INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT"

Transcription

1 INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts By Erin Marie Fleming August 2010

2 Thesis written by Erin Marie Fleming B.A., Kenyon College, 2007 M.A., Kent State University, 2010 Approved by David C. Riccio, Ph.D., Advisor Maria S Zaragoza, Ph. D., Chair, Department of Psychology Timothy Moerland, Ph.D., Dean, College of Arts and Sciences ii

3 TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS v INTRODUCTION...1 EXPERIMENT 1 9 METHOD..10 Subjects.10 Materials 10 Procedure...11 RESULTS.. 15 DISCUSSION 17 EXPERIMENT METHOD..21 Subjects. 21 Materials 21 Procedure...22 RESULTS..26 DISCUSSION 32 GENERAL DISCUSSION 34 REFERENCES..43 iii

4 LIST OF FIGURES TABLE 1 TABLE 2 FIGURE 1 FIGURE 2 FIGURE 3 Study design of experiment Study design of experiment Mean total time on white (in seconds) on day of testing for each group in experiment 1 16 Mean total time on white (in seconds) on day of testing for single long exposure groups in experiment 2 30 Mean total time on white (in seconds) on day of testing for each group in experiment 2, including collapsed single long exposure groups.31 iv

5 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank all of those people who made the completion of this project possible. Thank you goes first to my thesis advisor, Dr. David Riccio, for all of his guidance throughout the research for this project as well as outside of this project. Without his patience and input I would not be where I am to him and I am eternally grateful to him. I would like to thank my fellow graduate students, Christie Bartholomew, Amber Chenoweth, and Patrick Cullen for their support and guidance throughout this project. I would also like to thank my committee members: Dr. Stephen Fountain, Dr. Beth G. Wildman, and Dr. John Dunlosky for their valuable comments and review of my thesis. I would like to especially thank my family for all of their love and support throughout this process. Without them I would not have been able to make it through the long hours and hard work and I appreciate them more than they will ever know. v

6 Introduction Learning, and memory for learned information, involves much more than just the specific material that is being learned. Memory, in general, includes a number of attributes that are tied to a representation of some past information that can serve as cues to better aid the retrieval of that memory (Spear, 1978; Underwood, 1969). These attributes would include not only information specific to the stimulus and response that bring about a given outcome, but also the contextual (background) cues that were present at the time of learning. More specifically, stimulus attributes are features or characteristics of the training stimuli or peripheral stimuli in the environment. The peripheral stimuli have also been called contextual cues and are stimuli other than the training stimulus that are present at the time of training (Spear & Riccio, 1994). These contextual cues include the room or environment, lighting, visual cues, odors, sounds, and even internal states and emotions. Studies investigating learning and memory using animal models have focused on the specific response and what the cues associated with that response predict, rather than focusing on all of the stimuli or context present during the learning episode. For example, earlier studies looked at Pavlovian conditioning pairing some unconditioned stimulus with a conditioned response. In doing so, they focus only on that conditioned response and the unconditioned stimulus that predicts that response and not the other cues that are present during the learning episode, such as the background environment and other cues 1

7 2 from the environment other than the unconditioned stimulus. One way in which researchers have investigated the influence of these other stimuli has been by looking at what is known as stimulus generalization within the realm of associative learning. Stimulus generalization involves the control of other stimuli over responding and focuses on those cues that are not specific predictive cues, such as the unconditioned stimulus. The importance of these stimuli, and stimulus generalization in particular, has been noted by R. Shepard (1987) as being as fundamental to the learning process as the principle of gravity is to physics. Because the stimuli and contexts at the time of retrieval are rarely ever exactly the same as they were at the time of training, stimulus generalization provides the basis for the transfer of learning to new situations. One aspect of stimulus attributes and their involvement in learning is that their effects change over time. It has been found that more stimuli come to control a response over time rather than just one specific cue. The weakening of control by a specific stimulus that originally predicted a response is reflected in a flattening of the generalization gradient and occurs not only for conditional or discriminative stimuli but also contextual cues (Zhou and Riccio, 1996). Perkins and Weyant (1958) interpret this flattening of the generalization gradient as the forgetting of stimulus attributes or features. The strength of the learned response is basically unchanged over time but comes to be expressed to a wider range of cues that were not associated with the original learning episode. What is most interesting about the finding that the generalization gradient flattens over time is that there need not be any discrimination training involved for the context

8 3 shift to occur. Specifically, after learning information in one context it has been found that performance is disrupted if testing occurs in a context that is different from that in which they had the original learning episode (Rosas and Bouton, 1997; Smith, 1979; Borovsky and Rovee-Collier, 1990). This context shift effect, which did not include discriminative training, then may diminish over time resulting in the subjects performing equally well in both contexts (McAllister and McAllister, 1963; Rosas and Bouten, 1997; Zhou and Riccio, 1996). The study done by Zhou and Riccio (1996) showed just how important stimulus attributes are in learning and memory retrieval. Zhou and Riccio noted that it is difficult to quantitatively compare aspects of the context and stimulus attributes, such as trying to compare various sounds to various odors. However, a qualitative assessment is possible in that if a change in one aspect of the context does disrupt performance, then it is reasonable to infer that the stimulus was part of the information attended to and encoded by the subject. They wanted to utilize this qualitative assessment by altering both the proximal cues, which are cues associated with the conditioning apparatus, as well as distal cues, which are cues related to the more general background environment. By doing this, they could examine whether both distal and proximal stimuli exert control over performance and investigate the effects of changing more than one element of a context. They also wanted to determine whether the loss of the context shift effect over time would be differential (i.e. are distal attributes forgotten more rapidly than proximal?). Zhou and Riccio s experiment (1996) consisted of two different rooms and two

9 4 different passive avoidance boxes. All rats were trained in Room A in Box A but where the rats were tested varied. They were either tested in the same room and box, different room, different box, or both a different room and different box. The rats were also tested in different time increments, with half being tested 24 hours after training and the other half being tested 2 weeks after training. Using these manipulations they found that at a short delay of 1 day altering either the distal or proximal cues resulted in a severe performance decrement. However, after a 2 week delay it didn t matter which type of cue was altered, distal or proximal, there was no performance decrement. As long as only either the distal or proximal cues were altered, and not both, the generalization gradient flattened at a long delay and all contexts were treated as the original training context. This study illustrates how important the effect of stimulus attributes is on learning at longer intervals and indicates that there are ways that the parameters can be changed to alter typical learning behavior. Without any specific treatments between original learning episode and testing but by merely altering the length of time between train and test and what type of cues are present during testing can reduce the usual performance decrement we would see with the context shift effect. Another very important example of the implications of forgetting stimulus attributes comes from studies investigating the reinstatement of fear. If the UCS is presented outside of the training situation after the conditioned fear response has been extinguished the original fear memory will return (Rescorla and Heth, 1975). Subsequent research indicates that the reinstatement effect may depend on associative processes of generalization between the training and reinstating conditions similar to what is seen with

10 5 the generalization gradient. MacArdy and Riccio (1995) showed that a reinstatement treatment in a context different from the original training context was ineffective when administered at a short interval (either immediately or 1 day after extinction) but became effective at a longer interval (7 days after extinction). These findings are consistent with both the generalization account and the principle of forgetting of attributes. A more recent study (McAllister and McAllister, 2006) compared non-contingent and contingent UCS exposure in several contextual conditions either 1 or 9 days after extinction and confirmed the previous findings. More importantly, the authors noted that the forgetting of detailed characteristics of contexts would make the representations of the original conditioning situations less distinguishable from the post extinction shock situation (p.48). This brings us to the purpose of the present study which is to investigate possible determinants of forgetting of stimulus attributes. The finding that a flatter generalization gradient indicates that conditioned responses occur to a wider array of stimuli may be interpreted as meaning that some of the specific stimulus attributes of the learning episode may have been forgotten. This phenomenon of forgetting of stimulus attributes, in which perceptually distinguishable stimuli become more functionally interchangeable over a retention interval and result in an increase in total responding, contrasts with the performance deficits typically referred to as forgetting (Riccio, Rabinowitz, and Axelrod, 1994). While most research has been done focusing on decrements in performance associated with forgetting of specific learned responses, the present study is concerned with the effects of forgetting of stimulus attributes. The research herein should

