Perceptual consequences of centre-surround antagonism in visual motion processing

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Perceptual consequences of centre-surround antagonism in visual motion processing"

Transcription

1 1 Perceptual consequences of centre-surround antagonism in visual motion processing Duje Tadin, Joseph S. Lappin, Lee A. Gilroy and Randolph Blake Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Vanderbilt University, st Ave S, Nashville, TN Intuition tells us that increasing the size of a moving object should make its motion more visible. The receptive fields of neurons throughout the visual system, however, often exhibit centre-surround antagonism, which inhibits the responses of such neurons when stimulated with increasingly larger objects 1. Here we report that increasing the size of a high-contrast moving object can make its motion very difficult to perceive. We further show that this counterintuitive finding is consistent with receptive field properties of neurons in the middle temporal visual area (MT) and provide converging evidence that impaired motion discrimination for large stimuli is a perceptual correlate of centre-surround antagonism in MT 2-4. The spatial suppression of motion signals observed at high contrast gradually changes to spatial summation as contrast decreases, indicating that integration of motion signals over space crucially depends on stimulus properties. In this way, the visual system processes motion efficiently and adaptively by employing computationally advantageous inhibitory mechanisms only when the sensory input is sufficiently strong to ensure visibility. When evolution stumbles onto an adaptive biological implementation, that implementation often appears in other, related applications. In the case of vision, one such adaptation is centre-surround receptive field organisation, a ubiquitous property in mammalian visual systems presumably tailored for extracting image features that are differentially distributed over space 1. In motion perception, this is evident as an antagonistic interaction between centre and surround regions of the receptive fields of most neurons in MT 2-4 a cortical region importantly involved in motion processing. A centre-surround MT neuron responds well to motion localised within the central region of its receptive field, but its response is gradually attenuated as the spatial extent of stimulation expands to include the surround region. These neurons are believed to be critically involved in perceiving object motion 3,5, and probably play a role in figureground segmentation 5-7 and shape-from-motion processing The surround inhibition enables MT neurons to perform these important computational tasks, but is there a perceptual downside to the decreased activity of centre-surround neurons when stimulated with a large moving object? Intuition leads one to expect that increasing the size of a moving stimulus should improve its visibility, rather than impair it as the decrease in the activity of centresurround neurons suggests. Indeed, increasing the size of a low-contrast moving stimulus does enhance its visibility 11,12, presumably owing to spatial summation. While psychophysical estimations of receptive field properties tend to be based on lowcontrast or noisy stimuli, physiological descriptions of centre-surround motion neurons have been obtained with high-contrast moving stimuli. Moreover, in visual cortex, the nature of centre-surround interactions is often dependent on contrast, with surround suppression stronger at high contrast and spatial summation more pronounced at low

2 contrast Thus, threshold contrast measurements may not fully describe the spatial properties of human motion perception, especially at high contrast. With this in mind, we devised alternative approaches for measuring motion sensitivity. In our first experiment, we measured the threshold exposure duration required for human observers to accurately discriminate motion direction of a drifting Gabor patch. In separate conditions, observers viewed foveally presented Gabor patches of various sizes and contrasts. Spatial frequency and speed were fixed to 1 cycle/ and 2 /s, respectively. The contrast of a Gabor patch was ramped on and off with a temporal Gaussian envelope, allowing the presentation of brief motion stimuli. The spatial integration of motion signals importantly depends on contrast (Fig. 1). At low contrast (2.8%), duration thresholds decreased with increasing size, reaching a lower asymptote around 40 ms (Fig. 1a, c). This result, implying spatial summation of motion signals, is consistent with earlier reports 11,12. At high contrast, however, duration thresholds increased four-fold as the Gabor patch width increased from 0.7 to 5. This result is highly surprising and implies neural processes fundamentally different from spatial summation. Duration threshold elevation was greatest for Gabor patches larger than 2.6 in width. At this size, the Gabor patch is large enough to impinge on the surrounds of MT neurons with foveal receptive fields (Fig. 1d). We speculate that surround inhibition is responsible for the observed decrease in motion sensitivity and explore this hypothesis in the experiments that follow. It is intriguing that increasing stimulus contrast dramatically changes the way motion signals are integrated over space. Re-plotting the data as a function of contrast (Fig. 1b) reveals that, for small Gabors, the duration thresholds decreased with increasing contrast. For large Gabors, however, duration thresholds increased more than three-fold as the contrast increased from 2.8 to 92%. This result is compatible with the notion that the relative strength and/or spatial extents of the excitatory centre and inhibitory surround change with contrast At high contrast, the computational benefits of surround suppression 5-10 probably outweigh the obligatory decrease in neuronal activity and reduced sensitivity. At low contrast, high sensitivity is essential, so it makes functional sense that receptive field organisation shifts from surround suppression to spatial summation. Our data suggest that the transition from summation to suppression occurs around 5% contrast (Fig. 1c), which is the contrast where an average MT neuron attains about 25% of its maximum response 17. The results reported here are robust. We obtained similar patterns of results with faster moving stimuli (8 /s) and when the spatial bandwidth was held constant by scaling spatial frequency with Gabor width (1 cycle/s). Next, we manipulated the effective stimulus contrast by adding variable amounts of dynamic noise to a fixed contrast (46%) Gabor patch. The results (Fig. 2a) indicate spatial summation when the stimulus is noisy (46% noise contrast). In contrast, when noise is less salient (or absent), increasing the size results in poorer performance. For large Gabor patches, increasing the noise actually improves visibility of stimulus motion. 2

