The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep"

Transcription

1 Sleep. 19(7): American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep Mats Gillberg, Garan Kecklund, John Axelsson and Torbjarn Akerstedt Karolinska Institute and National Institute for Psychosocial Factors and Health, Stockholm, Sweden Summary: Eight subjects participated on three occasions in a study investigating the effect of a 30-minute daytime nap opportunity on alertness/sleepiness. The baseline condition was a normal home sleep (7.5 hours, with bedtime at 2300 hours). Sleep during the other two conditions was between 2400 hours and 0400 hours. During one of the two 4-hour conditions, a short nap was allowed (between 1045 hours and 1115 hours). Self-ratings of sleepiness/alertness (Karolinska Sleepiness Scale) were recorded every hour. At 10, 12, and 15 hours, the subjects performed a 28-minute visual vigilance task. Electroencephalograms (EEG) and electrooculograms (EOG) were recorded continuously, including during a lo-minute standardized recording procedure at the beginning of each day. Mean total sleep time during the nap was 19.8 (standard error 2.4) minutes. Compared to baseline, EEGIEOG sleepiness and subjective sleepiness were significantly higher and vigilance performance at 10 hours lower, respectively, after the two short sleeps. The nap brought performance to baseline levels, and subjective sleepiness decreased significantly. It was concluded that the short nap had a clear positive effect on alertness. Key Words: Daytime nap--restricted sleep--performance-eegieog-subjective sleepiness. Sleepiness is an unavoidable consequence of shift work and irregular work hours and severely increases the risk for accidents and mistakes. One countermeasure against sleepiness might be to allow naps during work. Naps have been shown to have positive effects on alertness in several different settings. Naps taken during the day or evening in advance of expected sleepiness, so-called prophylactic naps, have efficiently. counteracted the loss of alertness during the ensuing night whether they have been 1 (1),2 (2-5), or 3 hours (6,7) long. It has also been demonstrated that naps can counteract the effects of sleep deprivation. In a study of our own (1), alertness measured during the early morning was increased by a I-hour nap at 0430 hours. Rodgers et al. (8), however, found only a limited effect of a I-hour nap on overnight performance. Dinges et al. (2,3) have shown that 2-hour naps efficiently counteracted sleepiness after 30, 42, and 54 hours awake, respectively. Positive effects were also demonstrated by Naitoh (9) in an experiment with naps scheduled after 45 or 54 hours awake. Accepted for publication April Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Mats Gillberg, Department of Public Health Sciences, Division for Stress Research, Karolinska Institute, Box 220, S Stockholm, Sweden. 570 Although a nap of several hours' duration seems unrealistic in work situations, a short nap that is, of no more than 30 minutes' duration, might be acceptable. There are studies of repeated short naps, for example, 20-minute naps repeated every 6 hours (10), but very few studies have investigated the effects of single short naps. Lumley et al. (11) compared the effects of naps that were 0, 15, 30, 60, and 120 minutes long, respectively. The naps were allowed at 0900 hours after 1 night without sleep. Alertness was assessed with sleep latency tests at 2-hour intervals for 8 hours following the nap. Three findings emerged: first, all naps except the I5-minute nap were beneficial; second, increasing the nap duration from 60 to 120 minutes did not further increase alertness; and third, the positive effects did not appear until 4 hours after the nap. The purpose of the present experiment was to investigate the effects of a short nap on performance, electrophysiologically defined sleepiness, and subjective sleepiness. Furthermore, the purpose was to simulate the moderate levels of alertness encountered in connection with, for example, shift work, rather than sleep deprivation. In the present case we chose to simulate the daytime sleepiness appearing during an early morning shift.

2 EFFECTS OF A SHORT DAYTIME NAP 571 Subjects and design METHODS Eight male subjects participated (mean age 32 years, range 24-45) in a cross-over repeated measures design with three conditions appearing in a counterbalanced order. The baseline condition was a normal 7- to 7.5- hour (approximate bedtime at 2300 hours) sleep at home, followed by an 8-hour (0800 hours to 1600 hours) test period in the laboratory. The other two conditions involved 4-hour sleeps (between 2400 hours and 0400 hours). Both 4-hour conditions were followed by the same 8-hour test period as for the baseline condition. However, during one of the two 4-hour conditions a 30-minute nap was allowed (between 1045 hours and 1115 hours). In the following the conditions are referred to as BL (baseline), Nap, and NN (no nap). To ascertain that the subjects were in good health and had no sleep problems, they were given a questionnaire. This questionnaire also included the diurnal type scale (DTS) (12). The mean rating of general health ("how has your general health been the last year?"; 5-point scale with 5 = very good) was 4.87 [standard error (SE) 0.12]. Four subjects took no medication at all; three subjects took aspirin occasionally, and one subject used hypnotics "once, or a couple of times per year". The general sleep quality rating ("how is on the average your sleep quality?"; 5-point scale with 5 = very good) was 4.37 (SE 0.18). Mean bedtime during weekends was 2430 hours (SE 30 minutes), and mean sleep duration was 9.2 hours (SE 0.7). The corresponding values for work days were hours (SE 18 minutes) and 7.8 hours (SE 0.4). None of the subjects took naps. The mean coffee consumption was 1.6 cups per day (SE 0.6); there was no consumption of other caffeinated beverages. The subjects varied between 1.14 and 2.71, with a mean of 1.93 and an SE of 0.23, on the Diurnal Type Scale. This scale ranges from 1 to 4; higher values mean more "morningness" and lower values more "eveningness". Procedure For the BL condition the subjects were instructed to go to bed at their habitual bedtime, but not later than 2300 hours, and to rise between 0600 hours and 0630 hours. For the other two conditions the subjects were instructed to go to bed at 2400 hours and to rise at 0400 hours. The home sleeps were monitored by means of subjective sleep diaries (13). The subjects were told not to drink caffeinated beverages after rising, and to spend their time, after awakening and before traveling to the laboratory, reading or doing similar sedentary activities. The participation of the subjects was carefully planned on each of the three occasions so that prior work or leisure activities should not interfere with the experiment. In addition, the subjects were asked to report any deviations from the instructions, for example, failure to fall asleep and sleep according to the above instructions, for which there was a risk, especially in connection with the restricted sleeps. None of the subjects reported such deviations from the instructions. In connection with all three conditions, the subjects arrived at the laboratory at 0800 hours for instructions and application of electrodes. The experiment proper started at 0900 hours with a 1O-minute (5 minutes with open eyes, followed by 5 minutes with closed eyes) standardized electroencephalographic (EEG)/electrooculographic (EOG) recording procedure, the Karolinska alertness test (KAT, see below), and ended at 1600 hours with removal of electrodes. Subjective sleep quality The sleep quality measure extracted from the sleep diaries was an index calculated as a mean of the following 5-point items (5 = best quality): "how was your sleep?", "unrestful sleep", "did you get enough sleep", "difficulties falling asleep", "ease of awakening", and "well rested". Test blocks and nap Three identical 45-minute test blocks were presented. They were scheduled at 0945 hours, 1145 hours, and 1500 hours, respectively. The test blocks contained a 1O-minute reaction time (RT) task and a 28-minute visual vigilance task. Subjective ratings were obtained before and after the RT task and after the vigilance task. The nap during the Nap condition was scheduled between 1045 hours and 1115 hours. There was exactly 30 minutes between lights out and awakening. A 30-minute period was then allowed for sleep inertia (14) to dissipate before the start of the next test block (at 1145 hours). Reaction time task The RT task was 10 minutes long and was presented on a handheld computer. Sixteen signals per minute were presented at random intervals. Unfortunately, approximately half of the recordings were lost due to technical failure and therefore the data could not be analyzed. Sleep. Vol. 19. No.7, 1996

