Phasic Alertness and Residual Switch Costs in Task Switching

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Phasic Alertness and Residual Switch Costs in Task Switching"

Transcription

1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2017, Vol. 43, No. 2, American Psychological Association /17/$ Phasic ness and Residual Switch osts in Task Switching Darryl W. Schneider Purdue University Residual switch costs are deficits in task-switching performance that occur despite considerable time to prepare for a task switch. In the present study, the author investigated whether increased phasic alertness modulates residual switch costs. In 2 experiments involving the task-cuing procedure, subjects performed numerical categorization tasks on target digits, with and without an alerting stimulus presented shortly before the target (alert and no-alert trials, respectively). Switch costs were obtained that decreased with a longer cue target interval, indicating subjects engaged in preparation, but large residual switch costs remained. ing effects were obtained in the form of faster overall performance on alert than on no-alert trials, indicating the alerting stimuli increased phasic alertness. ritically, residual switch costs were similar on alert and no-alert trials in both experiments, unaffected by manipulations of alert type, alert availability, and alert target interval. Implications of the results for understanding the relationship between phasic alertness and cognitive control in task switching are discussed. Public Significance Statement People are slower and more error-prone at switching than repeating tasks, even when they are provided with time to prepare for a task switch. In the present study, the author investigated whether the cost of task switching is influenced by temporary changes in alertness. In 2 experiments, college students completed numerical categorization tasks faster when alerting stimuli were presented than when they were not, indicating that simple alerts can improve general task performance. However, large costs of switching tasks still occurred and were unaffected by the meaning, availability, and timing of the alerts. These findings suggest that preparation and control associated with switching tasks are not influenced by a person s state of alertness, clarifying the role of attention in task switching. Keywords: task switching, residual switch cost, preparation, phasic alertness One of the reasons why the topic of task switching has attracted interest in cognitive psychology is that it provides insight regarding preparatory aspects of cognitive control (for reviews, see Kiesel et al., 2010; Vandierendonck, Liefooghe, & Verbruggen, 2010). For example, consider the task-cuing procedure, in which a cue indicates which of two tasks to perform on a target on each trial (for an overview, see Meiran, 2014). By presenting the cue before the target and manipulating the cue target interval (), one varies the time available to prepare for the cued task. A common finding is that switch costs worse performance for task switches than for task repetitions decrease as the gets longer (e.g., Meiran, 1996; Monsell & Mizon, 2006; Schneider & Logan, 2011), indicating that preparatory processing occurred during the. However, there seems to be a limit to this preparation because switch costs sometimes persist despite s of 1,000 ms or longer (e.g., Longman, Lavric, Munteanu, & Monsell, 2014; Meiran, This article was published Online First November 10, I thank Allison Blake, Megan Boston, Jamie olton, Natalie Harpenau, Bailey Masterson, and hasity Ricker for assistance with data collection. orrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Darryl W. Schneider, Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University, 703 Third Street, West Lafayette, IN dws@purdue.edu 1996; Meiran, horev, & Sapir, 2000; Schneider, 2016). These residual switch costs are the focus of the present study. Efforts to reduce or eliminate residual switch costs have been based primarily on De Jong s (2000) failure-to-engage theory. The central premise of the theory is that subjects occasionally fail to engage in preparation, even when considerable time is provided for it. When these failures happen, control processing involved in switching tasks must occur after target onset, resulting in residual switch costs. In support of his theory, De Jong showed that response time (RT) distributions for task-switch trials at long preparation intervals can be modeled as mixtures of prepared and unprepared states (for revised mixture models, see Brown, Lehmann, & Poboka, 2006; Poboka, Karayanidis, & Heathcote, 2014). If residual switch costs arise from failures to engage in preparation, then they might be attenuated by inducing subjects to prepare more often. Nieuwenhuis and Monsell (2002) found that incentivizing preparation via instructions and payoffs had only a modest effect on the estimated probability of preparation and still produced large residual switch costs. Lien, Ruthruff, Remington, and Johnston (2005) found that encouraging preparation by imposing strict time deadlines still resulted in residual switch costs, although not for all stimulus response pairs (but see Lindsen & De Jong, 2010). Verbruggen, Liefooghe, Vandierendonck, and Demanet (2007) found that briefly presenting the cue yielded smaller 317

2 318 SHNEIDER (and nonsignificant) residual switch costs relative to having the cue available for the full and after target onset, but the robustness of that finding is questionable (see Schneider, 2016). On the basis of these studies, it is unclear what can be done to effectively reduce residual switch costs. One possibility that has received limited investigation concerns increasing phasic alertness, a component of attention that reflects rapid but temporary changes in sensitivity to external stimulation (Posner, 1978, 2008; Posner & Boies, 1971). Phasic alertness is usually increased by presenting an alerting stimulus shortly before the target on which a task is to be performed. ing effects are indicated by shorter RTs on alert trials than on no-alert trials. For example, when subjects classify the direction of a target arrow appearing above or below fixation in the Attention Network Test (ANT; Fan, Mcandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002), RTs are typically shorter when an alerting stimulus (e.g., a pair of asterisks) is presented 500 ms before the target than when no alerting stimulus is presented. Phasic alertness has been investigated with multiple tasks hypothesized to involve cognitive control, such as the flanker (Fan et al., 2002; Redick & Engle, 2006), Simon (Böckler, Alpay, & Stürmer, 2011; Fischer, Plessow, & Kiesel, 2010), and Stroop (Weinbach & Henik, 2012) tasks. ing effects on overall RTs are commonly observed, but a counterintuitive finding is that interference effects attributed to cognitive control are often larger on alert trials than on no-alert trials (for overviews, see MacLeod et al., 2010; Nieuwenhuis & De Kleijn, 2013). The interpretation of this finding continues to be debated, with proposed explanations based on inhibition of cognitive control (allejas, Lupiáñez, & Tudela, 2004), activation of established stimulus response associations (Fischer, Plessow, & Kiesel, 2012), and early onset of evidence accumulation (Nieuwenhuis & De Kleijn, 2013). Drawing an analogy from the alerting literature to task switching, one might predict larger residual switch costs on alert than on no-alert trials, especially if residual switch costs reflect interference, as suggested by some authors (e.g., Allport, Styles, & Hsieh, 1994; Meiran, 2000; for discussion, see Vandierendonck et al., 2010). Alternatively, if residual switch costs reflect failures to engage in preparation (De Jong, 2000), then an alerting stimulus might trigger preparatory processing, leading to the prediction of smaller residual switch costs on alert than on no-alert trials. Thus, there are competing predictions regarding the relationship between phasic alertness and residual switch costs. ing effects on task-switching performance have been investigated in few studies. Rogers and Monsell (1995, Experiment 5) examined performance in the alternating-runs procedure, in which task switching was predictable because targets were presented in successive spatial locations associated with tasks. On no-alert trials, the target was presented after a 1,000-ms intertrial interval from the previous response. On alert trials, an alerting stimulus (a rectangle at the location of the next target) was presented 500 ms before the target and remained visible after target onset. Rogers and Monsell found significant alerting benefits and costs for overall RTs and error rates, respectively, but nonsignificant differences in switch costs between alert and no-alert trials. They concluded that increased phasic alertness induced a speed accuracy tradeoff that did not affect task switching. Meiran et al. (2000, Experiment 4) examined performance in the task-cuing procedure, in which subjects classified the up down or left right position of a target appearing in a 2 2 grid based on arrow cues. On no-alert trials, the target was presented after one of four s ranging from 166 to 1,182 ms. On alert trials, an alerting stimulus (a highlighted grid) was presented before the cue at one of four alert cue intervals (AIs) ranging from 132 to 1,082 ms, then the target was presented after a of 166 ms. Meiran et al. found that switch costs decreased with on no-alert trials, indicating subjects engaged in preparation. They also found a significant alerting benefit for overall RTs when the three longest AIs were compared with the shortest AI, but switch costs did not vary significantly across AIs. They concluded that the control processing involved in preparing for a task switch (yielding reductions in switch costs with ) is not a byproduct of phasic alertness. Meiran and horev (2005) examined performance in the taskcuing procedure using the same spatial tasks as did Meiran et al. (2000), except all trials involved alerting stimuli. In their Experiments 1 3, the alerting stimulus (a double-lined grid or an outer frame surrounding the entire stimulus display) was presented before the target at one of four alert target intervals (s) ranging from 66 to 1,032 ms. The two s used in each experiment were always relatively long (1,800 and 2,800 ms in Experiments 1 and 3; 1,800 and 10,000 ms in Experiment 2). In their Experiment 4, the alerting stimulus (a double-lined grid) was presented before the cue at one of four AIs ranging from 116 to 916 ms, then the target was presented after a of either 116 or 816 ms. Meiran and horev found significant alerting benefits for overall RTs when the two or three longest s or AIs were compared with the shortest intervals. They also found that residual switch costs (verified to be residual in Experiments 1 3 because of nonsignificant differences in switch costs across the long s) were significantly smaller at the two longest s or AIs compared with the shortest intervals in all four experiments. They concluded that increased phasic alertness reduces residual switch costs by strengthening task-goal representations. These previous studies do not paint a clear picture of the relationship between phasic alertness and task switching. On the one hand, all the studies consistently showed alerting benefits on overall RTs, indicating subjects processed the alerting stimuli and attained a heightened state of alertness. On the other hand, two studies showed no alerting effects on switch costs (Meiran et al., 2000; Rogers & Monsell, 1995), one study showed smaller residual switch costs on alert trials associated with more optimal alerting intervals (Meiran & horev, 2005), and no studies showed larger residual switch costs on alert than on no-alert trials the pattern seen for interference effects in the alerting literature (MacLeod et al., 2010; Nieuwenhuis & De Kleijn, 2013). However, previous studies were limited in ways that constrain the conclusions that can be drawn from them. One limitation is that using a single intertrial interval (Rogers & Monsell, 1995, Experiment 5) or only long s (Meiran & horev, 2005, Experiments 1 3) does not allow one to determine whether subjects actually engaged in preparation. Recall that preparation is typically inferred from a reduction in switch cost with, which can be established only by using short and long s. Another limitation is that using a single intertrial interval (Rogers & Monsell, 1995, Experiment 5), a single short on alert trials (Meiran et al., 2000, Experiment 4), or a single long (Meiran & horev, 2005, Experiment 4) does not allow one to determine whether the observed switch

