Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento. Brasil

Similar documents
Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento. Brasil

The abbreviated form of the Brief Cognitive Battery in the diagnosis of dementia in Alzheimer s disease

Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) Normative data for the Brazilian middle-age and elderly populations

Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento. Brasil

Translation, cultural adaptation and psychometric evaluation of the Leganés cognitive test in a low educated elderly Brazilian population

COMPARISON BETWEEN NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATION INSTRUMENTS FOR SEVERE DEMENTIA

Brief cognitive battery in the diagnosis of mild Alzheimer s disease in subjects with medium and high levels of education

The relation of education and gender on the attention items of the Mini-Mental State Examination in Spanish speaking Hispanic elders

Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento. Brasil

A semantic verbal fluency test for English- and Spanish-speaking older Mexican-Americans

VALIDATION OF THE BRAZILIAN VERSION OF MINI-TEST CASI-S

Cambridge Cognitive Examination: performance of healthy elderly Brazilians with low education levels

Applicability of a cognitive questionnaire in the elderly and proxy

CHARACTERISTICS OF PATIENTS ASSISTED AT AN AMBULATORY OF DEMENTIA FROM A UNIVERSITY HOSPITAL

Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento. Brasil

Phonemic verbal fluency and age

Diagnosis of mild dementia in Alzheimer s Disease

Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento. Brasil

EXIT25 Executive interview applied to a cognitively healthy elderly population with heterogeneous educational background

Correlation between neuropsychiatric symptoms and caregiver burden in a population-based sample from São Paulo, Brazil

Normative data of Fuld Object Memory Evaluation test for brazilian elderly population

Screening of cognitive impairment by general internists using two simple instruments

Short IQCODE as a screening tool for MCI and dementia

Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento. Brasil

Age and educational level effects on the performance of normal elderly on category verbal fluency tasks

Ann Acad Med Singapore 2013;42:315-9 Key words: Cognitive impairment, Dementia, SPMSQ, Validation

The effect of aging on cognitive function in a South Indian Population

Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination- Revised is accurate for detecting dementia in Parkinson s disease patients with low educational level

A normative study of the CERAD neuropsychological assessment battery in the Korean elderly

The influence of schooling on working memory performance in elderly individuals without cognitive decline

Use of Days of the Week in a Modified Mini-Mental State Exam (M-MMSE) for Detecting Geriatric Cognitive Impairment

Original Article. Diane da Costa Miranda 1, Sonia Maria Dozzi Brucki 1, Mônica Sanches Yassuda 1

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Detecting Dementia With the Mini-Mental State Examination in Highly Educated Individuals

UDS Progress Report. -Standardization and Training Meeting 11/18/05, Chicago. -Data Managers Meeting 1/20/06, Chicago

Education does not equally influence all the Mini Mental State Examination subscales and items: inferences from a Brazilian community sample

Education does not equally influence all the Mini Mental State Examination subscales and items: inferences from a Brazilian community sample

Diversity and Dementia

Diabetes mellitus (DM) is a chronic disease. The influence of diabetes mellitus II on cognitive performance. Original Article INTRODUCTION

Geriatric Grand Rounds

Worries about memory loss and knowledge on Alzheimer s disease in community-dwelling elderly from Brazil

José R. Wajman, Fabricio F. Oliveira, Sheilla M. C. Marin, Rodrigo R. Schultz, Paulo H. F. Bertolucci

Normative data for the Brief Cognitive Screening Battery stratified by age and education

Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento. Brasil

Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento. Brasil

Functional assessment scales in detecting dementia

Comparison of clock drawing with Mini Mental State Examination as a screening test in elderly acute hospital admissions

NEUROPSYCHOMETRIC TESTS

Prospective and retrospective memory in mild Alzheimer s disease

Educational differentials on life expectancy with and without cognitive impairment in Brazil

Strategies to disseminate and implement Screening of Harmful use of Alcohol and Brief Intervention in PHC services in Juiz de Fora city (Brazil).