3 572 M. GILLBERG ET AL. Vigilance task The 28-minute vigilance task was presented on a computer display and contained 60 signals, occurring at random time intervals. The temporal distribution of signals was similar for each. quarter of the test. The task was described in more detail in a previous paper (15). Subjective ratings of sleepiness The scale used was the 9-point (9 = highest level of sleepiness) Karolinska sleepiness scale (KSS) (16). In addition to the ratings obtained in connection with the test blocks, ratings were obtained every hour, beginning at 0800 hours and ending at 2200 hours. Ratings from 1700 hours and onward were given outside the laboratory. Electroencephalography and EOG Electroencephalography (leads: C3-A2 and 02-P4) and EOG (left eye) were recorded continuously during each experimental condition. These recordings were scored visually in order to derive an electrophysiological measure of sleepiness during both the KAT and the vigilance tests. During the KAT the subjects sat in a reclining position and were instructed to relax, but try to remain awake, and first look at a black dot in the ceiling for 5 minutes and then to close their eyes for 5 minutes. The instructions were given via an intercom. The EEGIEOG recordings were considered to show signs of sleepiness if one, or more, of the following activities was present: alpha activity (8-12 Hz), theta activity (4-8 Hz), slow rolling eye movements (defined as > I-second slow, rolling excursions of the EOG of at least 100 f.lv amplitude), or regular sleep (stage 1 activity or "deeper"). Each 30-second epoch was visually scored, could contain from 0 to 100% signs of sleepiness, and was scored in six intervals (0, 10, 30, 50, 70, and 90%). If no EEGIEOG signs of sleepiness were present, 0 was scored. If signs of sleepiness were present, but for less than 20% of an epoch, 10% was scored. Thirty percent was scored for signs of sleepiness in the range of 20-40% of an epoch, and so on, for the remaining 20% intervals, up to 100%. In addition, the recordings outside the nap and the tests were scanned to ascertain the amount of unauthorized sleep. The nap sleep was scored according to standard procedures (17). The subjects were studied in pairs but spent their time in separate, sound-insulated rooms containing a bed and a reclining chair in which they sat during the KAT and while performing the tasks. The two rooms were parts of a sound-insulated small apartment also containing a living roomlkitchen and a toilet. The subjects were served a light meal (sandwiches and mineral water) on three occasions: before the KAT, after the nap, and before the third test block. During these meals the subjects sat together. Between tests the subjects were allowed to read and listen to music. Clocks and other time cues were not allowed. Statistical analyses If not otherwise stated, repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOV As) were used to evaluate the experimental effects. Because such analyses are sensitive to violations of the requirement of equal variances, p values are given after the Huynh-Feldt epsilon (E) correction (18). E is a measure of the degree to which the requirements of equal variances are violated; it is used to adjust (downwards) the degrees of freedom (df) before performing the F test. Although the F tests were performed with adjusted df, the original df are maintained in the ANOV A presentations. The 5% alpha level was adopted. Post-hoc comparisons were made with the Newman-Keuls test. Prior night sleep RESULTS Data extracted from the sleep diaries show that the subjective sleep durations (calculated as time in bed minus time to fall asleep and time awake in connection with awakenings) varied in the expected manner (means and SEs in hours): 6.73 ± 0.16 for BL, 3.75 ± 0.06 for Nap, and 3.71 ± 0.06 for NN. The times of awakening varied significantly (means and SEs: 0615 hours ± 7 minutes for BL, 0411 hours ± 8 minutes for Nap, and 0412 hours ± 9 minutes for NN). Finally, the subjective sleep qu,ality of the two curtailed sleeps was significantly lower compared to the BL sleep [means and SEs: BL = 4.0 ± 0.25, Nap = 3.2 ± 0.22, and NN = 2.8 ± 0.29; p < 0.002; F(2,14) = 10.20; E = 0.98]. Effect of sleep curtailment/extended wake time on alertness Subjective alertness rated after arrival to the laboratory but before the start of the experiment (the mean of ratings at 0800 hours and 0900 hours) showed no significant differences between conditions [BL = 4.56, Nap = 4.75, and NN = 5.13; P < 0.160; F(2,14) = 2.285; E = 0.859]. Results from the KAT with open eyes showed low levels of sleepiness and no differences between conditions. KAT with closed eyes, on Sleep. Vol. 19. No