11 6 give valuable information on some aspects of the forgetting of stimulus attributes that can lead to a theoretical understanding of the phenomenon. One important memory phenomenon that may play a role in the forgetting of stimulus attributes is retroactive interference. Retroactive interference occurs when new information is presented after the activation of a memory and conflicts with that memory resulting in forgetting (Spear & Riccio, 1994). In a general retroactive interference design, the interfering information is encountered during the retention interval of the original learned information (Hulse, et al., 1975). The difference in the amount of retention of the first task reflects the influence of the learning of the second task. Experimentally produced forgetting occurs when the control group, which receives no other information after the first learning episode, retains more than the experimental group, which receives other information during the retention interval. The first theories of retroactive interference focused on competition between original and interfering information (McGeoch, 1932) as well as intrusion of the original information during the learning of the information encountered during the retention interval which results in an unlearning of the original information (Melton & Irwin, 1941). More modern views of retroactive interference focus on a combination of associative memory and cognition to gain a more detailed understanding of interference. This new way of investigating interference resulted in retroactive interference being more applicable to many more aspects of memory than originally thought. These applications ranged from its significant role in everyday memory through misinformation effects (Loftus & Palmer, 1974) to changes in the effects of retroactive interference from a

12 7 developmental stand-point (Harris, 1975). It is apparent that retroactive interference has been studied extensively within general memory mechanisms as a reason for why performance declines or why forgetting occurs. However, there is little, if any, investigation into the effects of retroactive interference on the forgetting of stimulus attributes. While it is not known at this point whether stimulus memory is susceptible to retroactive interference, retroactive interference for context has been indirectly suggested in a study of contextual reminders in college students by Smith (1979). In this study, Smith found that reminders of the original context in which the information was learned resulted in an alleviation of the usual performance impairment seen with the context shift effect. However, Smith also discovered that the effectiveness of the reminder was reduced if the students were exposed to several different contexts in between training and testing. Thus, it appears as though the additional contextual stimuli experienced in between training and testing interfered with the memory for the original context which made reinstatement less effective. When studying retroactive interference of stimulus attributes the use of the usual index of performance loss does not apply. With the majority of studies on retroactive interference, retroactive interference results in forgetting of the originally learned information as indicated by a decrease in performance. To investigate retroactive interference of stimulus attributes, on the other hand, improved performance will indicate that retroactive interference of stimulus attributes has occurred. Retroactive interference from stimulus attributes would interfere with the memory for stimulus attributes from the

13 8 original learning episode and the learned information will be retrieved just as well in any novel context. At the basis of this is the idea that stimulus attributes will become more functionally interchangeable and performance will be similar in all contexts.

14 Experiment 1 Previous research has shown that many different aspects of learning can interfere with previously acquired learning and result in a deficit in responding. Experiment 1 investigated the effects of context, and stimulus attributes in particular, on the memory for a single learning episode. This experiment was based on the approach used by Smith (1979) but where Smith assessed interference indirectly in terms of impaired reinstatement of the target memory, the current experiment evaluated the flattening of the contextual gradient as an index of forgetting of stimulus attributes and as caused by retroactive interference. The flattening of the contextual gradient was evaluated by changes in the context shift effect. Experiment 1 tested the hypothesis that exposure to novel stimuli during the retention interval will serve as sources of retroactive interference which will result in a more rapid loss of the context shift effect when compared with retention controls. 9

15 METHOD Subjects Subjects were 40 female Long-Evans rats approximately 180 days old at start of the experiment. Animals were singly housed in the animal colony at Kent State University in a rectangular plastic transparent plastic box (45.7 (L) x 25.4 (W) x 20.3 (H) cm). Animals were on a 15/9 light/dark cycle and were provided ad lib food and water throughout the experiment. All handling and experiments took place during the light portion at the same time each day. Materials Five different rooms and passive avoidance chambers were used in the experiment. Context A was a 1.83 x 2.74 m room painted white. Posters were placed on each wall to provide visual cues. Long-Evans rat species are known to have adequate vision perception so they can distinguish between varying visual contextual stimuli. The room was illuminated by a standard 40w bulb. Context A had no odor present, but had white noise for background noise. Context B was a 1.83 x 2.74 m room painted white. Posters were also placed on each wall to provide different visual cues. To provide extra differing visual stimuli, context B was illuminated with a 25w red light and no odor present. Context C was a 1.83 x 2.74 m room painted white with posters placed on each 10

16 11 wall and normal lighting. Context C also had the odor of lemon present which was distributed in the air by a cotton ball soaked in 50mL of lemon cooking extract and placed on a paper plate below the passive avoidance apparatus. Context D was a 1.83 x 2.74 m room painted white with posters placed on each wall and normal lighting. Context D also had the odor of vanilla present which was distributed in the air by a cotton ball soaked in 50mL of vanilla cooking extract and placed on a paper plate below the passive avoidance apparatus. Context E was a 1.83 x 2.74 m room painted white with posters placed on each wall and normal lighting. Context E also had the odor of peppermint present which was distributed in the air by a cotton ball soaked in 50mL of peppermint cooking extract. Training, exposure, and testing were conducted in a (L) x (W) x (H) cm black-white Plexiglas shuttle box with a grid floor (2mm grids spaced at 1mm apart center to center). The two compartments were equal in size and divided by a guillotine door that was manually lifted and lowered by a pulley system controlled by the experimenter. The white compartment consisted of a transparent Plexiglas lid with white Plexiglas walls. The black compartment consisted of a transparent Plexiglas lid with black Plexiglas walls. Footshock was delivered through the grid floor via a constant current AC shock generator (Model 5806 Lafayette Instruments Co., Lafayette, IN). Procedure The plan of this experiment was to condition fear in rats either with or without exposure to multiple novel contexts during the retention interval between training and testing. The Ss were tested for their avoidance of the shock compartment with no shock

17 12 present 48 hours after training in either the original training context or a novel context. All subjects were handled for 3 days prior to training. Handling consisted of physical contact between experimenter and subject for 5 minutes at the same time each day 3 days in a row. There were a total of ten rats in each group in the experiment. Rats were randomly assigned to the four groups. Group Same and Shift were the control groups that received no contextual exposure in between training and testing. The Multiple Context Exposure (MCE) Same and MCE Shift groups were the experimental groups that received exposure to novel contexts between training and testing (See Table 1). Given the usual degree of variability among rats and past experience with similar studies ten rats was an acceptable amount in each group and allowed adequate power to reject the null hypothesis if there was an experimental effect. This experiment used Pavlovian differential fear conditioning in a passive avoidance chamber. This type of conditioning involves pairing a neutral cue with a foot shock and results in learning of fear of the originally neutral cue. In this study, the neutral cue was the black compartment of the passive avoidance apparatus. During training, the subject was placed in the white compartment of the apparatus. After 10 seconds, the guillotine door was opened, and the subject was free to cross to the black compartment of the apparatus. Once the subject crossed to the black compartment the guillotine door was closed and the subject received 2 brief 1 second inescapable foot shocks of 0.5 ma intensity 5 seconds apart. Cross latency scores were calculated which involved recording how long it took the rats to cross from the white to the black side once the guillotine door was opened. After training was complete, the rats were then returned to their home cages.