3 Motion strength of a periodic Gabor patch may also be varied by adjusting the magnitude of an abrupt phase shift 18 : increasing phase shifts from 0 to 90 enhances the visibility of motion. We conducted an experiment where observers identified the motion direction of a fixed contrast Gabor patch that abruptly shifted in phase in the middle of a 100 ms presentation interval. Threshold phase shift was obtained for a range of contrasts and sizes with spatial frequency fixed at 0.5 cycles/. Results (Fig. 2b) replicated the duration threshold findings, again demonstrating the spatial suppression of motion signals. There is an ongoing debate about the role of MT in the analysis of isoluminant moving stimuli (i.e. varying in hue rather than luminance contrast) 19-21, but it is clear that MT s response to such stimuli is typically attenuated 20. Given a weak response of MT near isoluminance, we may expect spatial summation of isoluminant stimuli similar to that observed for low-contrast stimuli. Using methods similar to those of the previous experiment, we compared phase shift thresholds for isoluminant (red-green) and luminance contrast (yellow-black) stimuli for a range of Gabor patch sizes. Luminance contrast stimuli showed surround suppression similar in magnitude to that in the previous experiments (Fig. 3a). Thresholds for isoluminant stimuli, however, decreased with increasing size, exhibiting spatial summation. This result is also unexpected because it shows that, for large stimuli, colour-defined motion is perceived more accurately than luminance-defined motion, presumably because the latter is affected by surround suppression. This superiority of colour-defined motion contrasts with previous reports indicating that perception of motion at isoluminance is generally less reliable 19. Surround inhibition in motion-sensitive neurons is characterized by a decrease in neural activity in response to a large moving stimulus. This decreased activity may be evident in weaker adaptation of motion-sensitive centre-surround neurons during prolonged exposure to motion. The motion aftereffect (MAE) an illusory perception of motion following prolonged adaptation to motion is thought to reflect the adaptation of motion-sensitive neurons 22. As adaptation is associated with neuronal activity, our results suggest that adapting to a large high-contrast moving stimulus should result in a weaker MAE. We explored this prediction by inducing MAE with moving Gabor patches of various sizes and contrasts and testing for MAE with a small test stimulus. As predicted, MAE strength decreased with increasing size when observers adapted to a high-contrast Gabor patch, suggesting spatial suppression (Fig. 3b). This result is consistent with earlier observations 23,24 that were restricted to highcontrast adapting stimuli. On the other hand, when a low-contrast adapting stimulus was used, MAE strength increased with increasing size. These results support the notion that neurons involved in perceiving motion direction are inhibited when stimulated with a large, high-contrast moving stimulus. Contrast dependency of this effect again indicates that spatial properties of motion perception are dynamic and stimulus-dependent. A prominent attribute of MT receptive fields is their increasing size with retinal eccentricity 4,25. Similarly, the critical size beyond which surround suppression begins to affect neuronal response increases with eccentricity, although not as rapidly as receptive 3

4 field size 4. Thus, we would expect that the detrimental effect of stimulus size should diminish with increasing eccentricity. To test this prediction, we investigated the effect of size for a range of eccentricities with contrast fixed to 92%. The results for foveal presentation (0 ) once again show surround suppression (Fig. 4). As eccentricity increased, the duration thresholds dropped for all sizes (Fig. 4a). More importantly, the size dependency of duration thresholds systematically changed with eccentricity, with almost no effect at the largest eccentricity tested (Fig. 4b). A closer examination of the results reveals that for each eccentricity (except 54 ) there is a size near which a small increase in size had a relatively large effect on the duration thresholds (crossed circles in Fig. 4b). This critical size increases with eccentricity, paralleling the eccentricitydependent increase in critical stimulus size required for surround suppression to occur in macaque MT 4. The present study reveals the detrimental effect of increasing stimulus size on human motion perception. This finding stands in contrast to earlier attempts to characterise the receptive field properties of human motion perception. Spatial summation has often been assumed as a basic characteristic of motion processing 11,12, but we show that spatial summation is restricted to low visibility conditions. Several previous reports are consistent with parts of the results reported here. Verghese and Stone showed that dividing a large high-contrast object into smaller parts actually improved performance in a speed discrimination task and suggested surround suppression as one possible explanation 26. Derrington and Goddard reported that increasing the contrast of a brief large drifting grating reduced performance 27. This result agrees with a portion of our findings (filled diamonds in Fig. 1b), but their explanation differs from ours. They emphasised that brief motion stimuli, by virtue of their broad temporal frequency spectrum, should stimulate motion filters tuned to opposing directions of motion. At high contrast, these paired filters could saturate, minimising the imbalance of activation, and causing the impaired motion discrimination. This account provides a possible explanation for the effects of contrast reported here, but it fails to capture the effects of size that are central to this paper, and it cannot explain the MAE results (Fig. 3b) that generalise our principal findings to prolonged motion stimuli. The series of results reported here both contradicts everyday intuition and defies accepted ideas about spatial properties of motion perception. These highly counterintuitive results, however, become much less surprising when considered in the context of the receptive field properties of neurons in cortical area MT. Indeed, our results provide converging evidence consistent with the current understanding of how MT neurons respond to moving objects differing in size, eccentricity, prolonged exposure, luminance contrast, and colour contrast. Taken together, these results suggest that impaired motion direction discrimination for large high-contrast stimuli is a perceptual correlate of centre-surround mechanisms in MT. Furthermore, our results generate a series of predictions that can be tested using neurophysiological and neuroimaging approaches. For example, the results with isoluminant stimuli (Fig. 3a) predict that surround inhibition in MT should diminish at isoluminance. The observed contrast dependence of size effects is particularly interesting. It agrees with contrast-dependent changes in receptive field properties observed in primary visual cortex 13-16, and it predicts that similar effects may also occur in MT. Our results clearly indicate that the 4