3 RESIDUAL SWITH OSTS 319 costs are actually residual (i.e., that they would not be reduced at a longer interval). Stronger claims about switch costs being residual can be made by finding similar switch costs at multiple long s (as in Meiran & horev, 2005, Experiments 1 3). A final limitation is that presenting alerting stimuli on all trials (Meiran & horev, 2005) does not allow one to determine whether the observed residual switch costs differ from those obtained on no-alert trials under typical task-cuing conditions. The purpose of the present study was to investigate the relationship between phasic alertness and residual switch costs in task-switching performance while avoiding the limitations of previous studies. In two experiments involving the task-cuing procedure, subjects performed odd even and small large tasks on target digits, cued by the letters and U. I chose these tasks and cues because they produced large and highly reliable residual switch costs on RTs in a previous study (Schneider, 2016, Experiment 4). On no-alert trials, the cue preceded the target by a of 200, 1,500, or 2,000 ms, which allowed me to determine whether preparation occurred (as indicated by a reduction in switch cost from the 200-ms to the longer s) and whether residual switch costs were obtained (as indicated by similar switch costs at the 1,500- and 2,000-ms s) under typical task-cuing conditions. On alert trials, there was a fixed of 2,000 ms, during which time an alerting stimulus was presented. I chose to present the alerting stimulus after (instead of before) the cue to provide an opportunity to trigger preparatory processing that might not have occurred following cue onset. The type of alerting stimulus and the timing of its presentation were manipulated, as described later for each experiment. I compared performance on no-alert and alert trials in two respects to assess the influence of alerting stimuli. First, I examined whether overall performance (primarily with respect to RTs) was better on alert than on no-alert trials, which served as a manipulation check that the alerting stimuli actually increased phasic alertness. Second, I compared switch costs at the 2,000-ms for alert and no-alert trials, which allowed me to determine whether increased phasic alertness modulated residual switch costs. Experiment 1 The type of alerting stimulus presented on alert trials was manipulated between subjects in Experiment 1. An example of each alert type is shown in Figure 1, which illustrates the sequence of events on alert trials. For subjects in the cue alert group, the alerting stimulus was a more salient version of the cue letter. For subjects in the target-location alert group, the alerting stimulus was a more salient version of the fixation cross that indicated the spatial location of the forthcoming target digit, analogous to the alerting stimulus in Rogers and Monsell (1995, Experiment 5). For subjects in the outer-frame alert group, the alerting stimulus was an outlined rectangle that surrounded the main stimulus display, analogous to the alerting stimulus in Meiran and horev (2005, Experiment 3). I chose these alert types to investigate whether their informational value would modulate any alerting effects on task-switching performance. The cue alert is the most informative because it signals the relevant task. If there were a failure to engage in preparation on the basis of the originally presented cue, then the cue alert might trigger preparation, thereby reducing residual Blank (500 ms) switch costs. The target-location alert is less informative because it signals where the target will be but neither the target identity nor the task to be performed. However, attracting attention to the forthcoming target location could facilitate target encoding, thereby shortening RT and possibly also affecting target processing associated with residual switch costs. The outer-frame alert is mostly uninformative because it provides no information related to either the identities or the locations of the cue and the target. However, on the basis of Meiran and horev s (2005) findings, it might be expected to reduce residual switch costs on alert trials relative to no-alert trials. A single of 500 ms was used in Experiment 1, which meant that all three alert types reliably predicted the time of target onset. I chose that particular value because it was associated with optimal or near-optimal performance in previous work involving tasks performed on single-character stimuli (Posner, 1978; Posner & Boies, 1971) and because it is the typical value used in the ANT (Fan et al., 2002). It was the value used by Rogers and Monsell (1995), and it falls in the middle of the range of values examined by Meiran and horev (2005). The potential effects of manipulating are examined in Experiment 2. Method Fixa on (500 ms) ue ue Time Target Loca on Subjects. A total of 120 students (40 per alert-type group) from Purdue University participated for course credit. Data from three additional subjects were excluded because their mean error rates exceeded a preset inclusion criterion of 20%. All subjects in the present study reported having normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Apparatus. The experiment was conducted on desktop computers that displayed stimuli on monitors and registered responses with millisecond-accurate timing from hronos response devices (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.). Except for the alerting stimuli described later, the stimuli were displayed in white 18-point Arial font on a black background at a viewing distance of approximately 50 cm. Responses were made by using the left and right index (one of) ue Target Interval (; ms) Outer Frame 8 Target Target Interval (; ms) Figure 1. Schematic of events on alert trials in Experiment 1. The category response mapping reminders at the bottom of each display have been omitted.