The impact of cognitive performance on quality of life in individuals with Parkinson s disease

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

This PDF is available for free download from a site hosted by Medknow Publications

Who discovers the cutaneous melanoma * Quem descobre o melanoma cutâneo *

Diagnostic criteria for dementia and Alzheimer disease

ORIGINAL ARTICLE Neuroscience INTRODUCTION MATERIALS AND METHODS

The Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) in the diagnosis of vascular dementia

Mini Mental State Examination and the Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination: Effect of education and norms for a multicultural population

Barbara Spenciere 1, Liana Chaves Mendes-Santos 1, Christina Borges-Lima 1, Helenice Charchat-Fichman 1

The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scale, a global

Article. Erich Belzunces dos Santos 1, Ivanda de Souza Silva Tudesco 1,2, Luis Otávio S.F. Caboclo 1, Elza Márcia T. Yacubian 1

Cognitive Reserve and the Relationship Between Depressive Symptoms and Awareness of Deficits in Dementia

Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston

Mauricio de Miranda Ventura 1, Antonio Carlos de Paiva Melo 2, Henrique Carrete Jr 3, Ricardo Vieira Botelho 4

Study on Level of Depression among Elderly Residing in an Old Age. Home in Hyderabad, Telangana

The Mini Mental S HOW TO DO IT

Post-Mortem diagnosis of dementia by informant interview

Validation of geriatric depression scale in a general outpatient clinic

Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised: Norms for Elderly African Americans

MINI-MENTAL STATE EXA MINATION (M MSE)

CROSS-CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF THE QUALITY OF LIFE ASSESSMENT SCALE ON ALZHEIMER DISEASE

Neuropsychiatric symptoms as the main determinant of caregiver burden in Alzheimer s disease

Partners in Care: A Model of Social Work in Primary Care

SCREENING FOR COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT IN PRIMARY CARE: Is screening or case finding necessary at the primary care level?

THE Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), first introduced

MODIFIED INFORMANT QUESTIONNAIRE ON COGNITIVE DECLINE IN THE ELDERLY (IQCODE) AS A SCREENING TEST FOR DEMENTIA FOR THAI ELDERLY

Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument, Chinese Version 2.0 (CASI C-2.0): Administration and Clinical Application

Recognizing Dementia can be Tricky

Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento. Brasil

Planning sample size for impact evaluations

Brasil. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, vol. 9, núm. 3, septiembre, 2015, pp São Paulo, Brasil

Restrictions in participation and mental state in new hearing aids users

REV. HOSP. CLÍN. FAC. MED. S. PAULO 56(6): , 2001 ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Impaired abstract thinking may discriminate between normal aging and vascular mild cognitive impairment

Changing Patient Base. A Knowledge to Practice Program

Features associated with cognitive impairment and dementia in a community-based sample of illiterate elderly aged 75+ years

Brazilian Journal of OTORHINOLARYNGOLOGY. Association between language development and auditory processing disorders

THE ROLE OF ACTIVITIES OF DAILY LIVING IN THE MCI SYNDROME

Normative data for a brief neuropsychological battery administered to English- and Spanish-speaking community-dwelling elders

Knowledge about Alzheimer s disease in the Brazilian population

January 18 th, 2018 Brixen, Italy

Trail Making Test part A part B. Mild Cognitive Impairment MCI MCI. Kennedy 1981 Bornstein Rey Osterrieth 3. Berry 1991.

Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria Psychiatry

Depression in the elderly: are doctors investigating it?

