Similarity and Categorization : Taxonomic and Meronomic parts of Similes

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Similarity and Categorization : Taxonomic and Meronomic parts of Similes"

Transcription

1 Similarity and Categorization : Taxonomic and Meronomic parts of Similes Charles Tijus Cognition & Usages 2 rue de la Liberté Saint-Denis tijus@univ-paris8.fr Sébastien Poitrenaud Cognition & Usages 2 rue de la Liberté Saint-Denis poitrenaud@univ-paris8.fr Denis Chene Cognition & Usages 2 rue de la Liberté Saint-Denis chene@francetelecom.com Abstract What is a category made of? What are a category's different parts? We propose that the parts of a category are the properties of this category, which is to say that parts of the definition of a category are the properties listed to describe this category. Thus, the color "gray", which is not a physical part of a mouse, is a part of the description of the "mouse category", whilst "whiskers" that is a physical part of a given mouse and is also a part of the description of the "mouse category". We shall define partonomy as the decomposition of an object into its physical parts and meronomy as the decomposition of a category description into its cognitive parts and show how this approach helps to solve some of problems of computing similarity by first building a hierarchy of contextual categories that provides a way to measure similarity. Keywords: Similarity, Categorization, Galois Lattice. 1 How similar is mouse to elephant? Measuring similarity is a key problem in A.I. and in Cognitive science and finding out how similar something (an object, an image, an idea, a text, a situation, a discovery) is to something else would be helpful for the purposes of detection, identification, analogy, thinking, reasoning and problem solving [5, 15]. The usual problem involves decision making: given some knowledge about x, to measure how similar y is to x and, then to accept or reject y as being of the same kind than x. Thus, the decision-making problem is to provide as output a categorization decision about y, given a similarity value between x and y as input. People are supposed to daily solve this kind of decision-making problem. Surprisingly, studies in cognitive psychology show, first, that similarity is not necessarily transitive: For instance, if similarity (Russia, Cuba) = 7 and similarity (Cuba, Jamaica) = 8, then, one should expect similarity (Russia, Jamaica) to be around 7, but the similarity score quoted is 1! Second, similarity is not necessarily asymmetric [17]: x can be quoted as more similar to y, than y to x. The letter «F» is more similar to «E», than «E» to «F». Third, people put things that they have evaluated to be very similar into different categories: similarity does not fit with categorization [5, 8, 13]. A first solution proposed by nonmetric multidimensional scaling (MDS) models, based on Richardson s ideas, could be that the dimensions for comparison differ. A second solution is that natural categorization is guided by similarity, but that people do optimize categorization performance by paying attention to some core properties that are weighted more in similarity than in categorization [10]. However, the problem remains to identify what are the right properties to consider for computing similarity.

2 For instance, Signal Detection Theory is useful for the evaluation of how good a detection system is, if it is possible to define precisely what x and y are and what x and y are not. However, contrary to the Ideal Receiver of the Signal Detection Theory, we usually do not know what x and y are, and are not for computing similarity between x and y. In short, we don t know along which dimensions x and y have to be compared? Thus an initial problem, - the definition problem-, is to find the right dimensions along which x and y can be compared, which means having the right description of x and y. As another solution, we advocate that human categorization is not guided by similarity. That is similarity that is based on categorization. We argue that similarity is the product of categorization and not the reverse. Thus the problem at hand would be how categories are constructed in such a way similar objects could belong to different categories and dissimilar objects could belong to a same category. We present the contextual reciprocal effect theory of categorization [12, 15] and discuss how it may help to solve some of these similarity-categorization problems, to better understand how people process but also for automatic categorization. 2 /Mammal/ is part of both /Elephant/ and /Mouse/ The physical and ecological world is composed of objects. Any object has a number of parts and an arrangement of these parts that defines its structure. Any object also has features along physical dimensions (what it is made of, its shape, color, brightness, ). For people, physical objects do have the physical properties they perceive and the unseen physical properties they know as being those of the objects, but also the cognitive (non-physical) dimensions (functionality and usability) of the object. Note that function and the how-to-use it (cognitive properties) should be more crucial dimensions for people and should be important criteria for categorization, but that parts and arrangements of parts (physical dimensions) justify why an object can be used for a function and why it should be used that way. Verbal descriptors of an object by people can include words that designate parts, and the way parts are arranged, as well as physical and cognitive features. Unlike physical objects, categories are cognitive (or mental) entities. They are representations of groups of objects for cognitive purposes: identification (this is a cat), inference making (it should like drinking milk), and so on. Thus, a category is a three terms "concept": the category itself as a group of objects and a group of shared properties, its extension (the objects it groups: cats) and its intension (the criteria for grouping: eg. having a cat s shape, having hairs, it hums, it scratches, ). Moreover, for cognitive purposes, categories entail categories (cats are mammals, mammals are animals, animals are living beings, ). As entailment of categories forms a hierarchy of categories, we shall use the term taxonomy with a is a kind of link between categories (a cat is a kind of animal, an animal is a kind of living thing). The classical and implicit view of categorization is that one object is to belong to one category, and to one hierarchy of categories. It has been shown [2] that categories are created for goal purpose (objects saved from the fire are objects from a burning house). Our proposal is that people create any kind of categories and of hierarchies of categories along inclusive dimensions (e.g. the objects that are on the table, are objects in the room, are objects in the building, and so on) and that any kind of properties of the objects can be used to build categories. That is what we name contextual categorization. For instance, a book and a newspaper can be categorized as things to read. Adding garbage, the book, the newspaper and the bottle can then be category of things to put in the garbage, and the book an object to save from garbage, while adding Mary s bag can made the book, the newspaper and the bottle

