The Cross-Cultural Generalizability of Zuckerman s Alternative Five-Factor Model of Personality

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Cross-Cultural Generalizability of Zuckerman s Alternative Five-Factor Model of Personality"

Transcription

1 JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT, 89(2), Copyright C 2007, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Cross-Cultural Generalizability of Zuckerman s Alternative Five-Factor Model of Personality Jérôme Rossier Institute of Psychology University of Lausanne, Switzerland Anton Aluja and Luis F. García Department of Pedagogy and Psychology University of Lleida, Spain Alois Angleitner Department of Psychology University of Bielefeld, Germany Vilfredo De Pascalis Department of Psychology University of Rome La Sapienza, Italy Wei Wang Department of Medical Psychology Zhejiang University, China Michael Kuhlman and Marvin Zuckerman Department of Psychology University of Delaware The aim of this study was to analyze the cross-cultural generalizability of the Alternative fivefactor model (AFFM; Zuckerman, Kuhlman, & Camac, 1988). The total sample was made up of 9,152 subjects from six countries: China, Germany, Italy, Spain, Switzerland, and the United States. The internal consistencies for all countries were generally similar to those found for the normative American sample. Factor analyses within cultures showed that the normative American structure was replicated in all cultures; however, the congruence coefficients were slightly lower in China and Italy. A similar analysis at the facet level confirmed the high cross-cultural replicability of the AFFM. Mean-level comparisons did not always show the hypothesized effects. The mean score differences across countries were very small. The Five-factor model (FFM; McCrae & John, 1992) is a hierarchical model based on an empirical generalization about the covariation of personality traits (Digman, 1990). This model postulates that five dimensions named Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness adequately map these personality traits (McCrae & Costa, 1985, 1999). A large consensus exists about the FFM (Rossier, Meyer de Stadelhofen, & Berthoud, 2004), and these five dimensions are similar to the Big Five identified in numerous lexical studies (De Raad, 2000). According to the Five-factor theory (McCrae & Costa, 1999), these dimensions are biologically rooted, pointing to evidence that the five factors and their structure are heritable (McCrae, Jang, Livesley, Riemann, & Angleitner, 2001). Because all people share the same human genome, Five-factor theory claims that certain characteristics of traits,

2 CROSS-CULTURAL GENERALIZABILITY OF THE AFFM 189 including their structure and development, should be universal. Indeed, cross-cultural studies have shown that the FFM is replicable across cultures (e.g., Rolland, 2002). Universality of personality traits does not extend to mean levels, however, but McCrae (2002) suggested that these mean levels might vary across cultures in a predictable way (Rossier, Dahourou, & McCrae, 2005). Much research on the biological correlations of personality traits have used other dimensions, however, such as sensation seeking or impulsivity, which are not well represented in the FFM according to Zuckerman, Kuhlman, and Camac (1988), who developed an Alternative five-factor model (AFFM). The goal of this research is to evaluate the cross-cultural equivalence of the AFFM and to study if mean level differences according to the AFFM are similar to those observed according to the FFM. To develop their AFFM, Zuckerman and colleagues (1988) studied the structure underlying 46 scales selected from 8 tests, which had been used as measures of temperament or involved in psychobiological studies of personality, and identified five replicable factors (Zuckerman, Kuhlman, Thornquist, & Kiers, 1991). Zuckerman, Kuhlman, Joireman, Teta, and Kraft (1993) developed the Zuckerman Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ) to measure the five dimensions of the AFFM: Impulsive Sensation Seeking (ImpSS), Neuroticism-Anxiety (N-Anx), Aggression- Hostility (Agg-Host), Activity (Act), and Sociability (Sy). These five dimensions were almost orthogonal as theoretically postulated (r.37 ). Zuckerman (2002) also suggested a hierarchical structure and three global factors that might be divided into two facet scales: The ImpSS factor contains a Sensation Seeking and an Impulsivity facet, the Act factor contains a Need for General Activity and a Need for Work Activity facet, and the Sy factor contains a Liking Lively Parties and Friends and an Intolerance of Social Isolation facet. Aluja, García, and García (2002) compared the AFFM with the FFM using the ZKPQ and the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992). Considering only correlations above.40, they found that ImpSS correlated negatively with Conscientiousness (r =.53), N-Anx correlated positively with Neuroticism (r =.81), Agg-Host correlated negatively with Agreeableness (r =.59), and Sy correlated positively with Extraversion (r =.66). Thus, four dimensions of the AFFM and of the FFM appear quite similar, and the difference between these two models is the presence of a single dimension in each model that has no conceptual equivalent counterpart (Joireman & Kuhlman, 2004; Zuckerman et al., 1993). Indeed, the Openness to Experience domain is represented only poorly in the ZKPQ and the Activity factor is only poorly represented in the NEO-PI-R. Angleitner, Riemann, and Spinath (2004) studied a sample of 338 pairs of adult twins reared together and found that the heritability of the factors measured by the ZKPQ ranged from.43 to.51. These results are similar to those found for the FFM (McCrae et al., 2001). Zuckerman (2002) reported several personality differences according to gender. In the American normative sample, men scored higher on ImpSS, Agg-Host, and Act, and women scored higher on N-Anx and Sy. The most robust differences were the ones for ImpSS and N-Anx. Gender differences for personality measurement were found to be highly consistent across cultures for the FFM (Costa, Terracciano, & McCrae, 2001). Concerning the AFFM, Aluja and colleagues (2002) reported gender differences similar to those reported by Zuckerman (2002) for a Spanish sample. However, De Pascalis and Russo (2003) observed only a difference on N-Anx in an Italian sample. Concerning adult trait development no data are available for the AFFM but for the FFM, Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness show usually moderate declines, whereas Agreeableness and Conscientiousness increase between age 18 and 30 (McCrae et al., 2002). Cross-cultural equivalence is an important criterion of validity for personality models like the FFM and the AFFM that claim personality is biologically rooted and should be universal (Rossier, Dahourou, & McCrae, 2005). This equivalence might be studied at a structural level or at a metric or scalar level (Rossier, 2005). Over the past decade, a large number of cross-cultural studies have been conducted and confirmed the high structural equivalence of the FFM (Allik, 2005; McCrae et al., 2005). A review by Rolland (2002) based on a reanalyses of data from 16 cultures (from Asia, Europe, and America) compared the factorial structure of these countries with the American normative structure and found that the mean congruence coefficients were above.90 for all five dimensions of the FFM. Comparison of mean scores for cultures should be carried out when structural, metric, and scalar equivalence have been demonstrated. The structural equivalence of the NEO-PI-R was found to be high, and several studies have shown that distortion due to translation is very small studying bilingual samples (McCrae, Yik, Trapnell, Bond, & Paulhus, 1998). Metric and scalar equivalence, however, are difficult to assess. Nevertheless, the attempt of comparing mean scores for cultures generally lead to meaningful results (Rossier, Dahourou, & McCrae, 2005). McCrae (2002, p. 112) observed, for example, that Americans score slightly lower on Neuroticism than Chinese, Germans, Italians, Spaniards, or German Swiss. However, mean score differences across cultures are generally small. These analyses usually have been done using the NEO-PI-R, and no study has tried to replicate these results using the ZKPQ. The cross-cultural structural equivalence of the AFFM was never studied systematically although several translations of the ZKPQ are available (Zuckerman, 2002). Gomài-Freixanet, Valero, Punti, and Zuckerman (2004) made a first study of the structural replicability by comparing the structure obtained in their Catalan sample with the normative American structure. For women, congruence coefficients of all factors were above the factorial replication

