Exploring the Use of a Mobile Robot as an Imitation Agent with Children with Low- Functioning Autism

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Exploring the Use of a Mobile Robot as an Imitation Agent with Children with Low- Functioning Autism"

Transcription

1 1 Exploring the Use of a Mobile Robot as an Imitation Agent with Children with Low- Functioning Autism Audrey Duquette, François Michaud, Henri Mercier Abstract Unpredictability and complexity of social interactions are important challenges for a child with low-functioning autism. The objective of this research is to study how a mobile robot can, by appearing more predictable, appealing and simple than a human being, facilitate reciprocal interaction such as imitative play. By conducting an exploratory study with four children, we found that forms of shared conventions such as imitation of body movements and of familiar actions are higher with two children paired with a human mediator, compared to two children paired with a robot mediator. However, the two children paired with the robot mediator demonstrate increased shared focused attention (visual contact, physical proximity) and in imitating smiling more than the children paired with the human mediator. Index Terms Mobile robotics, Pediatric rehabilitation, Low-functioning autism, Human-robot interaction. Manuscript submitted January 25 th, This work was supported in part by the Canada Research Chair (CRC), the Fonds québécois de recherche sur la nature et les technologies (FQRNT) and the Canadian Foundation for Innovation (CFI). Audrey Duquette was a Master s student at the Department of Psycho-Education of the Université de Sherbrooke, Québec CANADA J1K 2R1 ( audreyduq@hotmail.com). F. Michaud holds the Canada Research Chair in Mobile Robotics and Intelligent Autonomous Systems. He is with the Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Engineering at the Université de Sherbrooke, Québec CANADA J1K 2R1 (phone: x 62107; fax: ; Francois.Michaud@USherbrooke.ca). Contact author. Henri Mercier is a Professor at the Department of Psycho-Education of the Université de Sherbrooke, Québec CANADA J1K 2R1 ( Henri.Mercier@USherbrooke.ca).

2 2 I. INTRODUCTION Autism is one of the Pervasive Developmental Disorder subtypes (e.g., Asperger syndrome, Rett syndrome, Childhood disintegrative disorder, Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified) described in DSM- IV revised diagnostic tool [1], reaching 9 out of people [2]. Even though many intervention programs exist, progress made by children with autism on overcoming their disorders is limited, and no program has yet demonstrated how to initiate a desire within them to learn and communicate with the intent of sharing with others instead of for satisfying primary needs [3]. This work focuses on children aged 5 that are diagnosed with low-functioning autism. These children show very limited language skills and symbolic or pretend play [4,5]. They present similar sensory interests to 8-9 month old typically developing children. However, their sensory play patterns are more repetitive [4], their imitation is selective and used with an aim of increasing the stimuli [6]. They present unexploited abilities (e.g., attribute intentions to the imitator; plan and induce imitative behaviors and understand incitation to imitate) [7,8]. They also show deficits in sharing attention (avoids eye contact, does not respond to smiling) and conventions (poor imitation of facial expressions and gestures such as moving the head to say no or raising a hand to say goodbye) for communicating common interests [6]. Also noted is the quasi-absence of verbal language and pretend play [4]. These deficits are explained by a difficulty in perceiving and understanding stimuli from their environment, affecting their understanding of social signals (gestures, words and intentions of others) [9]. Typically developing 8-9 month old children demonstrate communication intent from eye gaze and imitation of gestures. Thus, assuming that the complexity of human interaction is limiting reciprocal communication of children with autism because of their sensory impairments, we believe that children with low-functioning autism need interventions that take into account their particular interests and their deficits in comprehension by using a predictable and simple medium, able to catch their attention and that is also easy to understand. Mobile robots show potential in this regard because they are predictable, simple and easy to comprehend [10] and they can be designed in accordance of the particular interests and comprehension deficits of children with autism. They can generate more interest and a wide variety of interplay situations compared to static objects, and also bring into play social interactions skills (visual contact, imitation) [11]. More specifically, this exploratory study aims to verify the research hypothesis that an animated object, more predictable and less complex than interacting with humans, can make the child with autism demonstrate reciprocal communication, observed by: 1) the reduction of avoidance mechanisms, namely repetitive and stereotyped play with inanimate objects; 2) an increase in shared focused attention and shared conventions; and 3) the appearance of symbolic mode of communication like verbal language [12]. This paper presents the results of this study, as follows. Section 2 presents a review of related works. Section 3 explains the experimental scenario. Section 4 describes the robot designed to fulfill the requirements of the experimental scenario. Section 5 presents the experimental methodology Section 6 describes the results obtained, and

3 3 the paper concludes with observations made from the results and finally by outlining future work. 2. MOBILE ROBOTS AND AUTISM In 1976, the use of a remote controlled robot with a 7 year old boy diagnosed with autism was, we believe, the first use of mobile robotic devices as a remedial tool for children [13]. In this work, the mobile robot was used to catalyze communication in the boy, and positive results were reported. The AuRoRA project (Autonomous Robotic platform as a Remedial tool for children with Autism) [14,15,16,17] is certainly now one of the best-known initiatives within this subject area. AuRoRA investigates how a robot can become a `toy' that could possibly serve an educational or therapeutic role for children with autism. They hope to encourage children with autism in coordinated and synchronized interactions with the environment so as to help them develop and increase their communication and social interaction skills [14]. Previously, the project conducted experiments with a Labo-1 mobile platform, a robot with a rectangular body and four wheels, based on the observation that children with autism prefer predictable, stable environments, and that they have difficulty interpreting facial expressions and other social cues. The AuRoRA project was attempting to use the robot to bridge the gap between the complex and unpredictable world of human social behavior and the safe predictable world of simple toys. It was shown that children with autism are capable of distinguishing between animated and unanimated objects, expressing greater attention, laughter and vocal responses toward a robotized platform compared with a toy truck of same dimension [14,16,18]. Other experiments conducted by AuRoRA include a humanoid doll, Robota [19], engaged in interactions with children with autism. Robota has been deployed in various experiments including the investigation of the possible therapeutic effects of a humanoid robotic doll to autism therapy. Their approach was based on the assumption that bodily interaction in imitative interaction games is normally an important factor in a child's development of social skills and that teaching of such skills (in a playful and exploratory context but nevertheless from an educational point of view focusing explicitly on specific types of interactions) could help children with autism in coping with the normal dynamics of social interactions [20]. Robota, with its bidirectional communication capability (requests imitation and imitates) has been used to investigate the effect of a robot's design (appearance) in facilitating and encouraging interaction of children with autism [21]. In a longitudinal study, Robota was also used with the aim to encourage imitation and social interaction skills. Different behavioral criteria (including Eye Gaze, Touch, and Imitation) were evaluated based on the video data of the interactions [10]. Other uses of Robota include the study of joint attention [22] and also being used as a salient object mediating joint attention between four children [23]. The experimental methodology followed a case study evaluation using an analysis of interaction informed by conversation analytic principles [24]. Related to this is the work of Kozima and Yano [25,26], suggesting that an infanoid robot (a humanoid torso designed to have a youthful appearance) programmed to play contingency-games could benefit both children with autism and typically developing children in learning communication skills. Pioggia et al. [27] developed a robotic head with believable facial display and explored its use in social skills and emotional therapy for an individual with autism. Finally, work by Scassellati [28] is investigating the use of robots to help diagnose autism.

4 4 The initiative described in this paper differs from the AuRoRA project. The motivation is to determine how a robot's motion, communication capabilities, and appearance can engage a child in interaction and once engaged, to investigate the ability of the robot to initiate and sustain the child's interest and motivation to learn, thus facilitating in the development of social and communication skills. We achieve this by designing and building custom-made mobile robots with the unique needs of children diagnosed with autism in mind. Premack [29] advances the hypothesis that the perception of intentionality in moving elements is innate to humans. An infant divides the world in two classes of objects, i.e., whether or not it is self-propelled. For self-propelled objects, an infant has the ability to distinguish the objects that can generate a change in their movements, with or without the assistance of another [30]. In the case of an autonomous change (excluding cyclic movements such as those associated with a bouncing ball), Premack suggests that infants perceive intentionality and show preference toward these objects. While adults can identify the nature of a self-propelled object and associate intentionality to it, for infants what matters is not the object but the type of movements it generates [31] and its velocity [32,33]. A child is also able to attribute self-propelled capabilities once having seen the robot intentionally moving, and this association will continue even when the robot has stopped [29]. Preliminary work [34,35,36] confirms that mobile robots, with their motion capabilities, their appearances, their sounds and their decision-making capabilities, have the ability to not only engage, but also to hold the attention of children with autism. This study wants to go one step further and design a mobile robot used specifically to validate the hypothesis outlined in the introduction. 3. EXPERIMENTAL SCENARIO Our experimental scenario consists of having a group of children interact with a robotic mobile mediator (animated object with human-like appearance), and another group interact with a human mediator. The two mediators execute the similar imitation play patterns involving three levels, namely facial expressions (joy, sadness, angry), body movements (raise the arms, dance, move forward or backward), familiar actions with objects (to point to the hat and request that it is given back, to point at the door, to point at the mediator s picture) or familiar actions without objects (wave hello or bye-bye, play peek-a-boo). For familiar actions, the mediator executes the actions and names it out loud using simple words. Based on the deficits of children with low-functioning autism and of their similarities to 8-9 month old typically developing children, twenty activity sessions were elaborated, following the same general scenario, i.e.: Before entering the experimental room (a square room), the educator explains to the child that a play period is planned by showing a picture of the mediator. This picture is either put on the door or on the wall inside the room, depending on the activity session. The child enters the room, and 12 seconds later the educator directs the child to the chair placed in the center. The child s favorite toy is placed at its feet. The educator then sits on the other chair placed near the door. A panel is placed in the room so that the mediator can hide behind it. Execute an activity session. Each activity session involves the three levels of imitation (facial expressions, body movements and familiar actions). They are organized in increasing complexity (initially focussing more on facial

