A Longitudinal Study of the Achievements Progress and Attitudes of Severely Inattentive, Hyperactive and Impulsive Young Children

Similar documents
Prevalence of Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder among Primary School Students in Jeddah city, KSA.

Title: ADHD: Symptom Reduction in Follow up Period CMS ID: PP3 NQF #: N/A

Wonder Girls to Wonder Women: Lessons Learned from a Longitudinal Study of Girls with ADHD

For more than 100 years, extremely hyperactive

EFFECTS OF ADHD ON EARLY LEARNING AND ACDEMIC PERFORMANCE 1

Look inside for some tips on managing your child s ADHD as well as a list of online resources for further reading

Clinical evaluation of children testing positive in screening tests for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary report

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder How to manage these disorganized and inattentive children.

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Symptoms in Icelandic Schoolchildren: Assessment with the Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Rating Scale-IV

Working with Difficult Children in Primary Schools

SPN102: Attention Deficit Disorders Handout

Current Research on the Effective Treatment of. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Carolyn Micheli

The Role of Executive Functions in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Learning Disabilities

Citation for published version (APA): Jónsdóttir, S. (2006). ADHD and its relationship to comorbidity and gender. s.n.

To evaluate psychostimulants in the. ADHD treatment and academic performance: A case series. Brief Report. Practice recommendations

Understanding Students with Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

CHAPTER 2 CRITERION VALIDITY OF AN ATTENTION- DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER (ADHD) SCREENING LIST FOR SCREENING ADHD IN OLDER ADULTS AGED YEARS

Accessibility and Disability Service. A Guide to Services for Students with

STRATEGIES FOR STUDENTS WITH ADHD. Classroom Management Strategies for Students with ADHD Mary Johnson Seattle Pacific University 2016

Disorders of working memory: Causes and remediation

Week 2: Disorders of Childhood

I. Diagnostic Considerations (Assessment)...Page 1. II. Diagnostic Criteria and Consideration - General...Page 1

Use of medication by young people with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

Dr Veenu Gupta MD MRCPsych Consultant, Child Psychiatrist Stockton on Tees, UK

Childhood ADHD is a risk factor for some Psychiatric Disorders and co-morbidities

Background Information on ADHD

2. Conduct Disorder encompasses a less serious disregard for societal norms than Oppositional Defiant Disorder.

Conditions affecting children and adolescents

Oklahoma Psychological Association DSM-5 Panel November 8-9, 2013 Jennifer L. Morris, Ph.D.

Factors related to neuropsychological deficits in ADHD children

CHALLENGES AND INTERVENTIONS FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD IN THE EARLY YEARS

Other Health Impairment

Verbal Reasoning: Technical information

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), Conduct disorder biological treatments

ADHD and the classroom: Positive attitudes, positive strategies, positive results

Working memory: A cognitive system that supports learning?

QUT Digital Repository:

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Nathan J. Blum, M.D.

Prevalence of the ADHD phenotype in 7- to 9-year-old children: effects of informant, gender and non-participation

Attention-deficit hyperactive disorder in school-aged children in Saudi Arabia

!" #$! %./% ) 0 ' 10!" #

Effects of SEL education on children s socio-emotional competencies across Europe: results of EAP_SEL project

EDUCATORS TOOLKIT FOR DEALING WITH ADHD IN THE CLASSROOM

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

Presented by the National Resource Center on ADHD

among primary school children in Cachar, Assam, North-East India

Working memory in. development: Links with learning between. typical and atypical populations. TRACY PACKIAM ALLOWAY Durham University, UK

ADHD Guidance September 2013

ATTENTION DEFICIT DISORDER; AEROMEDICAL CONSIDERATIONS

Daily report cards as a schoolbased intervention for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

SAMPLE. Conners 3 Comparative Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

A circumstance or event that precedes a behavior. Uneasiness of the mind, typically shown by apprehension, worry and fear.