3 190 ROSSIER ET AL. threshold of.85 (Haven & ten Berge, 1977). For men, congruence coefficients of three factors were above this threshold, but the congruence coefficients of two factors, ImpSS and Agg-Host, were equal to or below this threshold. Recently, Aluja and colleagues (2006) proposed a shortened form of the ZKPQ, made up of 50 items, which had a very stable factor structure across the four studied western countries. A large number of studies have demonstrated that personality domains of the FFM are partly biologically rooted and might be considered as universal. Knowing that the AFFM also demonstrated high heritability but is slightly different from the FFM, this study addressed the question of the cross-cultural generalizability of the AFFM. For this reason, the structural equivalence of the ZKPQ at the factor and facet levels across six countries was analyzed. Congruence coefficients similar to those found for the FFM were expected. Moreover, several authors claimed that mean scores comparison across cultures provided meaningful results (McCrae, 2002; Rossier, Dahourou, & McCrae, 2005). If these between-country differences are meaningful, using the ZKPQ instead of the NEO-PI-R should lead to similar results. Based on the correlations between the ZKPQ and the NEO-PI-R (Aluja et al., 2002), and on the mean scores for cultures for the NEO-PI-R (McCrae, 2002, p. 112), compared with Americans the Germans and Swiss were expected to score lower on ImpSS, the Chinese, Italians, Spaniards, and Swiss were expected to score higher on N-Anx, the Chinese were expected to score higher and the Swiss lower on Agg- Host, and the Chinese and Italians were expected to score lower on Sy. Sample METHOD The total sample consisted of 9,152 subjects from 6 countries made up of 5,956 females and 3,196 males. The number of subjects, the percentage of female and male, and the age mean and standard deviation for each country are reported in TABLE 1 Characteristics of the Samples Age (years) Standard Country Language N % female % male Mean deviation China Chinese 1, Germany German Italy Italian Spain Spanish 2, Switzerland French 1, United States English 2, Note. Dashes in cells indicate data were not available. Table 1. The sample from China, Italy, Spain, Switzerland, and the United States consisted mainly of university students. For the United States, age was not available but is believed to range from 19 to 21, with a mode around 19 years. The German subjects pooled a sample of university students with participants in the Bielefeld twins study. Measure The ZKPQ (Zuckerman, 2002; Zuckerman et al., 1993) is made up of 99 true false items and measures the following major scales: Impulsive Sensation Seeking (ImpSS), Neuroticism-Anxiety (N-Anx), Aggression-Hostility (Agg- Host),Activity (Act),Sociability (Sy), and Infrequency (Inf). ImpSS consists of 19 items (2 reversed keyed items) and can be divided according to Zuckerman (2002) in two facets Sensation Seeking (SS), which describes a general need for change, novelty, and excitement (i.e., I like doing things just for the thrill of it ), and Impulsivity (Imp), which describes a lack of planning and acting on impulse (i.e., I very seldom spend much time on the details of planning ahead ). N-Anx consists of 19 items (2 reversed keyed items) describing emotional upset and instability, tension, anxiety, lack of self-confidence, and sensitivity to criticism (i.e., I sometimes feel edgy and tense ). Agg-Host consists of 17 items (6 reversed keyed items) describing the readiness to express verbal aggression or rude and antisocial behavior (i.e., When I get mad, I say ugly things ). Act consists of 17 items (5 reversed keyed items) and can be divided into two facets: need for General Activity (GA), which describes a need for activity and the tendency to experience impatience and restlessness when being inactive (i.e., I like to keep busy all the time ), and need for Work Effort (WE), which describes the tendency to invest a lot of energy for hard work and other tasks (i.e., I like a challenging task more than a routine one ). Sy consists of 17 items (7 reversed keyed items) and can be divided into two facets scales: liking lively Parties and Friends (PF), which describes the preference of interacting with many people and of having many friends (i.e., I am a very sociable person ), and Isolation Intolerance (II), which describes an intolerance for social isolation (i.e., I need to feel that I am a vital part of a group ). Finally, Inf consists of 10 items (no reversed keyed item) and is a control scale used to eliminate subjects with possibly invalid records. Contents of these items are exaggerated socially desirable and are unlikely to be true (i.e., I have always told the truth ). According to Zuckerman (2002) a score higher than 3 on this scale might indicate an invalid record. For this research Mandarin (Wu et al., 2000), English (Zuckerman et al., 1993), French (Rossier, Verardi, Massoudi, & Aluja, in press), German (Ostendorf & Angleitner, 1994), Italian (De Pascalis, & Russo, 2003), and Spanish (Aluja et al., 2002) versions of the ZKPQ have been used.

4 CROSS-CULTURAL GENERALIZABILITY OF THE AFFM 191 Translations Translations followed established guidelines regarding the translation of self-report inventories (Geisinger, 1994). For each language, the 99 ZKPQ items were translated by a team of experts in personality psychology and back-translated into English by an independent psychologist. These backtranslations were checked by the authors of the ZKPQ and amendments were made, translated, back-translated, and reviewed. This process continued until the authors agreed with the different translations of the ZKPQ. Procedure In each country, subjects completed anonymously the appropriate language-version of the ZKPQ. This research complies with the ethical rules of the American Psychological Association. Analyses To compare the cross-cultural generalizability of the AFFM with the cross-cultural generalizability of the FFM, the analyses done were similar to the ones usually carried out for the NEO-PI-R (see, for example, Rolland 2002). For each county, reliability was assessed using Cronbach s alphas, and gender differences and correlations with age were analyzed. The factorial structures were assessed by conducting exploratory factor analyses with varimax rotations at the factor and facet levels. To assess structural replicability, orthogonal Procrustes rotations were carried out with the American loadings as the target. Mean score comparisons were conducted on gender corrected z-scores and data restricted to the college-age subjects (18 to 25 years) in order to avoid bias due to age (see, for example, McCrae, 2002). All analyses were conducted on the entire sample including subjects with a score higher than 3 on the Inf scale. Participants had an Inf score > 3 with the following frequencies: China = 1.8%, Germany = 13.7%, Italy = 18.6%, Spain = 14.8%, Switzerland = 19.3%, and United States = 5.6%. This control scale seems very sensitive to culture and might thus not be used in cross-cultural studies (Rossier, 2005). RESULTS Internal Consistency and Influence of Gender and Age In the full sample, the internal consistencies were similar to those found in the American sample. For the five global scales they were.79,.85,.72,.73, and.76 for ImpSS, N-Anx, Agg-Host, Act, and Sy, respectively. For the six facet scales they were.76,.66,.71,.56,.69, and.69 for SS, Imp, GA, WE, PF, and II respectively. Table 2 presents the internal consistencies of each scale for each country. For the five main factors, the internal consistencies seemed slightly lower in China (M =.68) and Italy (M =.73) compared with Germany (M =.80), Spain (M =.79), Switzerland (M =.78), and the United States (M =.79). In Italy the internal consistency was lower especially for Agg-Host. At the facet level the same trend was observed with lower internal consistencies in China (M =.56) and Italy (M =.63) compared with Germany (M =.71), Spain (M =.70), Switzerland (M = TABLE 2 Internal Consistencies and Correlations with Age for Each Sample ImpSS Act Sy Sample SS Imp Total N-Anx Agg-Host GA WE Total PF II Total Inf Internal consistencies China Germany Italy Spain Switzerland United States Mean Correlations with age China German Italy Spain Switzerland United States Mean Note. ImpSS: Impulsive Sensation Seeking; SS: Sensation Seeking; Imp: Impulsivity; N-Anx: Neuroticism-Anxiety; Agg-Host: Aggression-Hostility; Act: Activity; GA: General Activity; WE: Work Effort; Sy: Sociability; PF: Parties and Friends; II: Isolation Intolerance; Inf: Infrequency; Dashes in cells indicate data were not available.