4 EFFECTS OF A SHORT DAYTIME NAP 573 TABLE 1. Percentage of time with EEGIEOG sleepiness during the Karolinska alertness test (KAT) BL Nap NN F(2,14) P (E) Open eyes 0 (0) 3.7 (2.3) 6.3 (3.7) 1.9 ns (1.0) Closed eyes 6.5 (4.2) 48.2 (12.6) 44.3 (15.2) (1.0) Data shown are means and SEs. Results are from one-way AN OVAs. the other hand, showed markedly higher levels of sleepiness when prior sleep was curtailed (Table O. Sleep parameter TABLE 2. TST Stage 1 Stage 2 Stage 3 Stage 4 REM Latency stage 1 Nap sleep parameters Duration 19.8 (2.4) 7.1 (1.2) 9.0 (1.5) 2.4 (1.3) 1.3 (0.7) (2.4) Range Data (means and SEs) are in minutes. TST, total sleep time; REM, rapid eye movement. Nap sleep parameters Table 2 shows that all subjects fell asleep during the nap opportunity and that the sleep obtained consisted mainly of stage 1 and stage 2 sleep. Effects of the nap on performance and alertness There were clear significant effects of conditions on vigilance performance [F(2,14) = 7.4; P < 0.008; E = 0.91]. Neither changes across the three tests nor the interaction were significant [F(2,14) = 0.83 and F(4,28) = 1.75, respectively] (Fig. 1). Pairwise comparisons revealed that there were significantly (p < 0.05) fewer detected signals (hits) during the NN condition compared to both of the other two conditions. Comparing results from the first test across conditions shows significantly fewer hits after both the Nap and NN conditions compared to BL. Pairwise comparisons of the results on the second test, that is, the one performed after the nap, showed significantly fewer hits during the NN condition. compared to the other conditions. During the third test, only the NN and BL conditions differed from each other. Because the main issue concerned the Nap and NN conditions and the changes from before to after the nap, we carried out an additional, restricted ANO VA using only this 2 X 2 design. The analysis shows a significant effect of condition [F(I,7) = 9.2; P < 0.019; E = 1.00] and a significant interaction [F(l,7) = 10.9; P < 0.013; E = 1.00], indicating a differential, positive effect of the nap. ElectroencephalographiclEOG signs of sleepiness during tests Only the effect of condition was significant (Table 3). Pairwise comparisons showed that the BL and NN conditions differed significantly (p < 0.05) during the first test as well as for all tests combined. Although data suggest that the nap decreased sleepiness, this is not supported by the statistical analyses. As for the vigilance results, a restricted 2 X 2 ANOV A was ap- plied to the data in Table 3. Neither the main effects nor the interaction, however, were significant [F(l,7) conditions = 3.6; F(I,7) test number = 1.8; F(1,7) interaction = 1.4]. Subjective sleepiness In order to obtain an integrated measure of subjective sleepiness before and after the nap (and the corresponding period for the other two conditions), individual averages of all KSS ratings recorded from 0800 hours until the nap and of ratings recorded after the nap until after the third vigilance test, respectively, were calculated. The data and results from ANOV A are given in Table 4. The analysis showed a significant variation across conditions and a significant interaction between condition and time (i.e. before and after the nap). A closer inspection by means of pairwise comparisons showed that sleepiness before the nap (or the corresponding period) was significantly (p < 0.05) lower during the BL condition compared to both of the other conditions. Comparing sleepiness after this period revealed sig- ~ s:. '0 Z E ::J c:: Baseline ' '.'.,8.,.,.,. ill-,.,.,.,.,.,.,. '.'.'.'.'.'.'.' 86 No nap I range of s.e. 30+L~~~-r~~~-'--~r-~ time of day FIG. 1. The number of hits on the vigilance task. Shown are the means of eight subjects. The range of the SE: (i.e. the largest and the smallest SE) is shown to the left. Sleep, Vol. 19, No.7, 1996

5 574 M. GILLBERG ET AL. TABLE 3. Percentage of time with EEGIEOG signs of sleepiness during the vigilance task Test Condition First Second Third Results from two-way ANOV A BL 0.7 (0.5) 5.0 (4.9) 9.3 (7.9) F(2,14) conditions = 5.2; P < 0.04; E = 0.74 Nap 20.2 (8.2) 11.2 (6.1) 11.4 (6.7) F(2,14) test no. = 0.3; ns; E = 0.91 NN 25.4 (11.5) 22.3 (8.4) 22.6 (8.8) F(4,28) interaction = 0.9; ns; E = 0.48 Data shown are means and SEs. nificantly (p < 0.05) higher levels during the NN condition compared to both the BL and the Nap conditions. The subjective ratings obtained outside the laboratory during the afternoon and evening (between 1700 hours and 2200 hours) were averaged to demonstrate the long-term effects of prior sleep manipulation and of the nap. The mean level of sleepiness was lower during the BL condition compared to the other two conditions [means and SEs: BL = 4.7 (0.3), Nap = 5.3 (0.3), NN = 5.3 (0.3)]. However, the variation between conditions was not significant (ns) (ANOV A: F(2,14) = 2.53, ns, E = 0.61). DISCUSSION Restricting the period of sleep to the first 4 hours of the night resulted in subjective total sleep times that were approximately 3 hours and 45 minutes, compared to 6 hours and 45 minutes for the baseline night. The two shorter nights also showed lower subjective quality. The restricted sleeps both had clear negative effects on daytime alertness, as measured by performance and electrophysiological variables. Vigilance performance on the first vigilance test was approximately 20% lower after the two shorter sleep periods compared to that after a normal sleep duration. When the subjects were asked to relax and close their eyes for 5 minutes, there were electrophysiological signs of sleepiness for about 50% of this period if prior night sleep had been 3 hours and 45 minutes, whereas such signs were present for only 10% of the recording period after the baseline night. Subjective alertness was also lower when prior sleep was curtailed. EEGIEOG signs of sleepiness during the vigilance test also tended to be higher after the restricted sleeps. Interestingly, there were no differences in subjective alertness between conditions immediately after arrival to the laboratory (0800 hours to 0900 hours), but such were revealed later, presumably when the subjects were placed in the relaxing and monotonous experimental situation. The effects of sleep curtailment observed in the present experiment are in agreement with similar findings in other studies (19-22). When discussing the effects of sleep curtailment one must, however, keep in mind that sleep was not polygraphically recorded but assessed by subjective methods. Hence, we do not know the exact duration or architecture of sleep. In our experience there is, however, a fair agreement between sleep diary data and polygraphically recorded sleep (13). Also, the lower morning alertness after the two curtailed nights clearly indicates that subjects actually had shorter sleeps. All subjects managed to fall asleep during the allotted time, and the effects on vigilance performance and subjective alertness were clear and positive. The nap brought vigilance performance to levels nearly identical to those observed during the baseline condition. Similar results were observed for subjective sleepiness. EEGIEOG signs of sleepiness during the vigilance test, on the other hand, were not as clearly affected by the nap. As indicated by the vigilance results, the positive effects of the nap were observed starting 30 minutes after the nap. It is obvious from Fig. 1 that the positive effect on performance had partly dissipated by the third vigilance test. However, because the nap condition did not differ significantly, either from the baseline or from the no-nap condition during the third test, it seems that some of the effect remained. Also, the subjective ratings integrated for the post-nap period suggest that the positive effect persisted until the subjects left the laboratory. Finally, the integrated ratings obtained outside the laboratory (1700 hours to 2200 hours) revealed no significant dif- TABLE 4. Mean levels Karolinska sleepiness scale (KSS) ratings obtained before and after the nap (or the corresponding period for the other conditions). Condition BL Nap NN Data shown are means and SEs. Before nap 5.3 (0.5) 6.2 (0.5) 6.3 (0.6) After nap 5.8 (0.2) 5.7 (0.5) 6.7 (0.6) Results from two-way ANOV A F(2,14) condition = 6.0; P < 0.01; E = 1.0 F(l,7) before/after = 0.6; ns; E = 1.0 F(2,14) interaction = 4.0; P < 0.04; E = 0.99 Sleep, Vol. 19, No.7, 1996