5 5 expressions and then progress onto body movements and familiar actions) to facilitate the child s familiarization with the mediator s action. The activity sessions are composed of the imitative play patterns during which the mediator asks the child to imitate the play. The twenty imitative play patterns are composed as follows (identified by their session identification numbers): 1) 2) express happiness twice, and then anger once (after colliding with a chair, indicating no by nodding); 3) smile and then express happiness, twice; 4) 5) 6) say hello twice, and then dance twice; 7) move both arms and then express happiness; 8) move both arms, and then say hello; 9) move forward and then move backward, twice; 10) say hello twice and move forward; 11) say hello, move forward and move backward; 12) 13) say hello, point to the hat and ask for the hat back, twice; 14) say hello, move backward, point to the photo of the mediator; 15) say hello twice, move backward, point the photo of the mediator; 16) 17) say hello, and express sadness twice after colliding with the panel; 18) say hello, ask the child to point the photo of the mediator twice; 19) say hello, ask to point the photo of the mediator and play peek-a-boo twice hiding behind the panel; 20) say hello, ask to point the photo of the mediator and play peek-a-boo four times. Imitative play patterns in an activity session are executed in sequence, each one followed by a 12 sec observation period. The reason why some actions are repeated twice in play patterns is to let the child integrate better the sequence of actions in the play, in hope of getting him or her to react more. At the beginning of the activity session, the mediator is either hiding behind the panel (2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 16, 17) or close to the child s chair (being at a two arms length distance for patterns 1, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13; or at one arm length distance for patterns 14, 15, 18, 19 20). At the end of each session, the mediator points the door and says Door to indicate to the child that the activity is ending. The mediator then waves and says Bye bye, and goes behind the panel. When the educator goes toward the child to leave the room, the robot exits from behind the panel, waves and says Bye bye, and again goes back behind the panel. In all of the activities, if the child imitates correctly the mediator s activity, the mediator smiles and raises both its arms and says Happy!. 4. TITO THE ROBOT MEDIATOR Designing a mobile robot that can implement such activity sessions is a challenge, even if teleoperation (i.e., Wizard-of-Oz experimental settings) is used to navigate the robot and respond to the child's interaction. The four major design considerations in designing such a robot are: (1) Overall Requirements - The robot should be able to fulfill all the abilities required in the activity session; (2) Longevity Study - The robot should be able to correctly function for a period of approximately two months; (3) Operator - The robot is to be operated by non-robotic experts; (4) Experimental Analysis Identifying the data that will be analyzed during evaluation can influence the design, the choice of sensors and the capabilities of the robot. Tito is the robot we developed to address these specifications, and is shown in Figure 1. Tito is 28 inches tall and is colored red, yellow, and blue. Its clothes are washable and made of soft material. Tito has wheels to move, but its

6 6 structure shows two feet and two legs to emulate a humanoid shape. It has two arms that can move up and down rapidly, a head that can rotate (making a no gesture) and rise up (expressing surprise for making the peek-a-boo gesture), a mouth (using light emitting diodes, for smiling), two eyes, a nose, hair (made from fiber optic cable that can be illuminated) and a hat. Also, since we were interested in measuring eye gaze toward Tito, a small wireless microphone-camera device was installed in one eye of the robot. Different parts of Tito's body can also be illuminated, and it is able to sense if it is being shaken or if it has flipped over. Tito is sufficiently robust to sustain rough interplay situations, and it is heavy enough so that it is difficult for a child to lift. It can sustain autonomous action for approximately 1 hour before needing to be recharged. Tito was built in less than 6 months using modular distributed subsystems previously designed or used on other robots (e.g., energy monitoring, locomotion control, remote operation, sensing) [37], and a sound-generating device similar to the ones used on the RoboToys and Roball [34,36]. The materials used to develop Tito cost approximately 2500 $CAD. Fig. 1.Tito. Tito generates vocal requests through pre-recorded messages, using a masculine voice with intonations that can be interrogative, neutral or cheerful. Tito has a vocabulary of 25 words, which are used either in isolation or with gestures and illuminated parts. The use of pre-recorded messages facilitates the use of appropriate voice intonations, for which children with autism are more sensitive to than simply the use of words [38]. A wireless remote control (using a Playstation video game controller) is used for teleoperation, and an on-board microcontroller enables preprogrammed sequences of behaviors (motion and vocal messages) for Tito. Examples of pre-programmed behaviors with vocal responses are: raising its left arm and saying Hello! ; moving the left arm up and down three times whilst

7 7 saying Bye bye ; pointing at an object whilst saying what the object is; making its hat fall and saying Give hat ; playing hide-and-seek and saying Search Tito or Tito search, and once found saying Peek-a-boo! ; expressing happiness by raising both arms, illuminating its mouth and saying Happy! ; smiling whilst illuminating a body part; raising one arm and saying Arm ; moving forward or backward and saying Forward or Backward ; singing and rotating on the spot by alternatively raising each arm; or rotating its head and saying No. Tito also emits a sound when it starts the execution of an experimental scenario, allowing synchronization of video data recorded with an external camera. Tito s activation button is hidden at the bottom of the robot so that the child is not tempted to play with it. The timing between the interactions of the child (from sensory data and according to the experimental scenarios) is recorded and stored internally on-board Tito. 5. EXPERIMENTAL METHODOLOGY Our methodology consists of conducting an exploratory study following a single case protocol [39]. More specifically, an ABA / AB A model is adopted, where A represents the baseline level of children with autism with the presence of the experimenter. The sample of children with autism is divided into two groups: one group paired with the robot mediator (B), and the other with the human mediator (B ). Adopting a single case protocol for this study is appropriate when considering the research hypothesis, i.e., studying the effect of a change in an individual and to establish links between variables [40]. The children that participated in the trials are all diagnosed with low-functioning autism. They show a deficit in imitation, also a severe delay in receptive and expressive language and have a sensori-motor interest that interferes with the development of their communication skills. They can, however, have very different stereotyped repetitive play patterns and interests. To ensure homogeneity of the population and a valid baseline level, the following measures were taken. Children selected were diagnosed with autism as specified by the ADOS-G (Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic) module 1, showing severe disorders in receptive and expressive language skills and deficits in symbolic games. Children reactions were observed and evaluated for 10 pre-determined actions of the experimenter, using a pre-determined set of criteria. A third criterion is the result of the imitation tool PEP-R (Psycho Educational Profile Review) [41]. These measures, supported also by the comments gathered from the educators regularly working with the children, provided an appropriate baseline level for the composition of the sample group. Selection of the children was done following a reasoned non-probabilistic sampling, based on the experimenter s judgment and in accordance to the subjects character [40]. All children were part of the same intervention group from the Centre de réadaptation le Florès, Laurentides Québec. From the nine possible children around 5 years of age available in this group, eight parents provided their consent, two were disqualified because they were able to talk (few words), with one of them receiving private specialized care. Two others were also removed from the sample because they attended the school only once a week. Therefore, the sample consisted of a homogeneous group of four children addressing all criteria, 3 boys and 1 girl. Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of the participants according

8 8 to these selection criteria. Two of them were paired with the robot mediator (identified using labels R1 and R2 for anonymous identification), and the other two children were paired with the human mediator (labeled E1 and E2). Based on the similarities between the participants characteristics, child R1 is to be compared with child E1, and child R2 with child E2. The overall average age is 5 years and 1 month old. Table 1 Population group for the trials. Participants R1 R2 E1 E2 Age (years :months) 4 :4 5 :1 5 :5 5 :0 Communication Non-verbal Pre-verbal Non-verbal Pre-verbal Stimulation Visual and vestibular stimulation Visual stimulation Visual, audible and tactile stimulation Visual, vestibular and audible stimulation Mannerism Yes No Yes No Imitation (PEP-R, over 16 items) Success = 0 Emerging = 2 Success = 5 Emerging = 6 Success = 0 Emerging = 1 Success = 4 Emerging = 5 Stimulation (hours/week) The trials were conducted in a 4.5 m 3.3 m room with a window, a chair and a toy familiar to the child placed in the center of the room, the imitating agent (either Tito for children R1 and R2, or the experimenter playing the role of the human mediator for children E1 and E2), the educator s chair placed next to the door, a panel, a video camera and a helper. The experimenter has previous clinical experience with children with autism, is 1.56 m tall and knelt during the activity sessions. The experimenter was not known by the children prior to the trials, but they did meet a couple of times before executing the experimental scenario. Before executing the experimental scenario, trials were conducted with three children not part of the selected sample of children with autism, to assess the feasibility of the experimental methodology, the validity of the evaluation sheets, correct operation of the robot and the potential reactions from children. This enabled us to make adjustments to the experimental scenario. For instance, the introductory phase before an activity session (explained in Section 3) was derived from observations made in these pre-experimental trials. The time interval for each action in an imitative play pattern was readjusted from 20 seconds (based on [42]) to 12 seconds, after observing that children showed disinterest after this length of time. These pre-experimental trials also revealed the importance of conducting a session that allowed the child to become familiar with the robot mediator prior to the actual experimental trials, limiting therefore the novelty effects. This familiarization session consists of six steps: 1) the robot is off and located in a corner of the room; 2) the robot is activated (with some illuminated parts) but remains immobile; 3) the robot is