5 192 ROSSIER ET AL..69), and the United States (M =.70). Interestingly, in China the internal consistency was especially low for Inf, which is not a personality dimension but a control scale. For analyzing gender differences, the effect of age was controlled by restricting the data to the college-age subjects (18 to 25 years). This restriction resulted in a small number of men in Germany (N = 37). Globally across all samples men had a significantly higher score on ImpSS, t(6539) = 11.23, p<.001 (d =.29). This difference was significant in Spain (d =.32), Switzerland (d =.25), and the United States (d =.35), but not in China (d =.07), Italy (d =.16), and Germany (d =.20). These discrepancies are due to the fact that men scored consistently higher on SS (ds ranged from.24 to.30) in all countries and that the results were more mixed concerning Imp. Indeed, men scored significantly higher on Imp in Spain (d =.22) and the United States (d =.34), but women scored significantly higher on Imp in China (d =.21), and no significant difference was found in Germany (d =.09), Italy (d =.04), and Switzerland (d =.10). Globally women scored significantly higher on N-Anx, t(6547) = 25.47, p<.001 (d =.67), and this difference was strongly consistent across countries (ds ranged from.47 to.92). Globally, the difference for Agg- Host, where men scored slightly higher, was associated with a negligible effect size, t(6540) = 3.88, p<.001 (d =.10). Actually, men scored significantly higher only in the United States (d =.24). Globally, the difference for Act, where men scored slightly higher, was associated with a negligible effect size, t(6546) = 5.43, p<.001 (d =.14). This difference was nonsignificant in Germany (d =.30) and Italy (d =.08), significant but associated with a negligible effect size in Spain (d =.12) and the United States (d =.12), and significant with modest size in China (d =.29) and Switzerland (d =.28). These discrepancies were due to the fact that men scored consistently higher on WE (ds ranged from.22 to.62) in all countries and that no significant difference was observed on GA in all countries (ds ranged from.00 to.11). Globally, no significant difference was observed for Sy, t (6545) = 1.66, p>.05 (d =.04), and men had only significantly higher scores in China (d =.33) and women in Spain (d =.19) on this factor. Finally, men scored consistently higher on Inf, t(6548) = 17.18, p<.001 (d =.43), and this occurred in all countries (ds ranged from.31 to.53). To summarize these gender differences, women scored consistently higher on N-Anx, and men scored consistently higher on SS, WE, and Inf. Globally, age correlated negatively with ImpSS (r =.26, due to a negative correlation with SS, r =.31), N-Anx (r =.16), and Sy (r =.24, due to correlations with both PF [r =.22] and II [r =.17]), and positively with Inf (r =.14). All other correlations were negligible (r <.10 ). Correlations for each country except the United States are presented in Table 2. Correlations between age and ImpSS, SS, Sy, PF, and II, were especially consistent across countries. Replicability of the Factor Structure Across Countries On the full sample, a principal component factor analysis with varimax rotation of the 89 items (excluding items from the Inf scale) allowed extracting five factors using Cattell s criterion. These factors explained 24.94% of the total variance. The first six eigenvalues were 6.52, 6.14, 3.73, 3.16, 2.66, and A one-to-one association was observed between the five factors and the five main scales of the ZKPQ. Factor 1 correlated with N-Anx (r =.97), factor 2 with ImpSS (r =.94), factor 3 with Act (r =.94), factor 4 with Sy (r =.90), and factor 5 with Agg-Host (r =.93). In order to assess the structural replicability of the AFFM, a principal component factor analysis with varimax rotation of the 89 items was conducted for each country. For China, the five-factor solution explained 20.97% of the total variance and the first six eigenvalues were 6.09, 4.07, 3.68, 2.65, 2.18, and For Germany, the five-factor solution explained 28.48% of the total variance and the first six eignevalues were 7.97, 6.97, 3.98, 3.38, 3.05, and For Italy, the five-factor solution explained 25.83% of the total variance and the first six eignevalues were 7.40, 6.39, 3.82, 2.72, 2.67, and For Spain, the five-factor solution explained 26.94% of the total variance and the first six eignevalues were 7.04, 6.77, 3.92, 3.53, 2.72, and For Switzerland, the five-factor solution explained 27.06% of the total variance and the first six eignevalues were 7.49, 5.97, 4.19, 3.46, 2.97, and For the United States, the five-factor solution explained 26.36% of the total variance and the first six eignevalues were 7.06, 5.76, 4.10, 3.54, 3.00, and A one-to-one association was observed between the five factors and ZKPQ s five main scales for each country. To compare the structure observed for the six countries, each loading matrix (except the American matrix) was subjected to an orthogonal Procrustes rotation using the American matrix as the target. Convergence after rotation was determined by congruence coefficients, which range from 1 to +1 (Haven & ten Berge, 1977). Congruence coefficients higher than.90 indicated a high structural equivalence, congruence coefficients ranging from.81 to.90 indicated a borderline structural equivalence, and congruence coefficients equal to or below.80 indicated a poor structural equivalence (McCrae, Zonderman, Costa, Bond, & Paunonen, 1996). A congruence coefficient was calculated for the complete matrix, giving an estimation of the global structural equivalence (total congruence coefficient) for each column, giving an estimation of the structural equivalence for each factor (factor congruence coefficient), and for each line, giving an estimation of the structural equivalence for each item (variable congruence coefficient; e.g., Rossier, Dahourou, & McCrae, 2005). We found a mean total congruence coefficient (CC) of.92 (see Table 3). The mean CCs for factors ranged from.88 to.94 (Mdn =.92). The mean CCs for items ranged from.69 to.98 (Mdn =.94). Sixty-three items

6 CROSS-CULTURAL GENERALIZABILITY OF THE AFFM 193 TABLE 3 Congruence Coefficients for Factors, Comparing the American Sample With the 5 Other Samples Sample ImpSS N-Anx Agg-Host Act Sy Total China Germany Italy Spain Switzerland Mean Note. ImpSS: Impulsive Sensation Seeking; N-Anx: Neuroticism- Anxiety; Agg-Host: Aggression-Hostility; Act: Activity; Sy: Sociability. were associated with a high CC, 19 items were associated with a borderline CC, and 7 items were associated with a poor CC. For China, the total CC was.88, the factor CCs ranged from.84to.91(mdn =.86), and item CCs ranged from.46 to.99 (Mdn =.92). Fifty-three items were associated with a high CC, 18 with a borderline CC, and 18 with a poor CC. For Germany, the total CC was.95, the factor CCs ranged from.93to.97(mdn =.94), and item CCs ranged from.81 to 1.00 (Mdn =.97). Eighty items were associated with a high CC and 9 with a borderline CC. For Italy, the total CC was.89, the factor CCs ranged from.78 to.93 (Mdn =.91), and item CCs ranged from.16 to 1.00 (Mdn =.94). Sixty-four items were associated with a high CC, 11 with a borderline CC, and 14 with a poor CC. For Spain, the total CC was.92, the factor CCs ranged from.88 to.95 (Mdn =.91), and item CCs ranged from.56 to 1.00 (Mdn =.95). Sixty-three items were associated with a high CC, 19 with a borderline CC, and 7 with a poor CC. For Switzerland, the total CC was.96, the factor CCs ranged from.93 to.97 (Mdn =.96), and item CCs ranged from.73 to 1.00 (Mdn =.97). Eighty-two items were associated with a high CC, 6 with a borderline CC, and one with a poor CC. For China, 8 items seemed to be particularly problematic (CC.70): items 9, 14, 21, 38, 47, 53, 82, and 85. When removing these 8 items, the total CC was.91, the factor CCs ranged from.87 to.93 (Mdn =.91), and item CCs ranged from.71to1.00(mdn =.93). For Italy, 11 items seemed to be problematic: items 12, 14, 36, 53, 54, 67, 72, 77, 82, 88, and 89. When removing these 11 items, the total CC was.92, the factor CCs ranged from.88 to.95 (Mdn =.91), and item CCs ranged from.75 to.99 (Mdn =.95). For Spain, 3 items seemed to be problematic: items 14, 82, and 89. When removing these 3 items, the total CC was.96, the factor CCs ranged from.94 to.97 (Mdn =.95), and item CCs ranged from.82 to 1.00 (Mdn =.97). No problematic item was found for Germany and Switzerland. Factor replicability is usually assessed by factoring facetlevel scales, not items (e.g., Rolland, 2002). Because of the brevity and unbalanced nature of single items, it is often difficult to replicate factor structures at the item level. That makes the observed replicability even more striking. Replicability of the Facet Scales Structures Across Countries To assess cross-cultural structural replicability at the facet level, three separate series of principal component factor analysis with varimax rotation of the items of ImpSS, Act, and Sy were carried out. For each factor and for each country, two factors were extracted. The loading matrix for each factor and country except the United States was subjected to an orthogonal Procrustes rotation using the American matrix as the target. Concerning ImpSS, the mean total CC for the two-factor solutions and the mean CCs for facets were above.90 (see Table 4). The mean CCs for items ranged from.92 to 1.00 (Mdn =.98). All items were associated with a high CC. Concerning Act, the mean total CC and the mean CCs for facets were above.90. The mean CCs for items ranged from.84 to 1.00 (Mdn =.98). Fifteen items were associated with a high CC and 2 with a borderline CC. Concerning Sy, the mean total CC and the mean CCs for facets were also above.90. The mean CCs for items ranged from.61 to 1.00 (Mdn =.97). Twelve items were associated with a high CC, 4 with a borderline CC, and 1 with a poor CC. Across countries, except for PF in China and II in Italy, all factor congruence coefficients suggested replication by Haven and ten Berge s criterion (1977). TABLE 4 Congruence Coefficients for Facet Scales, Comparing the American Sample With the 5 Other Samples Impulsive Sensation Seeking Activity Sociability Sample Sensation seeking Impulsivity General activity Work effort Parties and Friends Isolation intolerance China Germany Italy Spain Switzerland Mean

