biij Effects of single-trial averaging on spatial extent of brain activation detected by fmri are subject and task dependent

Similar documents
Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

The effects of single-trial averaging upon the spatial extent of fmri activation

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging with Arterial Spin Labeling: Techniques and Potential Clinical and Research Applications

Visualization strategies for major white matter tracts identified by diffusion tensor imaging for intraoperative use

The BOLD fmri refractory effect is specific to stimulus attributes: evidence from a visual motion paradigm

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala. California Institute of Technology

Does stimulus quality affect the physiologic MRI responses to brief visual activation?

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Evidence for a Refractory Period in the Hemodynamic Response to Visual Stimuli as Measured by MRI

Visual cortical activations on fmri upon stimulation of the vision-implicated acupoints

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Stroke Patients After Upper Limb Robot-Assisted Therapy: A Pilot Study

Supplementary Online Content

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

Daniel Bulte. Centre for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain. University of Oxford

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Investigation and Validation of Intersite fmri Studies Using the Same Imaging Hardware

APPLICATIONS OF ASL IN NEUROSCIENCE

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease

The hippocampus operates in a threshold manner during spatial source memory Scott D. Slotnick a and Preston P. Thakral b

Impact of Signal-to-Noise on Functional MRI

Multiresolution Data Acquisition and Detection in Functional MRI

An Unified Approach for fmri-measurements used by a New Real-Time fmri Analysis System

P2 Visual - Perception

PHYSICS OF MRI ACQUISITION. Alternatives to BOLD for fmri

Stimulus Repetition and Hemodynamic Response Refractoriness in Event-Related fmri

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

Experimental design. Experimental design. Experimental design. Guido van Wingen Department of Psychiatry Academic Medical Center

Titelmaster The physics of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri)

Effects Of Attention And Perceptual Uncertainty On Cerebellar Activity During Visual Motion Perception

Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy

BRAIN STATE CHANGE DETECTION VIA FIBER-CENTERED FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY ANALYSIS

INTRO TO BOLD FMRI FRANZ JOSEPH GALL ( ) OUTLINE. MRI & Fast MRI Observations Models Statistical Detection

Advances in Clinical Neuroimaging

Hierarchical clustering analysis for visual-movement response functional MRI data processing

Supplementary Information

The Critical Relationship between the Timing of Stimulus Presentation and Data Acquisition in Blocked Designs with fmri

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

Brain, language, and handedness: a family affair

Word Length Processing via Region-to-Region Connectivity

Supplementary Online Content

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION In format provided by Frank et al. (JULY 2010)

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

Source Memory is not Properly Differentiated from Object Memory in Early Schizophrenia An fmri study using virtual reality

Neuroimaging vs. other methods

The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy

Supporting Information. Demonstration of effort-discounting in dlpfc

Brain gray matter volume changes associated with motor symptoms in patients with Parkinson s disease

MATERIALS AND METHODS

ARTICLE IN PRESS. St. Louis, MO 63130, USA d Department of Radiology, Washington University, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA

Hemodynamic responses in visual, motor, and somatosensory cortices in schizophrenia

Combining microstimulation and fmri in an awake behaving monkey

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview. Functional connectivity. Scott Peltier. FMRI Laboratory University of Michigan

fmri Evidence for Modality-Specific Processing of Conceptual Knowledge on Six Modalities

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

Temporal Sensitivity of Event-Related fmri

Author's personal copy

Curriculum Vitae. Wan Yu Hsu EDUCATION ACADEMIC APPOINTMENTS RESEARCH EXPERIENCE. Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco,

Supplementary Note Psychophysics:

Experimental Assessment of Infarct Lesion Growth in Mice using Time-Resolved T2* MR Image Sequences

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

Supporting Online Material for

Final Report 2017 Authors: Affiliations: Title of Project: Background:

