EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE. RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas"

Transcription

1 The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ON MULTIPLE SCHEDULE PERFORMANCE RAFAEL BEJARANO University of Kansas The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine whether interresponse-time (IRT) shaping can produce different response rates in 2 components of a multiple schedule that are equated with respect to reinforcement rate. To this end, pigeons' key pecks were reinforced with food, if they terminated IRTs that were more extreme (shorter in 1 component and longer in the other) than 20 of the most recent 24 IRTs, and an average of 30 s had elapsed since the previous reinforcement. Large intercomponent differences in response rate occurred in all of the subjects, even though the intercomponent differences in reinforcement rate were negligible. The relevance of the present findings to prior research on IRT shaping is discussed. I n a series of experiments, Alleman and Platt (1973) showed that different response rates can be generated by differentially reinforcing progressively longer or shorter interresponse times (lrts). To this end, these investigators rank ordered a number of the most recent IRTs and reinforced those that exceeded a preselected rank-a procedure known technically as a percentile schedule of reinforcement (Galbicka, 1988; Platt, 1973). Specifically, the rank above which a given IRT had to fall to be eligible for reinforcement is given by the equation k = (m + 1)(1 - w) (1 ) where k is the rank that a given IRT had to exceed to be reinforced, m is the length of a list of the most recent IRTs that is updated after every response, and w is the probability of reinforcement per IRT. In some This research was conducted at the University of Florida and prepared for publication at the University of Kansas under the support of National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Grant No. T I thank Timothy Hackenberg for the generous use of his laboratory facilities, Chris Bullock and Teresa Foster for their assistance with data collection, and Kate Saunders and Dean Williams for their helpful comments concerning this paper. Correspondence pertaining to this manuscript should be addressed to Rafael Bejarano, Parsons Research Center, P. O. Box 738, Parsons, KS ( Bejarano@ku.edu).

2 480 BEJARANO experimental conditions, for example, m was equal to 40, and w was equal to 0.1, which means that a given I RT was reinforced if it was more extreme (shorter or longer) than 37 of the most recent 40 IRTs. Because percentile schedules control reinforcement probability, Alleman and Platt attributed their findings to the differential reinforcement of extreme IRTs (i.e., to IRT shaping). Another possibility, however, is that their findings were due to the direct relation between response rate and reinforcement rate arranged by their procedures. That is, reinforcing progressively shorter IRTs would have produced response- and reinforcement-rate increases, whereas reinforcing progressively longer I RTs would have produced response- and reinforcement-rate decreases. In their fourth experiment, for example, these investigators shaped short and long IRTs in two components of a multiple schedule (i.e., in the presence of two exteroceptive stimuli that alternated), thereby producing large response- and reinforcement-rate differences between components. Numerous studies have shown that intercomponent differences in reinforcement rate are sufficient to produce intercomponent differences in response rate (for reviews, see McSweeney, Farmer, Dougan, & Whipple, 1986; Williams, 1983), suggesting that the intercomponent differences in response rate in Alleman and Platt's fourth experiment likewise may have been due to intercomponent differences in reinforcement rate, and not to IRT shaping as they supposed. In subsequent studies, Platt and his colleagues (Galbicka & Platt, 1986; Kuch & Platt, 1976) showed that IRT shaping can produce different response rates across experimental conditions that are equated with respect to reinforcement rate (i.e., in situations where the direction of IRT shaping is reversed across blocks of sessions while reinforcement rate is kept constant). Such studies suggest that IRT shaping also should be capable of establishing different response rates in two components of a multiple scheduie that yield equal rates of reinforcement. This possibility needs to be investigated empirically, however, given the aforementioned finding that intercomponent differences in response rate vary directly with intercomponent differences in reinforcement rate. The experiment reported herein was conducted to determine whether selectively reinforcing extreme IRTs can produce different response rates in two components of a multiple schedule that are equated with respect to reinforcement rate. To this end, pigeons' key pecks were reinforced with food, if they terminated IRTs that were more extreme (shorter in one component and longer in the other) than 20 of the most recent 24 IRTs, and an average of 30 s had elapsed since the previous reinforcement (i.e., reinforcements were scheduled to occur at a rate of 2 per min in both components). If IRT shaping is sufficient to produce different rates of responding under multiple schedules, then this procedure should establish significant intercomponent differences in response rate, even though it minimizes any intercomponent differences in reinforcement rate. If different rates of reinforcement are necessary to