6 EFFECTS OF A SHORT DAYTIME NAP 575 ferences, although there was a trend towards lower alertness after the two restricted nights. The alerting effects of the nap were obtained despite the fact that the mean nap duration was only 20 minutes and that it consisted mainly of stage 1 and 2 sleep. All subjects increased their performance from before to after the nap. Interestingly, because the minimum nap duration was 11 minutes (with no slow wave sleep), this suggests that even such short naps may have recuperative effects. As mentioned earlier, the positive effect of the nap was most prominent when measured 30 minutes after it ended. Lumley et al. (11), on the other hand, concluded that the positive effect of their 30-minute nap did not occur until alertness was measured 4 hours after the end of the nap. In their case, however, the nap was given at 0900 hours after 1 night without sleep (11). At that time the subjects were very sleepy, as indicated by the 1.8-minute mean sleep latency. In addition, because they were allowed to sleep for 30 minutes after sleep onset, a more severe sleep inertia could be expected. In conclusion, the alerting effects of a short daytime nap are obvious. A short nap will not necessarily be as successful if given during the night, when there is longer prior wakefulness and the circadian rhythm is close to its trough. In another study (23) we found no effect of a 30-minute night nap opportunity (resulting in 18.7 minutes of sleep) whether the nap was scheduled at 0100 hours or 0330 hours. Similarly, Saito and Sasaki (24) could not demonstrate positive effects of a 30-minute nap opportunity (resulting in 20.5 minutes of sleep) scheduled at 0300 hours. A nighttime nap may require longer duration for effect, as evidenced by our earlier study (1) showing alerting effects of a I-hour nighttime nap. Hence, the effect of a short nap may depend on the circadian phase and on the amount of prior wakefulness. Acknowledgement: This study was made possible through a grant from the Swedish Work Environment Fund. REFERENCES 1. Gillberg M. The effects of a I-hour nap on early morning performance. Bioi Psychol 1984;19: Dinges DF, Orne MT, Whitehouse WG, Orne EC. Temporal placement of a nap for alertness: contributions of circadian phase and prior wakefulness. Sleep 1987;10: Dinges DF, Whitehouse WG, Orne EC, Orne MT. The benefits of a nap during prolonged work and wakefulness. Work Stress 1988;2: Lavie P, Weier B. Timing of naps: effects on post-nap sleepiness levels. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1989;72: Bonnet MH. The effect of varying prophylactic naps on performance, alertness and mood throughout a 52-hour continuous operation. Sleep 1991;14: Sugerman JL, Walsh JK. Physiological sleep tendency and ability to maintain alertness at night. Sleep 1989;12: Schweitzer PK, Muehlbach MJ, Walsh JK. Countermeasures for night performance deficits: the effect of napping or caffeine on continuous performance at night. Work Stress 1992;6: Rogers AS, Spencer MB, Stone BM, Nicholson AN. The influence of a 1 h nap on performance overnight. Ergonomics 1989;32: Naitoh P. Circadian cycles and restorative power of naps. In: Johnson LC, Tepas DI, Colquhoun Wp, Colligan MJ, eds. Biological rhythms: sleep and shift work. New York: Spectrum Publications, 1981 : Naitoh P, Kelly TL, Babkoff H. Napping, stimulant, and fourchoice performance. In: Broughton RI, Ogilvie RD, eds. Sleep, arousal, and performance. Boston: Birkhiiuser, Lumley M, Roehrs T, Zorick F, Lamphere J, Roth T. The alerting effects of naps in sleep-deprived subjects. Psychophysiology 1986;23: Torsvall L, Akerstedt T. A diurnal type scale. Scand J Work Environ Health 1980;6: Akerstedt T, Hume K, Minors D, Waterhouse J. The meaning of good sleep: a longitudinal study of polysomnography and subjective sleep quality. J Sleep Res 1994; Dinges DF, Orne EC, Evans FJ, Orne MT. Performance after naps in sleep-conducive and alerting environments. In: Johnson LC, Tepas DI, Colquhoun WP, Colligan MJ, eds. Biological rhythms: sleep and shift work. New York: Spectrum Publications, 1981: Gillberg M, Kecklund G, Akerstedt T. Relations between performance and subjective ratings of sleepiness during a night awake. Sleep 1994;17: Akerstedt T, Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci 1990;52: Rechtschaffen A, Kales A, eds. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. Bethesda, MD: U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Public Health Servi,\e, Huynh, H, Feldt LS. Estimation at the box 'correction for degrees of freedom for sample data in randomised block and split-plot designs. J Educ Stat 1976;1: Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Cumulative effects of sleep restriction on daytime sleepiness. Psychophysiology 1981;18: Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Nocturnal determinants of daytime sleepiness. Sleep 1982;5(Suppl):S Tilley AI, Wilkinson RT. The effects of a restricted sleep regime on the composition of sleep and on performance. Psychophysiology 1984;21: Gillberg M, Akerstedt T. Sleep restriction and SWS suppression: effects on day time alertness and night time recovery. J Sleep Res 1994;3: Gillberg M, Kecklund G, Akerstedt T. Sleepiness and performance of professional drivers in a truck simulator--comparisons between day and night driving. J Sleep Res 1996;5: Saito Y, Sasaki T. Sleepiness and fatigue during early morning hours after a 30 minute nocturnal nap. Shiftwork Inti Newsl 1995;12:53. Sleep, Vol. 19, No.7, 1996

Relations Between Performance and Subjective Ratings of Sleepiness During a Night Awake

Relations Between Performance and Subjective Ratings of Sleepiness During a Night Awake Sleep, 17(3):236--241 1994 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Relations Between Performance and Subjective Ratings of Sleepiness During a Night Awake Mats Gillberg, Goran Kecklund

More information

Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness

Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness Sleep 20(3):215-223 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness Goran Kecklund Torbjom Akerstedt and Arne Lowden National

More information

Daytime Sleepiness and Antihistamines

Daytime Sleepiness and Antihistamines Sleep, 7(2): 137-141 1984 Raven Press, New York Daytime Sleepiness and Antihistamines imothy A. Roehrs, Elizabeth I. ietz, Frank J. Zorick, and homas Roth Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford

More information

Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals

Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals Sleep 2(5):449-457, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 989 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals Timothy Roehrs, Victoria Timms, Ardith Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos,

More information

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days Sleep Research Online 5(1): 13-17, 2003 http://www.sro.org/2003/hayashi/13/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 96-214X 2003 WebSciences The Effects of Short Daytime s for Five Consecutive Mitsuo

More information

Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation

Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation Sleep. 18( I): 1--6 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation Validation of the Sand C Components of the Three-Process Model of Alertness Regulation

More information

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep Sleep, 6(4):319-325 1983 Raven Press, New York Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep T. Roehrs, F. Zorick, J. Sicklesteel, R. Wittig, and T. Roth Sleep Disorders and Research

More information

Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People

Sleep and Body Temperature in Morning and Evening People Sleep. 8(4):311-318 1985 Raven Press. New York Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People Jean Foret, *Nathalie Touron, *Odile Benoit, and *Ginette Bouard Laboratoire de Physiologie Neurosensorielle

More information

Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects

Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects Sleep I ():54-60, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 988 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects Edward Stepanski,

More information

Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value

Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value Sleep 10(6):590-599, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1987 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value Brian Levine, Timothy Roehrs, Edward Stepanski, Frank

More information

The Effects of Caffeine on Simulated Night-Shift Work and Subsequent Daytime Sleep

The Effects of Caffeine on Simulated Night-Shift Work and Subsequent Daytime Sleep Sleep. 18(1):22-29 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society The Effects of Caffeine on Simulated Night-Shift Work and Subsequent Daytime Sleep Mark J. Muehlbach and James K.