Alessandro Ferrari Jacinto 1, Paulo José Fortes Villas Boas 1, Vânia Ferreira de Sá Mayoral 2, Vanessa de Albuquerque Citero 3

e Wolf-Klein, e um quinto pesquisador repetiu o método de Manos para o estudo de confiabilidade inter-aferidor. Resultados: 211

Validity and reliability of a simplified version of the Taylor Complex Figure Test for the assessment of older adults with low formal education

Prevalence of depressive symptoms among elderly in the city of Tremembé, Brazil

Transcription:

Dementia & Neuropsychologia ISSN: 1980-5764 demneuropsy@uol.com.br Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento Brasil Dozzi Brucki, Sonia Maria; Nitrini, Ricardo Mini-Mental State Examination among lower educational levels and illiterates. Transcultural evaluation Dementia & Neuropsychologia, vol. 4, núm. 2, abril-junio, 2010, pp. 120-125 Associação Neurologia Cognitiva e do Comportamento São Paulo, Brasil Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=339529017008 How to cite Complete issue More information about this article Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Scientific Information System Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative

Original Article Mini-Mental State Examination among lower educational levels and illiterates Transcultural evaluation Sonia Maria Dozzi Brucki 1, Ricardo Nitrini 1 Abstract Cognitive performance among illiterates and low educational levels is poorer than that observed in individuals with greater schooling. This difference can be a confounding factor in reaching an accurate diagnosis of cognitive impairment. In addition, there is great heterogeneity in performance among illiterates, probably due to different environmental demands and sociocultural backgrounds. Many reports have described the influence of education on neuropsychological measures and screening tests such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Objectives: To analyze performance in two samples with the same educational level, but different social and cultural backgrounds. Methods: Subjects from two different locations in Brazil (rural sample from Northern region and urban sample residing in the largest city of the Southeastern region) were matched for age and education, and submitted to the MMSE. Results: Significant differences between the groups were found in total scores on the MMSE and in temporal orientation and serial-sevens sub-items for which the urban sample performed best but analysis of illiterates alone yielded the same results, except for the copying pentagons task which was performed better by the rural sample. Conclusions: Cultural and social backgrounds, as well as demands from the environment, influence results of screening tests. Factors other than education must be taken into account when analyzing tests. Key words: Mini-Mental State Examination, illiteracy, educational level, screening test. Mini-Exame do Estado Mental na baixa escolaridade e em analfabetos: avaliação transcultural Resumo O desempenho cognitivo entre iletrados e indivíduos com baixos níveis de escolaridade é pior do que observado entre indivíduos bem escolarizados. Esta diferença pode ser um fator de confusão quando um diagnóstico preciso de comprometimento cognitivo se faz necessário. Além disto, há uma grande heterogeneidade no desempenho destas pessoas, provavelmente devido a diferentes demandas ambientais, e base sociocultural. Muitos estudos tem descrito a influência educacional em medidas neuropsicológicas, bem como em testes de rastreio, como o Mini-Exame do Estado Mental (MEEM). Objetivos: Analisar o desempenho de duas amostras de mesmo nível educacional e diferentes bases sociais e culturais. Métodos: Indivíduos de dois locais no Brasil foram submetidos ao MEEM, pareados por idade e escolaridade. Resultados: Houve diferença significativa entre as amostras quanto ao escore total e sub-itens de orientação temporal e sete-seriados, com melhor desempenho para a amostra urbana, mas quando analisados somente os analfabetos, os mesmos resultados foram observados, exceto para a cópia dos pentágonos, onde a população rural teve desempenho melhor. Conclusões: A base social e cultural além das demandas do meio ambiente influenciam nos testes de rastreio. Outros fatores, além da educação, devem ser revistos quando analisarmos os testes. Palavras-chave: Mini-Exame do Estado Mental, analfabetismo, nível educacional, teste de rastreio. Cognitive performance among illiterates and low educational levels is poorer than that observed in individuals with greater schooling. This difference can be a confounding factor in reaching an accurate diagnosis of cognitive impairment. In addition, there is great heterogeneity in performance among illiterates, probably due to different 1 MD, Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology Unit, Department of Neurology, University of São Paulo School of Medicine, São Paulo SP, Brazil. Sonia MD Brucki Rua Rio Grande 180 / 61-04018-000 São Paulo SP - Brazil. E-mail: sbrucki@uol.com.br Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest. Received January 19, 2009. Accepted in final form March 20, 2010. 120 Mini-Mental State Examination: transcultural evaluation Brucki SMD, Nitrini R