4 320 SHNEIDER fingers to press the leftmost and rightmost buttons, respectively, on the response device. The response buttons (each 1.3 cm 2.5 cm) were 10.3 cm apart and separated by three unused buttons. White light emitting diodes located approximately 5 cm behind the response buttons were illuminated continuously during the experiment to indicate the relevant buttons. Tasks. Two numerical categorization tasks were performed on the target digits 1 9, excluding 5. The odd even task involved pressing a response button to categorize a target as odd or even, whereas the small large task involved pressing a response button to categorize a target as smaller or larger than 5. The tasks were cued by the letters and U, with cue task mappings counterbalanced across subjects. One of the categories associated with each task was mapped to each response button (e.g., odd and small categories mapped to the left button; even and large categories mapped to the right button), with category response mappings counterbalanced across subjects. Reminders of the category response mappings were displayed throughout each trial at the bottom of the screen. Procedure. Subjects were seated at computers in individual testing rooms after providing informed consent for a study protocol approved by the Purdue University Institutional Review Board. Instructions were presented onscreen and read aloud by the experimenter. Subjects completed eight example trials (with accuracy feedback and, if necessary, experimenter guidance) during the instructions before beginning the experiment proper, which consisted of six blocks of 64 trials per block (without accuracy feedback or experimenter guidance). Each block included 48 noalert trials and 16 alert trials presented in random order. For no-alert trials, every possible combination of, cue, and target occurred once per block. For alert trials, which involved a fixed, every possible combination of cue and target occurred once per block. Key aspects of the trial procedure are illustrated in Figure 1. Each trial started with a blank display for 500 ms, followed by a fixation display consisting of two vertically arranged crosses (the distance between the centers of the crosses was 1.1 cm) presented centrally for 500 ms. The top fixation cross was then replaced by a cue. On no-alert trials, the cue display appeared for a of 200, 1,500, or 2,000 ms, then the bottom fixation cross was replaced by a target. On alert trials, the cue display appeared for 1,500 ms, then one of three types of alerting stimuli (see Figure 1) appeared for an of 500 ms, resulting in a of 2,000 ms. For the cue alert group, the alerting stimulus was a more salient version of the cue (displayed in green 28-point Arial bold font) that replaced the original cue. For the target-location alert group, the alerting stimulus was a more salient version of the bottom fixation cross (displayed in green 28-point Arial bold font) that replaced the original cross. For the outer-frame alert group, the alerting stimulus was an outlined rectangle (displayed in green, measuring 1.4 cm 2.1 cm) that surrounded the cue and the bottom fixation cross. After the elapsed, the alerting stimulus disappeared (for the cue alert group, the original cue reappeared in its place) and the bottom fixation cross was replaced by a target. On both no-alert and alert trials, the final display consisted of the cue and the target, which remained onscreen until the subject responded or a response deadline of 2,500 ms elapsed, and then the next trial commenced. Subjects were made aware of the response deadline and instructed to respond quickly and accurately. They were informed that the cues would be selected randomly (which meant that tasks would occur unpredictably) but that the could be used to prepare for the cued task. They were also informed about the nature (but not the purpose) of the alerting stimulus that would sometimes appear during the. Results The first block, the first trial of each subsequent block, and any trials on which subjects failed to respond before the deadline (.8% of trials) were excluded from analysis. Error trials were excluded from the RT analyses. Mean RTs and error rates are presented in Figure 2, collapsed over alert type because it had no significant effects on performance (see Table 1 for the means for all conditions). Statistical results from analyses of variance (ANOVAs) and contrasts are summarized in Table 2. Baseline analysis. This analysis focused on no-alert trials to determine whether preparation effects occurred and whether switch costs at the long s could be considered residual. Mean RTs and error rates for no-alert trials were submitted to 2 (transition: task switch or task repetition) 3 (: 200, 1,500, or 2,000 ms) repeated-measures ANOVAs. 1 As shown in Figure 2, RTs were longer and error rates were higher for task switches (946 ms and 5.9%) than for task repetitions (818 ms and 3.6%), yielding significant main effects of transition. Performance improved from the 200-ms (923 ms and 5.4%) to the 1,500-ms (864 ms and 4.5%), yielding significant main effects of. Switch costs for RTs and error rates decreased by 99 ms and 1.5% from the 200-ms to the 1,500-ms, yielding significant interactions between transition and. Planned contrasts for the interactions revealed that switch costs did not differ significantly between the 1,500-ms (94 ms and 1.9%) and the 2,000-ms (95 ms and 1.8%). Bayesfactor analyses using scaled JZS Bayes factors with r 1 (Rouder, Speckman, Sun, Morey, & Iverson, 2009) for the same contrasts indicated that no difference in switch costs was 13.8 and 13.7 times more likely than was a difference for RTs and error rates, respectively. ing analysis. This analysis focused on no-alert and alert trials at the longest to determine whether alerting effects occurred. Mean RTs and error rates were submitted to 3 (alert type: cue, target location, or outer frame) 2 (transition: task switch or task repetition) 2 (alert status: no alert or alert) mixed-measures ANOVAs. RTs were shorter on alert trials (810 ms) than on no-alert trials (858 ms), yielding a significant main effect of alert status. As shown in Figure 2, switch costs were similar on no-alert and alert trials for both RTs (95 and 86 ms, respectively) and error rates (1.8% and 1.9%, respectively), yielding significant main effects of transition but nonsignificant interactions involving alert status. Bayes-factor analyses indicated that no difference in switch costs between no-alert and alert trials was 9.0 and 13.7 times more likely than was a difference for RTs and error rates, respectively. No other effects were significant in the ANOVAs. 1 The analysis was repeated with alert type (cue, target location, or outer frame) included as a between-subjects factor to assess contextual effects on no-alert trials. There were no significant effects involving alert type.

5 RESIDUAL SWITH OSTS 321 Discussion A Mean Response Time (ms) Switch ost (ms) 1,400 1,300 1,200 1,100 1, Task Switch (No ) Task Repe on (No ) Task Switch () Task Repe on () ,500 2,000 2,000 No ,500 2,000 2,000 There were several important findings in Experiment 1. The baseline analysis revealed switch costs that decreased with on no-alert trials, indicating subjects often engaged in preparation. That analysis also revealed that switch costs did not differ significantly between the two long s (and Bayes factors favored no differences), indicating they can be considered residual switch costs. The alerting analysis revealed that the large residual switch costs at the longest did not differ significantly between noalert and alert trials (and Bayes factors favored no differences), indicating the alerting stimuli did not influence task-switching performance in particular. However, that analysis also revealed an alerting benefit on overall RTs, indicating subjects attended to the alerting stimuli and experienced a general improvement in performance. Finally, there were no significant effects of alert type, indicating the informational value of the alerting stimulus affected B Mean Error Rate (%) D Switch ost (%) Task Switch (No ) Task Repe on (No ) Task Switch () Task Repe on () ,500 2,000 2,000 No ,500 2,000 2,000 Figure 2. Experiment 1 data. Mean response times (Panel A) and error rates (Panel B) as a function of transition, alert status, and cue target interval (; ms). Switch costs for response times (Panel ) and error rates (Panel D) as a function of alert status and. The alert target interval (; ms) is included on the x-axes. neither task switching in particular nor performance more generally. Experiment 2 Even though alerting effects on overall RTs were observed in Experiment 1, there are two potential concerns regarding how alerting was implemented. The first concern is that because the of 500 ms falls in the middle of the four s (66, 350, 632, and 1,032 ms) examined by Meiran and horev (2005, Experiments 1 3), who found that residual switch costs at their two longest s were significantly smaller than those at their shortest, it might not have been long enough to increase phasic alertness to the extent that it would modulate residual switch costs. It is possible that a longer might be needed to reliably detect a difference in residual switch costs between alert and no-alert trials.

6 322 SHNEIDER Table 1 Mean Response Times (in ms) and Error Rates (as Percentages) for Experiment 1 Transition and (ms) status Response time Error rate type: ue Task switch 200 No alert 990 (30) 8.3 (.9) 1,500 No alert 873 (30) 5.4 (.8) 2,000 No alert 886 (31) 5.3 (.9) 831 (31) 4.8 (.8) Task repetition 200 No alert 805 (26) 4.2 (.7) 1,500 No alert 804 (25) 3.8 (.7) 2,000 No alert 798 (26) 4.1 (.6) 747 (27) 2.6 (.6) type: Target location Task switch 200 No alert 1,021 (24) 6.6 (.9) 1,500 No alert 925 (29) 5.1 (.7) 2,000 No alert 909 (30) 5.4 (.7) 861 (32) 5.6 (.9) Task repetition 200 No alert 822 (18) 3.0 (.5) 1,500 No alert 820 (23) 2.9 (.6) 2,000 No alert 817 (27) 3.0 (.6) 771 (24) 2.7 (.4) type: Outer frame Task switch 200 No alert 1,048 (34) 6.2 (1.0) 1,500 No alert 937 (34) 5.8 (.9) 2,000 No alert 923 (34) 5.2 (.8) 865 (35) 4.5 (.8) Task repetition 200 No alert 852 (29) 3.9 (.7) 1,500 No alert 828 (26) 4.0 (.7) 2,000 No alert 817 (27) 3.3 (.6) 783 (30) 3.9 (.7) Note. Standard errors of the means appear in parentheses. cue target interval. A second potential concern is that the alerting stimulus was transient in Experiment 1: After the elapsed, the alerting stimulus was replaced by the original cue (cue alert) or the target (target-location alert), or it simply disappeared (outer-frame alert). Although transient alerting stimuli are typical in studies using the ANT (Fan et al., 2002), persistent alerting stimuli that remain present after target onset have tended to be used in previous studies of phasic alertness in task switching (Meiran & horev, 2005; Rogers & Monsell, 1995). It is possible that the offset of a transient alerting stimulus has a disruptive effect on performance that makes it difficult to detect a difference in residual switch costs between alert and no-alert trials. I introduced two manipulations in Experiment 2 to address these concerns. First, was manipulated within subjects at three levels (100, 500, and 900 ms) to determine whether alerting effects on overall RTs and possibly also residual switch costs vary across s. I chose those s because the shortest one is close to the least optimal (66 ms) and the longest one is in between the two most optimal s (632 and 1,032 ms) used by Meiran and horev (2005); the middle was included to maintain continuity with Experiment 1. Second, alert availability was manipulated between subjects. For subjects in the transient alert group, the alerting stimulus which was always an outer frame disappeared at target onset, as in Experiment 1. For subjects in the persistent alert group, the alerting stimulus remained present after target onset, such that the outer frame surrounded the cue and the target until either a response was made or the response deadline elapsed. I chose the outer frame as the alerting stimulus because it most closely resembles the alerting stimuli used in Meiran and horev s study, in which the manipulation of was found to affect residual switch costs. Thus, Experiment 2 conceptually replicates some of the key methodological aspects of their study. Method Subjects. A total of 80 students (40 per alert-availability group) from Purdue University participated for course credit. None of the subjects had participated in Experiment 1. Data from six additional subjects were excluded because their mean error rates exceeded 20% (four subjects) or they failed to respond before the deadline on at least 10% of trials (two subjects). Apparatus and tasks. These aspects of the experiment were identical to those of Experiment 1. Procedure. The procedure was identical to that of Experiment 1, except for the following changes. The experiment consisted of 14 blocks of 48 trials per block. Each successive pair of blocks included 48 no-alert trials and 48 alert trials presented in random order. The no-alert trials were identical to those of Experiment 1, including the manipulation of (200, 1,500, or 2,000 ms). The alert trials involved the outer-frame alerting stimulus from Experiment 1 and two new manipulations. First, whereas the remained fixed at 2,000 ms for all alert trials, the was 100, 500, or 900 ms (occurring equally often with every possible combination of cue and target). Second, alert availability was manipulated between subjects: For subjects in the transient alert group, the Table 2 Analyses of Variance and ontrasts for Experiment 1 Analysis and effect df Response time Error rate F MSE 2 p F MSE 2 p Baseline T 1, , , , T 2, , ontrast a 1, , ing Y 2, , T 1, , A 1, , Y T 2, , Y A 2, , T A 1, , Y T A 2, , Note. T transition; cue target interval; Y alert type; A alert status. a Planned contrast for the interaction, comparing switch costs at the two longest cue target intervals. p.05.