7 194 ROSSIER ET AL. TABLE 5 Gender Corrected z-score Ms and SDs for Peoples Aged 18 to 25 Years From 6 Countries ImpSS N-Anx Agg-Host Act Sy Inf N M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD China c c c c c c.87 Germany c c c c c 1.18 Italy b a c c c c 1.24 Spain 1, c b c c 1.14 Switzerland c c c c 1.26 United States 2, Note. ImpSS: Impulsive Sensation Seeking; N-Anx: Neuroticism-Anxiety; Agg-Host: Aggression-Hostility; Act: Activity; Sy: Sociability; Inf: Infrequency; Significance level of Bonferroni post hoc tests comparing the z-scores of each country with the American z-scores: a p<.05; b p<.01; c p<.001. Mean Score Comparison In order to control the effect of age and gender, the data were restricted to the college-age subjects (18 to 25 years) and all scores were standardized by calculating z-scores using the Ms and SDs from the gender-appropriate American norms. The gender corrected standardized scores for each factor and facet can be seen in Table 5. The mean standardized SDswere all smaller than the one observed in the American sample. The mean standardized SD was especially low for China. This value was also lower for Italy compared with the other European countries. The difference between countries was assessed with oneway ANOVAs on the gender-corrected standardized scores. ImpSS was significantly affected by the country variable, F (5, 6484) = 24.89, p<.001, and was lower in China, Germany, and Italy than in the United States. This difference seemed mainly due to the SS facet, F (5, 6488) = 32.40, p<.001, compared with the Imp facet, F (5, 6494) = 9.30, p<.001. The country variable had a significant impact on N-Anx, F (5, 6491) = 12.20, p<.001, which was most notably lower in Germany. Agg-Host was significantly affected by the country variable, F (5, 6484) = 38.31, p<.001, and was lower in China, Italy, and Switzerland than in the United States. Act was significantly affected by the country variables, F (5,6490) = 34.58, p<.001, and was lower in Switzerland and higher in China, Germany, Italy, and Spain than in the United States. This difference seemed mainly due to GA, F (5, 6495) = 60.45, p<.001, compared with WE, F (5, 6494) = 17.71, p<.001. GA was lower in Switzerland and higher in China, Italy, and Spain, and WE was higher in Germany. The country variable also had a significant impact on Sy, F (5, 6491) = 55.11, p<.001. Sy was lower in China, Germany, Italy, and Switzerland than in the United States. This difference seemed mainly due to II, F (5, 6495) = 91.99, p<.001, compared with PF, F (5, 6493) = 30.99, p<.001. II was especially low in China, Italy, and Switzerland, whereas PF was especially low in China and Germany. Inf was higher in all countries compared with the United States and was significantly affected by the country variable, F (5, 6492) = 98.14, p<.001. The impact of the country variable on the factors and facets were generally small. Indeed, all differences were associated with a small or negligible effect size except II, which was associated with a medium effect size. On average, the country variable accounted for 2.48% of the variability of the five main personality factors and for 2.98% of the variability of the six facet scales. DISCUSSION The observed internal consistencies across countries were all similar to those found in the American sample (Zuckerman, 2002). However, they were slightly lower in China and in Italy. Several previous studies using the NEO- PI-R with Asian or African samples also have found lower internal consistencies. These have been attributed to smaller standard deviations, which can lower alphas (McCrae, 2002). In this study, standard deviations were indeed smaller in the Chinese sample. Rossier and colleagues (2005) have suggested that this range restriction could be due to a bias toward more socially desirable responding or that, in collectivistic cultures, behavior might be determined more by social context than by traits. For all countries, the scale with the highest internal reliability was N-Anx. The scale with the lowest internal reliability was Act in the United States and Agg-Host in Germany, Italy, Spain, and Switzerland. In China, Agg- Host and Act shared the lowest internal reliability. Gomài-Freixanet and colleagues (2004) reported that the internal reliability for Agg-Host is usually lower in translated versions of the ZKPQ. This seems to be the case in China and in Italy. At the facet level, reliabilities were also similar to those found in the American sample, with lower values for China and Italy. The main gender differences observed in this study, higher scores on N-Anx for women and higher scores on SS, WE, and Inf for men, were very consistent across countries, confirming previous results about gender differences and the AFFM (Aluja, García, & García, 2002; De Pascalis & Russo, 2003; Zuckerman, 2002). Moreover, this consistency across cultures confirmed results obtained for the FFM (Costa, Terracciano, & McCrae, 2001). For example, women scored

8 CROSS-CULTURAL GENERALIZABILITY OF THE AFFM 195 consistently higher on Neuroticism, confirming the difference obtained on N-Anx in this study. Concerning adult trait development, stable negative correlations across countries between age and the ZKPQ s five main dimensions were observed for ImpSS and Sy. These results are in line with the moderate decline usually observed for Extraversion for the FFM (McCrae et al., 2002). Globally the analysis of the effect of age and gender showed a relatively high stability across countries and meaningful correspondence with results obtained using other personality measurements, such as the NEO-PI-R. Varimax rotations of ZKPQ data showed the expected structures in all countries with one-to-one associations between factors and ZKPQ s five main dimensions. The mean correlation between the factors and the five theoretical dimensions was always above.88. Moreover, correlations between the five dimensions of the ZKPQ are very low (r < 0.32 ) in all countries. After targeted factor rotation, the mean total congruence coefficient was high and similar to the one yielded by the reanalysis of NEO-PI-R data from 16 countries by Rolland (2002). The cross-cultural structural replicability seems especially high among Germany, Spain, Switzerland, and the United States. For these countries the factor congruence coefficients were all above.90 except for Agg-Host, in Spain, which was associated with a borderline congruence coefficient. The cross-cultural structural replicability however, seems slightly lower in China and in Italy. The values observed for China might be attributed to the cultural context insofar as the congruence coefficients of all factors are slightly lower. In Italy, the low cross-cultural replicability is caused by the fact that the Agg-Host factor does not replicate well. This might be due to slight lexical differences in the formulation of certain items of this factor. Actually, after removing the problematic items listed in the result section the congruence coefficients improved noticeably. This was also the case for China and Spain. Globally, this study indicates that the structural cross-cultural replicability of the AFFM is similar to that observed for the FFM. Previous studies (McCrae, 2002; Rossier et al., 2005) have suggested that culture-level comparisons of mean scores are meaningful. Consistent with our expectations, based on the correlation between the ZKPQ and the NEO-PI-R (Aluja and al., 2002) and on the mean scores for cultures reported by McCrae (2002), Germans scored lower on ImpSS, Swiss scored lower on Agg-Host, and Chinese and Italians scored lower on Sy compared with Americans. Unexpectedly, compared with Americans, the Swiss did not score lower on ImpSS and Chinese and Italians did; Chinese, Germans, Italians, Spaniards, and Swiss scored lower on N-Anx; Chinese, Italians, and Spaniards scored lower on Agg-Host; and Germans and Swiss scored lower on Sy. Thus expectations were only partly true for ImpSS, Agg-Host, and Sy, and totally erroneous for N-Anx, although N-Anx is highly correlated with the NEO-PI-R Neuroticism domain (Zuckerman et al., 1993). The study of mean-score differences for cultures with two different personality measurements would be more informative if researchers administred both instruments to the same samples. Indeed, the differences across cultures are very small and accounted for less than 3% of the total variance. They therefore might be masked by sampling differences or by the differences between the dimensions postulated by both models. For example, the Sy and NEO-PI-R Extraversion domain have about 67% of common variance, after correcting for attenuation (Aluja and al., 2002). The results of this study cannot exclude that personality measurements simply do not reach metric or scalar equivalence. Moreover, this study did not control for several variables such as social desirability, which might have an impact on mean scores. Scalar equivalence across cultures is, of course, not required for the use of a test within a single culture, however, and the ZKPQ is fairly reliable across most cross-cultural samples. The present study, using only the ZKPQ, might provide reference standardized gender corrected mean scores for other crosscultural comparisons using this personality questionnaire. Of course, this study has several limitations. The participants within each culture are samples of convenience mostly made up of students. The fact that only the ZKPQ was used allowed analyzing the structural equivalence of this personality measurement but it made the comparison with the NEO-PI-R mean scores for cultures difficult (Okazaki & Sue, 1995). For further cross-cultural mean-score comparisons or analysis of the scalar equivalence of personality measurement, it might be advisable to use more than one inventory, select a more diverse samples of cultures, and use other methods of assessing equivalence (e.g., bilingual retest studies). The ZKPQ was developed mainly to assess the five factors of the AFFM and does not provide facet scales for each factor. Usually structural equivalence of the FFM is assessed by conducting principal component factor analysis of the facet scales, which decrease the variance due to error and increases congruence coefficients. This article focused on the cross-cultural generalizability of the AFFM. This generalizability seems similar to the one observed for the FFM, which was examined in a large number of studies during this last decade. This replicability across cultures applies not only at the factor level but also at the facet level and might thus encourage other studies aimed at describing more precisely this AFFM and at comparing it more extensively with the original FFM. The cross-cultural replicability is an important criterion for assessing the validity of models claiming that personality traits are biologically rooted and might be universal. The results presented give a first confirmation of the universality of the AFFM personality traits, and these results should be confirmed by analyzing data from a larger number of countries. ACKNOWLEDGMENT This research was supported by a Swiss National Science Foundation grant number