Perceptual Learning of Motion Direction Discrimination with Suppressed and Unsuppressed MT in Humans: An fmri Study

fmri of Visual Encoding: Reproducibility of Activation

Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning

Perfusion-Based fmri. Thomas T. Liu Center for Functional MRI University of California San Diego May 7, Goal

Pattern Recognition of Functional Neuroimage Data of the Human Sensorimotor System after Stroke

FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE EVIDENCE OF CORTICAL ALTERATIONS IN A CASE OF REVERSIBLE CONGENITAL LYMPHEDEMA OF THE LOWER LIMB: A PILOT STUDY

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

FREQUENCY DOMAIN HYBRID INDEPENDENT COMPONENT ANALYSIS OF FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING DATA

Supplemental Information

Activated Fibers: Fiber-centered Activation Detection in Task-based FMRI

Supporting Information

Impact of Early Deafness and Early Exposure to Sign Language on the Cerebral Organization for Motion Processing

Normal and Abnormal Cerebrospinal Fluid Dynamics Evaluated by Optimized Cine Phase-contrast MR Imaging

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Human Visual Cortex during Face Matching: A Comparison with Positron Emission Tomography

Prediction of Successful Memory Encoding from fmri Data

Reproducibility is necessary but insufficient for addressing brain science issues.

Resting-State functional Connectivity MRI (fcmri) NeuroImaging

International Journal of Innovative Research in Advanced Engineering (IJIRAE) Volume 1 Issue 10 (November 2014)

Functional Connectivity in the Motor Cortex of Resting Human Brain Using Echo-Planar MRI

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri

Transcription:

Available online at http://www.biij.org/2006/3/e27 doi: 10.2349/biij.2.3.e27 biij Biomedical Imaging and Intervention Journal TECHNICAL NOTE Effects of single-trial averaging on spatial extent of brain activation detected by fmri are subject and task dependent SY Fang 1,2, MS, JJ Wang 1,3, PhD, YY Hsu 4, MD, YL Wan 1,3, MD, YY Wai 1,3, MD, HL Liu*,1,3, PhD, DABR 1 MRI Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taoyuan, Taiwan; 2 Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Connecticut, USA; 3 Department of Medical Imaging and Radiological Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan; 4 Department of Medical Imaging, Buddhist Tzu Chi General Hospital, Taipei, Taiwan Received 19 January 2006; received in revised form 12 April 2006; accepted 20 April 2006 ABSTRACT Aim: The effects of single-trial averaging on the spatial extent of event-related fmri activation may vary between subjects and tasks. The purpose of this study was to evaluate this variability using a visual task and a word generation task. Patients, materials, and methods: Five Chinese right-handed male volunteers participated in the experiment. Experiments were conducted using a 1.5 T clinical MRI scanner with a T2*-weighted single-shot gradient-echo EPI sequence. Each task contained 150 trials that were separated into 5 runs. For each voxel, time courses averaged across different numbers of randomly selected trials, were obtained. They were applied for determining the voxels with significant activations, using a students t-test (p<0.001, uncorrected). Results: Consistent with previous findings, the number of the activated voxels increased monotonically with the number of trials combined. The ascending rate and the maximum number of the activated voxels were different, however, between tasks and among subjects. Conclusions: The effects of single-trial averaging were found to vary significantly between tasks and subjects. Therefore, we strongly advise to carefully consider such variability when using the spatial extent of activation as a measure in a group or a task comparison. 2006 Biomedical Imaging and Intervention Journal. All rights reserved. Keywords: Spatial extent; event-related fmri; trial averaging INTRODUCTION * Corresponding author. Present address: MRI Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, 5 Fuhsing Street, Kweishan, Taoyuan, 333, Taiwan. Tel: +886-3-3281200 ext. 8407; Fax: +886-3-3275798; E-mail: hlaliu@mail.cgu.edu.tw (Ho-Ling Liu). The spatial extent of the activation is a critical measure in many fmri studies [1-3], and the differences may be interpreted as hypoactive or dysfunctional. In addition, the size of the spatial extent can be used to reduce the detection of the false activation [4]. The true