18 13 24 hours after training, the subjects in the experimental group experienced multiple context exposure and the subjects in the control groups received handling. The multiple context exposure consisted of the mere exposure to 3 different contexts for 10 minutes in each context. The order of the contextual exposures was randomized and the contexts used were Contexts C, D, and E. During exposure, the subjects were placed on the white side of a passive avoidance chamber with the guillotine door closed. The guillotine door remained closed during the duration of the exposure session so there was no exposure to the black compartment in an attempt to avoid extinction. The rats remained in the context for 10 minutes and were then transported immediately to the next context where the exposure process was repeated. Subjects in the control groups received handling 24 hours after training. Handling consisted of physical contact between experimenter and rat in the colony for 30 minutes (the total amount of time the experimental groups received multiple exposures). Testing occurred 24 hours after the multiple context exposure session and took place in either the training context (groups Same and MCE Same) or a novel context (groups Shift, MCE Shift). Testing involved behavioral observations over a period of 10 minutes during which no foot shock was administered. The behavioral observations included latency scores, or how long it took the subjects to cross from the white compartment to the black compartment, as well as total time on white scores which was simply the total amount of time the rat spent in the white compartment of the apparatus. Maintained memory of stimulus attributes was reflected in short latency scores and/or lower total time on white scores.

19 14 Group 1: Same Group 2: Shift Group 3: Multiple Context Exposure Same Group 4: Multiple Context Exposure Shift Day One Day Two Day Three Train in Context Handled for 30 Test in Context A A minutes Train in Context Handled for 30 Test in Context B A minutes Train in Context 10 minute exposure to Test in Context A A Context C, 10 minute exposure to Context D, 10 minute exposure to Train in Context A Context E 10 minute exposure to Context C, 10 minute exposure to Context D, 10 minute exposure to Context E Test in Context B Table 1. Study design for experiment 1. The six groups are listed in the first column and the days of the experiment are listed in the second, third, and fourth columns. This table indicates what procedure each group underwent each day.

20 15 RESULTS Training latencies were evaluated in a one-way ANOVA to determine whether all animals exhibited comparable cross latencies, which indicates rats natural preference of the black compartment at training. The ANOVA showed there were no significant differences among the training latencies for the 4 groups (F(3,36)= 1.381, p=.26). Testing cross latencies were evaluated in a one-way ANOVA and used to determine retention of original fear memory where shorter cross latencies would indicate strong fear and, thus, retention of the information from the training episode. The ANOVA showed that there was a statistically significant difference among the testing cross latencies for the 4 groups (F(3,36)=2.901, p<.05)(same (M=332.20, SD=285.94), Shift (M=216.40, SD=259.86), MCE A (M=83.00, SD=92.91, MCE B (M=102.60, SD=158.64)). A post-hoc Tukey test was run to investigate where the differences were between the groups but it yielded no statistically significant results at p<.05. Mean total time on white during testing was evaluated using a one-way ANOVA to see whether the memory for stimulus attributes was affected by the multiple context exposure treatment. The ANOVA showed that there was a marginally statistically significant difference among the total time on white scores for the 4 groups (F(3,36)=2.768, p=.056). A post-hoc Tukey test for group showed that there was a difference between the total time on white scores for the group that was trained and tested in the same context with no experimental manipulation on the second day (Same) and the group that was trained in one context and tested in a different context after being exposed

21 16 to multiple contexts during the retention interval (Multiple Context Exposure Shift) with p<.05. Group Multiple Context Exposure Shift showed little fear in the novel context, with total time on white scores below half of the total time of testing, while Same group exhibited a large amount fear with total time on white scores showing that they spent very near to the entire ten minutes in the white compartment. The mean total time on white scores for each group are shown in figure 1. Figure 1. Mean total time on white (TTW) scores (±SEM) in experiment 1 for the control groups, Same and Shift, and the experimental groups, Multiple Context Exposure Same (MCE Same) and Multiple Context Exposure Shift (MCE Shift), at retention test. The Same group that was trained in one context and tested in the same context with no experimental manipulation during the retention interval had significantly different TTW scores when compared with the Multiple Context Exposure Shift group that was trained in one context and tested in a context different form the training context after receiving exposure to multiple contexts during the retention interval.

22 DISCUSSION The results from experiment 1 did not support the hypothesis that multiple context exposures serve as sources of retroactive interference and result in a more rapid loss of the context shift effect. Rats that received multiple context exposures during the retention interval and were tested in a context that was different from the training context showed significantly less fear to the black compartment when compared with rats trained and tested in the same context with no experimental manipulation during the retention interval. The results also showed that there was no statistically significant difference between the group that was trained and tested in the same context with no experimental manipulation during the retention and the group that was tested in a context that was different from the training context with no experimental manipulation during the retention interval. These results go against the usual findings of the context shift effect that has been shown in many previous studies. While the overall cross latency scores at test were statistically significantly different, the post-hoc analyses were not statistically significant thus giving no information as to where the differences were. The discussion of the results will be based on the total time on white scores at testing. Many researchers that use passive avoidance training in their research tend to rely more heavily on the total time on white scores than the cross latency scores at test. In a recent article by Santucci & Cardiello (2004) they posit that animals may have short cross latencies with long total time on white scores, indicating fear and a strong memory for initial learning, because they were not reminded 17

23 18 of the aversive event until they enter the black compartment. Thus, the rats may remember their initial training, as indexed by longer total time on white scores, but they may not activate that memory until they cross to the compartment in which the aversive event occurred. Because of this, the total time one white scores will be discussed herein as they are a more accurate indication of fear than cross latencies at test. Based on the hypothesis that multiple context exposures should produce retroactive interference resulting in a more rapid loss of the context shift effect, there should not have been a significant difference in total time on white scores between the Same group and the Multiple Context Exposure Shift group. By having a statistically significant difference for total time on white scores between these groups there was not the amount of generalization to stimulus attributes that the hypothesis predicted. The first explanation for why this may have occurred has to do with extinction. The way in which the multiple context exposure groups received their multiple exposures may have resulted in extinction of their original learning. Because they are placed in the white compartment of a passive avoidance apparatus in each context in between training and testing, they may have extinguished the memory that they should fear the black side of the apparatus. However, this seems unlikely not only because they were never exposed to the black compartment but also because if this were the case there would have been no significant difference between the two Multiple Context Exposure groups because they received the same exposure procedure. A second possible reason why there was a significant difference between Same and Multiple Context Exposure Shift is that being placed in the white compartment of the

24 19 passive avoidance apparatus, even in novel contexts, may have acted as a reminder treatment of the original training, similar to what was found in Rescorla and Heth s study (1975). This explanation makes more sense than the extinction reasoning because it can explain why there is no difference between the multiple context exposure group that was tested in the same context that they were trained in (Multiple Context Exposure Same) and group Same but there is a difference between group Same and the Multiple Context Exposure Shift group. If the animals were receiving a reminder treatment with the current exposure procedure, then the memory for the original learning would be strengthened and they would not be affected by the multiple contexts. Because of this, the Multiple Context Exposure group tested in the same context they were trained (MCE Same) in would show a strong fear memory and the Multiple Context Exposure group tested in a different context from the training context (MCE Shift) would show a strong memory for stimulus attributes and, thus, distinguish between the training context and the novel testing context and act with no fear. This is exactly what happened, based on the results of this study, and is a major confound. A major issue with the current results is the absence of the usual context shift effect. Based on previous studies a statistically significant difference was expected between the Same group and the Shift group that would show the context shift effect that has been found in many studies carried out before this one. The reason for the lack of the context shift is unclear.