7 RESIDUAL SWITH OSTS 323 outer-frame alerting stimulus disappeared at target onset, as in Experiment 1, whereas for subjects in the persistent alert group, the alerting stimulus remained present after target onset, until the end of the trial. Results The first block, the first trial of each subsequent block, and any trials on which subjects failed to respond before the deadline (.7% of trials) were excluded from analysis. Error trials were excluded from the RT analyses. Mean RTs and error rates are presented in Figure 3, collapsed over alert availability because it had only one significant effect on performance (described later; see Table 3 for the means for all conditions). Statistical results from ANOVAs and contrasts are summarized in Table 4. Baseline analysis. This analysis focused on no-alert trials to determine whether preparation effects occurred and whether switch costs at the long s could be considered residual. Mean RTs and error rates for no-alert trials were submitted to 2 (transition: task switch or task repetition) 3 (: 200, 1,500, or 2,000 ms) repeated-measures ANOVAs. 2 As shown in Figure 3, RTs were longer and error rates were higher for task switches (913 ms and 5.9%) than for task repetitions (797 ms and 3.4%), yielding significant main effects of transition. Performance improved from the 200-ms (909 ms and 5.4%) to the 1,500-ms (839 ms and 4.8%), yielding significant main effects of. Switch costs for RTs and error rates decreased by 100 ms and 2.0% from the 200-ms to the 1,500-ms, yielding significant interactions between transition and. Planned contrasts for the interactions revealed that switch costs did not differ significantly between the 1,500-ms (87 ms and 2.0%) and the 2,000-ms (71 ms and 1.6%). Bayes-factor analyses for the same contrasts indicated that no difference in switch costs was 3.9 and 8.6 times more likely than was a difference for RTs and error rates, respectively. ing analysis. This analysis focused on no-alert and alert trials at the longest to determine whether alerting effects occurred. Mean RTs and error rates were submitted to 2 (alert availability: transient or persistent) 2 (transition: task switch or task repetition) 2 (alert status: no alert or alert) mixed-measures ANOVAs, averaging over for the alert trials. RTs were shorter on alert trials (773 ms) than on no-alert trials (817 ms), yielding a significant main effect of alert status. This alerting effect was larger when the alerting stimulus was persistent (53 ms) than when it was transient (34 ms), yielding a significant interaction between alert availability and alert status. As shown in Figure 3, switch costs were similar on no-alert and alert trials for both RTs (71 and 75 ms, respectively) and error rates (1.6% and 1.8%, respectively), yielding significant main effects of transition but nonsignificant interactions involving alert status. Bayes-factor analyses indicated that no difference in switch costs between no-alert and alert trials was 10.2 and 10.6 times more likely than was a difference for RTs and error rates, respectively. No other effects were significant in the ANOVAs. ing interval analysis. This analysis focused on alert trials to determine whether alerting effects differed across s. Mean RTs and error rates for alert trials were submitted to 2 (transition: task switch or task repetition) 3 (: 100, 500, or 900 ms) repeated-measures ANOVAs. RTs became progressively shorter (782 to 774 to 764 ms) from the shortest to the longest, yielding a significant main effect of. As shown in Figure 3, switch costs were similar from the shortest to the longest for both RTs (80, 74, and 70 ms) and error rates (1.9%, 2.0%, and 1.6%), yielding significant main effects of transition but nonsignificant interactions involving. Bayes-factor analyses indicated that no difference in switch costs between the shortest and the longest s was 6.4 and 9.9 times more likely than was a difference for RTs and error rates, respectively. No other effects were significant in the ANOVAs. Discussion There were several important findings in Experiment 2 that replicated those of Experiment 1. The baseline analysis revealed switch costs that decreased with on no-alert trials, indicating subjects often engaged in preparation. That analysis also revealed that switch costs did not differ significantly between the two long s (and Bayes factors favored no differences), indicating they can be considered residual switch costs. The alerting analysis revealed that the large residual switch costs at the longest did not differ significantly between no-alert and alert trials averaged across s (and Bayes factors favored no differences), indicating the alerting stimuli did not influence task-switching performance in particular. However, that analysis also revealed an alerting benefit on overall RTs (the effect was larger for persistent than for transient alerting stimuli), indicating subjects attended to the alerting stimuli and experienced a general improvement in performance, especially when the alerting stimulus remained present after target onset. The alerting interval analysis revealed that the residual switch costs on alert trials did not differ significantly across s, despite progressively shorter RTs as the varied from 100 to 900 ms, indicating that a more optimal alerting interval was ineffective for reliably reducing residual switch costs (cf. Meiran & horev, 2005). General Discussion My goal in the present study was to achieve insight regarding the relationship between phasic alertness and residual switch costs in task-switching performance. I conducted two taskcuing experiments in which subjects did odd even and small large tasks on target digits, with and without an alerting stimulus presented shortly before the target (alert and no-alert trials, respectively). Both experiments involved short and long s on no-alert trials to assess preparation effects and residual switch costs, whereas a long was used on alert trials to assess potential alerting effects on overall performance and residual switch costs. In Experiment 1, I manipulated alert type (cue, target location, or outer frame) to determine whether the informational value of alerting stimuli would modulate alerting effects. In Experiment 2, I manipulated the and alert availability (transient or persistent) to determine whether more optimal conditions existed for detecting alerting effects on residual switch costs. The experiments produced similar patterns of results (see Figures 2 and 3). Switch costs were obtained that decreased with 2 The analysis was repeated with alert availability (transient or persistent) included as a between-subjects factor to assess contextual effects on no-alert trials. There were no significant effects involving alert availability.

8 324 SHNEIDER A Mean Response Time (ms) Switch ost (ms) 1,400 1,300 1,200 1,100 1, Task Switch (No ) Task Repe on (No ) Task Switch () Task Repe on () ,500 2,000 2,000 2,000 2,000 No ,500 2,000 2,000 2,000 2,000 on no-alert trials, indicating subjects engaged in preparatory processing that benefitted task-switching performance. Switch costs were similar at the two long s on no-alert trials, indicating those s were long enough for subjects to finish whatever preparation they could do, and the switch costs could be considered residual. ing effects were obtained in the form of shorter RTs on alert than on no-alert trials, which served as a manipulation check that the alerting stimuli actually increased phasic alertness. ritically, residual switch costs were similar on alert and no-alert trials in both experiments for RTs and error rates, regardless of alert type, alert availability, and. This finding represents evidence that increased phasic alertness does not modulate residual switch costs. The results of the present study are consistent with some previous findings in the literature. The reduction in switch costs from the shortest to the long s replicates extant evidence of B Mean Error Rate (%) D Switch ost (%) Task Switch (No ) Task Repe on (No ) Task Switch () Task Repe on () ,500 2,000 2,000 2,000 2,000 No ,500 2,000 2,000 2,000 2,000 Figure 3. Experiment 2 data. Mean response times (Panel A) and error rates (Panel B) as a function of transition, alert status, cue target interval (; ms), and alert target interval (; ms). Switch costs for response times (Panel ) and error rates (Panel D) as a function of alert status, cue target interval (), and alert target interval (). preparation in task switching (e.g., Meiran, 1996; Monsell & Mizon, 2006; Schneider & Logan, 2011). The observation of large and reliable residual switch costs at the long s fits with other findings of performance decrements in task switching despite ample preparation time (e.g., Longman et al., 2014; Meiran, 1996; Meiran et al., 2000; Rogers & Monsell, 1995), in addition to replicating recent results from my laboratory using the same tasks and cues (Schneider, 2016, Experiment 4). The observation of alerting effects on overall RTs is consistent with what has been found in both the alerting literature (e.g., Fan et al., 2002; Nieuwenhuis & De Kleijn, 2013) and the task-switching literature (Meiran & horev, 2005; Rogers & Monsell, 1995). Given my goal of determining whether phasic alertness modulates residual switch costs, it is reassuring that my experimental methods were suitable for producing robust alerting effects on overall RTs and large residual switch costs in the first place.