environmental demands and sociocultural backgrounds. The theory of cognitive reserve seeks to explain these findings, since more cognitive demands due to place and activities are factors stimulating brain development. In Brazil, many reports have described the influence of education on neuropsychological measures. Screening tests such as the Mini-Mental State Examination are influenced by schooling, with different cut-off scores. Other cognitive batteries show the same effect, with significance difference among scores by literacy and educational level, as observed on the Dementia Rating Scale, 1 CERAD cognitive battery 2 and ADAS-Cog. 3 Surprisingly, some tests, apparently free of the influence of education, such as Luria s fist-edge-palm, immediate memory, naming simple drawings are also affected by education. Illiterates possibly do not have an usual grapheme-phoneme association, hence their learning and retrieval of information via the semantic pathway, 4 which could be an important difference in illiterates from different backgrounds. Adaptation and validation of tests in each country and in different environments are crucial to minimize the false-positive rate for cognitive impairment in low educated people. Another important point is the concept of functional illiteracy, which can cause a bias in cognitive performance. Cognitive measures are effective for providing diagnostic clues in higher educated subjects, but are unable to achieve this in illiterates or functional illiterates. Norms are necessary for low educated individuals, and neuropsychological tests should be adapted and extensively applied to this group in order to define their cognitive profile. Since the devising and publication of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) by Folstein et al. 5 many studies have been published about the influence of educational levels on scores. The MMSE is commonly used in Brazil as a screening test for cognitive evaluation and as an instrument to follow-up demented patients. This test has become widely used as a screening tool for cognitive impairment worldwide, with numerous translations and adaptations. It was recommended by the Scientific Department of Cognitive Neurology and Aging of the Brazilian Academy of Neurology as a screening test for Alzheimer s disease. 6 Almost immediately, researchers perceived a bias in scores in relation to educational level. 7-9 Lower educated individuals presented different cut-off scores. Similarly, Anthony et al. 10 described that the sensitivity of the test was lower in African Americans than in White patients, and that this difference could be due to an educational bias, while specificity was also found to be lower in subjects with less than 8 years of schooling. Other authors have studied low educated populations along with other peculiarities such as cultural background and race, but the common factor linking results for differences among samples was educational level. 11,12 More recently, Rosselli et al. 13 carried out a population-based survey involving a random sample of urban and rural residents in Colombia. In this study, the MMSE scores correlated with educational level, where its effect was greater than that of age or gender. These authors concluded that low specificity led to the identification of many nondemented subjects with low educational status requiring further investigation. Many reports have shown similar effects in Brazil, with different scores according to schooling. 14-19 Performance of illiterates and lower educational groups have interested and led our group to study the influence of the cultural and natural environment on some tests. To this end, we compared people residing in the largest city of São Paulo (a highly industrialized city), Southeast Brazil, to those residing in the Amazonian region (Mamirauá Sustainable Development Reserve - MSDR), North of Brazil. There are many differences between these two locations, besides geographical and environmental differences. For instance, subjects from the Northern region are more likely to have native Indian roots and consequently different sociocultural habits and values 20 to those of inhabitants of a Southern city. These differences allowed comparison of individuals with the same educational levels but different cultural backgrounds for a widely used screening cognitive test, the MMSE. We expected that with the greater environmental demands of a big industrialized city, subjects from São Paulo would outperform Northerners on scores. Methods Subjects and environmental setting A sample of subjects residing in the Mamirauá Sustainable Development Reserve (MSDR) was matched by education to subjects residing in São Paulo. All subjects were Portuguese-speakers and part of a larger sample evaluated in the region. Participants included those present in the MSDR at the time of the research team s visit, aged 50 years or over, who were given a thorough medical examination. Clinical, neurological, neuropsychological and anthropometric evaluations were carried out. For this report only results on MMSE test are presented. The visits were made by boat to the MSDR over seven periods of 12 to 15 days each, comprising single visits for periods of one or two days per community. Subjects were interviewed at their homes or in community centers. Subjects were interviewed on the day of the research team s arrival. The MSDR is located about 600 km West of Manaus (Amazonas), in the Brazilian Amazonian region (Figure 1). These are conservation units designed to combine the preservation of habitats with the sustainable development of local resident communities. These areas are seasonally Brucki SMD, Nitrini R Mini-Mental State Examination: transcultural evaluation 121