3 being things Mary is going to transport, and the bottle things to be first filled with water. These are examples of on-line categories that people should build in situation in order to understand what happens around them. What is determining the properties are the goal at hand and the way objects can be aligned in order to build categories that differentiate the objects. Thus, categories are not only the well formed dictionary categories, but also contextual categories, each object being categorized relative to the others objects. It follows that two objects (let s say the cat and the dog of Mary) that could be in some circumstances in a same category in a context (Mary feeding her pets), would be in different categories in another context (things Mary is going to bring to the Park), while objects that are quite dissimilar (the cat and the book of Mary) can be put in the same category (things Mary is going to bring to the Park). Given the current situation, we can predict intransitivity, why Mary s dog and cat could have a high similarity score, why Mary s dog and book could have a high similarity score and why Mary s cat and book would have a low similarity score. We can also predict asymmetry in similarity: Mary s dog is more similar than Mary s cat because the dog is a pet as the cat, although the cat is not a pet as the dog that Mary is going to bring to the Park (see figure 2). The main proposal is that categories are built in context: the objects at hand are categorized in one hierarchy of categories. This cognitive flexibility that is responsible for creating categories from properties in context allows metonymy (a Mary s friend in the park: the book and the dog are coming to be understood as the coming of Mary in the Park), and metaphor construction [15]. We formalize this flexibility by what we call meronomy. Meronomy is the whole-to-part decomposition of a category. Meronomy has to be differentiated from partonomy that is the decomposition of an object into its physical parts. The color " gray", which is not a physical part of a mouse, is a part of the description of the mouse category, whilst whiskers which is a physical part, is also a part of the description of the "mouse category". Based on this theoretical distinction, we argue first, that taxonomy (is a kind of) and meronomy (is part of) are two sides of the same coin, X is a kind of Y and Y a part of X, - what Rips and Conrad [13, 14] named the reciprocal effect -, and second, that this approach is helpful for the similarity-categorization problem. 3 Reciprocal effect of categorization Categorization is the ability to group objects within a hierarchy of categories. We focus on categorization instead of category because the cognitive purpose of a category is not primarily to group, but to differentiate between objects. Differentiating between objects is achieved by creating subcategories and, in doing so, hierarchies of categories. Hierarchies of categories have a mathematical formalism labeled Galois Lattices [1, 11]. The On x Pm boolean matrix, which indicates whether each of n objects has or not each of m properties, enables the creation of one hierarchy of categories with transitivity, asymmetry and reflexivity. The maximum number of categories is either 2 n-1, or m if m < 2 n-1, in a lattice whose complexity depends on the way properties are distributed over objects (figure 1). Hierarchies of categories have the inheritance principle of taxonomies (if birds are animals, birds have animal properties in addition to their own properties). For instance in figure 1b, object O 1 is an object of category P3,P5 which has category P1,P2 as superordinate. If P1 is "on the table", P2 "red", P3 "large" and P5 "square", O 1 here would be a kind of "large square" that is a kind of "red thing on the table"; O 2 and O 3 being, for instance, small (P4) triangle (P6) and rectangle (P7). In Figure 1b (bottom), let us label X the category made of P1,P2 and Y the category made of P3,P5. Y is a kind of X. Due to the inheritance principle, category Y includes P1,P2, the properties of category X. This can be seen in the boolean table of figure 1b.

4 Figure 1: Binary descriptions and corresponding Galois Lattices. Figure 1a (top) and 1b (bottom) show that graph complexity does not depend on the number of objects (O) or of properties (P) but on the way properties are distributed over objects. As Rips & Conrad [1], footnote 3, reported, the property hypothesis of the reciprocal effect states: "If Y is a kind of Y, then Y must have all X's defining properties (as in traditional property models of classification, see [6, 8, 9]. As a consequence, Galois lattice formalism, as well as the property hypothesis, advocates that "mammals are parts of elephants, tools are parts of hammers" in the meronomic sense that "all of the descriptions of mammals are part of the description of elephants and all of the descriptions of tools are part of the description of hammers". This contrasts with the views of [1], p. 201, who wrote "More general classes of objects don't seem to be parts of more specific ones -mammals are not parts of elephants, tools are not parts of hammers, and so on.-" The meaning of the sentence mammals are part of elephants is not that all objects that are mammals are part-of all objects that are elephants. This is evidently false. It means : what one can say about mammals is part-of what one can say about elephants. This can be experimentally verified with participants. To do so, we advocate for a clear distinction between objects in the physical world and the categories used to represent them and to think, talk and communicate about them. Unlike objects, which have a physical counterpart, categories are cognitive. We propose that all the cognitive facts that are related to categories must be studied as cognitive, not physical, entities and require, as such, verbal responses from participants about symbols. Are elephants mammals? should be reformulated as such. Are elephants said to be mammals? which is a better proposal. This provides a way of distinguishing between what the object "elephant" is and what the category ELEPHANT is, since there is nothing in the world that would have only the properties of the category ELEPHANT (being asked to draw an elephant does not tell which real elephant you should draw ). The abstraction of properties from objects in order to form categories is a cognitive operation and has to be treated as such. The meronomic Galois lattice is just the reverse of the taxonomic Galois lattice. The corresponding part-of graph of the kind-of graph of figure 1-bottom is just the reverse, top-down, graph. So, Figure 1-bottom can also be read as a part-of graph. Suppose that O1 is ELEPHANT having nails (p3), defenses (p5), but also having mammals (p1) and four legs (p2) as other ANIMALS which have fur (p4), such as O2-MONKEY with a long tail (p6) and O3-CAT with whiskers (p7). In figure 1-bottom, mammals (p1) and four legs (p2) are common parts of ELEPHANT (O1), MONKEY (O2) and CAT (O3), while fur (p4) is part of MONKEY (O2) and CAT (O3). All of the properties of a category are a subset of all of the properties of a subordinate category: all the parts of the description of MAMMAL (such as having a head, a body, mammals, and so on) are parts of the description of ELEPHANT, the reverse not being true (eg. has tusks). Thus, the meronomic relation is the part-of relation between descriptions, including descriptions of physical parts, while the partonomic relation is the part-of relation between physical parts From our knowledge, the Galois Lattice formalism has never been used to simulate how a given situation can be analyzed through categorization.

5 The Galois Lattice formalism provides a similarity measure. Consider figure 2. The top graph is the Galois Lattice of 3 objects, a cat and a dog that are pets, the dog being brought to the park as the book to be read. (top graph). In this situation, and only in this context, Dog is a kindof (feeding, pet) as CAT, but also a kind-of (topark) as BOOK. Similarity can be computed as the number of valued arcs to join two objects: 1 in the kind-of relation, 2 in the opposite direction. Thus DOG-CAT similarity value would be of 1, while the CAT-DOC similarity value would be of 2 (asymmetry). The DOG- BOOK (or the BOOK-DOG) similarity value would be of 3, while the CAT-BOOK similarity value would be of 5 (no-transitivity). Thus, DOG would be evaluated as more similar than CAT than CAT is similar to DOG. The same phenomenon occurs in the Russia-Cuba-Jamaica case: CUBA is more similar to RUSSIA than RUSSIA to CUBA. Figure 2: The Galois Lattice of 3 objects with properties (f: feature), a cat and a dog that are pets, the dog being brought to the park as the book to be read (top graph), and the Galois lattice of Russia Cuba - Jamaica case. (bottom graph). The cognitive flexibility of creating contextual categories is possible only if any of the properties can be used for categorization. This is why the reciprocal effect in the merononic relation is important in the contextual categorization theory: if X is a part of Y, then Y is a kind of X. This means than any property that belongs to the description of a category can be used to form a superordinate category. For instance, to bring to the park which is one of the properties of Mary s dog (figure 2, top) can be used if necessary to form a category; for instance the category of the things Mary takes along to the Park. The cognitive flexibility underlying contextual categorization would be based on the meronomic reciprocal effect. The meronomic reciprocal effect appears to be a complex process when building a hierarchy of categories. Let s consider, to bring to the veterinarian as being a property of Mary s cat. Since the category things bring to the veterinarian is built, let s say by Mary s friend after Mary, - who is going to the park with her dog and book-, said I will also bring the cat to the veterinarian, a hierarchy of categories is to be built with to bring to the park a property of the Mary s dog and book and with to bring outside the house that is a property of Mary s cat, dog and book. Building the hierarchy of categories for a set of objects in a given context is to find the shared and unshared properties with lines of properties [12]: to bring to the park and to bring to the veterinarian are values of to bring outside the house, the reverse being not true. If people do make contextual hierarchies of categories, they should agree that if p i is a property of category C i, then C i is a kind-of category Cp i and if p jj is a property of category Cp i, then Cp i is a kind-of category Cp ij and so on. They also should disagree that p i is a part-of the description of Cp ij (we don t know). We report below experiments we run in order to check if the reciprocal effect in hierarchy of categories is a human ability. If so, results would indicate that people could make categories, and categories of categories, from features.