SY Fang et al. Biomed Imaging Interv J 2006; 2(3):e27 2 Figure 1 The number of activated voxels plotted against the number of trials combined from all subjects. regions of activation tend to occur over contiguous voxels, where noise showed much less tendency to form clusters [5]. Few studies have, however, directly addressed those issues related to the spatial extent of the BOLD response. One previous study found that an exponential relationship existed between the number of trials and the spatial extent. Over 100 trials were averaged in this event-related study, using a visual stimulation task [6]. Saad et al. also reported the spatial extent increased monotonically with trial averaging in a block-designed visual task [7]. Recently, event-related fmri has been widely applied in the exploration of human cognitive process [8]. The shape of the hemodynamic response function, however, varies between areas in the visual and the motor cortices even in the same subject [9]. Therefore, the effect on the spatial extents in averaged trials would remain unclear in different brain areas using different stimuli, such as, cognitive tasks. The optimised number of trials for the steady-state response in a cognitive experiment using event-related fmri design requires further investigation. In this study, a cognitive task using word generation [10] was designed to examine the effects of single-trial averaging. The result was compared with that from a visual task. Different effects between both tasks will be discussed and an optimised value of averaging trials will be considered for the future applications. It should be noted that the spatial extents detected by fmri can be interpreted as involvement of neural activity only if the steady-state condition in single-trial averaging is reached, i.e., independent on contrast-to-noise ratio (CNR). Therefore, the current study is crucial for the experiments studying graded activations, as well as clinical fmri where CNR of the hemodynamic responses in patients can be quite different. MATERIALS AND METHODS The experiments were conducted at the MRI Centre, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, LinKou, Taiwan. Five male volunteers participated in this study. They were aged 20-26 years, with a mean age of 22.4 years. They were native Chinese speakers. The participants had no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders and reported to be right handed, which was confirmed with the Handiness inventory [11]. A written, informed consent was obtained in all cases. Neural activation was monitored by a 1.5 T Magnetom Vision MRI scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a single-shot gradient-echo EPI sequence (TR / TE /FA = 1000 ms / 60 ms / 90, Slice Thickness =