5 integration of motion signals over space is an adaptive process that enables the visual system to more efficiently process moving stimuli by employing computationally important suppressive mechanisms only when the sensory input is sufficiently strong to guarantee visibility. Methods Patterns were displayed on a linearized monitor with 800 x 600 resolution at 120 Hz (main results were confirmed at 160 and 200 Hz). Viewing was binocular at 83 cm. Ambient illumination was 4.8 cd/m2. Background luminance was 60.5 cd/m2. The size was defined as 2s width of a Gabor patch (Fig. 1d) a drifting vertical sine grating windowed by a stationary two-dimensional Gaussian envelope (s = standard deviation of the Gaussian). Duration was defined as 2s of the temporal Gaussian envelope. Threshold exposure durations were obtained for a range of Gabor patch sizes and peak contrasts. On each trial, a drifting Gabor patch was presented foveally and observers indicated perceived direction (left vs. right) by a key press. Feedback was provided. Duration thresholds (82%) were estimated by interleaved Quest staircases. For each condition, five observers ran four pairs of interleaved staircases, with the first pair discarded as practice. All experiments complied with institutionally reviewed procedures for human subjects. Added noise experiment Speed was increased to 4 /s. A Gabor patch was presented foveally and masked by of a 20 by 20 noise pattern. Noise consisted of dynamic dark and light elements (2.5 by 2.5 arcmin) that were randomly generated at 120 Hz. Other methods were the same as in the first experiment. Isoluminant motion experiment The Gabor patch consisted of spatially overlapping isoluminant red and green gratings. The red-green isoluminant point was obtained with the minimum motion technique 28. If isoluminant red and green gratings are presented spatially in anti-phase, the resulting red-green compound grating has no luminance contrast. Presenting the same two gratings in phase produces a yellow-black luminance grating. Spatial frequency was 1 cycle/. A control experiment measuring duration thresholds yielded results qualitatively similar to those in Fig. 3a. MAE experiment Observers (n = 3) adapted to a foveally presented 1 cycle/ Gabor patch moving at 4 /s. In separate conditions, MAE was measured for various sizes and contrasts. The test stimulus consisted of two overlapping Gabor patches drifting in opposite directions (stimulus parameters were the same as the adapting Gabor except for width, which was fixed to 1 ). If the contrasts of two Gabors were identical, the motion was ambiguous and the test stimulus appeared to flicker. Adapting to a motion in one direction effectively reduces motion strength of the Gabor moving in the adapted direction and the test stimulus will appear to move in the opposite direction. Then, to restore the 5

6 perception of flicker, the contrast of the Gabor moving in the adapted direction is increased and the contrast of the other Gabor lowered. The contrast ratio required to restore flicker perception is taken as a measure of MAE strength (higher contrast ratio = stronger MAE). Initial adaptation was for 30s (10s after the first trial), followed by a 0.3s blank screen and a 1s test stimulus. After viewing the test stimulus, observes indicated the perceived direction. The contrast ratio of two Gabor patches composing the test stimulus was then adjusted under control of two interleaved 1-up 1-down staircases. Each staircase converged after 6 reversals, with the average of the last 4 reversals taken as the result. Eight measurements were made for each condition. Eccentric presentation experiment To present visually larger stimuli within the limited monitor area, a Gaussian spatial envelope was replaced with the 2D raised cosine envelope. Size was defined as the distance between two diametrically opposing points on the raised cosine envelope where the contrast is 60.7% of the peak contrast (analogous to the 2s of a Gaussian distribution). Spatial frequency was lowered to 0.5 cycles/, increasing the speed to 4 /s. In this experiment only, motion directions were vertical (up vs. down). Other methods were the same as in the first experiment Allman, J., Miezin, F. & McGuinness, E. Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 8, (1985). 2. Allman, J., Miezin, F. & McGuinness, E. Direction- and velocity-specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field in the middle temporal visual area (MT). Perception 14, (1985). 3. Born, R. T. & Tootell, R. B. Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area. Nature 357, (1992). 4. Raiguel, S. E., van Hulle, M. M., Xiao, D. K., Marcar, V. L. & Orban, G. A. Shape and spatial distribution of receptive fields and antagonistic motion surround in the middle temporal area (V5) of the macaque. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7, (1995). 5. Born, R. T., Groh, J. M., Zhao, R. & Lukasewycz, S. J. Segregation of object and background motion in visual area MT: effects of microstimulation on eye movements. Neuron 26, (2000). 6. Nakayama, K. & Loomis, J. M. Optical velocity patterns, velocity-sensitive neurons, and space perception: a hypothesis. Perception 3, (1974). 7. Gautama, T. & Van Hulle, M. M. Function of center-surround antagonism for motion in visual area MT/V5: a modeling study. Vision Res. 41, (2001). 8. Buracas, G. T. & Albright, T. D. Contribution of area MT to perception of three-dimensional shape: computational study. Vision Res. 361, (1996). 9. Liu, L. & Van Hulle, M. M. Modeling the surround of MT cells and their selectivity for surface orientation in depth specified by motion. Neural Comput. 10, (1998). 10. Xiao, D. K., Raiguel, S., Marcar, V., Koenderink, J. & Orban, G. A. Spatial heterogeneity of inhibitory surrounds in the middle temporal visual area. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, (1995). 11. Anderson, S. J. & Burr, D. C. Spatial summation properties of directionally sensitive mechanisms in human vision. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 8, (1991). 12. Watson, A. B. & Turano, K. The optimal motion stimulus. Vision Res. 35, (1995). 13. Kapadia, M. K., Westheimer, G. & Gilbert, C. D. Dynamics of spatial summation in primary visual cortex of alert monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, (1999). 14. Levitt, J. B. & Lund, J. S. Contrast dependence of contextual effects in primate visual cortex. Nature 387, (1997).

7 7 15. Sceniak, M. P., Ringach, D. L., Hawken, M. J. & Shapley, R. Contrast's effect on spatial summation by macaque V1 neurons. Nature Neurosci. 2, (1999). 16. Somers, D. C. et al. A local circuit approach to understanding integration of long-range inputs in primary visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 8, (1998). 17. Sclar, G., Maunsell, J. H. & Lennie, P. Coding of image contrast in central visual pathways of the macaque monkey. Vision Res. 30, 1-10 (1990). 18. Nakayama, K. & Silverman, G. H. Detection and discrimination of sinusoidal grating displacements. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, (1985). 19. Dobkins, K. R. & Albright, T. D. in High-level motion processing (ed. Watanabe, T.) (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1998). 20. Gegenfurtner, K. R. et al. Chromatic properties of neurons in macaque MT. Vis. Neurosci. 11, (1994). 21. Thiele, A., Dobkins, K. R. & Albright, T. D. Neural correlates of chromatic motion perception. Neuron 32, (2001). 22. Huk, A. C., Ress, D. & Heeger, D. J. Neuronal basis of the motion aftereffect reconsidered. Neuron 32, (2001). 23. Murakami, I. & Shimojo, S. Modulation of motion aftereffect by surround motion and its dependence on stimulus size and eccentricity. Vision Res. 35, (1995). 24. Sachtler, W. L. & Zaidi, Q. Effect of spatial configuration on motion aftereffects. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 10, (1993). 25. Albright, T. D. Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. J. Neurophysiol. 52, (1984). 26. Verghese, P. & Stone, L. S. Perceived visual speed constrained by image segmentation. Nature 381, (1996). 27. Derrington, A. M. & Goddard, P. A. Failure of motion discrimination at high contrasts: evidence for saturation. Vision Res. 29, (1989). 28. Cavanagh, P., MacLeod, D. I. & Anstis, S. M. Equiluminance: spatial and temporal factors and the contribution of blue-sensitive cones. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 4, (1987). 29. Rees, G., Friston, K. & Koch, C. A direct quantitative relationship between the functional properties of human and macaque V5. Nature Neurosci. 3, (2000). 30. Kastner, S. et al. Modulation of sensory suppression: implications for receptive field sizes in the human visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 86, (2001). Acknowledgements We thank Jeff Schall, Stephanie Shorter-Jacobi and Anup Panduranga for helpful comments on the manuscript. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to D.T. ( duje.tadin@vanderbilt.edu).