6 4 Testing the reciprocal effect of categorization For two categories X (e.g. elephant) and Y (e.g. Animal) testing an X-to-Y reciprocal effect is a three parts question: 1. Does X is kind-of Y imply Y is part-of X? 2. Does Y is part-of X imply X is kind-of Y? 3. Does Y is kind-of X imply X is part-of Y? 4. Does X is part-of Y imply Y is kind-of X? A kind-of-to-part-of oriented reciprocal effect can be observed (figure 3): - If X kind-of Y implies Y part-of X while X partof Y is not observed and when Y part-of X implies X kind-of Y while Y kind-of X is not observed, or - If Y kind-of X implies X part-of Y while Y partof X is not observed and when X part-of Y implies Y kind-of X while X kind-of Y is not observed. Note that there is usually a high degree of variability and instability in category representations, in tasks of both property generation and judgment of typicality (Barsalou, 1989). There was no significant difference between taxonomy and meronomy and between the two orders of presentation (X-to-Y vs. Y-to-X). In contrast, the predicted interaction, (taxonomy vs. meronomy) x (X-to-Y vs. Y-to-X), was found across all experiments (Table 1). Table 1: The reciprocal effect was found across the whole set of experiments both for categories (top table) and for parts (bottom table): no X-to- Y order effect, no difference between kind-of and part-of relations, but a strong order-relation interaction effect. (X-to-Y vs Y-to-X) parrot bird animal living being cacatoes 0.3 (p=.6) 0.1 (p=.81) (p=.74) parrot 0 2 (p=.16) 1 (p=.32) bird 0.1 (p=.79) 1.4 (p=.24) animal 0.1 (p=.77) oack tree plant natural thing cork oack 0.9 (p=.76) 0.1 (p=.75) 0.26 (p=.61) 0.1 (p=.79) oack 11.1 (p=.002) 0.7 (p=.4) 0.1 (p=.79) tree 1.5 (p=.22) 0 plant 0 (Taxonomy vs Meronomy) parrot bird animal living being cacatoes 1.4 (p=.25) 2.6 (p=.12) 1.4 (p=.25) 0.9 (p=.36) parrot (p=.74) 0.1 (p=.71) bird 0.6 (p=.43) 0 animal 0.12 (p=.73) oack tree plant natural thing cork oack 0.4 (p=.53) 0.1 (p=.73) 2.4 (p=.13) 2.9 (p=.1) oack 6.2 (p=.02) 0.1 (p=.77) 1 (p=.32) tree 0.4 (p=.56) 4.6 (p=.04) plant 0 (Taxonomy vs Meronomy) x (X-to-Y vs Y-to-X) parrot bird animal living being cacatoes 48.9 (p<.0001) 54.9 (p<.0001) 48.9 (p<.0001) 81.9 (p<.0001) parrot (p<.0001) 79.1 (p<.0001) (p<.0001) bird 37.1 (p<.0001) 57.8 (p<.0001) animal (p<.0001) oack tree plant natural thing cork oack 12.7 (p<.001) (p<.0001) 14.4 (p<.005) 57.8 (p<.0001) oack (p<.0001) 18.8 (p<.0001) 81.0 (p<.0001) tree 3.1 (p<.08) 99.3 (p<.0001) plant 57.8 (p<.0001) Figure 3: Expected data from the reciprocal effect of categorization: if Y is a kind of X (high YES score), then X is not a kind of Y (low YES score), X is a part of Y (high YES score) and Y is not a part of X (low YES score). Running four experiments, with forty participants for each experiment, we tested the reciprocal effect of categorization using animals and plants (experiment 1), parts of animals and parts of plants (experiment 2), verbs of displacement (experiment 3), and the set of verbs of mental activities used by [13, 14] and verbs provided by [4] (experiment 4). (X-to-Y vs Y-to-X) feather wing bird's body bird feather's point 0.8 (p=.37) 2.8 (p=.1) 1.3 (p=.27) 6.3 (p=.017) feather 1 (p=.34) 3 (p=.09) 1.4 (p=.25) wing 0.2 (p=.66) 0.2 (p=.63) bird's body 9.6 (p=.004) leaf branch tree forest leaf nerve 1.3 (p=.3) (p=.1) 3 (p=.1) leaf 0.8 (p=.4) 0.1 (p=.8) 2.7 (p=.1) branch 3.9 (p=.1) 7.3 (p=.01) tree 18.8 (p=.0001) (Partonomy vstaxonomy) feather wing bird's body bird feather's point 0.3 (p=.6) 0.6 (p=.44) 3.2 (p=.08) 2.5 (p=.12) feather 1.3 (p=.26) 4.8 (p=.04) 9.6 (p=.004) wing 7.5 (p=.009) 3.2 (p=.08) bird's body 5.3 (p=.03) leaf branch tree forest leaf nerve 0.3 (p=.6) 2.3 (p=.1) 4.9 (p=.03) 0.7 (p=.4) leaf 0.8 (p=.4) 6.8 (p=.01) 0.1 (p=.8) branch 0.5 (p=.47) 1.3 (p=.26) tree 0.1 (p=.8) (Partonomy vstaxonomy) x (X-to-Y vs Y-to-X) feather wing bird's body bird feather's point 4.2 (p<.05) 1.7 (p=.20) 7.3 (p<.01) 4.5 (p<.05) feather 32.7 (p<.0001) 10.7 (p<.003) 16.0 (p<.0003) wing 14.7 (p<.0005) 7.3 (p<.01) bird's body 3.4 (p<.07) leaf branch tree forest leaf nerve.98 (p=.33) 1.0 (p=.31).89 (p=.35) 23.8 (p<.0001) leaf 0.08 (p=.77) 18.8 (p<.0001) 20.0 (p<.0001) branch 21.8 (p<.0001) 39.9 (p<.0001) tree 35.0 (p<.0001)