9 9 activated and then proceeds to move around following the wall, without going too close to the child; 4) the robot moves toward the child without touching and 5) touching; 6) the robot moves its arm and emits vocal messages. To progress from one step to the other, the child had to show self-initiated interest toward Tito by going toward it and touching it. These steps were repeated until no sign of distress or discomfort were observed. Note that when Tito was used as the imitating agent, the experimenter remained in a corner of the room, teleoperating the robot according to the experimental scenarios. The educator s role during the trials is to provide assistance depending on the action in the imitative play patterns and the child s behavior (e.g., put the robot s hat back if the child did not comply to the request before 12 seconds or if he or she is having difficulties putting the hat back on). Each child was exposed a total of 22 times to its paired mediator, at a rate of three times per week over seven weeks and a maximum of two times per day (with a 15 minutes free play time in between). All the activity sessions were recorded. The baseline level (A), conducted in the first and the 22 nd exposure, consisted of the mediator carrying out the following actions: say hello, express happiness, point and give back the hat, point to the mediator, show the door to exit and say goodbye. The other twenty sessions (B/B ) were conducted by carrying out the activity sessions following the experimental scenario described in Section 3. Each activity session is repeated twice during a trial. 6. RESULTS To analyze what happened during the trials, twenty-one evaluation sheets (i.e, one for the A and twenty for the B/B ) were created, i.e., one for each activity session plus one for the baseline level. Each evaluation sheet lists the child s expressions or reactions observed according to the mediator s imitative play patterns. These expressions or reactions and the terminology were set according to the work of Camaioni and Aureli [43] and observations made during the pre-experimental sessions. The following four categories of variables (i.e., expressions or reactions) are observed during a session: Shared focused attention (4 variables): visual contact / eye gaze directed toward the mediator for longer than 3 sec. [43], physical proximity (moving closer to two arm length distance from the mediator, with visual contact), imitation of facial expressions (happiness, anger, sadness) or imitation of gestures that is not directed toward the mediator. Shared focused attention here does not include joint visual attention, which would involve having the child direct attention to an object that the mediator is looking at. Shared focused attention means that the child shows an interest in the mediator s eyes or body movements, or is responding to having seen the mediator s facial expressions or gestures by imitating them. Shared conventions (4 variables): imitation of facial expressions, gestures, actions and words, all directed toward the mediator, with the purpose of sharing and communicating with the mediator. Shared conventions occur when both the mediator and the child refer to a common point of interest through conventional means, such as imitation of facial expressions, gestures, actions and words.

10 10 Absence of sharing (7 variables): no visual contact or visual contact lasting less than 3 sec., leave the communication area, avoid the mediator, repetitive sensori-motor play with their favorite toy, mannerisms, ritual, aggression. Other phenomenon (4 variables): imitation of noise made by the mediator moving (e.g., motor noise with Tito), imitation of words or gestures out of context, or any other behavior not listed in the previous categories. Dichotomic coding (presence or absence) of these variables was done over 12 sec. windowing, by two coders (98% fidelity, based on data from the pre-experimental sessions) looking at the video footage of the sessions. If the child exhibited both sharing and absence of sharing in the same 12 sec. window, the presence of reciprocal communication was prioritized. This data was used to conduct chronological series analysis with cross-section multiple regression analysis between mediators (with ** p < 0.05). The results do not reveal significant variances between the baseline sessions (A, one conducted at the beginning of the experimental scenario and one conducted at the end of the experimental scenario). Therefore, analysis focuses on the child s behavior in relation to the activity session and the child s mediator, for which significant influences appeared. The results are presented in the following sections. 6.1 Shared focused attention Figure 2 illustrates the chronological analysis for the shared focused attention variables. The proportion of sharing is calculated by summing the number of occurrences of shared focused attention variables and dividing it with the number of variables planned for the activity session. Non-verbal participants (R1 and E1) demonstrated less shared focused attention than pre-verbal children (R2 and E2). Also, children paired with the robot mediator demonstrated more shared focused attention than the ones paired with the human mediator. Fig. 2.Chronological series analysis for shared focused attention variables. Analyzing the results for the robot mediator reveals a statistically significant higher proportion in shared focused

11 11 attention, in comparison to the human mediator trials, for only two of the four variables (visual contact: standardized regression coefficient =0.35**; physical proximity: =0.40**), while a smaller proportion is observed for undirected gesture ( =-0.28**). This means that children paired with the robot exhibited more visual contact and proximity with their mediator compared to the children paired with the experimenter, and demonstrated less gestures that were not directed toward the robot mediator compared to the children paired with the human mediator. Fig. 3.Occurrences of shared focused attention variables in relation to the imitative play patterns. To further analyze this observation, we examined more closely the sessions during which children paired with the robot mediator (R1 and R2) performed better that the children paired with the human mediator (E1 and E2) (i.e., sessions 1, 4, 11 to 14, 16 and 17). Figure 3 shows which imitative play patterns initiated such increases in shared focused attention. R1 and R2 were more interested by the expression of happiness and sadness by Tito compared to the same expression by the experimenter. This was displayed by visual contact, physical proximity and laughter (when sadness was expressed). For actions with objects (like pointing and giving back the hat), R1 and R2 made more visual contact and moved toward the robot, while E1 and E2 preferred to engage into a repetitive play routine with the hat. For actions without objects, even though all children showed interest in imitative play (such as saying hello and goodbye), R1 and R2 had more eye gaze directed toward the robot and they also moved closer to the robot. When comparing children with the same language level (R1 with E1, and R2 with E2), we observe that children paired with the robot mediator exhibited more shared focused attention, mostly in activities involving facial expressions, actions with objects and actions without objects. Also, because of their severe deficits, non-verbal

12 12 children displayed less shared focused attention than pre-verbal children in all activities. 6.2 Shared conventions Figure 4 illustrates the chronological analysis for shared convention variables. Again, non-verbal participants (R1 and E1) demonstrated less shared conventions than pre-verbal children (R2 and E2). Only one of the four variables shows statistically significant results, with a decrease in the imitation of words ( =-0.44**). This indicates that pairing a child with autism with the robot mediator had a negative influence on the imitation of words. For the other variables, even though they revealed to be not statistically significant, they indicate that pairing a child with the robot mediator led to an increase in facial expression (mostly happiness, for R2 compared to E2) but a decrease in the imitation of gestures and actions. This is understandable considering Tito s limited physical capabilities. Proportion of shared conventions 0,06 0,05 0,04 0,03 0,02 0,01 0 R1 R2 E1 E Exposures Fig. 4.Chronological series analysis for shared convention variables. Figure 5 illustrates the number of imitations exhibited for shared conventions in relation to the mediator s imitative play patterns. Shared conventions such as imitation of body movements and of familiar actions are higher with the two children paired with the human mediator (E1 and E2). The two children paired with the robot mediator (R1, R2) imitate facial expressions more than the children paired with the human mediator (E1, E2). Imitation of words mostly appeared only for one participant (E2), who started to imitate saying Bye bye at the end of the sessions, even initiating saying these words before the human mediator, two times in context and one time out of context. E2 also imitated facial expression (smiling), some gestures (raising an arm) and some actions (hello and goodbye gestures), but non-related to objects (contrary to R2).

13 13 Fig. 5.Occurrences of imitation in relation to shared convention variables. 6.3 Absence of sharing In accordance with Figure 2 and Figure 4, periods when there was an absence of sharing were observed more with non-verbal children (R1 and E1) compared to pre-verbal children (R2 and E2). Two variables showed statistically significant influences, mainly that the children paired with the robot mediator (R1 and R2) left the communication area ( = 0.38*) and initiated rituals ( = 0.17*) more compared to the children paired with the human mediator. Leaving the communication area may be surprising because, as described in Section 6.1, R1 and R2 showed increased proximity with the mediator. However, R1 and R2 also presented less absence of sharing than E1 and E2 (but statistically non-significant: no visual contact =-0.32; repetitive sensori-motor play =-0.03).