Ask the Expert Educator Edition

Teacher s Report Form Kindergarten/Year 1 Fast Track Project Technical Report Cynthia Rains November 26, 2003

SAMPLE. Conners 3 Self-Report Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

NICHQ Vanderbilt Assessment Follow-up PARENT Informant

Children's fitness testing: feasibility study summary

BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS AND SUBTYPES OF ATTENTION-DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER WITH COMORBIDITIES

ADHD in the classroom

March 10, Group 1 presentation 2. Personality and emotional development, and Gender (pwr. pnt. slides from Mar. 3, 2010) 4.

Improving Reading Comprehension for Students with ADHD

COGMED CLINICAL EVALUATION SERIES

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

A. The Broad Continuum of Attention Problems

SAMPLE. Conners 3 Self-Report Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

DOWNLOAD OR READ : PARENTING ADD ADHD CHILDREN PDF EBOOK EPUB MOBI

Behavioural interventions for ADHD.

Scoring Instructions for the VADTRS:

Constructing a Cloud-based ADHD Screening System: a Perspective of Norm Development

Adediran Olusola Akintunde Department of Educational Psychology Emmanuel Alayande College of Education, Oyo, Nigeria

ADHD Assessment and Treatment in Primary Care

Atomoxetine (First known as Tomoxetine) (Adopted by the CCG until review and further notice)

Summary ID# Clinical Study Summary: Study B4Z-SB-LYDD

Attention Deficit Disorder. Evaluation Scale-Home Version 16. The Attention Deficit Disorders. Evaluation Scale-School Version 17

ADHD: Shifting the paradigm to improve function and modify the natural history. Daryl Efron

Scoring Instructions for the VADPRS:

Russell Barkley, PhD. Internationally recognized authority on ADHD in children and adults

Can trait Emotional Intelligence. predict differences in attainment and progress. in secondary school?

Impact of Comorbidities on Self-Esteem of Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

FORM 3: ATTENTION DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER VERIFICATION

Author's response to reviews

The Context and Purpose of Executive Function Assessments: Beyond the Tripartite Model

8 th International Research Conference on Adolescents and Adults with FASD. April 18-21, 2018

CADDAC, a national not-for-profit organization that provides leadership in education, awareness and advocacy for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disor

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder The Impact of ADHD on Learning. Miranda Shields, PsyD

A Pair-Matched Case Control Study of Family-Environmental Factors Associated with ADHD in Chinese Children

SAMPLE. Conners 3 Parent Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

ADHD Symptomology and Prescription Stimulant Misuse in College Students

NICHQ Vanderbilt Assessment Follow-up PARENT Informant

Title Page for all entries in the Biomedical Breakthroughs and My Life Contest

Depressive disorders in young people: what is going on and what can we do about it? Lecture 1

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Adult ADHD for GPs. Maria Mazfari Associate Nurse Consultant Adult ADHD Tina Profitt Clinical Nurse Specialist Adult ADHD

DSM-5 Criteria for ADHD from

TEACHING CHILDREN WITH ADHD BEHAVIORAL INTERVENTIONS (ILLINOIS)

Academic Achievement Over 8 Years Among Children Who Met Modified Criteria for Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder at 4 6 Years of Age

studied and the number of sources used to establish a diagnosis (home, school, clinics, etc.). The absence of laboratory tests or neuroimaging

More boys than girls with attention deficit hyperactivity

SAMPLE REPORT. Conners 3 Parent Short Form Assessment Report. By C. Keith Conners, Ph.D.

Transcription:

A Longitudinal Study of the Achievements Progress and Attitudes of Severely Inattentive, Hyperactive and Impulsive Young Children Christine Merrell and Peter Tymms, CEM Centre, Durham University. Contact: Christine.Merrell@cem.dur.ac.uk Paper presented at BERA Annual Conference, Glamorgan, September 2005 Abstract This paper investigates the academic achievement and attitudes of a particular group of children during their time in primary school. The behaviour of children in a sample of just over 4000 pupils was assessed at the end of the Reception year to identify children who were exceptionally inattentive, hyperactive and impulsive, in other words children with symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). The achievement and progress of these children were followed up at regular intervals during their time in primary school up to age 11 years and compared with other children with no behavioural problems. The results showed that children with symptoms of ADHD start school with lower reading and mathematics scores than their pupils and continue to fall behind to a significant level over time.