7 Unlike Rips and Conrad, we argue that "MAMMAL is part of ELEPHANT", which is to say that all the description of mammals is a part of the description of elephant. Furthermore, any part of the description of a category can be used to form a superordinate category. This is a reciprocal effect we found with people in our experiments. Our view is to distinguish between words and the categories denoted by words, and between category extension and intension. For instance, [4] write "The entailment relation between nouns must be strictly distinguished from the meronymic relation, which is always independent of, and never included in, the hyponymic relation "a leaf is a part of tree" but it is not the case that "a leaf is a kind of tree." (p.569). First, a "noun" entails nothing per se, but what it refers to, may entail something. The noun "leaf" doesn't entail "is a part of tree" per se. It is a for a certain person, in a certain context that the noun "leaf" will evoke something that is a part of a tree (for instance, in a sentence like "the wind blows the leaves") or will not evoke something that is a part of a tree ("the wind blows the dead leaves"). Second, the "something" which is designated by the word "leaf" may not be an object but a category: "leaf" may refer to "any oak leaf" ("that leaf... is missing from my collection of leaves"). Like [2, 3] and others [7], we think that categories are constructed on line, as ad hoc categories, but also as contextual categories in which real objects can be instantiated in the context of a situation ("look at that leaf"), or of a representation ("the dead leaves of autumn. A. Rimbaud). Thus, if people are able to built hierarchies of categories from properties of objects of the current situation, doing so, they find the right dimensions along which objects can be compared, which means having a description that put the objects in relation. This is a way to solve the problem of dimensions identification for computing similarity. 5 The reciprocal effect of categorization: a key to similarity Similarity judgment is dependent on context [9, 17] and categories are very sensitive to context [15]. This is the case because properties used for similarity or for categorization will be dependent on context. For instance, in the two easily comprehensible sentences "to move the pieces of furniture" and "to polish furniture", furniture does not have the same set of properties and subsequently the same extension: ''made of wood'' would be restricting the furniture to be polished. Taking into account the context is to compute similarity and categorization within the context. For categorization, it is a new approach to consider that categories are built on line within the context, each object being categorized in the context of the other objects. This is different from Barsalou s ad hoc categories (objects are grouped because they participate to an agent s goal). In the contextual categorization theory, objects of the situation at hand are put in relation, grouped and differentiated in a hierarchy of categories. The goal intervenes to filter among properties and objects. The cognitive reciprocal effect means that people can build up categories from parts of the description of the object at hand. This process of extracting properties to build up categories of actual objects and a hierarchy of categories is what we think to be the basic cognitive mechanism of contextual categorization. Contextual categorization approach differs from the classical view that limits categories as being long-term memory entities that intervene to recognize and identify the objects at hand. In the contextual categorization view, categories in memory, dictionaries categories (cats are animals) as well as empirical categories (cats in the garden are Mary s cats) are mixed with contextual properties (to hear mewings) to build up a hierarchy of categories. Thus the contextual categorization theory is mainly a theory about understanding, thinking and reasoning (these are mewings of cats, these are mewings of Mary s cats, these are mewings of Mary s cats because they are hungry, ). What s about similarity? Computing similarity is often processed by starting considering all is known about the objects to be compared, then to decide if yes or no these objects can be put in a same category. More precisely, knowing the category of X, the problem is to decide if Y can be put in the category of X. If such decision-making can be done, then procedures that are applied to X could be adapted to Y.

8 Considering the context is to add the other objects of the current situation in the comparison process. Categorizing the objects using the properties they share and the properties that differentiate the objects limits the number of properties. A number that can be reduce by the goal at hand. Then, similarity merges from the hierarchy of categories. In addition, for a same person, a same object can belong to different dictionary or empirical categories: 0 can be a digit or a letter; - can be minus or a dash. This is the reason why we advocate for a clear distinction between objects in the physical world and the categories used to represent them. And this is why context is important: it helps to state what kind of things the object is while the reciprocal effect helps to take some parts of the description to build categories. Finally, we suggest also that context and contextual categorization are providers of properties. For instance, through the categorization process, two objects that are different from each other could be put together in a same category because they differ from all the other objects that share some properties. For instance, in the set of 9 objects made of 1 A, b, 9, 2, ), K, L,*, although ) and * are very dissimilar, they can be categorized as being different of the others alphanumerical objects. The context determines categorization and categorization determines similarity, not the reverse. References [1] M. Barbut & B. Monjardet (1970). Ordre et classification: algèbre et combinatoire. Paris: Hachette. [2] L.W. Barsalou (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory and Cognition, 11, [3] L.W. Barsalou (1989). Intraconcept similarity and its implications for interconcept similarity. In S. Vosniadou, & A. Ortony (Eds.) Similarity and Analogical Reasoning. New York: Cambridge University Press (pp ) [4] C. Fellbaum & G.A. Miller (1989). Folk Psychology or semantic entailment. Psychological Review, 97, [5] R.L. Goldstone (1994). The role of similarity in categorization: providing a groundwork. Cognition, 52, [6] R.L. Goldstone & J. Son. (2005). Similarity. In K. Holyoak & R. Morrison (Eds.). Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (pp ). [7] G. Lakoff (1986). Women, fire and dangerous things: What categories tell us about the nature of thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [8] D.L. Medin (1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist. 44, [9] D.L. Medin, R.L. Golstone & D. Gentner (1993). Respects for similarity. Psychological Review, 100, [10] R.M. Nosofsky (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identificationcategorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, [11] S. Poitrenaud (1995). The Procope Semantic Network: an alternative to action grammars. International. Journal of Human- Computer Studies, 42, [12] S. Poitrenaud, J.-F. Richard & C. Tijus. (2005). Properties, categories and categorization. Thinking and Reasoning, 11, [13] L.J. Rips & Collins, A. (1993). Categories and resemblance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, [14] L.J. Rips & F.G. Conrad (1989). Folk Psychology of Mental Activities. Psychological Review, 96, [15] C. Tijus (2001). Contextual Categorization and Cognitive Phenomena, in V. Akman, P. Bouquet, R. Thomason, & R. A. Young, Modeling and Using Context. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, pp [17] A. Tversky (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84,

Similarity and Categorization : Taxonomic and Meronomic parts of Similes

Similarity and Categorization : Taxonomic and Meronomic parts of Similes Similarity and Categorization : Taxonomic and Meronomic parts of Similes Charles Tijus, Sébastien Poitrenaud Denis Chene Laboratoire Cognition & Usages 2 rue de la Liberté 93523 Saint-Denis Cedex tijus@univ-paris8.fr

More information

Chapter 7. Mental Representation

Chapter 7. Mental Representation Chapter 7 Mental Representation Mental Representation Mental representation is a systematic correspondence between some element of a target domain and some element of a modeling (or representation) domain.

More information

Properties, categories, and categorisation

Properties, categories, and categorisation THINKING & REASONING, 2005, 11 (2), 151 208 Properties, categories, and categorisation Se bastien Poitrenaud, Jean-Franc ois Richard, and Charles Tijus Universite Paris VIII, France We re-evaluate existing

More information

How do Categories Work?

How do Categories Work? Presentations Logistics Think about what you want to do Thursday we ll informally sign up, see if we can reach consensus. Topics Linear representations of classes Non-linear representations of classes

More information

The Logic of Categorization

The Logic of Categorization From: FLAIRS-02 Proceedings. Copyright 2002, AAAI (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved. The Logic of Categorization Pei Wang Department of Computer and Information Sciences, Temple University Philadelphia,

More information

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use Amanda Hahn (achahn30@gmail.com) Takashi Yamauchi (tya@psyc.tamu.edu) Na-Yung Yu (nayungyu@gmail.com) Department of Psychology, Mail

More information

9.65 March 29, 2004 Concepts and Prototypes Handout

9.65 March 29, 2004 Concepts and Prototypes Handout 9.65 - Cognitive Processes - Spring 2004 MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences Course Instructor: Professor Mary C. Potter 9.65 March 29, 2004 Concepts and Prototypes Handout Outline: I. Categorization:

More information

Concepts and Categories

Concepts and Categories Concepts and Categories Functions of Concepts By dividing the world into classes of things to decrease the amount of information we need to learn, perceive, remember, and recognise: cognitive economy They