9 RESIDUAL SWITH OSTS 325 Table 3 Mean Response Times (in ms) and Error Rates (as Percentages) for Experiment 2 Transition and (ms) status (ms) Response time Error rate availability: Transient Task switch 200 No alert (31) 7.2 (.9) 1,500 No alert (31) 5.2 (.8) 2,000 No alert (31) 3.9 (.6) (30) 4.1 (.5) (32) 5.1 (.6) (31) 4.2 (.6) Task repetition 200 No alert (24) 2.5 (.4) 1,500 No alert (25) 3.4 (.5) 2,000 No alert (25) 2.5 (.5) (25) 2.4 (.5) (26) 2.0 (.3) (26) 2.7 (.5) availability: Persistent Task switch 200 No alert 0 1,030 (23) 7.5 (1.0) 1,500 No alert (24) 6.4 (.8) 2,000 No alert (25) 5.4 (.8) (27) 5.0 (.7) (26) 4.0 (.6) (23) 5.0 (.8) Task repetition 200 No alert (20) 4.2 (.7) 1,500 No alert (22) 4.1 (.6) 2,000 No alert (22) 3.6 (.6) (19) 2.9 (.5) (19) 3.1 (.6) (19) 3.4 (.5) Note. Standard errors of the means appear in parentheses. cue target interval; alert target interval. In contrast, the present results are inconsistent with the competing predictions outlined in the Introduction section. Recall that if residual switch costs reflect interference, then on the basis of findings with various interference effects in the alerting literature (e.g., MacLeod et al., 2010; Nieuwenhuis & De Kleijn, 2013), one would expect larger residual switch costs on alert than on no-alert trials. However, if residual switch costs reflect failures to engage in preparation (De Jong, 2000), then an alerting stimulus might trigger preparatory processing, yielding smaller residual switch costs on alert than on no-alert trials. The present finding of no differences in residual switch costs between alert and no-alert trials (supported by Bayes factors favoring no differences) in two experiments does not support either prediction. A logical interpretation of this finding is that phasic alertness does not affect task switching. One possible explanation for this outcome can be derived from the early onset hypothesis proposed by Nieuwenhuis and De Kleijn (2013) to explain why alerting stimuli speed responding but increase interference effects in the ANT and related tasks. The hypothesis is based on the assumptions that increased phasic alertness shortens target encoding time (enabling response selection to start sooner) and that cognitive control is implemented during response selection. The first assumption is supported by behavioral data showing that alerting stimuli can increase temporal orienting to facilitate target encoding (orrea, Lupiáñez, & Tudela, 2005) and electrophysiological data showing that alerting stimuli have neural effects linked to target onset (Jepma, Wagenmakers, Band, & Nieuwenhuis, 2009). The second assumption is supported by modeling work showing that interference effects in the flanker task were captured best by a model with gradual adjustment of attentional control during response selection (White, Ratcliff, & Starns, 2011). Nieuwenhuis and De Kleijn reported simulation results showing the early onset hypothesis could account for the relationship between phasic alertness and interference effects. A variant of the early onset hypothesis can explain the present findings if one assumes that control processing in task switching has to finish before response selection starts. onsider how control processing might have occurred on alert trials in Experiment 1, in which the and were always 2,000 and 500 ms, respectively. The data from no-alert trials indicate preparation was finished by 1,500 ms, given the similar switch costs obtained at the 1,500-ms and 2,000-ms s. This implies that the control processing involved in preparation had already finished by the time the alerting stimulus was presented on alert trials, providing no opportunity for changes in phasic alertness to influence such processing. Increased phasic alertness still could have facilitated target encoding, though, resulting in a benefit to overall RTs on alert trials. If subjects failed to engage in preparation during the and control processing started after target onset (De Jong, 2000) or if the target triggered additional control processing (Rogers & Monsell, 1995), then faster target encoding would allow control processing to start sooner on alert than on no-alert trials. However, if control processing is responsible for residual switch costs and has to finish before response selection starts, then in- Table 4 Analyses of Variance and ontrasts for Experiment 2 Analysis and effect df Response time Error rate 2 2 F MSE p F MSE p Baseline T 1, , , , T 2, , ontrast a 1, , ing V 1, , T 1, , A 1, , V T 1, , V A 1, , T A 1, , V T A 1, , ing interval T 1, , , , T 2, , Note. T transition; cue target interval; V alert availability; A alert status; alert target interval. a Planned contrast for the interaction, comparing switch costs at the two longest cue target intervals. p.05.

10 326 SHNEIDER creased phasic alertness would affect the onset but not the duration of response selection, thereby failing to modulate residual switch costs. This explanation suggests that the effects of phasic alertness depend on the nature and the timing of control processing in task performance. Weinbach and Henik (2012) provided evidence in support of this point, showing that the typical modulation of interference effects by phasic alertness occurs only for tasks that involve spatial attentional control. This is consistent with neuropsychological work showing that alerting stimuli improve spatial attention in patients with unilateral neglect (Robertson, Mattingley, Rorden, & Driver, 1998). It might also help to explain why Meiran and horev (2005) observed modulations of residual switch costs across s when subjects switched between spatial tasks. An important goal for future research will be to delineate the specific circumstances under which phasic alertness affects performance in task-switching situations and in other contexts that involve cognitive control. References Allport, A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and Performance XV: onscious and nonconscious information processing (pp ). ambridge, MA: MIT Press. Böckler, A., Alpay, G., & Stürmer, B. (2011). Accessory stimuli affect the emergence of conflict, not conflict control: A Simon-task ERP study. Experimental Psychology, 58, Brown, S., Lehmann,., & Poboka, D. (2006). A critical test of the failure-to-engage theory of task switching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, allejas, A., Lupiáñez, J., & Tudela, P. (2004). The three attentional networks: On their independence and interactions. Brain and ognition, 54, orrea, A., Lupiáñez, J., & Tudela, P. (2005). Attentional preparation based on temporal expectancy modulates processing at the perceptual level. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, De Jong, R. (2000). An intention-activation account of residual switch costs. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and Performance XVIII: ontrol of cognitive processes (pp ). ambridge, MA: MIT Press. Fan, J., Mcandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of ognitive Neuroscience, 14, Fischer, R., Plessow, F., & Kiesel, A. (2010). Auditory warning signals affect mechanisms of response selection: Evidence from a Simon task. Experimental Psychology, 57, Fischer, R., Plessow, F., & Kiesel, A. (2012). The effects of alerting signals in action control: Activation of S-R associations or inhibition of executive control processes? Psychological Research, 76, Jepma, M., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Band, G. P. H., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2009). The effects of accessory stimuli on information processing: Evidence from electrophysiology and a diffusion model analysis. Journal of ognitive Neuroscience, 21, Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). ontrol and interference in task switching A review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, Lien, M.-., Ruthruff, E., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J.. (2005). On the limits of advance preparation for a task switch: Do people prepare all the task some of the time or some of the task all the time? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, Lindsen, J. P., & De Jong, R. (2010). Distinguishing between the partialmapping preparation hypothesis and the failure-to-engage hypothesis of residual switch costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, Longman,. S., Lavric, A., Munteanu,., & Monsell, S. (2014). Attentional inertia and delayed orienting of spatial attention in task-switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, MacLeod, J. W., Lawrence, M. A., Mconnell, M. M., Eskes, G. A., Klein, R. M., & Shore, D. I. (2010). Appraising the ANT: Psychometric and theoretical considerations of the Attention Network Test. Neuropsychology, 24, Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and ognition, 22, Meiran, N. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, Meiran, N. (2014). The task-cuing paradigm: A user s guide. In J. A. Grange & G. Houghton (Eds.), Task switching and cognitive control (pp ). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Meiran, N., & horev, Z. (2005). Phasic alertness and the residual taskswitching cost. Experimental Psychology, 52, Meiran, N., horev, Z., & Sapir, A. (2000). omponent processes in task switching. ognitive Psychology, 41, Monsell, S., & Mizon, G. A. (2006). an the task-cuing paradigm measure an endogenous task-set reconfiguration process? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, Nieuwenhuis, S., & de Kleijn, R. (2013). The impact of alertness on cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, Nieuwenhuis, S., & Monsell, S. (2002). Residual costs in task switching: Testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, Poboka, D., Karayanidis, F., & Heathcote, A. (2014). Extending the failure-to-engage theory of task switch costs. ognitive Psychology, 72, Posner, M. I. (1978). hronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Posner, M. I. (2008). Measuring alertness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1129, Posner, M. I., & Boies, S. J. (1971). omponents of attention. Psychological Review, 78, Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2006). Working memory capacity and attention network test performance. Applied ognitive Psychology, 20, Robertson, I. H., Mattingley, J. B., Rorden,., & Driver, J. (1998, September 10). Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness. Nature, 395, Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). osts of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, Schneider, D. W. (2016). Investigating a method for reducing residual switch costs in cued task switching. Memory & ognition, 44, Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2011). Task-switching performance with 1:1 and 2:1 cue-task mappings: Not so different after all. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and ognition, 37, Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task switching: Interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological Bulletin, 136,