8 Tadin et al., Figure a 92% b 5 Duration threshold (ms) Log threshold change c % 22% 11% 5.5% 2.8% 92% 46% 22% 11% 5.5% 2.8% Contrast d 2 deg Figure 1 Effects of size and contrast on motion discrimination. Individual data points are averages for five observers. Error bars are SEM. a, Duration thresholds as a function of stimulus size at different contrasts. b, Duration thresholds as a function of contrast for a range of stimulus sizes. c, Log threshold change as a function of stimulus size at different contrasts. For each observer, log threshold change was calculated relative to the duration threshold for the smallest size (0.7º) at each contrast level. d, A Gabor patch 2.7º wide shown relative to an average foveal macaque MT receptive field. The dashed dark circle illustrates the stimulus size beyond which an average foveal MT center-surround neuron exhibits surround suppression 4. The radius of the surround is usually about 3 times larger than the center radius, as indicated by the full circle. Full spatial extent of the Gabor patch (r = 3σ = 4º) is indicated by the light dashed circle. This comparison assumes that the properties of human and macaque MT are comparable 29, and that the receptive field sizes are similar for two species 30.

9 Tadin et al., Figure a 80 b Duration threshold (ms) 40 46% 30 16% 0% Threshold phase shift (deg) % % 92% 22% 11% 5.5% Figure 2 Results from added noise and phase shift experiments. a, Duration thresholds as a function of stimulus size at different noise contrasts. b, Phase shift thresholds as a function of stimulus size at different contrasts. Error bars are SEM.

10 Tadin et al., Figure 3. Threshold phase shift (deg) a Isoluminant Luminance contrast MAE strength (contrast ratio) b AP RB DT Adapting Figure 3 Results from isoluminant motion and MAE experiments. a, Phase shift thresholds as a function of stimulus size for luminance contrast and isoluminant stimuli. b, Effects of stimulus size and contrast on the MAE strength for three subjects. Filled symbols show MAE strength for low-contrast adaptation (2.8%). Empty symbols show results for high-contrast adaptation (26%). Error bars are SEM.

11 Tadin et al., Figure 4. Duration threshold (ms) a Log threshold change b Figure 4 Effects of size and eccentricity on motion discrimination. a, Duration thresholds as a function of stimulus size at different eccentricities. Error bars are SEM. b, Log threshold change as a function of stimulus size at different eccentricities. For each observer, log threshold change was calculated relative to the duration threshold for the smallest size at each eccentricity. For clarity, error bars (SEM) for each eccentricity were averaged and are shown at the end of each curve. Crossed circles mark the points where increasing the stimulus width by 1.3º had a particularly large effect on the duration thresholds.

CENTER-SURROUND INTERACTIONS IN VISUAL MOTION PROCESSING. Duje Tadin

CENTER-SURROUND INTERACTIONS IN VISUAL MOTION PROCESSING. Duje Tadin CENTER-SURROUND INTERACTIONS IN VISUAL MOTION PROCESSING By Duje Tadin Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Vanderbilt University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for

More information

Time [ms]

Time [ms] Magazine R1051 Brief motion stimuli preferentially activate surroundsuppressed neurons in macaque visual area MT Jan Churan, Farhan A. Khawaja, James M.G. Tsui and Christopher C. Pack Intuitively one might

More information

Contrast Dependence of Suppressive Influences in Cortical Area

Contrast Dependence of Suppressive Influences in Cortical Area Articles in PresS. J Neurophysiol (October 13, 2004). doi:10.1152/jn.00629.2004 FINAL ACCEPTED VERSION Contrast Dependence of Suppressive Influences in Cortical Area MT of Alert Macaque Authors: Christopher

More information

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I Monocular Sensory Processes of Vision: Color Vision Mechanisms of Color Processing . Neural Mechanisms of Color Processing A. Parallel processing - M- & P- pathways B. Second

More information

Contrast Dependence of Suppressive Influences in Cortical Area MT of Alert Macaque

Contrast Dependence of Suppressive Influences in Cortical Area MT of Alert Macaque J Neurophysiol 93: 1809 1815, 2005. First published October 13, 2004; doi:10.1152/jn.00629.2004. Report Contrast Dependence of Suppressive Influences in Cortical Area MT of Alert Macaque Christopher C.

More information

Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex

Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex David Whitney,* 1 Herbert C. Goltz, 2 Christopher G. Thomas, 1 Joseph S. Gati, 2 Ravi S. Menon, 2 Melvyn A. Goodale 1 1 The Department

More information

A Single Mechanism Can Explain the Speed Tuning Properties of MT and V1 Complex Neurons

A Single Mechanism Can Explain the Speed Tuning Properties of MT and V1 Complex Neurons The Journal of Neuroscience, November 15, 2006 26(46):11987 11991 11987 Brief Communications A Single Mechanism Can Explain the Speed Tuning Properties of MT and V1 Complex Neurons John A. Perrone Department

More information

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity S. B. Stevenson *, L. K. Cormack Vision Research 40, 2881-2884. (2000) * University of Houston College of Optometry, Houston TX 77204

More information

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli Vision Research 39 (1999) 3849 3854 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli A. Johnston a, *, C.P.