3 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING 481 produce differences in response rate between components, however, then the procedure used in the present study should not be capable of producing large intercomponent differences in response rate, because it minimizes any intercomponent differences in reinforcement rate. Method Subjects The subjects were 4 white Carneau pigeons (Columba livia), numbered 30, 40, 77, and 863, that were maintained at 80% of their freefeeding weights by means of postsession feeding. Before serving in this experiment, all 4 birds had been exposed briefly to basic schedules of intermittent reinforcement in a laboratory course on operant behavior. The pigeons were individually housed in a colony room, where they had continuous access to water and grit. The room was illuminated on a 16.5:7.5 hr light/dark schedule. Apparatus One standard Lehigh Valley Electronics operant conditioning chamber for pigeons (35 cm long, 35 cm high, and 30 cm wide) was used. Two response keys (2.5 cm in diameter) were located on the experimental panel cm above the grid floor. The right and left keys were 16.5 and 8.5 cm from the right and left walls, respectively. A force of approximately 0.2 N was required to operate the response keys. The right key, which was the only one used in this experiment, could be lit from behind by red and white lights. A houselight (mounted 4.4 cm above the right key) provided diffuse illumination, and a centrally located aperture (measuring 5 cm by 5.6 cm and positioned 10 cm above the grid floor) provided access to mixed grain. A ventilation fan helped mask extraneous sound. Experimental events were programmed and data were collected with MED-PC software on an IBM-compatible computer located in a different room. Procedure The procedure involved a two-component multiple schedule, each component of which was associated with a different key color (red and white). The first 24 responses that occurred during the first presentation of each color in each session were reinforced on a tandem variable-time 30-s random-ratio 5 schedule (i.e., with a 0.2 probability, once an average of 30 s had elapsed since the color's onset). All subsequent responses were reinforced according to variable-time (VT) 30-s schedules that operated in tandem with percentile schedules. The VT schedules were constructed from 15 intervals according to the procedure described by Fleshier and Hoffman (1962), and the percentile schedules were programmed according to Equation 1, with values of m and w equal to 24 and 0.2, respectively. Thus, after the first 24 responses, food was made available in the presence of each key color, if an average of 30 shad

4 482 BEJARANO elapsed since its onset, and a key peck terminated an IRT that was more extreme than 20 of the most recent 24 IRTs. Before each cycle of two components, the computer sampled a probability gate to determine which component would be presented first in that cycle (i.e., if the red-key component was presented first, the whitekey component was presented second, and vice versa). To minimize schedule interactions, all lights in the experimental chamber were darkened for 30 s between components. Each component was presented 15 times in each session, for a total of 30 reinforcements per session. Sessions were conducted 7 days a week. The experiment consisted of two conditions. In Condition 1, long IRTs were shaped in the presence of red, and short IRTs were shaped in the presence of white. In Condition 2, the reinforcement contingencies were reversed, so that short IRTs were shaped in the presence of red, and long IRTs were shaped in the presence of white. Each condition was in effect for a minimum of 30 sessions, and until no increasing or decreasing trend was evident in the mean red- and white-keycomponent IRTs from each of the most recent five sessions. Thus, responding had to be stable in both components before conditions could be terminated. Results Figure 1 shows means and standard deviations of the response and reinforcement rates (responses per minute and reinforcers per hour, respectively) in the red- and white-key components. The response-rate means and standard deviations were calculated on the basis of the individual red- and white-key rates of responding in each of the last five sessions of Conditions 1 and 2, which in turn were computed by multiplying the reciprocals of the mean red- and white-key IRTs in these sessions by 60. Likewise, the reinforcement-rate means and standard deviations were calculated on the basis of the individual red- and whitekey rates of reinforcement in each of the last five sessions of these conditions, which in turn were computed by multiplying the reciprocals of the mean interreinforcement intervals in the red- and white-key components of these sessions by The top panels of this figure show that large intercomponent differences in response rate occurred in every subject. The largest difference occurred in Subject 863 (Condition 2), whose mean response rates were (SO = 5.75) responses per min in the red-key component and (SO = 2.14) responses per min in the white-key component. The smallest difference, in contrast, occurred in Subject 40 (Condition 1), whose red- and white-key response rates averaged (SO = 10.80) and (SO = 10.07) responses per min, respectively. The bottom panels of Figure 1 show that the intercomponent differences in reinforcement rate were negligible. The largest difference occurred for Subject 77 (Condition 2), whose mean reinforcement rates

5 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING ~ 180 E160 -E ~100 IJ) c 8. IJ) a:: Condition 1 Red D White Condition "- <1) 0- IJ)... <1) e.e c '(5 a:: Subjects Figure 1. Means and standard deviations of the rates of responding (top panels) and reinforcement (bottom panels) for Subjects 30, 40, 77, and 863, calculated from the last five sessions in each condition. Filled and unfilled bars show data from the red- and white-key components, respectively. were (SO = 9.05) reinforcers per hr in the red-key component and (SO = 12.77) reinforcers per hr in the white-key component. The smallest difference occurred for Subject 863 (Condition 2), whose redand white-key reinforcement rates averaged (SO = 5.56) and (SO = 8.47) reinforcers per hr, respectively. Figures 2 through 5 show relative frequency distributions of all and reinforced IRTs (in.2-s bins) for Subjects 30, 40, 77, and 863, respectively. The left-hand panels show the red-key IRTs from the last five