SY Fang et al. Biomed Imaging Interv J 2006; 2(3):e27 3 Table 1 The number of total expected activated voxels, V max, and the ascending rate, ξ, from all subjects in both visual stimulation and work generation experiments. Subject Visual stimulation Word generation V max ξ V max ξ 1 92.25 126.38 56.32 69.51 2 120.51 104.52 41.32 32.65 3 25.30 38.74 57.39 122.85 4 15.89 32.17 34.58 52.07 5 64.32 95.68 30.23 83.47 8 mm, in-plane resolution = 3.3 mm). The stimuli were shown through a goggle display system (Resonance Technology Inc., CA, USA). Corrective lenses were used when necessary. Prior to the MR imaging, each subject was visually familiarised with the procedures and the experimental conditions, to minimise anxiety and enhance task performance. The subject, before being transferred to the scanner, was fitted with a plastic earmuff and a tightly fitting, thermally moulded, plastic facial mask that extended from the hairline to the chin. Subjects participated in both event-related fmri experiments, a visual stimulus and a word generation task, respectively, on two separate days. This was done to reduce subject discomfort in a prolonged scanning session. The time span between both experiments was set at one week. The experiment consisted of five separate runs of total 150 trials. Each run was made of 460 s, starting with 10-s dummy scans and followed by 30 trials (0.5 s stimulus + 14.5 s fixation). The visual task was a black and white circular checkerboard flashing at 8Hz (Figure 2a). Seven contiguous slices were acquired parallel to the calcarine sulcus. In the word generation task a different, single Chinese character was displayed each time (Figure 2b), and the subject was asked to silently associate it with a semantically related word. All characters used were sampled from the Mandarin Promotion Council (Ministry of Education, Taiwan), with a frequency ranging between 15 and 25 per 10 million occurrences. Seven contiguous slices were acquired parallel to the AC-PC line. The data analyses for each subject were performed within an anatomically defined region of interest (ROI) within the middle slice. ROIs were chosen, based on the anatomical images, in the gray matter along the calcarine fissure in the primary visual cortex in visual tasks or at BA10, 44, 45, and 47 in word generation tasks [12]. All analyses were conducted in Matlab (The Math Works, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). The time-serial data were normalised to the averaged signal intensity of the 1st, 2nd, and 15th time points within each single trial. The single trial populations were sampled randomly. A series of averaged signals, varying in the number of trials combined, were then computed for each subject. Twenty averaged signals were computed for each possible number of trials combined. For each averaged signal, a Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient was calculated between the mean time course and a gamma variate function. Activated voxels were determined using a student s t-test (t >3.8, p < 0.001, uncorrected). The number of voxels that exceeded the threshold was determined for each of the twenty averaged signals, at each number of combined trials for each subject. RESULTS Figure 1 shows, from all five subjects, the number of the activated voxels plotted against the number of trials combined. Consistent with previous studies, the volume of activation increased with the number of trials combined in the visual task as well as the word generation task, in all subjects in this experiment. The number of activated voxels at x trials combined, V x, was then fitted with an exponential function: V x = V max [ 1 exp( x / ξ )] (1) where V max represented the number of total expected activated voxels and ξ, the ascending rate, equalled to the number of trials combined when V x is equal to 63% of V max. Table 1 showed the V max and ξ from all subjects in both experiments, respectively. The ascending rate and the number of total expected activated voxels varied significantly between tasks and among subjects. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION Our findings are consistent with the observations of Huettel and McCarthy [6]. The spatial extent of the fmri activation in a visual task is influenced by the number of trials combined. Similar results were observed in a word generation task, which indicated that the spatial extent

SY Fang et al. Biomed Imaging Interv J 2006; 2(3):e27 4 (a) Figure 2 An example of the slide used in the a) visual task; and b) word generation task. (b) increased with the number of trials combined in an event-related fmri experiment; despite the different tasks performed or brain areas studied. Figure 1 shows that the number of the activated voxels did not reach the plateau even after averaging 150 trials. Table 1 shows that the maximum number of the detected active voxels is, however, close to V max in each subject. This suggests that most of the activated voxels were identified and is consistent with the report of Huettel and MaCarthy [6]. Our experiment showed that the effects of singletrial averaging on the spatial extent of an event-related fmri study may vary between tasks and among subjects. The spatial extent can be affected by the amplitude of the hemodynamic response [13] or the voxel-wise noise. Our results indicate that such effects should be taken into consideration in experiments using different tasks and involving different brain areas or in a comparative study on subjects. Similar findings were reported by Duann et al. [14], who demonstrated that the hemodynamic response varied substantially across trials as well as sessions, subjects, and brain areas. Purdon et al. [15] concluded that there exists a wide range of noise variances in the BOLD signal within and between subjects, which could affect the determination of the spatial extent. The wide range of noise variances might have led to the different ξ between tasks and subjects in our experiment. An optimised number of trials have to be carefully considered in future applications, to reduce the noise. The variances in V max in this study can be explained by the between-subjects reproducibility effect as was reported by the previous studies [16, 17]. From the current study and the within-subject reproducibility of the spatial extent [16, 18-21], the use of the spatial extent of the activation as an index to compare the results from different groups and/or tasks could introduce errors. Furthermore, the use of spatial extent as a measure of activation could be a source of variance in itself [22]. To conclude, ξ, the ascending rate of the detected activation volume and V max, the number of total expected activated voxels vary across different tasks and subjects when increasing number of trials are combined. This may be due to the complex contrast and noise characteristics from the various effects in the hemodynamic changes in different task performances, brain areas, and other subject-dependent physiological parameters. Special precautions should be taken in fmri studies, when using detected spatial extent as an index for quantitative comparisons. REFERENCES 1. Meador KJ, Allison JD, Loring DW, et al. Topography of somatosensory processing: cerebral lateralization and focused attention. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8(3):349-59. 2. Wang Y, Sereno JA, Jongman A, et al. fmri evidence for cortical modification during learning of Mandarin lexical tone. J Cogn Neurosci 2003;15(7):1019-27. 3. Vannini P, Almkvist O, Franck A, et al. Task demand modulations of visuospatial processing measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 2004;21(1):58-68. 4. Friston KJ, Worsley KJ, Frackowiak RSJ, et al. Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent. Hum Brain Mapp 1994;1:214-20. 5. Roland PE, Levin B, Kawashima R, et al. Three-Dimensional analysis of clustered voxels in 15O-Butanol brain activation images. Hum Brain Mapp 1993;1:3-19. 6. Huettel SA, McCarthy G. The effects of single-trial averaging upon the spatial extent of fmri activation. Neuroreport 2001;12(11):2411-6. 7. Saad ZS, Ropella KM, DeYoe EA, et al. The spatial extent of the BOLD response. Neuroimage 2003;19(1):132-44. 8. Buckner RL, Bandettini PA, O'Craven KM, et al. Detection of cortical activation during averaged single trials of a cognitive task