More information

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants Sleep, 5:S128-S134 1982 Raven Press, New York Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants T. Roth, T. Roehrs, and F. Zorick Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, Michigan

More information

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults Sleep 13(5):385-394, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1990 Association of Professional Sleep Societies The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults M.

More information

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle Sleep 10(1):62-68, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components

More information

Behavioral Treatment and Sleep. The Effects of Regularizing Sleep-Wake Schedules on Daytime Sleepiness

Behavioral Treatment and Sleep. The Effects of Regularizing Sleep-Wake Schedules on Daytime Sleepiness Sleep, 19(5):432-441 1996 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Behavioral Treatment and Sleep The Effects of Regularizing Sleep-Wake Schedules on Daytime Sleepiness *Rachel Manber,

More information

The Alpha Attenuation Test: Assessing Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Narcolepsy-Cataplexy

The Alpha Attenuation Test: Assessing Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Narcolepsy-Cataplexy Sleep, 20(4):258-266 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society.j The Alpha Attenuation Test: Assessing Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Narcolepsy-Cataplexy Christi E. D. Alloway,

More information

Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence

Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence Sleep, 5:S165S174 1982 Raven Press, New York Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence F. Zorick, T. Roehrs, G. Koshorek, J. Sicklesteel, *K. Hartse, R. Wittig, and T.

More information

Evaluation of In-Car Countermeasures to Sleepiness: Cold Air and Radio

Evaluation of In-Car Countermeasures to Sleepiness: Cold Air and Radio Evaluation of In-Car Countermeasures to Sleepiness: Cold Air and Radio L.A. Reyner, J.A. Horne Sleep Research Laboratory, Loughborough University, UK Summary: The efficacy of putative in-car countermeasures

More information

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm Sleep, 2(3):299-307 1980 Raven Press, New York The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm L. C. Johnson Naval Health Research Center, San Diego, California Summary: Two studies, using data from fragmented

More information

Stage at Awakening, Sleep Inertia and Performance

Stage at Awakening, Sleep Inertia and Performance Sleep Research Online 5(3): 89-97, 2003 http://www.sro.org/2003/cavallero/89/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 2003 WebSciences Stage at Awakening, Sleep Inertia and Performance Corrado

More information

When are you too tired to be safe?

When are you too tired to be safe? When are you too tired to be safe? The development of a fatigue index tool Andrew Kilner EUROCONTROL The European Organisation for the Safety of Air Navigation Motivation Developing a fatigue index for

More information

COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE

COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE J. Snel 1, M.B. de Ruiter 1, J. Ruijter 1 and M.M. Lorist 2 1 Department of Psychonomics, Faculty of Psychology, University of Amsterdam 2 Faculty of Psychology, Rijks

More information

Sleep latency testing as a time course measure of state arousal

Sleep latency testing as a time course measure of state arousal J. Sleep Res. (2005) 14, 387 392 Sleep latency testing as a time course measure of state arousal MICHAEL H. BONNET 1,2,3 and DONNA L. ARAND 2,3 1 Dayton Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, 2

More information

Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation

Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation Sleep Research Online 2(4): 101-105, 1999 http://www.sro.org/1999/hodoba/101/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 1999 WebSciences Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation

More information

Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis

Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis J. Sleep Res. (1999) 8, 237 245 REVIEW Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis NANCY JO WESENSTEN, THOMAS J. BALKIN and GREGORY BELENKY Department of Neurobiology and Behavior,

More information

Andrew Tilley, Frank Donohoe, and Sharon Hensby. Department of Psychology, University of Queensland, Queensland, Australia

Andrew Tilley, Frank Donohoe, and Sharon Hensby. Department of Psychology, University of Queensland, Queensland, Australia Sleep 1(6):6-65, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1987 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Homeostatic Changes in Slow Wave Sleep during Sleep Following Nocturnal Sleep and Partial Slow Wave Sleep during

More information

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night?

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night? Sleep Research Online 2(1): 15-19, 1999 http://www.sro.org/1999/ferrara/15/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 1096-214X 1999 WebSciences Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound:

More information

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children QUESTION #1: ARE SLEEP ISSUES IN CHILDREN THE SAME AS IN ADULTS? Distinctive Features

More information

sleep latencies Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients the tendency to fall asleep

sleep latencies Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients the tendency to fall asleep Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients Sleepiness the tendency to fall asleep Elke De Valck, Raymond Cluydts, Sandra Pirrera Department of Cognitive and Physiological Psychology, Vrije Universiteit

More information

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children SLEEP DISORDERS Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children Distinctive Features of Pediatric Sleep Daytime sleepiness uncommon

More information

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle Sleep Sleep is described as a state of unconsciousness or partial consciousness from which a person can be roused by stimulation Period of rest and recovery People spend about a third of their lives sleeping

More information

Validation of Nighttime Sleepability Scale Against Objective and Subjective Measures of Sleep Quality

Validation of Nighttime Sleepability Scale Against Objective and Subjective Measures of Sleep Quality Sleep Hypn. 2018 Mar;20(1):25-30 http://dx.doi.org/10.5350/sleep.hypn.2016.18.0131 Sleep and Hypnosis A Journal of Clinical Neuroscience and Psychopathology ORIGINAL ARTICLE Validation of Nighttime Sleepability

More information

Benefits of napping in healthy adults: impact of nap length, time of day, age, and experience with napping

Benefits of napping in healthy adults: impact of nap length, time of day, age, and experience with napping J. Sleep Res. (2009) 18, 272 281 Benefits of napping doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00718.x Benefits of napping in healthy adults: impact of nap length, time of day, age, and experience with napping CATHERINE

More information

The Forty-Eight Hour Day

The Forty-Eight Hour Day Sleep, 1(2):191197 1978 Raven Press, New York The FortyEight Hour Day Wilse B. Webb Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida Summary: Four normal young adult male subjects

More information

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan Sleep 10(4):393-397, Raven Press, New York 1987, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Short Report Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan C. Guilleminault, S. Mondini,