6 484 BEJARANO 1 0.8~ 0.6-} Red-key components II D AIIIRTS Reinforced IRTS Subject 30 Condition 1 White-key components 0.2 Condition s I RT bins Figure 2. Relative frequency distributions of interresponse times (in.2-s bins) for Subject 30, calculated from the last five sessions in each condition. Fi lled and unfilled bars show distributions of all and reinforced interresponse times, respectively. sessions in Conditions 1 and 2, and the right-hand panels show the whitekey IRTs in the last five sessions of these conditions. The filled bars show all IRTs, and the unfilled bars show reinforced IRTs. Two characteristics of these distributions are worth noting. First, shaping short IRTs produced all-irt distributions with a highly positive

7 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING 485 Subject 40 Condition 1 Red-key components White-key components g:~~ II AJllRTs ~ Reinforced D IRTs o o 1 Conoition o s IRT bins Figure 3. Relative frequency distributions of interresponse times (in.2-s bins) for Subject 40, calculated from the last five sessions in each condition. Filled and unfilled bars show distributions of all and reinforced interresponse times, respectively. skew, whereas shaping long IRTs produced all-irt distributions that are considerably flatter in shape. This difference is most striking in Subject 863's Condition-2 IRT distributions (Figure 5, bottom panels), although it is readily discernible in every case. Second, the reinforced-irt distributions are displaced to the left of the all-irt distributions in those cases wherein short IRTs were shaped (Condition 1, white-key component; Condition 2, red-key component), and to the right of the all-irt distributions in those cases wherein long IRTs were shaped (Condition 1, red-key component;

8 486 BEJARANO Subject 77 Condition 1 1 Red-key components White-key components 0.8~ _ ~. 0.6-j, _ ALL IRTs -j,- D Reinforced IRTs C/) 0 c ::J 0- ~ ~1 o ro 0.8~ ~ 0.6-)0 2 3 o 1 Condition 2 ~ 0.2 o o s I RT bins Figure 4. Relative frequency distributions of interresponse times (in.2-s bins) for Subject 77, calculated from the last five sessions in each condition. Filled and unfilled bars show distributions of all and reinforced interresponse times, respectively. Condition 2, white-key component), indicating that the procedure selectively reinforced extreme IRTs, as intended. Table 1 lists the median, and the first and third quartiles, of the latencies to the first response in each component, calculated from the last five sessions in each condition. This table shows that reversing the directions of IRT shaping in Condition 2 (i.e., toward short IRTs in the redkey component and toward long IRTs in the white-key component) had

9 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING O.8~ 0.6-j. 0 Red-key components II D AU IRTs Reinforced IRTs Subject 863 Condition 1 ~ White-key components C/) '0 c :l 0- ~ ~ +=i n:s c:: 0 1 O.8~ o 1 Condition 2 o o 1.2-s IRT bins Figure 5. Relative frequency distributions of interresponse times (in.2-s bins) for Subject 863, calculated from the last five sessions in each condition. Filled and unfilled bars show distributions of all and reinforced interresponse times, respectively. reliable, albeit small, effects on these measures. Specifically, 3 of the 4 subjects (Subjects 30, 40, and 77) showed decreases in their red-key latencies and increases in their white-key latencies, even though reinforcement was not contingent on latency duration.

10 488 BEJARANO Table 1 Latencies to the First Response in the Red- and White-Key Components of the Multiple Schedule Subject Condition Component red white red white red white red white red white red white red white red white Note. This table shows the medians (02) and the first and third quartiles (01 and 03, respectively) from the last five sessions of each condition for each subject. Discussion The results of this experiment demonstrate that differentially reinforcing extreme IRTs can establish different response rates in two components of a multiple schedule, even though the reinforcement rates in the two components are comparable. This is evident both in Figure 1 (which shows that response rates differed considerably between components but that reinforcement rates did not) and in Figures 2 through 5 (which show large intercomponent differences in the distributions of all and reinforced IRTs). Moreover, the latencies to the first response in each component also varied systematically across conditions, further attesting to the effects of the reinforcement contingencies. These findings add to the literature on behavior shaping in several ways. First, recall the aforementioned possibility, that the intercomponent differences in response rate reported by Alleman and Platt (1973, Experiment 4) might have been due to the direct relation between response rate and reinforcement rate arranged by their procedures. Although this possibility cannot be ruled out, the present findings suggest that Alleman and Platt's results also could have been a function of the differential reinforcement of extreme IRTs, as these investigators supposed. Second, by showing that differentially reinforcing extreme IRTs can establish different response rates in two components of a multiple schedule that yield comparable reinforcement rates, the present results establish the generality of prior findings, that IRT shaping can produce