9 196 ROSSIER ET AL. REFERENCES Allik, J. (2005). Personality dimension across cultures. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, Aluja, A., García, O., & García, L. F. (2002). A comparative study of Zuckerman s three structural models for personality throught the NEO-PI- R, ZKPQ-III, EPQ-RS and Goldberg s 50-bipolar adjectives. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, Aluja, A., Rossier, J., García, L. F., Angleitner, A., Kuhlman, M., & Zuckerman, M. (2006). A cross-cultural shortened form of the ZKPQ (ZKPQ- 50-cc) adapted to English, French, German, and Spanish languages. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Spinath, F. M. (2004). Investigating the ZKPQ-III-R: Psychometric properties, relations to the Five-factor model, and genetic and environmental influences on its scales and facets. In R. M. Stelmack (Ed.), On the psychobiology of personality: Essays in honor of Marvin Zuckerman (pp ). New York: Elsevier Science. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Costa, P. T., Jr., Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, De Pascalis, V., & Russo, P. M. (2003). Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire: Preliminary results of the Italian version. Psychological Reports, 92, De Raad, B. (2000). The Big Five personality factors: The psycholexical approach to personality. Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers. Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the Five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, Geisinger, K. F. (1994). Cross-cultural normative assessment: Translation on adaptation issues influencing the normative interpretation of assessment instruments. Psychological Assessment, 6, Gomà-i-Freixanet, M., Valero, S., Punti, J., & Zuckerman, M. (2004). Psychometric properties of the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire in a Spanish sample. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 20, Haven, S., & ten Berge, J. M. F. (1977). Tucker s coefficient of congruence as a measure of factorial invariance: An empirical study. Unpublished manuscript. University of Groningen. Joireman, J., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2004). The Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire: Origin, development, and validity of a measure to assess an alternative Five-factor model of personality. In R. M. Stelmack (Ed.),On the psychobiology of personality: Essays in honor of Marvin Zuckerman (pp ). New York: Elsevier Science. McCrae, R. R. (2002). NEO-PI-R Data from 36 cultures: Further intercultural comparisons. In R. R. McCrae & J. Allik (Eds.), The Five-factor model of personality across cultures (pp ). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1985). Comparison of EPI and Psychoticism scales with measures of the Five-factor model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1999). A Five-factor theory of personality. In L. A. Perving & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp ). New York: Guilford. McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., Terracciano, A., Parker, W. D., Mills, C. J., De Fruyt, F., et al. (2002). Personality traits development from age 12 to age 18: Longitudinal, cross-sectional, and cross-cultural analyses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, McCrae, R. R., Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., Riemann, R., & Angleitner, A. (2001). Sources of structure: Genetic, environmental, and artifactual influences on the covariation of personality traits. Journal of Personality, 69, McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the Five-factor Model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60, McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 78 Members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project (2005). Universal features of personality traits from observer s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, McCrae, R. R., Yik, M. S. M., Trapnell, P. D., Bond, M. H., & Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpreting personality profiles across cultures: Bilingual, acculturation, and peer rating studies of Chinese undergraduates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, McCrae, R. R., Zonderman, A. B., Costa, P. T., Jr., Bond, M. H., & Paunonen, S. V. (1996). Evaluating replicability of factors in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory: Confirmatory factor analysis versus Procrustes rotation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, Okazaki, S., & Sue, S. (1995). Methodological issues in assessment research with ethnic minorities. Psychological Assessment, 7, Ostendorf, F., & Angleitner, A. (1994). A comparison of different instruments proposed to measure the big-five. European Review of Applied Psychology, 44, Rolland, J.-P. (2002). The cross-cultural generalizability of the Five-factor model of personality. In R. R. McCrae & J. Allik (Eds.), The Five-factor model of personality across cultures (pp. 7 28). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Rossier, J. (2005). A review of the cross-cultural equivalence of several frequently used personality inventories. International Journal of Educational and Vocational Guidance, 5, Rossier, J., Dahourou, D., & McCrae, R. R. (2005). Structural and mean level analyses of the Five-factor model and Locus of Control: Further evidence from Africa. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, Rossier, J., Meyer de Stadelhofen, F., & Berthoud S. (2004). A comparison of the hierarchical structures of the NEO-PI-R and of the 16PF5. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 20, Rossier, J., Verardi, S., Massoudi, K., & Aluja, A. (in press). Psychometric properties of the French version of the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology. Wu, Y.-X., Wang, W., Du, W.-Y., Li, J., Jiang, X.-F., & Wang, Y.-H. (2000). Development of a Chinese version of the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire: Reliabilities and gender/age effects. Social Behaviour and Personality, 28, Zuckerman, M. (2002). Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ): An alternative Five-factorial model. In B. de Raad & M. Perugini (Eds.), Big Five assessment (pp ). Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers. Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., & Camac, C. (1988). What lies beyond E and N? Factor analyses of scales believed to measure basic dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Joireman, J., Teta, P., & Kraft, M. (1993). A comparison of three structural models for personality : The Big Three, the Big Five, and the Alternative Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Thornquist, M., & Kiers, H. (1991). Five (or three) robust questionnaire scale factors of personality without culture. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, Jérôme Rossier Institute of Psychology University of Lausanne Anthropole-3127 CH-1015 Lausanne, Switzerland Jerome.Rossier@unil.ch Received June 22, 2006 Revised November 27, 2006

El-nabgha Fathy Mohammed, Michael Unher and Masakazu Sugawara (Accepted 4 March, 2009)

El-nabgha Fathy Mohammed, Michael Unher and Masakazu Sugawara (Accepted 4 March, 2009) El-nabgha Fathy Mohammed, Michael Unher and Masakazu Sugawara (Accepted 4 March, 2009) Abstract : The present study aimed to identify the differences between Japanese and Egyptian university students in

More information

Escapable Conclusions: Toomela (2003) and the Universality of Trait Structure

Escapable Conclusions: Toomela (2003) and the Universality of Trait Structure Journal of Personality and Social Psychology In the public domain 2004, Vol. 87, No. 2, 261 265 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.261 Escapable Conclusions: Toomela (2003) and the Universality of Trait Structure

More information

Personality measures under focus: The NEO-PI-R and the MBTI

Personality measures under focus: The NEO-PI-R and the MBTI : The NEO-PI-R and the MBTI Author Published 2009 Journal Title Griffith University Undergraduate Psychology Journal Downloaded from http://hdl.handle.net/10072/340329 Link to published version http://pandora.nla.gov.au/tep/145784

More information

NEO-PI-R Factor Structure in College Students

NEO-PI-R Factor Structure in College Students 17 Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology, January 2009, Vol. 35, No.1, 17-25. Special Feature NEO-PI-R Factor Structure in College Students Kamlesh Singh Indian Institute of Technology, New

More information

Spanish normative data of the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire in a general population sample

Spanish normative data of the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire in a general population sample Psicothema 28. Vol. 2, nº 2, pp. 324-33 www.psicothema.com ISSN 214-9915 CODEN PSOTEG Copyright 28 Psicothema Spanish normative data of the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire in a general population

More information

Personality and Individual Differences

Personality and Individual Differences Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008) 649 654 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Reliability and

More information

A comparison between three and ve factor models of Pakistani personality data

A comparison between three and ve factor models of Pakistani personality data Personality and Individual Differences 31 2001) 1311±1319 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid A comparison between three and ve factor models of Pakistani personality data Shagufta Aziz a, Chris J. Jackson b,

More information

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81.

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81. MSP RESEARCH NOTE B5PQ Reliability and Validity This research note describes the reliability and validity of the B5PQ. Evidence for the reliability and validity of is presented against some of the key

More information

Scientific Information System

Scientific Information System Scientific Information System Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Anton Aluja, Angel Blanch The Five and Seven Factors Personality Models: Differences and

More information

Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries

Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries Donald Macnab Psychometrics Canada Copyright Psychometrics Canada 2011. All rights reserved. The Work Personality Index is a trademark of

More information

Chiba University AKIO WAKABAYASHI 1 *

Chiba University AKIO WAKABAYASHI 1 * bs_bs_banner Japanese Psychological Research 2014, Volume 56, No. 3, 211 223 doi: 10.1111/jpr.12045 A sixth personality domain that is independent of the Big Five domains: The psychometric properties of

More information

Reliability, factor structure and validity of the Dutch Eysenck Personality Pro ler

Reliability, factor structure and validity of the Dutch Eysenck Personality Pro ler Personality and Individual Di erences 29 (2000) 857±868 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Reliability, factor structure and validity of the Dutch Eysenck Personality Pro ler Peter Muris a, *, Henk Schmidt b,

More information

Traits & Trait Taxonomies

Traits & Trait Taxonomies Traits & Trait Taxonomies Chapter 3 Three fundamental questions guide those who study traits: How should we conceptualize traits? How can we identify which traits are the most important from among the

More information

Age Trends and Age Norms for the NEO Personality Inventory 3 in Adolescents and Adults

Age Trends and Age Norms for the NEO Personality Inventory 3 in Adolescents and Adults ASSESSMENT McCrae 10.1177/1073191105279724 et al. / NEO-PI-3 Trends and Norms for the NEO Personality Inventory 3 in Adolescents and Adults Robert R. McCrae National Institute on Aging Thomas A. Martin

More information

Rosenthal, Montoya, Ridings, Rieck, and Hooley (2011) Appendix A. Supplementary material

Rosenthal, Montoya, Ridings, Rieck, and Hooley (2011) Appendix A. Supplementary material NPI Factors and 16-Item NPI Rosenthal, Montoya, Ridings, Rieck, and Hooley (2011) Table 2 examines the relation of four sets of published factor-based NPI subscales and a shortened version of the NPI to

More information

The Doctrine of Traits. Lecture 29

The Doctrine of Traits. Lecture 29 The Doctrine of Traits Lecture 29 1 The Doctrine of Traits Allport (1937) [A trait is] a generalized and focalized neuropsychic system... with the capacity to render many stimuli functionally equivalent,

More information

Multiple Act criterion:

Multiple Act criterion: Common Features of Trait Theories Generality and Stability of Traits: Trait theorists all use consistencies in an individual s behavior and explain why persons respond in different ways to the same stimulus

More information

Abstract. Key words: bias, culture, Five-Factor Model, language, NEO-PI-R, NEO-PI-3, personality, South Africa

Abstract. Key words: bias, culture, Five-Factor Model, language, NEO-PI-R, NEO-PI-3, personality, South Africa Dedication I dedicate this research to my parents who have supported me throughout my university career. Without their emotional and financial support, none of my academic accolades could have been possible.

