based actions Roland Pfister Andrea Kiesel Joachim Hoffmann

More information

Precueing Spatial S-R Correspondence: Is There Regulation of Expected Response Conflict?

Precueing Spatial S-R Correspondence: Is There Regulation of Expected Response Conflict? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2008, Vol. 34, No. 4, 872 883 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.872

More information

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 13 (1), 6-65 Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects JULIE M. C. BAKER and JOHN

More information

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18 (2003) 11 18 The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children Abstract Ralph M. Reitan, Deborah Wolfson Reitan Neuropsychology

More information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1 (Visual) Attention Perception and awareness of a visual object seems to involve attending to the object. Do we have to attend to an object to perceive it? Some tasks seem to proceed with little or no attention

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance VOL. I I, NO. 6 DECEMBER 1985 Separability and Integrality of Global and Local Levels of Hierarchical Patterns Ruth Kimchi University

More information

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning E. J. Livesey (el253@cam.ac.uk) P. J. C. Broadhurst (pjcb3@cam.ac.uk) I. P. L. McLaren (iplm2@cam.ac.uk)

More information

Why do we need vision? We need vision for two quite different but complementary reasons We need vision to give us detailed knowledge of the world beyo

Why do we need vision? We need vision for two quite different but complementary reasons We need vision to give us detailed knowledge of the world beyo So, What Has Vision Done For Me Lately? Prepared for ICBO Ontario, CA By: R. A. Hohendorf OD April 2010 Why do we need vision? We need vision for two quite different but complementary reasons We need vision

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 95 (23) 4 49 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc What do humans learn in a double, temporal

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception Competing Frameworks in Perception Lesson II: Perception module 08 Perception.08. 1 Views on perception Perception as a cascade of information processing stages From sensation to percept Template vs. feature

More information

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception Competing Frameworks in Perception Lesson II: Perception module 08 Perception.08. 1 Views on perception Perception as a cascade of information processing stages From sensation to percept Template vs. feature

More information

How Far Away Is That? It Depends on You: Perception Accounts for the Abilities of Others

How Far Away Is That? It Depends on You: Perception Accounts for the Abilities of Others Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2015, Vol. 41, No. 3, 000 2015 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000070 OBSERVATION

More information

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game Ivaylo Vlaev (ivaylo.vlaev@psy.ox.ac.uk) Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX1

More information

Parallel Response Selection after Callosotomy

Parallel Response Selection after Callosotomy Parallel Response Selection after Callosotomy Eliot Hazeltine 1, Andrea Weinstein 2, and Richard B. Ivry 2 Abstract & Two studies [Ivry, R. B., Franz, E. A., Kingstone, A., & Johnston, J. C. The psychological

More information