11 EFFECTS OF INTERRESPONSE-TIME SHAPING 489 large differences in response rate across experimental conditions that yield comparable rates of reinforcement (Galbicka & Platt, 1986; Kuch & Platt, 1976). This is important because, as Alleman and Platt (1973) noted, the generality of percentile reinforcement schedules would be seriously limited if they could not establish different response rates in close temporal proximity to one another. Finally, the finding that the latencies to the first response in each component varied systematically across conditions is worth noting, given that reinforcement was not contingent on latency duration. The rationale for excluding the response latencies from the reinforcement contingencies was that, by definition, they are not IRTs. That is, latency is defined as the time to the first response after stimulus onset. IRT, in contrast, is defined as the interval between the end of one response and the start of the next, although it typically is measured as the time between the start of two consecutive responses. Given the present findings, however, excluding the response latencies from the reinforcement contingencies may not be necessary, as latencies and IRTs may be controlled by the same variables. Future research might be conducted to investigate this possibility. Future studies also might examine certain variables that may determine the size of the intercomponent differences in response rate that IRT shaping can generate. Two such variables that may be worth investigating are the number of observations used to determine whether a given response is eligible for reinforcement (m in Equation 1) and reinforcement probability (w in that equation), both of which have been shown to influence IRT shaping under procedures other than multiple schedules. In particular, Alleman and Platt (1973, Experiment 2) found that between-condition reversals in the direction of IRT shaping produced changes in response rate, the size of which varied directly with m and inversely with w. This finding suggests that m and walso may determine the size of the intercomponent differences in response rate that IRT shaping can generate, although, as just noted, additional research is necessary to determine whether this is the case. References ALLEMAN, H. D., & PLATT, J. R. (1973). Differential reinforcement of interresponse times with controlled probability of reinforcement per response. Learning and Motivation, 4, FLESHLER, M., & HOFFMAN, H. S. (1962). A progression for generating variable-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, GALBICKA, G. (1988). Differentiating the behavior of organisms. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50,

12 490 BEJARANO GALBICKA, G., & PLATT, J. R. (1986). Parametric manipulation of interresponsetime contingency independent of reinforcement rate. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, KUCH, D. O., & PLATT, J. R. (1976). Reinforcement rate and interresponse time differentiation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, MCSWEENEY, F. K., FARMER, V. A., DOUGAN, J. D., & WHIPPLE, J. E. (1986). The generalized matching law as a description of multiple-schedule responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, PLATT, J. R. (1973). Percentile reinforcement: Paradigms for experimental analysis of response shaping. In H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 7, pp ). New York: Academic Press. WILLIAMS, B. A. (1983). Another look at contrast in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 39,

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES By CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

More information

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W.

STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE ORDER TERRY W. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 319 331 NUMBER 3(MAY) STUDIES OF WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: PARAMETERS OF HERRNSTEIN S (1970) RESPONSE-STRENGTH EQUATION VARY WITH SCHEDULE

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2

SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 217, 17, 123 135 NUMBER 1 (JANUARY) SUBSTITUTION EFFECTS IN A GENERALIZED TOKEN ECONOMY WITH PIGEONS LEONARDO F. ANDRADE 1 AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG 2 1

More information

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Brian D. Kangas, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

More information

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2015, 103, 269 287 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) THE SEVERAL ROLES OF STIMULI IN TOKEN REINFORCEMENT CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA AND TIMOTHY D. HACKENBERG

More information

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 24, 191-197 NUMBER 2 (SEPTEMBER) PERFORMANCE IN CONCURRENT INTERVAL SCHEDULES: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION' BRENDA LOBB AND M. C. DAVISON UNIVERSITY

More information

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length

The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length Animal Learning & Behavior 1994, 22 (3), 252-266 The generality of within-session patterns of responding: Rate of reinforcement and session length FRANCES K. MCSWEENEY, JOHN M. ROLL, and CARI B. CANNON

More information

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 551-559 NUMBER 4 (JULY) PROBABILITY OF REINFORCEMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF STIMULUS CONTROL' CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA Pigeons

More information

between successive DMTS choice phases.

between successive DMTS choice phases. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1996, 66, 231 242 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) SEPARATING THE EFFECTS OF TRIAL-SPECIFIC AND AVERAGE SAMPLE-STIMULUS DURATION IN DELAYED MATCHING TO SAMPLE IN PIGEONS

More information

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1997, 4 (1), 113-117 Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer JANICE E. WEAVER University of Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky JANICE N. STEIRN

More information

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 00, 79, 6 NUMBER (MARCH) RESPONDING FOR SUCROSE AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: EFFECTS OF SUCROSE CONCENTRATION AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCER DURATION

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 8-2012 Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Meredith

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 89 (2012) 212 218 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes j o ur nal homep age : www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Providing a reinforcement history

More information

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY

UNIVERSITY OF WALES SWANSEA AND WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 05, 3, 3 45 NUMBER (JANUARY) WITHIN-SUBJECT TESTING OF THE SIGNALED-REINFORCEMENT EFFECT ON OPERANT RESPONDING AS MEASURED BY RESPONSE RATE AND RESISTANCE

More information

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement

Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement Learning & Behavior 2005, 33 (3), 309-323 Remembering: The role of extraneous reinforcement GLENN S. BROWN and K. GEOFFREY WHITE University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand In two experiments, pigeons responding

More information

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues

Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues Animal Learning & Behavior 1995, 23 (3), 273-279 Pigeons transfer between conditional discriminations with differential outcomes in the absence of differential-sample-responding cues LOU M. SHERBURNE and

More information

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE?

DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? The Psychological Record, 2004, 54, 319-332 DOES THE TEMPORAL PLACEMENT OF FOOD-PELLET REINFORCEMENT ALTER INDUCTION WHEN RATS RESPOND ON A THREE-COMPONENT MULTIPLE SCHEDULE? JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, KELSEY

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N.

PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND PHILIP N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2000, 73, 93 102 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) PIGEONS CHOICES BETWEEN FIXED-RATIO AND LINEAR OR GEOMETRIC ESCALATING SCHEDULES PAUL NEUMAN, WILLIAM H. AHEARN, AND

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1)

PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE V /V (A /A )(D /D ), (1) JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 23, 79, 233 242 NUMBER 2(MARCH) PREFERENCE REVERSALS WITH FOOD AND WATER REINFORCERS IN RATS LEONARD GREEN AND SARA J. ESTLE WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY Rats

More information

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT

ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2003, 80, 43 58 NUMBER 1(JULY) ECONOMIC AND BIOLOGICAL INFLUENCES ON KEY PECKING AND TREADLE PRESSING IN PIGEONS LEONARD GREEN AND DANIEL D. HOLT WASHINGTON

More information

AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS. Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS. Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of AN EXPLORATION OF THE TITRATING DELAY MATCH TO SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS Jonathan E. Friedel, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December 2011 APPROVED:

More information

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem

Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Illinois Wesleyan University Digital Commons @ IWU Honors Projects Psychology 2000 Behavioral Contrast: A New Solution to an Old Problem Sara J. Estle '00 Illinois Wesleyan University Recommended Citation

More information

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Learning and Motivation 39 (2008) 71 94 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q Luís Oliveira,

More information

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight

Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (3), 264-270 Concurrent schedule responding as a function ofbody weight FRANCES K. McSWEENEY Washington State University, Pullman, Washington 99163 Five pigeons

More information

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses & Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Summer 2005 Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli William

More information

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A.

EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING. Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. EFFECTS OF A LIMITED HOLD ON PIGEONS MATCH-TO-SAMPLE PERFORMANCE UNDER FIXED-RATIO SCHEDULING Joseph Leland Cermak, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December

More information

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON

UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA CHAPEL HILL AND UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA WILMINGTON JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2005, 83, 281 296 NUMBER 3(MAY) MORPHINE TOLERANCE AS A FUNCTION OF RATIO SCHEDULE: RESPONSE REQUIREMENT OR UNIT PRICE? CHRISTINE E. HUGHES, STACEY C. SIGMON,

More information

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons

Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons Learning & Behavior 2004, 32 (4), 477-490 Processing of empty and filled time intervals in pigeons DOUGLAS S. GRANT and DIANE C. TALARICO University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Pigeons were trained

More information

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY Volume 6, Number 2 Submitted: September 8, 2000 Resubmitted: January 22, 2001 Accepted: January 23, 2001 Publication date: January 26, 2001 THE EFFICACY OF REINFORCEMENT

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN

RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES P. KOHN JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1998, 70, 165 183 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) RESPONSE PERSISTENCE UNDER RATIO AND INTERVAL REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES KENNON A. LATTAL, MARK P. REILLY, AND JAMES

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task

Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task Analysis of Gambling Behavior Volume 11 Article 2 2017 Pigeons Choose to Gamble in a Categorical Discrimination Task Nathaniel C. Rice 1. United States Army Medical Research Institute of Chemical Defense,

More information

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE

RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE RESPONSE-INDEPENDENT CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN AN OBSERVING PROCEDURE By ANTHONY L. DEFULIO A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE

More information

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS. LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee The Psychological Record, 1997,47,145-166 THE EFFECTS OF TERMINAL-LINK STIMULUS ARRANGEMENTS ON PREFERENCE IN CONCURRENT CHAINS LAUREL COLTON and JAY MOORE University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Pigeons served

More information

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS'

CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 261-268 NUMBER 2 (MARCH) CONCURRENT SCHEULES OF PRIMARY AN CONITIONE REINFORCEMENT IN RATS' ONAL W. ZIMMERMAN CARLETON UNIVERSITY Rats responded

More information

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of

CONCURRENT CHAINS UNIVERSITY-IMPERIAL VALLEY CAMPUS. links differ with respect to the percentage of JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1987, 47, 57-72 CHOICE BETWEEN RELIABLE AND UNRELIABLE OUTCOMES: MIXED PERCENTAGE-REINFORCEMENT IN CONCURRENT CHAINS MARCIA L. SPETCH AND ROGER DUNN DALHOUSIE