More information

Grounding Ontologies in the External World

Grounding Ontologies in the External World Grounding Ontologies in the External World Antonio CHELLA University of Palermo and ICAR-CNR, Palermo antonio.chella@unipa.it Abstract. The paper discusses a case study of grounding an ontology in the

More information

The interplay of domain-specific and domain general processes, skills and abilities in the development of science knowledge

The interplay of domain-specific and domain general processes, skills and abilities in the development of science knowledge The interplay of domain-specific and domain general processes, skills and abilities in the development of science knowledge Stella Vosniadou Strategic Professor in Education The Flinders University of

More information

Forum. and Categorization. 8. Three Distinctions about Concepts EDWARD E. SMITH. Metaphysical versus Epistemological Accounts of Concepts

Forum. and Categorization. 8. Three Distinctions about Concepts EDWARD E. SMITH. Metaphysical versus Epistemological Accounts of Concepts Mind b Language Vol. 4 Nos. 1 and 2 Spring/Summer 1989 [SSN 0268-1064 0 Basil Blackw~ll Forum 8. Three Distinctions about Concepts and Categorization EDWARD E. SMITH Some of the problems raised in the

More information

Concepts. Edouard Machery. History and Philosophy of Science University of Pittsburgh

Concepts. Edouard Machery. History and Philosophy of Science University of Pittsburgh Concepts Edouard Machery History and Philosophy of Science University of Pittsburgh Take-Home Message It is a mistake to attempt to develop general theories of concepts,! and to avoid ceding to the temptation,

More information

Categorization. University of Jena.

Categorization. University of Jena. Categorization Holger Diessel University of Jena holger.diessel@uni-jena.de http://www.holger-diessel.de/ Categorization Categories are the basic elements of human cognition; they are the glue of our mental

More information

Introduction to Categorization Theory

Introduction to Categorization Theory Introduction to Categorization Theory (Goldstein Ch 9: Knowledge) Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 05/15/2018: Lecture 08-2 Note: This Powerpoint presentation may contain

More information

Over the years, philosophers, psychoanalysts, psychologists, psychiatrists,

Over the years, philosophers, psychoanalysts, psychologists, psychiatrists, DEVELOPING A SELF: A GENERAL SEMANTICS WAY MILTON DAWES Over the years, philosophers, psychoanalysts, psychologists, psychiatrists, and others have asked: Is there such a thing as a self? Does the self

More information

Survey of Knowledge Base Content

Survey of Knowledge Base Content Survey of Content Introduction Fundamental Expression Types Top Level Collections Time and Dates Spatial Properties and Relations Event Types Information More Content Areas Copyright 2002 Cycorp This set

More information

STIN2103. Knowledge. engineering expert systems. Wan Hussain Wan Ishak. SOC 2079 Ext.: Url:

STIN2103. Knowledge. engineering expert systems. Wan Hussain Wan Ishak. SOC 2079 Ext.: Url: & Knowledge STIN2103 engineering expert systems Wan Hussain Wan Ishak SOC 2079 Ext.: 4786 Email: hussain@uum.edu.my Url: http://www.wanhussain.com Outline Knowledge Representation Types of knowledge Knowledge

More information

News English.com Ready-to-use ESL / EFL Lessons

News English.com Ready-to-use ESL / EFL Lessons www.breaking News English.com Ready-to-use ESL / EFL Lessons The Breaking News English.com Resource Book 1,000 Ideas & Activities For Language Teachers http://www.breakingnewsenglish.com/book.html Japanese

More information

Bill Wilson. Categorizing Cognition: Toward Conceptual Coherence in the Foundations of Psychology

Bill Wilson. Categorizing Cognition: Toward Conceptual Coherence in the Foundations of Psychology Categorizing Cognition: Toward Conceptual Coherence in the Foundations of Psychology Halford, G.S., Wilson, W.H., Andrews, G., & Phillips, S. (2014). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press http://mitpress.mit.edu/books/categorizing-cognition

More information

DRK-12 Carbon Assessment, Form A. Fall, 2012

DRK-12 Carbon Assessment, Form A. Fall, 2012 DRK-12 Carbon Assessment, Form A Fall, 2012 Please don t include this first sheet in student copies. This assessment is designed to elicit middle school or high school students accounts of carbon- transforming

More information

Warrant, Research, and the Practice of Gestalt Therapy

Warrant, Research, and the Practice of Gestalt Therapy Warrant, Research, and the Practice of Gestalt Therapy Philip Brownell, MDiv; PsyD An Address to the Research Methods Training Seminar European Association for Gestalt Therapy May 2, 2014 Rome, Italy Introduction

More information

Welcome back to Science Junior Science. Easy to read Version

Welcome back to Science Junior Science. Easy to read Version Welcome back to Science Junior Science Easy to read Version 1a What is Science? Science is both a collection of knowledge and the process for building that knowledge. Science asks questions about the natural

More information

Something to think about. What happens, however, when we have a sample with less than 30 items?

Something to think about. What happens, however, when we have a sample with less than 30 items? One-Sample t-test Remember In the last chapter, we learned to use a statistic from a large sample of data to test a hypothesis about a population parameter. In our case, using a z-test, we tested a hypothesis

More information

Comparison processes in category learning: From theory to behavior

Comparison processes in category learning: From theory to behavior BRAIN RESEARCH 15 (008) 10 118 available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Comparison processes in category learning: From theory to behavior Rubi Hammer a,b,, Aharon

More information

Curriculum Guide for Kindergarten SDP Science Teachers

Curriculum Guide for Kindergarten SDP Science Teachers Curriculum Guide for Kindergarten SDP Science Teachers Please note: Pennsylvania & Next Generation Science Standards as well as Instructional Resources are found on the SDP Curriculum Engine Prepared by:

More information

A Correlation of. to the. Common Core State Standards for Mathematics Grade 1

A Correlation of. to the. Common Core State Standards for Mathematics Grade 1 A Correlation of 2012 to the Introduction This document demonstrates how in Number, Data, and Space 2012 meets the indicators of the,. Correlation references are to the unit number and are cited at the

More information

Coherence Theory of Truth as Base for Knowledge Based Systems

Coherence Theory of Truth as Base for Knowledge Based Systems Association for Information Systems AIS Electronic Library (AISeL) AMCIS 1996 Proceedings Americas Conference on Information Systems (AMCIS) 8-16-1996 Coherence Theory of Truth as Base for Knowledge Based

More information

The Role of Action in Object Categorization

The Role of Action in Object Categorization From: FLAIRS-02 Proceedings. Copyright 2002, AAAI (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved. The Role of Action in Object Categorization Andrea Di Ferdinando* Anna M. Borghi^ Domenico Parisi* *Institute of Cognitive

More information

This module illustrates SEM via a contrast with multiple regression. The module on Mediation describes a study of post-fire vegetation recovery in

This module illustrates SEM via a contrast with multiple regression. The module on Mediation describes a study of post-fire vegetation recovery in This module illustrates SEM via a contrast with multiple regression. The module on Mediation describes a study of post-fire vegetation recovery in southern California woodlands. Here I borrow that study