Chunking away task-switch costs: a test of the chunk-point hypothesis

Chunking away task-switch costs: a test of the chunk-point hypothesis Psychon Bull Rev (2015) 22:884 889 DOI 10.3758/s13423-014-0721-3 BRIEF REPORT Chunking away task-switch costs: a test of the chunk-point hypothesis Darryl W. Schneider & Gordon D. Logan Published online:

More information

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm

Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (6), 1111-1116 doi:10.3758/pbr.15.6.1111 Inhibition of task set: Converging evidence from task choice in the voluntary task-switching paradigm Mei-Ching Lien Oregon

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Extending the Failure-to-Engage. Theory of Task Switch Costs

Extending the Failure-to-Engage. Theory of Task Switch Costs Extending the Failure-to-Engage Theory of Task Switch Costs Dane Poboka, Frini Karayanidis & Andrew Heathcote* School of Psychology, The University of Newcastle, Australia *Corresponding Author: Andrew

More information

Task-Switching Performance With 1:1 and 2:1 Cue Task Mappings: Not So Different After All

Task-Switching Performance With 1:1 and 2:1 Cue Task Mappings: Not So Different After All Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2011, Vol. 37, No. 2, 405 415 2011 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/11/$10 DOI: 10.1037/a0021967 Task-Switching Performance

More information

Control processes in voluntary and explicitly cued task switching

Control processes in voluntary and explicitly cued task switching THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2014 Vol. 67, No. 10, 1944 1958, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.879390 Control processes in voluntary and explicitly cued task switching Michael

More information

Priming or executive control? Associative priming of cue encoding increases switch costs in the explicit task-cuing procedure

Priming or executive control? Associative priming of cue encoding increases switch costs in the explicit task-cuing procedure Journal Memory & Cognition 2006,?? 34 (?), (6),???-??? 1250-1259 Priming or executive control? Associative priming of cue encoding increases switch costs in the explicit task-cuing procedure GORDON D.

More information

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),???-??? 794-799 What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues? ULRICH MAYR University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon Schneider and

More information

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2007, Vol. 33, No. 3, 484 502 Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484

More information

Current Task Activation Predicts General Effects of Advance Preparation in Task Switching

Current Task Activation Predicts General Effects of Advance Preparation in Task Switching Current Task Activation Predicts General Effects of Advance Preparation in Task Switching Edita Poljac, Ab de Haan, and Gerard P. van Galen Nijmegen Institute for Cognition and Information Radboud University

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

More attention to attention? An eye-tracking investigation of. selection of perceptual attributes during a task switch

More attention to attention? An eye-tracking investigation of. selection of perceptual attributes during a task switch More attention to attention? An eye-tracking investigation of selection of perceptual attributes during a task switch Cai S. Longman, Aureliu Lavric*, and Stephen Monsell Centre for Cognitive Control and

More information

Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences

Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2007, 14 (5), 970-976 Task-set inhibition in chunked task sequences Darryl W. Schneider Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee Exploring the hierarchical control relationship

More information

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr University of Oregon Running head: Cue-specific versus task-specific switch costs Ulrich Mayr Department of Psychology University

More information

SUBMITTED TO: JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY: GENERAL

SUBMITTED TO: JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY: GENERAL RUNNING HEAD: SEQUENTIAL EFFECTS IN DEDUCTION Attentional Effects in deduction: Cost of inference shift Emilio G. Milán (1), Sergio Moreno Ríos (1), Orlando Espino (2), Matej Hochel (1) and Carlos Santamaría

More information

Contextual control over task-set retrieval

Contextual control over task-set retrieval Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (8), 2047-2053 doi:10.3758/app.72.8.2047 BRIEF REPORTS Contextual control over task-set retrieval MATTHEW J. C. CRUMP AND GORDON D. LOGAN Vanderbilt University,

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Memory Search, Task Switching and Timing 1 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press Timing is affected by demands in memory search, but not by task switching Claudette

More information

Cognitive Control Under Stress

Cognitive Control Under Stress Cognitive Control Under Stress How Stress Affects Strategies of Task-Set Reconfiguration Marco Steinhauser, Martin Maier, and Ronald Hübner Universität Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany ABSTRACT This study investigated

More information

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching

Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (7), 1040-1050 doi:10.3758/mc.37.7.1040 Verbal representation in task order control: An examination with transition and task cues in random task switching ERINA SAEKI AND SATORU

More information

Task Switching Versus Cue Switching: Using Transition Cuing to Disentangle Sequential Effects in Task-Switching Performance

Task Switching Versus Cue Switching: Using Transition Cuing to Disentangle Sequential Effects in Task-Switching Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 007, Vol. 33, No., 370 378 Copyright 007 by the American Psychological Association 078-7393/07/$1.00 DOI: 10.1037/078-7393.33..370 Task

More information

Voluntary Task Switching: Chasing the Elusive Homunculus

Voluntary Task Switching: Chasing the Elusive Homunculus Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2005, Vol. 31, No. 4, 683 702 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.4.683

More information

Selecting a Response in Task Switching: Testing a Model of Compound Cue Retrieval

Selecting a Response in Task Switching: Testing a Model of Compound Cue Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2009, Vol. 35, No. 1, 122 136 2009 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013744 Selecting a Response

More information

Acta Psychologica. Modeling graded response congruency effects in task switching. Darryl W. Schneider. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Acta Psychologica. Modeling graded response congruency effects in task switching. Darryl W. Schneider. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica 153 (2014) 160 168 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/ locate/actpsy Modeling graded response congruency effects in task switching

More information

Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-Signal Paradigm

Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-Signal Paradigm Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2011, Vol. 37, No. 2, 392 404 2010 American Psychological Association 0278-7393/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021800 Balancing Cognitive Demands:

More information

Stimulus-cued completion of reconfiguration and retroactive adjustment as causes for the residual switching cost in multistep tasks

Stimulus-cued completion of reconfiguration and retroactive adjustment as causes for the residual switching cost in multistep tasks EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 18 (4), 652 668 Stimulus-cued completion of reconfiguration and retroactive adjustment as causes for the residual switching cost in multistep tasks Roy Luria,

More information

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search

Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (4), 697-702 Attentional set interacts with perceptual load in visual search JAN THEEUWES Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, the Netherlands and ARTHUR F. KRAMER and

More information

On the Difficulty of Task Switching: Assessing the Role of Task-Set Inhibition

On the Difficulty of Task Switching: Assessing the Role of Task-Set Inhibition On the Difficulty of Task Switching: Assessing the Role of Task-Set Inhibition Mei-Ching Lien Oregon State University Eric Ruthruff University of New Mexico David Kuhns Oregon State University This study

More information

RUNNING HEAD: EXPLORING TASK-SET RECONFIGURATION. Exploring task-set reconfiguration with random task sequences

RUNNING HEAD: EXPLORING TASK-SET RECONFIGURATION. Exploring task-set reconfiguration with random task sequences RUNNING HEAD: EXPLORING TASK-SET RECONFIGURATION Exploring task-set reconfiguration with random task sequences Emilio G. Milán, Daniel Sanabria, Francisco Tornay, & Antonio González Departamento de Psicología

More information

Hierarchical Control of Cognitive Processes: Switching Tasks in Sequences

Hierarchical Control of Cognitive Processes: Switching Tasks in Sequences CORRECTED APRIL 26, 2007; SEE LAST PAGE Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 135, No. 4, 623 640 0096-3445/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.623

More information

On the Limits of Advance Preparation for a Task Switch: Do People Prepare All the Task Some of the Time or Some of the Task All the Time?