14 Discussion and other observations The results indicate that non-verbal children demonstrate less interest and participation than pre-verbal children. However, children exposed to the robotic mediator showed reduced repetitive plays with inanimate objects of interest (their favorite toy), and no repetitive or stereotyped behavior toward the robot. These reduced behaviors can be explained by the interest children have for the sensory properties of the robot (movements, colors, lights). Their attention is focused on the robot rather than on the inanimate object. These results are the ones reported in [14,18] indicating increased attention directed by children with autism towards a robot compared to an object such as a toy truck. The trials also revealed that children exposed to the robot mediator demonstrated more visual contact and proximity compared to children paired with the human mediator. This confirms the hypothesis that shared focused attention is facilitated by the appealing characteristics and predictability of the robot. In fact, when the robot expressed emotions of joy and sadness or made simple actions, children with autism reacted to the voice intonation, the lights that represented emotions and the simple slow motion of the robot, by looking and moving toward the robot. Tito was designed to facilitate comprehension of these interaction cues by children with autism. For shared conventions however, the results do not confirm that interactions increased with the robot mediator. Smaller occurrence of imitation by the children paired with the robot can be explained by many factors. First, having two non-verbal children with very low-functioning autism does not allow us to observe use of words or imitation to communicate. Nadel [7,8] indicates that this form of communication needs some cognitive and meta-cognitive abilities that appear after 18 months old, such as the capacities to attribute intentions to the imitator and to understand incitation to imitate. These abilities were not observed with the non-verbal children with autism (R1 and E1). Looking at the pre-verbal children (R2 and E2), the child paired with the robot (R2) presented less shared conventions in general than the child paired with human. This observation is explained by the difficulty in understanding communication intent from gestures, actions and words made by Tito. Tito was not used to imitate the child after the child imitated it, making the imitative plays unidirectional. Tito also lacked reciprocity compared to a human being. The use of a different robot like Robota [11] has shown better performance in responding to the child s gestures. The pre-verbal child with autism exposed to the human mediator (E2) showed better imitation in familiar actions and words (waving and saying goodbye), but used it as a means of getting out of the room and ending the interaction. On the other hand, in the study, the imitation of facial expression of joy (smiling) appears more with children exposed to Tito than with children exposed to the experimenter. The results are explained by the simplicity to comprehend the expression of the robot (illuminated smile). Smiling is the first indicator of communication named by Spitz [44]. Tito s capabilities were therefore sufficient to allow the pre-verbal child with autism (R2) to develop this first form of communication. The pre-verbal child with autism exposed to Tito also showed better imitation of actions with objects (giving back the hat to the robot, pointing toward the door) and sustained interactions with the robot. Emergent interactions were noted with the pre-verbal child with autism paired with the robot. They were

15 15 observed through the imitation of gestures (handing back the hat, pointing at the door, waving good bye) and imitation of motor noises at the same rhythm as the robot (more specifically when the robot was waving). Furthermore, the child reproduced the same posture as the robot (e.g., by kneeling). The Ricard and Gouin-Décarie familiarization process [45] was observed in the trials with children exposed to robot mediator, by sharing attention to get interaction (proximity, smiling, visual contact). In general, children with low-functioning autism do not initiate interaction appropriately. They often use adults as objects to satisfy their primary needs, and they can be avoidant or overbearing. Further analysis of video footage revealed that the robot appears to be an interesting way in helping children initiate contact, something that typically developing children can do when meeting strangers. Children firstly created a distance with the robot, allowing them to initiate interactions such as eye contact and smiles. Then children moved closer to the robot, enabling them to continue interacting with the robot. Even when the robot hid behind the panel, children enthusiastically sought the robot. Despite at times the children displaying ritual behavior and at times also leaving the communication area, which can be associated to an avoidance behavior, they did so as part of such familiarization process. This pattern was not observed with the children paired to the human mediator, nor did behavior associated with a Ricard and Gouin-Décarie familiarization process. Finally, another interesting observation is that children eventually made the discovery that Tito was teleoperated, which generated enthusiastic interactions between the child and the experimenter. This concurs with Robins et al.'s [11] observation that a robotic device reveals to be an interesting intermediate between the child and an educator. II. CONCLUSION The goal of this work is to address the research hypothesis that an animated object like a robot, more predictable and less complex in its interacting modalities compared with a human, can make a child with autism demonstrate a reduction in the use of avoidance mechanisms, namely repetitive and stereotyped play patterns with inanimate objects, increase in shared focused attention and shared conventions; and facilitate the appearance of symbolic modes of communication like verbal language. The results confirm only parts of the research hypothesis. Children paired with the robot mediator showed increased shared focused attention (visual contact, physical proximity) than the children paired with the human mediator in all types of imitation play patterns including facial expressions, body movements, familiar actions with objects and familiar actions without objects. This validates that Tito the robot has appealing characteristics for interacting with children with autism. However, forms of shared conventions such as imitation of body movements and of familiar actions are higher with the two children paired with the human. This may be explained by with the children with low-functioning autism having more difficulty in understanding the communication intent from the limited motion capabilities of the robot. Even though conducting an exploratory study following a single case protocol has known limitations such as the

16 16 difficulty to generalize results [39], it revealed to be a valuable experience in terms of validating the use of a customdesigned mobile robot as a pedagogical tool for children with autism. Throughout the sessions, Tito was easy to use and required no maintenance during the experiments. In addition, no session had to be interrupted because a child felt uneasy in the presence of the mediator, or behaved inappropriately. The study gives insight into the processes needed for decreasing children s anguish and increasing their attention to learn or to see forms of communication emerge. The results are very encouraging and support the continuation of work on this research question. As such we plan to repeat these trials with a greater number of subjects and consolidate these conclusions. The role of the robot is not to replace human educators but rather to assist them in helping a child acquire social and communication skills that can later be exploited in social interaction with people. To do so, future work will also involve a typically developing child serving as a model for understanding the communication intents of the robot to help children with autism develop interaction skills. ACKNOWLEDGMENT The authors would like to thank the CRDI Le Florès, Laurentides, for their collaboration with this work. Thanks also to Tamie Salter for her helpful comments on this manuscript. REFERENCES [1] American Psychiatric Association, DSM-IV-TR Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV Text Revision, [2] E. Fombonne, Études épidémiologiques de l autisme et des troubles apparentés, Psychiatrie, recherche et intervention en santé mentale de l enfant (PRISME), Approcher l énigme de l autisme, 34: 26-32, [3] P. Doehring, Programmes d intervention comportementale auprès des enfants autistes: quelles sont les meilleurs? Psychiatrie, recherche et intervention en santé mentale de l enfant (PRISME), Approcher l énigme de l autisme, 34: 80-91, [4] R. Blanc, M. Gomot, M.P. Gattegno, C. Barthélémy, J.L. Adrien, Les troubles de l'activité symbolique chez des enfants autistes, dysphasiques et retardés mentaux et l'effet de l'étayage de l'adulte. Revue Québécoise de Psychologie, [5] A. M. Wetherby, C. A. Prutting, Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children, Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28: , [6] M. Lemay, L'autisme aujourd'hui. Paris:Éd. Odile Jacob, [7] J. Nadel, Imitation and imitation recognition: Functional use in preverbal infants and nonverbal children with autism. The Imitative Mind Development Evolution and Brain Bases. Eds Meltzoff and Prinz, [8] J. Nadel, A. Revel, P. Andy, Ph. Gaussier, Toward communication, first imitations in infants, low-functioning children with autism and robots. Interaction Studies, 5(1):45-74, 2004.

17 17 [9] M. Zilbovicius, Imagerie cérébrale et autisme infantile. Revue Cerveau et Psychologie, [10] J. Nadel, Imitation et autisme, Revue Cerveau et Psycho, 68-71, [11] B. Robins, K. Dautenhahn, R. te Boekhorst, A. Billard, Effects of repeated exposure to a humanoid robot on children with autism, in Proc. Cambridge Workshop on Universal Access and Assistive Technology, Springer- Verlag, pp , [12] A. Duquette, L exploration d une intervention avec un robot-jouet animé chez les enfants autistes, Master s thesis, Department of Psycho-Education, Université de Sherbrooke, [13] S. Weir, R. Emanuel, Using LOGO to catalyse communication in an autistic child. Technical Report DAI Research Report No. 15, University of Edinburgh, [14] K. Dautenhahn, Design issues on interactive environments for children with autism, in Proc. International Conference on Disability, Virtual Reality and Associated Technologies, [15] K. Dautenhahn, I. Werry, J. Rae, P. Dickerson, P. Stribling, B. Ogden, Robotic playmates: Analysing interactive competencies of children with autism playing with a mobile robot, in Intelligent Agents Creating Relationships with Computer and Robots, Kluwer Academic Publishers, [16] K. Dautenhahn and I. Werry, A quantitative technique for analyzing robot-human interactions, in Proc. IEEE/RSJ Int. Conf. on Intelligent Robots and Systems, pp , [17] K. Dautenhahn, I. Werry, Towards interactive robots in autism therapy: Background, motivation and challenges, Pragmatics and Cognition, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 1-35, [18] I. Werry, K. Dautenhahn, W. Harwin, Evaluating the response of children with autism to a robot, in Proc. Rehabilitation Engineering and Assistive Technology Society of North America (RESNA), [19] A. Billard, Play, dreams and imitation in Robota, in Proc. Workshop on Interactive Robotics and Entertainment, pp , [20] K. Dautenhahn, A. Billard, Games children with autism can play with Robota, a humanoid robotic doll, in Proc. Cambridge Workshop on Universal Access and Assistive Technology, Springer-Verlag, pp , [21] B. Robins, K. Dautenhahn, R. te Boekhorst, A. Billard, Robots as assistive technology Does appearance matter? in Proc. IEEE International Workshop on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, [22] B. Robins, P. Dickerson, P. Stribling, K. Dautenhahn, Robot-mediated joint attention in children with autism: A case study in robot-human interaction, Interaction Studies: Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems, 5(2): , [23] B. Robins, P. Dickerson, K. Dautenhahn, Robots as embodied beings Interactionally sensitive body movements in interactions among autistic children and a robot, Proceedings IEEE International Workshop on Robots and Human Interactive Communication,