More information

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept Matthew D. Zeigenfuse (mzeigenf@msu.edu) Department of Psychology, Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48823 USA

More information

Perception LECTURE FOUR MICHAELMAS Dr Maarten Steenhagen

Perception LECTURE FOUR MICHAELMAS Dr Maarten Steenhagen Perception LECTURE FOUR MICHAELMAS 2017 Dr Maarten Steenhagen ms2416@cam.ac.uk Last week Lecture 1: Naive Realism Lecture 2: The Argument from Hallucination Lecture 3: Representationalism Lecture 4: Disjunctivism

More information

Making Inferences from Experiments

Making Inferences from Experiments 11.6 Making Inferences from Experiments Essential Question How can you test a hypothesis about an experiment? Resampling Data Yield (kilograms) Control Group Treatment Group 1. 1.1 1.2 1. 1.5 1.4.9 1.2

More information

Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments

Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments Aniket Kittur (nkittur@ucla.edu) Keith J. Holyoak (holyoak@lifesci.ucla.edu) Department of Psychology, University

More information

Modeling and Environmental Science: In Conclusion

Modeling and Environmental Science: In Conclusion Modeling and Environmental Science: In Conclusion Environmental Science It sounds like a modern idea, but if you view it broadly, it s a very old idea: Our ancestors survival depended on their knowledge

More information

READTHEORY Passage and Questions

READTHEORY Passage and Questions READTHEORY Passage and Questions Reading Comprehension Assessment Directions: Read the passage. Then answer the questions below. Name Date Sports Drinks: Better Than Water? You are playing basketball with

More information

Mutability, Conceptual Transformation, and Context

Mutability, Conceptual Transformation, and Context Mutability, Conceptual Transformation, and Context Bradley C. Love Department of Psychology Northwestern University 2029 Sheridan Road Evanston, IL 60208 loveb@nwu.edu Abstract Features differ in their

More information

The scope of perceptual content, II: properties

The scope of perceptual content, II: properties The scope of perceptual content, II: properties Jeff Speaks November 16, 2009 1 What are the candidates?............................ 1 2 Arguments for inclusion............................. 2 2.1 From

More information

Selection at one locus with many alleles, fertility selection, and sexual selection

Selection at one locus with many alleles, fertility selection, and sexual selection Selection at one locus with many alleles, fertility selection, and sexual selection Introduction It s easy to extend the Hardy-Weinberg principle to multiple alleles at a single locus. In fact, we already

More information

Prototypes Vs Exemplars in Concept Representation

Prototypes Vs Exemplars in Concept Representation Prototypes Vs Exemplars in Concept Representation Marcello Frixione 1 and Antonio Lieto 2 1 DAFIST, University of Genova, Genoa, Italy 2 Computer Science Department, University of Turin, Turin, Italy marcello.frixione@gmail.com,

More information

Some New Evidence for Concept Stability

Some New Evidence for Concept Stability Some New Evidence for Concept Stability Sam Scott (sscott@carleton.ca) Department of Cognitive Science Carleton University, Ottawa, ON K1S 5B6 Abstract It is generally assumed that concepts are stored

More information

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use Michael Romano (mrr2@nyu.edu) Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place New York, NY 1000 USA Abstract Traditional

More information

Professor Greg Francis

Professor Greg Francis Professor Greg Francis 7/31/15 Concepts Representation of knowledge PSY 200 Greg Francis What is the information in Long Term Memory? We have knowledge about the world w May be several different types

More information

Indices, Meaning and Topic Maps: Some Observations

Indices, Meaning and Topic Maps: Some Observations Indices, Meaning and Topic Maps: Some Observations Thea Miller (University of Toronto, Canada) Hendrik Thomas (Technische Universität of Ilmenau, Germany) 1 Thea Miller doctoral candidate at the Faculty

More information

Categorizing: Delineating the Phenomena

Categorizing: Delineating the Phenomena Categorizing: Delineating the Phenomena http://philosophy.ucsd.edu/faculty/wuthrich/ 12 Scientific Reasoning Acknowledgements: Bill Bechtel Individuals and categories The basics Categorizing letters The

More information

Qualitative Data Analysis. Richard Boateng, PhD. Arguments with Qualitative Data. Office: UGBS RT18 (rooftop)

Qualitative Data Analysis. Richard Boateng, PhD. Arguments with Qualitative Data. Office: UGBS RT18 (rooftop) Qualitative Data Analysis Lecturer/Convenor: Richard Boateng, PhD. Email: richard@pearlrichards.org Office: UGBS RT18 (rooftop) Arguments with Qualitative Data Photo Illustrations from Getty Images www.gettyimages.com

More information

Introduction to Science Junior Science. Easy to read Version

Introduction to Science Junior Science. Easy to read Version Introduction to Science Junior Science Easy to read Version 1 1a What is Science? Science is both a collection of knowledge and the process for building that knowledge. Science asks questions about the

More information

Interaction of Taxonomic and Contextual Knowledge in Categorization of Cross-Classified Instances

Interaction of Taxonomic and Contextual Knowledge in Categorization of Cross-Classified Instances Interaction of Taxonomic and Contextual Knowledge in Categorization of Cross-Classified Instances Nadya Y. Vasilyeva (vasilyeva.n@neu.edu) Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, 125 NI Huntington

More information

Concepts & Categorization

Concepts & Categorization Geometric (Spatial) Approach Concepts & Categorization Many prototype and exemplar models assume that similarity is inversely related to distance in some representational space B C A distance A,B small

More information

SIMILARITY AND CATEGORIZATION

SIMILARITY AND CATEGORIZATION SIMILARITY AND CATEGORIZATION James A. Hampton Department of Psychology City University j.a.hampton@city.ac.uk Abstract The importance of similarity as a basis for categorization is reviewed, and it is

More information

CPS331 Lecture: Coping with Uncertainty; Discussion of Dreyfus Reading

CPS331 Lecture: Coping with Uncertainty; Discussion of Dreyfus Reading CPS331 Lecture: Coping with Uncertainty; Discussion of Dreyfus Reading Objectives: 1. To discuss ways of handling uncertainty (probability; Mycin CF) 2. To discuss Dreyfus views on expert systems Materials:

More information

Concepts and Categories

Concepts and Categories Concepts and Categories Informatics 1 CG: Lecture 11 Mirella Lapata School of Informatics University of Edinburgh mlap@inf.ed.ac.uk February 4, 2016 Informatics 1 CG: Lecture 11 Concepts and Categories

More information

UNIT 4 ALGEBRA II TEMPLATE CREATED BY REGION 1 ESA UNIT 4

UNIT 4 ALGEBRA II TEMPLATE CREATED BY REGION 1 ESA UNIT 4 UNIT 4 ALGEBRA II TEMPLATE CREATED BY REGION 1 ESA UNIT 4 Algebra II Unit 4 Overview: Inferences and Conclusions from Data In this unit, students see how the visual displays and summary statistics they

More information

Cube Critters Teacher s Guide

Cube Critters Teacher s Guide Cube Critters Teacher s Guide Relevant Life Science Content Standards from the National Science Education Standards 5-8: Diversity and Adaptations of Organisms Hereditary information is contained in genes,

More information

Inference Using Categories

Inference Using Categories Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2, Vol. 26, No. 3,776-795 Copyright 2 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 278-7393//S5. DOI:.37//278-7393.26.3.776 Inference

More information

CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA

CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA Data Analysis: Describing Data CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA In the analysis process, the researcher tries to evaluate the data collected both from written documents and from other sources such

More information

Graphic Organizers. Compare/Contrast. 1. Different. 2. Different. Alike

Graphic Organizers. Compare/Contrast. 1. Different. 2. Different. Alike 1 Compare/Contrast When you compare and contrast people, places, objects, or ideas, you are looking for how they are alike and how they are different. One way to organize your information is to use a Venn

More information

Biostatistics 3. Developed by Pfizer. March 2018

Biostatistics 3. Developed by Pfizer. March 2018 BROUGHT TO YOU BY Biostatistics 3 Developed by Pfizer March 2018 This learning module is intended for UK healthcare professionals only. Job bag: PP-GEP-GBR-0986 Date of preparation March 2018. Agenda I.

