LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT

Similar documents
VENTRAL TEGMENTAL AREA LESIONS DIFFERENTIALLY AFFECT RESPONSES CONTROLLED BY CS-US CONTIGUITY AND RESPONSE-REINFORCER CONTINGENCY IN THE RAT

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

Supplementary Methods

GABA injected into the anterior dorsal tegmentum ADT of the midbrain blocks stepping initiated by stimulation of the hypothalamus

Nucleus accumbens lesions impair context, but not cue, conditioning in rats

Supporting Online Material for

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

-80 Figure 1. Identification of dopaminergic neurons in. VTA slices (a) Micrographs demonstrate the location of the VTA with

Effects of limbic corticostriatal lesions on a u t o shaping performance in rats

Brain Research Bulletin 67 (2005) Toronto, Ont., Canada M6A 2E1 b Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque,

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn.4642

AMOUNT OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CHANGE IN THE CS AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING 1

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Behavioral effects of intra septal injections of adrenergic drugs in rats

Brainstem: Midbrain. 1. Midbrain gross external anatomy 2. Internal structure of the midbrain:

Inhibition of the Acquisition of Conditioned Place Aversion by Dopaminergic Lesions of the Central Nucleus of the Amygdala in Morphine-Treated Rats

Organizing behavior in time is a key feature of goal-directed behavior and humans

Hippocampus and Two-Way Active Avoidance Conditioning: Contrasting Effects of Cytotoxic Lesion and Temporary Inactivation

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Infusions of AP5 into the basolateral amygdala impair the formation, but not the expression, of step-down inhibitory avoidance

NSCI 324 Systems Neuroscience

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

Summary of behavioral performances for mice in imaging experiments.

The Journal of Physiology Neuroscience

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats

Supplemental Information. Dorsal Raphe Dual Serotonin-Glutamate Neurons. Drive Reward by Establishing Excitatory Synapses

Representation of negative motivational value in the primate

Short communication NORADRENERGIC STIMULATION OF THE LATERAL HYPOTHALAMUS AS A REINFORCEMENT IN T MAZE LEARNING IN RATS

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)

Parkinson s disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disorder that mainly affects individuals

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

BRIEF REPORT. Impairments in Sodium Appetite after Lesions of Gustatory Thalamus: Replication and Extension I

Reinforcement learning and the brain: the problems we face all day. Reinforcement Learning in the brain

Behavioural Brain Research

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Down regulation of the HCN2 channel in rat models of Parkinson s disease

<student name> Undergraduate Research Grant Proposal

Brainstem. Amadi O. Ihunwo, PhD School of Anatomical Sciences

Supporting Online Material

Does nicotine alter what is learned about non-drug incentives?

Dopamine in Ube3a m-/p+ mice. Online Supplemental Material

Dorsomedial thalamic lesions and amphetamine: Acquisition and retention of a visual pattern discrimination escape task

Nigral Projections to the Inferior and the Superior Colliculus in the Rat: A Horseradish Peroxidase Study

Some determinants of second-order conditioning

FUNCTION OF CAT'S CAUDATE NUCLEUS IN TASKS INVOLVING SPATIAL DISCONTIGUITY BETWEEN LOCATION OF CUE AND RESPONSE

The ventral hippocampus and fear conditioning in rats

TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE. The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in

The influence of the information value provided by prior-cuing treatment on the reactivation of memory in preweanling rats

Is There Savings for Pavlovian Fear Conditioning after Neurotoxic Basolateral Amygdala Lesions in Rats?

Mk-801 Administration in Adolescent Male Rats and Cocaine Conditioned Place

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Dopamine receptors in the ventral tegmental area affect motor, but not motivational or reflexive, components of copulation in male rats

MeCP2 and psychostimulantinduced behavioral adaptations. Anne E. West, M.D., Ph.D. Department of Neurobiology Duke University Medical Center

The Nottingham eprints service makes this work by researchers of the University of Nottingham available open access under the following conditions.