25 Experiment 2 In order to test the original hypothesis from experiment 1 without confounds that may have arisen due to the exposure procedure that procedure needed to be altered. Experiment 2 was run to test the hypothesis that exposure to novel stimuli during the retention interval will serve as a source of retroactive interference, resulting in a more rapid loss of the context shift effect when compared with retention controls. To test this hypothesis, major changes were made in the way that the exposure procedure was done to avoid any possible extinction or reinstatement effects. Extra control groups were also added that would show whether any effect is due to the multiple contexts or just a single novel contextual exposure over the same amount of time. 20

26 METHOD Subjects Subjects were 59 female Long-Evans rats approximately 90 days old at start of the experiment. Animals were singly housed in the animal colony at Kent State University in a rectangular plastic transparent plastic box (45.7 (L) x 25.4 (W) x 20.3 (H) cm). Animals were on a 15/9 light/dark cycle and were provided ad lib food and water throughout the experiment. All handling and experiments took place during the light portion at the same time each day. Materials Five different rooms and passive avoidance chambers were used in the experiment. Context A was a 1.83 x 2.74 m room painted white. Posters were placed on each wall to provide visual cues. Long-Evans rat species are known to have adequate vision perception so they can distinguish between varying visual contextual stimuli. The room was illuminated by a standard 40w bulb. Context A had no odor present, but had white noise for background noise. Context B was a 1.83 x 2.74 m room painted white. Posters were also placed on each wall to provide different visual cues. To provide extra differing visual stimuli, context B was illuminated with a 25w red light and no odor present. Context C was a 1.83 x 2.74 m room painted white with posters placed on each wall and normal lighting. Context C also had the odor of lemon present which was distributed in the air by a cotton ball soaked in 50mL of lemon cooking extract and 21

27 22 placed in a plastic dish in the room. The odor was distributed throughout the room for 10 minutes prior to exposure as well as during exposure. Context D was a 1.83 x 2.74 m room painted white with posters placed on each wall and normal lighting. Context D also had the odor of vanilla present which was distributed in the air by a cotton ball soaked in 50mL of vanilla cooking extract and placed in a plastic dish in the room. The odor was distributed throughout the room for 10 minutes prior to exposure as well as during exposure. Context E was a 1.83 x 2.74 m room painted white with posters placed on each wall and normal lighting. Context E also had the odor of peppermint present which was distributed in the air by a cotton ball soaked in 50mL of peppermint cooking extract and placed in a plastic dish in the room. The odor was distributed throughout the room for 10 minutes prior to exposure as well as during exposure. Training and testing were conducted in a (L) x (W) x (H) cm black-white Plexiglas shuttle box with a grid floor (2mm grids spaced at 1mm apart center to center). The two compartments were equal in size and divided by a guillotine door that was manually lifted and lowered by a pulley system controlled by the experimenter. The white compartment consisted of a transparent Plexiglas lid with white Plexiglas walls. The black compartment consisted of a transparent Plexiglas lid with black Plexiglas walls. Footshock was delivered through the grid floor via a constant current AC shock generator (Model 5806 Lafayette Instruments Co., Lafayette, IN). All multiple context exposures and long exposures took place in a clear Plexiglas shoebox measuring (30.5) x (15.2) x (15.2) cm with a clear Plexiglas lid. Procedure

28 23 As with experiment 1, the plan of this experiment was to condition fear in rats either with or without exposure to multiple novel contexts during the retention interval between training and testing. The Ss were tested for their avoidance of the shock compartment with no shock present 48 hours after training in either the training context or a novel context. The procedure for experiment 2 was the same as for the procedure of experiment 1 with a few changes to address exposure issue in experiment 1 and to include the new control groups (See table 2). There were ten groups with 8 rats in the Same group, Multiple Context Exposure Same group, and Multiple Context Exposure Shift group and 6 rats in the Shift group. There were 5 rats in each of the Long Exposure (LE) groups except for the Long Exposure Context D Same group which had a total of 4 rats. Rats were randomly assigned to the ten groups, with group Same, Shift, and all 6 Long Exposure groups the control groups and Multiple Context Exposure Same and Multiple Context Exposure Shift the experimental groups. Given the usual degree of variability among rats and past experience with similar studies this number of rats was an acceptable amount in each group and allowed adequate power to reject the null hypothesis if there was an experimental effect. Control groups were included that controlled for possible effects of duration of exposure in each of the separate contexts used in the exposure treatment (Context C, D, and E). The control groups, groups 5-10 (Table 2), had the same training and testing procedure, but the exposure procedure on day 2 was altered. 24 hours after training the rats in groups 5-10 were placed in one of the 3 contexts for 30 minutes. The Long

29 24 Exposure Context C group was placed in context C for 30 minutes, the Long Exposure Context D group was placed in context D for 30 minutes, and the Long Exposure Context E group was placed in context E for 30 minutes. The rats in the Long Exposure groups were placed in a clear Plexiglas shoebox in the context for the entire duration and were then taken to their home cages until testing which occurred 48 hours after training. Again, the testing procedure was the same for experiment 2 as it was for experiment 1. The Ss in groups Long Exposure Context C Same, Long Exposure Context D Same, and Long Exposure Context E Same were tested in the same context as the training context. The Ss in groups Long Exposure Context C Shift, Long Exposure Context D Shift, and Long Exposure Context E Shift were tested in a novel context that was different from the training context and different from the context they were exposed to on day 2.

30 25 Group 1: Same Group 2: Shift Group 3: Multiple Context Exposure Same Group 4: Multiple Context Exposure Shift Group 5: Long Exposure Context C Same Group 6: Long Exposure Context D Same Group 7: Long Exposure Context E Same Group 8: Long Exposure Context C Shift Group 9: Long Exposure Context D Shift Group 10: Long Exposure Context E Shift Day One Day Two Day Three Train in Context Handle for 30 minutes Test in Context A A Train in Context Handle for 30 minutes Test in Context B A Train in Context 10 minute exposure to Test in Context A A Context C, 10 minute exposure to Context D, 10 minute exposure to Train in Context A Train in Context A Train in Context A Train in Context A Train in Context A Train in Context A Train in Context A Context E 10 minute exposure to Context C, 10 minute exposure to Context D, 10 minute exposure to Context E 30 minute exposure to Context C 30 minute exposure to Context D 30 minute exposure to Context E 30 minute exposure to Context C 30 minute exposure to Context D 30 minute exposure to Context E Test in Context B Test in Context A Test in Context A Test in Context A Test in Context B Test in Context B Test in Context B Table 2. Study design for experiment 2. The ten groups are listed in the first column and the days of the experiment are listed in the second, third, and fourth columns. This table indicates what procedure each group underwent each day.

31 RESULTS Training latencies were evaluated in a simple one-way ANOVA to evaluate whether all animals exhibited comparable cross latencies, which indicates rats natural preference of the black compartment at training. The ANOVA showed there were no significant differences among the training latencies for the 10 groups (F(9,49)=.912, p=.523). Testing cross latencies for the long exposure groups were evaluated in a one-way ANOVA and used to determine retention of original fear memory where shorter cross latencies would indicate strong fear and, thus, retention of the information from the training episode. The ANOVA showed that there was a significant difference among the cross latencies for testing for the long exposure groups (F(5,23)=11.556, p<.001). A posthoc Tukey test indicated that the difference was between the group that received long exposure in Context C and was trained and tested in the same context (M=399.75, SD=279.59) and those that were tested in a context different from the training context after they received long exposure either in Context C (M=62.20, SD=51.31), Context D (M=56.20, SD=45.63), or Context E (M=31.60, SD=19.17) with p<.05. A post-hoc Tukey test indicated that there was also a difference between the group that received long exposure in Context D and was trained and tested in the same context (M=325.60, SD=262.15) and those that were tested in a context different from the training context after they received long exposure in Context E (M=31.60, SD=19.17) with p<.05 but was 26

32 27 not statistically significantly different from those groups exposed to Context C or Context D. The post-hoc Tukey test also indicated that there was a difference between the group that received long exposure in Context E and was trained and tested in the same context (M=600.00, SD=.00) and those that were tested in a context different from the training context after they received long exposure in Context C (M=62.20, SD=51.31), Context D (M=56.20, SD=45.63), or Context E (M=31.60, SD=19.66) with p<.001. Total time on white scores for testing for the long exposure groups were first evaluated in a one-way ANOVA. The ANOVA showed that there was a significant difference among the total time on white scores for testing for the long exposure groups (F(5,23)=33.671, p<.001). A post-hoc Tukey test indicated that the difference was between the groups that were tested in the original context (Same) and those groups that were tested in a novel context (Shift) with p<.001. Mean total time on white scores are shown for the long exposure groups in Figure 2. Because there was no significant difference between contexts (Contexts C, D, and E) for total time on white scores, the groups were collapsed into 2 groups, one of which included the Long Exposure groups that were tested in the same context they were trained in and the other which included the Long Exposure groups that were tested in a novel context that was different from the training context. A one-way ANOVA was used to test any differences in the cross latency scores at testing for those groups that received multiple context exposures, those who received long contextual exposures to one context, and those who received no experimental manipulation during the retention interval between training and testing. The ANOVA