More information

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, 152 162 2329-8456/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000053 Memory Systems

More information

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION

INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION INTERACTIONS AMONG UNIT PRICE, FIXED-RATIO VALUE, AND DOSING REGIMEN IN DETERMINING EFFECTS OF REPEATED COCAINE ADMINISTRATION By JIN HO YOON A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY

More information

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives

Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(4),621-629 Oddity learning in the pigeon: Effect of negative instances, correction, and number of incorrect alternatives THOMAS R. ZENTALL University ofkentucky, Lexington,

More information

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action

Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Behavioural Processes 66 (2004) 213 233 Quantitative analyses of methamphetamine s effects on self-control choices: implications for elucidating behavioral mechanisms of drug action Raymond C. Pitts, Stacy

More information

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice

Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio on Pigeons' Choice Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 12-1991 Sequences of Fixed-Ratio Schedules of Reinforcement: The Effect of Ratio Size in the Second and Third Fixed-Ratio

More information

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination

Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Learning and Motivation 35 (2004) 348 370 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Pigeons' memory for number of events: EVects of intertrial interval and delay interval illumination Chris Hope and Angelo Santi Wilfrid

More information

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a

CRF or an Fl 5 min schedule. They found no. of S presentation. Although more responses. might occur under an Fl 5 min than under a JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR VOLUME 5, NUMBF- 4 OCITOBER, 1 962 THE EFECT OF TWO SCHEDULES OF PRIMARY AND CONDITIONED REINFORCEMENT JOAN G. STEVENSON1 AND T. W. REESE MOUNT HOLYOKE

More information

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT'

SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR SECOND-ORDER SCHEDULES: BRIEF SHOCK AT THE COMPLETION OF EACH COMPONENT' D. ALAN STUBBS AND PHILIP J. SILVERMAN UNIVERSITY OF MAINE, ORONO AND WORCESTER

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

UNIVERSITY OF IOWA AND SOUTHERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY AT CARBONDALE

UNIVERSITY OF IOWA AND SOUTHERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY AT CARBONDALE JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2002, 78, 365 373 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) BRIEF PRESENTATIONS ARE SUFFICIENT FOR PIGEONS TO DISCRIMINATE ARRAYS OF SAME AND DIFFERENT STIMULI EDWARD A. WASSERMAN,

More information

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus.

DISCRIMINATION IN RATS OSAKA CITY UNIVERSITY. to emit the response in question. Within this. in the way of presenting the enabling stimulus. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR EFFECTS OF DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT PROCEDURES ON THE ACQUISITION AND MAINTENANCE OF SUCCESSIVE DISCRIMINATION IN RATS SHIN HACHIYA AND MASATO ITO

More information

TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND MARC N.

TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND MARC N. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2008, 90, 207 218 NUMBER 2(SEPTEMBER) TOLERANCE TO EFFECTS OF COCAINE ON BEHAVIOR UNDER A RESPONSE-INITIATED FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULE MATTHEW T. WEAVER AND

More information

THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL

THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2012, 97, 85 100 NUMBER 1(JANUARY) THE SUNK COST EFFECT WITH PIGEONS: SOME DETERMINANTS OF DECISIONS ABOUT PERSISTENCE ANNE C. MACASKILL UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

More information

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS. Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of OBSERVING AND ATTENDING IN A DELAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PREPARATION IN PIGEONS Bryan S. Lovelace, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS December 2008 APPROVED:

More information

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 74-84 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.74 Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure PATRICK VAN ROOYEN AND ANGELO

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, &

STEPHEN P. KRAMER. (Kojima, 1980; Lattal, 1975; Maki, Moe, & JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR1 1982, 38, 71-85 NUMBER I (JULY) MEMORY FOR RECENT BEHAVIOR IN THE PIGEON STEPHEN P. KRAMER CORRECTIONS DIVISION, DRAPER, UTAH Variations of the symbolic

More information

REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ATTENUATES APOMORPHINE S EFFECTS ON OPERANT PECKING JONATHAN W. PINKSTON AND R. J. LAMB

REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ATTENUATES APOMORPHINE S EFFECTS ON OPERANT PECKING JONATHAN W. PINKSTON AND R. J. LAMB JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2012, 98, 273 282 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ATTENUATES APOMORPHINE S EFFECTS ON OPERANT PECKING JONATHAN W. PINKSTON AND R. J. LAMB UNIVERSITY

More information

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1975, Vol. 3 (2), 85-89 Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons CHARLES R. LEITH and WILLIAM S. MAKI, JR. University ofcalifornia, Berkeley, California

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers in Behavior and Biological Sciences Papers in the Biological Sciences 1969 Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning

More information

Transfer Across Delayed Discriminations: II. Differences in the Substitutability of Initial Versus Test Stimuli

Transfer Across Delayed Discriminations: II. Differences in the Substitutability of Initial Versus Test Stimuli Joulmal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/98/$3.00 1998, VoL 24, No. 1, 47-59 Transfer Across Delayed Discriminations:

More information

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination

Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional Discrimination Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2008 Testing the Functional Equivalence of Retention Intervals and Sample-Stimulus Disparity in Conditional

More information

Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed Through Partial Versus Total Reversals of Many-to-One Conditional and Simple Discriminations

Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed Through Partial Versus Total Reversals of Many-to-One Conditional and Simple Discriminations Journal of Experimental Psycholo Animal Behavior Processes 1991, Vol. 17, No. 2, 194-201 Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/91/43.00 Common Coding in Pigeons Assessed

More information

SEQUENTIAL DEPENDENCIES IN INTERRESPONSE TIMES AND REINFORCEMENT/RESPONSE-COST. KENTARO KAWASHIMA Waseda University, Tokyo

SEQUENTIAL DEPENDENCIES IN INTERRESPONSE TIMES AND REINFORCEMENT/RESPONSE-COST. KENTARO KAWASHIMA Waseda University, Tokyo The Psychological Record, 2003, 53, 67-84 SEQUENTIAL DEPENDENCIES IN INTERRESPONSE TIMES AND REINFORCEMENT/RESPONSE-COST KENTARO KAWASHIMA Waseda University, Tokyo This study investigates sequential dependencies

More information

Evidence for a Magnitude Effect in Temporal Discounting With Pigeons

Evidence for a Magnitude Effect in Temporal Discounting With Pigeons Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2012, Vol. 38, No. 1, 102 108 2012 American Psychological Association 0097-7403/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0026345 BRIEF REPORT Evidence for a

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Learn Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 February 26. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Learn Behav. 2009 November ; 37(4): 357 364. doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.357. Behavioral Momentum and Relapse of Extinguished Operant Responding

More information

THE EFFECT OF DISPLAY TIMING ON CHANGE BLINDNESS IN PIGEONS (COLUMBA LIVIA) WALTER T. HERBRANSON AND EVA T. DAVIS

THE EFFECT OF DISPLAY TIMING ON CHANGE BLINDNESS IN PIGEONS (COLUMBA LIVIA) WALTER T. HERBRANSON AND EVA T. DAVIS JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2016, 105, 85 99 511 NUMBER 1 (JANUARY) THE EFFECT OF DISPLAY TIMING ON CHANGE BLINDNESS IN PIGEONS (COLUMBA LIVIA) WALTER T. HERBRANSON AND EVA T. DAVIS

More information

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences

Resistance to Change Within Heterogeneous Response Sequences Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2009, Vol. 35, No. 3, 293 311 2009 American Psychological Association 0097-7403/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013926 Resistance to Change Within

More information

Variation in Herrnsein s r<sub>0</sub> as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate

Variation in Herrnsein s r<sub>0</sub> as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate Illinois Wesleyan University From the SelectedWorks of James Dougan 1985 Variation in Herrnsein s r0 as a Function of Alternative Reinforcement Rate James Dougan, Illinois Wesleyan University

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data

Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 2004, 36 (4), 661 669 Measures of temporal discrimination in fixedinterval performance: A case study in archiving data PAULO GUILHARDI and RUSSELL M.

More information

J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY. The relative inability of the usual differential. to ask whether performance under DRL schedules

J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY. The relative inability of the usual differential. to ask whether performance under DRL schedules JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 27-38 NUMBER I (JANUARY) THE EFFECT OF INFORMATIVE FEEDBACK ON TEMPORAL TRACKING IN THE PIGEON' J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY Pigeons emitted

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/319/5871/1849/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Rule Learning by Rats Robin A. Murphy,* Esther Mondragón, Victoria A. Murphy This PDF file includes: *To whom correspondence

More information

Instrumental Conditioning I

Instrumental Conditioning I Instrumental Conditioning I Basic Procedures and Processes Instrumental or Operant Conditioning? These terms both refer to learned changes in behavior that occur as a result of the consequences of the

More information

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction

Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 3-2016 Experience with Dynamic Reinforcement Rates Decreases Resistance to Extinction Andrew R. Craig Utah State University

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2005 November 14. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2005 April ; 31(2): 213 225. Timing in Choice Experiments Jeremie Jozefowiez and Daniel T. Cerutti

More information

Confounding variables in the discriminated Irt procedure.

Confounding variables in the discriminated Irt procedure. University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 1983 Confounding variables in the discriminated Irt procedure. David C. Palmer University of Massachusetts

More information

Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules

Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules Animal Learning & Behavior 1979, 7 (2), 203-210 Stimulus control of behavior during the postreinforcement pause of FI schedules NANCY K. INNIS University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada

More information

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior.

IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA. because no special deprivation or home-cage. of other independent variables on operant behavior. JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR TECHNIQUES FOR ESTABLISHING SCHEDULES WITH WHEEL RUNNING AS REINFORCEMENT IN RATS IVER H. IVERSEN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA 1993, 60, 219-238 NUMBER 1

More information

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats

Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2010, 11, 79-91 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2010) 79 Travel Distance and Stimulus Duration on Observing Responses by Rats Rogelio Escobar National Autonomous University of Mexico

More information

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's

MCMASTER UNIVERSITY. more values independently of food delivery. In any case, the well known result of Skinner's JOURNAt, OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1968, 1 1, 1-8 NUMBER I (JANUARY) A UTO-SHAPING OF THE PIGEON'S KEY-PECK' PAUL L. BROWN AND HERBERT M. JENKINS MCMASTER UNIVERSITY Reliable acquistion

More information

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1965, Vol. 59, No. 1, 13-17 AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 GORDON BOWER, RONALD STARR, AND LEAH LAZAROVITZ Stanford

More information

Coding of feature and no-feature events by pigeons performing a delayed conditional discrimination

Coding of feature and no-feature events by pigeons performing a delayed conditional discrimination Animal Learning & Behavior 1993, 21(2), 92-1 Coding of feature and no-feature events by pigeons performing a delayed conditional discrimination LOU M. SHERBURNE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University of Kentucky,

More information

Timing with opportunity cost: Concurrent schedules of reinforcement improve peak timing

Timing with opportunity cost: Concurrent schedules of reinforcement improve peak timing Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (3), 217-229 doi:10.3758/lb.37.3.217 Timing with opportunity cost: Concurrent schedules of reinforcement improve peak timing FEDERICO SANABRIA, ERIC A. THRAILKILL, AND PETER

More information

SERIAL CONDITIONING AS A FUNCTION OF STIMULUS, RESPONSE, AND TEMPORAL DEPENDENCIES

SERIAL CONDITIONING AS A FUNCTION OF STIMULUS, RESPONSE, AND TEMPORAL DEPENDENCIES University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of January 199 SERIAL CONDITIONING AS A FUNCTION OF

More information

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL'

PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1973, 2, 393-43 NUMBER 3 (NOVEMBER) PREFERENCE FOR FIXED-INTERVAL SCHEDULES: AN ALTERNATIVE MODEL' M. C. DAVISON AND W. TEMPLE UNIVERSITY OF AUCKLAND AND

More information

Some effects of reinforcer availability on the pigeon's responding in 24-hour sessions

Some effects of reinforcer availability on the pigeon's responding in 24-hour sessions Animal Learning & Behavior 1981,9 (3), 4lJ.424 Some effects of reinforcer availability on the pigeon's responding in 24-hour sessions GARY A. LUCAS Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana 47405 Restrictions

More information

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon

Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (1986) 38B, 81-95 Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon P. J. Durlach and N. J. Mackintosh Department of Experimental Psychology, University

More information

Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers

Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 5-2008 Preference, Resistance to Change, and Qualitatively Different Reinforcers Christopher Aaron Podlesnik

More information

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a

The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a Effects of Food Deprivation on Memory Performance. Jacob. L. Kerewaro Abstract The effects of two different states of food deprivation for 6 roosters was measured with a delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)

More information

Schedules of Reinforcement

Schedules of Reinforcement Schedules of Reinforcement MACE, PRATT, ZANGRILLO & STEEGE (2011) FISHER, PIAZZA & ROANE CH 4 Rules that describe how will be reinforced are 1. Every response gets SR+ ( ) vs where each response gets 0

More information

EVALUATIONS OF DELAYED REINFORCEMENT IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES

EVALUATIONS OF DELAYED REINFORCEMENT IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES EVALUATIONS OF DELAYED REINFORCEMENT IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES By JOLENE RACHEL SY A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

More information

AUTOSHAPING OF KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS

AUTOSHAPING OF KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1969, 12, 521-531 NUMBER 4 (JULY) AUTOSHAPING OF KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS WITH NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT' HOWARD RACHLIN HARVARD UNIVERSITY Pigeons exposed to

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research

REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS. Presented to. The Faculty of the Division of Graduate. Studies and Research TWO-KEY CONCURRENT RESPONDING: CHOICE AND DELAYS OF REINFORCEMENT AT CONSTANT RELATIVE IMMEDIACY OF REINFORCEMENT A THESIS Presented to The Faculty of the Division of Graduate Studies and Research By George

More information

Learning and Motivation

Learning and Motivation Learning and Motivation 4 (29) 178 18 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Learning and Motivation journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Context blocking in rat autoshaping: Sign-tracking

More information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information

Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information Animal Learning & Behavior 1978,6 (4),380-384 Copyright 1978 by The Psychonornic Society, nc. Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information BRUCE A. WALD Utah State University, Logan,

More information