More information

Neuronal responses to plaids

Neuronal responses to plaids Vision Research 39 (1999) 2151 2156 Neuronal responses to plaids Bernt Christian Skottun * Skottun Research, 273 Mather Street, Piedmont, CA 94611-5154, USA Received 30 June 1998; received in revised form

More information

Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons

Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons Vincent Bonin *, Valerio Mante and Matteo Carandini Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute 2318 Fillmore Street San Francisco, CA 94115, USA Institute of Neuroinformatics

More information

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing C Contextual Influences in Visual Processing TAI SING LEE Computer Science Department and Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Synonyms Surround influence;

More information

Monocular and Binocular Mechanisms of Contrast Gain Control. Izumi Ohzawa and Ralph D. Freeman

Monocular and Binocular Mechanisms of Contrast Gain Control. Izumi Ohzawa and Ralph D. Freeman Monocular and Binocular Mechanisms of Contrast Gain Control Izumi Ohzawa and alph D. Freeman University of California, School of Optometry Berkeley, California 9472 E-mail: izumi@pinoko.berkeley.edu ABSTACT

More information

Stimulus-Dependent Modulation of Suppressive Influences in MT

Stimulus-Dependent Modulation of Suppressive Influences in MT 678 The Journal of Neuroscience, January 12, 2011 31(2):678 686 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Stimulus-Dependent Modulation of Suppressive Influences in MT J. Nicholas Hunter 1,2 and Richard T. Born 1 1

More information

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

Fundamentals of Psychophysics Fundamentals of Psychophysics John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3045! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Visual neuroscience physiology stimulus How do we see the world? neuroimaging

More information

Supplemental Material

Supplemental Material Supplemental Material Recording technique Multi-unit activity (MUA) was recorded from electrodes that were chronically implanted (Teflon-coated platinum-iridium wires) in the primary visual cortex representing

More information

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1 Announcements exam 1 this Thursday! review session: Wednesday, 5:00-6:30pm, Meliora 203 Bryce s office hours: Wednesday, 3:30-5:30pm, Gleason https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zdw7pvgz0um M Cells M cells

More information

Seeing Sound: Changing Visual Perception Through Cross-Modal Interaction. Tracey D. Berger. New York University. Department of Psychology

Seeing Sound: Changing Visual Perception Through Cross-Modal Interaction. Tracey D. Berger. New York University. Department of Psychology Cross-Modal Effects on Perception 1 Seeing Sound: Changing Visual Perception Through Cross-Modal Interaction. Tracey D. Berger New York University Department of Psychology Faculty Sponsor : Denis Pelli

More information

Models of Attention. Models of Attention

Models of Attention. Models of Attention Models of Models of predictive: can we predict eye movements (bottom up attention)? [L. Itti and coll] pop out and saliency? [Z. Li] Readings: Maunsell & Cook, the role of attention in visual processing,

More information

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex Javier R. Movellan Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093-0515 movellan@inc.ucsd.edu Thomas

More information

Consciousness The final frontier!

Consciousness The final frontier! Consciousness The final frontier! How to Define it??? awareness perception - automatic and controlled memory - implicit and explicit ability to tell us about experiencing it attention. And the bottleneck

More information

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence Lecture 5:. Reading Assignments: None 1 Projection 2 Projection 3 Convention: Visual Angle Rather than reporting two numbers (size of object and distance to observer),

More information

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities David P. McGovern, Andrew T. Astle, Sarah L. Clavin and Fiona N. Newell Figure S1: Group-averaged learning

More information

Spatial Distribution of Contextual Interactions in Primary Visual Cortex and in Visual Perception

Spatial Distribution of Contextual Interactions in Primary Visual Cortex and in Visual Perception Spatial Distribution of Contextual Interactions in Primary Visual Cortex and in Visual Perception MITESH K. KAPADIA, 1,2 GERALD WESTHEIMER, 1 AND CHARLES D. GILBERT 1 1 The Rockefeller University, New

More information

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences Marc Pomplun 1, Yueju Liu 2, Julio Martinez-Trujillo 2, Evgueni Simine 2, and John K. Tsotsos 2 1 Department

More information

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 49 (29) 29 43 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres A framework for describing the effects of attention on visual responses

More information

Contrast sensitivity and retinal ganglion cell responses in the primate

Contrast sensitivity and retinal ganglion cell responses in the primate PSYCHOLOGY NEUROSCIENCE Psychology & Neuroscience, 2011, 4, 1, 11-18 DOI: 10.3922/j.psns.2011.1.003 Contrast sensitivity and retinal ganglion cell responses in the primate Barry B. Lee 1,2 and Hao Sun

More information

Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey

Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey Visual Neuroscience (2001), 18, 501 516. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2001 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238001 $12.50 DOI: 10.1017.S0952523801184014 Motion direction signals in the primary visual

More information

Visual Selection and Attention

Visual Selection and Attention Visual Selection and Attention Retrieve Information Select what to observe No time to focus on every object Overt Selections Performed by eye movements Covert Selections Performed by visual attention 2

More information

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation Baran Çürüklü Department of Computer Science and Engineering Mälardalen University Västerås S-721 23, Sweden Abstract. In this paper

More information

Smooth pursuit eye movements and motion perception share motion signals in slow and fast motion mechanisms

Smooth pursuit eye movements and motion perception share motion signals in slow and fast motion mechanisms Journal of Vision (2015) 15(11):12, 1 15 1 Smooth pursuit eye movements and motion perception share motion signals in slow and fast motion mechanisms Kazumichi Matsumiya Research Institute of Electrical

More information

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth 1574 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/ Vol. 25, No. 7/ July 2008 Shioiri et al. Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth Satoshi Shioiri, 1, * Tomohiko

More information

Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system

Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system Vision Research 39 (1999) 4333 4340 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Depth aliasing by the transient-stereopsis system Mark Edwards *, Clifton M. Schor School of Optometry, Uni ersity of California, Berkeley,