More information

th Ave NE Suite F Bellevue, WA Phone: (425) Fax: (425) Excessive Daytime Sleepiness

th Ave NE Suite F Bellevue, WA Phone: (425) Fax: (425) Excessive Daytime Sleepiness 1414 116 th Ave NE Suite F Bellevue, WA 98004 Phone: (425) 451-8417 Fax: (425) 455-4089 Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Nearly everyone has days when they feel sleepy. But for some people, excessive sleepiness

More information

Fatigue and Circadian Rhythms

Fatigue and Circadian Rhythms 16.400/453J Human Factors Engineering Fatigue and Circadian Rhythms Caroline Lowenthal Lecture 19 1 16.400/453 Outline Situations where fatigue is a factor Effects of fatigue Sleep Components Circadian

More information

Effect of Sleep Disruption on Sleep, Performance, and Mood

Effect of Sleep Disruption on Sleep, Performance, and Mood Sleep, 8(1):11-19 1985 Raven Press, New York Effect of Sleep Disruption on Sleep, Performance, and Mood Michael H. Bonnet Jerry L. Pettis Memorial Veterans Administration Hospital, Loma Linda, California,

More information

The Efficacy of a Restart Break for Recycling with Optimal Performance Depends Critically on Circadian Timing

The Efficacy of a Restart Break for Recycling with Optimal Performance Depends Critically on Circadian Timing RECYCLING OPTIMAL PERFORMANCE DEPENDS ON CIRCADIAN TIMING DOI: 10.5665/SLEEP.1128 The Efficacy of a Restart Break for Recycling with Optimal Performance Depends Critically on Circadian Timing Hans P.A.

More information

Why are we so sleepy?

Why are we so sleepy? Why are we so sleepy? Focus Fall 2017 Conference 37 th Annual Michael Aiello Memorial Respiratory Care & Sleep Medicine Poughkeepsie, NY Dr. Steven A Thau MD Director, Pulmonary and Sleep Medicine Phelps

More information

EFFECTS OF NIGHTTIME NAPS ON BODY TEMPERATURE CHANGES, SLEEP PATTERNS, AND SELF-EVALUATION

EFFECTS OF NIGHTTIME NAPS ON BODY TEMPERATURE CHANGES, SLEEP PATTERNS, AND SELF-EVALUATION J. Human Ergol., 10:173-184, 1981 EFFECTS OF NIGHTTIME NAPS ON BODY TEMPERATURE CHANGES, SLEEP PATTERNS, AND SELF-EVALUATION OF SLEEP Kazuya MATSUMOTO Department of Hygiene, Kyorin University School of

More information

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology INSOMNIAS INSOMNIAS General criteria for insomnia A. Repeated difficulty with sleep initiation, duration, consolidation or quality. B. Adequate sleep opportunity, persistent sleep difficulty and associated

More information

Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH Professor of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Pediatrics Temple University Philadelphia, PA

Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH Professor of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Pediatrics Temple University Philadelphia, PA 37 th Annual Meeting Society of Behavioral Medicine The Impact of School Start Time Change on Adolescents Sleep, Health, Safety, and School Functioning Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH Professor of Epidemiology,

More information

Diurnal variations in the waking EEG: comparisons with sleep latencies and subjective alertness

Diurnal variations in the waking EEG: comparisons with sleep latencies and subjective alertness J. Sleep Res. (2000) 9, 243±248 Diurnal variations in the waking EEG: comparisons with sleep latencies and subjective alertness C. LAFRANCE and M. DUMONT Laboratoire de chronobiologie, Hoà pital du SacreÂ

More information

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11:

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11: Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11: 876-881. CLINICAL PEARL Managing the Effects of Shift Work in Medicine Holger Link, MD, and Robert Sack,

More information

T. Roth, Ph.D., T. Roehrs, Ph.D., G. Koshorek, J. Sicldost^, B.A., and

T. Roth, Ph.D., T. Roehrs, Ph.D., G. Koshorek, J. Sicldost^, B.A., and T. Roth, Ph.D., T. Roehrs, Ph.D., G. Koshorek, J. Sicldost^, B.A., and F. Zorick, M.D. Detroit, Mich. The central effects of a newly developed, long-acting H, antihistamine, loratadine (10 and ), were

More information

Managing Insomnia: an example sequence of CBT-based sessions for sleep treatment

Managing Insomnia: an example sequence of CBT-based sessions for sleep treatment Managing Insomnia: an example sequence of CBT-based sessions for sleep treatment Session 1: Introduction and sleep assessment -Assess sleep problem (option: have client complete 20-item sleep questionnaire).

More information

Article printed from

Article printed from What Are Sleep Disorders? Sleep disorders are conditions that affect how much and how well you sleep. The causes range from poor habits that keep you awake to medical problems that disrupt your sleep cycle.

More information

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association Sleep in patients with a number of sleep disorders and

More information

Teenagers: Sleep Patterns and School Performance

Teenagers: Sleep Patterns and School Performance The National Healthy Sleep Awareness Project involves a partnership between the American Academy of Sleep Medicine, Center for Disease Control and Sleep Research Society. The long term goal of the project

More information

Do You Get Enough Sleep?

Do You Get Enough Sleep? LP 3A sleep deprivation 1 Do You Get Enough Sleep? Many college students do not get enough sleep. In a survey of more than 200,000 first year students, more than 80% say that stayed up all night at least

More information

Sleep and Human Performance

Sleep and Human Performance Sleep and Human Performance Kevin Walker MD Scott Schauss PA-C Intermountain Sleep Disorders Clinic Disclosures: Objectives How sleep extension and restriction impact performance To Nap or Not to Nap Circadian

More information

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs?

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs? Sleep, 6(1): 10-\5 1983 Raven Press, New York What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs? Peter Rauri and Elaine Olmstead Dartmouth Medical School, Hanover, New Hampshire, U,S,A, Summary: Subjective

More information

An experimental study of adolescent sleep restriction during a simulated school week: changes in phase, sleep staging, performance and sleepiness

An experimental study of adolescent sleep restriction during a simulated school week: changes in phase, sleep staging, performance and sleepiness J Sleep Res. (2017) 26, 227 23 Adolescent sleep restriction and performance An experimental study of adolescent sleep restriction during a simulated school week: changes in phase, sleep staging, performance

More information

Sleep Science: better sleep for you and your patients CHUNBAI ZHANG, MD MPH UW MEDICINE VALLEY MEDICAL CENTER

Sleep Science: better sleep for you and your patients CHUNBAI ZHANG, MD MPH UW MEDICINE VALLEY MEDICAL CENTER Sleep Science: better sleep for you and your patients CHUNBAI ZHANG, MD MPH UW MEDICINE VALLEY MEDICAL CENTER Disclosure: Financial - none Non-Financial - none Selected Topics Sleep epidemiology Sleep