Introduction Severely inattentive, hyperactive and impulsive behaviour has a major impact on various aspects of the lives of many children. Some of these children are diagnosed as having Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder (ADHD). The 4 th version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV), published by the American Psychiatric Association (1994) has a set of diagnostic criteria for ADHD and divides the disorder into three main sub-types: Combined subtype where individuals display symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsiveness, Predominantly Inattentive subtype where individuals mainly display symptoms of inattention, Predominantly Hyperactive-Impulsive subtype where individuals mainly display symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsiveness. The DSM-IV recommends that to be diagnosed with ADHD, behavioural problems must be observed in at least two different environments and present before the age of 7 years. The prevalence of children with ADHD has been estimated to be between 3% and 5% of the population (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). The proportion of children observed by their teachers as displaying severe ADHD symptoms in the classroom setting is somewhat higher and has been estimated to be between 8.1% and 17% (Merrell and Tymms, 2001, Gaub and Carlson, 1997, Wolraich, Hannah, Pinnock, Baumgaertel, and Brown, 1996). Pupils diagnosed as having ADHD are more likely to display delinquent, antisocial behaviour as adolescents, achieve lower grades at school than their peers and have less positive attitudes towards school (Barkley et al., 1991, Nussbaum et al., 1990, Solanto, 1990). Merrell and Tymms (2001) found that some of these trends extended to children with behavioural problems in the classroom but not necessarily a formal diagnosis of ADHD. They reported the size of the differences for reading and mathematics of children who were rated by their teachers as severely inattentive, hyperactive and impulsive against children with no reported problems between the start of Reception and Year 2. Over this period of time, large differences (Effect Sizes of 1 in some cases) were found for reading and mathematics. Children with behavioural problems started Reception with poorer reading and mathematics and continued to fall further behind. Merrell and Tymms argued that much of the evidence about the extent to which children with ADHD fall behind their peers in 2

academic subjects had been restricted to small sample sizes and the attainment and progress of children who exhibit the types of behaviour associated with ADHD in the classroom setting, but had not been formally diagnosed as having the disorder, merited investigation and comparison with studies of children who had been diagnosed to see if they were at risk of similar outcomes. They did indeed find evidence that such pupils were at risk of falling behind their peers academically. This paper extends that work by following up the reading and mathematics attainment, progress and attitudes of children from their start in school to the end of primary school, (Year 6, age 11 years). The 2001 paper used a sample of children that was nationally representative, however the analysis in this paper spans a far longer period and due to sample attrition over time, it was decided to include more children in the initial sample. The measures were the same as those used by Merrell and Tymms in 2001 with the addition of further assessments in later years. This paper also investigated differences and variation across schools. 3

Sample and Measures The data were routinely collected as part of the Performance Indicators in Primary Schools (PIPS) project run by the CEM Centre at Durham University. Schools, and in some cases whole local education authorities, pay to join the project which is intended to monitor the attainment, progress and attitudes of children throughout primary school. Schools complete PIPS assessments at specified times of year and then return all the assessment information to the CEM Centre for analysis. They receive pupil-level standardised feedback and as a result of that system the CEM Centre holds large longitudinal datasets that form a valuable resource for research (Tymms, 1999, www.pipsproject.org). Measures Pupils were assessed at the start and end of Reception (their first year of formal school, aged 4 5 years) with the PIPS On-entry baseline assessment. This included measures of handwriting, English vocabulary, concepts about print, phonological awareness, letter and word identification, reading, ideas about mathematics, counting and numerosity, informal sums, digit identification and more difficult mathematics problems. The assessment was administered on an individual basis and took 15 20 minutes to complete at both the beginning and end of the year. From this assessment, scores for reading and mathematics were calculated. At the end of the Reception Year, class teachers assessed the behaviour of each pupil using a rating scale that was based on the 18 criteria for the diagnosis of ADHD in DSM IV. (See Merrell and Tymms, 2001 for a full list of items.) This was an optional part of the PIPS assessment that could be completed in addition to the main components of reading and mathematics. Teachers were advised to consider a criterion met if the behaviour had persisted for at least six months and was considerably more frequent than that of children of the same gender and developmental level. One mark was awarded for each criterion met and the scores for each of the sub-types of ADHD (Combined, Predominantly Inattentive and Predominantly Hyperactive/Impulsive) were calculated using the same cut-off points as the DSM-IV recommends for ADHD i.e. 6 or more criteria relating to inattention for the Predominantly Inattentive subtype, 6 or more criteria relating to Hyperactivity and Impulsivity for the Predominantly Hyperactive/Impulsive subtype, and 6 ore more criteria relating to inattention plus 6 or more relating to hyperactivity and impulsivity for the Combined subtype. 4