33 28 showed that there was a significant difference among the cross latency scores at test for the 6 groups (F(5,53)=6.518, p<.001). A post-hoc Tukey test was done to investigate the differences in means between the 6 groups. The Tukey test showed that a difference between the Shift group that was tested in a context different from the training context with no experimental manipulation during the retention interval (M=65.50, SD=108.82) and the group that received long exposure to a single context and was trained and tested in the same context (M=444.79, SD=233.54) with p<.01. The Tukey test also revealed a difference between the group that received multiple context exposures during the retention interval but were trained and tested in the same context (M=377.50, SD=289.25) and the group that received long exposure to a single context during the retention interval and was tested in a context different from the training context (M=50.00, SD=40.50) with p<.01. The Tukey test revealed no significant differences in cross latency scores during test between the group that received multiple contextual exposures and was tested in a context different from the training context (M=195.25, SD=264.56) and any of the other groups analyzed. A one-way ANOVA was used to test any differences in the total time on white scores during testing among the 6 groups (Same, Shift, Multiple Context Exposure Same, Multiple Context Exposure Shift, Long Exposure Same, and Long Exposure Shift). The ANOVA showed that there was a statistically significant difference for total time on white during training for the 6 groups (F(5,53)=30.139, p<.001). A post-hoc Tukey test showed that the difference was between the Same and Shift groups, Same and Long Exposure Shift groups, Shift and Multiple Context Exposure Same groups, Shift and

34 29 Multiple Context Exposure Shift groups, Shift and Long Exposure Same groups, and Long Exposure Same and Long Exposure Shift groups (p<.001). The context shift was found in the following conditions: group Same, which was trained and tested in the same context with no experimental manipulation on day two, and group Shift, which was trained in one context and tested in a different context with no experimental manipulation on day two, shows the usual context shift effect; group Same and the Long Exposure Shift group, which was trained in one context and tested in another with exposure to one context for an extended period of time on day two; group Shift, which was trained in one context and tested in another with no experimental manipulation on day two, and Long Exposure Same group, which was trained and tested in the same context with exposure to a single context for a prolonged period of time on day two; the Long Exposure groups based on the statistical significance between Long Exposure Same group and Long Exposure Shift group; the Multiple Context Exposure Shift group, which were exposed to multiple contexts on the second day and tested in a novel context, and the Shift group, with the Multiple Context Exposure Shift group showing similar amounts of fear as the Same group, which received no experimental treatment and was trained and tested in the same context. Mean total time on white scores are shown for all groups, including collapsed Long Exposure groups, in Figure 3.

35 30 Figure 2. Mean total time on white (TTW) scores (±SEM) for Long Exposure (LE) groups in experiment 2 at retention test. The three Same groups (LE Context C Same, LE Context D Same, & LE Context E Same) that were trained and tested in the same context with exposure to a single context during the retention interval were significantly different from the three Shift groups (LE Context C Shift, LE Context D Shift, & LE Context E Shift) that were trained in one context and tested in a novel context with exposure to a single context during the retention interval.

36 31 Figure 3. Mean total time on white (TTW) scores (±SEM) for all groups in experiment two at retention test, including collapsed Long Exposure (LE) groups. The Same and Shift groups were statistically significantly different which indicates the context shift effect occurred. The Multiple Context Exposure groups (MCE Same & MCE Shift) were not statistically significant from one another nor from the Same group indicating retroactive interference from the multiple contexts which resulted in a flattening of the generalization gradient. The Long Exposure Same and Long Exposure Shift groups were statistically significant from one another which shows the usual context shift effect comparable to the normal context shift effect seen with the Same and Shift groups comparison. This indicates that retroactive interference effects occurred because of the exposure to multiple contexts and was not just time dependent nor was there anything particularly salient about any one of the contexts used during exposure.

37 DISCUSSION The results from experiment 2 support the hypothesis that exposure to novel stimuli during the retention interval serve as a source of retroactive interference resulting in a more rapid loss of the context shift effect. The groups that were exposed to multiple contexts between training and testing showed an alleviation of the decrement in performing usually seen when being tested in a novel context, based on the total time on white scores. This indicates that the multiple exposures that these groups experienced acted as retroactive interference for the memory for stimulus attributes from the original training context and, because of the retroactive interference, the rats failed to distinguish the training context and the novel testing context. The results also showed that interference was not an issue of the amount of time in a novel context, as can be seen by the total time on white scores for the long exposure control groups. Because the long exposure control groups, that were exposed to only one novel context for the same amount of time the multiple exposure groups received exposure, did not show a flattening of the stimulus generalization gradient, it can be inferred that the alleviation of the performance decrement in the multiple context exposure groups was due to having been exposed to multiple contexts and experiencing multiple types of stimuli during the retention interval. The long exposure groups used in this study not only allows us to control for any effect duration of time exposed may have had on the forgetting of stimulus attributes but also controlled for the possibility that one 32

38 33 context may have proven to be particularly effective at serving as retroactive interference. Because none of the contexts were statistically significantly different from one another we can deduce that none of the contexts were particularly salient on their own and the retroactive interference effect found was a result from a combination of multiple contexts. This is important in understanding some of the determinants of forgetting of stimulus attributes and what can serve to interfere retroactively with an encoded learning episode and the contextual cues that go along with it.

39 GENERAL DISCUSSION The results of experiment 1 and 2 have given some insight into what can affect the forgetting of stimulus attributes. With experiment 1, the projected results were not obtained, but the results still give some idea of what can affect memory for stimulus attributes. If the results were due to a reminder effect, as postulated, then this indicates that perhaps the usual flattening of the stimulus generalization gradient can be avoided by using reminder treatments even in novel contexts. While previous studies have investigated in part, they used footshock reminders in new contexts to serve as a reminder of the original training (McAllister and McAllister, 2006; Rescorla and Heth, 1975). Zhou & Riccio (1994) showed that you can use a reminder treatment to alleviate the context shift effect but their reminder treatment involved exposure to the training context. The results from the first experiment may indicate that a footshock nor exposure to the original training stimulus is not necessary for a reminder treatment to be effective, but perhaps just being placed in a similar apparatus without exposure to the feared side can provide enough cues to serve as a reminder of original training. The results from the second experiment give some insight into what can affect the forgetting of stimulus attributes. The finding that being exposed to multiple contexts without any training or apparatus exposure can interfere with the memory for stimulus attributes from the original training context further reinforces the importance of context in learning and memory and indicates that contextual stimuli are subject to retroactive 34

RETENTION OF SPECIFICITY OF MEMORY FOR CONTEXT USING REINSTATEMENT

RETENTION OF SPECIFICITY OF MEMORY FOR CONTEXT USING REINSTATEMENT RETENTION OF SPECIFICITY OF MEMORY FOR CONTEXT USING REINSTATEMENT A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts by Christie Lee

More information

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition Scholarly Commons Psychology Faculty Publications 2015 Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition James F. Briggs Timothy A. Toth Brian P. Olson Jacob G.

More information

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats Animal Learning & Behavior 1992. 20 (3). 233-239 The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats JAMES S. MILLER and JOYCE A.