On the Limits of Advance Preparation for a Task Switch: Do People Prepare All the Task Some of the Time or Some of the Task All the Time? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2005, Vol. 31, No. 2, 299 315 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.299

More information

The Cost of a Voluntary Task Switch Catherine M. Arrington and Gordon D. Logan

The Cost of a Voluntary Task Switch Catherine M. Arrington and Gordon D. Logan PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Cost of a Voluntary Task Switch Catherine M. Arrington and Gordon D. Logan Vanderbilt University ABSTRACT Task-switching paradigms are widely used to study executive

More information

The number line effect reflects top-down control

The number line effect reflects top-down control Journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006,?? 13 (?), (5),862-868???-??? The number line effect reflects top-down control JELENA RISTIC University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

More information

Multiple Priming Instances Increase the Impact of Practice-based but not Verbal. Code-based Stimulus-Response Associations

Multiple Priming Instances Increase the Impact of Practice-based but not Verbal. Code-based Stimulus-Response Associations INSTRUCTION AND MULTIPLE PRIMING 1 Multiple Priming Instances Increase the Impact of Practice-based but not Verbal Code-based Stimulus-Response Associations Christina U. Pfeuffer 1, Karolina Moutsopoulou

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Mem Cogn (2017) 45:1384 1397 DOI 10.3758/s13421-017-0734-z Components of competitor priming in task switching Morgan L. Teskey 1 & Michael E. J. Masson 1 Published online: 17 July 2017 # Psychonomic Society,

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant University of North Florida UNF Digital Commons All Volumes (2001-2008) The Osprey Journal of Ideas and Inquiry 2008 The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant Leslie

More information

Bivalency is Costly: Bivalent Stimuli Elicit Cautious Responding

Bivalency is Costly: Bivalent Stimuli Elicit Cautious Responding Bivalency is Costly: Bivalent Stimuli Elicit Cautious Responding Todd S. Woodward, 1,2 Beat Meier, 1 Christine Tipper, 1 and Peter Graf 1 1 Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver,

More information

When the voluntary mind meets the irresistible event: Stimulus response correspondence effects on task selection during voluntary task switching

When the voluntary mind meets the irresistible event: Stimulus response correspondence effects on task selection during voluntary task switching Psychon Bull Rev (2013) 20:1195 1205 DOI 10.3758/s13423-013-0437-9 BRIEF REPORT When the voluntary mind meets the irresistible event: Stimulus response correspondence effects on task selection during voluntary

More information

Task conflict effect in task switching

Task conflict effect in task switching Psychological Research (2010) 74:568 578 DOI 10.1007/s00426-010-0279-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Task conflict effect in task switching Ami Braverman Nachshon Meiran Received: 2 November 2009 / Accepted: 25 February

More information

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification

Ideomotor Compatibility in the Psychological Refractory Period Effect: 29 Years of Oversimplification Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2002, Vol. 28, No. 2, 396 409 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.396

More information

Running head: AUTOMATIC EFFECTS OF INSTRUCTIONS 1. Automatic effects of instructions do not require the intention to execute these instructions.

Running head: AUTOMATIC EFFECTS OF INSTRUCTIONS 1. Automatic effects of instructions do not require the intention to execute these instructions. Running head: AUTOMATIC EFFECTS OF INSTRUCTIONS 1 Automatic effects of instructions do not require the intention to execute these instructions. Baptist Liefooghe & Jan De Houwer Department of Experimental-Clinical

More information

Task switching: The effect of task recency with dual- and single-affordance stimuli

Task switching: The effect of task recency with dual- and single-affordance stimuli THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2006, 59 (7), 1255 1276 Task switching: The effect of task recency with dual- and single-affordance stimuli Petroc Sumner and Lubna Ahmed Imperial College

More information

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze

Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift of Visual Attention Induced by Averted Gaze Psychology, 2014, 5, 451-460 Published Online April 2014 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2014.55055 Effect of Pre-Presentation of a Frontal Face on the Shift

More information

Task Switching. Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008

Task Switching. Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008 Task Switching Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008 Monsell, 2003 Task Switching Basic phenomena Reaction time (RT) cost for switching tasks Higher error rate Diminished if prepared for switch Slower RT

More information

Task switching: effects of practice on switch and mixing costs

Task switching: effects of practice on switch and mixing costs DOI 10.1007/s00426-011-0323-x ORIGINAL ARTICLE Task switching: effects of practice on switch and mixing costs Tilo Strobach Roman Liepelt Torsten Schubert Andrea Kiesel Received: 16 July 2010 / Accepted:

More information

Infant Behavior and Development

Infant Behavior and Development Infant Behavior & Development 33 (2010) 245 249 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Infant Behavior and Development Brief report Developmental changes in inhibition of return from 3 to 6 months of

More information

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the

Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the 1 Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 835-54. doi:10.1037/a0012726

More information

Attention shifting between global and local target levels: The persistence of level-repetition effects

Attention shifting between global and local target levels: The persistence of level-repetition effects Attention shifting between global and local target levels: The persistence of level-repetition effects Ronald Hübner Technische Universität Braunschweig, Germany The aim of the reported experiments was

More information

Attentional Control of Response Selection in Task Switching

Attentional Control of Response Selection in Task Switching Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2015, Vol. 41, No. 5, 1315 1324 2015 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000091 Attentional

More information

The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition

The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (4), 603-609 The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition MIRIAM GADE Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany AND IRING

More information

Sticky Rules: Integration Between Abstract Rules and Specific Actions

Sticky Rules: Integration Between Abstract Rules and Specific Actions Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2005, Vol. 31, No. 2, 337 350 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.2.337

More information

Inhibition in motor imagery: a novel action mode switching paradigm

Inhibition in motor imagery: a novel action mode switching paradigm DOI 10.3758/s13423-016-1095-5 BRIEF REPORT Inhibition in motor imagery: a novel action mode switching paradigm Martina Rieger 1 & Stephan F. Dahm 1 & Iring Koch 2 # The Author(s) 2016. This article is

More information

Stroop Dilution Depends on the Nature of the Color Carrier but Not on Its Location

Stroop Dilution Depends on the Nature of the Color Carrier but Not on Its Location Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2006, Vol. 32, No. 4, 826 839 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.826

More information

Executive Control, Working Memory, and Action Planning: An Individual Differences Approach

Executive Control, Working Memory, and Action Planning: An Individual Differences Approach Lehigh University Lehigh Preserve Theses and Dissertations 2012 Executive Control, Working Memory, and Action Planning: An Individual Differences Approach Kaitlin Reiman Lehigh University Follow this and

More information

Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return

Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return Memory & Cognition 2009, 37 (6), 935-944 doi:10.3758/mc.37.6.935 Impaired color word processing at an unattended location: Evidence from a Stroop task combined with inhibition of return JONG MOON CHOI

More information

Switching Between Tasks of Unequal Familiarity: The Role of Stimulus-Attribute and Response-Set Selection

Switching Between Tasks of Unequal Familiarity: The Role of Stimulus-Attribute and Response-Set Selection Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2003, Vol. 29, No. 2, 455 469 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.455

More information

Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs

Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 15 (0), 000±000 Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs Joachim Hoffmann, Andrea Kiesel, and Albrecht Sebald University

More information

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks

Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (3), 504-513 Prime display offset modulates negative priming only for easy-selection tasks Christian Frings Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany and Peter Wühr Friedrich

More information

Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/No-Go and Stop-Signal Paradigms

Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/No-Go and Stop-Signal Paradigms Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Copyright 008 by the American Psychological Association 008, Vol. 137, No. 4, 649 67 0096-3445/08/$1.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0013170 Automatic and Controlled Response

More information

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Sequential Effects in Spatial Exogenous Cueing: Theoretical and Methodological Issues Alessandro Couyoumdjian (alessandro.couyoumdjian@uniroma1.it) Faculty of Psychology 1, University La Sapienza via dei

More information

Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles

Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2008, 15 (5), 945-949 doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.5.945 Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles Xingshan Li and Gordon D. Logan Vanderbilt

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 11: Attention & Decision making Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis

More information

Is the psychological refractory period effect for ideomotor compatible tasks eliminated by speed-stress instructions?