More information

BIOLOGY 1408 What is Biology?

BIOLOGY 1408 What is Biology? BIOLOGY 1408 Lecture 2 Chris Doumen, Ph.D. Collin College, 2014 What is Biology? The scientific study of life Contains two important elements Scientific study Life 1 The Process Of Science The word science

More information

A Difference that Makes a Difference: Welfare and the Equality of Consideration

A Difference that Makes a Difference: Welfare and the Equality of Consideration 84 A Difference that Makes a Difference: Welfare and the Equality of Consideration Abstract Elijah Weber Philosophy Department Bowling Green State University eliweber1980@gmail.com In Welfare, Happiness,

More information

Social and cultural influences on causal models of illness

Social and cultural influences on causal models of illness Social and cultural influences on causal models of illness Elizabeth B. Lynch (bethlynch@northwestern.edu) Department of Psychology, 2029 Sheridan Road Evanston, IL 60208 USA Abstract Causal models of

More information

This is really a study about coder error. I think in some ways it follows Jim s talk about inference and validity.

This is really a study about coder error. I think in some ways it follows Jim s talk about inference and validity. Classifying Open Occupation Descriptions in the Current Population Survey Fred Conrad This is really a study about coder error. I think in some ways it follows Jim s talk about inference and validity.

More information

MODULE 2 FOUNDATIONAL DEFINITIONS

MODULE 2 FOUNDATIONAL DEFINITIONS MODULE 2 FOUNDATIONAL DEFINITIONS Contents 2.1 Definitions............................................ 6 2.2 Performing an IVPPSS..................................... 8 2.3 Variable Types..........................................

More information

Alignment and Category Learning

Alignment and Category Learning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1998, Vol. 24., No. 1,144-160 Alignment and Category Learning Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/98/$3.00

More information

Wason's Cards: What is Wrong?

Wason's Cards: What is Wrong? Wason's Cards: What is Wrong? Pei Wang Computer and Information Sciences, Temple University This paper proposes a new interpretation

More information

COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND MEANING

COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND MEANING COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS AND MEANING Cognitive linguistics as a part of cognitive science 1987. official beginning of cognitive l. George Lakoff: Women, Fire And Dangerous Things (c. semantics) Rod Langacker:

More information

Artificial Intelligence For Homeopathic Remedy Selection

Artificial Intelligence For Homeopathic Remedy Selection Artificial Intelligence For Homeopathic Remedy Selection A. R. Pawar, amrut.pawar@yahoo.co.in, S. N. Kini, snkini@gmail.com, M. R. More mangeshmore88@gmail.com Department of Computer Science and Engineering,

More information

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions Hidehito Honda (hito@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Kimihiko Yamagishi (kimihiko@ky.hum.titech.ac.jp) Graduate School of Decision Science

More information

Introduction to Research Methods

Introduction to Research Methods Introduction to Research Methods 8-10% of the AP Exam Psychology is an empirical discipline. Psychologists develop knowledge by doing research. Research provides guidance for psychologists who develop

More information

Translate the problem to an equation. Do not solve. 1) Anita drives her car at 63 mph. How long did she drive to cover a distance of 246 miles?

Translate the problem to an equation. Do not solve. 1) Anita drives her car at 63 mph. How long did she drive to cover a distance of 246 miles? Chapter 1 practice This is an aid to help you review the chapter 1 material. Disclaimer: The actual test is different. SHORT ANSWER. Write the word or phrase that best completes each statement or answers

More information

ONTOLOGY. elearnig INITIATIVE PRAISE: Version number 1.0. Peer Review Network Applying Intelligence to Social Work Education

ONTOLOGY. elearnig INITIATIVE PRAISE: Version number 1.0. Peer Review Network Applying Intelligence to Social Work Education elearnig INITIATIVE PRAISE: Peer Review Network Applying Intelligence to Social Work Education Grant agreement number: 2003-4724 / 001-001 EDU - ELEARN ONTOLOGY Version number 1.0 30.10.2005 Executive

More information

S.No. Chapters Page No. 1. Plants Animals Air Activities on Air Water Our Body...

S.No. Chapters Page No. 1. Plants Animals Air Activities on Air Water Our Body... 1 Contents S.No. Chapters Page No. 1. Plants... 1 2. Animals... 7 3. Air... 14 4. Activities on Air... 16 5. Water... 18 6. Our Body... 21 7. Food & Nutrition... 25 8. Safety and First Aid... 28 9. Up

More information

Artificial Intelligence: Its Scope and Limits, by James Fetzer, Kluver Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Boston, London. Artificial Intelligence (AI)

Artificial Intelligence: Its Scope and Limits, by James Fetzer, Kluver Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Boston, London. Artificial Intelligence (AI) Artificial Intelligence: Its Scope and Limits, by James Fetzer, Kluver Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Boston, London. Artificial Intelligence (AI) is the study of how to make machines behave intelligently,

More information

Features of Similarity and Category-Based Induction

Features of Similarity and Category-Based Induction Proceedings of the Interdisciplinary Workshop on Categorization and Similarity, University of Edinburgh, 115-121. Features of Similarity and Category-Based Induction Evan Heit Department of Psychology

More information

Wriggle and Crawl Medium Term Plan

Wriggle and Crawl Medium Term Plan Wriggle and Crawl Medium Term Plan Subject/ Topic: Science living things, habitats, animals including humans Year Group: 1/2 Date: Summer II 2017 Key questions are differentiated - orange = basic, turquoise

More information

Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior

Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior Multiple Choice Questions 1. A theory is a(n): A. plausible or scientifically acceptable, well-substantiated explanation of some aspect of the

More information

BIAS WHY MUST FORENSIC PRACTICE BE. Itiel Dror. For more information, see:

BIAS WHY MUST FORENSIC PRACTICE BE. Itiel Dror. For more information, see: BIAS WHY MUST FORENSIC PRACTICE BE BASED ON COGNITIVE RESEARCH? Itiel Dror University College London (UCL) & Cognitive Consultants International (CCI) For more information, see: www.cci-hq.com Itiel@CCI-hq.com

More information

Social inclusion as recognition? My purpose here is not to advocate for recognition paradigm as a superior way of defining/defending SI.