18 18 [24] B. Robins, P. Dickerson, P. Stribling, K. Dautenhahn, Robot-mediated joint attention in therapy and education of children with autism: can a small humanoid robot help encourage social interaction skills?, Universal Access in the Information Society, 4(2), [25] H. Kozima and H. Yano, Designing a robot for contingency-detection game, in Proc. International Workshop on Robotic and Virtual Agents in Autism Therapy, [26] H. Kozima, C. Nakagawa, Y. Yasuda, Wowing together: What facilitates social interactions in children with autistic spectrum disorders, Proceedings International Workshop on Epigenetic Robotics, Modeling Cognitive Development in Robotics Systems, 177-, [27] G. Pioggia, M. Ferro, M. L. Sica, G. Dalle Mura, S. Casalini, D. De Rossi, R. Igliozzi, F. Muratori, Imitation and learning of the emotional behaviour: Towards an android-based treatment for people with autism, Proceedings International Workshop on Epigenetic Robotics, Modeling Cognitive Development in Robotics Systems, , [28] B. Scassellati, Quantitative metrics of social response for autism diagnosis, Proceedings IEEE International Workshop on Robots and Human Interactive Communication, [29] D. Premack, The infant s theory of self-propelled objects, Cognition, 26: 1-16, [30] M. D. Hauser, A nonhuman primate s expectation about object motion and destination: The importance of selfpropelled movement and animacy, Developmental Science, 1(1): 31-37, [31] R. Sekuler, Visual motion perception, Handbook of Perception: Volume 5 Seeing, E. C. Carterette and M. P. Friedman (Eds), Academic Press, , [32] R. N. Aslin, S. L. Shea, Velocity threshold in human infants: implications for the perception of motion, Developmental Psychology, 26: , [33] J. L. Dannemiller, R. Freeland, The detection of slow stimulus movement in 2- to 5- month olds, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47: , [34] F. Michaud and C. Théberge-Turmel, Mobile robotic toys and autism, in Socially Intelligent Agents Creating Relationships with Computers and Robots, K. Dautenhahn, A. Bond, L. Canamero and B. Edmonds, eds, Kluwer Academic, pp , [35] F. Michaud, A. Duquette, and I. Nadeau, Characteristics of mobile robotic toys for children with pervasive developmental disorders, in Proc. IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, pp [36] F. Michaud, T. Salter, A. Duquette, and J.-F. Laplante, Perspectives on mobile robots used as tools for pediatric rehabilitation, to appear in Assistive Technologies, 2007.

19 19 [37] F. Michaud, D. Létourneau, M. Arsenault, Y. Bergeron, R. Cadrin, F. Gagnon, M.-A. Legault, M. Millette, J.-F. Paré, M.-C. Tremblay, P. Lepage, Y. Morin, S. Caron, Multi-modal locomotion robotic platform using legtrack-wheel articulations, Autonomous Robots, Special Issue on Unconventional Robotic Mobility, 18(2): , [38] J.-L. Adrien, Autisme du jeune enfant. Développement psychologique et regulation de l activité, Collection Autisme, éd Expansion Scientifique Française, [39] A.E. Kazdin, Statistical analysis for single-case experimental designs. Single Case Experimental Designs. Eds Hersen and Barlow, [40] M.-F. Fortin, Le processus de la recherche, de la conception à la réalisation, Québec Décarie Éditeur, [41] E. Schopler, R.J. Reichler, A. Bashford, M.D. Lansing, L.M. Marcus, The Psychoeducational Profile Revised (PEP-R). Austin Pro-Ed., [42] T. Charman, S. Baron-Cohen, J. Swettenham, G. Baird, A. Cox, A. Drew, Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind, Cognitive Development, 15: , [43] L. Camaioni, T. Aureli, Trajectoires développementales et individuelles de la transition vers la communication symbolique. Enfance, 3, [44] R.A. Spitz (1976). De la naissance à la parole : la première année de vie. Paris, Presse Universitaires de France, 5 e éd., [45] M. Ricard, T. Gouin-Décarie, L humain et l inanimé pour l enfant de 9-10 mois. Enfance, 45 (4), , 1990.

20 20 Audrey Duquette received her bachelor s degree ( 02) in psychoeducation and she has completed her Master s degree ( 05) in psychoeducation at the Université de Sherbrooke. Her research interests cover interventions using play, imitation and mobile robot for children with autism to improve their communication and social interaction. Since 2004, she is working at Ste-Justine Hospital as a psychoeducator performing assessments and follow-up of children between the ages of 0 to 5 years old, mainly with pervasive development disorders and autism. Audrey Duquette is a member of OCCOPPQ (Ordre des conseillères et conseillers en orientation et psychoéducatrices et psychoéducateurs du Québec). François Michaud (M 90) received his bachelor s degree ( 92), Master s degree ( 93) and Ph.D. degree ( 96) in electrical engineering from the Université de Sherbrooke, Québec Canada. After completing postdoctoral work at Brandeis University, Waltham MA ( 97), he became a faculty member in the Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Engineering of the Université de Sherbrooke, and founded LABORIUS, a research laboratory working on designing intelligent autonomous systems that can assist humans in living environments. His research interests are in architectural methodologies for intelligent decision-making, design of autonomous mobile robotics, social robotics, robot for children with autism, robot learning and intelligent systems. Prof. Michaud is the Canada Research Chairholder in Autonomous Mobile Robots and Intelligent Systems. He is a member of IEEE, AAAI and OIQ (Ordre des ingénieurs du Québec). In 2003 he received the Young Engineer Achievement Award from the Canadian Council of Professional Engineers. Henri Mercier received his Master s in psychoeducation and his Ph.D. in clinical sciences from the Université de Montréal, Québec Canada. He is a retired Professor from the Department of Psycho- Education of the Université de Sherbrooke and a psychotherapist. His research interest focusses on the integration of children with Pervasive Developmental Disorders and the manifestation of depression in children.

Clara Liu M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Clara Liu M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders Critical Review: Is robot-assisted therapy a viable treatment option for improving social interaction in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)? Clara Liu M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western

More information

Investigating a Robot as a Therapy Partner for Children with. Autism

Investigating a Robot as a Therapy Partner for Children with. Autism Investigating a Robot as a Therapy Partner for Children with Abstract: Autism Iain Werry 1 & Kerstin Dautenhahn 2 & William Harwin 1 1. Department of Cybernetics, University Of Reading, UK 2. Department

More information

Autism. Laura Schreibman HDP1 11/29/07 MAIN DIAGNOSTIC FEATURES OF AUTISTIC DISORDER. Deficits in social attachment and behavior

Autism. Laura Schreibman HDP1 11/29/07 MAIN DIAGNOSTIC FEATURES OF AUTISTIC DISORDER. Deficits in social attachment and behavior Autism Laura Schreibman HDP1 11/29/07 MAIN DIAGNOSTIC FEATURES OF AUTISTIC DISORDER Deficits in social attachment and behavior Deficits in verbal and nonverbal communication Presence of perseverative,

More information

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi We ll cover Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) ASD in young adults Social Communication (definition, components, importance,

More information

A Comparison of Interactive and Robotic Systems in Therapy and Education for Children with Autism

A Comparison of Interactive and Robotic Systems in Therapy and Education for Children with Autism A Comparison of Interactive and Robotic Systems in Therapy and Education for Children with Autism Megan Davis a, 1, Ben Robins a, Kerstin Dautenhahn a, Chrystopher Nehaniv a and Stuart Powell b a Adaptive

More information

From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM IV

From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM IV From Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM IV (I) A total of six (or more) items from (A), (B), and (C), with at least two from (A), and one each from (B) and (C) (A) qualitative impairment

More information

12/19/2016. Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview. What is the Autism Spectrum? Autism Spectrum Disorder

12/19/2016. Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview. What is the Autism Spectrum? Autism Spectrum Disorder Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview What is the Autism Spectrum? DSM V (2013) included revisions with more specific diagnostic criteria and Further distinctions will

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview

Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview What is the Autism Spectrum? DSM V (2013) included revisions with more specific diagnostic criteria and Further distinctions will

More information

WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One

WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One WHAT IS AUTISM? Chapter One Autism is a life-long developmental disability that prevents people from understanding what they see, hear, and otherwise sense. This results in severe problems with social

More information

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D.