Is the psychological refractory period effect for ideomotor compatible tasks eliminated by speed-stress instructions? Psychological Research (2007) 71: 553 567 DOI 10.1007/s00426-006-0066-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Yun Kyoung Shin Æ Yang Seok Cho Æ Mei-Ching Lien Robert W. Proctor Is the psychological refractory period effect

More information

Temporal expectancy modulates inhibition of return in a discrimination task

Temporal expectancy modulates inhibition of return in a discrimination task Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2010, 17 (1), 47-51 doi:10.3758/pbr.17.1.47 Temporal expectancy modulates inhibition of return in a discrimination task SHAI GABAY AND AVISHAI HENIK Ben-Gurion University

More information

SDT Clarifications 1. 1Colorado State University 2 University of Toronto 3 EurekaFacts LLC 4 University of California San Diego

SDT Clarifications 1. 1Colorado State University 2 University of Toronto 3 EurekaFacts LLC 4 University of California San Diego SDT Clarifications 1 Further clarifying signal detection theoretic interpretations of the Müller Lyer and sound induced flash illusions Jessica K. Witt 1*, J. Eric T. Taylor 2, Mila Sugovic 3, John T.

More information

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2001, 8 (2), 315-323 Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return ANA B. VIVAS City Liberal Studies: Affiliated Institution of the University of Sheffield, Thessaloniki,

More information

Attentional Capture With Rapidly Changing Attentional Control Settings

Attentional Capture With Rapidly Changing Attentional Control Settings Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2010, Vol. 36, No. 1, 1 16 2010 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0015875 Attentional Capture With

More information

Decomposing the Mean: Using Distributional Analyses to Provide a Detailed Description of Addition and Multiplication Latencies

Decomposing the Mean: Using Distributional Analyses to Provide a Detailed Description of Addition and Multiplication Latencies Decomposing the Mean: Using Distributional Analyses to Provide a Detailed Description of Addition and Multiplication Latencies Marcie Penner-Wilger (mpwilger@connect.carleton.ca) Institute of Cognitive

More information

'This article may not exactly replicate the final version published in the APA journal. It is not the copy of record.'

'This article may not exactly replicate the final version published in the APA journal. It is not the copy of record.' Reference: Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G.D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition: associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,

More information

Aging, Emotion, Attention, and Binding in the Taboo Stroop Task: Data and Theories

Aging, Emotion, Attention, and Binding in the Taboo Stroop Task: Data and Theories Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 12803-12833; doi:10.3390/ijerph121012803 OPEN ACCESS Article International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health ISSN 1660-4601 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph

More information

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Exp Brain Res (2002) 142:349 353 DOI 10.1007/s00221-001-0899-2 RESEARCH ARTICLE Richard A. Abrams Mark B. Law Random visual noise impairs object-based attention Received: 10 May 2000 / Accepted: 4 September

More information

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1 The attentional window configures to object boundaries University of Iowa Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 2 ABSTRACT When

More information

Flexible Conflict Management: Conflict Avoidance and Conflict Adjustment in Reactive. Cognitive Control

Flexible Conflict Management: Conflict Avoidance and Conflict Adjustment in Reactive. Cognitive Control 1 RUNNING HEAD: AVOIDANCE OF AND ADJUSTMENT TO CONFLICT Flexible Conflict Management: Conflict Avoidance and Conflict Adjustment in Reactive Cognitive Control David Dignath, Andrea Kiesel, & Andreas B.

More information

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks Memory & Cognition 2005, 33 (6), 1085-1095 Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks IRING KOCH and WOLFGANG PRINZ Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Munich, Germany

More information

TESTING THE INTERACTION OF STIMULUS REPETITION WITH SWITCH COSTS ACROSS AGE GROUPS

TESTING THE INTERACTION OF STIMULUS REPETITION WITH SWITCH COSTS ACROSS AGE GROUPS TESTING THE INTERACTION OF STIMULUS REPETITION WITH SWITCH COSTS ACROSS AGE GROUPS A Thesis Presented to The Academic Faculty by Susan LaGrone In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

More information

Dissociation of S-R Compatibility and Simon Effects With Mixed Tasks and Mappings

Dissociation of S-R Compatibility and Simon Effects With Mixed Tasks and Mappings Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2012, Vol. 38, No. 5, 000 2012 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0029923 Dissociation of S-R Compatibility

More information

Running head: FEATURE INTEGRATION AND TASK SWITCHING 1. Feature Integration and Task Switching:

Running head: FEATURE INTEGRATION AND TASK SWITCHING 1. Feature Integration and Task Switching: Running head: FEATURE INTEGRATION AND TASK SWITCHING 1 Feature Integration and Task Switching: Diminished Switch Costs After Controlling for Stimulus, Response, and Cue Repetitions James R. Schmidt and

More information

Satiation in name and face recognition

Satiation in name and face recognition Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (5), 783-788 Satiation in name and face recognition MICHAEL B. LEWIS and HADYN D. ELLIS Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales Massive repetition of a word can lead to a loss of

More information

Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the processes underlying the object advantage

Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the processes underlying the object advantage Perception & Psychophysics 2007, 69 (4), 606-618 Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the processes underlying the object advantage JAMES M. BROWN AND HOPE I. DENNEY University of Georgia,

More information

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? University of Iowa Honors Theses University of Iowa Honors Program Spring 2017 Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group? Ann Walsh University of Iowa Follow this and additional works at:

More information

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Automatic detection - 1 Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1986, 24 (6), 431-434 SIU L. CHOW The University of Wollongong,

More information

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003)

How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003) Cognition 97 (2005) 89 97 www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit How to trigger elaborate processing? A comment on Kunde, Kiesel, and Hoffmann (2003) Filip Van Opstal a, Bert Reynvoet b, Tom Verguts a, * a Department

More information

Cross-modal attentional deficits in processing tactile stimulation

Cross-modal attentional deficits in processing tactile stimulation Perception & Psychophysics 2001, 63 (5), 777-789 Cross-modal attentional deficits in processing tactile stimulation ROBERTO DELL ACQUA and MASSIMO TURATTO University of Padua, Padua, Italy and PIERRE JOLICŒUR

More information

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2009, 16 (1), 57-61 doi:10.3758/pbr.16.1.57 The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words DANIEL SMILEK, GRAYDEN J. F. SOLMAN, PETER MURAWSKI, AND

More information

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (3), 488-494 Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention BONNIE M. LAWRENCE Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Flexible Conflict Management: Conflict Avoidance and Conflict Adjustment in Reactive Cognitive Control David Dignath, Andrea Kiesel,

More information

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching

The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching The Role of Task-Related Learned Representations in Explaining Asymmetries in Task Switching Ayla Barutchu 1 *, Stefanie I. Becker 2, Olivia Carter 3, Robert Hester 3, Neil L. Levy 1,4 1 Florey Institute

More information

The Task Switching Paradigm. PDF created with pdffactory trial version

The Task Switching Paradigm. PDF created with pdffactory trial version The Task Switching Paradigm Overview Executive functions Task switching paradigms Simple phenomena Complex phenomena Executive Control Executive processes deal with the control of action, thought, and

More information

The path of visual attention

The path of visual attention Acta Psychologica 121 (2006) 199 209 www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy The path of visual attention James M. Brown a, *, Bruno G. Breitmeyer b, Katherine A. Leighty a, Hope I. Denney a a Department of Psychology,

More information

Attention Capture While Switching Search Strategies: Evidence for a Breakdown in Top-Down Attentional Control

Attention Capture While Switching Search Strategies: Evidence for a Breakdown in Top-Down Attentional Control Attention Capture While Switching Search Strategies: Evidence for a Breakdown in Top-Down Attentional Control Lien, M. C., Ruthruff, E., & Naylor, J. (2014). Attention capture while switching search strategies:

More information

Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect

Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect Q. Gao, Z. Chen, P. Russell Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect Quanying Gao, Zhe Chen, & Paul Russell University of Canterbury Although the effect of working memory (WM) load on the

More information

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1994, Vol. 123, No. 3, 316-320 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0096-3445/94/S3.00 COMMENT Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention:

More information

Separating Limits on Preparation Versus Online Processing in Multitasking Paradigms: Evidence for Resource Models

Separating Limits on Preparation Versus Online Processing in Multitasking Paradigms: Evidence for Resource Models Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2017, Vol. 43, No. 1, 89 102 2016 American Psychological Association 0096-1523/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000277 Separating

More information

Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation

Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (7), 1791-1802 doi:10.3758/app.72.7.1791 Parallel response selection in dual-task situations via automatic category-to-response translation SANDRA A J. THOMSON,

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research Vision Research xxx (2008) xxx xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction

More information

Differential Effects of Cue Changes and Task Changes on Task-Set Selection Costs

Differential Effects of Cue Changes and Task Changes on Task-Set Selection Costs Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2003, Vol. 29, No. 3, 362 372 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.362

More information

The role of task rules and stimulus response mappings in the task switching paradigm

The role of task rules and stimulus response mappings in the task switching paradigm Psychological Research () : DOI./s--- ORIGINAL ARTICLE Gesine Dreisbach Æ Thomas Goschke Æ Hilde Haider The role of task rules and stimulus response mappings in the task switching paradigm Received: May

More information