More information

Test Partnership TPAQ Series Psychometric Properties

Test Partnership TPAQ Series Psychometric Properties Test Partnership TPAQ Series Psychometric Properties 2018 1 Construct Validity The IPIP-NEO-120 (Johnson, 2014) is a validated measure of the Big-5 model of personality, specifically the OCEAN model (Costa

More information

Measurement equivalence of the Big Five: Shedding further light on potential causes of the educational bias. Rammstedt, B. & Kemper, C. J.

Measurement equivalence of the Big Five: Shedding further light on potential causes of the educational bias. Rammstedt, B. & Kemper, C. J. Measurement equivalence of the Big Five: Shedding further light on potential causes of the educational bias Rammstedt, B. & Kemper, C. J. GESIS - Leibniz-Institute for the Social Sciences, Mannheim, Germany

More information

Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality

Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality Graduate Faculty Psychology Bulletin Volume 3, No. 1, 2005 Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality Brian Norensberg, M.A. 1 & Peter Zachar Ph.D. 2 Abstract ~ This study investigates the

More information

Impulsivity is Important

Impulsivity is Important Impulsivity is Important Involved in every major system of personality Vital role in the understanding & diagnosis of psychopathology: - DSM IV impulse control disorders - Criteria for BPD, ASPD, ADHD

More information

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) PSYSOC 2013

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) PSYSOC 2013 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 140 ( 2014 ) 506 510 PSYSOC 2013 Personality Traits and Different Career Stages A Study on Indian School

More information

Client Personality and Preference for Counseling Approach: Does Match Matter?

Client Personality and Preference for Counseling Approach: Does Match Matter? CLIENT PERSONALITY AND PREFERENCE 33 Professional Issues in Counseling 2010, Volume 10, Article 4, p. 33-39 Client Personality and Preference for Counseling Approach: Does Match Matter? Client Personality

More information

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas

Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas 1 Three Subfactors of the Empathic Personality Kimberly A. Barchard, University of Nevada, Las Vegas Reference: Barchard, K.A. (2002, May). Three subfactors of the empathic personality. Poster presented

More information

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children

Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Individual Differences of Temperament in Primary School Children Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia - Social and Behavioral Scienc es 86 ( 2013 ) 435 440 V Congress of Russian Psychological Society Genetic and Environmental Influences on

More information

Sociodemographic Effects on the Test-Retest Reliability of the Big Five Inventory. Timo Gnambs. Osnabrück University. Author Note

Sociodemographic Effects on the Test-Retest Reliability of the Big Five Inventory. Timo Gnambs. Osnabrück University. Author Note Running head: TEST-RETEST RELIABILITY Sociodemographic Effects on the Test-Retest Reliability of the Big Five Inventory Timo Gnambs Osnabrück University Author Note Timo Gnambs, Institute of Psychology,

More information

ADHD and Personality Disorders

ADHD and Personality Disorders ADHD and Personality Disorders J. Antoni Ramos-Quiroga Department of Psychiatry Hopsital Universitari Vall d Hebron Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona Geneva Centro de Investigación Biomédica En Red de

More information

The Relationship between Personality Styles and Academic Self- Efficacy among Jordanian EFL Learners

The Relationship between Personality Styles and Academic Self- Efficacy among Jordanian EFL Learners Journal of Global Management Research (Online) ISSN 2367-962X P a g e 9 The Relationship between Personality Styles and Academic Self- Efficacy among Jordanian EFL Learners Dr. Nadia AHOUARI-IDRI, and

More information

A new five factor model of psychopathology: Preliminary psychometric characteristics of the five-dimensional personality test (5DPT) q

A new five factor model of psychopathology: Preliminary psychometric characteristics of the five-dimensional personality test (5DPT) q Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2008) 1326 1334 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid A new five factor model of psychopathology: Preliminary psychometric characteristics

More information

Psychobiology of Personality

Psychobiology of Personality Psychobiology of Personality SECOND EDITION, REVISED AND UPDATED Praise for the first edition An up-to-date review of thinking and research dealing with personality, primarily from a psychobiological point

More information

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 1633 1639 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Short Communication The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic

More information

Universal Features of Personality Traits from the Observer s Perspective: Data from 50 Cultures. Robert R. McCrae and Antonio Terracciano

Universal Features of Personality Traits from the Observer s Perspective: Data from 50 Cultures. Robert R. McCrae and Antonio Terracciano This is the Pre-Published Version Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, in press. Universal Features of Personality Traits from the Observer s Perspective: Data from 50 Cultures Robert R. McCrae

More information

Psychometric Properties of Farsi Version State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2 (FSTAXI-2)

Psychometric Properties of Farsi Version State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2 (FSTAXI-2) Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia - Social and Behavioral Scienc es 82 ( 2013 ) 325 329 World Conference on Psychology and Sociology 2012 Psychometric Properties of Farsi Version State-Trait

More information

Making a psychometric. Dr Benjamin Cowan- Lecture 9

Making a psychometric. Dr Benjamin Cowan- Lecture 9 Making a psychometric Dr Benjamin Cowan- Lecture 9 What this lecture will cover What is a questionnaire? Development of questionnaires Item development Scale options Scale reliability & validity Factor

More information

CHAPTER 2 Personality Traits: A Good Theory

CHAPTER 2 Personality Traits: A Good Theory CHAPTER 2 Personality Traits: A Good Theory Behavioural residue: when people live in an environment, they leave behind traces that hint at the personality of the person. What is a personality trait? Def.:

More information

Paul Irwing, Manchester Business School

Paul Irwing, Manchester Business School Paul Irwing, Manchester Business School Factor analysis has been the prime statistical technique for the development of structural theories in social science, such as the hierarchical factor model of human

More information

Fifty scales of grey? - A common analysis of dominance self-report scales

Fifty scales of grey? - A common analysis of dominance self-report scales Fifty scales of grey? - A common analysis of dominance self-report scales Carolin Palmer 13th European Conference on Psychological Assessment July 22-25, 2015 Zurich, Switzerland what is dominance? dominance

More information

The Difference Analysis between Demographic Variables and Personal Attributes The Case of Internal Auditors in Taiwan

The Difference Analysis between Demographic Variables and Personal Attributes The Case of Internal Auditors in Taiwan The Difference Analysis between Demographic Variables and Personal Attributes The Case of Internal Auditors in Taiwan Li-Jia Chiu and Neng-Tang Norman Huang Department of Technology Application and Human

More information

Relational tendencies associated with broad personality dimensions

Relational tendencies associated with broad personality dimensions 1 British Journal of Medical Psychology (2004), 00, 1 6 q 2004 The British Psychological Society www.bps.org.uk Brief report Relational tendencies associated with broad personality dimensions Dionyssios

More information

Follow this and additional works at: Part of the Psychology Commons

Follow this and additional works at:   Part of the Psychology Commons Grand Valley State University ScholarWorks@GVSU Papers from the International Association for Cross- Cultural Psychology Conferences IACCP 2011 Structure of Acculturation Attitudes and their Relationships

More information

USING PERSONALITY TESTS IN RESEARCH: ARE LONGER TESTS NECESSARILY BETTER?

USING PERSONALITY TESTS IN RESEARCH: ARE LONGER TESTS NECESSARILY BETTER? Chapter #23 USING PERSONALITY TESTS IN RESEARCH: ARE LONGER TESTS NECESSARILY BETTER? David Freeze 1, Tracy A. Freeze 2, Lisa A. Best 1, & Kathryn E. Flood 1 1 Department of Psychology, University of New

More information

Key words: State-Trait Anger, Anger Expression, Anger Control, FSTAXI-2, reliability, validity.