Figure 1. Mamirauá Sustainable Development Reserve and São Paulo City. flooded, with water levels rising 10 to 12 meters above regular levels. The MSDR had 5,615 inhabitants, 435 of whom were over 49 years old (7.7%) (data provided by the Mamirauá Institute, census of 2002). The population lives in small communities along the river banks, each with 13 domestic households on average, where these are typically linked by kinship ties characterizing them as nuclei of small groups of related people. It is essentially a subsistence-based economy with very low incomes (annual family incomes of about US$900). Activities comprise fishing, growing manioc for flour and in some communities, hunting. The houses are timber-built and elevated from the ground because of high water levels. Activities extend throughout the lifetimes of most subjects. Elderly individuals were the only group presenting reduced physical activities. Women tended to work making flour, housekeeping and taking care of children. Almost all communities have schools, providing education for up to four years. The communities have limited access to radio, and television broadcasting, newspapers, daily use of telephone, and bank accounts. Electricity is produced by diesel generator (when available). The state of health is considered good, but there is a great difficulty in accessing health services which entails travelling to other urban centers, sometimes involving a 12 to 18-hour trip by small motor boat. The resident population has little access to formal education. Of the total population older than 15 years of age, 38% is illiterate while the others have an educational level of up to 4 years. Furthermore, the majority of the population is exposed to the same influences and opportunities of educational increments. Reading habits were scarce, generally limited to the Bible or reading in the classroom by the school children. Many adults that reported receiving several years of formal education were unable to read simple sentences. We evaluated 65 inhabitants aged 50 and older, based on collection of demographic data as well as a self-evaluation of subjects reading, writing and capacity to sign. Individuals were considered illiterate when they fulfilled all of the following three conditions: they had never attended school, or had attended for less than one year; they considered themselves unable to read, and were unable to read the phrase close your eyes from the Mini-Mental State Examination. None of the subjects were taking medications with central nervous system action. Participants were matched by educational level, gender, and age (within a five-year range) to subjects without cognitive complaints and independent daily activities, spouses or caregivers of patients of the Neurology outpatient clinic in Hospital Santa Marcelina (HSM). This hospital is located in a suburb of the Eastern region of São Paulo city; it is responsible for healthcare assistance of about three million people, predominantly subjects with lower educational level and socioeconomic conditions (C, D, and E classes - ABIPEME). Clinical evaluation was not performed in this sample. Exclusion criteria for both samples were motor difficulties, neurological or psychiatric complaints, and uncorrected visual or hearing deficits. Evaluation All subjects answered a semi-structured questionnaire about general conditions and subsequently performed the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE). For the MSDR sample, we provided some modifications in spatial orientation due 122 Mini-Mental State Examination: transcultural evaluation Brucki SMD, Nitrini R