More information

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons Animal Learning & Behavior 1999, 27 (2), 206-210 Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons BRIGETTE R. DORRANCE and THOMAS R. ZENTALL University

More information

Context dependent memory: The role of environmental cues

Context dependent memory: The role of environmental cues Ashland University From the SelectedWorks of Mitchell Metzger, PhD 2002 Context dependent memory: The role of environmental cues Mitchell M. Metzger, Ashland University Available at: https://works.bepress.com/mitchell_metzger/8/

More information

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1980,8(3),363-367 An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock WLLAM C. GORDON and ROBERT R. MOWRER University ofnew Mexico, Albuquerque, New

More information

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery L132 NT MJA/cla Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (1), 60-73 doi:10.3758/lb.37.1.60 Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery Gonzalo P. Urcelay University of Cambridge, Cambridge,

More information

Generalization of Fear Effects in Reinstatement to a Discrete Stimulus

Generalization of Fear Effects in Reinstatement to a Discrete Stimulus University of South Carolina Scholar Commons USC Aiken Psychology Theses Psychology Department 8-2014 Generalization of Fear Effects in Reinstatement to a Discrete Stimulus Lauren Best University of South

More information

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1968, Vol. 66, No. I, 1-5 PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1 ROBERT A. RESCORLA Yale University 2 experiments

More information

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

Some determinants of second-order conditioning Learn Behav (2011) 39:12 26 DOI 10.1007/s13420-010-0002-6 Some determinants of second-order conditioning James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 September 2010 # Psychonomic Society 2010

More information

Spontaneous Recovery From Forward and Backward Blocking

Spontaneous Recovery From Forward and Backward Blocking Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2005, Vol. 31, No. 2, 172 183 Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 0097-7403/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.31.2.172

More information

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1996, Vol. 22. No. 1, 68-75 Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 0097-7403/96/53.00 Value Transfer in a Simultaneous

More information

Context and Pavlovian conditioning

Context and Pavlovian conditioning Context Brazilian conditioning Journal of Medical and Biological Research (1996) 29: 149-173 ISSN 0100-879X 149 Context and Pavlovian conditioning Departamento de Psicologia, Pontifícia Universidade Católica

More information

Renewal of Fear Following Immediate Extinction in a Passive Avoidance Paradigm

Renewal of Fear Following Immediate Extinction in a Passive Avoidance Paradigm Renewal of immediate extinction Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis Vol. 15, No. 2 Copyright 2019 by Reysen Group. 1539-8714 www.jasnh.com 97 Renewal of Fear Following Immediate Extinction

More information

Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem

Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem Animal Learning & Behavior 1981,9 (4),581-586 Interference in pigeons' long-term memory viewed as a retrieval problem DAVID R. THOMAS, ALAN R. McKELVIE, MICHAEL RANNEY, and THOMAS B. MOYE University ofcolorado,

More information

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 1965, Vol. 59, No. 1, 13-17 AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1 GORDON BOWER, RONALD STARR, AND LEAH LAZAROVITZ Stanford

More information

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback acquisition associative learning in classical conditioning the initial stage when one links a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus so that the neutral stimulus begins triggering the conditioned

More information

Characteristics of retrograde amnesia for CS preexposure

Characteristics of retrograde amnesia for CS preexposure Learn Behav (2017) 45:243 251 DOI 10.3758/s13420-017-0259-0 Characteristics of retrograde amnesia for CS preexposure James F. Briggs 1 & Brian P. Olson 1 Published online: 8 February 2017 # Psychonomic

More information

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Chapter 6/9: Learning Chapter 6/9: Learning Learning A relatively durable change in behavior or knowledge that is due to experience. The acquisition of knowledge, skills, and behavior through reinforcement, modeling and natural

More information

Converging Evidence for One-Trial Context Fear Conditioning With an Immediate Shock: Importance of Shock Potency

Converging Evidence for One-Trial Context Fear Conditioning With an Immediate Shock: Importance of Shock Potency University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of March 1997 Converging Evidence for One-Trial Context

More information

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT

PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT The Psychological Record, 2007, 57, 577 592 PURSUING THE PAVLOVIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO INDUCTION IN RATS RESPONDING FOR 1% SUCROSE REINFORCEMENT JEFFREY N. WEATHERLY, AMBER HULS, and ASHLEY KULLAND University

More information

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn?

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Chapter 5: How Do We Learn? Defining Learning A relatively permanent change in behavior or the potential for behavior that results from experience Results from many life experiences, not just structured

More information

MK-801 induced retrieval, but not acquisition, deficits for passive avoidance conditioning

MK-801 induced retrieval, but not acquisition, deficits for passive avoidance conditioning Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 69 (2001) 585 593 www.elsevier.com/locate/pharmbiochembeh MK-801 induced retrieval, but not acquisition, deficits for passive avoidance conditioning S.B. Harrod

More information

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives Theoretical Perspectives REBT (1955) Albert Ellis Action & Result Oriented Teaches how to identify self-defeating thoughts Replaces thoughts w/ life enhancing ones 1 A B C s of personality formation: A

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience. Classical Conditioning Learning through Association Ivan Pavlov discovered the form of learning called Classical

More information

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs

Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Increasing the persistence of a heterogeneous behavior chain: Studies of extinction in a rat model of search behavior of working dogs Eric A. Thrailkill 1, Alex Kacelnik 2, Fay Porritt 3 & Mark E. Bouton

More information

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE 이자료는이바로바스교수의응용행동수정강의를리차드손임상심리학박사가요약해서 정리한것입니다. Lovaas Method Philosophy Children stay with family at home If not working (no positive changes

More information

FEATURED ARTICLE. Paula M. Millin * and Emily N. Newman Department of Psychology, Kenyon College

FEATURED ARTICLE. Paula M. Millin * and Emily N. Newman Department of Psychology, Kenyon College Journal of Behavioral and Neuroscience Research 2008, Vo1. 6(Fall), 6-14 2008 The College of Saint Rose FEATURED ARTICLE A Comparison of the Effects of State and Non-State Reminder Treatments on Morphine

More information

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A. Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing Social Consequences as Reinforcers A Thesis Presented by Hilary A. Gibson The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational

More information

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals

Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1998, 5 (3), 516-522 Timing in pigeons: The choose-short effect may result from pigeons confusion between delay and intertrial intervals LOU M. SHERBURNE Wabash College, Crawfordsville,

More information

Chapter 5 Study Guide

Chapter 5 Study Guide Chapter 5 Study Guide Practice Exam Questions: Which of the following is not included in the definition of learning? It is demonstrated immediately Assuming you have eaten sour pickles before, imagine

More information

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN'

KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 1975, 23, 199-206 NUMBER 2 (march) KEY PECKING IN PIGEONS PRODUCED BY PAIRING KEYLIGHT WITH INACCESSIBLE GRAIN' THOMAS R. ZENTALL AND DAVID E. HOGAN UNIVERSITY

More information

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5 Learning AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5 Learning Learning is a lasting change in behavior or mental process as the result of an experience. There are two important parts: a lasting change a simple reflexive reaction

More information

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit Denniston, J. C., Savastano, H. I., Blaisdell, A. P., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Cue competition as a retrieval deficit. Learning and Motivation, 34(1): 1-31. (Feb 2003) Published by Elsevier (ISSN: 1095-9122).

More information

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation.

Unit 06 - Overview. Click on the any of the above hyperlinks to go to that section in the presentation. Unit 06 - Overview How We Learn and Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Operant Conditioning s Applications, and Comparison to Classical Conditioning Biology, Cognition, and Learning Learning By

More information

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice.

Learning. Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice. Learning Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior acquired through experience or practice. What is Learning? Learning is the process that allows us to adapt (be flexible) to the changing conditions

More information

Unit 6 Learning.

Unit 6 Learning. Unit 6 Learning https://www.apstudynotes.org/psychology/outlines/chapter-6-learning/ 1. Overview 1. Learning 1. A long lasting change in behavior resulting from experience 2. Classical Conditioning 1.