More information

Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation

Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation articles Spatial characteristics of the second-order visual pathway revealed by positional adaptation Paul V. McGraw 1, Dennis M. Levi 2 and David Whitaker 1 1 Department of Optometry, University of Bradford,

More information

Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system

Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system Vision Research 38 (1998) 3057 3068 Spatial-frequency and contrast tuning of the transient-stereopsis system Clifton M. Schor *, Mark Edwards, David R. Pope School of Optometry, Uni ersity of California,

More information

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function VISION Vol. 20, No. 2, 67 71, 2008 Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function Keizo SHINOMORI Department of Information Systems Engineering, Kochi University of Technology

More information

Learning to find a shape

Learning to find a shape articles Learning to find a shape M. Sigman and C. D. Gilbert The Rockefeller University, 1230 York Avenue, New York, New York 10021-6399, USA Correspondence should be addressed to C.D.G. (gilbert@rockvax.rockefeller.edu)

More information

C ontextual modulation is a general phenomenon that relates to changes in the perceived appearance of

C ontextual modulation is a general phenomenon that relates to changes in the perceived appearance of OPEN SUBJECT AREAS: HUMAN BEHAVIOUR CORTEX Received 1 April 2014 Accepted 13 November 2014 Published 28 November 2014 Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to U.P. (urip@post.tau.

More information

Speed can go up as well as down at low contrast: Implications for models of motion perception

Speed can go up as well as down at low contrast: Implications for models of motion perception Vision Research 46 (2006) 782 786 Brief communication Speed can go up as well as down at low contrast: Implications for models of motion perception Peter Thompson a, *, Kevin Brooks b, Stephen T. Hammett

More information

Local Disparity Not Perceived Depth Is Signaled by Binocular Neurons in Cortical Area V1 of the Macaque

Local Disparity Not Perceived Depth Is Signaled by Binocular Neurons in Cortical Area V1 of the Macaque The Journal of Neuroscience, June 15, 2000, 20(12):4758 4767 Local Disparity Not Perceived Depth Is Signaled by Binocular Neurons in Cortical Area V1 of the Macaque Bruce G. Cumming and Andrew J. Parker

More information

Sensory Adaptation within a Bayesian Framework for Perception

Sensory Adaptation within a Bayesian Framework for Perception presented at: NIPS-05, Vancouver BC Canada, Dec 2005. published in: Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems eds. Y. Weiss, B. Schölkopf, and J. Platt volume 18, pages 1291-1298, May 2006 MIT

More information

Latency differences and the flash-lag effect

Latency differences and the flash-lag effect Vision Research 43 (2003) 1829 1835 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Latency differences and the flash-lag effect Derek H. Arnold a,b, *, Szonya Durant a,c, Alan Johnston a,b,c a Department of Psychology,

More information

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the Some transformations of color information from lateral geniculate nucleus to striate cortex Russell L. De Valois*, Nicolas P. Cottaris, Sylvia D. Elfar*, Luke E. Mahon, and J. Anthony Wilson* *Psychology

More information

Central neural mechanisms for detecting second-order motion Curtis L Baker Jr

Central neural mechanisms for detecting second-order motion Curtis L Baker Jr 461 Central neural mechanisms for detecting second-order motion Curtis L Baker Jr Single-unit neurophysiology and human psychophysics have begun to reveal distinct neural mechanisms for processing visual

More information

Modeling the Deployment of Spatial Attention

Modeling the Deployment of Spatial Attention 17 Chapter 3 Modeling the Deployment of Spatial Attention 3.1 Introduction When looking at a complex scene, our visual system is confronted with a large amount of visual information that needs to be broken

More information

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye: Problems confronting the visual system: Visual Physiology image contains a huge amount of information which must be processed quickly. image is dim, blurry and distorted. Light levels vary enormously.

More information

Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color tuning properties

Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color tuning properties Visual Neuroscience (2004), 21, 275 282. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2004 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238004 $16.00 DOI: 10.10170S0952523804213426 Chromatic adaptation, perceived location, and color

More information

Motion energy versus position tracking: spatial, temporal, and chromatic parameters

Motion energy versus position tracking: spatial, temporal, and chromatic parameters Vision Research 40 (2000) 3613 3635 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Motion energy versus position tracking: spatial, temporal, and chromatic parameters Qasim Zaidi a,*, Jeremy S. DeBonet b a SUNY College

More information

Lateral interactions in visual perception of temporal signals: cortical and subcortical components

Lateral interactions in visual perception of temporal signals: cortical and subcortical components PSYCHOLOGY NEUROSCIENCE Psychology & Neuroscience, 2011, 4, 1, 57-65 DOI: 10.3922/j.psns.2011.1.007 Lateral interactions in visual perception of temporal signals: cortical and subcortical components Claudio

More information

Motion parallel to line orientation: Disambiguation of motion percepts

Motion parallel to line orientation: Disambiguation of motion percepts ion, 1999, volume 28, pages 1243 ^ 1255 DOI:.68/p298 Motion parallel to line orientation: Disambiguation of motion percepts Gregory Francis, Hyungjun Kim Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University,

More information

Visual Motion Detection

Visual Motion Detection MS number 1975 Visual Motion Detection B K Dellen, University of Goettingen, Goettingen, Germany R Wessel, Washington University, St Louis, MO, USA Contact information: Babette Dellen, Bernstein Center

More information

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit Vision Research 45 (2005) 1877 1884 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit Mark Edwards *, John A. Greenwood School of Psychology, Australian National

More information

Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 8 November 1972)

Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 8 November 1972) J. Physiol. (1973), 232, pp. 149-162 149 With 8 text-figures Printed in Great Britain PSYCHOPHYSICAL EVIDENCE FOR SUSTAINED AND TRANSIENT DETECTORS IN HUMAN VISION BY J. J. KULIKOWSKI AND D. J. TOLHURST*

More information

A model of encoding and decoding in V1 and MT accounts for motion perception anisotropies in the human visual system

A model of encoding and decoding in V1 and MT accounts for motion perception anisotropies in the human visual system B R A I N R E S E A R C H 1 2 9 9 ( 2 0 0 9 ) 3 1 6 a v a i l a b l e a t w w w. s c i e n c e d i r e c t. c o m w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / b r a i n r e s Research Report A model of