The PIPS assessments administered in Year 2 (January), Year 4 (June) and Year 6 (January) followed the same format. They were all group assessments that included a half-hour assessment of reading, a half-hour assessment of mathematics, a halfhour assessment of science was included in Year 6 only, and a half-hour context section. The context section included assessments of English vocabulary, nonverbal ability and attitudes to reading, mathematics, science (in Year 6 only) and school. The content of the curriculum-based sections was based on the English National Curriculum (Department for Education and Employment, 1995a, b and c). The measures were the same as those used by Merrell and Tymms in 2001 with the obvious exception of the follow-up assessments at Years 4 and 6. For information about their reliability and predictive validity see Merrell and Tymms, 2001, Tymms, 1999 and www.pipsproject.org. Sample 5569 pupils (52.3% boys and 47.7% girls) from 208 schools in England were assessed at the start and end of reception and selected for this study on the basis of their teachers having completed the optional behaviour rating scale for all pupils in the class. For the analysis in this paper, children were then assigned to an ADHD subtype on the basis of their score on the behaviour rating scale. Children in the Zero scores group had met no criteria and children in the Combined, Predominantly Inattentive or Predominantly Hyperactive/Impulsive groups had met criteria equal to or higher than the cut-off points described in the previous section. Some children completed PIPS assessments at all time-points but others did not. Table 1 shows the number of pupils at each time-point for reading and mathematics respectively. Table 1 Pupil numbers Combined Inattentive Hyperactive/ Impulsive Zero scores Start Reception 150 (79% boys) 324 (65% boys) 164 (68% boys) 2544 (45% boys) End Reception 150 (79% boys) 323 (65% boys) 163 (68% boys) 2544 (45% boys) Year 2 Year 4 Year 6 150 (79% boys) 322 (65% boys) 163 (68% boys) 2544 (45% boys) 74 (77% boys) 180 (68% boys) 82 (76% boys) 1548 (45% boys) 27 (78% boys) 71 (68% boys) 37 (73% boys) 767 (47% boys) 5

Results Reading and Mathematics Attainment The reading and mathematics scores of the entire population of pupils completing PIPS assessments at each time-point were normalised before the analysis in this paper was carried out. There were always more boys than girls with high scores in each subtype of ADHD. The ratio of pupils meeting each ADHD subtype and the ratio of boys to girls were consistent with previous studies of children with ADHD symptoms in the classroom (Baumgaertel et al. 1995, Gaub and Carlson, 1997, Merrell, 2001). The differences in attainment between the zero scores group and each ADHD group were expressed as Effect Sizes, which were calculated using the following formula: Mean score of the experimental group Mean score of the control group Pooled standard deviation (Experimental group is each ADHD group, Control group is the zero scores group.) The Effect Sizes are summarised in Tables 2 and 3 for reading and mathematics respectively. Table 2 Effect Sizes for reading attainment Start Reception End Reception Year 2 Year 4 Year 6 Combined -0.69-0.83-1.07-1.18-1.23 Inattentive -0.79-0.92-1.04-1.15-1.19 Hyperactive/Impulsive -0.19-0.34-0.45-0.65-0.52 Table 3 Effect Sizes for mathematics attainment Start Reception End Reception Year 2 Year 4 Year 6 Combined -0.86-0.88-0.99-1.03-1.23 Inattentive -0.89-1.05-1.05-1.12-1.21 Hyperactive/Impulsive -0.24-0.2-0.3-0.5-0.49 Clear trends are apparent within each ADHD group of children starting Reception with lower mean scores than their peers who didn t have behavioural problems and that difference increasing with time. This is illustrated in Figures 1 and 2 where the 6