More information

Associative Learning

Associative Learning Learning Learning Associative Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning Biological Components of Learning Cognitive Components of Learning Behavioral Therapies Associative

More information

Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare. Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos

Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare. Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos Supplemental Experimental Procedures Subjects Seven adult capuchin monkeys were tested.

More information

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues

Contextual Effects in Conditioning, Latent Inhibition, and Habituation: Associative and Retrieval Functions of Contextual Cues Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 1989, Vol. 15, No. 3, 232-241 Copyright 1989 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-740389$00.75 Contextual Effects in Conditioning,

More information

Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning

Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning PART II NEUROSCIENCE 1996, UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning Rudolf Cardinal St John s College, Cambridge. Supervised by Dr C.H. Bennett and Professor N.J.

More information

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition A Thesis Presented By Robert Mark Grant In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science

More information

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible? Michael D. Byrne byrne@acm.org Department of Psychology Rice University Houston, TX 77251 Abstract Many simple performance parameters about human memory

More information

Perceptual Learning in Flavor Aversion: Evidence for Learned Changes in Stimulus Effectiveness

Perceptual Learning in Flavor Aversion: Evidence for Learned Changes in Stimulus Effectiveness Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2003, Vol. 29, No. 1, 39 48 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0097-7403/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0097-7403.29.1.39

More information

Classical Conditioning. AKA: Pavlovian conditioning

Classical Conditioning. AKA: Pavlovian conditioning Classical Conditioning AKA: Pavlovian conditioning What is it? A type of learning where a stimulus gains the power to cause a response because it predicts another stimulus that already produces that response.

More information

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs

Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive relationships in the context in which the target stimulus occurs The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ISSN: 1747-0218 (Print) 1747-0226 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pqje20 Human latent inhibition and the density of predictive

More information

Department of Psychology, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48104

Department of Psychology, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48104 BEHAVIORAL BIOLOGY, 8, 749-754 (1973), Abstract No. I228R Effects of Prior Exposure to Animate Objects on Approach Tendency in Chicks SUSAN SAEGERT and D. W. RAJECKI 1 Department of Psychology, The University

More information

Objectives. 1. Operationally define terms relevant to theories of learning. 2. Examine learning theories that are currently important.

Objectives. 1. Operationally define terms relevant to theories of learning. 2. Examine learning theories that are currently important. Objectives 1. Operationally define terms relevant to theories of learning. 2. Examine learning theories that are currently important. Learning Theories Behaviorism Cognitivism Social Constructivism Behaviorism

More information

Which of the following is not an example of a reinforced behavior?

Which of the following is not an example of a reinforced behavior? Learning that is not mechanically acquired through reinforcement, void of overt responses, and is gained through paying attention to other's behavior is called learning. 1. observational 2. association

More information

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior A. Learning-long lasting changes in the environmental guidance of behavior as a result of experience B. Learning emphasizes the fact that individual environments also play

More information

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock Animal Learning & Behavior 1976, Vol. 4 (3), 293-298 The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock THOMAS s. HYDE Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

More information

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning PSY402 Theories of Learning Chapter 8, Theories of Appetitive and Aversive Conditioning Operant Conditioning The nature of reinforcement: Premack s probability differential theory Response deprivation

More information

Chapter 7 Behavior and Social Cognitive Approaches

Chapter 7 Behavior and Social Cognitive Approaches Chapter 7 Behavior and Social Cognitive Approaches What is Learning o What Learning Is and Is Not Learning - a relatively permanent influence on behavior, knowledge, and thinking skills that comes about

More information

The effect of controllable and uncontrollable neonatal preshocks on adult escape/avoidance behavior in the guinea pig (Cavia porcellus)

The effect of controllable and uncontrollable neonatal preshocks on adult escape/avoidance behavior in the guinea pig (Cavia porcellus) Animal Learning & Behavior 1979, 7 (3),372-376 The effect of controllable and uncontrollable neonatal preshocks on adult escape/avoidance behavior in the guinea pig (Cavia porcellus) FRED FENTON, ANNE

More information

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes

Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes Journal Learning & Behavior 2005,?? 33 (?), (4),???-??? 464-478 Amount of training effects in representationmediated food aversion learning: No evidence of a role for associability changes PETER C. HOLLAND

More information

Short article Backward blocking: The role of within-compound associations and interference between cues trained apart

Short article Backward blocking: The role of within-compound associations and interference between cues trained apart THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 2008, 61 (2), 185 193 Short article Backward blocking: The role of within-compound associations and interference between cues trained apart Miguel A. Vadillo

More information

L IB R A R Y Michigan State University

L IB R A R Y Michigan State University r:.1 L IB R A R Y Michigan State University I....III...~. l. I. I.C..........l:. I.. I.I....... m.!. I.I.I...uflfl..i. o «H. \I.. J.. I I. E. i - 4.~ aw It ll un'srnl.lulflhll k.h E.FlLIn "nil-i; Etch.335....y.315

More information

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work? Stephen W. H. Lim (psylwhs@nus.edu.sg) Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Block AS6, 11 Law Link, Singapore 117570 Chua

More information

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999 TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL A Senior Honors Thesis Presented By Vanessa E. Castagna June 999 999 by Vanessa E. Castagna ABSTRACT TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: A TEST OF

More information

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, Vol. 32, No. 3, 347 363 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0278-7393/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

More information

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement*

Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* Animal Learning & Behavior 1974, Vol. 2,No. 2, 148-152 Stimulus control of foodcup approach following fixed ratio reinforcement* RICHARD B. DAY and JOHN R. PLATT McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario,

More information

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor

Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Learn Behav (2012) 40:24 33 DOI 10.3758/s13420-011-0039-1 Extinction context as a conditioned inhibitor Cody W. Polack & Mario A. Laborda & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 24 July 2011 # Psychonomic

More information

Learning. Learning. Learning

Learning. Learning. Learning Learning PSY 1000: Introduction to Psychology Learning Monkeys beginning to wash their food before they eat it The rituals that athletes perform before/during contests Birds learning to flutter their wings

More information

Geometrical and Spatial Cues

Geometrical and Spatial Cues The Huron University College Journal of Learning and Motivation Volume 45 Issue 1 Article 20 2007 Geometrical and Spatial Cues Josée Viau Follow this and additional works at: http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/hucjlm

More information

What is Learned? Lecture 9

What is Learned? Lecture 9 What is Learned? Lecture 9 1 Classical and Instrumental Conditioning Compared Classical Reinforcement Not Contingent on Behavior Behavior Elicited by US Involuntary Response (Reflex) Few Conditionable

More information

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training

Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 23, 343-367 (1992) Occasion Setting without Feature-Positive Discrimination Training CHARLOTTE BONARDI University of York, York, United Kingdom In four experiments rats received

More information

Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on Fear Extinction

Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on Fear Extinction Behavioral Neuroscience 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 123, No. 2, 303 314 0735-7044/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014988 Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on

More information

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2015 Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure Carrie Suzanne Prentice University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee

More information

DEFINITION. Learning is the process of acquiring knowledge (INFORMATIN ) and new responses. It is a change in behavior as a result of experience

DEFINITION. Learning is the process of acquiring knowledge (INFORMATIN ) and new responses. It is a change in behavior as a result of experience LEARNING DEFINITION Learning is the process of acquiring knowledge (INFORMATIN ) and new responses. It is a change in behavior as a result of experience WHAT DO WE LEARN? 1. Object :we learn objects with

More information

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Psychon Bull Rev (2011) 18:309 315 DOI 10.3758/s13423-010-0045-x Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations? Ryoichi Nakashima & Kazuhiko Yokosawa Published online:

More information

Contrasting AAC and ABC renewal: the role of context associations

Contrasting AAC and ABC renewal: the role of context associations Learn Behav (2011) 39:46 56 DOI 10.3758/s13420-010-0007-1 Contrasting AAC and ABC renewal: the role of context associations Mario A. Laborda & James E. Witnauer & Ralph R. Miller Published online: 2 December

More information

The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance

The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 8-1972 The Application of the Species Specific Defense Reaction Hypothesis to Free Operant Avoidance Deborah Ann Cory Western

More information

from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor.

from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor. Learning & Behavior 2010, 38 (1), 68-79 doi:10.3758/lb.38.1.68 Protection from extinction provided by a conditioned inhibitor BRIDGET L. MCCONNELL AND RAR LPH R. MILLER State University of New York, Binghamton,

More information

Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition

Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition LEARNING AND MOTIVATION 13, 391-416 (1982) Extinction of the Context and Latent Inhibition A. G. BAKER AND PIERRE MERCIER McGill University The hypothesis that latent inhibition could be reduced by extinguishing

More information

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context Animal Learning & Behavior 1998, 26 (3), 257 263 Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context BRIGETTE R.