More information

Adaptation of performance during a week of simulated night work

Adaptation of performance during a week of simulated night work ERGONOMICS, 5FEBRUARY, 2004, VOL. 47, NO. 2, 154 165 Adaptation of performance during a week of simulated night work NICOLE LAMOND*, JILL DORRIAN, HELEH J. BURGESS, ALEX L. HOLMES, GREGORY D. ROACH, KIRSTY

More information

COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY

COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY Jeklin, A., Aguirre, A., Guttkuhn, R., Davis, W. Circadian Technologies Inc., Boston, United States Introduction Petrochemical

More information

Subjective sleepiness is a sensitive indicator of insufficient sleep and impaired waking function

Subjective sleepiness is a sensitive indicator of insufficient sleep and impaired waking function J Sleep Res. (2014) 23, 242 254 Subjective sleepiness Subjective sleepiness is a sensitive indicator of insufficient sleep and impaired waking function TORBJÖRN ÅKERSTEDT 1,2, ANNA ANUND 3, JOHN AXELSSON

More information

IN ITS ORIGINAL FORM, the Sleep/Wake Predictor

IN ITS ORIGINAL FORM, the Sleep/Wake Predictor Commentary on the Three-Process of Alertness and Broader ing Issues Jaques Reifman and Philippa Gander REIFMAN J, GANDER P. Commentary on the three-process model of alertness and broader modeling issues.

More information

YOU REALLY NEED TO SLEEP: Several methods to improve your sleep

YOU REALLY NEED TO SLEEP: Several methods to improve your sleep YOU REALLY NEED TO SLEEP: Several methods to improve your sleep Sleep is essential to our well-being. When humans fail to get good sleep over a period of time, numerous problems can occur. CAN T SLEEP!!

More information

I t is now well established that night work negatively

I t is now well established that night work negatively 1of9 ELECTRONIC PAPER The impact of a week of simulated night work on sleep, circadian phase, and performance N Lamond, J Dorrian, G D Roach, K McCulloch, A L Holmes, H J Burgess, A Fletcher, D Dawson...

More information

Facts. Sleepiness or Fatigue Causes the Following:

Facts. Sleepiness or Fatigue Causes the Following: www.drowsydriving.org Facts Sleepiness and driving is a dangerous combination. Most people are aware of the dangers of drinking and driving but don t realize that drowsy driving can be just as fatal. Like

More information

Arousal and Vigilance: Do They Differ? Study in a Sleep Inertia Paradigm

Arousal and Vigilance: Do They Differ? Study in a Sleep Inertia Paradigm Sleep Research Online 5(3): 83-87, 2003 http://www.sro.org/2003/tassi/83/ Printed in the USA. All rights reserved. 096-24X 2003 WebSciences Arousal and Vigilance: Do They Differ? Study in a Sleep Inertia

More information

Unit 3 Psychology ~ 2013

Unit 3 Psychology ~ 2013 Unit 3 Psychology ~ 2013 Sleep is described as regularly occurring altered state of consciousness that typically occurs spontaneously and is characterised by a loss of conscious awareness Sleep occurs

More information

Stage REM. Stage 3/4. Stage 2. Sleep 101. NREM vs. REM. Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Is Needed To: 9/24/2013

Stage REM. Stage 3/4. Stage 2. Sleep 101. NREM vs. REM. Circadian Rhythms. Sleep Is Needed To: 9/24/2013 The Power of Sleep: Supporting Healthy Sleep in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders REM Stage 1 TERRY KATZ, PHD UNIVERSITY OF COLORADO SCHOOL OF MEDICINE JFK PARTNERS CHILD DEVELOPMENT UNIT, CHILDREN

More information

The Consequences of a Week of Insomnia II: Patients with Insomnia

The Consequences of a Week of Insomnia II: Patients with Insomnia THE INSOMNIAS The Consequences of a Week of Insomnia II: Patients with Insomnia Michael H. Bonnet and Donna L. Arand Dayton Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Wright State University, and Kettering

More information

Section 53 FATIGUE MANAGEMENT

Section 53 FATIGUE MANAGEMENT 1. Purpose The purpose of this policy is to establish the requirements for managing fatigue. It is intended that this policy will reduce the risk of fatigue-related injuries and incidents in the workplace.

More information

Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia

Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia Rocky Garrison, PhD, CBSM Damon Michael Williams, RN, PMHNP-BC In House Counseling Laughing Heart LLC 10201 SE Main St. 12 SE 14 th Ave. Suite 10

More information

P08 Reversible loss of consciousness. E365 Aviation Human Factors

P08 Reversible loss of consciousness. E365 Aviation Human Factors P08 Reversible loss of consciousness E365 Aviation Human Factors Need to sleep Sleep is a natural state of rest for the body and mind that involves the reversible loss of consciousness. You sleep to not

More information

Multiple Naps and the Evaluation of Daytime Sleepiness in Patients with Upper Airway Sleep Apnea

Multiple Naps and the Evaluation of Daytime Sleepiness in Patients with Upper Airway Sleep Apnea Sleep. 3(3/4):425-439 1980 Raven Press. New York, Multiple Naps and the Evaluation of Daytime Sleepiness in Patients with Upper Airway Sleep Apnea T. Roth, K. M. Hartse, F. Zorick, and W. Conway Sleep

More information

Sleep Questionnaire. If yes, what? If yes, how would you describe it? Please explain? If yes, what times are these?

Sleep Questionnaire. If yes, what? If yes, how would you describe it? Please explain? If yes, what times are these? THE ADRENAL THYROID REVOLUTION Professional Mastermind Aviva Romm MD Sleep Questionnaire Sleep is important for musculoskeletal healing and for healthy immune function, mood, cognitive and brain function,

More information

Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical

Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical Director, Faulkner EEG and Sleep Testing Center Course

More information

EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE

EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE Shih-Bin Yeh 1 and Carlos Hugh Schenck 2,3 1 Department of Neurology

More information

Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness

Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness Sleep, 18(10):890-894 1995 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness

More information

Sleep in the NCANDA cohort

Sleep in the NCANDA cohort Sleep in the NCANDA cohort Fiona C. Baker, Ph.D. Center for Health Sciences, SRI International, California and Brain Function Research Group, University of the Witwatersrand, South Africa NCANDA FUNDING:

More information

Pharmacology and Sleep

Pharmacology and Sleep Sleep. 16(4):31-35 1993 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Pharmacology and Sleep Sedative Effects and Plasma Concentrations Following Single Doses of Triazolam, Diphenhydramine,

More information

Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison With Young Adults

Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison With Young Adults Sleep 20(4):294-300 1997 American Sleep Disorders Association and Sleep Research Society Sleep In Normal Subjects Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison

More information

Australian Centre for Education in Sleep (ACES)