Cooling as Reinforcing Stimulus in Aplysia

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION

Conditioned Stimulus Familiarity Determines Effects of MK-801 on Fear Extinction

Abstract. R. Roesler 1, J. Quevedo 1, C. Rodrigues 1, M. Madruga 1, M.R.M. Vianna 1 and M.B.C. Ferreira 2

The effect of controllable and uncontrollable neonatal preshocks on adult escape/avoidance behavior in the guinea pig (Cavia porcellus)

Pattern Memory Involves Both Elemental and Configural Processes: Evidence From the Effects of Hippocampal Lesions

INTRODUCTION. Ninglei Sun 1, Steven R Laviolette*,1,2,3 and Addiction Research Group

Neurobiology of 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats: Electrode mapping, lesion, and pharmacology studies

Key words: amphetamine; apomorphine; phencyclidine; 8-OH-DPAT; prepulse inhibition; dopamine; motor behavior; schizophrenia; rats; startle

Functional Inactivation of the Amygdala before But Not after Auditory Fear Conditioning Prevents Memory Formation

Supplementary Figure 1

Research Paper. Christopher W. Butler, 1 Yvette M. Wilson, 1 Jenny M. Gunnersen, and Mark Murphy

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Auditory and Vestibular Systems

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

Basolateral amygdala lesions in the rat produce an abnormally persistent latent inhibition with weak preexposure but not with context shift

Class 16 Emotions (10/19/17) Chapter 10

GLUTAMATE RECEPTORS IN THE VENTRAL TEGMENTAL AREA: A POTENTIAL MECHANISM INVOLVED IN LONG TERM POTENTIATION

Brain Mechanisms of Emotion 1 of 6

EFFECTS OF CHLOROGENIC ACID ON LEARNING AND MEMORY IN RATS

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

Green Tea Polyphenol Epigallocatechin-3-Gallate Attenuates Behavioral Abnormality in Hemi-Parkinsonian Rat

INFLUENCE OF RETROACTIVE INTERFERENCE ON THE CONTEXT SHIFT EFFECT

Zahedan Journal of Research in Medical Sciences. Journal homepage:

Basal Ganglia General Info

Encoding Specific Associative Memory: Evidence From Behavioral and Neural Manipulations

Learning. Association. Association. Unit 6: Learning. Learning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Different Types of Learning

Neural Recording Methods

Chemogenetic manipulation of ventral pallidal neurons impairs acquisition of sign-tracking in rats

The role of dopamine in learning, memory, and performance of a water escape task

Supplementary Materials for

Classical conditioning using stimulation of the inferior olive as the unconditioned stimulus

Association. Operant Conditioning. Classical or Pavlovian Conditioning. Learning to associate two events. We learn to. associate two stimuli

Myers PSYCHOLOGY. (7th Ed) Chapter 8. Learning. James A. McCubbin, PhD Clemson University. Worth Publishers

SAMPLE EXAMINATION QUESTIONS

Supplementary Figure 1. Recording sites.

Supramammillary and adjacent nuclei lesions impair spatial working memory and induce anxiolitic-like behavior

Classical Conditioning II. Generalization and Discrimination. Generalization Example. Generalization/Discrimination; Time and the Order of Events

EFFECT OF CONVULVULUS PLURICAULIS CHOISY. ON LEARNING BEHAVIOUR AND MEMORY ENHANCEMENT ACTIVITY IN RODENTS

The Effects of Temporary Inactivation of the Basolateral Amygdala on the Maternal Behavior of Post-partum Rats

Transcription:

ACTA NEUROBIOL: EXP. 1988, 48: 117-121 Short communication LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT W. Jeffrey WILSON and Jennifer C. HALL Department of Psychological Sciences Indiana University - Purdue University at Fort Wayne 2101 Coliseum Blvd. East, Fort Wayne, IN 46805 USA Key words: 6-hydroxydopamine, rat, escape, classical conditioning, mesolimbic dopamine Abstract. Classically conditioned shuttling and escape responses of 14 rats with 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the ventral tegmental area (VTA) were compared with those of 16 sham-lesioned control rats in an aversive classical conditioning paradigm. Although the 6-OHDA lesion did not enhance classically conditioned shuttling, it eliminated the facilitation of escape responses by the conditioned stimulus. Thus, whereas the classically conditioned sham-lesioned rats were faster to escape shock than were the pseudoconditioned sham-lesioned rats, the 6-OHDA classically conditioned and pseudoconditioned rats were equally slow to escape. This suggests that the ability of the rat to respond appropriately to the motivational or informational content of the warning signal was disrupted following damage to the mesolimbic dopamine system. Electrolytic lesions of the ventral tegmental area (VTA) in the rat enhance signalled active avoidance performance (3, 5). Using a procedure developed by Izquierdo (I), we have previously demonstrated that this enhancement results from the facilitation of classically conditioned fear responses to the signal (8). Based on earlier studies involving dopamine infusion, or damage to the nucleus accumbens (7, 9), we proposed that this effect was due to damage to the mesolimbic dopamine system, rather than to damage to other neural systems present at the site of the lesion.