Table 1. Sample comparisons regarding age, schooling, and MMSE scores. MSDR (n=65) HSM (n=65) p-value Age 50.1 (19.5) 52.5 (17.3) 0.448 Schooling 1.2 (1.7) 1.1 (1.8) 0.799 Total score 19.8 (3.6) 21.2 (3.5) 0.023 Temporal orientation 3.8 (1.2) 4.5 (0.7) 0.003 Spatial orientation 4.2 (0.9) 4.3 (0.9) 0.601 Immediate memory 2.9 (0.2) 2.9 (0.2) 0.812 Serial sevens 0.4 (1.2) 1.5 (1.4) <0.001 Recall 1.8 (1.0) 1.9 (1.1) 0.603 Naming 2.0 (0.17) 2.0 (0.0) 0.299 Repetition 0.8 (0.4) 0.8 (0.4) 0.732 Command 2.9 (0.3) 2.9 (0.4) 0.299 Reading 0.3 (0.5) 0.3 (0.4) 0.599 Writing 0.3 (0.4) 0.2 (0.4) 0.604 Copy of pentagons 0.4 (0.5) 0.2 (0.4) 0.063 *t-test. to local peculiarities, and therefore the items became: name of the community, name of the city, nearest city, nearest community, and State, replacing the original items: street or county, specific place, general place. 15 The remaining items were as per the Brazilian version. For the HSM sample all items of the version of Brucki et al were used. 15 Statistical analyses For statistical analyses, comparisons involving the MSDR and HSM samples were carried out using the Student t- test. The significance level was 0.05. The Statistica 4.3 (Statsoft, Inc, 1993) program was used for the analyses. The survey was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Hospital das Clínicas of the São Paulo University School of Medicine, of Hospital Santa Marcelina, and of the Mamirauá Institute. All subjects had given written consent for their participation in the study, or relatives had given written consent on behalf of those subjects unable to sign. Results Overall, there were 37 females in the samples (56.9%), and in the MSDR sample, 84.6% of subjects performed subsistence agriculture activities, while subjects in the São Paulo sample carried out a variety of activities. There was no difference between the two samples regarding age, schooling (Table 1) or gender. Results on the MMSE are shown in Table 1 and Figure 2. Analyzing only illiterates 30 26 22 18 14 10 BOX & WHISKER PLOT MEMTOTAL MEMTOT2 ±1.96*Std. Dev. ±1.00*Std. Dev. Mean Figure 2. Means on MMSE. MEMTOTAL, MSDR sample; MEMTOT2, sample of HSM. Table 2. Sample comparisons regarding age, and MMSE scores among illiterates. MSDR (n=42) HSM (n=42) p value* Age 58.1 (19.0) 51.9 (16.4) 0.331 Total score 18.2 (2.8) 19.6 (2.7) 0.024 Temporal orientation 3.5 (1.3) 4.4 (0.7) 0.005 Spatial orientation 4.0 (0.9) 4.3 (0.9) 0.246 Immediate memory 2.9 (0.2) 2.9 (0.2) 0.632 Serial sevens 0.02 (0.2) 1.1 (1.2) <0.001 Recall 1.7 (1.1) 1.9 (1.1) 0.627 Naming 2.0 (0.2) 2.0 (0.0) 0.284 Repetition 0.8 (0.4) 0.8 (0.4) 0.646 Command 2.9 (0.3) 2.8 (0.4) 0.204 Copy of pentagons 0.3 (0.5) 0.04 (0.2) 0.001 (n= 42) from both samples, the copy of pentagons task was the only different result in relation to the full samples, on which the MSDR sample presented significantly better performance (Table 2), a pattern repeated in the full sample. Discussion Our results showed a statistically significant difference between the samples in total scores on the MMSE, temporal orientation and calculation, where subjects of São Paulo city performed better than those of Mamirauá. In contrast, illiterates from Mamirauá outperformed subjects from São Brucki SMD, Nitrini R Mini-Mental State Examination: transcultural evaluation 123