More information

Learning Chapter 6 1

Learning Chapter 6 1 Learning Chapter 6 1 Learning is a relatively permanent change in an organism s behavior due to experience. 2 Stimulus- Stimulus Learning Learning to associate one stimulus with another. 3 Response- Consequence

More information

PSYCHOLOGY ADVANCED LEVEL

PSYCHOLOGY ADVANCED LEVEL PSYCHOLOGY ADVANCED LEVEL AIMS This syllabus aims to provide candidates with an opportunity to: 1. acquire a basic knowledge of psychological theories and research; 2. consider the applications of this

More information

Latent inhibition in a navigation task: Evidence for the use of associative processes in spatial memory. José Prados* Universitat de Barcelona

Latent inhibition in a navigation task: Evidence for the use of associative processes in spatial memory. José Prados* Universitat de Barcelona Psicológica (1999) 20, 151-162. ARTICULOS Latent inhibition in a navigation task: Evidence for the use of associative processes in spatial memory José Prados* Universitat de Barcelona Rats were trained

More information

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D. Hippocampal Pyramidal Neuron of Mice and Rats Figure 1 Open Field Apparatus Open Field Behavior

More information

Factors Modulating Social Influence on Spatial Choice in Rats

Factors Modulating Social Influence on Spatial Choice in Rats Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 2015 American Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 41, No. 3, 286 300 2329-8456/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000063 Factors

More information

Counterconditioning of an Overshadowed Cue Attenuates Overshadowing

Counterconditioning of an Overshadowed Cue Attenuates Overshadowing Journal of F.xperirmm~ Psycholosy: Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological hau~ocialioa, Inc. Animall~ehavioa'Procc~es 0097-7403/00/$5.110 DOI: 10.10371/0097-7403.26.1.74 2000, VoL 26, No. 1, 74-86

More information

Outline. History of Learning Theory. Pavlov s Experiment: Step 1. Associative learning 9/26/2012. Nature or Nurture

Outline. History of Learning Theory. Pavlov s Experiment: Step 1. Associative learning 9/26/2012. Nature or Nurture Outline What is learning? Associative Learning Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning History of Learning Theory Nature or Nurture BEHAVIORISM Tabula Rasa Learning: Systematic,

More information

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength by Anita Dyan Barber BA, University of Louisville, 2000 MS, University

More information

January 8. EQ- What are the major elements of classical conditioning?

January 8. EQ- What are the major elements of classical conditioning? January 8 EQ- What are the major elements of classical conditioning? 1. Vocab, Test Q s, Do Now 2. Elements of CC Chart 3. Review Q s for CC 4. Classical Conditioning in Advertising 5. Operant Conditioning

More information

Recovery of Fear Memories in Rats: Role of Gamma-Amino Butyric Acid (GABA) in Infantile Amnesia

Recovery of Fear Memories in Rats: Role of Gamma-Amino Butyric Acid (GABA) in Infantile Amnesia Behavioral Neuroscience Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 120, No. 1, 40 48 0735-7044/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.1.40 Recovery of Fear Memories in Rats: Role

More information

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Behavioural Brain Research 110 (2000) 97 108 www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning Stephen Maren *, William Holt Department of Psychology

More information

Psyc 3705, Cognition--Introduction Sept. 13, 2013

Psyc 3705, Cognition--Introduction Sept. 13, 2013 Cognitive Psychology: Introduction COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY The domain of Cognitive Psychology A brief history of Cognitive Psychology Professor: Dana R. Murphy, Ph.D. Meeting times: Fridays 9 AM to 11:50

More information

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates

Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Utah State University DigitalCommons@USU All Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies 8-2012 Reinforcer Magnitude and Resistance to Change of Forgetting Functions and Response Rates Meredith

More information

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement

Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Psicológica (2004), 25, 45-65. Retardation and summation tests after extinction: The role of familiarity and generalization decrement Matías López*, Raúl Cantora*, and Luis Aguado** 1 * Universidad de

More information

Behavioural Processes

Behavioural Processes Behavioural Processes 90 (2012) 311 322 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journa l h omepa g e: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc US specificity of occasion setting:

More information

Primacy and recency effects in extinction and latent inhibition: A selective review with implications for models of learning

Primacy and recency effects in extinction and latent inhibition: A selective review with implications for models of learning Behavioural Processes 69 (2005) 223 235 Primacy and recency effects in extinction and latent inhibition: A selective review with implications for models of learning Oskar Pineño, Ralph R. Miller Department

More information

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions

Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions Learning & Behavior 2009, 37 (4), 299-304 doi:10.3758/lb.37.4.299 Animal memory: The contribution of generalization decrement to delayed conditional discrimination retention functions REBECCA RAYBURN-REEVES

More information

RETROGRADE AMNESIA AND RECONSOLIDATION OF A CONTEXT-NO US ASSOCIATION. A dissertation submitted. to Kent State University in partial

RETROGRADE AMNESIA AND RECONSOLIDATION OF A CONTEXT-NO US ASSOCIATION. A dissertation submitted. to Kent State University in partial RETROGRADE AMNESIA AND RECONSOLIDATION OF A CONTEXT-NO US ASSOCIATION A dissertation submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

More information

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction

Learning Habituation Associative learning Classical conditioning Operant conditioning Observational learning. Classical Conditioning Introduction 1 2 3 4 5 Myers Psychology for AP* Unit 6: Learning Unit Overview How Do We Learn? Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Learning by Observation How Do We Learn? Introduction Learning Habituation

More information

Stimulus and Temporal Cues in Classical Conditioning

Stimulus and Temporal Cues in Classical Conditioning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 2 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 97-743//$5. DOI: 1.137//97-743.26.2.26 2, Vol. 26, No. 2, 26-219 Stimulus and Temporal

More information

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning Chapter 6: Learning 1 Learning 1. In perception we studied that we are responsive to stimuli in the external world. Although some of these stimulus-response associations are innate many are learnt. 2.

More information

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY

MOUNT ALLISON UNIVERSITY JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 00, 79, 6 NUMBER (MARCH) RESPONDING FOR SUCROSE AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCEMENT: EFFECTS OF SUCROSE CONCENTRATION AND WHEEL-RUNNING REINFORCER DURATION

More information

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit q

Cue competition as a retrieval deficit q Learning and Motivation 34 (2003) 1 31 www.elsevier.com/locate/l&m Cue competition as a retrieval deficit q James C. Denniston, a, * Hernan I. Savastano, b Aaron P. Blaisdell, c and Ralph R. Miller d a

More information

BEHAVIOR CHANGE THEORY

BEHAVIOR CHANGE THEORY BEHAVIOR CHANGE THEORY An introduction to a behavior change theory framework by the LIVE INCITE team This document is not a formal part of the LIVE INCITE Request for Tender and PCP. It may thus be used

More information

Extinction has been both highly researched and highly implemented in the treatment of

Extinction has been both highly researched and highly implemented in the treatment of Deepened Extinction of Conditioned Suppression in Rats and Implications for the Treatment of Behavioral Disorders Elana Canetti Capstone Advisor: David N. Kearns Spring Semester 2010 General University

More information