Effect Sizes with 95% Confidence Intervals are shown for reading and mathematics respectively. Figure 1 Reading Effect Sizes 0-0.2 Start End Year 2 Year 4 Year 6-0.4-0.6 Effect Size -0.8-1 -1.2-1.4-1.6-1.8 Combined Inattentive Hyperactive/Impulsive Figure 2 Mathematics Effect Sizes 0-0.2 Start End Year 2 Year 4 Year 6-0.4-0.6 Effect Size -0.8-1 -1.2-1.4-1.6-1.8 Combined Inattentive Hyperactive/Impulsive 7

The Effect Sizes show a general trend towards increasing differences between the ADHD groups and the Zero Scores group over time. The confidence intervals show that as the sample sizes decrease the level of uncertainty around the true-score increases and so the trend is not entirely clear. However, even if the true score of each ADHD group lies at the upper boundary of the confidence interval, the difference between those groups and the Zero Scores group is still significant and large in educational terms for those children in the Combined and Predominantly Inattentive groups. Figure 3 gives an alternative perspective on the impact of ADHD symptoms on children s attainment. Tables 1 and 2 give very similar effects for mathematics and reading and so the average has been used to represent school-based attainment. Further, Figures 1 & 2 show almost coincidental patterns for the inattentive and combined groups and since the definition of combined group included inattention the two groups were joined together and weighted averages for their attainments were calculated. Figure 3 shows how the attainment of these children has fallen relatively speaking from the age of 4 to 11. The line shows a gentle downward curve. If the children in each of the ADHD groups did not have any behavioural problems but did have low attainment scores compared with their peers, they would be expected to regress to the mean over time and the difference between their score and the sample average would decrease. The way in which they would regress to the mean is shown by dotted lines on the chart. Two points can be made from the chart. The first is that when the children reach the end of primary school, at the age of 11, their attainment is a full standard deviation behind the level that might be expected of children without such symptoms. The second is that an extrapolation of the line back towards birth would suggest that the hypothetical attainment levels would be in line with the average child at around that time. 8

Figure 3 Impact of Inattentiveness ADHD Symptoms on Attainment 0 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Age -0.2 Regression to mean -0.4 Relative Attainment -0.6-0.8-1 -1.2 Observed trajectory -1.4 Variation between classes Analysis using multi-level models in which pupils were nested within schools found that there was no significant variation between schools in the amount of progress made by the pupils in the ADHD groups. In other words, some teachers did not appear to be particularly effective with these kinds of children compared with other teachers. Attitudes Table 4 reports differences in attitudes towards reading, mathematics and school between children in the ADHD groups compared with the Zero Scores group. Children responded on a 3-point scale in Years 2 and 4, and a 5-point scale in Year 6 towards a series of statements. Typically there were 5 items for each domain and an example of a statement is I like reading. A mean score for each child was calculated for each subject. 9

Table 4 Attitudes Year 2 Combined Inattentive Hyperactive/Impulsive Year 4 Combined Inattentive Hyperactive/Impulsive Year 6 Combined Inattentive Hyperactive/Impulsive Reading Mathematics School p= 0.040 E.S. = - 0.2 p= 0.014 E.S. = -0.2 p= 0.560 E.S. = -0.04 p= 0.031 E.S.= -0.3 p=0.379 E.S. = -0.09 p=0.195 E.S. = -0.17 p= 0.849 E.S. = -0.04 p= 0.005 E.S. = -0.3 p= 0.264 E.S. =- 0.19 p= 0.004 E.S. = - 0.3 p= 0.044 E.S. = -0.1 p= 0.358 E.S. = -0.08 p= 0.031 E.S. = -0.2 p=0.966 E.S. = 0.00 p=0.485 E.S. = -0.07 p= 0.232 E.S. = -0.20 p= 0.984 E.S. = 0.00 p= 0.939 E.S. = 0.00 p= 0.042 E.S. = -0.2 p= 0.030 E.S.=0.2 p= 0.981 E.S. = 0.00 p= 0.071 E.S. = -0.29 p= 0.109 E.S. = -0.12 p= 0.011 E.S. = -0.3 p= 0.728 E.S. = -0.06 p= 0.179 E.S. = -0.15 p= 0.074 E.S. = -0.28 In Year 2, children in the Combined and Predominantly Inattentive groups were significantly more negative towards reading, mathematics and school than the comparison Zero Scores group although the Effect Sizes were quite small. As the children grew older, many of the earlier differences were no longer statistically significant although some of the Effect Sizes, for example attitudes to reading and to school of the hyperactive and impulsive children were actually similar to those seen in younger children. If the attitudes of the Combined and Predominantly Inattentive groups are combined to give an overall attitude score at ages 7, 9 and 11, the Effect Sizes for the differences between that group and the Zero Scores group were -0.10, -0.13 and -0.13 for the three ages respectively. A possible influential factor, for which there are no data available in this study, is that some of the children in the ADHD groups might have been prescribed Ritalin as they grew older. This could well have an impact on their attitudes but not necessarily their attainment. 10