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA Lisa Joseph, Ph.D. Autism Spectrum Disorder Neurodevelopmental disorder Reflects understanding of the etiology of disorder as related to alterations

More information

Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- NOS)

Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- NOS) Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- NOS) What is Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS)? (*Please note that the criteria according to the DSM-V changed

More information

EXPLORING EMOTIONAL AND IMITATIONAL ANDROID-BASED INTERACTIONS IN AUTISTIC SPECTRUM DISORDERS

EXPLORING EMOTIONAL AND IMITATIONAL ANDROID-BASED INTERACTIONS IN AUTISTIC SPECTRUM DISORDERS Journal of CyberTherapy & Rehabilitation Spring 2008, Volume 1, Issue 1 Virtual Reality Medical Institute 49 EXPLORING EMOTIONAL AND IMITATIONAL ANDROID-BASED INTERACTIONS IN AUTISTIC SPECTRUM DISORDERS

More information

The Effect of Robotic Intervention on the Imitation Skills of a Child with Autism

The Effect of Robotic Intervention on the Imitation Skills of a Child with Autism 3-59 Corbett Hall University of Alberta Edmonton, AB T6G 2G4 Ph: (780) 492-5422 Fx: (780) 492-1696 Email: atlab@ualberta.ca The Effect of Robotic Intervention on the Imitation Skills of a Child with Autism

More information

The Use of RoboParrot in the Therapy of Children with Autism Children: In Case of Teaching the Turn-Taking Skills

The Use of RoboParrot in the Therapy of Children with Autism Children: In Case of Teaching the Turn-Taking Skills The Use of RoboParrot in the Therapy of Children with Autism Children: In Case of Teaching the Turn-Taking Skills Pegah Soleiman 1, Hadi Moradi 1,2, Maryam Mahmoudi 3, Mohyeddin Teymouri 4, Hamid Reza

More information

Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders

Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders Background Autistic is a broad term coined in the twentieth century by Bleuler that

More information

5. Diagnostic Criteria

5. Diagnostic Criteria 5. Diagnostic Criteria The questions that are going to be answered in this chapter are: What are the diagnostic criteria of ASD? Are the diagnostic criteria laid down in the DSM-IV-TR or ICD-10 manuals

More information

What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA.

What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA. What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA. What is Autism? ) Autism is a complex developmental disability that has a neurological basis that causes impairments in social interactions, communication,

More information

Title: Symbol-Infused Play for Young Children with Complex Communication Needs

Title: Symbol-Infused Play for Young Children with Complex Communication Needs Title: Symbol-Infused Play for Young Children with Complex Communication Needs This presentation will discuss the importance of symbol-infused play to the development of communication and language in young

More information

AUTISM Definition. Symptoms

AUTISM Definition. Symptoms AUTISM Definition Autism spectrum disorder is a serious neurodevelopmental disorder that impairs a child's ability to communicate and interact with others. It also includes restricted repetitive behaviors,

More information

AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS. Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician

AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS. Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician Disclosure I do not have any financial relationships to disclose I do not plan to discuss

More information

Fact Sheet 8. DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder

Fact Sheet 8. DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder Fact Sheet 8 DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder A diagnosis of autism is made on the basis of observed behaviour. There are no blood tests, no single defining symptom and no physical characteristics that

More information

Floortime - Affectively rich play to match child s individual differences and stimulate developmental growth

Floortime - Affectively rich play to match child s individual differences and stimulate developmental growth What is DIR /Floortime? Developmental - children progress through stages toward ability to think and communicate flexibly in the world Individual Difference - each child has an unique set of biologically

More information

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not.

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not. Autism Summary Autism What is Autism? The Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a developmental disability that can have significant implications on a child's ability to function and interface with the world

More information

Section 5: Communication. Part 1: Early Warning Signs. Theresa Golem. December 5, 2012

Section 5: Communication. Part 1: Early Warning Signs. Theresa Golem. December 5, 2012 Section 5: Communication Part 1: Early Warning Signs Theresa Golem December 5, 2012 Deficits in the area of communication are one of the key characteristics of autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Early warning

More information

DSM-IV Criteria. (1) qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following:

DSM-IV Criteria. (1) qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following: DSM-IV Criteria Autistic Disorder A. A total of six (or more) items from (1), (2), and (3), with at least two from (1), and one each from (2) and (3): (1) qualitative impairment in social interaction,

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorder Pre Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.

Autism Spectrum Disorder Pre Cengage Learning. All rights reserved. Autism Spectrum Disorder Pre 2014 2012 Cengage Learning. All rights reserved. DSM- 5 In 2013, the American Psychiatric Association released the fifth edition of its Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of

More information

There are two types of activities: Think about it! And apply it! Each activity will be marked by an icon and a specified color as you can see below:

There are two types of activities: Think about it! And apply it! Each activity will be marked by an icon and a specified color as you can see below: Module 1 Introduction VCUAutism Center for Excellence There are two types of activities: Think about it! And apply it! Each activity will be marked by an icon and a specified color as you can see below:

More information

Harry H. Wright 1 / 26

Harry H. Wright 1 / 26 Harry H. Wright 1 / 26 The Robot Prototype: CHARLIE CHILD-CENTERED ADAPTIVE ROBOT for LEARNING in INTERACTIVE ENVIRONMENTS Robots as a New Tool for Autism Therapy CHARLIE: A New Robot Prototype Software

More information

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Overview Diagnostic Criteria Current: Diagnostic & Statistical

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs

Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs DSM-IV to DSM-5: Primary Changes Autism Spectrum Disorders: An update on research and clinical practices for SLPs Laurie Swineford, PhD CCC-SLP Washington State University DSM-IV Previously we used the

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorder What is it?

Autism Spectrum Disorder What is it? Autism Spectrum Disorder What is it? Robin K. Blitz, MD Director, Developmental Pediatrics Resident Autism Diagnostic Clinic Lecture Series #1 Learning Objectives What can we talk about in 20 minutes?

More information

Recognizing Autism Under the Age of 2

Recognizing Autism Under the Age of 2 Recognizing Autism Under the Age of 2 Beth Ellen Davis MD MPH Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics Clinical Professor of Pediatrics, UW, Seattle February 2, 2015 Objectives Recognize typical and atypical/delayed

More information

Autism or Something Else? Knowing the Difference

Autism or Something Else? Knowing the Difference Autism or Something Else? Knowing the Difference SUSAN BUTTROSS, M.D., FAAP PROFESSOR OF PEDIATRICS CENTER FOR THE ADVANCEMENT OF YOUTH UNIVERSITY OF MISSISSIPPI MEDICAL CENTER JACKSON, MISSISSIPPI Disclosure

More information

Page24. Indexed in: Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Sunny Agarwal Founder / Digital Marketing Specialist at Code- LIT. September 2018; 5(3):24-28

Page24. Indexed in: Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Sunny Agarwal Founder / Digital Marketing Specialist at Code- LIT. September 2018; 5(3):24-28 Page24 Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Sunny Agarwal Founder / Digital Marketing Specialist at Code- LIT Email: agarwalsunny68@gmail.com KEY WORDS: Autism, Sign/symptoms of ASD (Autism Spectrum Disorder),

More information

Recognizing Autism Under the Age of 2. Objectives YES! 11/29/2016. Are ASD symptoms present in infants and toddlers?

Recognizing Autism Under the Age of 2. Objectives YES! 11/29/2016. Are ASD symptoms present in infants and toddlers? Recognizing Autism Under the Age of 2 Beth Ellen Davis MD MPH Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics Clinical Professor of Pediatrics, UW, Seattle Objectives Recognize typical and atypical/delayed early social

More information

Fostering Communication Skills in Preschool Children with Pivotal Response Training

Fostering Communication Skills in Preschool Children with Pivotal Response Training Fostering Communication Skills in Preschool Children with Pivotal Response Training Mary Mandeville-Chase, MS, CCC-SLP 1 Training Objectives 1. Participants will name two pivotal behaviors associated with

More information

(p) (f) Echolalia. What is it, and how to help your child with Echolalia?

(p) (f) Echolalia. What is it, and how to help your child with Echolalia? (p) 406-690-6996 (f) 406-206-5262 info@advancedtherapyclinic.com Echolalia What is it, and how to help your child with Echolalia? Echolalia is repeating or echoing what another person has said. Children

More information

Autism/Autism Spectrum Disorders

Autism/Autism Spectrum Disorders Autism/Autism Spectrum Disorders Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are a group of developmental disabilities that can cause significant social, communication and behavioral challenges. People with ASDs

More information

Towards interactive robots in autism therapy

Towards interactive robots in autism therapy Towards interactive robots in autism therapy Background, motivation and challenges * Kerstin Dautenhahn and Iain Werry University of Hertfordshire This article discusses the potential of using interactive

More information

Effects of repeated exposure to a humanoid robot on children with autism B. Robins, K. Dautenhahn, R. te Boekhorst, and A. Billard

Effects of repeated exposure to a humanoid robot on children with autism B. Robins, K. Dautenhahn, R. te Boekhorst, and A. Billard 1 Chapter # Effects of repeated exposure to a humanoid robot on children with autism B. Robins, K. Dautenhahn, R. te Boekhorst, and A. Billard #.1 Introduction This work is part of the Aurora project which

More information

Interdepartmental Research Center "E. Piaggio"

Interdepartmental Research Center E. Piaggio Interdepartmental Research Center "E. Piaggio" Faculty of Engineering, University of Pisa, Via Diotisalvi 2, 56126 Pisa, Italy www.piaggio.ccii.unipi.it Human-Robot Interaction in Autism A. Fornai, G.