SY Fang et al. Biomed Imaging Interv J 2006; 2(3):e27 5 using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1996;93(25):14878-83. 9. Miezin FM, Maccotta L, Ollinger JM, et al. Characterizing the hemodynamic response: effects of presentation rate, sampling procedure, and the possibility of ordering brain activity based on relative timing. Neuroimage 2000;11(6 Pt 1):735-59. 10. Tan LH, Spinks JA, Gao JH, et al. Brain activation in the processing of Chinese characters and words: a functional MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 2000;10(1):16-27. 11. Snyder PJ, Harris LJ. Handedness, sex, and familial sinistrality effects on spatial tasks. Cortex 1993;29(1):115-34. 12. Tan LH, Liu HL, Perfetti CA, et al. The neural system underlying Chinese logograph reading. Neuroimage 2001;13(5):836-46. 13. Carpenter PA, Just MA, Keller TA, et al. Time course of fmriactivation in language and spatial networks during sentence comprehension. Neuroimage 1999;10(2):216-24. 14. Duann JR, Jung TP, Kuo WJ, et al. Single-trial variability in eventrelated BOLD signals. Neuroimage 2002;15(4):823-35. 15. Purden PL, Solo V, Brown E, et al. fmri noise variability across subjects and trials: Insights for noise estimation methods. Neuroimage 1998;7:s617. 16. Machielsen WC, Rombouts SA, Barkhof F, et al. FMRI of visual encoding: reproducibility of activation. Hum Brain Mapp 2000;9(3):156-64. 17. Vandenbroucke MW, Goekoop R, Duschek EJ, et al. Interindividual differences of medial temporal lobe activation during encoding in an elderly population studied by fmri. Neuroimage 2004;21(1):173-80. 18. Noll DC, Genovese CR, Nystrom LE, et al. Estimating test-retest reliability in functional MR imaging. II: Application to motor and cognitive activation studies. Magn Reson Med 1997;38(3):508-17. 19. Rutten GJ, Ramsey NF, van Rijen PC, et al. Reproducibility of fmri-determined language lateralization in individual subjects. Brain Lang 2002;80(3):421-37. 20. Chee MW, Lee HL, Soon CS, et al. Reproducibility of the word frequency effect: comparison of signal change and voxel counting. Neuroimage 2003;18(2):468-82. 21. Neumann J, Lohmann G, Zysset S, et al. Within-subject variability of BOLD response dynamics. Neuroimage 2003;19(3):784-96. 22. Cohen MS, DuBois RM. Stability, repeatability, and the expression of signal magnitude in functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Magn Reson Imaging 1999;10(1):33-40.