More information

Report. Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1

Report. Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1 Current Biology 19, 1356 1361, August 25, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.059 Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1 Report Jason Fischer

More information

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15 s PSY 200 Greg Francis Lecture 06 How do you recognize your grandmother? Action potential With enough excitatory input, a cell produces an action potential that sends a signal down its axon to other cells

More information

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA) Comments Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA) Is phase locking to transposed stimuli as good as phase locking to low-frequency

More information

The Precision of Single Neuron Responses in Cortical Area V1 during Stereoscopic Depth Judgments

The Precision of Single Neuron Responses in Cortical Area V1 during Stereoscopic Depth Judgments The Journal of Neuroscience, May 1, 2000, 20(9):3387 3400 The Precision of Single Neuron Responses in Cortical Area V1 during Stereoscopic Depth Judgments Simon J. D. Prince, Andrew D. Pointon, Bruce G.

More information

Spatiotemporal separability in the human cortical response to visual motion speed: a magnetoencephalography study

Spatiotemporal separability in the human cortical response to visual motion speed: a magnetoencephalography study Neuroscience Research 47 (2003) 109/116 www.elsevier.com/locate/neures Spatiotemporal separability in the human cortical response to visual motion speed: a magnetoencephalography study Lihong Wang, Yoshiki

More information

Stable Receptive Field Structure of Color Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex under Adapting and Non-adapting Conditions

Stable Receptive Field Structure of Color Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex under Adapting and Non-adapting Conditions Stable Receptive Field Structure of Color Neurons in Primary Visual Cortex under Adapting and Non-adapting Conditions Bevil R. Conway, Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston MA 02115.

More information

Effects of spatial attention and salience cues on chromatic and achromatic motion processing

Effects of spatial attention and salience cues on chromatic and achromatic motion processing Vision Research 47 (07) 1893 1906 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Effects of spatial attention and salience cues on chromatic and achromatic motion processing Karen R. Dobkins a, *, Amy A. Rezec a, Bart

More information

Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd

Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd Yong Gu, Dora E Angelaki,3 & Gregory C DeAngelis 3 Human observers combine multiple sensory cues synergistically to achieve greater perceptual

More information

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3 eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3 retinal specialization fovea: highest density of photoreceptors, aimed at where you are looking -> highest acuity optic disk: cell-free area, where

More information

Pre-Attentive Visual Selection

Pre-Attentive Visual Selection Pre-Attentive Visual Selection Li Zhaoping a, Peter Dayan b a University College London, Dept. of Psychology, UK b University College London, Gatsby Computational Neuroscience Unit, UK Correspondence to

More information

Fundamentals of psychophysics

Fundamentals of psychophysics Fundamentals of psychophysics Steven Dakin UCL Institute of Ophthalmology Introduction To understand a system as complex as the brain, one must understand not only its components (i.e. physiology) and

More information

Neural response time integration subserves. perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s. reduced model

Neural response time integration subserves. perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s. reduced model Neural response time integration subserves perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s reduced model Chris Ayers and Narayanan Krishnamurthy December 15, 2008 Abstract A neural network describing the

More information

Pursuit eye movements to second-order motion targets

Pursuit eye movements to second-order motion targets 2282 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/ Vol. 18, No. 9/ September 2001 M. J. Hawken and K. R. Gegenfurtner Pursuit eye movements to second-order motion targets Michael J. Hawken Center for Neural Science, New York University,

More information

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load Intro Examine attention response functions Compare an attention-demanding

More information

Luminance mechanisms mediate the motion of red green isoluminant gratings: the role of temporal chromatic aberration

Luminance mechanisms mediate the motion of red green isoluminant gratings: the role of temporal chromatic aberration Vision Research 43 (2003) 235 247 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Luminance mechanisms mediate the motion of red green isoluminant gratings: the role of temporal chromatic aberration Kathy T. Mullen *,

More information

The global slowdown effect: Why does perceptual grouping reduce perceived speed?

The global slowdown effect: Why does perceptual grouping reduce perceived speed? Atten Percept Psychophys (2014) 76:780 792 DOI 10.3758/s13414-013-0607-x The global slowdown effect: Why does perceptual grouping reduce perceived speed? Peter Jes Kohler & Gideon Paul Caplovitz & Peter

More information

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014 Goals for the lecture Be able to: Conceptually understand some of the analysis and jargon encountered in a typical (sensory)

More information

PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX NEURONS THAT CONTRIBUTE TO RESOLVE THE APERTURE PROBLEM

PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX NEURONS THAT CONTRIBUTE TO RESOLVE THE APERTURE PROBLEM Neuroscience 138 (26) 1397 146 PRIMARY VISUAL CORTEX NEURONS THAT CONTRIBUTE TO RESOLVE THE APERTURE PROBLEM K. GUO, a,b * R. ROBERTSON, a A. NEVADO, a M. PULGARIN, a S. MAHMOODI a AND M. P. YOUNG a a

More information

A Model of Neuronal Responses in Visual Area MT

A Model of Neuronal Responses in Visual Area MT A Model of Neuronal Responses in Visual Area MT Eero P. Simoncelli a and David J. Heeger b a Center for Neural Science 4 Washington Place, Room 809 New York, NY 10003 b Department of Psychology Stanford

More information

Contextual influences on orientation discrimination: binding local and global cues

Contextual influences on orientation discrimination: binding local and global cues Vision Research 41 (2001) 1915 1930 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Contextual influences on orientation discrimination: binding local and global cues Isabelle Mareschal *, Michael P. Sceniak 1, Robert

More information

Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk

Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk Supplementary Materials fmri response (!% BOLD) ).5 CD versus STS 1 wedge

More information

On the implementation of Visual Attention Architectures

On the implementation of Visual Attention Architectures On the implementation of Visual Attention Architectures KONSTANTINOS RAPANTZIKOS AND NICOLAS TSAPATSOULIS DEPARTMENT OF ELECTRICAL AND COMPUTER ENGINEERING NATIONAL TECHNICAL UNIVERSITY OF ATHENS 9, IROON

More information

Effects of attention on motion repulsion

Effects of attention on motion repulsion Vision Research () 139 1339 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Effects of attention on motion repulsion Yuzhi Chen a,1, Xin Meng a, Nestor Matthews b, Ning Qian a, * a Center for Neurobiology and Behavior,

More information

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity 1,2 Mark A. Elliott, 2 Zhuanghua Shi & 2,3 Fatma Sürer 1 Department of Psychology National University of Ireland Galway, Ireland.