More information

DSM V Criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder

DSM V Criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder And Autism What is Autism? Autism is a developmental disorder characterized by deficits in social skills and communication as well as stereotypical, repetitive behaviours. By definition, the symptoms must

More information

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams

Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Communication and ASD: Key Concepts for Educational Teams Marci Laurel, MA, CCC-SLP mlaurel@salud.unm.edu and Services (UCEDD) 1 Training Objectives Participants will: 1. Name at least three communication

More information

DSM 5 Criteria to Diagnose Autism

DSM 5 Criteria to Diagnose Autism DSM 5 Criteria to Diagnose Autism Patient Name Patient Date of Birth Patient Health Plan Provider Name and Credential Date of Exam Only a doctoral level clinician (MD, PhD, and/or PsyD) can complete this

More information

Table 1: Comparison of DSM-5 and DSM-IV-TR Diagnostic Criteria. Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Pervasive Developmental Disorders Key Differences

Table 1: Comparison of DSM-5 and DSM-IV-TR Diagnostic Criteria. Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Pervasive Developmental Disorders Key Differences Comparison of the Diagnostic Criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder Across DSM-5, 1 DSM-IV-TR, 2 and the Individuals with Disabilities Act (IDEA) 3 Definition of Autism Colleen M. Harker, M.S. & Wendy L.

More information

Valarie Kerschen M.D.

Valarie Kerschen M.D. Valarie Kerschen M.D. Greek word meaning self 1940 s Dr Leo Kanner describes classic autism 1940 s Dr Hans Asperger describes Aspergers Syndrome 1960 s Autism theorized to be due to refrigerator mothers

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorder What is it? Robin K. Blitz, MD Resident Autism Diagnostic Clinic Lecture Series #1

Autism Spectrum Disorder What is it? Robin K. Blitz, MD Resident Autism Diagnostic Clinic Lecture Series #1 Autism Spectrum Disorder What is it? Robin K. Blitz, MD Resident Autism Diagnostic Clinic Lecture Series #1 Learning Objectives What can we talk about in 20 minutes? What is Autism? What are the Autism

More information

From: What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015

From:   What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015 Overcoming Intellectual Disability and Autism to Achieve Vocational & Academic Success Pathway to Empowerment Objectives 1 2 4 Learn to distinguish between intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorders.

More information

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER What is the DSM-5? The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (the DSM) is developed by the

More information

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) Kim, E. S., Berkovits, L. D., Bernier, E. P., Leyzberg, D., Shic, F., Paul, R., & Scassellati, B. (2013). Social robots as embedded reinforcers of social behavior in children

More information

Overview. Clinical Features

Overview. Clinical Features Jessica Greenson, Ph.D. Autism Center University of Washington Clinical Features Overview Diagnostic & Statistical Manual IV (DSM IV) Prevalence Course of Onset Etiology Early Recognition Early Recognition

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorder What is it?

Autism Spectrum Disorder What is it? Autism Spectrum Disorder What is it? Robin K. Blitz, MD Resident Autism Diagnostic Clinic Lecture Series #1 Learning Objectives What can we talk about in 20 minutes? What is Autism? What are the Autism

More information

Jason Garner, M.A. ABA Clinical Director

Jason Garner, M.A. ABA Clinical Director Jason Garner, M.A. ABA Clinical Director Discuss Autism Definition Prevalence rates Discuss Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) Discuss challenging behavior Functions of Behavior Discuss behavior management

More information

Developmental Disabilities: Diagnosis and Treatment. Sara Sanders, Psy.D. 03/05/15

Developmental Disabilities: Diagnosis and Treatment. Sara Sanders, Psy.D. 03/05/15 Developmental Disabilities: Diagnosis and Treatment Sara Sanders, Psy.D. 03/05/15 1 Developmental Disability A severe and chronic disability that is attributable to a mental or physical impairment that

More information

Elements of Communication

Elements of Communication Elements of Communication Elements of Communication 6 Elements of Communication 1. Verbal messages 2. Nonverbal messages 3. Perception 4. Channel 5. Feedback 6. Context Elements of Communication 1. Verbal

More information

Copyright: Bopp & Mirenda.ASHA (2008) 1

Copyright: Bopp & Mirenda.ASHA (2008) 1 Heterogeneity of Language Development in Young Children with Autism Over 4-5 Years Karen D. Bopp, Ph.D., S-LP(C) Pat Mirenda, Ph.D., BCBA The University of British Columbia, Canada November 20, 2008 American

More information

Diagnosing Autism, and What Comes After. Natalie Roth, Ph. D. Clinical Psychologist, Alternative Behavior Strategies

Diagnosing Autism, and What Comes After. Natalie Roth, Ph. D. Clinical Psychologist, Alternative Behavior Strategies Diagnosing Autism, and What Comes After Natalie Roth, Ph. D. Clinical Psychologist, Alternative Behavior Strategies Cigna Autism Awareness Education Series, January 12, 2017 1 Autistic Spectrum Disorder

More information

Bonnie Van Metre M.Ed., BCBA Kennedy Krieger Institute Center for Autism and Related Disorders

Bonnie Van Metre M.Ed., BCBA Kennedy Krieger Institute Center for Autism and Related Disorders Bonnie Van Metre M.Ed., BCBA Kennedy Krieger Institute Center for Autism and Related Disorders Principles of behavior/ functions of behavior Preventive strategies Consequences strategies Aggression Pinching

More information

Autism & intellectual disabilities. How to deal with confusing concepts

Autism & intellectual disabilities. How to deal with confusing concepts Autism & intellectual disabilities How to deal with confusing concepts dr. Gerard J. Nijhof Orthopedagogue / GZ-psychologist PhD, Free University Amsterdam Private practice contact@gerardnijhof.nl +31

More information

ESDM Early Start Denver Model Parent Coaching P-ESDM

ESDM Early Start Denver Model Parent Coaching P-ESDM ESDM Early Start Denver Model Parent Coaching P-ESDM BCASLPA Conference October 22nd, 2016 ESDM Canada Mary McKenna Janet Harder Michaela Jelen ESDM ESDM draws on several different models as its foundation

More information

Hearing Loss and Autism. diagnosis and intervention

Hearing Loss and Autism. diagnosis and intervention Hearing Loss and Autism diagnosis and intervention Outline 1. Definitions 2. Prevalence 3. Diagnosis 4. Ideas for Intervention Definitions Definitions Autism A group of complex disorders of brain development

More information

Pivotal Response Training (PRT)

Pivotal Response Training (PRT) Pivotal Response Training (PRT) Workshop Handout Portions of the information in this handout were taken from the STAR Program Manual, published by Pro-Ed, Austin, TX (Arick, Loos, Falco, and Krug, 2004,

More information

DSM-V. Causes. No causal relationship with vaccinations. Overview of Students with Behavioral, Emotional and Social Development Needs

DSM-V. Causes. No causal relationship with vaccinations. Overview of Students with Behavioral, Emotional and Social Development Needs The Hong Kong Institute of Education DSM-V Overview of Students with Behavioral, Emotional and Social Development Needs (Autism & Asperger Syndrome) social communication & interaction impairments Ryan

More information

Trends and Considerations in Robot-Assisted Autism Therapy

Trends and Considerations in Robot-Assisted Autism Therapy 2010 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation Anchorage Convention District May 3-8, 2010, Anchorage, Alaska, USA Trends and Considerations in Robot-Assisted Autism Therapy Daniel J. Ricks

More information

Supporting Children and Adults with Autism to Access Sports and Leisure Activities

Supporting Children and Adults with Autism to Access Sports and Leisure Activities Supporting Children and Adults with Autism to Access Sports and Leisure Activities a development resource for those supporting individuals with autism to access leisure activities Autism is a hidden disability,

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Multidisciplinary Evaluation Team (MET) Report

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Multidisciplinary Evaluation Team (MET) Report Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Multidisciplinary Evaluation Team (MET) Report Date of Report: Student Name: Date of Birth: School: Evaluation Team: Psychologist: School Social Worker (SSW): Speech & Language

More information

INDICATORS OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

INDICATORS OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER INDICATORS OF AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER While many of the behaviors typical of Autism Spectrum Disorder are also typical of earlier stages of normal development, it is the combination or pattern of behaviors

More information

Tools for Autism Diagnosis

Tools for Autism Diagnosis Tools for Autism Diagnosis Aude Billard, Basilio Noris, Karim Benmachiche Learning Algorithms and Systems Laboratory EPFL Contact Address: Prof. Aude Billard EPFL STI - LASA Ecole Polytechnique Federale

More information

Objectives. Age of Onset. ASD: Communication Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (2000)

Objectives. Age of Onset. ASD: Communication Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (2000) Autism and ADHD What Every Orthodontist Should Know! Heather Whitney Sesma, Ph.D., L.P. Assistant Professor of Pediatrics Division of Clinical Neuroscience Objectives Review the core features of autism

More information

Emily A. Jones, PhD, BCBA 1

Emily A. Jones, PhD, BCBA 1 Joint Attention Intervention for Children with Autism Emily A. Jones, Ph.D., BCBA CW Post, Long Island University Joint Attention an early social-communicative behavior in which two people share al focus

More information

Improving Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Speech & Language Therapist

Improving Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Speech & Language Therapist Improving Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Speech & Language Therapist Communication in ASD We will cover Typical language development Language presentation in ASD Language

More information

An Autism Primer for the PCP: What to Expect, When to Refer

An Autism Primer for the PCP: What to Expect, When to Refer An Autism Primer for the PCP: What to Expect, When to Refer Webinar November 9, 2016 John P. Pelegano MD Chief of Pediatrics Hospital for Special Care Disclosures None I will not be discussing any treatments,

More information

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby

The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby A Fully Integrated Platform for Observation, Daily Reporting, Communications and Assessment For Early Childhood Professionals and the Families that they Serve Alignment