Key words: State-Trait Anger, Anger Expression, Anger Control, FSTAXI-2, reliability, validity. Psychometric Properties of Farsi version of the Spielberger s State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2 (FSTAXI-2) Mohammad Khodayarifard 1, Charles D. Spielberger 2 Masoud Gholamali Lavasani 1, Saeed Akbari

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

The five-factor personality structure of dissociative experiences

The five-factor personality structure of dissociative experiences The five-factor personality structure of dissociative experiences By: Thomas R. Kwapil, Michael J. Wrobel and Cameron A. Pope Kwapil, T.R., Wrobel, M.J., & Pope, C.A. (2002). The five-factor personality

More information

DOES SELF-EMPLOYED WORK MAKE 15-YEAR LONGITUDINAL PERSONALITY- BASED ANALYSIS

DOES SELF-EMPLOYED WORK MAKE 15-YEAR LONGITUDINAL PERSONALITY- BASED ANALYSIS Frontiers of Entrepreneurship Research Volume 35 Issue 3 CHAPTER III. THE ENTREPRENEUR AND CHARACTERISTICS Article 2 6-13-2015 DOES SELF-EMPLOYED WORK MAKE INDIVIDUALS NOT ONLY MORE ENTREPRENEURIAL BUT

More information

Using metaphorical items for describing personality constructs

Using metaphorical items for describing personality constructs Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 33 (2012) 178 182 PSIWORLD 2011 Using metaphorical items for describing personality constructs Catalin Nedelcea a *, Iulia Ciorbea b, Andrei Gabriel Ion c a Department

More information

The NEO PI-R in a North European Context

The NEO PI-R in a North European Context This file was downloaded from the institutional repository BI Brage - http://brage.bibsys.no/bi (Open Access) The NEO PI-R in a North European Context Øyvind Lund Martinsen BI Norwegian Business School

More information

Personality. Outline. Trait Approach. Individual Traits 10/22/2012. Lexical hypothesis: Enduring patterns of how people adapt to the world

Personality. Outline. Trait Approach. Individual Traits 10/22/2012. Lexical hypothesis: Enduring patterns of how people adapt to the world Outline Defining Personality The Big 5 Measuring Personality Across Cultures Cross-Cultural Studies of Personality Traits Big 5 Across Cultures Big 5 and National Character Where do we get our personality?

More information

Relationship between Teachers' Personality Traits and Self Efficacy: An Empirical Analysis of School Teachers in Karaikal Region (Puducherry)

Relationship between Teachers' Personality Traits and Self Efficacy: An Empirical Analysis of School Teachers in Karaikal Region (Puducherry) Volume 8, Issue 3, September 2015 Relationship between Teachers' Personality Traits and Self Efficacy: An Empirical Analysis of School Teachers in Karaikal Region (Puducherry) Dr. N. S. PANDEY Assistant

More information

The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items

The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items The more like me, the better : Individual differences in social desirability ratings of personality items Kenn Konstabel Department of Psychology, Estonian Centre of Behavioural and Health Sciences University

More information

A Structured Interview for the Assessment of the Five-Factor Model of Personality: Facet-Level Relations to the Axis II Personality Disorders

A Structured Interview for the Assessment of the Five-Factor Model of Personality: Facet-Level Relations to the Axis II Personality Disorders A Structured Interview for the Assessment of the Five-Factor Model of Personality: Facet-Level Relations to the Axis II Personality Disorders Timothy J. Trull University of Missouri Columbia Thomas A.

More information

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the M5-50: An Implementation of the International Personality Item Pool Item Set

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the M5-50: An Implementation of the International Personality Item Pool Item Set Psychological Assessment 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 22, No. 1, 43 49 1040-3590/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0017371 Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the M5-50: An Implementation of the

More information

Mini-project 2 report

Mini-project 2 report Mini-project 2 report Why E-Futures? Understanding the motivation of research students in doctoral training centres. Seyed Mohammad Ebrahimi Dtp10se@sheffield.ac.uk Supervised by: Dr Chris Jones 13/05/2011

More information

The Three-Factor Theory of Personality. David Center. There has been a plethora of theories of personality and measures of

The Three-Factor Theory of Personality. David Center. There has been a plethora of theories of personality and measures of The Three-Factor Theory of Personality David Center There has been a plethora of theories of personality and measures of personality generated by psychologists during this century. However, something approaching

More information

CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT

CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT CLINICAL VS. BEHAVIOR ASSESSMENT Informal Tes3ng Personality Tes3ng Assessment Procedures Ability Tes3ng The Clinical Interview 3 Defining Clinical Assessment The process of assessing the client through

More information

Personality: Definitions

Personality: Definitions Personality: Definitions Anastasi "Personality tests are instruments for the measurement of emotional, motivational, interpersonal and attitudinal characteristics, as distinct from abilities. Kaplan &

More information

The hierarchical structure of emotional expressivity: scale development and nomological implications

The hierarchical structure of emotional expressivity: scale development and nomological implications University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Theses and Dissertations Spring 2010 The hierarchical structure of emotional expressivity: scale development and nomological implications John Jeffrey Humrichouse

More information

Personality and intelligence as predictors of academic achievement: A cross-sectional study from elementary to secondary school

Personality and intelligence as predictors of academic achievement: A cross-sectional study from elementary to secondary school Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007) 441 451 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Personality and intelligence as predictors of academic achievement: A cross-sectional study from elementary to secondary

More information

Instrument equivalence across ethnic groups. Antonio Olmos (MHCD) Susan R. Hutchinson (UNC)

Instrument equivalence across ethnic groups. Antonio Olmos (MHCD) Susan R. Hutchinson (UNC) Instrument equivalence across ethnic groups Antonio Olmos (MHCD) Susan R. Hutchinson (UNC) Overview Instrument Equivalence Measurement Invariance Invariance in Reliability Scores Factorial Invariance Item

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Personality and Individual Differences 50 (2011) 142 147 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Predicting social

More information

Cross-cultural study of person-centred quality of life domains and indicators: a replication

Cross-cultural study of person-centred quality of life domains and indicators: a replication 734 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 49 part 10 pp 734 739 october 2005 Blackwell Science, LtdOxford, UKJIRJournal of Intellectual Disability Research0964-2633Blackwell Publishing Ltd,

More information

The stability of personality over time as a function of personality trait dominance

The stability of personality over time as a function of personality trait dominance The stability of personality over time as a function of personality trait dominance Author Published 2009 Journal Title Griffith University Undergraduate Psychology Journal Downloaded from http://hdl.handle.net/10072/340327

More information

Journal of Communication Disorders

Journal of Communication Disorders Journal of Communication Disorders 44 (2011) 218 222 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Communication Disorders Investigating personality in stuttering: Results of a case control study

More information

Prediction of Attitudes Towards Narcotics and Prediction of Increased Risk for Depression in Recruits with New Psychological Tools

Prediction of Attitudes Towards Narcotics and Prediction of Increased Risk for Depression in Recruits with New Psychological Tools Office of Naval Research International Field Office IAMPS 2001 Recruitment and Retention Workshop Estonia Research Grant Prediction of Attitudes Towards Narcotics and Prediction of Increased Risk for Depression

More information

Emotional Intelligence and Leadership

Emotional Intelligence and Leadership The Mayer Salovey Caruso Notes Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) 2 The Mayer Salovey Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) 2 The MSCEIT 2 measures four related abilities. 3 Perceiving Facilitating

More information

Quality of Life Assessment of Growth Hormone Deficiency in Adults (QoL-AGHDA)

Quality of Life Assessment of Growth Hormone Deficiency in Adults (QoL-AGHDA) Quality of Life Assessment of Growth Hormone Deficiency in Adults (QoL-AGHDA) Guidelines for Users May 2016 B1 Chorlton Mill, 3 Cambridge Street, Manchester, M1 5BY, UK Tel: +44 (0)161 226 4446 Fax: +44

More information

DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE JAPANESE SCALE OF MINDFULNESS SKILLS BASED ON DBT STRATEGIES

DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE JAPANESE SCALE OF MINDFULNESS SKILLS BASED ON DBT STRATEGIES DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE JAPANESE SCALE OF MINDFULNESS SKILLS BASED ON DBT STRATEGIES Keiko Nakano Department of Clinical Psychology/Atomi University JAPAN ABSTRACT The present study reports findings

More information

Research on the topic of personality structure has. Higher Order Factors of Personality: Do They Exist?

Research on the topic of personality structure has. Higher Order Factors of Personality: Do They Exist? Pers Soc Psychol Rev OnlineFirst, published on May 20, 2009 as doi:10.1177/1088868309338467 Higher Order Factors of Personality: Do They Exist? Michael C. Ashton Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario,

More information

Are Cross-Cultural Comparisons of Personality Profiles Meaningful? Differential Item and Facet Functioning in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory

Are Cross-Cultural Comparisons of Personality Profiles Meaningful? Differential Item and Facet Functioning in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Are Cross-Cultural Comparisons of Personality Profiles Meaningful? Differential Item and Facet Functioning in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory A. Timothy

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

IJPSS Volume 2, Issue 7 ISSN:

IJPSS Volume 2, Issue 7 ISSN: Teachers` as a Leader and their Traits: Evidence from secondary level Dr.Tahseen Mehmood Aslam* Zulfiqar Ali** Ijaz Ahmad Tatlah*** Dr Muhammad Iqbal**** _ Abstract Teacher must act as a leader because

More information

Correcting Big Five personality measurements for acquiescence: An 18-country cross-cultural study. Beatrice Rammstedt Christoph J. Kemper Ingwer Borg