More information

Adaptation to contingencies in macaque primary visual cortex

Adaptation to contingencies in macaque primary visual cortex Adaptation to contingencies in macaque primary visual cortex MATTO CARANDINI 1, HORAC B. BARLOW 2, LAWRNC P. O'KF 1, ALLN B. POIRSON 1, AND. ANTHONY MOVSHON 1 1 Howard Hughes Medical Institute and Center

More information

Attention enhances adaptability: evidence from motion adaptation experiments

Attention enhances adaptability: evidence from motion adaptation experiments Vision Research 44 (2004) 3035 3044 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Attention enhances adaptability: evidence from motion adaptation experiments Amy Rezec a, Bart Krekelberg b, Karen R. Dobkins a, * a Department

More information

Spectrograms (revisited)

Spectrograms (revisited) Spectrograms (revisited) We begin the lecture by reviewing the units of spectrograms, which I had only glossed over when I covered spectrograms at the end of lecture 19. We then relate the blocks of a

More information

Each neuron in the primary visual cortex (V1) is activated by

Each neuron in the primary visual cortex (V1) is activated by Dynamics of spatial summation in primary visual cortex of alert monkeys Mitesh K. Kapadia*, Gerald Westheimer*, and Charles D. Gilbert* *The Rockefeller University, New York, NY 10021; and Department of

More information

Evidence for parallel consolidation of motion direction. and orientation into visual short-term memory

Evidence for parallel consolidation of motion direction. and orientation into visual short-term memory 1 Evidence for parallel consolidation of motion direction and orientation into visual short-term memory Reuben Rideaux, Deborah Apthorp & Mark Edwards Research School of Psychology, The Australian National

More information

Ideal cue combination for localizing texture-defined edges

Ideal cue combination for localizing texture-defined edges M. S. Landy and H. Kojima Vol. 18, No. 9/September 2001/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2307 Ideal cue combination for localizing texture-defined edges Michael S. Landy and Haruyuki Kojima Department of Psychology

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Strength of Gamma Rhythm Depends on Normalization

Strength of Gamma Rhythm Depends on Normalization Strength of Gamma Rhythm Depends on Normalization The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Ray, Supratim, Amy

More information

A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND. CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica

A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND. CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND CONTINGENT AFTER-EFFECT CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica A model is advanced to explain the McCollough effect and the contingent motion after-effect.

More information

Region- and edge-based configurational effects in texture segmentation

Region- and edge-based configurational effects in texture segmentation Vision Research 47 (2007) 879 886 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Region- and edge-based configurational effects in texture segmentation Enrico Giora *, Clara Casco Department of General Psychology, University

More information

Selectivity and Spatial Distribution of Signals From the Receptive Field Surround in Macaque V1 Neurons

Selectivity and Spatial Distribution of Signals From the Receptive Field Surround in Macaque V1 Neurons J Neurophysiol 88: 2547 2556, 2002; 10.1152/jn.00693.2001. Selectivity and Spatial Distribution of Signals From the Receptive Field Surround in Macaque V1 Neurons JAMES R. CAVANAUGH, 2 WYETH BAIR, 1,2

More information

Vision Research 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 50 (2010) 1109 1116 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres A motion-energy model predicts the direction discrimination

More information

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* Perception & Psychophysics 1973. Vol. 13. So. 2.356-360 Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures* ANGEL VASSILEV+ Institu te of Physiology. Bulgarian Academy of Sciences.

More information

Perceptual Learning in the Absence of Task or Stimulus Specificity

Perceptual Learning in the Absence of Task or Stimulus Specificity Perceptual Learning in the Absence of Task or Stimulus Specificity Ben S. Webb*, Neil W. Roach, Paul V. McGraw Visual Neuroscience Group, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United

More information

A model of surround suppression through cortical feedback

A model of surround suppression through cortical feedback Neural Networks 19 (2006) 564 572 www.elsevier.com/locate/neunet A model of surround suppression through cortical feedback Thomas J. Sullivan a, *, Virginia R. de Sa b a Department of Electrical Engineering,

More information

Response Plasticity in Primary Visual Cortex and its Role in Vision and Visuomotor Behaviour: Bottom-up and Top-down Influences

Response Plasticity in Primary Visual Cortex and its Role in Vision and Visuomotor Behaviour: Bottom-up and Top-down Influences IETE Journal of Research Vol 49, No 2, March-April, 2003, pp Response Plasticity in Primary Visual Cortex and its Role in Vision and Visuomotor Behaviour: Bottom-up and Top-down Influences VALENTIN DRAGOI,

More information

The processing of feature discontinuities for different cue types in primary visual cortex

The processing of feature discontinuities for different cue types in primary visual cortex BRAIN RESEARCH 1238 (2008) 59 74 available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report The processing of feature discontinuities for different cue types in primary visual

More information

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017 V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8 Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017 Topography: mapping of objects in space onto the visual cortex contralateral representation

More information

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Ch 5. Perception and Encoding Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 2 nd Ed., M. S. Gazzaniga,, R. B. Ivry,, and G. R. Mangun,, Norton, 2002. Summarized by Y.-J. Park, M.-H. Kim, and B.-T. Zhang

More information

Spatial selectivity of the watercolor effect

Spatial selectivity of the watercolor effect Devinck et al. Vol. 31, No. 4 / April 214 / J. Opt. Soc. Am. A A1 Spatial selectivity of the watercolor effect Frédéric Devinck, 1, Peggy Gerardin, 2,3 Michel Dojat, 4, and Kenneth Knoblauch 2,3, * 1 Université

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:10.1038/nature14066 Supplementary discussion Gradual accumulation of evidence for or against different choices has been implicated in many types of decision-making, including value-based decisions

More information