More information

Autism and other developmental disorders. Dr. Martin Maldonado

Autism and other developmental disorders. Dr. Martin Maldonado Autism and other developmental disorders Dr. Martin Maldonado Autistic Spectrum Autistic Disorder Autistic Psychopathy Childhood psychoses (Europe and Latin America) Multi-system developmental disorder

More information

Words are not enough: Social Communication & Early Signs of Autism Spectrum Disorders

Words are not enough: Social Communication & Early Signs of Autism Spectrum Disorders Words are not enough: Social Communication & Early Signs of Autism Spectrum Disorders Pamela Rollins, MS Ed.D. University of Texas at Dallas, School of Behavior and Brain Sciences, Callier Center for Communication

More information

Factors for Measuring Dramatic Believability. Brian Magerko, Ph.D. Games for Entertainment and Learning Lab Michigan State University

Factors for Measuring Dramatic Believability. Brian Magerko, Ph.D. Games for Entertainment and Learning Lab Michigan State University Factors for Measuring Dramatic Believability Brian Magerko, Ph.D. Games for Entertainment and Learning Lab Michigan State University Outline Introduction Deconstruction Evaluation from Player Perspective

More information

Teaching the Language of Emotions

Teaching the Language of Emotions Teaching the Language of Emotions ASHA Convention November 20, 2010 Philadelphia, PA Catherine Horton, MS, CCC-SLP Lori Frost, MS, CCC-SLP Jo-Anne Matteo, MS, CCC-SLP Jill Waegenaere, MA, CCC-SLP www.pecs.com

More information

A Multimodal Interface for Robot-Children Interaction in Autism Treatment

A Multimodal Interface for Robot-Children Interaction in Autism Treatment A Multimodal Interface for Robot-Children Interaction in Autism Treatment Giuseppe Palestra giuseppe.palestra@uniba.it Floriana Esposito floriana.esposito@uniba.it Berardina De Carolis berardina.decarolis.@uniba.it

More information

Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences

Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences DATE: October 13, 2018 PRESENTED BY: Jill Dolata, PhD, CCC-SLP & Cynthia Green, MS, CCC-SLP Goals Describe

More information

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) DSM-5 (Criteria and Major Changes for SLP-Related Conditions) Individuals meeting the criteria will be given a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder with three levels of severity based on degree of support

More information

The Clinical Progress of Autism Spectrum Disorders in China. Xi an children s hospital Yanni Chen MD.PhD

The Clinical Progress of Autism Spectrum Disorders in China. Xi an children s hospital Yanni Chen MD.PhD The Clinical Progress of Autism Spectrum Disorders in China Xi an children s hospital Yanni Chen MD.PhD Conception The autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are neurodevelopmental disability characterized by

More information

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) What is Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)? (*Please note that the diagnostic criteria for ASD according to the DSM-V changed as of May, 2013. Autism Spectrum Disorder now is

More information

In Young Children with ASD Kristy Benefield Speech-Language Pathologist St. Tammany Parish School System

In Young Children with ASD Kristy Benefield Speech-Language Pathologist St. Tammany Parish School System DEVELOPING COMMUNICATION SKILLS In Young Children with ASD By Kristy Benefield Speech-Language Pathologist St. Tammany Parish School System Today we will learn.. OVERVIEW The definition of communication

More information

Starting Strong 2015 Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders and An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis

Starting Strong 2015 Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders and An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis Starting Strong 2015 Understanding Autism Spectrum Disorders and An Introduction to Applied Behavior Analysis Robin Talley, M.Ed., BCBA UW Autism Center Presentation Overview Overview of Autism Spectrum

More information

Toward Therapist-in-the-Loop Assistive Robotics for Children with Autism and Specific Language Impairment

Toward Therapist-in-the-Loop Assistive Robotics for Children with Autism and Specific Language Impairment Toward Therapist-in-the-Loop Assistive Robotics for Children with Autism and Specific Language Impairment Mark B. Colton 1, Daniel J. Ricks 1, Michael A. Goodrich 2, Behzad Dariush 3, Kikuo Fujimura 3,

More information

Agenda. Making the Connection. Facts about ASD. Respite Presentation. Agenda. Facts about ASD. Triad of Impairments. 3 Diagnoses on spectrum

Agenda. Making the Connection. Facts about ASD. Respite Presentation. Agenda. Facts about ASD. Triad of Impairments. 3 Diagnoses on spectrum Making the Connection Respite Presentation Agenda Agenda Facts about ASD Triad of Impairments 3 Diagnoses on spectrum Characteristics of ASD Behaviour Facts about ASD It is the most common form of any

More information

Parent s Guide to Autism

Parent s Guide to Autism Parent s Guide to Autism Facing Autism If you have picked up this booklet, chances are your family is facing autism for the very first time and you may be overwhelmed, confused, and scared. Autism can

More information

Neurobehavioral disorder Spectrum disorder Prevalence Causes Treatment

Neurobehavioral disorder Spectrum disorder Prevalence Causes Treatment Neurobehavioral disorder Spectrum disorder Prevalence Causes Treatment Persistent deficits in social communication and social interaction across multiple contexts Restrictive, repetitive patterns of behavior,

More information

Understanding Autism. Julie Smith, MA, BCBA. November 12, 2015

Understanding Autism. Julie Smith, MA, BCBA. November 12, 2015 Understanding Autism Julie Smith, MA, BCBA November 12, 2015 2 Overview What is Autism New DSM-5; changes to diagnosis Potential causes Communication strategies Managing difficult behaviors Effective programming

More information

AUTISM PARENT HANDBOOK. Answers to common questions. Artwork: Hey Diddle Diddle, by Eytan Nisinzweig, an artist with autism.

AUTISM PARENT HANDBOOK. Answers to common questions. Artwork: Hey Diddle Diddle, by Eytan Nisinzweig, an artist with autism. AUTISM PARENT HANDBOOK Answers to common questions Artwork: Hey Diddle Diddle, by Eytan Nisinzweig, an artist with autism. 2nd Edition Dear Parents: We are very pleased to publish the 2nd edition of the

More information

AUTISM: THE MIND-BRAIN CONNECTION

AUTISM: THE MIND-BRAIN CONNECTION AUTISM: THE MIND-BRAIN CONNECTION Ricki Robinson, MD, MPH Co-Director, Descanso Medical Center for Development and Learning - La Canada CA Clinical Professor of Pediatrics, Keck School of Medicine-USC

More information

Early Autism Detection Screening and Referral. What is Autism? ASD Epidemiology. ASD Basic Facts 10/10/2010. Early Autism Detection and Referral

Early Autism Detection Screening and Referral. What is Autism? ASD Epidemiology. ASD Basic Facts 10/10/2010. Early Autism Detection and Referral Early Autism Detection and Referral Early Autism Detection Screening and Referral Learning Objectives: Define autistic spectrum disorders, their epidemiology and etiology; Recognize the earliest signs

More information

What are the common features of Asperger s Syndrome?

What are the common features of Asperger s Syndrome? Aspergers Syndrome What is Asperger s Syndrome? **Please note that the diagnostic criteria according to the DSM-V changed as of May, 2013. Autism Spectrum Disorder now is a single category that encompasses

More information

INTERACTIVE GAMES USING KINECT 3D SENSOR TECHNOLOGY FOR AUTISTIC CHILDREN THERAPY By Azrulhizam Shapi i Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia

INTERACTIVE GAMES USING KINECT 3D SENSOR TECHNOLOGY FOR AUTISTIC CHILDREN THERAPY By Azrulhizam Shapi i Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia INTERACTIVE GAMES USING KINECT 3D SENSOR TECHNOLOGY FOR AUTISTIC CHILDREN THERAPY By Azrulhizam Shapi i Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia INTRODUCTION Autism occurs throughout the world regardless of race,

More information

Aspect Positive Behaviour Support

Aspect Positive Behaviour Support Aspect Positive Behaviour Support Individual Autism Profile Information Sheet Why develop an individual autism profile? People on the autism spectrum may have a range of support needs based on their preferences,

More information

Presents: Asperger Syndrome: From Diagnosis to Independence

Presents: Asperger Syndrome: From Diagnosis to Independence Presents: Asperger Syndrome: From Diagnosis to Independence Presents: Asperger Syndrome: From Diagnosis to Independence made possible by our underwriter: Presents: Asperger Syndrome: From Diagnosis to

More information

OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION

OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS IN EMERGING ADULTS Douglas J. Scambler, Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist Presented at the University of Wyoming November 6, 2012 OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION What are the autism spectrum

More information

Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update. Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011

Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update. Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011 Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011 Overview Diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorders Screening/referral

More information

Potential Outcomes for Children Who Are Deaf-Blind with Cochlear Implants

Potential Outcomes for Children Who Are Deaf-Blind with Cochlear Implants Facilitating Communication & Language for Children with Cochlear Implants and Vision Impairments EARLY HEARING DETECTION & INTERVENTION Addison, Texas March 10 th, 2009 Kathleen Stremel stremelk@wou.edu

More information

Section three: answers for part one

Section three: answers for part one Section three: answers for part one Sound 1. How did you try to concentrate? As above. 2. How did you react to the noise? 3. How would noises at school cause difficulties when trying to concentrate on

More information