We tested this hypothesis in the present experiment by infusing 6-hydroxydopamine into the VTA, then examining the behavior of the rats in a classical conditioning procedure similar to that used by Wilson and Baeske (8). Thirty-two female Sprague-Dawley rats obtained from Harlan Sprague-Dawley, Indianapolis, IN, USA served as subjects. The rats were housed individually with ad lib. access to food and water, and were maintained on a reversed 12 h light-dark cycle. Rats were approximately 100 days old at the time of surgery. Bilateral lesions of the mesolimbic dopamine system were produced in 16 rats by the infusion of 2 pg 6-OHDA (Sigma Chemical Co., St. Louis, MO, USA) in a volume of 1 p1 isotonic saline + mg/ml ascorbic acid (4). The 6-OHDA solution was prepared immediately before surgery on a group of 3 rats, and was kept on ice until infusion. Infusion was done manually via a 10 pl Hamilton microsyringe mounted in the stereotaxic device, at a rate of 1 pllmin. Stereotaxic coordinates used were 4.8 mm posterior to Bregma, + 1.0 mm lateral to the midline, and 8.4 mrn ventral to skull surface, from the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (6). Vehicle alone was infused into 16 additional rats, using the same procedure. Rats were anesthetized during surgery with sodium pentobarbitol (Nembutal, 45 mglkg), and were allowed at least one week to recover following surgery. Behavioral testing was conducted in two identical plastic shuttleboxes, as described previously (8). Briefly, the shuttleboxes measured 44.3 X X22.5 X 21.5 cm, had floors consisting of stainless steel bars, and were housed in sound-attenuating chambers. Movement of the rat from one side to the other caused the box to tilt slightly, constituting a response. A Sonalert speaker, mounted above the shuttlebox, provided a tone that served as the conditioned stimulus (CS). A 1.5 ma shock scrambled through the floor bars served as the unconditioned stimulus (US). Presentation of stimuli and the timing and recording of responses were carried out by IBM-compatible microcomputers through locally developed hardware interfaces and software. Beginning at least 1 week after surgery, 8 Lesion and 8 Control rats received 8 daily Classical Conditioning sessions in which a 5 s tone CS was paired with a 1.5 ma' scrambled foot shock US. The remaining rats received Pseudoconditioning sessions in which the tone and shock were explicitly unpaired. Each session consisted of 50 presentations of both tone and shock, with tones occurring randomly every 10 to 40 s. The shock remained on until the rat shuttled, or for a maximum of 5 s in the absence of a shuttle response. Responses during the tone and during the inter-tone interval were recorded, as was the latency to escape

the shock. At the conclusion of the experiment, the rats were euthanized with an overdose of sodium pentobarbitol, then perfused transcardially with isotonic saline and formalin. The brains were removed, embedded in gelatin, and sliced into 80 pm sections for verification of the site of injection. In 14 of the 16 6-OHDA rats, injections occurred bilaterally within the VTA. In two rats the injections missed the VTA on both sides. Data from these two subjects (one Classically Conditioned and one Pseudoconditioned) were not included in the data analysis. Very few classically conditioned shuttle responses occurred in either the 6-OHDA- or the Sham-lesioned groups in each session over the 8 days (Means + SEM: 6-OHDA Classical, 1.18 + 0.29; 6-OHDA Pseudo, 1.09 + 0.23; Sham Classical, 1.93 + 0.54; Sham Pseudo, 1.32 0.45). These numbers are comparable to those reported for unlesioned rats in Fig. 1. Schematic coronal section through the rat brain illustrating the sites of injection of 6-OHDA in 16 rats. Solid circles mark sites in 14 rats with bilateral injections within the VTA. Open circles mark sites in two rats in which the injections missed bilaterally. Data from these two rats were not included in data analysis. Brain section adapted from Paxinos and Watson (5, Fig. 37). Abbreviations as follow: LG, lateral geniculate n.; MG, medial geniculate n.; MB: mammillary body; PR, prerubral field; SN, substantia nigra; ZI, zona incerta; cp, cerebral peduncle; f, fasciculus retroflexus; ml, medial lemniscus; pc, posterior commissure. our study of the effects of electrolytic lesions in this apparatus (8). Unlike the electrolytic lesion employed in that study, the 6-OHDA lesion failed to enhance the classically conditioned responses. Overall responsiveness in the inter-tone interval was similarly unaffected by the 6-OHDA lesion (Means + SEM: 6-OHDA Classical,