Paulo on the copy of pentagons task, perhaps possessing better visuospatial perception than the illiterates from São Paulo city. Although there was no significant difference in age, the MSDR sample was older and thus aging-related perceptual deficits could not explain this finding. Samples were also matched by gender, minimizing possible gender differences. In developing countries such as Brazil, we have some difficulty classifying an examined illiterate because of different social, economic, and cultural backgrounds. Although some adaptations were made in spatial orientation for the MSDR subjects on the MMSE, no performance difference was detected on this sub-item. In fact, only items that are more closely related to environmental demands such as temporal orientation, and serial sevens and that seem more important for those living in a big city, such as making changes, or obeying strict schedules may explain this difference. We could not explain this difference as a function of other differences between samples, because the samples were matched by gender, and educational levels. Several studies have described comparisons among heterogeneous populations on MMSE scores, and emphasized the need to review specific differences. 21-25 Some differences in sub items analysis are due to language group differences, such as those described in a report comparing English and Spanish-speaking subjects, in which discrepant items were orientation to season, state, repeat phrase, and follow command. 26 A number of Brazilian studies have shown median or mean scores for illiterates to be very analogous considering this is a pool of subjects with different performances, reporting median scores of 18; 14 20; 15 and 19 16 points, and a mean of 17.4 (4.0). 18 However, in a large sample, Kochhann et al. 19 observed no differences between illiterates and schooled subjects, but noted a major impact of education among different educational groups. In clinical practice, we observed heterogeneous performances for the same conditions of illiteracy. This subjective impression is confirmed by systematic evaluation. Analyzing 79 healthy illiterates (between 26 to 82 years of age) and their performance on MMSE, and then dividing them into two groups based on lower quartile (P25=16 points) and upper quartile (P75=20 points), Brucki noted a significant difference in temporal orientation (year, semester, day of the week), spatial orientation (floor), calculation, copy of pentagons and reading. No differences were detected in naming, repetition or verbal command. 27 This study proved that many other factors probably influence illiterate performance besides education, including social and cultural factors. Previous history and occupations in life are important aspects to observe because they demand varying requirements and cognitive effort. We can hypothesize that the cultural influences and environmental demands a person experiences represent an influencing factor. It is likely that in the larger cities, even illiterates are better able to cope with some formal demands made in test situations. Indeed, when we homogenized samples by excluding literates, the differences were found to persist. In this study, we demonstrated that environmental and other sociocultural aspects are very important in cognitive performance, besides education, among individuals from the same country and with the same language. Other surveys should be performed on samples from various regions of Brazil to determine specific differences by geographical area. References 1. Porto CS, Fichman HC, Caramelli P, Bahia VS, Nitrini R. Brazilian version of the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale: diagnosis of mild dementia in Alzheimer s Disease. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2003;61:339-345. 2. Bertolucci PH, Okamoto IH, Brucki SM, Siviero MO, Toniolo Neto J, Ramos LR. Applicability of the CERAD neuropsychological battery to Brazilian elderly. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2001; 59:532-6. 3. Schultz RR, Silviero MO, Bertolucci PHF. The cognitive subscale of the Alzheimer s Disease Assessment Scale in a Brazilian sample. Braz J Med Biol Res 2001;34:1295-1302. 4. Reis A, Castro-Caldas A. Illiteracy: a cause for biased cognitive development. J Intern Neuropsychol Soc 1997;3:444-450. 5. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-198. 6. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bottino CM, Damasceno BP, Brucki SM, Anghinah R. Diagnosis of Alzheimer s disease in Brazil: cognitive and functional evaluation. Recommendations of the Scientific Department of Cognitive Neurology and Aging of the Brazilian Academy of Neurology. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2005;63:720-727. 7. Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS, et al. Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. JAMA 1993;269:2386-2391. 8. Fratiglioni L, Jorm AF, Grut M, et al. Predicting dementia from Mini-Mental State Examination in an elderly population: the role of education. J Clin Epidemiol 1993;46:281-287. 9. Magaziner J, Bassett SS, Hebel JR. Predicting performance on the Mini-Mental State Examination: use of age- and education-specific equations. J Am Geriatr Soc 1987;35:996-1000. 10. Anthony JC, LeResche L, Niaz U, et al. Limits of the Mini- Mental State as a screening test for dementia and delirium among hospital patients. Psychol Med 1982;12:397-408. 11. Escobar JI, Burnam A, Karno M, et al. Use of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in a community population of mixed ethnicity. J Nerv Ment Dis 1986;174: 607-614. 124 Mini-Mental State Examination: transcultural evaluation Brucki SMD, Nitrini R