Social inclusion as recognition? My purpose here is not to advocate for recognition paradigm as a superior way of defining/defending SI. Social inclusion as recognition? My purpose here is not to advocate for recognition paradigm as a superior way of defining/defending SI. My purpose is just reflective: given tradition of thought and conceptual

More information

Content Scope & Sequence

Content Scope & Sequence Content Scope & Sequence GRADE 2 scottforesman.com (800) 552-2259 Copyright Pearson Education, Inc. 0606443 1 Counting, Coins, and Combinations Counting, Coins, and Combinations (Addition, Subtraction,

More information

The Common Priors Assumption: A comment on Bargaining and the Nature of War

The Common Priors Assumption: A comment on Bargaining and the Nature of War The Common Priors Assumption: A comment on Bargaining and the Nature of War Mark Fey Kristopher W. Ramsay June 10, 2005 Abstract In a recent article in the JCR, Smith and Stam (2004) call into question

More information

Conduct an Experiment to Investigate a Situation

Conduct an Experiment to Investigate a Situation Level 3 AS91583 4 Credits Internal Conduct an Experiment to Investigate a Situation Written by J Wills MathsNZ jwills@mathsnz.com Achievement Achievement with Merit Achievement with Excellence Conduct

More information

Perception Lie Paradox: Mathematically Proved Uncertainty about Humans Perception Similarity

Perception Lie Paradox: Mathematically Proved Uncertainty about Humans Perception Similarity Perception Lie Paradox: Mathematically Proved Uncertainty about Humans Perception Similarity Ahmed M. Mahran Computer and Systems Engineering Department, Faculty of Engineering, Alexandria University,

More information

DRK-12 Carbon Assessment, Form A

DRK-12 Carbon Assessment, Form A DRK-12 Carbon Assessment, Form A Fall, 2013 Please don t include this first sheet in student copies. This assessment is designed to elicit middle school or high school students accounts of carbon-transforming

More information

HARRISON ASSESSMENTS DEBRIEF GUIDE 1. OVERVIEW OF HARRISON ASSESSMENT

HARRISON ASSESSMENTS DEBRIEF GUIDE 1. OVERVIEW OF HARRISON ASSESSMENT HARRISON ASSESSMENTS HARRISON ASSESSMENTS DEBRIEF GUIDE 1. OVERVIEW OF HARRISON ASSESSMENT Have you put aside an hour and do you have a hard copy of your report? Get a quick take on their initial reactions

More information

Lesson 11 Correlations

Lesson 11 Correlations Lesson 11 Correlations Lesson Objectives All students will define key terms and explain the difference between correlations and experiments. All students should be able to analyse scattergrams using knowledge

More information

The Inferential Model of Concepts Joshua Cowley

The Inferential Model of Concepts Joshua Cowley The Inferential Model of Concepts Joshua Cowley It is often claimed that concepts are the building blocks of thoughts. If this claim is true, as I think it is, any adequate theory of cognition will require

More information

Communication. Jess Walsh

Communication. Jess Walsh Communication Jess Walsh Introduction. Douglas Bank is a home for young adults with severe learning disabilities. Good communication is important for the service users because it s easy to understand the

More information

The Trajectory of Psychology within Cognitive Science. Dedre Gentner Northwestern University

The Trajectory of Psychology within Cognitive Science. Dedre Gentner Northwestern University The Trajectory of Psychology within Cognitive Science Dedre Gentner Northwestern University 1. How has Psychology fared within Cognitive Science? 2. How have areas within Psychology risen and fallen? 3.What

More information

Relational Versus Attributional Mode of Problem Solving?

Relational Versus Attributional Mode of Problem Solving? Relational Versus Attributional Mode of Problem Solving? Svetoslav Bliznashki (valsotevs@gmail.com) New Bulgarian University, 2 Montevideo Street Sofia 1618, Bulgaria Boicho Kokinov (bkokinov@nbu.bg) New

More information

Chapter 7: Descriptive Statistics

Chapter 7: Descriptive Statistics Chapter Overview Chapter 7 provides an introduction to basic strategies for describing groups statistically. Statistical concepts around normal distributions are discussed. The statistical procedures of

More information

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children Leslee J. Martin (martin.1103@osu.edu) Department of Psychology & Center for Cognitive Science Ohio State University 216 Lazenby

More information

Research Questions, Variables, and Hypotheses: Part 2. Review. Hypotheses RCS /7/04. What are research questions? What are variables?

Research Questions, Variables, and Hypotheses: Part 2. Review. Hypotheses RCS /7/04. What are research questions? What are variables? Research Questions, Variables, and Hypotheses: Part 2 RCS 6740 6/7/04 1 Review What are research questions? What are variables? Definition Function Measurement Scale 2 Hypotheses OK, now that we know how

More information

The Mind-Body Problem: Physicalism

The Mind-Body Problem: Physicalism The Mind-Body Problem: Physicalism Physicalism According to physicalism, everything that makes you up is physical, material, spatial, and subject to laws of physics. Because of that, we need not worry

More information

Making Sense of Measures of Center

Making Sense of Measures of Center Making Sense of Measures of Center Statistics are numbers that are part of your everyday world. They are used in reporting on baseball, basketball, football, soccer, the Olympics, and other sports. Statistics

More information

Structure mapping in spatial reasoning

Structure mapping in spatial reasoning Cognitive Development 17 (2002) 1157 1183 Structure mapping in spatial reasoning Merideth Gattis Max Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich, Germany Received 1 June 2001; received in revised

More information

Automaticity of Number Perception

Automaticity of Number Perception Automaticity of Number Perception Jessica M. Choplin (jessica.choplin@vanderbilt.edu) Gordon D. Logan (gordon.logan@vanderbilt.edu) Vanderbilt University Psychology Department 111 21 st Avenue South Nashville,

More information

An experimental investigation of consistency of explanation and graph representation

An experimental investigation of consistency of explanation and graph representation An experimental investigation of consistency of explanation and graph representation Nana Kanzaki (kanzaki@cog.human.nagoya-u.ac.jp) Graduate School of Information Science, Nagoya University, Japan Kazuhisa

More information

How do the SLA Learners Understand the Deictic Verb, Come? A case study from the perspective of cognitive semantics.

How do the SLA Learners Understand the Deictic Verb, Come? A case study from the perspective of cognitive semantics. How do the SA earners Understand the Deictic Verb, Come? A case study from the perspective of cognitive semantics. Norifumi Ueda Waseda University 1. Purpose The purpose of this study is to examine : 1.how

More information

Semantic Memory. Learning & Memory

Semantic Memory. Learning & Memory Semantic Memory Learning & Memory Semantic Memory General knowledge about the world, not linked to any time or context Some examples: What is the capital of North Dakota? Bismarck What is the population

More information

Phenomenal content. PHIL April 15, 2012

Phenomenal content. PHIL April 15, 2012 Phenomenal content PHIL 93507 April 15, 2012 1. The phenomenal content thesis... 1 2. The problem of phenomenally silent contents... 2 3. Phenomenally sneaky contents... 3 3.1. Phenomenal content and phenomenal

More information