20.63 + 2.31; 6-OHDA Pseudo, 16.45 fi 4.02; Sham Classical, 11.91 k + 1.65; Sham Pseudo, 16.27 + 5.85; all differences nonsignificant). However, the lesion did affect latency to escape the shock: whereas Classically Conditioned Control rats tended to escape the shock more rapidly than the Pseudoconditioned Control rats (Fl,14 = 7.37, P, < 0.05), 6-OHDA Classically Conditioned and Pseudoconditioned rats were equally slow to escape (F1,lP = 0.13 (see Fig. 2), responding with latencies comparable to those of the Pseudoconditioned controls. Thus the 6-OHDA lesion disrupted the rats' ability to respond to the signal for shock by preparing to escape. Pain sensitivity or threshold was not affected by the lesion, as indicated by the similarities in the latencies to escape of the lesioned rats and the Pseudoconditioned controls. 2.00- I 1.50-1.00-.50- - 6-OHDA, Class *--6-OHDA, Pseudo -Sham, Class ---Shorn, Pseudo 0 I I I I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Days Fig. 2. Latency (mean + SEM) to escape shock in the four groups of rats. The Sham-lesioned Classically Conditioned group escaped significantly faster than the Sham-lesioned Pseudoconditioned group. The two 6-OHDA groups did not differ in their latencies to escape. I Two important conclusions can be drawn from the results of this experiment: First, the failure of the 6-OHDA lesion to enhance classically conditioned shuttling responses suggests that the enhancement that is produced by electrolytic lesions does not involve the mesolimbic dopamine system in any critical way. Further studies with discrete chemical lesions will be necessary to determine the system or systems responsi'ble for the effect of the electrolytic lesion. Second, because lesioned classically conditioned rats escaped as slowly

as lesioned and unlesioned pseudoconditioned rats, the results indicate that the ability of a warning signal to facilitate escape responses is disrupted by this lesion. Thus, although Koob et al. (2) have suggested that lesions of both the mesolimbic and nigrostriatal dopamine systems are necessary to disrupt avoidance performance, damage to the mesolimbic system alone is not without its effect in aversively motivated behavior. Perhaps the nigrostriatal and mesolimbic dopamine systems play complementary roles in aversively motivated behaviors, with the mesolimbic primarily involved in affective or motivational aspects, and the nigrostri~,tal in motor components of the behavior. The authors wish to thank C. L. Wilson for Her comments on an early. version of this manuscritpt. This investigation was supported in part by a Project from the Indiana University - Purdue University at Fort Wayne Research and Instructional Development Support Program. 1. IZQUIERDO, I. and ELISABETSKY, E. 1979. Physiological and pharmacological dissection of the main factors in the acquisition and retention of shuttle behaviour. In M. A. B. Brazier (ed.), Brain mechanisms in memory and learning: from the single neuron to man. Raven Press, New York, p. 227-248. 2. KOOB, G.'F., SIMON, H., HERMAN, J. P. and LeMOAL, M. 1984. Neuroleptic-like disruption of the conditioned avoidance response requires destruction of both the mesolimbic and nigrostriatal dopamine systems. Brain Res. 303: 319-329. 3. LeMOAL, M., CARDO, B. and STINUS, L. 1969. Influences of ventral mesencephalic lesions on various spontaneous and conditioned behaviors in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 4: 567-573. 4. LeMOAL, M., GALEY, D. and CARDO, B. 1975. Behavioral effects of local injection of 6-hydroxydopamine in the medial ventral tegmentum in the rat. Possible role of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system. Brain Res. 88: 190-194. 5. LORENS, S. A., SORENSEN, J. P. and HARVEY, J. A. 1970. Lesions in thd nuclei accumbens septi of the rat: behavioral and neurochemical effects. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 73: 284-290. 6. PAXINOS, G. and WATSON, C. 1986. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates (second edition). Academic Press, New York. 7. WILSON, W. J. 1983. Nucleus accumbens inhibits specific motor but not nonspecific classically conditioned responses. Brain Res. Bull. 10: 505-515. 8. WILSON, W. J. and BAESKE, E. G. 1987. Ventral tegmental area lesions differentially affect responses controlled by CS-US contiguity and responsereinforcer contingency in the rat. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 47: 83-91. 9. WILSON, W. J. and SOLTYSIK, S. S. 1985. Pharmacological manipulations of the nucleus accumbens: effects on classically conditioned responses and locomotor activity in the cat. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 45: 91-105. Acceped I December 1987