Correcting Big Five personality measurements for acquiescence: An 18-country cross-cultural study. Beatrice Rammstedt Christoph J. Kemper Ingwer Borg Correcting Big Five personality measurements for acquiescence: An 18-country cross-cultural study Beatrice Rammstedt Christoph J. Kemper Ingwer Borg GESIS Leibniz Institute for the Social Sciences, Mannheim,

More information

The comparison of sensation seeking and five big factors of personality between internet dependents and non-dependents

The comparison of sensation seeking and five big factors of personality between internet dependents and non-dependents Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 15 (2011) 1029 1033 WCES-2011 The comparison of sensation seeking and five big factors of personality between internet

More information

Personality level on the big five and the structure of intelligence

Personality level on the big five and the structure of intelligence Personality and Individual Differences 40 (2006) 909 917 www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Personality level on the big five and the structure of intelligence Sergio Escorial a, *, Luis F. García b, Lara Cuevas

More information

GENDER DIFFERENCES IN PREDICTING UNSAFE DRIVING BEHAVIORS IN YOUNG ADULTS

GENDER DIFFERENCES IN PREDICTING UNSAFE DRIVING BEHAVIORS IN YOUNG ADULTS GENDER DIFFERENCES IN PREDICTING UNSAFE DRIVING BEHAVIORS IN YOUNG ADULTS Nicole R. Skaar, John E. Williams University of Northern Iowa Cedar Falls, Iowa, USA E-mail: nskaar24@uni.edu Summary: Past research

More information

Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality: A Longitudinal Multiple-Rater Twin Study

Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality: A Longitudinal Multiple-Rater Twin Study Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 98, No. 6, 995 1008 0022-3514/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019558 Sources of Cumulative Continuity in Personality:

More information

They re Not Abnormal and We re Not Making Them Abnormal : A Longitudinal Study

They re Not Abnormal and We re Not Making Them Abnormal : A Longitudinal Study Curtin, L., Martz, D., Bazzini, D., & Vicente, B. (2004). They're not "abnormal" and we're not making them "abnormal": A longitudinal study. Teaching of Psychology, 31(1): 51-53. (Winter 2004) Published

More information

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2008) 1060 1066 Brief Report Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies

More information

Personality, academic majors and performance: Revealing complex patterns Author(s): Vedel, A., Thomsen, D. K., & Larsen, L.

Personality, academic majors and performance: Revealing complex patterns Author(s): Vedel, A., Thomsen, D. K., & Larsen, L. Coversheet This is the accepted manuscript (post-print version) of the article. Contentwise, the post-print version is identical to the final published version, but there may be differences in typography

More information

Halesworth & District. Malcolm Ballantine

Halesworth & District. Malcolm Ballantine Halesworth & District Malcolm Ballantine Personality Assessment What is personality? Two Approaches Trait: Many independent descriptors Type: Single pithy descriptor Two Approaches - Examples Trait approach

More information

Five Factor Model Prototype Matching Scores: Convergence Within Alternative Methods

Five Factor Model Prototype Matching Scores: Convergence Within Alternative Methods Journal of Personality Disorders, 25(5), 571 585, 2011 2011 The Guilford Press Five Factor Model Prototype Matching Scores: Convergence Within Alternative Methods Douglas B. Samuel, PhD, Maryanne Edmundson,

More information

The NEO PI 3: A More Readable Revised NEO Personality Inventory

The NEO PI 3: A More Readable Revised NEO Personality Inventory Journal of Personality Assessment ISSN: 0022-3891 (Print) 1532-7752 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjpa20 The NEO PI 3: A More Readable Revised NEO Personality Inventory Robert

More information

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model 50 Educational Measurement and Evaluation Review (2012), Vol. 3, 50-59 2012 Philippine Educational Measurement and Evaluation Association Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model Adonis

More information

The complete Insight Technical Manual includes a comprehensive section on validity. INSIGHT Inventory 99.72% % % Mean

The complete Insight Technical Manual includes a comprehensive section on validity. INSIGHT Inventory 99.72% % % Mean Technical Manual INSIGHT Inventory 99.72% Percentage of cases 95.44 % 68.26 % -3 SD -2 SD -1 SD +1 SD +2 SD +3 SD Mean Percentage Distribution of Cases in a Normal Curve IV. TEST DEVELOPMENT Historical

More information

The Relationship of Trait EI with Personality, IQ and Sex in a UK Sample of Employees

The Relationship of Trait EI with Personality, IQ and Sex in a UK Sample of Employees The Relationship of Trait EI with Personality, IQ and Sex in a UK Sample of Employees Adriane Arteche*, Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic*, Adrian Furnham** and John Crump*** *Goldsmiths, University of London, London,

More information

CHAPTER 2. RESEARCH METHODS AND PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT (64 items)

CHAPTER 2. RESEARCH METHODS AND PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT (64 items) CHAPTER 2. RESEARCH METHODS AND PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT (64 items) 1. Darwin s point of view about empirical research can be accurately summarized as... a. Any observation is better than no observation

More information

Methodology Introduction of the study Statement of Problem Objective Hypothesis Method

Methodology Introduction of the study Statement of Problem Objective Hypothesis Method 3.1. Introduction of the study 3.2. Statement of Problem 3.3. Objective 3.4. Hypothesis 3.5. Method 3.5.1. Procedure Sample A.5.2. Variable A.5.3. Research Design A.5.4. Operational Definition Of The Terms

More information

The State Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI)

The State Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI) The State Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI) The STAXI was developed with two goals in mind.. The first was to develop a measure of the components of anger in the context of both normal and abnormal

More information

The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism, Stressor and Stress Response

The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism, Stressor and Stress Response International Journal of Economics and Finance; Vol. 7, No. 8; 2015 ISSN 1916-971X E-ISSN 1916-9728 Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism,

More information

Personality. Chapter 13

Personality. Chapter 13 + Personality Chapter 13 + Personality An individual s characteristic pattern of thinking, feeling, and acting. Each Dwarf has a distinct personality. + Psychodynamic Perspective Freud s clinical experience

More information

Personality. Unit 3: Developmental Psychology

Personality. Unit 3: Developmental Psychology Personality Unit 3: Developmental Psychology Personality Personality: The consistent, enduring, and unique characteristics of a person. There are many personality theories that provide a way of organizing

More information

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 6.1 Research Design Research is an organized, systematic, data based, critical, objective, scientific inquiry or investigation into a specific problem, undertaken with the

More information

1. Evaluate the methodological quality of a study with the COSMIN checklist

1. Evaluate the methodological quality of a study with the COSMIN checklist Answers 1. Evaluate the methodological quality of a study with the COSMIN checklist We follow the four steps as presented in Table 9.2. Step 1: The following measurement properties are evaluated in the

More information

Correspondence should be addressed to Krisztian Jozsa;

Correspondence should be addressed to Krisztian Jozsa; Child Development Research, Article ID 803061, 16 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2014/803061 Research Article Age and Cultural Differences in Self-Perceptions of Mastery Motivation and Competence in American,

More information

Development of Big Five Domains and Facets in Adulthood: Mean-Level Age Trends and Broadly Versus Narrowly Acting Mechanisms

Development of Big Five Domains and Facets in Adulthood: Mean-Level Age Trends and Broadly Versus Narrowly Acting Mechanisms Development of Big Five Domains and Facets in Adulthood: Mean-Level Age Trends and Broadly Versus Narrowly Acting Mechanisms Christopher J. Soto 1 and Oliver P. John 2 1 Colby College 2 University of California,

More information

THE INDIVIDUALISM-COLLECTIVISM INTERPERSONAL ASSESSMENT INVENTORY (ICIAI):

THE INDIVIDUALISM-COLLECTIVISM INTERPERSONAL ASSESSMENT INVENTORY (ICIAI): Matsumoto ICIAI Test Booklet page 8 THE INDIVIDUALISM-COLLECTIVISM INTERPERSONAL ASSESSMENT INVENTORY (ICIAI): General IC Indices Scoring Key We have used the ICIAI to produce general indices of IC scores

More information

2016 Sara Patricia Bolen ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

2016 Sara Patricia Bolen ALL RIGHTS RESERVED 2016 Sara Patricia Bolen ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Foremost, I would like to thank my advisor, Dr. Allen, for the countless hours he spent guiding me in this rewarding process. His selfless

More information

Personality, Sensation Seeking and Holiday Preference. Wiebke Zumdick S Münster

Personality, Sensation Seeking and Holiday Preference. Wiebke Zumdick S Münster Personality, Sensation Seeking and Holiday Preference Wiebke Zumdick S0071242 31.08.2007 Münster Bachelorthese Opleiding Psychologie University of Twente, Enschede Abstract The objective of this study

More information

Personality Traits and the Feeling of Loneliness of Post-Graduate University Students

Personality Traits and the Feeling of Loneliness of Post-Graduate University Students The International Journal of Indian Psychology ISSN 2348-5396 (e) ISSN: 2349-3429 (p) Volume 3, Issue 3, No.1, DIP: 18.01.005/20160303 ISBN: 978-1-365-03416-9 http://www.ijip.in April - June, 2016 Personality

More information