12. Bird HR,Canino G, Rubio-Stipec M, et al. Use of the Mini- Mental State Examination in a probability sample of hispanic population. J Nerv Ment Dis 1989; 175: 731-735. 13. Rosselli D, Ardila A, Pradilla G, et al. El examen mental abreviado (Mini-Mental State Examination) como prueba de selección para el diagnóstico de demencia: estudio poblacional colombiano. Rev Neurol 2000;30:428-432. 14. Bertolucci PHF, Brucki SMD, Campacci S, et al. O Mini-Exame do Estado Mental em uma população geral: impacto da escolaridade. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 1994;52:1-7. 15. Brucki SMD, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bertolucci PH, Okamoto IH. Suggestions for utilization of the Mini-Mental State Examination in Brazil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2003;61:777-781. 16. Laks J, Baptista EM, Contino AL, de Paula EO, Engelhardt E. Mini-Mental State Examination norms in a community dwelling sample of elderly with low schooling in Brazil. Cad Saude Publica 2007;23:315-319. 17. Lourenço RA & Veras RP. Mini-Mental State Examination: psychometric characteristics in elderly outpatients. Rev Saúde Pública 2006;40:712-719. 18. Scazufca M, Almeida OP, Vallada HP, Tasse WA, Menezes PR. Limitations of the Mini-Mental State Examination for screening dementia in a community with low socioeconomic status. Results from the Sao Paulo Ageing & Health Study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2009;259:8-15. 19. Kochhann R, Cerveira MO, Godinho C, Camozzato A, Chaves MLF. Evaluation of Mini-Mental State Examination scores according to different age and education strata, and sex, in a large Brazilian healthy sample. Dem Neuropsychol 2009;3:88-93. 20. Hofstede G, Malvezzi S, Tanure B, Vinken H. Comparing regional cultures within a country: lessons from Brazil. J Cross Cult Psychol 2010;41:336-352. 21. Escobar JI, Burnam A, Karno M, Forsythe A, Landsverk J, Golding JM. Use of Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in a community population of mixed ethnicity. Cultural and linguistic artifacts. J Nerv Ment Dis 1986;174:607-614. 22. Tiwari SC, Tripathi RK, Kumar A. Applicability of the Mini- Mental State Examination (MMSE) and the Hindi Mental State Examination (HMSE) to the urban elderly in India: a pilot study. Int Psychogeriatr 2009;21:123-128. 23. Cross-cultural comparisons of the Mini-Mental State Examination between Japanese and US cohorts. Int Psychogeriatr 2009;21:113-122. 24. Ng TP, Niti M, Chiam PC, Kua EH. Ethnic and educational differences in cognitive test performance on Mini-Mental State Examination in Asians. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2007;15: 130-139. 25. Shah A, Ellanchenny N, Suh GH. A comparative study of behavioral and psychological signs and symptoms of dementia in patients with dementia referred to psychogeriatric services in Korea and the United Kingdom. Int Psychogeriatr 2004; 16: 219-236. 26. Jones RN. Identification of measurement differences between English and Spanish language versions of the Mini-Mental State Examination. Detecting differential item functioning using MIMIC modeling. Med Care 2006;44 (11 suppl 3):S124-133. 27. Brucki SMD. Diferenças intragrupo no desempenho no Mini- Exame do Estado Mental (MEM) entre analfabetos: temas em neuropsicologia e neurolingüística. Série de Neuropsicologia vol 4. Ed. Benito P Damasceno, Maria Irma H Coudry. Tec Art 1995, São Paulo. Brucki SMD, Nitrini R Mini-Mental State Examination: transcultural evaluation 125