Australian Centre for Education in Sleep (ACES) Australian Centre for Education in Sleep (ACES) High School workbook 1 Table of Contents for High School student workbook Topic Page 1. Introduction Dear Student 1 2. Top five reasons why you need to sleep

More information

Automobile Accidents in Patients with Sleep Disorders

Automobile Accidents in Patients with Sleep Disorders Sleep 12(6):487-494, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 1989 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Automobile Accidents in Patients with Sleep Disorders Michael S. Aldrich Department of Neurology, University

More information

Clinical Trial Synopsis TL , NCT#

Clinical Trial Synopsis TL , NCT# Clinical Trial Synopsis, NCT#00492011 Title of Study: A Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled, Parallel-Group Study to Evaluate the Ability of Ramelteon 1 mg, 4 mg, and 8 mg to Alleviate the Insomnia

More information

Fatigue Management. Sample Only

Fatigue Management. Sample Only Fatigue Management Sample Only Reference CPL_PCR_Fatigue_Management Revision Number SAMPLE ONLY Document Owner Sample Only Date 2015 File Location Procedure Revision Date Major Change Description Reviewed

More information

STOP! REVIVE! SURVIVE!

STOP! REVIVE! SURVIVE! STOP! REVIVE! SURVIVE! DRIVER FATIGUE: AN AWARENESS SESSION FOR DRIVERS AND DRIVER SUPERVISORS SUPPORTING OUR DRIVE FOR GOAL ZERO 1 Globally, it is estimated that up to 20% of all road traffic fatalities

More information

Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance

Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance Clin Sports Med 24 (2005) 237 249 Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance Hans P.A. Van Dongen, PhD*, David F. Dinges, PhD Division of Sleep and Chronobiology, Department of Psychiatry, University

More information

The Effect of Varying Prophylactic Naps on Performance, Alertness and Mood throughout a 52-Hour Continuous Operation

The Effect of Varying Prophylactic Naps on Performance, Alertness and Mood throughout a 52-Hour Continuous Operation Sleep, 14(4):307-315 1991 Association of Professional Sleep Societies The Effect of Varying Prophylactic Naps on Performance, Alertness and Mood throughout a 52-Hour Continuous Operation M. H. Bonnet Veterans

More information

Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed. NAPNAP National Conference 2018

Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed. NAPNAP National Conference 2018 Medications that are not FDA approved for children will be discussed NAPNAP National Conference 2018 (Honaker & Meltzer, 2016; Keyes, Maslowsky, Hamilton & Schulenberg, 2015) Chronically disrupted sleep

More information

Sleep and Dreams UNIT 5- RG 5A

Sleep and Dreams UNIT 5- RG 5A Sleep and Dreams UNIT 5- RG 5A Goals for today Can you Discuss the circadian rhythm, what it is and how it effects us. Identify and explain each of the 5 stages of sleep. As well as the typical waves of

More information

Comparing performance on a simulated 12 hour shift rotation in young and older subjects

Comparing performance on a simulated 12 hour shift rotation in young and older subjects 58 Occup Environ Med 2001;58:58 62 Comparing performance on a simulated 12 hour shift rotation in young and older subjects K Reid, D Dawson Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, The University of Adelaide,

More information

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AG Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AG Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology Biological Psychology Unit Two AG Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology Consciousness Consciousness is your awareness of how and why you react to your surroundings. During this lesson, you may realize

More information

Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Sleep Medicine. journal homepage:

Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Sleep Medicine. journal homepage: Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 72 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Sleep Medicine journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/sleep Original Article Subjective and objective sleepiness among

More information

Age and adjustment to night work

Age and adjustment to night work 568 Institute of Occupational Health, Department of Physiology, Helsinki M Harma T Hakola Department of Stress Research, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm T Akerstedt Department of Physiology, Kuopio University,

More information

Sleep and Ageing. Siobhan Banks PhD. Body and Brain at Work, Centre for Sleep Research University of South Australia

Sleep and Ageing. Siobhan Banks PhD. Body and Brain at Work, Centre for Sleep Research University of South Australia Sleep and Ageing Siobhan Banks PhD Body and Brain at Work, Centre for Sleep Research University of South Australia Health and Active Ageing, 22 nd September 2015 Sleep and Aging How does sleep change as

More information

Contents. Page. Can t sleep 3. Insomnia 4. Sleep 5. How long should we sleep? 8. Sleep problems 9. Getting a better night s sleep 11

Contents. Page. Can t sleep 3. Insomnia 4. Sleep 5. How long should we sleep? 8. Sleep problems 9. Getting a better night s sleep 11 I Cannot Sleep Contents Page Can t sleep 3 Insomnia 4 Sleep 5 How long should we sleep? 8 Sleep problems 9 Getting a better night s sleep 11 Treatment for insomnia 15 For more information 19 2 Can t sleep

More information

Chapter Five. Sleep McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved.

Chapter Five. Sleep McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. Chapter Five Sleep 2011 McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. Endocrine System Made up of ductless glands that produce hormones Hormones control various body functions/processes Hormones are

More information

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch Dr Alex Bartle Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch 11:00-11:55 WS #113: Circadian Sleep Disorders 12:05-13:00 WS #125: Circadian Sleep Disorders (Repeated) Overview The Structure of Sleep

More information

Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): A Standard Measure of Sleepiness *

Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): A Standard Measure of Sleepiness * Sleep 9(4):519-524, Raven Press, New York 1986, Association of Professional Sleep Societies Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): A Standard Measure of Sleepiness * Chairman: Mary A. Carskadon

More information

Driver Sleepiness Assessed by Electroencephalography - Different Methods Applied to One Single Data Set

Driver Sleepiness Assessed by Electroencephalography - Different Methods Applied to One Single Data Set University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2015 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 25th, 12:00 AM Driver Sleepiness Assessed by Electroencephalography - Different Methods Applied

More information

Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours

Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours Halszka OGINSKA Department of Ergonomics, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland Abstract:

More information

RESEARCH REPORT 446. The development of a fatigue / risk index for shiftworkers HSE

RESEARCH REPORT 446. The development of a fatigue / risk index for shiftworkers HSE HSE Health & Safety Executive The development of a fatigue / risk index for shiftworkers Prepared by QinetiQ Centre for Human Sciences & Simon Folkard Associates Limited for the Health and Safety Executive

More information

A Good Night s Sleep Participant s Guide

A Good Night s Sleep Participant s Guide FCD 08-002 September 2007 A Good Night s Sleep Participant s Guide "Sleep is better than medicine." --- English Proverb "The beginning of health is sleep." --- Irish Proverb "Disease and sleep keep far

More information

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms

Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms Sleep, Dreaming and Circadian Rhythms People typically sleep about 8 hours per day, and spend 16 hours awake. Most people sleep over 175,000 hours in their lifetime. The vast amount of time spent sleeping

More information