Discussion The analysis reported in this paper build upon the earlier work of Merrell and Tymms (2001), following up the attainment and attitudes across the whole primary phase of a large, single cohort of children with and without ADHD symptoms. The size of the school-based sample of children with severe ADHD symptoms in English schools and the longitudinal nature of the study make its findings important. This is the first time that the attainment over time of such a group has been quantified and the extent to which the ADHD groups of children fall behind their peers gives reason for concern and intervention. A recent intervention study (Tymms and Merrell, 2004) found that providing teachers with advice about how to teach and manage the behaviour of children with severe ADHD behaviour had a positive impact on their behaviour and attitudes and although it did not impact on attainment at a significant level, there was evidence that increased attainment was associated with the more teachers reported using the booklet. Further large-scale evaluations of intervention programmes are essential if the long-term outcomes of children with severe ADHD behaviour are to be improved. 11

References American Psychiatric Association, (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Association. Barkley, R. A., Anastopoulos, A. D., Guevremont, D.C, and Fletcher, K. E. (1991). Adolescents with ADHD: Patterns of behavioural adjustment, academic functioning, and treatment utilisation. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adult Psychiatry 30(5): 752-761. Baumgaertel, A., Wolraich, M. and Dietrich, M. (1995). Comparison of diagnostic criteria for attention deficit disorders in a German elementary school sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 34: 629-638. Department for Education and Employment (1995a). English in the National Curriculum. London, HMSO. Department for Education and Employment, (1995b). Mathematics in the National Curriculum. London, HMSO. Department for Education and Employment, (1995c). Science in the National Curriculum. London, HMSO. Gaub, M. and Carlson, C. L. (1997). Behavioural Characteristics of DSM-IV ADHD Subtypes in a School-Based Population. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 25(2): 103-111. Merrell, C. (2001) The Prevalence, Attainment And Progress Of Severely Inattentive, Hyperactive And Impulsive Young Children, PhD Thesis, Durham University, UK. Merrell, C. and Tymms, P. (2001). Inattention, hyperactivity and impulsiveness: Their impact on academic achievement and progress. British Journal of Educational Psychology 71: 43-56. Nussbaum, N. L., Grant, M. L., Roman, M.J., Poole, J.H. and Bigler, E.D. (1990). Attention Deficit and the mediating effect of age on academic and behavioural variables. Journal of Behavioural and Developmental Paediatrics 11: 22-26. Solanto, M. V. (1990). Increasing Difficulties with Age in ADHD Children. Developmental and Behavioural Pediatrics 11(1): 27. Tymms, P. (1999). Baseline Assessment and Monitoring in Primary Schools: Achievements, Attitudes and Value-added Indicators, (London: David Fulton Publishers). Tymms, P. and Merrell, C. (2004) Screening and Classroom Interventions For Inattentive, Hyperactive and Impulsive Young Children A Longitudinal Study. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, U.S.A, April 2004. Wolraich, M. L., Hannah, J. N., Pinnock, T.Y., Baumgaertel, A. and Brown, J. (1996). Comparison of diagnostic criteria for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a country-wide sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 35(3): 319-324. 12